Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 14

Received: 14 November 2022 Revised: 13 September 2023 Accepted: 13 September 2023

DOI: 10.1111/puar.13750

RESEARCH ARTICLE

‘Feeling out’ the rules: A psychological process theory


of red tape

Randall S. Davis 1 | Sanjay K. Pandey 2

1
College of Business and Analytics, School of Abstract
Management and Marketing, Southern Illinois
University, Carbondale, Illinois, USA
Over the past 30 years, red tape has emerged as a key concept in public
2
Trachtenberg School of Public Policy and Public
management. Yet, researchers continue to debate the relative merits of system-
Administration, The George Washington centric versus individual-centric approaches. In this article, we articulate an
University, Washington, DC, USA individual-centric psychological process theory, a theory that confronts the ‘modu-
larity assumption’ relegating the subjective individual experience as inconsequen-
Correspondence
Randall S. Davis, College of Business and
tial in much red tape research. We theorize that the appraisal mechanism
Analytics, School of Management and influences the meaning attached to external compliance demands. Our process
Marketing, Southern Illinois University, Mail theory advances the claim that experience, emotion, and expectancy influence
Code 4629, Carbondale, IL 62901, USA.
Email: rsdavis@siu.edu
one’s perceptions of, and behavioral response to, red tape. Thus, compliance
expectations in rules are informed by other social demands embedded in roles,
identities, and collective interests in ways that influence the perceived legitimacy
of rules and the compliance burdens they impose. The psychological process the-
ory offers a holistic perspective on red tape by treating the subjective individual
experience as consequential for understanding red tape.

Evidence for practice


• Policy makers should recognize that the subjective experience of red tape has
emotional roots.
• Policy makers should recognize the economic cost of a stunted understanding
of the role of psychological processes (and costs) in experiences of red tape.
• Managers should be aware that there are cognitive and behavioral strategies to
mitigate red tape even in the absence of rule changes.
• Organizational leaders should recognize that the behavioral outcomes of red
tape vary according to expressed emotion and the belief that their behavior will
relieve negative emotions.

INTRODUCTION Pandey et al., 2017; Walker & Brewer, 2008). Arguably, the
system-centric approach to red tape, which is closely
Over the past 30 years, several scholars have established aligned with the postpositivist worldview, adds value by
red tape as one of the native concepts in public offering an explanation that connects a rule’s compliance
management, and a cornerstone of public administration demands with organizational resource consumption
research. Research, however, can approach red tape from (Campbell, 2019; Campbell et al., 2023).
either a system-centric or an individual-centric vantage Pandey (2021), however, argues that a significant
(Pandey, 2023; Pandey & Kingsley, 2000). In this article, we amount of red tape research is characterized by the
propose a psychological process theory of red tape that “modularity assumption, [which] – simply put – is the
prioritizes the individual’s subjective experience in contrast idea that cognitive evaluation of bureaucratic red tape is
to system-centric approaches that emphasize rules as a simple matter of mechanically fitting the module made
the unit of analysis (for other individual-level studies of dysfunction evaluation with the module made of bur-
see Ahn & Campbell, 2022; Brewer & Walker, 2010a, 2010b; den evaluation” (p. 261). The modularity assumption
Campbell, 2017; Davis, 2013; Davis & Pink-Harper, embedded in postpositivist system-centric conceptual
2016; Hattke et al., 2020; Pandey & Kingsley, 2000; models of red tape discounts the full range of human

Public Admin Rev. 2023;1–14. wileyonlinelibrary.com/journal/puar © 2023 American Society for Public Administration. 1
15406210, 0, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/puar.13750 by Cochrane Portugal, Wiley Online Library on [07/12/2023]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
2 A PSYCHOLOGICAL PROCESS THEORY OF RED TAPE

subjective experience. This article is motivated by our Brewer & Walker, 2010a, 2010b; Davis, 2013; Walker &
desire to challenge the modularity assumption, which Brewer, 2008).
regards human subjective experience as inconsequential, The rest of the article is structured as follows. In the
in red tape research (Ahn & Campbell, 2022; first section, we justify the need for a psychological pro-
Pandey, 2021, 2023).1 Alternatively, we propose that cess perspective in red tape research by drawing from
social processes associated with organizational routines the various “images of rules” described by March, Schulz,
and other non-rule factors influence the subjective experi- and Zhou (2000, p. 11). Second, we describe the psycho-
ences that give meaning and shape behavioral responses logical process of appraisal and emotion, as discussed by
to red tape (see Campbell et al., 2023; Davis & Pink- Lazarus (1999, 2000, 2001); see also Lazarus &
Harper, 2016; Pandey, 2021). In so doing, we articulate a Folkman, 1984; Lazarus & Smith, 1988), to detail the
psychological process theory that treats compliance bur- mechanisms through which one generally evaluates
den as subjectively experienced through multiple organi- external demands.3 Next, we draw from the extant litera-
zational channels, as contrasted with a simple ture to situate the primitive concepts associated with red
adjudication based on resource consumption associated tape within the more general appraisal process. The third
with rule following. section argues that individuals’ behaviors are activated by
In proposing this psychological process theory, we certain categories of emotions, which have important
explore two questions. First, how are individual percep- implications for rule-related behavior so long as one
tions of red tape negotiated during social interactions expects the behavior will remedy negative emotion
with organizations? Second, what is the relationship (e.g., Seo et al., 2004; Spector, 2005; Spector & Fox, 2002).
between red tape and work motivation? System-centric Finally, we conclude with a discussion about how the pro-
research defines red tape as rules that entail compliance posed theory encourages deeper inquiry and advances
burden in the absence of attaining a functional goal or understanding the relationships between red tape and
objective (Bozeman, 1993, p. 283). Compliance burden, in work motivation.
Bozeman’s definition, can be objectively evaluated
in terms of organizational resource consumption. Alterna-
tively, Johan Olsen (2006) offers an understanding of rules JUSTIFYING A PSYCHOLOGICAL PROCESS
as individual-centric concepts challenging the notion that VIEW OF RED TAPE: IMAGES OF RULES
one can understand compliance burden absent the social
processes that frame human perception: In one of the most comprehensive theoretical and empiri-
cal treatments of rules, March et al. (2000) describe sev-
Rules provide codes of meaning that facili- eral perspectives commonly used to examine rules in
tate the interpretation of ambiguous worlds. academic literature. One image of rules described by
They embody collective and individual roles, March et al. (2000) refers to rules as rational efforts to
identities, rights, obligations, interests, values, organize (p. 11–13). Through this lens, researchers exam-
worldviews, and memory and thus constrain ine rules as intentional attempts to increase the perfor-
the allocation of attention, standards of eval- mance of an organization that can be objectively
uation, priorities, perceptions, and resources evaluated. Performance increases due to rules, from this
(p. 9). perspective, stem from enhancing coordination among
individuals with shared goals or by binding individuals
Given Olsen’s (2006) understanding of rules as codes with conflicting interests to a set of common compliance
of meaning rooted in social roles and identities, the psy- expectations. A significant proportion of research views
chological mechanisms that govern subjective experience red tape as a rule that fails to rationally and cost effec-
become necessary for examining compliance burden, the tively organize human effort in pursuit of a common goal.
primitive concept2 associated with red tape. It is not our intent to marginalize this perspective. View-
Our theory of red tape is grounded in a constructivist ing red tape as a failure of organizations to rationally pur-
worldview that prioritizes the individual’s subjective expe- sue functional goals at minimal cost has significantly
rience with bureaucratic organization and asserts that vari- informed our understanding of bureaucracy (e.g., Aucoin,
able understandings evolve from these experiences (see 1990; Bozeman, 1993; Bozeman & Feeney, 2011; also see
also Pandey, 2021, 2023). We also argue that the appraisal Campbell et al., 2023). However, this perspective assumes
mechanism frames one’s perceptions of bureaucratic that scholarly evaluations of rule functionality fully cap-
structure and understanding of how to behave in relation ture the individual’s subjective experience. As such, the
to the external demands levied by organizations (see social negotiations that influence rule meaning are rele-
Lazarus, 1999, 2000, 2001). From this vantage, we argue gated to the periphery of inquiry.
that formal expectations in rules couple with other social Another image of rules March et al. (2000) discuss,
demands informed by roles, identities, and collective inter- which is particularly relevant to a psychological process
ests in ways that influence the perceived legitimacy of view of red tape, views rules as social constructions of
rules and the burden they impose (e.g., Blom et al., 2021; meaning (p. 14–16; see also Olsen, 2006). March et al. (2000)
15406210, 0, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/puar.13750 by Cochrane Portugal, Wiley Online Library on [07/12/2023]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
PUBLIC ADMINISTRATION REVIEW 3

