Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 5

See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.

net/publication/234775714

FUN

Article in ACM SIGCHI Bulletin · January 1988


DOI: 10.1145/49108.1045604

CITATIONS READS
119 1,493

2 authors, including:

John M. Carroll
Pennsylvania State University
793 PUBLICATIONS 30,917 CITATIONS

SEE PROFILE

All content following this page was uploaded by John M. Carroll on 20 May 2014.

The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file.


FU N

JOHN NI . CARROL L
JOHN C . THOMA S

We are in many ways just getting started on the proble m the system was probably no easier than other more
of developing a set of concepts and a vocabulary fo r conventional systems of that period (Carroll and Mazur ,
describing the quality of software and in particular its 1986) . The learning problems were different in som e
usability. Consider : easy to use, easy to learn, use r respects to be sure, but there were plenty of them .
friendly, productive, fun . How do these, and other terms Nevertheless, judging from the popular and trade pres s
used to describe software differ ; how do they overlap? In reviews and the impacts on later system interface designs,
this paper, we examine the confusion between being fun people really liked the Lisa-style interface, and that i s
and being easy, as these terms are applied to end-use r very important . We suggest that the Lisa is in fact a n
software and systems . This confusion is interesting in example of a system that was relatively more fun ,
that we can ask how such a circumstance might have eve r although not necessarily more easy to learn and use .
occurred . It is challenging in that we need to separately
clarify the concepts of ease and fun, if they are to provid e What is the relationship between ease and fun? For us ,
us any analytical leverage in understanding softwar e ease fundamentally implies simplicity . Things that are
quality . easy are learned quickly, with few steps and few errors .
They can be communicated to others with few words an d
THE CONFUSION BETWEEN FUN AND EAS E performed fluently . Fun not only fails to carry thi s
When the ascription easy is applied to a system or some monolithic implication of simplicity, but ofte n
software, for example, in a trade press review, wha t contradicts it (e .g., learning to operate a new foo d
exactly is being alleged? Invocation of the term easy i s processor or to hit curve balls) . Paradigmatic example s
rarely based on any real measurement of the behavior of offun indeed must have sufficient complexity or they fal l
humans actually using the system or software . Peopl e flat (jokes that are too obvious, games that are no t
who call software easy have rarely spent the time makin g challenging) . The complexity must appear warranted t o
the usability measurements and observations that migh t the person (tacking on ad hoc complications will no t
seriously sustain this judgment . They don't really kno w evoke fun) . It seems that things are fun when we expec t
whether the software is easy or not in any technical sense . them to be of moderate complexity (interesting bu t
But whatever it is that they are basing their judgments o n tractable) and then in fact find them to be so (i .e ., not
is still of interest, for often these judgments express a too difficult or too easy) .
consensus view .
We admit, however, that there are complications inheren t
An example of what we have in mind can be found in th e in splitting fun from ease . To begin with, it does see m
story of the Lisa system . During its lifetime (1983-1985 ) that there can be tangling attributions involving both .
Lisa was widely labeled as a breakthrough in ease—eas e Thus, if the experience of learning to use a particula r
of use and ease of learning . However, a laboratory stud y piece of software is fun (because it has been structure d
of professionals learning to use Lisa revealed that in fact into a training game, for example), one might also come

