Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 4

379

© 2006 Universities Federation for Animal Welfare Animal Welfare 2006, 15: 379-382
The Old School, Brewhouse Hill, Wheathampstead, ISSN 0962-7286
Hertfordshire AL4 8AN, UK

Long-term social memory in the laboratory rat (Rattus norvegicus)

OHP Burman* and M Mendl


Division of Farm Animal Science, Department of Clinical Veterinary Science, University of Bristol, Langford, Bristol BS40 5DU, UK
* Contact for correspondence and request for reprints: oliver.burman@bristol.ac.uk

Abstract
A key question in the management of group-housed captive animals is how long can an individual be removed from a social group
and still be reintroduced with minimal social upheaval. In order to answer this question we require a knowledge of how long cage-
mates, following a specified period of group-housing, can remember one another after separation. This issue was investigated in labo-
ratory rats (Rattus norvegicus). Rats were group-housed for 18 days before being housed individually. One hour, 48 hr, and 96 hr
after separation, they were exposed simultaneously to odour cues originating from unfamiliar rats and from former cage-mates. The
rats spent significantly more time investigating the unfamiliar odour 1 hr and 48 hr, but not 96 hr, after separation, suggesting that,
after 18 days of group-housing, juvenile rats remember former cage-mates for between 48 and 96 hr. The implications of this result
for animal welfare are discussed.

Keywords: animal welfare, habituation/discrimination, laboratory rats, long-term memory, social memory, social recognition

Introduction experimental purposes, and, second, established tests exist


Research into different areas of animal cognition eg studies for assessing social memory in laboratory rats (Thor &
of social memory and recognition (Burman & Mendl 2000, Holloway 1982; Engelmann et al 1995). Whilst short-term
2004), spatial memory (Laughlin & Mendl 2000, 2004), memory for conspecifics, typically over periods of 30 - 120
visual perspective taking (Held et al 2001a,b), not only min, has been the focus of much research (Thor &
furthers our understanding of the fundamental cognitive Holloway 1982; Dantzer et al 1987; Hlinak & Krejci 1991;
processes of non-human animals, but also provides us with Gheusi et al 1994; Engelmann et al 1995), comparatively
information that may help to improve animal husbandry and little attention has been directed towards long-term social
welfare (Nicol 1996; Mendl et al 2001). memory. Using spontaneous behaviour tests (eg the habitu-
For example, because the majority of group housed captive ation/discrimination technique [see later]), researchers have
animals are kept together for extended periods (eg weeks: shown that rats appear unable to remember conspecifics for
broiler chickens; months: laboratory rodents; years: zoo longer than two hours (Bluthé & Dantzer 1990; Taylor et al
animals), studies of long-term social memory abilities may 1999) when the initial encounter with the to-be-remembered
provide important information on how best to manage and conspecific is around 5 min long. Even when rats received
control husbandry and housing procedures that disrupt and six × 5 min encounters each separated by 10 min intervals
reunite established social groupings (Mendl 1999). An they appeared unable to remember conspecifics 24 h later
increase in aggression, and therefore an apparent reduction (Sekiguchi et al 1991).
in welfare, can occur when animals are reunited after a However, it may not be realistic to expect rats to remember
period of separation (Ewbank & Meese 1971). Thus, if an conspecifics long-term when they have only had the oppor-
animal is to be removed from a stable social group for tunity to encounter those conspecifics for such a brief initial
husbandry or experimental reasons, it is important to know period of time. Such studies also do not reflect real-life
for how long that individual can be separated from its husbandry procedures, whereby animals are often group-
group-mates while still allowing safe reintroduction. In housed for several days before any individuals are
order to answer this question we need to know for how long, temporarily removed. We decided, therefore, to investigate
following a given period of group-housing, former cage- long-term social memory in laboratory rats by housing indi-
mates can remember one another after separation. viduals together for 18 days, and then assessing whether or
We investigated this issue using laboratory rats because, not, following this extended familiarisation period, the rats
first, they tend to be housed in groups for several weeks and were subsequently able to remember former cage-mates
can be separated from their social groups temporarily for after separation – from one hour up to four days.