argue that, from this vantage, researchers assess rules in Pandey, and Bozeman, 1995). Bozeman (1993) offered the
terms of the social meaning they communicate to first thorough theoretical treatment, where red tape
observers. Specifically, rules facilitate interpretations of referred to a bureaucratic pathology rooted in the over con-
bureaucratic structure by documenting political victories sumption of organizational resources in service of a rule
and losses within an organization. Given this conceptualiza- that fails to achieve a legitimate organizational goal – or at
tion they illustrate that rules least a goal valued by a given stakeholder group. Boze-
man’s (1993) initial definition treats the rule as the unit of
…record who won and who lost, and by that analysis, where red tape refers to objectively pathological
recording alone they remove the necessity of rules derived from a rational calculation of rule costs in rela-
renewing the dispute or implementing its tion to benefits (see Campbell, 2019; Pandey, 2021; Pandey
outcome. A rule can be a declaration of vic- et al., 2017). This approach, however, discounts the role
tory even if it is never implemented or even human experience plays in evaluating compliance
evoked demands. Alternatively, we can treat the individual as the
(p. 16). unit of analysis, where red tape refers to a “negative per-
ceptual assessments of compliance burden imposed by an
Conceptualizing rules as constructions of meaning organization” (Pandey, 2021, pp. 262). One’s negative per-
derived from historical records detailing political victories ceptual assessment flows from socially constructed expres-
challenges the traditional definition of red tape in sions of reality derived from contextual information
three ways. available to the individual given their organizational role or
First, given that rules document political winners and vantage point (see also Brewer & Walker, 2010a, 2010b;
losers, they often reflect the exploitation of legitimate Campbell, 2017; Davis, 2013; Davis & Pink-Harper, 2016;
sources of organizational influence for political ends; in Pandey et al., 2017; Pandey & Kingsley, 2000; Walker &
other words, rules chronicle power imbalances (see also Brewer, 2008).
Mintzberg, 1983, pp. 186–187). As Hattke et al. (2020) A psychological process theory of red tape can rea-
note, rules that reflect power disparities evoke negative sonably rest on the well-established and empirically vali-
emotional responses for those on the losing end of a dated appraisal theory of emotion articulated by Richard
power struggle. Second, although many assert red tape (Lazarus, 1966, 1999, 2000, 2001; see also Lazarus &
can only flow from a rule that is enforced, March et al. Folkman, 1984; Lazarus & Smith, 1988). Lazarus’s (1999,
(2000) show that rules are imbued with meaning to par- 2000, 2001) theory of appraisal offers a useful starting
ticipants even in the absence of implementation. Finally, point by directing attention away from rationalist
while the rational, or cost–benefit, perspective of red tape assumptions of objectively determined red tape based on
requires evaluating a rule in relation to its formally stated the ratio of rule cost to rule benefit. Instead, it encourages
functional object, March and colleagues illustrate that scholars to treat red tape as unfavorable emotional
rules convey meaning beyond their functional object. appraisals of externally imposed compliance demands
Instead, rules often remind parties of earlier failures to (see Pandey, 2021). A psychological process theory of red
achieve status in an organization. As such, the victors of tape squares with interdisciplinary perspectives that
political dialogue can rationally assess rules as beneficial deploy red tape as an example of appraising external
for protecting preferred values, whereas the losers can demands as hindrances to goal attainment (e.g., Dawson
conduct an equally valid rational assessment of the rule’s et al., 2016; LePine et al., 2005) and provides a unifying
failure. Given that rules can be conceptualized as social explanation for empirical evidence that indicates variation
constructions of meaning, the psychological mechanisms in perceived red tape across individuals (e.g., Blom
that influence perception can inform how one evaluates et al., 2021; Brewer & Walker, 2010a, 2010b; Davis, 2013;
the burden imposed by demands, as well as how they Davis & Pink-Harper, 2016; Feeney & Bozeman, 2009;
come to label rules as red tape. The following Scott & Pandey, 2005; Walker & Brewer, 2008).
section describes the appraisal process, which is an Lazarus (1966) extensively examined one’s response to
empirically validated psychological mechanism that external demands as a relational and emotional process,
shapes perception. which he labeled appraisal. One’s appraisal of external
demands can serve as the theoretical foundation for under-
standing a host of work attitudes and behaviors; both favor-
RED TAPE AND THE PSYCHOLOGICAL able and unfavorable (Spector, 2005; Spector & Fox, 2002).
PROCESS OF APPRAISAL Some public administration scholarship examines one’s
interaction with external demands through a relational lens.
Although scholarly interest in red tape has a rich intellectual For example, Davis et al. (2020) take this approach by argu-
history in public administration, a sustained theoretical dis- ing that one’s response to work demands is an
cussion of red tape emerged during the 1990s
(e.g., Bozeman, 1993, 2000; Bozeman & Feeney, 2011; Kauf- Adaptive [emotional] response, moderated
man, 1977; Pandey, 1995; Pandey & Kingsley, 2000; Rainey, by individual differences, that is a
15406210, 0, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/puar.13750 by Cochrane Portugal, Wiley Online Library on [07/12/2023]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
4 A PSYCHOLOGICAL PROCESS THEORY OF RED TAPE

consequence of [appraising] any action, situ- broadly termed organizational citizenship behaviors
ation, or event that places [physical or psy- (Organ, 1988; Spector, 2005; Spector & Fox, 2002), those
chological] demands upon a person (Davis who experience negative emotions at work appear more
et al., 2020, p. 408; Matteson & Ivancevich, likely to engage in counterproductive behaviors designed
1987, p. 10; Lazarus, 1999, 2000, 2001; to harm the organization or other individuals
Lazarus & Smith, 1988). (Spector, 2005; Spector & Fox, 2002). In other words, the
appraisal process indicates that voluntary human behav-
We argue that introducing the psychological ior occurs in response to emotions evoked by external
mechanism of appraisal as foundational to red tape the- demands as opposed to the objective content of
ory better enables empirical researchers to address demands themselves. Individuals subsequently engage in
subject-dependent variation in rule perceptions. post hoc rationalizations to justify how they acted in
Appraisal unfolds in two stages and refers to the response to experienced emotion (Argyris, 1957;
extent to which an individual evaluates whether condi- Katz, 1960; March, 1994; Pandey & Kingsley, 2000;
tions that affect them are personally harmful (Lazarus & Perrow, 1986, Ch. 4). This rationalization can manifest as
Folkman, 1984; Lazarus & Smith, 1988, p. 283). The first causal attributions that “blame” external objects for why
stage, termed primary appraisal, requires that the individ- one behaved in a specific way (Weiner, 1986).
ual determine the extent to which external demands To this point, we have discussed how the process of
imposed upon them threaten their welfare. In this phase, appraisal influences emotion and behavior generally, yet
the individual seeks to make sense of the demands they the connections it elucidates between external demands,
confront given situational and contextual cues provided emotions, and behavior can inform how the public
in the broader social environment. However, appraising a administration research community understands the
specific demand, or set of demands, as threatening need complex social and political systems that give meaning to
not imply unfavorable emotional responses. Variability in compliance burden and red tape (see Olsen, 2006;
one’s emotional response to environmental demands Perrow, 1986; Scott, 2013). Figure 1 offers a graphical
occurs because in the second stage, termed secondary depiction of the appraisal process.
appraisal, one weighs available coping resources to
address perceived threats. To the extent that one pos-
sesses a sufficient stock of resources to address perceived APPRAISAL AND RED TAPE: ELEMENTS OF
threats, the demands levied are assessed as a motivating EXPERIENCE, EMOTION, AND EXPECTANCY
challenge to overcome. Alternatively, if one appraises a
given demand, or set of demands as threatening, and Given we seek to articulate a process theory, it is useful to
they lack sufficient resources to address the threat, the conceptually detail each stage of the process. Consistent
demand will evoke unfavorable emotions and hinder with Lazarus’s (1966, 1999, 2000, 2001) theory of appraisal
their ability to attain preferred goals. and emotion, we describe how elements of experience,
Behavioral consequences also flow from the emo- emotion, and expectancy shape perceptions of red tape
tional byproducts of environmental demands. Spector and the behavioral consequences that follow. In sum-
and Fox (2002), as well as Spector (2005), persuasively mary, an individual must first experience, or interact with,
illustrate that many, if not most, voluntary organizational the external compliance demands in rules before they
behaviors represent a direct response to experienced can reasonably determine if compliance demands consti-
emotions at work (see also Seo et al., 2004). While those tute burdensome red tape. Second, following the experi-
who experience positive emotions due to work demands ence with external compliance demands, one exhibits
are more likely to engage in the cluster of behaviors emotion in response to the compliance demands levied

FIGURE 1 The appraisal process.