SIGCHI Bulletin 21 January 1988 Volume 19 Number 3


to believe that the learning was easy in the sense that i t intrinsic reward of the colored pencils alone was a
required relatively less discipline, teeth grinding, etc . t o stronger incentive than the extrinsic reward offered fo r
accomplish . Conversely, if learning to use a piece of playing with them, and that in fact, offering the extrinsic
software is easy (i .e ., takes very little time, very fe w reward weakened the attractiveness of the pencils .
steps), then one might similarly come to believe that th e
learning was fun in the sense that it was relativel y This work has stimulated some analysis of compute r
pleasant compared with other software learning environments and the sources of intrinsic reward tha t
experiences(!) . may (or may not be) exploited by them . Malone (1981 ,
1984) analyzed the attractiveness of educationa l
But even aside from tangling attributions, there ar e computer games for children, stressing the capacity of th e
plausible causal connections between ease and fun . If a games to promote an intrinsic motivation toward
system is more fun, it stands to reason that it will be used learning . Children playing these games are encouraged t o
more, which could have the effect of promoting mor e construe difficulty as challenge, unfamiliarity as fantasy ,
thorough learning, which in turn would make it easier t o and uncertainty as curiosity . Thus, instead of having t o
use at least in the long term . This scenario makes th e rely on an extrinsic incentive, like a teacher's praise, i n
problem begin to seem like asking whether the chicke n order to overcome difficulty, unfamiliarity, an d
caused the egg or the reverse . The difficulties in splittin g uncertainty, the child decides himself to take on
ease and fun are complemented by the psychological challenge and fantasy . This intrinsic, self-generate d
mechanism of "cognitive consistency" (Festinger, 1957), motivation is more powerful .
the tendency for people to rationalize all their belief s
into a simple and highly consistent framework . The drive Carroll (1982) analyzed adult navigational games lik e
for cognitive consistency encourages people t o Adventure . Games are typically divided into episodes o r
subjectively smear their impressions into monolithi c phases, and these in turn are clearly subdivided, givin g
categorizations . Thus, despite the details of whether a the user a sense of place in the game scenario . Within an
system is seen as fun because it is easy, seen as eas y episode, games are very responsive : when the user doe s
because it is fun, or even eventually experienced as eas y something, it has a visible consequence . Making progres s
because it was initially fun and therefore got used a lot , toward situational goals is also made obvious (e .g., by a
there is a tendency for people to generalize across detail s score) . These properties of spatiality, interactiveness, an d
and more simply classify the system as "good"—meanin g feedback are sources of intrinsic motivation and draw th e
easy, fun, and perhaps more besides . Indeed, the only user into the game . The object of a game is to succeed ,
way to avoid subjective smearing like this is to place both but games also typically encourage the attitude that it i s
systems side by side and force the contrasts out in a all right to fail . Moreover, the consequence of failing i s
usability experiment . cushioned : nothing too terrible happens . Both of thes e
properties encourage users to take risks they might, i n
HOW IMPORTANT IS FUN ? other situations, eschew . Finally, a game always project s
The potential for confusion between ease and fun is to the user some sense of control ; the person always ha s
serious . As we have argued, it is often difficult for peopl e substantial influence on the outcome . Clearly, some o f
to split the two in analyzing their own experience, yet it these techniques are not exploited by current softwar e
is necessary to do this if we are to understand software and systems . Users often have little sense of where the y
quality (and improve it) . To the extent that we confus e are in a system or task scenario or of whether they are
the two, we may be doing the wrong things to improv e making progress ; errors can be catastrophic (e .g. ,
software quality . This confusion could lead to the accidentally reformatting a hard disk) . But clearly als o
recommendation that the key to software quality is t o many such techniques could be exploited . For example,
make software as simple as possible . (Indeed, thi s presenting an interface that is initially simple but
conclusion is often voiced now in discussions of increases its complexity over time to challenge the user :
usability .) Yet, as we observed above, fun requires tha t success is within reach at each stage, but becomes mor e
things not be as simple as possible . Our view is that th e and more demanding. Interfaces that arouse imagery an d
issues are more subtle, that we do not necessarily want t o emotions via the use of metaphors (animated mess y
merely make things as simple as possible . We ought t o desks) can stimulate fantasy . Providing novel stimulatio n
make them fun . via the use of media (sound, graphics) or randomnes s
and humor can stimulate curiosity .
Experiences that are fun are more attractive to people of
course, but fun is also likely to have powerful influence s These ideas are tantalizing . There are, of course, alread y
on what people will even try to do and on how long they commercial systems that have exploited some of them .
will persist . In a classic experiment, Lepper, Green an d The metaphoric icons of the Lisa are manipulated b y
Nisbett (1973) studied a situation in which childre n pointing gestures and create a fantasy electronic deskto p
played with colored pencils . Half the children were for the user . The high resolution graphic presentatio n
simply allowed to play with the pencils, the other hal f helps create a sense of spatiality in which data an d
were rewarded for doing so . The effect of this operations are there . The simple mouse pointing devic e
manipulation was that the unrewarded children played enhances the interactiveness of the system and the sens e
with the pencils longer than those who had bee n of control the user feels. But despite a growing numbe r
rewarded . Lepper, Green and Nisbett suggested that the of such examples, it is still more striking that there is a

