Can Imagined Interactions Produce Positive Perceptions

You might also like

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 10

Can Imagined Interactions Produce Positive Perceptions?

Reducing Prejudice Through Simulated Social Contact

Richard J. Crisp University of Kent


Rhiannon N. Turner University of Leeds

The contact hypothesis states that, under the right condi- an older Black man). What might be the impact of imag-
tions, contact between members of different groups leads to ining this encounter? The imagined interaction was a suc-
more positive intergroup relations. The authors track re- cess: You found lots to talk about, it was a pleasant en-
cent trends in contact theory to the emergence of extended,
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

counter, and your trip went more quickly as a result. You


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

or indirect, forms of contact. These advances lead to an might therefore reason that if you actually met an older
intriguing proposition: that simply imagining intergroup Black man on a train in the future, you might have more
interactions can produce more positive perceptions of out- positive expectations about how that encounter would go.
groups. The authors discuss empirical research supporting These positive expectations may make you feel more pos-
the imagined contact proposition and find it to be an itive and open toward older Black men in general. The next
approach that is at once deceptively simple and remarkably time you actually met an older Black man, you might well
effective. Encouraging people to mentally simulate a pos- feel less anxious and more confident about starting a con-
itive intergroup encounter leads to improved outgroup versation.
attitudes and reduced stereotyping. It curtails intergroup
The scenario you imagined is pretty much what we
anxiety and extends the attribution of perceivers’ positive
have used in our research on imagined intergroup contact
traits to others. The authors describe the advantages and
disadvantages of imagined contact compared to conven- (although, as we describe later, the benefits of the technique
tional strategies, outline an agenda for future research, can be observed with a much less involving narrative). We
and discuss applications for policymakers and educators in believe that imagined contact is such an exciting prospect
their efforts to encourage more positive intergroup rela- because it provides a simple, flexible, and effective means
tions. of promoting more positive perceptions of outgroups. We
do not advocate imagined contact as a replacement for
Keywords: intergroup contact, imagined contact, prejudice existing interventions, such as extended or actual contact
One day you find yourself on a busy train. You get a seat and start (which we expect to have a stronger overall impact on
reading the novel you brought with you to pass the time. At the attitude change). Rather, we assert that the value in imag-
next stop, an older Black man boards the train and sits down next ined contact is in its ability to encourage people to seek out
to you. After a few minutes, the man looks at what you are contact, to remove inhibitions associated with existing prej-
reading and comments that it is one of his favorite books. This udices, and to prepare people to engage outgroups with an
begins a discussion in which you share your thoughts on the open mind. We argue that imagined contact could be highly
book and what you both enjoyed about it. The conversation
meanders, and by the time you get off the train, 30 minutes
effective as a first step on the route toward reconciliation
later, you have discussed a whole range of topics, from the and reduced prejudice, on a continuum of contact that
stresses of having to commute to work every day, to what neigh- provides a road map for the use of multiple contact strat-
borhood you live in, to what your children’s favorite subjects are egies in improving intergroup relations.
at school. In what follows, we track recent trends in contact
theory, from the use of actual contact to the development of

M ost people will have found it easy to imagine


this scenario. There may have been unfamiliar
elements (perhaps they do not often travel on
trains, or perhaps they do not have children), but imagina-
tion is a powerful tool, and people can envisage themselves
indirect forms of contact. From here, we outline the imag-
ined contact proposition, describe supportive empirical
studies, and discuss how we isolated the most effective
imagined contact narratives. Finally, we consider how fu-
ture research might develop the technique further and the
in almost any situation. The power of imaginative thought range of potential applications it may offer.
lies at the heart of their hopes, fears, aspirations, and
innovations. In this article, we argue that psychologists can
harness this power to encourage more positive intergroup Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Richard J.
relations. Crisp, Centre for the Study of Group Processes, Department of Psychol-
ogy, University of Kent, Canterbury, Kent CT2 7NP, United Kingdom, or
Take our opening illustration. It did not involve just a to Rhiannon N. Turner, Institute of Psychological Sciences, University of
train, a book, and a conversation. If you are young, White, Leeds, Leeds, LS2 9JT, United Kingdom. E-mail: r.crisp@kent.ac.uk or
or a woman, it also featured an intergroup encounter (with r.n.turner@leeds.ac.uk

May–June 2009 ● American Psychologist 231


© 2009 American Psychological Association 0003-066X/09/$12.00
Vol. 64, No. 4, 231–240 DOI: 10.1037/a0014718
Recent Trends in Contact Theory remain unrealized. Is this an intractable limitation? We
argue that it is not and that the power inherent to the
Promoting the peaceful coexistence of different social concept of contact enables an intriguing and enabling prop-
groups is perhaps the biggest social issue people face. New osition: that even imagining intergroup contact could im-
and emerging conflicts serve as vivid reminders of the prove intergroup attitudes.
importance of this endeavor; immigration and globalization
underscore the need for informed policies that encourage Opportunity for Contact
co-operation and tolerance. Do people face an inevitable
clash of civilizations (Huntington, 1996) as tensions erupt Contact can reduce prejudice only when social groups and
and escalate around differences in religion and ethnicity? group members have the opportunity to engage positively
Social psychologists have long been concerned with this with one another (e.g., Phinney, Ferguson, & Tate, 1997;
question and have sought to understand the relationships Turner, Hewstone, &Voci, 2007). Unfortunately, there are
among segregation, social contact, and intergroup relations. many examples where the local intergroup relations afford
This work has maintained sustained interest in one of the few such opportunities. In the United States, for example,
most important concepts in psychologists’ developing un- segregation of Latino and White communities remains per-
vasive (Martin, 2006), and the average White person lives
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

derstanding of the nature of prejudice: intergroup contact.


in a predominantly White neighborhood with less than 10%
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