15406210, 0, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/puar.13750 by Cochrane Portugal, Wiley Online Library on [07/12/2023]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
PUBLIC ADMINISTRATION REVIEW 5

against them. Finally, one develops a behavioral response foundations that reinforce the utility of a psychological
to red tape based on the expectancy that certain behav- perspective (e.g., Borst et al., 2019; Campbell, 2017, 2020;
iors will relieve the negative emotions associated with red Carrigan et al., 2020; Giauque et al., 2013; Grimmelikhuijsen
tape. Examining the development of red tape across et al., 2017; Hattke et al., 2020; Muylaert et al., 2022; Pandey
these three conceptual domains can offer researchers et al., 2017; Steijn & Van der Voet, 2019). Our intent here is
and managers a clearer picture of how red tape develops to draw from the evidence presented in prior research, and
and how to address red tape in complex organizations. the logic of appraisal articulated above, to expand red tape
Given elements of experience, emotion, and expec- research. The framework described hereafter avoids the
tancy embedded in the appraisal process, we argue that implicit assumption that one can understand a solitary rule
red tape is best conceptualized as “negative perceptual in the absence of examining the social relationships
assessments of compliance burden imposed by an orga- between individuals, external demands, and organizational
nization” (Pandey, 2021, pp. 262). Thus, red tape is an context. Figure 2 provides an illustration of the appraisal
unfavorable emotion in response to evaluating external process specific to the red tape concepts we develop
demands and compliance burdens based on rule-related below.
information accumulated from multiple sources. One sub-
sequently evaluates external demands embedded in rule
related information based on the totality of social Experience: rule-related stimuli, threat
demands they confront, which are differentially inter- perception, and excess compliance burden
preted given variable coping resources. Specifically, one
subjectively assigns a perceived threat level to a demand Red tape scholars, even those who favor rationalist
they confront. Subsequently, based on contextual cues, approaches, highlight that red tape cannot exist in the
espoused values, and available psychological resources, absence of external demands. Bozeman (1993) conceptu-
one responds emotionally based on their capacity to cope alized compliance burden in terms of organizational
with the perceived threat. As such, compliance expecta- resource consumption and implied that compliance bur-
tions are infused with meaning based on the extent to den could be objectively assessed (see also Bozeman &
which one possesses sufficient resources to offset the per- Feeney, 2011; Zahradnik, 2022). Yet, as March et al. (2000)
ceived degree of threat they impose. illustrate, compliance burden derives meaning from the
While understanding red tape as part of broader psy- organizational context even in the absence of external
chological processes has been documented in the public enforcement. Given this complexity, recent attempts to
management literature (see Campbell et al., 2023; Davis & advance red tape measurement often assess perceived
Pink-Harper, 2016; Pandey, 2021; Pandey & Kingsley, 2000; burden (e.g., Borry, 2016; Davis & Stazyk, 2014; Pandey
Pandey & Welch, 2005; Quratulain & Khan, 2015; Scott & et al., 2017; Van Loon et al., 2016). These measures treat
Pandey, 2005), we seek to offer a perspective to unite these compliance burden as an individualized evaluative judg-
studies under a common conceptual umbrella. Red tape ment but also fall prey to the modularity assumption by
research in the vein of experimental and behavioral public avoiding a formal description of the mechanisms that
administration, as well as those studies grounded in the govern human evaluation (see Pandey, 2021; Pandey
job demands–resources framework, offer empirical et al., 2017 for similar arguments).

FIGURE 2 A psychological process theory of red tape.


15406210, 0, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/puar.13750 by Cochrane Portugal, Wiley Online Library on [07/12/2023]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
6 A PSYCHOLOGICAL PROCESS THEORY OF RED TAPE

Importantly, individuals must experience rules’ stated The psychological process theory, we propose explic-
compliance demands before they can evaluate the itly accounts for the negative valence attached to compli-
degree or character of burden imposed by those ance burden and acknowledges that what constitutes
demands. As the model presented in Figure 2 illustrates, excess burden varies across individuals. We argue that
the demands embedded in rules precede one’s emotive excessive compliance burden, in the psychological pro-
assessment of organizational characteristics. In the broad- cess framework, refers to circumstances where individuals
est sense, rules refer to “explicit or implicit norms, regula- appraise information regarding behavioral constraints,
tions, and expectations that regulate the behavior of demands, and expectations learned from rule-related
individuals” (March et al., 2000, p. 5). Experience describes stimuli as a direct threat to personal welfare. Alternatively,
one’s interaction with “a stimulus that has resulted in if the individual does not appraise rule-related stimuli as
learning,” where stimulus is “any agent, event, or threatening, they will interpret the demands imposed
situation – internal or external – that elicits a response upon them as normal, rather than excess, burden. In the
from an organism” (American Psychological Association next subsection, we turn our attention to those condi-
[APA], 2020a). In the context of rules and red tape, we tions where rule-related stimuli appear threatening to
argue that experience refers to any encounter with rule- individuals.
related stimuli that conveys social information to an actor
from which the actor learns about, and responds to,
behavioral expectations within the organization. Rule- Emotion: perceived red tape and coping
related stimuli convey information about demands, and
elicit a response, but originate from myriad sources Although emotion and affect did not feature prominently
including, minimally, objective rule text, hierarchy, resolu- in early theorizing, Bozeman (1993) introduced the con-
tion of political debate, and other social cues in organiza- cept of emotion in relation to red tape. Specifically, he
tions (see Davis & Pink-Harper, 2016; DeHart-Davis, 2009, illustrates that, while there had been various conceptuali-
2017; DeHart-Davis et al., 2015; March et al., 2000; zations in the 20th century, “frustration and vexation”
Mintzberg, 1983). By drawing together the definitions of were commonly associated with the experience of red
rules, experience, and stimulus, we define rule-related tape (Bozeman, 1993, pp. 274–275; see also Van Loon
stimuli as external demands levied on an individual in the et al., 2016). As research examining red tape evolved, sev-
form of norms, regulations, and/or expectations embed- eral scholars empirically examined the extent to which
ded in an organization’s structural transmission channels various emotions, including alienation, dissatisfaction,
that elicit a response, where structural transmission chan- confusion, and anger, followed one’s experience with red
nels include, at least, hierarchy, formalization, centraliza- tape (e.g., DeHart-Davis & Pandey, 2005; Hattke
tion, organizational politics, social cues, and bureaucratic et al., 2020; Quratulain & Khan, 2015; Steijn & Van der
routine (see Kaufman, 1960; Kaufmann et al., 2019; March Voet, 2019). Researchers often situate red tape as an ante-
et al., 2000; Olsen, 2006; Pandey, 2021; Pandey & cedent to emotion based on the assumption that red
Scott, 2002).4 tape needlessly compels behaviors, which reinforces
The logic in the psychological process theory, where power disparities that favor the organization (e.g., Hattke
experiences are filtered through primary appraisal to et al., 2020). Since most individuals prefer individual
inform perceived threat levels, offers a tool to identify the agency and autonomy, they will express anger when con-
subject-dependent boundary between normal and excess fronted with illegitimate demands of subservience (see
compliance burden. In the traditional system-centric con- Argyris, 1957). Yet, other scholars examining bureaucracy
ceptualization, Bozeman (1993) understood compliance challenge the assertion that one’s experience of red tape
burden as the resources expended during compliance with precedes emotion.
external demands. As such, compliance burden was not Goodsell (1985) articulates an alternative perspec-
inherently negative, so long as the resources spent tive that specifies the causal direction between compli-
achieved organizational purposes. Yet, as research shifted ance demands, emotion, and red tape. He argues that
to an individualized conceptualization, scholars began to red tape represents one’s unfavorable symbolic assess-
recognize that compliance burden possesses a negative ment of bureaucracy derived from a much wider array
valence and varies depending on assumed social roles of subjectively held feelings (Goodsell, 1985). Goodsell’s
(e.g., Brewer & Walker, 2010a, 2010b; Coursey & (1985) perspective is consistent with Olsen’s (2006)
Pandey, 2007; Davis, 2013; Feeney & Bozeman, 2009; understanding of rules and our subject-dependent con-
Moynihan et al., 2012; Van Loon et al., 2016). For example, ceptualization of burden. Goodsell (1985) and Olsen
Van Loon et al. (2016) illustrate the negative connotation (2006) directly challenge the conventional wisdom that
attached to compliance burden by indicating that, “rules red tape is an antecedent to emotion. While others have
perceived as red tape entail a compliance burden: the argued that emotions follow one’s experience with red
employee feels burdened in executing the rule because it tape, based on the alternative causal explanations of
requires excessive time or energy, or it is complex or frus- Goodsell (1985) and Olsen (2006), we argue that red
trating” (p. 664, emphasis is our own). tape represents a symbolic label for unfavorable
15406210, 0, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/puar.13750 by Cochrane Portugal, Wiley Online Library on [07/12/2023]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
PUBLIC ADMINISTRATION REVIEW 7