SIGCHI Bulletin 22 January 1988 Volume 19 Number 3


paucity of work on fun in user interfaces . This is worries about uncertainty (for example, many game s
unfortunate because fun is not only important in itsel f allow the player to save the game state before tryin g
(providing intrinsic reward to users) but, as we hav e something risky—the saved state can be recalled if al l
suggested, it is frequently tangled up with ease in people's does not go well) . However, people differ on the degre e
thinking about usability, and therefore can be a double - of risk they are willing to take ; tasks differ in terms o f
whammy impact on. buying decisions in computing . how much risk is associated with failure . Introducing
Perhaps worst of all, the general lack of attention to fun , uncertainty into a flight simulator game probabl y
and to motivational issues generally, is more clearly i n makes it more enjoyable . Introducing uncertainty into a
evidence in the research world than in the developmen t text editor or spreadsheet may or may not make it mor e
world ! enjoyable . What are the underlying differences here an d
from what do they arise?
So why has fun not been studied more thoroughly? On e
important reason is that it is difficult to empirically Fantasy is an aspect of games that also raises question s
measure . Ease can be measured in terms of th e about real work systems . Carroll and Thomas (1982 )
performance time, time to learn, inventories and count s suggested that logically equivalent tasks may b e
of errors . How do you measure fun? One interface migh t represented in various forms, thus helping to make
indeed be more fun than another but still not make user s routine tasks more intrinsically interesting . The y
laugh out loud . Another reason for the shortage of wor k imagined a situation in which a factory control jo b
on fun lies in the politics of research . Clearly, one can see could be presented to the worker as a variety of virtual
researchers could be somewhat inhibited about studyin g tasks (e .g . piloting an airplane) . The worker could b e
fun . Would a computer scientist who tries to build a performing the same information processing tasks, bu t
professional career studying fun be taken seriously? under different metaphoric cover stories . This strategy
could address the boredom and vigilance problems tha t
WE NEED TO STUDY FU N often inhere in very routine tasks . But it clearly raises
To this point, we have suggested that there is a commo n other issues too : Would virtual tasks, however well -
confusion in discussions of usability that conflates ease intentioned, seem exploitive to workers? Would the y
and fun . We have argued that this confusion is importan t ultimately be distractions that just get in the way ?
because designers will have to do different things t o
achieve these different ends . Finally, we have argued that Curiosity also might be exploited . Reading narrativ e
fun is potentially very important in its own right . Fro m texts, like novels, arouses curiosity by presentin g
this, we reason that there is an important need to develo p material embedded in a coherent thematic structure .
a research program in fun and motivation . We have littl e This structure causes readers to pose questions t o
to go on . The only extant empirical work, that o f themselves, and then later answers these questions .
Malone, focused on children playing educational games . Other textual structures stimulate curiosity far less—fo r
There may be quite a leap from there to typical compute r example, the parts list structure of reference
applications (accountants learning to use spreadsheets) . documentation . The tradition of presenting softwar e
How relevant that work will really turn out to be is a n and system documentation in businesslike, formal and
open question. dry prose is perhaps missing an opportunity to motivate
users to pay attention . Perhaps a narrative story abou t
Indeed, Malone (1981) himself raised the point tha t a scenario of use would be more intrinsically motivatin g
interacting with a game could be fundamentall y than a list of key functions and their syntax . Perhap s
different from using a computer tool to complete a this would lead to better learning and performance .
practical task . Sources of intrinsic motivation in game s
might not have the same effectiveness whe n We realize that many people will read this article as a
implemented in computer tools . There surely are many sort of joke . To this extent, we are the victims of ou r
differences between playing games and doing work , own analysis : there are risks in being serious about fun .
differences in the outcomes we and others expect , Still though, we continue to see, without humor, th e
differences in how intimately we identify ourselves wit h prospect of a decade of research analysis of usabilit y
the activity, differences in the situationa l possibly failing to provide the leverage it could o n
appropriateness of engaging in the activity . Currently , designing systems people will really want to use b y
we have some understanding of fun based on games, ignoring what could be a very potent determinant o f
but we have no understanding of how people feel abou t subjective judgments of usability—fun .
or relate to tools they use . We do not even have a
framework for posing such questions .
ACKNOWLEDGMENT S
Consider the question of uncertainty and risk . Carroll We are grateful to Amy Aaronson, Rick Herder an d
(1982) suggested that games are motivating in par t Jeff Kelley for commenting on earlier versions of thi s
because they project to the user the presumption that article .
being uncertain is all right, that risk taking i s
appropriate and necessary . He suggested that trainin g BIBLIOGRAPH Y
environments for computer software might be mor e Carroll, J .M . 1982 . The adventure of getting to know a
effective if they also encouraged risk taking and eased computer . IEEE Computer, 15/11, 49-58 .