In 1954, Gordon Allport proposed the idea that con-


tact between members of opposing groups, under the right Black residents (Logan, 2001). Many Catholic and Protes-
conditions, would lessen intergroup hostility and lead to tant communities in Belfast, Northern Ireland, include a
more positive intergroup attitudes. In the intervening years very low percentage of residents from the other commu-
this contact hypothesis has become one of the most exten- nity, and only 5% of Northern Irish children attend mixed
sively researched ideas in psychology (Oskamp & Jones, Catholic–Protestant schools (Office for National Statistics,
2000) and a recent meta-analysis of 515 of these studies has 2001). There are many other examples of more extreme
confirmed the core proposition: There is a robust, highly segregation, from the Green Line in Cyprus to the West
significant, negative relationship between contact and prej- Bank Wall in Israel (Pettigrew, 2008). Even in multicul-
udice (Pettigrew & Tropp, 2006). Of importance, this anal- tural communities where many different groups live in
ysis revealed that, although contact that met Allport’s op- close proximity to one another, there can be very little
timal conditions (co-operation, common goals, equal social opportunity for meaningful interaction, and people have a
status, and institutional support) led to the greatest reduc- tendency to interact mainly with those similar to them-
tions in prejudice, prejudice was still reduced in their selves, especially with regard to age, race, and gender
absence. This indicated that these optimal conditions are, in (Graham & Cohen, 1997). For instance, in the multiracial
fact, best considered facilitating conditions, rather than city of Bradford, United Kingdom, South Asian and White
necessary conditions. communities remain largely isolated from one another,
Key principles of contact have been distilled into with residents living “parallel lives.” This social segrega-
intergroup contact theory (Brown & Hewstone, 2005; Pet- tion is regarded as contributing to Britain’s worse ever race
tigrew, 1998), which specifies how, when, and why contact riots in July 2001 (Cantle, 2001). In all of these circum-
stances, interventions that involve intergroup contact may
is associated with reduced prejudice. Contact theory has
be very difficult to establish. Yet, it is in precisely these
become a sophisticated account of the conditions under
settings that contact interventions are needed the most.
which intergroup interactions have benefits for intergroup
What if there was a way to produce the positive effects
relations, and it has provided an important guide for diverse
of contact without there being any actual contact? This was
applications of the core idea. For instance, psychologists
the motivation behind the development of an ingenious
now know that a unique form of contact, cross-group
indirect form of intergroup contact: extended contact.
friendship, is more effective at improving outgroup atti-
tudes than less intimate forms of contact (Pettigrew, 1997; Extended Contact
Pettigrew & Tropp, 2006; Turner, Hewstone, & Voci,
2007; Turner, Hewstone, Voci, Paolini, & Christ, 2007). According to the extended contact hypothesis (Wright,
Despite its success, contact theory has had its critics. Aron, McLaughlin-Volpe, & Ropp, 1997), the benefits of
For instance, some have argued that researchers have been contact could accrue from vicarious experiences—learning
overly optimistic about the power of contact and that al- that people we know engage in positive interactions with
though studies have demonstrated prejudice reduction at an members of outgroups (even if we ourselves do not). A
individual level, it is not yet known whether this translates number of studies have now demonstrated the benefits of
into broader societal change (Dixon, Durrheim, & Tredoux, extended contact. In two studies, Wright et al. (1997)
2005). This is an important question and one that can be showed that White respondents who knew at least one
answered as contact interventions are tested with greater ingroup member with an outgroup friend reported weaker
frequency in field settings. Our concern is with a different prejudice toward that target group than those who had no
practical limitation: that contact can work only where the extended outgroup friends. Moreover, the more extended
opportunity for contact exists. When groups are highly outgroup friends a participant had, the weaker their preju-
segregated, physically or socially, or when there is little dice. In a third study, to demonstrate extended cross-group
motivation to engage in contact, the benefits of contact may friendship experimentally, Wright et al. initially created

232 May–June 2009 ● American Psychologist


intergroup conflict between two small artificial groups. One Extended contact may be especially useful when there is
participant from each group was then chosen to take part in less opportunity for contact because it does not depend on
an unrelated co-operative game designed to create interper- personal experience, but depends simply on the existence
sonal closeness. Individuals who experienced direct inter- of some contact somewhere in one’s social network. The
group contact returned to discuss the experience with the importance of this idea for policymakers and educators
rest of their group. Participants who merely learned about seeking to develop interventions to reduce prejudice cannot
the intergroup contact from the returning member showed be understated because it suggests that contact may be a far
less ingroup favoritism than they had before the extended more powerful and flexible means of improving intergroup
contact experience. relations than previously thought.
The extended contact effect has subsequently been So, just how powerful is the concept of contact?
demonstrated in a number of studies. In a survey of Cath- Researchers know that actual contact is not (at least not
olic and Protestant students in Northern Ireland, partici- always) a requirement: Simply learning about positive in-
pants completed a questionnaire focusing on the number of tergroup interactions can change intergroup attitudes. Does
ingroup friends they had who had outgroup friends, their extended contact solve the opportunity-for-contact prob-
attitudes toward the opposing community, and their expe- lem? The answer is yes, to some extent, but it cannot do so
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

