Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 19

278 M. Rutter and L. A.

Sroufe

far outweighed the similarities and that also it noff, 1996) might also provide part of the
was probably highly misleading to consider cognitive basis for autism. Doubts have been
autism as a psychosis that involved some raised about both (Griffith, Pennington,
break with a previous normality (Rutter, Wehner, & Rogers, 1999; Plaisted, Swetten-
1972). Research by Hermelin and O’Connor ham, & Rees, 1999) but they warrant further
(1970) and their colleagues was the first to study.
show the value of an experimental psycholog- A second area of autism research where a de-
ical approach that sought to examine the cog- velopmental psychopathology perspective has
nitive features in greater detail and to link been useful concerns the delineation of the di-
them with the social deficits. Nevertheless, agnostic boundaries. Genetic research has been
there were considerable difficulties in know- important in indicating that the phenotype, as
ing just how the cognitive deficits might lead defined by genetic liability evident from twin
to the particular social features that are diag- and family studies, extends far beyond the tra-
nostic of autism (Rutter, 1983). ditional boundaries of the diagnosis of autism
A major step forward was provided by the as a major handicapping condition (Bailey,
application of “theory of mind” concepts, Palferman, Heavey, & Le Couteur, 1998; Rut-
originally derived from studies of normal chil- ter, 2000a; Rutter, Maughan, et al., 1997).
dren (Baron–Cohen, Leslie, & Frith, 1985). This broader phenotype, or lesser variant, of
Subsequent research has confirmed that most autism may occur in as many as one in five of
autistic individuals lack the mentalizing skills the first-degree relatives of individuals with
to know what another person is likely to be autism, and the possibility that autistic fea-
thinking, given knowledge of the social con- tures might extend even further into the gen-
text or past happenings (Frith, 1989; Baron– eral population, although not considered plau-
Cohen, 1995; Happé, 1994). The detailed sible a few years ago, cannot now be ruled
findings provide good grounds for the infer- out entirely.
ence that this cognitive deficit plays a major One further feature to mention, with re-
role in autistic individuals’ severe limitations spect to recent autism research, concerns the
in the development of reciprocal relation- finding that atypical varieties of autism may
ships. One difficulty, however, has been the derive from congenital blindness (Brown,
fact that autism is manifest before age 2 years Hobson, & Lee, 1997) and profound early so-
in most instances, well before normal children cial and nutritional deprivation (Rutter, An-
show theory of mind skills when tested in the dersen–Wood, Beckett, Bredenkamp, Castle,
usual way. In part, this is probably a conse- Groothues, Kreppner, Keaveney, Lord,
quence of the fact that the particular experi- O’Connor, & the ERA Study Team, 1999).
mental situation used does not pick up theory The clinical pictures associated with these un-
of mind skills that are demonstrable in more usual risk factors seem somewhat different
naturalistic circumstances (Dunn, 1988, 1996). from “ordinary” autism, and it is doubtful
In addition, the focus shifted to earlier emerg- whether they have much direct relevance for
ing cognitive skills, such as joint attention, the causation of autism arising in more ordi-
that might constitute precursors of theory of nary circumstances. Nevertheless, the findings
mind or an alternative cognitive route to the do raise important questions about the hetero-
social dysfunction (Sigman & Ruskin, 1999). geneity of autism and the possibly different
Although theory of mind deficits constitute a routes by which it may arise.
plausible explanation for social dysfunction, it
is not so obvious that they could account for
Depressive disorders
the repetitive stereotyped behaviors that are
also characteristic of autism. For that and Up until about a generation ago, depressive
other reasons, research has sought to deter- disorders were rarely diagnosed in childhood
mine whether a lack of central coherence because psychoanalytic theory claimed that
(Happé, 1996) or deficits in particular aspects they could not occur in the young (see Rutter,
of executive planning (Pennington & Ozo- 1986b). The situation changed as people be-

"  """     !      !