emotions that arise from compliance burden perceived red tape literature examines unpleasant emotions (see
as a threat. DeHart-Davis & Pandey, 2005; Hattke et al., 2020;
Conceptualizing red tape as a symbolic label charac- Henderson & Borry, 2023; Kaufmann & Tummers, 2017;
terizing unfavorable emotions comports with Henry Min- Pandey & Kingsley, 2000). However, based on Lazarus’s
tzberg’s (1983) view of power in organizations. He argues (1999, 2000, 2001) logic of appraisal and emotion one
that power can be exercised in organizations through the need not assume that rule-related stimuli, no matter how
exploitation of otherwise legitimate systems of authority, onerous or demanding, uniformly result in unpleasant
for example through rules. Moreover, the exercise of emotion (see also Hattke et al., 2020). The emotions that
political power in organizations is, by definition, flow from engaging with external demands vary depend-
illegitimate because it is not sanctioned by “formal ing on available coping resources (see Döring, 2021; Dör-
authority, accepted ideology, nor certified expertise” ing & Madsen, 2022; Masood & Nisar, 2021; for similar
(Mintzberg, 1983, p. 172). Therefore, it stands to reason arguments in the context of administrative burden).
that when rules are used as a mechanism to solidify politi- Figure 3 illustrates the circumplex conceptualization of
cal power, the losers of political dialogue will rely on emo- emotion, and the categories of emotion that come to
tion when they characterize the acceptability of symbolize red tape.
compliance demands. Pandey and Kingsley (2000) suc- Assuming an individual perceives excess burden – or,
cinctly reinforce these perspectives by suggesting that in other words, appraises rule-related stimuli as a threat
emotions may represent a “state of mind likely to influ- to personal well-being – emotional outcomes result from
ence perceptions of red tape” (p. 784). We argue that an individual weighing available coping resources against
adopting a psychological process approach to red tape burden. According to the American Psychological Associa-
can resolve this inconsistency (i.e., whether emotions are tion, coping refers to “the use of cognitive and behavioral
defining features, or a consequence, of red tape) by illus- strategies to manage the demands of a situation when
trating that the primitive concept of compliance burden these are appraised as taxing or exceeding one’s
is a precursor to emotion, whereas red tape perceptions resources or to reduce the negative emotions and conflict
are symbolic responses to negative, emotional percep- caused by stress” (APA, 2020b). A definition of coping
tions of compliance burden. specific to red tape refers to one’s use of cognitive and
Emotions are complex reactions, characterized by two behavioral strategies to manage the demands embedded
dimensions indicating relative degrees of [un]pleasant- in rule-related stimuli when these demands are appraised
ness and [de]activation, resulting from the appraisal of as excessively burdensome. Individuals can possess a
specific events or stimuli (e.g., Lazarus & Smith, 1988; multitude of resources, both internal and external, neces-
Russell, 1980; Schlosberg, 1952; Welsh et al., 2020). Given sary for adapting to the perceived burden resulting from
our focus on rules and red tape, we adapt this definition rule-related stimuli. Dewe, O’Driscoll, and Cooper (2012;
such that rule-oriented emotions refer to complex reac- citing Nelson & Simmons, 2003) describe that “[p]ersonal
tions, characterized by degrees of [un]pleasantness and resources include attributes such as personal values
[de]activation, resulting from the appraisal of rule-related (e.g., the importance of achievement), personality traits
stimuli. The dimension of pleasantness refers to one’s (e.g., internal locus of control, hardiness, dispositional
subjective experience in terms of positive, or favorable, optimism, and generalized self-esteem), and other
and negative, or unfavorable, affective states (Seo characteristics, including positive affect” (p. 31). Alterna-
et al., 2004; Welsh et al., 2020). The second dimension, tively, external resources stem from organizational
activation, denotes physiological responses to external sources related to the work environment including
demands that prompt action (Seo et al., 2004; Welsh autonomy, feedback, and associated work rewards (Dewe
et al., 2020). Psychologists refer to this characterization of et al., 2012, p. 31).
emotion and affect as the “circumplex model” Acknowledging intraindividual variability in coping
(Russell, 1980; Russell & Barrett, 1999), where the poles of resources means that one’s ability to adapt to perceptions
[un]pleasantness and [de]activation can be conceptual- of excess compliance burden is subject-dependent. Nega-
ized in a two-dimensional circular space akin to a clock. tive emotional outcomes will result from secondary
It is not our intent to oversimplify the circumplex appraisal for those with an insufficient stock of coping
model, yet it highlights four broad categories of emotion: resources. This perspective is consistent with research
pleasant, activating emotions (e.g., excitement); pleasant, that emphasizes the subject-dependent nature of red
deactivating emotions (e.g., relaxation); unpleasant, deac- tape depending on roles, obligations, hierarchical posi-
tivating emotions (e.g., depression or boredom); and tions, values, or participation in social groups
unpleasant, activating emotions (e.g., anxiety or anger). (e.g., Brewer & Walker, 2010a, 2010b; Campbell, 2017;
The remainder of this subsection focuses primarily on the Davis, 2013) because these factors can confer necessary
[un]pleasant dimension of emotion, whereas the next coping resources. As such, we draw these streams of
section focuses on the [de]activation dimension. Given research together to argue, in line with Pandey and Kings-
widespread agreement that red tape represents a uni- ley (2000), that the unpleasant emotions stemming from
formly negative phenomenon, a significant amount of perceived excess compliance burden and insufficient
15406210, 0, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/puar.13750 by Cochrane Portugal, Wiley Online Library on [07/12/2023]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
8 A PSYCHOLOGICAL PROCESS THEORY OF RED TAPE

FIGURE 3 Situating red tape in unpleasant activating and deactivating emotions.

resources define red tape (see also Goodsell, 1985). Pan- breaking responses appear incompatible in response to
dey (2021) accounts for emotion and appraisal in his defi- the same stimuli. The psychological process theory can
nition of red tape, where red tape refers to “negative reconcile seemingly inconsistent behaviors likely to follow
perceptual assessments of compliance burden imposed one’s experience with red tape.
by an organization” (p. 262). In the psychological process We argue that one’s behavioral response to red tape
theory, red tape takes on meaning due to the attempt by depends on two interrelated factors, the degree to which
individuals to symbolically label external demands that perceived red tape is characterized by unpleasant activat-
give rise to negative emotional outcomes resulting from ing emotions, and one’s expectancy that a given behavior
insufficient coping resources. In the next subsection, we will alleviate unpleasant emotions. First, in the previous sec-
examine the behavioral outcomes likely to flow from the tion, we illustrated that although all red tape assessments
red tape. are characterized by negative emotion, only a fraction of
those emotions motivate behavior. The activation dimen-
sion refers to the ability of certain emotions to prompt
Expectancy: behavioral responses to red tape action (Seo et al., 2004; Welsh et al., 2020). To the extent
that one’s experience with rule-related stimuli invokes acti-
Although nearly all red tape research agrees it is uni- vating emotions, such as anger or frustration, the individual
formly negative, scholars examine myriad behavioral out- prepares a behavioral response. Active attempts to thwart
comes that flow from perceptions of excess burden external demands in rule-related stimuli, such as various
(e.g., Bozeman & Feeney, 2011; Davis & Pink-Harper, 2016; forms of rule-breaking behavior or voluntary turnover,
Feeney & DeHart-Davis, 2009; Hassan et al., 2021; become significantly more likely under these conditions
Quratulain & Khan, 2015; Scott & Pandey, 2000; (Davis & Pink-Harper, 2016; DeHart-Davis, 2007; Giauque
Taylor, 2018). However, the behavioral outcomes identi- et al., 2019; Potipiroon, 2022). Alternatively, one might
fied across studies appear inconsistent. For example, deploy the term red tape to refer to deactivating emotions
some evidence indicates that an individual who experi- resulting from rule-related stimuli, such as boredom or
ences red tape may respond by engaging in risk averse depression. Under these conditions, the individual is
behaviors (e.g., Bozeman & Kingsley, 1998; Feeney & more likely to express inactive responses, such as risk aver-
DeHart-Davis, 2009), whereas others illustrate that sion or withdrawal (e.g., Feeney & DeHart-Davis, 2009;
rule-breaking behavior may follow one’s encounter with Quratulain & Khan, 2015). Yet, the [de]activation dimension
red tape (e.g., Davis & Pink-Harper, 2016; DeHart- of emotions merely prepares an individual for a physical
Davis, 2007; Potipiroon, 2022). Yet, risk aversion and rule- response to the external demands they confront. When
15406210, 0, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/puar.13750 by Cochrane Portugal, Wiley Online Library on [07/12/2023]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
PUBLIC ADMINISTRATION REVIEW 9

one translates experiences into actual behavior, they resulting from experience with predicable relationships
implicitly evaluate the extent to which they believe a spe- between behavioral responses and rule-related stimuli
cific behavior will mitigate unpleasant emotion. characterized by one’s subjective belief that his or her
One will exhibit a behavioral response to red tape if actions will mitigate negative emotions that define red
they expect that behavior will relieve the negative emo- tape. Like our previous definitions, the concepts of activa-
tions that define red tape. The American Psychological tion and expectancy reinforce that the behavioral
Association defines expectancy as “the internal state response to red tape is subjective and value laden.
resulting from experience with predictable relationships Broadly speaking, one can frame beliefs about the
between stimuli or between responses and stimuli. … In connections between responses and stimuli from per-
motivation theory, it refers to an individual’s belief that sonal dispositional proclivities, role-specific social norms,
his or her actions can produce a particular outcome” or a combination of both (e.g., Ajzen & Fishbein, 1980;
(APA, 2020c). To craft a red tape-specific definition of Fishbein & Ajzen, 1975; Miller & Grush, 1988). We define
expectancy, we draw together the general and motiva- personal expectancies related to rules as dispositional
tional definitions. With respect to rules and red tape, we beliefs regarding the relationships between rule-related
define rule-related expectancies as the internal state stimuli, negative emotions, and behavioral responses.