rience of intergroup anxiety (Paolini, Hewstone, Cairns, & entirely. Under some circumstances, extended contact
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Voci, 2004). To measure intergroup anxiety, participants could suffer the same limitation as actual contact. In highly
were asked to think about how they would feel mixing segregated settings, one simply might not know anyone
socially with a group of people from the other community who has an outgroup friend (or even anyone who would
(e.g., how awkward, self-conscious, and defensive they interact in any meaningful way with an outgroup member).
would feel). Structural equation modeling revealed that We could call this a lack of extended opportunities for
extended contact was associated with lower levels of out- contact, where no outgroup friends exist within one’s wider
group prejudice, a relationship that was fully mediated by social network. This is a characteristic of many real con-
reduced intergroup anxiety. Research has also found that texts of conflict, ranging from the Middle East to Cyprus to
extended contact between White and Asian undergraduates the former Yugoslavia. However, if the concept of contact
produced positive outgroup attitudes (Turner, Hewstone, is powerful enough to extend to extended contact, perhaps
Voci & Vonofakou, 2008). In this research, participants it can be extended just a little further.
were asked how many White people they knew who had
South Asian friends and how this related to a number of
Imagery’s Social Impact
outcome measures. These included how anxious they felt at Pettigrew (1997, 1998, 2008) proposed that to take inter-
the prospect of future interactions with South Asians (in- group contact theory forward, researchers should focus on
tergroup anxiety), how much they thought their White understanding in greater depth how unique forms of contact
peers liked South Asians (ingroup norms), how much they reduce prejudice. Here, we describe one such new approach
thought South Asians liked White people (outgroup that capitalizes on the basic idea of contact but does so in
norms), and how much overlap they perceived there to be a way that discounts the limits of opportunity, an approach
between themselves and South Asians (inclusion of the based on the mental simulation of social contact.
outgroup in the self; Aron, Aron, & Smollan, 1992). Using The mind’s tremendous capacity for imagination has
structural equation modeling, Turner et al. (2008) found captivated psychologists since the earliest enquiries (Gal-
that extended contact was associated with more positive ton, 1883; James, 1890), and it is a concept that has
outgroup attitudes. Moreover, this relationship was fully enjoyed enduring appeal. Mental imagery has been found
mediated by lower intergroup anxiety, more positive per- to elicit emotional and motivational responses similar to
ceived ingroup norms, more positive perceived outgroup those of real experiences (Dadds, Bovbjerg, Redd, & Cut-
norms, and greater inclusion of the outgroup in the self. more, 1997), and neuropsychological studies have shown
Extended contact has also been successfully used as that mental imagery shares the same neurological basis as
an intervention to reduce prejudice among school children. perception and uses neurological mechanisms similar to
Following an intervention that involved being read stories those for memory, emotion, and motor control (Kosslyn,
about an ingroup member and a refugee, children between Ganis, & Thompson, 2001). Functionally, mental imagery
ages 5 and 11 had significantly more positive attitudes serves as an important element in the selection, rehearsal,
toward refugees compared with those in a control condition preparation, and planning of action and goal-directed be-
(Cameron, Rutland, Brown, & Douch, 2006). In other havior (Marks, 1999), and it has been shown to have
research, Finnish teenagers read stories in their schools significant benefits for activities ranging from academic
about same-age ingroup peers engaging in close friendships ability to sporting performance (Taylor, Pham, Rivkin, &
with foreigners (Liebkind & McAlister, 1999). Tolerance Armor, 1998).
toward foreigners increased or remained stable in schools There is a precedent for the social impact of imagery.
where the intervention was introduced, whereas it remained Recent research has shown that simply imagining a partic-
stable or decreased in some of the control schools. ular social situation can have the same effect as the expe-
This research is important because it shows that actual rience itself. Research has found that after imagining a
experience of contact with outgroups is not the only way (counterstereotypic) strong woman, participants demon-
that contact can have benefits for intergroup relations. strated fewer implicit stereotypes than participants who

May–June 2009 ● American Psychologist 233


engaged in neutral or stereotypical mental imagery (imag- We know that this is important for direct contact, and it is
ining a weak woman or a strong man) or participants who the same for imagined contact. Empirically, we have shown
had not engaged in any imagery (Blair, Ma, & Lenton, that imagined contact works better when it is positive
2001). Other research has investigated the role of imagin- compared to neutral (Stathi & Crisp, 2008, Experiment 1).
ing a social context on the bystander apathy effect, the idea A positive tone is also important to guard against a possible
that people are less likely to help others if other people are negative tone, which might emerge if participants are left to
present. Research has found that simply imagining being in their own devices (this may be likely, especially in contexts
a large group can lead to significantly less helping behavior of conflict or extreme segregation). Other than these two
on a subsequent task (Garcia, Weaver, Moskowitz, & Dar- core elements, we have found that other embellishments
ley, 2002). In their study, Garcia et al. (2002) found that have very little impact on the basic effect. For instance,
participants who imagined having a meal out with 10 other describing the imagined encounter as interesting has no
people were subsequently less likely to help the experi- additional impact (Stathi & Crisp, 2008, Experiments 1 and
menter by participating in a second study than those who 3), and there are no effects of adding detail like that used in
imagined having a meal out with just 1 other person. It the narrative at the start of this article (Turner, Crisp, &
therefore seems that imagining both people and situations Lambert, 2007, Experiment 1).
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

has a significant impact on perceptions, attitudes, and at- In summary, unlike actual contact, from the outset the
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

tributions. It turns out that when person and situation are imagined contact effect seems unfettered by a raft of nec-
combined in imagined interactions, something similar oc- essary conditions, other than the two core principles we
curs. have identified. Indeed, its applicability and effectiveness
are part of the power and flexibility of the approach. Of
Imagined Intergroup Contact course, as with actual contact, further research may well
Imagined intergroup contact is the mental simulation of a uncover a range of facilitating conditions that improve its
social interaction with a member or members of an out- effectiveness. We return to a discussion of these possible
group category. The basic idea is that mentally simulating conditions at the end of the article. Next, however, we
a positive contact experience activates concepts normally discuss the empirically observed impacts of imagined con-
associated with successful interactions with members of tact, before finally discussing its potential applications for
other groups. These can include feeling more comfortable policy and practice.
and less apprehensive about the prospect of future contact
with the group, and this reduced anxiety should reduce Impacts of Imagined Contact
negative outgroup attitudes. According to Garcia et al. Improved Intergroup Attitudes
(2002), imagery increases the accessibility of abstract con-
cepts associated with that social context. Imagining being In three studies, we found that participants who were asked
in a crowd, for example, activates feelings of being lost in to imagine a positive interaction with an outgroup member
a crowd and unaccountable, feelings that are associated subsequently expressed more positive attitudes and stereo-
with less helping behavior in real situations. Similarly, typed less than participants who did not (Turner, Crisp, &
when people imagine intergroup contact, they should en- Lambert, 2007). Two studies showed that young partici-
gage in conscious processes that parallel the processes pants who imagined a scenario in which they participated
involved in actual intergroup contact. They may, for ex- in a short positive interaction with an older person showed
ample, actively think about what they would learn about less ingroup favoring bias in subsequent evaluations (as-
the outgroup member, how they would feel during the sessed immediately after the task). This was the case
interaction, and how this would influence their perceptions whether participants imagined contact compared to simply
of that outgroup member and the outgroup more generally. imagining an outdoor scene (Experiment 1) or compared to
In turn, this should lead to more positive evaluations of the simply thinking about an older person (Experiment 2; i.e.,
outgroup, similar to the effects of face-to-face contact. without any interaction involved). In a third study, we
At the start of this article, we presented a fairly elab- focused on attitudes of heterosexual men toward homosex-
orate imagined contact narrative. In fact, we have found a ual men (see Herek & Capitanio, 1996; Vonofakou, Hew-
much more minimal instructional set is sufficient to yield stone, & Voci, 2007, for actual contact studies with these
positive effects. The core task instructions are very simple: groups). Participants who imagined talking to a homosex-
“We would like you to take a minute to imagine yourself ual man on a train subsequently evaluated homosexual men
meeting [an outgroup] stranger for the first time. Imagine in general more positively and stereotyped homosexual
that the interaction is positive, relaxed, and comfortable.” men less (perceived less homogeneity) than participants
This simple phrase includes two key elements that we have who imagined an outdoor scene.
found to be the critical components. First is the instruction
Projection to Outgroups
to engage in simulation. We have found that running
through the mental script of an interaction is critical for Research has shown that positively toned imagined contact
observing positive effects (thinking, in contrast, of just an leads to greater projection of positive traits to the target
outgroup member in the absence of any simulated interac- outgroup (Stathi & Crisp, 2008). Projection is a process by
tion has no positive effects on attitudes; Turner, Crisp, & which traits and attitudes are attributed to others and can
Lambert, 2007, Experiment 2). Second is a positive tone. constitute a fundamental “cognitive basis for ingroup fa-