Universities Federation for Animal Welfare Science in the Service of Animal Welfare

https://doi.org/10.1017/S0962728600030712 Published online by Cambridge University Press


380 Burman and Mendl

The results of this study have direct implications for the two stimuli within each test, rather than between tests, that
welfare of laboratory rats, and, if we consider the rat as a is important. In this study, rather than repeatedly presenting
model species, may also have implications for the welfare the odour of one individual to another to induce habituation
of other group-housed captive species. and the presumed familiarity of that odour; familiarity, and
hence habituation, were assumed to have occurred
Materials and methods
following the group housing of subject and stimulus
Subjects, housing, and care animals for an extended period of time (Epple & Niblick
1997; Mateo & Johnston 2000).
We used 16 unrelated (no shared parent) female (21 days
old at the start of experiment) Lister hooded rats (Harlan A potential problem with using an habituation/discrimina-
UK Limited, Bicester, UK). Unrelated rats were used to tion technique of this kind is that there can be difficulty with
ensure that it was familiarity per se that was examined; with the interpretation of ‘negative’ results. Whilst a statistically
no confounding influence of kin recognition. We used significant preference to investigate the novel stimulus, ie
juvenile rats in order to avoid any potentially confounding discrimination, can be interpreted relatively reliably as a
effects of dominance on performance in the recognition of, and habituation to, the original stimulus, any
habituation/discrimination technique (Carr et al 1976; failure to show discrimination should be interpreted with
Brown 1992). Previous research has suggested no differ- caution. For example, a negative result should be described
ence between the sexes in pre-maturation social memory as ‘a failure to demonstrate discrimination’ rather than ‘a
performance (Thor & Holloway 1982). All the rats were failure to discriminate’ per se.
housed in the same room in which the experiments took Testing procedure
place, with temperature 19 ± 1°C, relative humidity 46% At 21 days of age the rats were housed in four groups of
and a reverse dark-light cycle (light on from 1900 - 0700h). four for 18 days, to allow them to become familiar with the
Observations were made in the dark phase of the cycle, with other members of their particular group. They were then
dim ‘white’ light (10 watt) provided during experimenta- separated and housed individually for the duration of testing
tion. Home cages, (50 × 33 × 21 cm) (a total period of four days) after which they were group-
(length × width × height), contained sawdust bedding, an housed for use in a further experiment. Half the rats were
enrichment toy and ad libitum food (Harlan Teklad randomly selected as subjects (n = 8) and the remainder as
Laboratory Diet) and water. We identified individual rats odour donors. The subjects were tested at one hour, 48 h,
from natural variation in coat colour markings. Disposable and 96 h post separation. Tests were carried out in the home
gloves were worn when handling rats to restrict odour cage, and involved the simultaneous presentation of unfa-
transfer. This experiment was carried out with a University miliar and familiar (former cage-mate) odour cues for
of Bristol investigation number (UB/98/L038), as deter- 10 min. Significantly more time spent investigating the
mined by the University of Bristol Home Office Liaison unfamiliar odour was taken to indicate successful recogni-
Team (HOLT). tion of the familiar odour (Johnston 1993). For each of the
The habituation/discrimination technique three tests, odour cues from different familiar and unfa-
miliar rats were used, so that subjects never encountered the
We used the habituation/discrimination technique
same odour cues during repeated testing.
(Johnston & Jernigan 1994; Johnston & Bullock 2001).
This procedure involves repeated presentation of a Time (seconds) spent investigating the odours was recorded
stimulus (eg odour cue) obtained from a specific indi- (ie sniffing, licking, or the subject’s nose being within one
vidual to a subject animal. The behavioural response of cm of the odour container) using a Psion event recorder
the subject towards this stimulus, usually investigation, is (Psion PLC, London, UK) with ‘Observer’ software
recorded, and generally decreases with each presentation. (Noldus Information Technology, Wageningen, Holland).
Following this habituation, which is taken to indicate the Odour cues
subject’s familiarity with the stimulus, odour stimuli from Previous studies have indicated the suitability of using
both the original individual and a novel individual are odour cues as social stimuli in recognition tests (Carr et al
then introduced simultaneously. Successful discrimina- 1976; Sawyer et al 1984; Burman & Mendl 2002). We
tion between the two stimuli, in terms of significantly obtained the odour samples by placing a sheet of absorbent
more time spent investigating the novel odour stimulus, paper on the floor of the rats’ individual home-cages for one
indicates continued habituation for, and thus recogni- hour prior to testing. This ensured that urine samples were
tion/memory of, the original stimulus. relatively uniform in age. Where the rat had urinated on the
This technique should overcome any potentially paper (all rats urinated at least once within the hour),
confounding motivational changes between the introduction scissors were used to cut out the marked area and this
and subsequent reintroduction of the same stimuli (Johnston sample was then placed inside a spherical wire mesh
1993) that could, for example, result in a general elevation container (20 cm³). We attached the containers to a test cage
or decrease in response levels that mask/accentuate habitu- lid that could then be introduced into the home cage of the
ation or dishabituation. Therefore, regardless of motivation, subject rat when testing was to start. The two odour samples
it is the comparison between the level of investigation of the were presented at opposite sides of the subjects’ home cage,

© 2006 Universities Federation for Animal Welfare

https://doi.org/10.1017/S0962728600030712 Published online by Cambridge University Press