TABLE 1 Psychological process theory of red tape: definitions.


Elements of
appraisal (see Psychological process theory of red tape definitions
Figure 1) Psychological foundations (see Figure 2)

External demands Experience: a stimulus that has resulted in learning Rule-related stimuli: external demands levied on an individual
Stimulus: is any agent event or situation – internal or external in the form of norms, regulations, and/or expectations
– that elicits a response from an organism embedded in an organization’s structural transmission
channels that elicit a response, where structural
Rules: explicit or implicit norms, regulations, and expectations transmission channels include, at least, hierarchy,
that regulate the behavior of individuals formalization, centralization, organizational politics, social
cues, and bureaucratic routine
Primary appraisal Threat perception: one’s assessment that a situation is Burden evaluation: individual appraisal of information
and threat relevant to their well-being and that the situation inhibits regarding behavioral constraints, demands, and
perception personal goal attainment expectations learned from rule-related stimuli that are
perceived as a direct threat to personal welfare and goal
attainment
Secondary Coping: the use of cognitive and behavioral strategies to Rule-oriented coping: cognitive and behavioral strategies,
appraisal and manage the demands of a situation when these are which consumes available psychological resources, to
emotional appraised as taxing or exceeding one’s resources or to manage the demands embedded in rule-related stimuli
outcome reduce the negative emotions and conflict caused by when these demands are appraised as excessively
stress burdensome
Resources: personal attributes including values and Rule-oriented emotion: complex reactions, characterized by
personality traits, as well as features of the work degrees of [un]pleasantness and [de]activation, resulting
environment including autonomy, feedback, and rewards from the appraisal of rule-related stimuli
that facilitate one’s ability to adapt to perceived threats
Emotion: complex reactions, characterized by two dimensions Red tape: rule-oriented emotion characterized by negative, or
indicating relative degrees of [un]pleasantness and [de] unpleasant, perceptual assessments of compliance
activation, resulting from the appraisal of specific events burden imposed by an organization
or stimuli
Expectancy and Expectancy: the internal state resulting from experience with Rule-related expectancy: internal state resulting from
behavioral predictable relationships between stimuli or between experience with predicable relationships between
outcomes responses and stimuli, and, in motivation theory, an behavioral responses and rule-related stimuli
individual’s belief that his or her actions can produce a characterized by one’s subjective belief that his or her
particular outcome actions will mitigate negative emotions that define red
tape
Individual-specific expectancies (with respect to rules):
dispositional beliefs regarding the relationships between
rule-related stimuli, negative emotions, and behavioral
responses.
Role-specific expectancies (with respect to rules): role-
dependent normative beliefs regarding the relationships
between rule-related stimuli, negative emotion, and
behavioral responses
15406210, 0, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/puar.13750 by Cochrane Portugal, Wiley Online Library on [07/12/2023]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
10 A PSYCHOLOGICAL PROCESS THEORY OF RED TAPE

Alternatively, role-specific expectancies related to rules increased red tape simultaneously predict risk aversion
refer to role-dependent normative beliefs regarding the and rule violation? A psychological process theory
relationships between rule-related stimuli, negative emo- answers this question by specifying the mechanisms and
tion, and behavioral responses. Incorporating both per- social processes that connect emotions and expectancies
sonal and role-specific expectancies addresses empirical to external compliance demands. As such, scholarship is
findings that illustrate variation in red tape and outcomes better equipped to offer recommendations to organiza-
due to personality characteristics (e.g., DeHart-Davis, 2007; tions addressing the deleterious outcomes of red tape.
Kaufmann & Feeney, 2014) and cultural norms embedded Several research propositions flow from the psychological
in social roles (e.g., Brewer & Walker, 2010a, 2010b; process theory.
Campbell, 2017; Pandey et al., 2007; Walker & In a technical sense, the model we describe is a com-
Brewer, 2008). We summarize all psychological concept plex moderated-mediation model. Our model proposes
and psychological process definitions in Table 1. that there is a three-stage mediation process where the
processes of primary appraisal, secondary appraisal, and
expectancy carry compliance demands to behavior
CONCLUSION through threat perception and emotion. Admittedly, the
mediation stages of primary and secondary appraisal may
In this article, we sought to specify a psychological pro- be difficult to disentangle empirically. That said, it is
cess theory of red tape to address two interrelated aims. important to recognize that complex patterns of media-
Specifically, we engaged with the challenge posed by the tion unfold over time. As such, research could proceed by
“modularity assumption”, relegating subjective individual presenting individuals with specific compliance demands,
experience as inconsequential, embedded in a significant and assessing the extent to which individuals feel that
amount of red tape research (Pandey, 2021, 2023). We those expectations threaten personal goal attainment.
argued that system-centric theories assume that the Subsequently, after engaging with demands, the respon-
mechanisms connecting one’s subjective organizational dent’s emotional state can be evaluated. Alternatively,
experience to red tape are fully detailed in the conceptual researchers might seek to assess separate stages of the
definition. As such, a significant amount of previous appraisal process and examine them independently in
research asserts that the objective expenditure of organi- ways where knowledge accumulates over time. Our
zational resources adequately captures the true nature of model also proposes that each mediation step is moder-
compliance burden. ated by threat assessment, resource availability, and
Much like previous research, we examine compliance expectancies about behavior respectively. The categories
burden as the primitive concept associated with red tape, of variables that address each of the moderation research
but we argued that previous conceptual models are com- questions in Figure 2 are vast. As examples, researchers
posed of blind spots regarding human subjectivity. Alter- might examine how (a) personality traits encourage varia-
natively, we relied on Lazarus’s model of appraisal and tion in the relationship between compliance demands
emotion to more adequately specify the nature of compli- and threat assessment, (b) valued resources including
ance burden. From this vantage, “excess burden” repre- autonomy or supportive leadership change the relation-
sents a subjective, symbolic assessment of externally ship between threat perception and emotion, or
imposed compliance demands evolving from reciprocal (c) cognitive biases influence the relationship between
social interactions. This subjective assessment represents emotion and behavior. The research program that could
more than mere information processing and is inclusive flow from this model is extensive and cannot be fully
of the emotive consequences of bureaucratic structure. described in a single article. We hope that future scholars
Viewing excess burden as evolving from a socially negoti- will take up the call to unpack the relationships we
ated understandings of external demands, mediated describe here.
through the appraisal process, has important scholarly Second, from a practical perspective, a psychological
and practical implications for connecting rules to the process theory better informs the relationships between
experience of excess burden. red tape and employee motivation. Managers who
First, from a scholarly perspective, the psychological assume red tape is exclusively derived from objective
process theory we detail here rejects a universal under- considerations may find themselves perplexed about why
standing of red tape and asserts that social position and rules fail to motivate intended behavioral outcomes. In
role expectations frame one’s experience of red tape response to rule failure, it seems logical for managers to
(Pandey, 2023). To the extent organizations hope to lever- more forcefully emphasize compliance demands and
age compliance demands to shape employee behavior, strictly punish deviation from those demands. However,
the social processes that shape one’s understanding of managers who neglect the emotive elements of compli-
red tape is no trivial matter. A solely objective view of red ance demands, such as feelings of anger, anxiety, ner-
tape suppresses extended investigation of red tape’s vousness, depression, or powerlessness, will consistently
social causes, which leads to inconsistent findings regard- preside over underperforming organizations. Moreover,
ing behavioral consequences.5 For example, how can attempts to remedy performance failures through
15406210, 0, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/puar.13750 by Cochrane Portugal, Wiley Online Library on [07/12/2023]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
PUBLIC ADMINISTRATION REVIEW 11