234 May–June 2009 ● American Psychologist


voritism” (Robbins & Krueger, 2005, p. 42; see also attitude toward gay men in general and also perceived there
Cadinu & Rothbart, 1996). This is because projection of to be greater variability among the outgroup than partici-
positive self traits to similar others (i.e., the ingroup) is pants in the control condition. We found that the positive
generally stronger for ingroups than outgroups (Clement & relationship between imagined contact and outgroup atti-
Krueger, 2002) and occurs in lieu of stereotyping (Ames, tude was mediated by reduced intergroup anxiety. Anxiety
2004). If positive contact can make relevant outgroup did not, however, mediate the relationship between imag-
members seem more similar to self, which we know is the ined contact and outgroup variability. This parallels the
case for actual and extended contact (Eller & Abrams, relationships observed in research on actual contact. Al-
2004; Wright et al., 1997), then those outgroup members though some direct contact studies have found intergroup
should benefit from the positive projection that ingroup anxiety to mediate the effect of intergroup contact on
members typically benefit from (Robbins & Krueger, outgroup variability (e.g., Islam & Hewstone, 1993; Voci
2005). Imagined contact did indeed lead to more positive & Hewstone, 2003), the evidence for this is less consistent
trait projection to outgroups following imagined contact, than the effect of intergroup anxiety on affective outcome
compared to controls, with a variety of target groups, measures, such as feelings of intergroup comfort and liking
including Mestizos in Mexico and international students in (e.g., Tropp & Pettigrew, 2005; Turner, Hewstone, & Voci,
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

the United Kingdom (Stathi & Crisp, 2008). Notably, this 2007).
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

projection was consistent with the lower perceived homo-


geneity of outgroups observed by Turner, Crisp, and Lam- Reduced Stereotype Threat
bert (2007, Experiment 3). Imagined contact can also reduce self-stereotyping and,
Consistent with projection theory and the idea that correspondingly, the impact of negative self-stereotypes on
imagined contact activates cognitive– behavioral processes quantitative performance (the stereotype threat effect,
similar to those of actual contact, projection was greatest Steele, 1997; see Abrams et al., 2008; Crisp & Abrams,
under three conditions. Projection should be higher when 2008). Imagined contact could serve a protective function
the personal self is more salient than the collective self for older people exposed to contexts where they might
because the personal self is the informational base used for otherwise suffer performance decrements. Research with
projection (Cadinu & Rothbart, 1996). Correspondingly, older people has found that self-stereotyping affects a range
majorities (who have low personal self-salience) projected of cognitive abilities consistent with the stereotype that
more than minorities (who have high collective self-sa- cognitive performance declines with age (Hess, Hinson, &
lience). Moreover, lower identifiers (who have high per- Statham, 2004; Levy, 1996). The application of imagined
sonal self-salience) projected more than higher identifiers contact to this domain was based on the premise that
(who have high collective self-salience), and a direct ma- intergenerational contact is generally limited (Hagestad &
nipulation of personal self-salience versus social self-sa- Uhlenberg, 2005) and that actual contact has been found to
lience also led to greater projection (Stathi & Crisp, 2008). reduce threat effects in older people through reduced anx-
Reduced Anxiety iety (Abrams, Eller, & Bryant, 2006). Research has found
that older people (all over 60 years old) who imagined a
A key mediating process that accounts for the positive short social interaction with a young stranger (compared to
impact of actual contact on intergroup attitudes is anxiety a control) were subsequently immune to the depleting
(Islam & Hewstone, 1993). Correspondingly, our research effects of a threat comparison (i.e., a stated comparison of
has established intergroup anxiety as a mediator of imag- the performance of older and younger people) on cognitive
ined contact effects (Turner, Crisp, & Lambert, 2007). test performance (see Abrams et al., 2008).
Intergroup anxiety is the negative emotional reaction that
can occur at the prospect of having to engage in an inter- Alternative Explanations
group encounter. It is most likely to arise when there has
been minimal previous contact and when there are large Although the research programs outlined here demonstrate
differences in status (Stephan & Stephan, 1985). When the range of benefits that accrue from imagined contact, we
individuals have had a successful interaction with an out- have also been careful to rule out alternative explanations,
group member, however, their level of intergroup anxiety is such as cognitive load, demand characteristics, priming,
likely to be reduced, as they come to realize they have and generalized positive affect.
nothing to fear from such interactions. Consistent with this Cognitive Load
reasoning, a number of studies in diverse intergroup set-
tings have found the effect of intergroup contact on reduc- Perhaps imagined contact effects are not about imagining
ing prejudice to be mediated by intergroup anxiety (Islam contact at all but are just about the cognitive activity
& Hewstone, 1993; Paolini et al., 2004; Turner, Hewstone, required to engage with the task. A number of studies have
&Voci, 2007; Voci & Hewstone, 2003). demonstrated, however, that imagined contact has benefits
In one study, male heterosexual participants were not only compared to baseline (no task), but also relative to
asked to imagine a positive interaction with a homosexual control conditions that have equivalent cognitive load and
man (Turner, Crisp, & Lambert, 2007, Experiment 3). As require identical mental operations. We have found, for
noted earlier, participants who spent a few minutes imag- example, that imagined contact reduced the expression of
ining intergroup contact subsequently had a more positive biased evaluations compared to imagining an outdoor scene