SIGCHI Bulletin 23 January 1988 Volume 19 Number 3


Carroll, J .M . and Mazur, S .A . 1986 . LisaLearning . rewards : A test of the overjustification hypothesis .
IEEE Computer, 19/ 11, 35-49 . Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 28 ,
129-137 .
Carroll, J .M . and Thomas, J .C . 1982 . Metaphor an d
the cognitive representation of computing systems . Malone, T . W . 1981 . Toward a theory of intrinsicall y
IEEE Transactions on Systems, Man, and Cybernetics, motivating instruction . Cognitive Science, 4, 333-368 .
12, 107-116 .
Malone, T . W . 1984 . Heuristics for designing enjoyable
Festinger, L . 1957. A Theory of Cognitive Dissonance . user interfaces : Lessons from computer games . In J .
Evanston, IL : Row, Peterson . Thomas and M . Schneider, Human Factors in
Computer Systems, Norwood, N .J . : Ablex .
Lepper, M .R., Green, D ., and Nisbett, R .E . 1973 .
Undermining children's intrinsic interest with extrinsic

CHI Conference Proceedings CHI Conference Proceedings - hardcover editions, for


For credit card orders, call toll-free 1/800/342-6626 . All other sale outside the USA/Canada only:
orders : 301/528-4261 . Send mail orders with payment to : Elsevier Science Publishers, Book Order Dept ., P. O . Box
ACM Order Departmen t 211, 1000 AE Amsterdam, The Netherlands .
P .O . Box 64145
Baltimore, MD 2126 4 Human Factors in Computing Systems, Proceedings
of the CHI'83 Conference, Boston, December 12-15 . 296
CHI'83 Human Factors in Computing Systems , pages . Price : Dfl . 115 .00 .
Boston, December 12-15, 296 pages ,
58 papers . Hardcover edition . Human Factors in Computing Systems - I L
Order No. 60883 0 Proceedings of the CHI'85 Conference, San Francisco, Apri l
ACM/SIGCHI members : $55.00 14-18 . 232 pages . Price : Dfl .125 .00.
Others : $60.00
Human Factors in Computing Systems - III .
CHI'85 Human Factors in Computing Systems , Proceedings of the CH I'86 Conference, Boston, April 13-17 .
San Francisco, April 14-18 . 231 pages, 38 papers . 361 pages .
Order No . 60885 0
ACM/SIGCHI members: $15 .00 Human Factors in Computing Systems and
Others : $20 .00 Graphics Interface - IV . Proceedings of the CHI + G I
87 Conference, Toronto, April 5-9, 1987 .
CHI'86 Human Factors in Computing Systems ,
Boston, April 13-17 . 361 pages, 58 papers and panels .
Order No . 608860
ACM/SIGCHI members : $20 .00
Others: $27 .0 0

CHI + GI 87 Human Factors in Computin g


Systems and Graphics Interface, Toronto, April 5-9 ,
1987 . 344 pages, 58 papers and panels .
Order No . 60887 0
ACM/SIGCHI members: $20.00
Others : $27 .00

SIGCHI Bulletin 24 January 1988 Volume 19 Number 3

View publication stats

You might also like