3
strategic human resources management initiatives are Richard Lazarus was a pioneer in the field of psychology advocating
likely to bear little fruit. Recruitment strategies in for the importance of emotion and coping as drivers of behavior.
Beginning in the mid-20th century Lazarus and colleagues (Lazarus,
response to performance failures, for example, will only 1966, 1999, 2000, 2001); See also Lazarus & Folkman, 1984; Lazarus &
be effective so long as new employees are not subject to Smith, 1988) developed a general theory of appraisal and emotion that
the same emotional fate. Admittedly, including emotion contributed to his reputation as one of the most prominent psycholo-
and expectancy in a theory of red tape complicates the gists of the 20th century. The general theory of appraisal and emotion
connection between rules and motivation, but it presents has been adapted to multiple domains of inquiry ranging from work-
place issues and occupational health (Grandey, 2000) to information
a more realistic picture of the motivating potential of for- management (Gursoy et al., 2019).
mal compliance demands for managers seeking to ele- 4
Notably, the psychological definitions of experience and stimuli are
vate performance. broad and account for any learning that occurs when responding to
Importantly, it is not our intent to argue in opposition information drawn from “agents, events, or situations”. Our choice
to eliminating or altering rules. Rules can be dysfunc- to include some structural transmission channels in our explanation of
tional, excessively costly, or work at contrary purposes. rule-related stimuli including social cues, formalization, and bureau-
cratic routine, for example, represents an attempt to account for infor-
However, rules represent more than attempts to rationally
mation conveyed through agents, events, and situations. That is not to
structure an organization. Rules also construct meaning, say our list of structural transmission channels exhausts all possible
in part, as political exercise designed to control behavior agents, events, and situations. Any organization that conveys informa-
in pursuit of preferred patterns of value allocation (March tion to individuals regarding how one ought to behave in response to
et al., 2000; Mintzberg, 1983; Olsen, 2006). As such, sub- legitimate compliance demands constitutes rule-related stimuli. Alter-
natively, information one accumulates from organizations unrelated to
jective character of compliance demands can be indepen-
legitimate compliance expectations may be stimuli, but it does not
dent of their objective aims. Given the subjective represent rule-related stimuli.
dimension of compliance expectations, rules can become 5
Indeed, Campbell et al.’s (2023) recent meta-review highlights the
red tape not only because they fail to attain a legitimate importance of alternative research traditions on red tape. They call the
organizational purpose, but also because they reflect the “objective red tape” tradition the “administrative performance”
emotive distress that can arise from interacting with com- research tradition and contrast it with the rule quality research tradi-
pliance demands. Thus, reducing red tape may be par- tion and the behavioral impact research tradition. The rule quality and
behavioral impact research traditions on red tape highlight the
tially achieved through emotion management or salience of non-objective and non-rule factors and subscribe to a rule
cognitive behavioral therapy as much as rule alteration. ontology that emphasizes organizational embeddedness of rules and
As stoic philosopher, Marcus Aurelius, once reflected, “If experiential or perceptual holism (Campbell et al., 2023: 301).
you are distressed by an external thing, the thing itself is
not what troubles you, but only your judgment of it. And
RE FE RE NCE S
you can wipe this out at a moment’s notice.” While we
Ahn, Yongjin, and Jesse W. Campbell. 2022. “Red Tape, Rule Legitimacy,
acknowledge that future research could more fully specify and Public Service Motivation: Experimental Evidence from Korean
the psychological process theory, we believe that assum- Citizens.” Administration & Society 54(9): 1651–88.
ing subjective judgment is inconsequential for under- Ajzen, Icek, and Martin Fishbein. 1980. Understanding Attitudes and
standing compliance burden paints a partial and less Predicting Social Behavior. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.
American Psychological Association. 2020a. “Coping.” APA Dictionary of
realistic picture of red tape in public organizations.
Psychology. April 28, 2022. https://dictionary.apa.org/coping.
American Psychological Association. 2020b. “Experience.” APA Dictio-
ENDNOTES nary of Psychology. November, 4, 2021. https://dictionary.apa.org/
1
We acknowledge that there are multiple concepts in public administra- experience.
tion, including administrative burden and sludge, that take the individ- American Psychological Association. 2020c. “Stimulus.” APA Dictionary
ual subjective experience seriously. Distinguishing these concepts of Psychology. September, 5, 2021. https://dictionary.apa.org/
is beyond the scope of this article. Interested readers can review stimulus.
Pandey’s (2023) extensive discussion of the commonalities and differ- Argyris, Chris. 1957. Personality and Organization. New York, NY:
ences between these concepts (also see Madsen et al., 2022). Harper & Row.
2
Aucoin, Peter. 1990. “Administrative Reform in Public Management:
Leibniz was one of the strongest advocates for the idea of primitive Paradigms, Principles, Paradoxes and Pendulums.” Governance: An
concepts (Plaisted 2003). Dowding (2016) proposes a maxim for con- International Journal of Policy and Administration 3(2): 115–137.
ceptual analysis that invokes primitive as a matter of degree as Blom, Rutger, Rick T. Borst, and Bart Voorn. 2021. “Pathology or Inconve-
contrasted with primitive as the lowest-common denominator constit- nience? A Meta-Analysis of the Impact of Red Tape on People and
uent concept, “Concepts should be as primitive as possible” (See Organizations.” Review of Public Personnel Administration 41(4):
Dowding 2016: 191–193 for an exposition of this claim). Drawing on 623–650.
Dowding’s work, our use of the term primitive concept in conceptual Borry, Erin L. 2016. “A New Measure of Red Tape: Introducing the
analysis is based on two stipulations: (i) specifications of complex con- Three-Item Red Tape (TIRT) Scale.” International Public Management
cepts invoke primitive concepts; and (ii) primitive concepts, as con- Journal 19(4): 573–593.
trasted with complex concepts, are closer to empirical or experiential Borst, Rick T., Peter M. Kruyen, and Christiaan J. Lako. 2019. “Exploring
reality. In the context of red tape scholarship, we specifically draw on the Job Demands–Resources Model of Work Engagement. In
recent conceptual analyses that deploy the term primitive concept Government: Bringing in a Psychological Perspective.” Review of
(Pandey, 2021, 2023). For completeness sake, it is worth noting that Public Personnel Administration 39(3): 372–397.
similar terms (“primitive element”) have been used in earlier red tape Bozeman, Barry. 1993. “A Theory of Government “Red Tape”.” Journal of
studies (Pandey & Kingsley, 2000; Pandey & Scott, 2002). Public Administration Research and Theory 3(3): 273–303.
15406210, 0, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/puar.13750 by Cochrane Portugal, Wiley Online Library on [07/12/2023]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
12 A PSYCHOLOGICAL PROCESS THEORY OF RED TAPE