May–June 2009 ● American Psychologist 235


(Turner, Crisp, & Lambert, 2007, Experiments 1 and 3). special about mentally simulating the intergroup interac-
This was similarly the case when we examined the effect of tion.
imagined contact on trait projection (Stathi & Crisp, 2008,
Experiment 3). Although a priori it is difficult to see how Positive Affect
simply engaging in mental imagery in and of itself might Perhaps the imagined contact effect is not about imagining
lead to more positive attitudes (i.e., vs. specifically engag- contact at all but is just about the positive feelings that are
ing in imagined contact), these studies help to rule this associated with the interactive nature of the task. It is
possibility out as a competing explanation. certainly the case that contact works better when it is
positively toned (Pettigrew & Tropp, 2006). Consistent
Demand Characteristics with this, imagining positive interactions is more effective
Perhaps the imagined contact effect is not about imagining than imagining neutrally valenced interactions (Stathi &
contact at all but is just about participants guessing what Crisp, 2008, Experiment 1). However, this also raises the
the task is meant to do. We have, however, found no question of whether it is, in fact, just the positive charac-
evidence to suggest that imagined contact effects can be teristics of imagined contact that are responsible for the
explained by demand characteristics. Typically, no partic- positive effects. In other words, perhaps simply thinking
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

ipants report any awareness of the experimental hypotheses about some positive interaction (regardless of whether it
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

at feedback. For instance, in Turner, Crisp, and Lambert features the outgroup) yields more positive evaluations.
(2007) only four participants reported any suspicion about To rule out this possibility, an experiment tested
the purpose of the experiment, and not one participant whether imagined contact has benefits (for perceptions of
successfully identified the aims of the experiment. None- French targets by British participants) because of the iden-
theless, to better rule out this explanation, we have exam- tity of the target or just because of the interactive nature of
ined the effects of imagined contact on implicit measures of the imagined scene (Stathi & Crisp, 2008, Experiment 2).
attitudes (Turner & Crisp, in press). Whereas explicit atti- This was achieved by comparing imagined scenarios that
tudes are conscious, deliberative, and controllable (and are were identical in both conditions, except that the interact-
usually captured by traditional self-report measures), im- ing partner was a specified outgroup member in only one
plicit attitudes are unintentionally activated by the mere condition (in this case, a French person for our British
presence (actual or symbolic) of an attitude object and are participants). In both conditions, participants imagined in-
therefore less likely to be influenced by social desirability teracting with a stranger at a party, but only in the exper-
than are explicit measures. We asked young participants to imental condition was the interaction partner an outgroup
imagine talking to an older stranger, using the same in- member. Imagined contact was significantly more effective
structions as in Turner, Crisp, and Lambert (2007), before in the condition featuring the outgroup member.
completing an explicit outgroup attitude measure and a
measure of implicit attitude, the Implicit Association Test Future Research
(Greenwald, McGee, & Schwartz, 1998). Participants who It is important to extend our knowledge of imagined con-
had imagined contact with an older person not only sub- tact effects in several areas. We must (a) specify optimizing
sequently showed more positive explicit, self-reported, out- conditions, (b) distinguish underlying mechanisms, and (c)
group attitudes toward older people in general, but also elaborate on the range of positive impacts that the inter-
showed more positive implicit outgroup attitudes on a vention can have.
young– older version of the Implicit Association Test.
Optimizing Conditions
Stereotype Priming
Future research should establish the conditions that best
Perhaps the imagined contact effect is not about imagining facilitate the positive impacts of imagined contact. It is
contact at all but is just about thinking about the outgroup. important to be mindful that the same intervention can
The argument here is that imagined contact simply primes mean different things to different people (see Crisp, Stone,
participants to think of the outgroup (e.g., older people), & Hall, 2006, for a detailed discussion of such issues). For
which then invokes a conscious attempt to regulate behav- instance, imagined contact can lead to greater projection of
ior to appear nonprejudiced (e.g., Devine & Monteith, positive traits to outgroups for those individuals who are
1999). On the basis of feedback received from participants, less committed to their ingroup (Stathi & Crisp, 2008).
we are confident that a priming effect cannot account for Uncovering how such individual differences moderate
our findings. However, to empirically rule out this expla- imagined contact effects is an important endeavor, espe-
nation, we carried out an experiment in which we sought to cially if psychologists are to move toward effective imple-
replicate the imagined contact effect but used a control mentation. Perceived authenticity of the imagined encoun-
condition in which we simply primed the outgroup (Turner, ter is also important. In clinical and counseling psychology,
Crisp, & Lambert, 2007, Experiment 2). We observed less role playing is perceived as a more authentic representation
bias in the imagined contact condition compared to the of reality when it features actors who are culturally similar
control condition. This is consistent with the idea that (Pedersen, 2000). In imagined contact, by definition, the
imagined contact does not simply involve category priming actors are mismatched (and the lower likelihood of actual
and associated self-regulation, but that there is something interactions is the motivation to encourage imagined inter-

236 May–June 2009 ● American Psychologist


actions). Nonetheless, perceived authenticity of the imag- does not share: the mental simulation of one’s personal
ined encounter may be an important variable in future engagement with the outgroup. This brief analysis illus-
research. trates some of the theoretical similarities and differences
Other important questions are how long do the effects among actual, extended, and imagined contact. Further
of the intervention last (in the studies we have reported, specifying the psychological and functional distinctions
attitudes are typically assessed immediately after the imag- among all three approaches is an important task for future
ined contact task), and how many times must the task be research.
repeated to internalize attitude change? These are imple-
mentation issues that are common to all forms of contact Positive Impacts
intervention (or, indeed, to all attitude change interven- We have seen that imagined contact reduces anxiety about
tions). However, there may be some possible routes for the prospect of future intergroup encounters (Turner, Crisp,
further research that are unique to imagined contact. For & Lambert, 2007). As such, the positive impact of imag-
instance, longer term experience of imagined contact may ined contact may not be restricted to changes in attitudes
help to promote an internalized self-supervision process, but could have more direct benefits for actual interactions.
whereby more general adherence to egalitarian norms be- To elaborate, intergroup anxiety can arise as a conse-
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

comes a guiding principle. How many times imagined quence of negative expectations of rejection or discrimina-
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