Long-term social memory in rats 381

and these positions (either left or right side) were alternated Figure 1
between different subjects to avoid any effect of side pref-
erence. The odour containers, the test cage lid, and the
scissors were disinfected between each odour collection and
each test, and latex gloves were also used to minimise the
risk of odour transfer between samples.
Data analysis
The total amount of time (seconds) spent investigating each
of the two containers holding the odour samples was
analysed using Minitab version 12 (Minitab Inc,
Philadelphia, USA). The data met requirements for
normality and homogeneity of variance, and so a parametric
repeated measures General Linear Model (GLM) was used
to analyse the data with paired t-tests for post hoc analysis.
Descriptive statistics of normally distributed data are
means ± standard errors.
Results
Differences in the amount of investigation directed towards the
An initial analysis was carried out to compare the levels of
familiar (white columns) and unfamiliar (black columns) odour
total investigation (unfamiliar + familiar odour) between the stimuli by the subjects, at one hour, 48 hours, and four days after
three tests (1 hr, 48 hr, 96 hr). This revealed a significant separation.
difference between the tests (F2,14 = 15.04, P < 0.001), with
post hoc analysis indicating a significant reduction in total
investigation between the 1 hr and 48 hr tests (P < 0.01), but Our study thus demonstrates the spontaneous capability of
no difference between the 48 hr and 96 hr tests. This result rats for long-term social memory following an extended
suggests a general habituation to the test procedure (18 day) period of familiarisation. This suggests that
occurring between the 1 hr and 48 hr tests for both stimuli previous studies may have failed to demonstrate long-term
(familiar: F2,14 = 3.76, P < 0.05; unfamiliar: F2,14 = 14.5, social memory in rats because the initial familiarisation
P < 0.001), but not between the 48 hr and 96 hr tests. period selected was too brief (eg 5 min: Taylor et al 1999).
However, the habituation/discrimination technique used in The results also suggest that, following a familiarisation
this study was selected precisely because it is able to take period of 18 days, juvenile rats removed for at least
such changes in general motivational level into account (see 48 hours might be safely reintroduced into their original
‘Materials and methods’). social group, because it is still possible for them to success-
fully discriminate between the odours of previously familiar
We then carried out a repeated measures GLM with test (1 hr, (former cage-mates) and unfamiliar individuals. It should
48 hr, 96 hr) and odour (unfamiliar/familiar) as factors. This be noted, though, that the ability to successfully discrimi-
revealed a significant interaction between test and odour nate between individuals (or their cues) might not, in itself,
(F2,14 = 5.55, P < 0.01). Post hoc analysis of this test × odour be sufficient to avoid further aggression that may result as a
interaction revealed that both one hour (t8 = 4.27, P < 0.01) consequence of additional factors, ie the dominance status
and 48 hr (t8 = 2.59, P < 0.05) after separation, the rats did of the reintroduced individual (Ewbank & Meese 1971).
indeed spend significantly more time investigating the odour
It is also possible that the length of time between encounters
of a novel juvenile than that of a former cage-mate (see
in studies of long-term social memory allows greater oppor-
Figure 1). Thus, although there was evidence of a general
tunity for the disruption of social memory by challenging
habituation to the test procedure between 1 and 48 h, discrim-
environmental events, for example exposure to the odours
ination was apparent at both these time points. However,
of novel individuals during cage cleaning. Nevertheless,
96 hours after separation, by which time there had been no
even though short-term social memory does appear suscep-
further habituation to the test procedure – total levels of
tible to disruption (Burman & Mendl 2000), familiarity with
investigation were no different to those at 48 h – there was
a conspecific gained over an extended period of time (ie
now no evidence of any discrimination between the familiar
18 days) may make memory less vulnerable to disruption
and unfamiliar odours (t8 = 1.21, P = 0.27). (Burman & Mendl 2004).
Discussion Animal welfare implications
The results of this experiment suggest that juvenile female In conclusion, we have described how, following 18 days of
rats are able to remember the identity of former cage-mates group-housing, juvenile rats appear to remember cage-
that they had previously spent 18 days housed with for at mates with whom they were previously housed for at least
least 48 hours following separation but failed to demon- 48 hours, but that similarly successful recognition is no
strate successful recognition when this period of separation longer demonstrated after 96 hours of separation. This
was increased to 96 hours. indicates that juvenile rats that are temporarily separated

Animal Welfare 2006, 15: 379-382

https://doi.org/10.1017/S0962728600030712 Published online by Cambridge University Press