Bozeman, Barry. 2000. Bureaucracy and Red Tape. Upper Saddle River, NJ: Dewe, Philip J., Michael P. O’Driscoll, and Cary L. Cooper. 2012. “Theories
Prentice Hall. of Psychological Stress at Work.” In Handbook of Occupational
Bozeman, Barry, and Mary K. Feeney. 2011. Rules and Red Tape. A Prism Health and Wellness, edited by R. J. Gatchel and I. Z. Schultz, 23–38.
for Public Administration Theory and Research. Armonk, NY: M. E. New York, NY: Springer.
Sharpe. Döring, Matthias. 2021. “How-to Bureaucracy: A Concept of Citizens’
Bozeman, Barry, and Gordon Kingsley. 1998. “Risk Culture in Public and Administrative Literacy.” Administration & Society 53(8): 1155–77.
Private Organizations.” Public Administration Review 58(2): 109–118. Döring, Matthias, and Jonas Krogh Madsen. 2022. “Mitigating Psycholog-
Brewer, Gene A., and Richard M. Walker. 2010a. “Explaining Variation in ical Costs—The Role of Citizens’ Administrative Literacy and Social
Perceptions of Red Tape: A Professionalism-Marketization Model.” Capital.” Public Administration Review 82(4): 671–681.
Public Administration 88(2): 418–438. Feeney, Mary K., and Barry Bozeman. 2009. “Stakeholder Red Tape: Com-
Brewer, Gene A., and Richard M. Walker. 2010b. “The Impact of Red Tape paring Perceptions of Public Managers and their Private Consul-
on Governmental Performance: An Empirical Analysis.” Journal of tants.” Public Administration Review 69(4): 710–726.
Public Administration Research and Theory 20(1): 233–257. Feeney, Mary K., and Leisha DeHart-Davis. 2009. “Bureaucracy and Public
Campbell, Jesse W. 2017. “Red Tape and Transformational Leadership: Employee Behavior: A Case of Local Government.” Review of Public
An Organizational Echelons Perspective.” Journal of Organizational Personnel Administration 29(4): 311–326.
Change Management 30(1): 76–90. Fishbein, Martin, and Icek Ajzen. 1975. Belief, Attitude, Intention, and
Campbell, Jesse W. 2019. “Obtrusive, Obstinate and Conspicuous: Red Behavior: An Introduction to Theory and Research. Reading, MA:
Tape from A Heideggerian Perspective.” International Journal of Addison-Wesley.
Organizational Analysis. 27(5): 1657–72. Giauque, David, Simon Anderfuhren-Biget, and Frédéric Varone. 2013.
Campbell, Jesse W. 2020. “Red Tape, Rule Burden, and Legitimate “Stress Perception in Public Organisations: Expanding the Job
Performance Trade-Offs: Results from a Vignette Experiment.” Demands–Job Resources Model by Including Public Service Motiva-
Public Performance & Management Review 43(4): 741–765. tion.” Review of Public Personnel Administration 33(1): 58–83.
Campbell, Jesse W., Sanjay K. Pandey, and Lars Arnesen. 2023. Giauque, David, Simon Anderfuhren-Biget, and Frédéric Varone. 2019.
“The Ontology, Origin, and Impact of Divisive Public Sector “Stress and Turnover Intents in International Organizations: Social
Rules: A Meta-Narrative Review of the Red Tape and Administra- Support and Work–Life Balance as Resources.” The International
tive Burden Literatures.” Public Administration Review 83(2): Journal of Human Resource Management 30(5): 879–901.
296–315. Goodsell, Charles. 1985. The Case for Bureaucracy: A Public Administration
Carrigan, Christopher, Sanjay K. Pandey, and Gregg G. Van Ryzin. 2020. Polemic, 2nd ed. Chatham, NJ: Chatham House.
“Pursuing Consilience: Using Behavioral Public Administration Grandey, Alicia A. 2000. “Emotional Regulation in the Workplace: A New
to Connect Research on Bureaucratic Red Tape, Administrative Way to Conceptualize Emotional Labor.” Journal of Occupational
Burden and Regulation.” Public Administration Review 80(1): Health Psychology 5(1): 95–110.
46–52. Grimmelikhuijsen, Stephan, Sebastian Jilke, Asmus Leth Olsen, and Lars
Coursey, David H., and Sanjay K. Pandey. 2007. “Content Domain, Tummers. 2017. “Behavioral Public Administration: Combining
Measurement, and Validity of the Red Tape Construct: A Second- Insights from Public Administration and Psychology.” Public Admin-
Order Confirmatory Factor Analysis.” American Review of Public istration Review 77(1): 45–56.
Administration 37(3): 342–361. Gursoy, Dogan, Oscar Hengxuan Chi, Lu Lu, and Robin Nunkoo. 2019.
Davis, Randall S. 2013. “Union Commitment and Stakeholder Red Tape: “Consumers Acceptance of Artificially Intelligent (AI) Device Use in
How Union Values Shape Perceptions of Organizational Rules.” Service Delivery.” International Journal of Information Management
Review of Public Personnel Administration 33(4): 365–383. 49: 157–169.
Davis, Randall S., and Stephanie A. Pink-Harper. 2016. “Connecting Hassan, Hemin Ali, Xiaodong Zhang, and Ahmad Bayiz Ahmad. 2021.
Knowledge of Rule-Breaking and Perceived Red Tape: How Behav- “Red Tape and Change-Supportive Intention: An Extension of the
ioral Attribution Influences Red Tape Perceptions.” Public Theory of Planned Behavior.” Leadership & Organization Develop-
Performance & Management Review 40(1): 181–200. ment Journal 42(1): 99–113.
Davis, Randall S., and Edmund C. Stazyk. 2014. “Making Ends Meet: How Hattke, Fabian, David Hensel, and Janne Kalucza. 2020. “Emotional
Reinvention Reforms Complement Public Service Motivation.” Pub- Responses to Bureaucratic Red Tape.” Public Administration Review
lic Administration 92(4): 919–936. 80(1): 53–63.
Davis, Randall S., Edmund C. Stazyk, Allysha Kochenour, and Emily Henderson, Alexander C., and Erin L. Borry. 2023. “The Emotional Bur-
Neuhoff. 2020. “Coping with Conflict: Examining the Influence of dens of Public Service: Rules, Trust, and Emotional Labour in Emer-
PSM on Perceptions of Workplace Stressors.” Review of Public gency Medical Services.” Public Money & Management 43(5): 405–
Personnel Administration 40(3): 405–425. 414. https://doi.org/10.1080/09540962.2020.1831180.
Dawson, Kevin M., Kimberly E. O’Brien, and Terry A. Beehr. 2016. “The Katz, Daniel. 1960. “The Functional Approach to the Study of Attitudes.”
Role of Hindrance Stressors in the Job Demand–Control–Support Public Opinion Quarterly 24(2): 163–204.
Model of Occupational Stress: A Proposed Theory Revision.” Journal Kaufman, Herbert. 1960. The Forest Ranger: A Study in Administrative
of Organizational Behavior 37(3): 397–415. Behavior. Baltimore, MD: Johns Hopkins Press.
DeHart-Davis, L. 2009. “Green Tape: A Theory of Effective Organizational Kaufman, Herbert. 1977. Red Tape: Its Origins, Uses, and Abuses. Washing-
Rules.” Journal of Public Administration Research and Theory 19(2): ton, D.C.: Brookings.
361–384. Kaufmann, Wesley, Erin L. Borry, and Leisha DeHart-Davis. 2019. “More
DeHart-Davis, Leisha. 2007. “The Unbureaucratic Personality.” Public than Pathological Formalization: Understanding Organizational
Administration Review 67(5): 892–903. Structure and Red Tape.” Public Administration Review 79(2):
DeHart-Davis, Leisha. 2017. Creating Effective Rules in Public Sector Orga- 236–245.
nizations. Washington, DC: Georgetown University Press. Kaufmann, Wesley, and Mary K. Feeney. 2014. “Beyond the Rules: The
DeHart-Davis, Leisha, Randall S. Davis, and Zachary Mohr. 2015. “Green Effect of Outcome Favourability on Red Tape Perceptions.” Public
Tape and Job Satisfaction: Can Organizational Rules Make Administration 92(1): 178–191.
Employees Happy?” Journal of Public Administration Research and Kaufmann, Wesley, and Lars Tummers. 2017. “The Negative Effect of Red
Theory 25(3): 849–876. Tape on Procedural Satisfaction.” Public Management Review 19(9):
DeHart-Davis, Leisha, and Sanjay K. Pandey. 2005. “Red Tape and Public 1311–27.
Employees: Does Perceived Rule Dysfunction Alienate Managers?” Lazarus, Richard S. 1966. Psychological Stress and the Coping Process. New
Journal of Public Administration Research and Theory 15(1): 133–148. York, NY: McGraw-Hill.
15406210, 0, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/puar.13750 by Cochrane Portugal, Wiley Online Library on [07/12/2023]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
PUBLIC ADMINISTRATION REVIEW 13