contact should be implemented to achieve such internalized tion during cross-group interactions or because of fears that
changes and what form this implementation should take are the interaction partner, or the respondents themselves, may
pertinent questions for future research. behave in an incompetent or offensive manner (Stephan &
Underlying Mechanisms Stephan, 1985). These fears may lead people to avoid
intergroup contact altogether. It has been found, for exam-
We have found that, like actual and extended contact, ple, that the more negative White participants’ expectations
imagined contact can improve attitudes by reduced anxiety were about interacting with Black people, the more they
(Turner, Crisp, & Lambert, 2007). A reasonable question is reported avoiding interracial encounters (Plant & Devine,
whether imagined contact shares other mediating mecha- 2003). Similarly, people often explain their failure to ini-
nisms with established forms of contact. For instance, tiate intergroup contact in terms of their fear of being
extended contact works not only by reducing intergroup rejected by the outgroup member (Shelton & Richeson,
anxiety, but also by generating positive norms for inter- 2005). Even if contact is initiated, intergroup anxiety may
group relations and increasing the extent to which the increase the likelihood that individuals will enter the en-
outgroup is included in the self. These processes are based counter with feelings of hostility. In turn, this increases the
on a strong theoretical rationale (Wright et al., 1997) and likelihood that group members will interpret the interaction
are supported by empirical evidence in a variety of inter- in a negative light, with negative consequences for inter-
group contexts testing extended contact (e.g., British chil- group relations. If, however, participants first spend some
dren’s attitudes toward refugees, Cameron et al., 2006; time imagining the encounter, their levels of anxiety will be
Catholics and Protestants in Northern Ireland, Paolini et al., lower and their attitudes more positive when they subse-
2004; Whites and Asians in the United Kingdom, Turner et quently enter the interaction. Imagined contact may there-
al., 2008; Turner, Hewstone, & Voci, 2007). To appropri- fore increase the chances that a subsequent intergroup
ately position imagined contact in line with other methods, encounter will be of a high quality and will result in further
researchers should examine the possible role these multiple positive attitude change. Examining the direct behavioral
mediators play in imagined contact effects. impact of imagined contact is therefore an important focus
We believe that although imagined contact shares a for future research.
number of similarities with extended contact, it may be
distinct in terms of the social norm mechanism. In extended Limitations, Implications, and
contact, participants observe an ingroup member behaving Applications
positively toward an outgroup member, apparently reflect-
ing positive regard. This positive model constitutes an We have demonstrated the benefits of imagined contact in
ingroup norm that uniquely mediates extended contact but a variety of intergroup contexts. We have, for example,
not actual contact (see Turner et al., 2008). This is because showed how imagined contact can change young people’s
extended contact involves a perceptual focus on another attitudes toward older people (Turner, Crisp, & Lambert,
ingroup member, whereas actual contact does not. In actual 2007, Studies 1 and 2), straight men’s attitudes toward gay
contact, the perceiver is focused only on the outgroup and men (Turner, Crisp, & Lambert, 2007, Study 3), and Mex-
has no ingroup model from which to derive a behavioral ican people’s attitudes toward Mestizos in Mexico (Stathi
norm. Because the instructional set used in imagined con- & Crisp, 2008). We have also observed benefits for implicit
tact, like that for actual contact, focuses participants on the intergroup attitudes (Turner & Crisp, in press) and even a
outgroup, it is likely that ingroup norms will be unaffected. positive impact on self-stereotyping and stereotype threat
Thus, although extended and imagined contact are both (Abrams et al., 2008; Crisp & Abrams, 2008). Nonetheless,
indirect forms of contact, they can be distinguished by (at there are some things that imagined contact cannot do, and
least) one mechanism. This also highlights a similarity understanding its limitations is an essential part of devel-
between actual and imagined contact that extended contact oping effective implementation programs.

May–June 2009 ● American Psychologist 237


The most important limitation is that imagined contact cascade of positive interactions, along with all the benefits
probably does not have as powerful or long lasting an effect associated with actual intergroup contact.
as more direct forms of intergroup contact. This is because From a practical perspective, imagined contact may
direct experiences are thought to produce stronger attitudes prove important for the application of contact interventions
on an issue than indirect experiences (Fazio, Powell, & by policymakers and educators. Community leaders in ar-
Herr, 1983; Stangor, Sullivan, & Ford, 1991). Supporting eas with pervasive segregation and tensions between com-
this premise, research comparing actual and extended con- munities recognize that positive intergroup contact might
tact typically shows actual contact to have the stronger reduce prejudice, but they are often faced with practical
impact on prejudice (Paolini et al., 2004; Turner, Hew- difficulties that make such changes difficult. White flight
stone, & Voci, 2007). Given that imagined contact, like from inner city areas to the suburbs has resulted in ethnic
extended contact, is less direct than actual contact, we minority enclaves in the inner cities of Britain, whereas
might expect it to also have a weaker or more temporary White communities dominate the outer suburbs. A similar
effect on intergroup attitudes. phenomenon has occurred in public schools in San Jose,
To counter this limitation, imagined contact has a California, where the proportion of White students has
distinctive advantage over both direct and extended con- dropped by half over the past 10 years (Hwang, 2005).
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

tact. As we discussed at the start of this article, actual Such situations cannot easily be remedied because they are
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

contact can yield benefits only when there is opportunity often related to economic and social inequalities in the
for contact. Although extended contact avoids this problem wider society. When intergroup contact does occur, the
to some extent, it still requires knowledge of outgroup anticipation of the encounter for those who have had little
contact somewhere in one’s wider social network. Imag-
prior experience with the outgroup may lead to intergroup
ined contact requires no experience of outgroup contact,
anxiety, with potentially negative consequences.
either direct or extended.
Imagined contact, however, is an inexpensive and
It is this unique contribution of imagined contact that
practical means of reducing intergroup anxiety and improv-
we believe presents the most exciting prospects for future
application. This is not because it can serve as a replace- ing attitudes, and it can yield benefits even when opportu-
ment for existing approaches—it cannot. We know that nities for direct and extended contact are limited. In social
existing contact interventions have a highly significant (and settings where positive contact is not possible because of
stronger) impact on intergroup attitudes. Rather, it is be- segregation or lack of opportunity, the knowledge that
cause it provides access to these other forms of contact imagining contact can create positive attitudes that approx-
where they might otherwise be unavailable. We believe imate those associated with actual contact is invaluable for
that one of the most tangible benefits of imagined contact policymakers and educators. For example, schools could
is its ability to break inhibitions that go hand in hand with develop and apply teaching techniques that encourage con-
existing prejudices. Imagined contact encourages people to tact imagery to bring groups closer together and promote
seek out contact and prepares them to engage members of tolerance. To date, relatively few social psychological in-
outgroups with an open mind. We know that imagining an terventions to reduce prejudice have been implemented in
event reliably increases the likelihood that the event will schools. However, we believe that imagined contact—
occur and that individuals are more likely to carry out an either alone or in combination with existing interven-
imagined target behavior (Carroll, 1978); imagined contact tions— can provide a simple and practical means of inject-
may have a similar impact on future contact intentions. ing social psychological content into educational
Used in this way, imagined contact might be highly effec- interventions.
tive as a first step on the route toward reconciliation and
reduced prejudice. For this reason, imagined contact might Concluding Remarks
be usefully applied immediately before an intervention that
involves extended or direct contact. In this article, we described a new intervention strategy for
Thinking a little more broadly, an intriguing possibil- improving intergroup relations: imagined intergroup con-
ity is that imagined, extended, and actual contact might tact. Preliminary empirical research has shown that the
form a continuum of psychological interventions that are approach is deceptively simple but remarkably effective.
maximally effective at different stages of social integration. Encouraging people to mentally simulate a positive en-
At early stages of coexistence, there may be high segrega- counter with someone from an outgroup promotes more
tion and little opportunity for contact. In this situation, positive attitudes toward that group. Imagined contact pro-
imagined contact may be the only viable intervention to vides a firmly grounded intervention strategy with signifi-
help encourage attitude change and intentions to engage in cant potential application for policymakers and educators
preliminary contact (or to ensure that when that contact seeking to promote tolerance for social diversity. We be-
does occur, it does so with open minds and an increased lieve this is the strongest testament to the power, flexibility,
chance of success). At intermediate stages, when bound- and enduring appeal of the contact hypothesis. Further
aries have begun to permeate and some positive interac- refinement of this and other applications of the contact
tions have been initiated, extended contact works well to concept will elaborate, extend, and establish such methods
reinforce the impact of isolated (but known) contact en- as viable, defined, and tested strategies for improving in-
counters. Increasing extended contact may then lead to a tergroup relations.