382 Burman and Mendl

from their cage-mates for experimental or other reasons, Gheusi G, Bluthé RM, Goodall G and Dantzer R 1994
should be reunited them within 48 hours, and before Ethological study of the effects of Tetrahydroaminoacridine (THA)
96 hours. This should maximise the chances of social recog- on social recognition in rats. Psychopharmacology 114: 644-650
nition and a harmonious reintroduction. The results of this Held S, Mendl M, Devereux C and Byrne RW 2001a
study are therefore an essential first step in developing an Behaviour of domestic pigs in a visual perspective taking task.
understanding of the cognitive processes that inform the Behaviour 138: 1337-1354
practical issue of how and when to remove and reintroduce Held S, Mendl M, Devereux C and Byrne RW 2001b Studies in
animals from social groups. social cognition: from primates to pigs. Animal Welfare 10: S209-S217
Hlinak, Z and Krejci I 1991 Social recognition in male rats: age
Acknowledgments differences and modulation by MIF-I and Alaptide. Physiological
This work was funded by a University of Bristol Research 40: 59-67
Postgraduate Scholarship to Oliver HP Burman. The Johnston RE 1993 Memory for individual scent in hamsters
authors wish to thank two anonymous reviewers for their (Mesocricetus auratus) as assessed by habituation methods. Journal
helpful suggestions. of Comparative Psychology 107: 201-207
References Johnston RE and Jernigan P 1994 Golden hamsters recognize
individuals, not just individual scents. Animal Behaviour 48: 129-136
Bluthé RM and Dantzer R 1990 Social recognition does not
Johnston RE and Bullock TA 2001 Individual recognition by
involve vasopressinergic neurotransmission in female rats. Brain
use of odours in golden hamsters: the nature of individual repre-
Research 535: 301-304
sentations. Animal Behaviour 61: 545-557
Brown RE 1992 Responses of dominant and subordinate male
Laughlin K and Mendl M 2000 Pigs shift too: foraging strate-
rats to the odors of male and female conspecifics. Aggressive
gies and spatial memory in the domestic pig. Animal Behaviour 60:
Behavior 18: 129-138
403-410
Burman OHP and Mendl M 2000. Short-term social memory
Laughlin K and Mendl M 2004 Costs of acquiring and forget-
in the laboratory rat: its susceptibility to disturbance. Applied
ting information affect spatial memory and its susceptibility to
Animal Behaviour Science 67: 241-254
interference. Animal Behaviour 68: 97-103
Burman OHP and Mendl M 2002 Recognition of conspecific
Mateo JM and Johnston RE 2000 Retention of social recogni-
odours by laboratory rats (Rattus norvegicus) does not show con-
tion after hibernation in Belding’s ground squirrels. Animal
text specificity. Journal of Comparative Psychology 116(3): 247-252
Behaviour 59: 491-499
Burman OHP and Mendl M 2004 Disruptive effects of stan-
Mendl M 1999 Performing under pressure: stress and cognitive
dard husbandry practice on laboratory rat social discrimination.
function. Applied Animal Behaviour 65: 221-244
Animal Welfare 13: 125-133
Mendl M, Burman O, Laughlin K and Paul E 2001 Animal
Carr WJ, Yee L, Gable D and Marasco E 1976 Olfactory
memory and animal welfare. Animal Welfare 10: S141-S159
recognition of conspecifics by domestic Norway rats. Journal of
Nicol CJ 1996 Farm animal cognition. Animal Science 62: 375-391
Comparative and Physiological Psychology 90: 821-828
Sawyer TF, Hengehold AK and Perez WA 1984
Dantzer R, Bluthé RM, Koob GF and Le Moal M 1987
Chemosensory and hormonal mediation of social memory in male
Modulation of social memory in male rats by neurohypophyseal
rats. Behavioural Neurosciences 98: 908-913
peptides. Psychopharmacology 91: 363-368
Sekiguchi R, Wolterink G and van Ree JM 1991 Short dura-
Engelmann M, Wotjak CT and Landgraf R 1995 Social
tion of retroactive facilitation of social recognition in rats.
discrimination procedure – an alternative method to investi-
Physiology and Behavior 50: 1253-1256
gate juvenile recognition abilities in rats. Physiology and Behavior
Taylor G, Farr S, Griffin M, Humphrey W and Weiss J
58: 315-321
1999 Adult ontogeny of rat working memory of social interac-
Epple G and Niblick H 1997 Social memory in saddle-back
tions. Journal of Gerontology 54A: M145-M151
tamarins (Saguinus fuscicollis). Folia Primatologica 68: 265-271
Thor DH and Holloway WR 1982 Social memory of the male
Ewbank R and Meese GB 1971 Aggressive behaviour in groups
laboratory rat. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology
of domesticated pigs on removal and return of individuals. Animal
96: 1000-1006
Production 13: 685-693

© 2006 Universities Federation for Animal Welfare

https://doi.org/10.1017/S0962728600030712 Published online by Cambridge University Press

You might also like