Lazarus, Richard S. 1999. Stress and Emotion: A New Synthesis. London, Pandey, Sanjay K., and Gorden A. Kingsley. 2000. “Examining Red Tape
UK: Free Association. in Public and Private Organizations: Alternative Explanations from a
Lazarus, Richard S. 2000. “Toward Better Research on Stress and Coping.” Social Psychological Model.” Journal of Public Administration
American Psychologist 55: 665–673. Research and Theory 10(4): 779–800.
Lazarus, Richard S. 2001. “Relational Meaning and Discrete Emotions.” In Pandey, Sanjay K., Sheela Pandey, and Greg G. Van Ryzin. 2017.
Appraisal Processes in Emotion: Theory, Methods, Research, edited by “Prospects for Experimental Approaches to Research on
K. Scherer, A. Schorr, and T. Johnstone, 37–67. New York, NY: Bureaucratic Red Tape.” In Experiments in Public Management
Oxford University Press. Research: Challenges and Opportunities, edited by O. James, S. Jilke,
Lazarus, Richard S., and Susan Folkman. 1984. Stress, Appraisal, and and Greg G. Van Ryzin, 219–243. New York: Cambridge University
Coping. New York, NY: Springer. Press.
Lazarus, Richard S., and Craig A. Smith. 1988. “Knowledge and Appraisal Pandey, Sanjay K., and Patrick G. Scott. 2002. “Red Tape: A Review and
in the Cognition—Emotion Relationship.” Cognition & Emotion 2(4): Assessment of Concepts and Measures.” Journal of Public Adminis-
281–300. tration Research and Theory 12(4): 553–580.
LePine, Jeffery A., Nathan P. Podsakoff, and Marcie A. LePine. 2005. “A Pandey, Sanjay K., and Eric W. Welch. 2005. “Beyond Stereotypes: A
Meta-Analytic Test of The Challenge Stressor–Hindrance Stressor Multistage Model of Managerial Perceptions of Red Tape.”
Framework: An Explanation for Inconsistent Relationships Among Administration & Society 37(5): 542–575.
Stressors and Performance.” Academy of Management Journal Perrow, Charles. 1986. Complex Organizations: A Critical Essay, 3rd ed.
48(5): 764–775. New York, NY: Random House.
Madsen, Jonas K., Kim S. Mikkelsen, and Donald P. Moynihan. 2022. Potipiroon, Wisanupong. 2022. “Rule Formalization, Bureaucratic Red
“Burdens, Sludge, Ordeals, Red Tape, Oh my!: A User’s Guide to the Tape, and Prosocial Rule Breaking among Street-Level Bureau-
Study of Frictions.” Public Administration 100(2): 375–393. crats: A Citizen-Centered Perspective.” Public Performance & Man-
March, James G. 1994. A Primer on Decision Making: How Decisions Hap- agement Review 45: 638–671. https://doi.org/10.1080/15309576.
pen. New York, NY: The Free Press. 2022.2065644.
March, James G., Martin Schulz, and Xueguang Zhou. 2000. The Dynamics Quratulain, Samina, and Abdul Karim Khan. 2015. “Red Tape,
of Rules: Studies of Change in Written Organizational Codes 2000. Resigned Satisfaction, Public Service Motivation, and Negative
Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press. Employee Attitudes and Behaviors: Testing a Model of Moder-
Masood, Ayesha, and Muhammad Azfar Nisar. 2021. “Administrative ated Mediation.” Review of Public Personnel Administration 35(4):
Capital and Citizens’ Responses to Administrative Burden.” Journal 307–332.
of Public Administration Research and Theory 31(1): 56–72. Rainey, H. G., S. Pandey, and B. Bozeman. (1995). “Research Note: Public
Matteson, Michael T., and John M. Ivancevich. 1987. Controlling Work and Private Managers’ Perceptions of Red Tape.” Public Administra-
Stress: Effective Human Resource Management Strategies. New York, tion Review 55(6): 567–574. https://doi.org/10.2307/3110348.
NY: Wiley. Russell, James A. 1980. “A Circumplex Model of Affect.” Journal of Per-
Miller, Lynn E., and Joseph E. Grush. 1988. “Improving Predictions in sonality and Social Psychology 39(6): 1161–78.
Expectancy Theory Research: Effects of Personality, Expectancies Russell, James A., and Lisa Feldman Barrett. 1999. “Core Affect, Prototypi-
and Norms.” Academy of Management Journal 31(1): 107–122. cal Emotional Episodes, and Other Things Called Emotion: Dissect-
Mintzberg, Henry. 1983. Power in and Around Organizations. Englewood ing the Elephant.” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology
Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall. 76(5): 805–819.
Moynihan, Donald P., Bradley E. Wright, and Sanjay K. Pandey. 2012. Schlosberg, Harold. 1952. “The Description of Facial Expressions in Terms
“Working within Constraints: Can Transformational Leaders Alter of Two Dimensions.” Journal of Experimental Psychology 44(4):
the Experience of Red Tape?” International Public Management 229–237.
Journal 15(3): 315–336. Scott, Patrick G., and Sanjay K. Pandey. 2000. “The Influence of Red Tape
Muylaert, Jolien, Adellen Decramer, and Mieke Audenaert. 2022. “How on Bureaucratic Behavior: An Experimental Simulation.” Journal of
Leader’s Red Tape Interacts with Employees’ Red Tape from the Policy Analysis and Management 19(4): 615–633.
Lens of the Job Demands-Resources Model.” Review of Public Per- Scott, Patrick G., and Sanjay K. Pandey. 2005. “Red Tape and Public Ser-
sonnel Administration 43: 430–455. https://doi.org/10.1177/ vice Motivation: Findings from A National Survey of Managers in
0734371X221087420. State Health and Human Services Agencies.” Review of Public Per-
Nelson, Debra L., and Bret L. Simmons. 2003. “Health Psychology and sonnel Administration 25(2): 155–180.
Work Stress: A more Positive Approach.” In Handbook of Occupa- Scott, W. Richard. 2013. Institutions and Organizations: Ideas, Interests,
tional Psychology, edited by James Campbell Quick and Lois E. and Identities, 4th ed. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Tetrick, 97–119. Boston, MA: Houghton Mifflin. Seo, Myeong-Gu, Lisa Feldman Barrett, and Jean M. Bartunek. 2004. “The
Olsen, Johan P. 2006. “Maybe it Is Time to Rediscover Bureaucracy.” Jour- Role of Affective Experience in Work Motivation.” Academy of Man-
nal of Public Administration Research and Theory 16(1): 1–24. agement Review 29(3): 423–439.
Organ, Dennis W. 1988. Organizational Citizenship Behavior: The Good Spector, Paul E. 2005. “The Stressor Emotion Model of Counterpro-
Soldier Syndrome. Lexington, MA: Lexington Books. ductive Work Behavior.” In Counterproductive Work Behavior:
Pandey, Sanjay K. 1995. Managerial Perceptions of Red Tape. Ph.D. Disser- Investigations of Actors and Targets, edited by S. Fox and P. E.
tation, Syracuse University. Spector, 151–174. Washington, DC: American Psychological
Pandey, Sanjay K. 2021. “The Psychological Process View of Bureaucratic Association.
Red Tape.” In Research Handbook Human Resource Management in Spector, Paul E., and Suzy Fox. 2002. “An Emotion-Centered Model of
the Public Sector, edited by Eva Knies and Bram Steijn. Chetlenham, Voluntary Work Behavior: Some Parallels between Counterproduc-
UK: Edward Elgar. tive Work Behavior and Organizational Citizenship Behavior.”
Pandey, Sanjay K. 2023. “Elevating Subjective Individual Experience in Human Resource Management Review 12(2): 269–292.
Public Policy and Public Administration: Reflections on Red Tape, Steijn, Bram, and Joris Van der Voet. 2019. “Relational Job Characteristics
Administrative Burden, and Sludge.” Public Administration Review and Job Satisfaction of Public Sector Employees: When Prosocial
83: 1072–82. https://doi.org/10.1111/puar.13695. Motivation and Red Tape Collide.” Public Administration 97(1):
Pandey, Sanjay K., David H. Coursey, and Donald P. Moynihan. 2007. 64–80.
“Organizational Effectiveness and Bureaucratic Red Tape: A Multi- Taylor, Jeannette. 2018. “Working Extra Hours in the Australian Public
method Study.” Public Performance & Management Review 30(3): Service: Organizational Drivers and Consequences.” Review of Public
398–425. Personnel Administration 38(2): 193–217.
15406210, 0, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/puar.13750 by Cochrane Portugal, Wiley Online Library on [07/12/2023]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
14 A PSYCHOLOGICAL PROCESS THEORY OF RED TAPE

Van Loon, Nina M., Peter L. M. Leisink, Eva Knies, and Gene A. Brewer.
2016. “Red Tape: Developing and Validating A New Job-Centered public management and human resources including
Measure.” Public Administration Review. 76(4): 662–673. red tape, goal ambiguity, public service motivation, and
Walker, Richard M., and Gene A. Brewer. 2008. “An Organizational Eche- public sector unionization. Email: rsdavis@siu.edu
lon Analysis of the Determinants of Red Tape in Public Organiza-
tions.” Public Administration Review 68(6): 1112–27. Sanjay K. Pandey is a Shapiro Professor of Public Pol-
Weiner, Bernard. 1986. An Attributional Theory of Motivation and Emotion. icy and Public Administration at the Trachtenberg
New York, NY: Springer-Verlag.
School, The George Washington University. His schol-
Welsh, David T., Michael D. Baer, and Hudson Sessions. 2020. “Hot Pur-
suit: The Affective Consequences of System-Set Versus Self-Set arship focuses on public administration and public
Goals for Emotional Exhaustion and Citizenship Behavior.” Journal policy, dealing with questions central to leading and
of Applied Psychology 105(2): 166–185. managing public organizations. Email: skpandey@
Zahradnik, Stefan. 2022. “Red Tape: Redefinition and Reconceptuali- gwu.edu; sanjay.k.pandey@gmail.com
zation Based on Production Theory.” International Public
Management Journal: 1–20. https://doi.org/10.1080/10967494.
2022.2063462.

How to cite this article: Davis, Randall S., and


AUTHOR BIOGRAPHIES Sanjay K. Pandey. 2023. “‘Feeling Out’ the Rules: A
Psychological Process Theory of Red Tape.” Public
Randall S. Davis is an Associate Professor at the School Administration Review 1–14. https://doi.org/10.1111/
of Management and Marketing, Southern Illinois puar.13750
University. His research examines concepts related to

You might also like