238 May–June 2009 ● American Psychologist


REFERENCES association test. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74,
1464 –1480.
Abrams, D., Crisp, R. J., Marques, S., Fagg, E., Bedford, L., & Provias, Hagestad, G. O., & Uhlenberg, P. (2005). The social separation of old and
D. (2008). Threat inoculation: Experienced and imagined intergenera- young: A root of ageism. Journal of Social Issues, 61, 343–360.
tional contact prevents stereotype threat effects on older people’s math Herek, G. M., & Capitanio, J. (1996). “Some of my best friends”:
performance. Psychology and Aging, 23, 934 –939. Intergroup contact, concealable stigma, and heterosexuals’ attitudes
Abrams, D., Eller, A., & Bryant, J. (2006). An age apart: Effects of toward gay men and lesbians. Personality and Social Psychology Bul-
intergenerational contact and stereotype threat on performance and letin, 22, 412– 424.
intergroup bias. Psychology and Aging, 2, 691–702. Hess, T. M., Hinson, J. T., & Statham, J. A. (2004). Explicit and implicit
Allport, G. W. (1954). The nature of prejudice. Cambridge, MA: Perseus stereotype activation effects on memory: Do age and awareness mod-
Books. erate the impact of priming? Psychology and Aging, 19, 495–505.
Ames, D. (2004). Inside the mind-reader’s toolkit: Projection and stereo- Huntington, S. P. (1996). The clash of civilizations and the remaking of
typing in mental state inference. Journal of Personality and Social world order. New York: Simon & Schuster.
Psychology, 87, 340 –353. Hwang, S. (2005). The new white flight. Wall Street Journal: Online.
Aron, A., Aron, E. N., & Smollan, D. (1992). Inclusion of Other in the Retrieved March 19, 2008, from http://online.wsj.com/article_email/
Self Scale and the structure of interpersonal closeness. Journal of SB113236377590902105lMyQjAxMDE1MzEyOTMxNjkzWj.html
Personality and Social Psychology, 63, 596 – 612. Islam, R. M., & Hewstone, M. (1993). Dimension of contact as predictors
Blair, I. V., Ma, J. E., & Lenton, A. P. (2001). Imagining stereotypes of intergroup anxiety, perceived outgroup variability and outgroup
away: The moderation of implicit stereotypes through mental imagery.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

attitudes: An integrative model. Personality and Social Psychology


Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81, 828 – 841.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Bulletin, 19, 700 –710.


Brown, R., & Hewstone, H. (2005). An integrative theory of intergroup James, W. (1890). The principles of psychology (Vol. 2). New York:
contact. In M. P. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psy- Dover.
chology (Vol. 37, pp. 255–343). San Diego, CA: Academic Press. Kosslyn, S. M., Ganis, G., & Thompson, W. L. (2001). Neural founda-
Cadinu, M. R., & Rothbart, M. (1996). Self-anchoring and differentiation tions of imagery. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 2, 635– 642.
processes in the minimal group setting. Journal of Personality and Levy, B. R. (1996). Improving memory in old age by implicit self-
Social Psychology, 70, 661– 677. stereotyping. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71, 1092–
Cameron, L., Rutland, A., Brown, R., & Douch, R. (2006). Changing 1107.
children’s intergroup attitudes towards refugees: Testing different mod- Liebkind, K., & McAlister, A. L. (1999). Extended contact through peer
els of extended contact. Child Development, 77, 1208 –1219. modelling to promote tolerance in Finland. European Journal of Social
Cantle, T. (2001). Community cohesion: A report of the independent Psychology, 29, 765–780.
review team. London: Home Office. Logan, J. (2001). Ethnic diversity grows, neighborhood integration lags
Carroll, J. S. (1978). The effort of imagining an effect on expectations for behind. Albany: State University of New York at Albany, Lewis
the event: An interpretation in terms of the availability heuristic. Jour- Mumford Center.
nal of Experimental Social Psychology, 14, 88 –96.
Marks, D. F. (1999). Consciousness, mental imagery and action. British
Clement, R. W., & Krueger, J. (2002). Social categorization moderates
Journal of Psychology, 90, 567–585.
social projection. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 38, 219 –
Martin, M. E. (2006). Residential segregation patterns of Latinos in the
231.
United States, 1990 –2000. New York: Routledge.
Crisp, R. J., & Abrams, D. (2008). Improving intergroup attitudes and
Office for National Statistics. (2001). Neighbourhood statistics. Retrieved
reducing stereotype threat: An integrated contact model. In W. Stroebe
July 24, 2006, from http://neighbourhood.statistics.gov.uk
& M. Hewstone (Eds.), European review of social psychology (Vol. 19,
pp. 242–284). Hove, England: Psychology Press. Oskamp, S., & Jones, J. M. (2000). Promising practice in reducing
Crisp, R. J., Stone, C. H., & Hall, N. R. (2006). Recategorization and prejudice: A report from the President’s Initiative on Race. In S.
subgroup identification: Predicting and preventing threats from com- Oskamp (Ed.), Reducing prejudice and discrimination (pp. 319 –334).
mon ingroups. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 32, 230 – Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
243. Paolini, S., Hewstone, M., Cairns, E., & Voci, A. (2004). Effects of direct
Dadds, M. R., Bovbjerg, D. H., Redd, W. H., & Cutmore, T. R. (1997). and indirect cross-group friendships on judgments of Catholics and
Imagery in human classical conditioning. Psychological Bulletin, 122, Protestants in Northern Ireland: The mediating role of an anxiety-
89 –103. reduction mechanism. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 30,
Devine, P. G., & Monteith, M. J. (1999). Automaticity and control in 770 –786.
stereotyping. In S. Chaiken & Y. Trope (Eds.), Dual-process theories in Pedersen, P. (2000). Hidden messages in culture centered counseling.
social psychology (pp. 339 –360). New York: Guilford Press. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Dixon, J., Durrheim, K., & Tredoux, C. (2005). Beyond the optimal Pettigrew, T. F. (1997). Generalized intergroup contact effects on preju-
strategy: A reality check for the contact hypothesis. American Psychol- dice. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 23, 173–185.
ogist, 60, 697–711. Pettigrew, T. F. (1998). Intergroup contact: Theory, research and new
Eller, A., & Abrams, D. (2004). Come together: Longitudinal compari- perspectives. Annual Review of Psychology, 49, 65– 85.
sons of Pettigrew’s reformulated intergroup contact model and the Pettigrew, T. F. (2008). Future directions for intergroup contact theory
common ingroup identity model in Anglo–French and Mexican–Ameri- and research. International Journal of Intercultural Relations, 32, 187–
can contexts. European Journal of Social Psychology, 34, 229 –256. 199.
Fazio, R. H., Powell, M. C., & Herr, P. M. (1983). Toward a process Pettigrew, T. F., & Tropp, L. R. (2006). A meta-analytic test of intergroup
model of the attitude– behavior relation: Accessing one’s attitude upon contact theory. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 90,
mere observation of the attitude object. Journal of Personality and 751–783.
Social Psychology, 44, 723–735. Phinney, J. S., Ferguson, D. L., & Tate, J. D. (1997). Intergroup attitudes
Galton, F. (1883). Inquiries into human faculty and its development. among ethnic minority adolescents: A causal model. Child Develop-
London: Macmillan. ment, 68, 955–969.
Garcia, S. M., Weaver, K., Moskowitz, G. B., & Darley, J. M. (2002). Plant, E. A., & Devine, P. G. (2003). The antecedents and implications of
Crowded minds: The implicit bystander effect. Journal of Personality interracial anxiety. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 29,
and Social Psychology, 83, 843– 853. 790 – 801.
Graham, J. A., & Cohen, R. (1997). Race and sex as factors in children’s Robbins, J. M., & Krueger, J. I. (2005). Social projection to ingroups and
sociometric ratings and friendship choices. Social Development, 6, to outgroups: A review and meta-analysis. Personality and Social
355–372. Psychology Review, 9, 32– 47.
Greenwald, A. G., McGee, D. E., & Schwartz, J. L. K. (1998). Shelton, J. N., & Richeson, J. A. (2005). Intergroup contact and pluralistic
Measuring individual differences in implicit cognition: The implicit ignorance. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 88, 91–107.

May–June 2009 ● American Psychologist 239


Stangor, C., Sullivan, L. A., & Ford, T. E. (1991). Affective and cognitive implicit prejudice via direct and extended contact: The mediating role
determinants of prejudice. Social Cognition, 9, 359 –380. of self-disclosure and intergroup anxiety. Journal of Personality and
Stathi, S., & Crisp, R. J. (2008). Imagining intergroup contact promotes Social Psychology, 93, 369 –388.
projection to outgroups. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, Turner, R. N., Hewstone, M., Voci, A., Paolini, S., & Christ, O. (2007).
44, 943–957. Reducing prejudice via direct and extended cross-group friendship.
Steele, C. M. (1997). A threat in the air: How stereotypes shape the European Review of Social Psychology, 18, 212–255.
intellectual identities and performance of women and African Ameri- Turner, R. N., Hewstone, M., Voci, A., & Vonofakou, C. (2008). A test of
cans. American Psychologist, 52, 613– 629. the extended intergroup contact hypothesis: The mediating role of
Stephan, W. G., & Stephan, C. W. (1985). Intergroup anxiety. Journal of intergroup anxiety, perceived ingroup and outgroup norms, and inclu-
Social Issues, 41, 157–176. sion of the outgroup in the self. Journal of Personality and Social
Taylor, S. E., Pham, L. B., Rivkin, I. D., & Armor, D. A. (1998). Psychology, 95, 843– 860.
Harnessing the imagination: Mental simulation, self-regulation, and Voci, A., & Hewstone, M. (2003). Intergroup contact and prejudice
coping. American Psychologist, 53, 429 – 439. towards immigrants in Italy: The mediational role of anxiety and the
Tropp, L. R., & Pettigrew, T. F. (2005). Differential relationships between moderational role of group salience. Group Processes and Intergroup
intergroup contact and affective and cognitive dimensions of prejudice. Relations, 6, 37–54.
Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 31, 1145–1158. Vonofakou, C., Hewstone, M., & Voci, A. (2007). Contact with outgroup
Turner, R. N., & Crisp, R. J. (in press). Imagining intergroup contact friends as a predictor of meta-attitudinal strength and accessibility of
reduces implicit prejudice. British Journal of Social Psychology. attitudes toward gay men. Journal of Personality and Social Psychol-
Turner, R. N., Crisp, R. J., & Lambert, E. (2007). Imagining intergroup ogy, 92, 804 – 820.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

contact can improve intergroup attitudes. Group Processes and Inter- Wright, S. C., Aron, A., McLaughlin-Volpe, T., & Ropp, S. A. (1997).
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

group Relations, 10, 427– 441. The extended contact effect: Knowledge of cross-group friendships and
Turner, R. N., Hewstone, M., & Voci, A. (2007). Reducing explicit and prejudice. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 73, 73–90.

240 May–June 2009 ● American Psychologist

You might also like