Developmental psychopathology: Concepts and challenges 279

came more sceptical about the unvalidated tween depressive disorders arising for the first
claims of psychoanalytical theory, and as em- time in childhood and those arising in adoles-
pirical observations, and especially the use of cence or later (Duggal, Carlson, Sroufe, &
standardized interview methods, made it clear Egeland, in press).
that depressive phenomena could and did oc- One of the striking features of depressive
cur in children. The pendulum has now rather disorders is that they become much more fre-
swung to the opposite extreme, depressive quent at some point during the adolescent age
disorders being diagnosed with a high fre- period and that this increase in frequency is
quency (Lewinsohn, Rohde, & Seeley, 1998). much stronger in females than males (Silberg,
Indeed, there have even been claims that bipo- Pickles, Rutter, Hewitt, Simonoff, Maes, Car-
lar disorders are extremely common in high- bonneau, Murrelle, Foley, & Eaves, 1999).
risk groups (see Biederman, Faraone, Mil- Clearly, a developmental perspective is likely
berger, Guite, Meck, Chen, Mennin, Marrs, to be useful in investigating why depression
Ouellette, Moore, Spencer, Norman, Wilens, becomes so much more common during the
Kraus, & Perrin, 1996). A key problem is teenage years and why the sex ratio alters
that, at one extreme, feelings of depression from, roughly, parity in childhood to a
are simply a normal part of the human condi- marked female preponderance in adulthood
tion but that, at the other extreme, they consti- (Peterson, 1993). Again, the evidence is thin
tute a serious psychosis. A key issue, from a on the ground, but findings from the Virginia
developmental perspective, is whether or not twin study suggest that genetic influences re-
the depressive disorders arising in early life lated to both life events and depression play a
are the same as those occurring in adulthood. significant role (Silberg et al., 1999).
Several findings are crucial in drawing Research from many research groups but
conclusions. First, longitudinal studies have especially that from Brown and Harris (1978)
made it clear that children who suffer from a has shown the high frequency with which the
depressive disorder have a greatly increased onset of depressive disorder in adult life has
risk of a recurrence of depressive disorders in been preceded by a stressful life event that
adult life (Harrington, Fudge, Rutter, Pick- carries long-term threat. On the other hand, it
les, & Hill, 1990; Weissman, Wolk, Gold- is obvious that many people have life events
stein, Moreau, Adams, Greenwald, Klier, without developing depression, and it has
Ryan, Dahl, & Wickramaratne, 1999). How- been necessary to consider why there are such
ever, it is also relevant that very few of the marked individual differences in response to
recurrences in adult life take the form of a apparently similar circumstances. Three fea-
bipolar disorder. That has been true even tures have seemed influential. First, many
when samples have consisted of severe de- studies have shown that adverse experiences
pressive disorders in childhood that have led in childhood are associated with an increased
to hospital admission (Weissman et al., 1999). liability to depressive disorder in adult life
Second, there is some evidence from ques- (Harris, Brown, & Bifulco, 1990). Second, the
tionnaire studies that depressive phenomena personal meaning of the event seems to influ-
(not necessarily disorders) show a lower ge- ence its impact (Brown & Harris, 1978,
netic component in childhood than they do in 1989). Third, there is evidence that genetic
adolescence or adult life (Thapar & McGuf- factors may play a role in susceptibility to life
fin, 1994). Early-onset depressive disorders stressors (Kendler, Kessler, Walters, Mac-
may also be more likely to be associated with Lean, Neale, Heath, Corey, & Eaves, 1995;
antisocial problems in the individual and in Silberg et al., 2000).
the family (Harrington, Rutter, Weissman, For a long time, it has been implicitly as-
Fudge, Groothues, Bredenkamp, Pickles, sumed that all depressive disorders can be
Rende, & Wickramaratne, 1997), although treated as equivalent (apart from, perhaps, bi-
this finding requires replication. The data are polar and severe melancholic varieties). How-
too sparse for firm conclusions, but it seems ever, recent findings have raised queries as to
that there may be important differences be- whether the factors involved in the onset of

"  """     !      !


280 M. Rutter and L. A. Sroufe

the first episode of the disorder may not be investigated features of gender differences
the same as those involved in later recur- and of age changes in frequency of depressive
rences (Brown, Harris, & Hepworth, 1994). disorder, including features that maintain on
The issue is an important one, if only because or deflect individuals from a depression
most depressive disorders are recurrent (Judd, pathway.
1997). The evidence suggests that genetic fac-
tors may be more important in the lifetime lia-
Schizophrenia
bility to depression than in the onset of the
first episode (Kendler et al., 1995), and that Like autism, schizophrenia was long consid-
depressive-style attributions may also play a ered as a severe disorder that provided a
role in the persistence or recurrence of depres- sharp, qualitative departure from normality. It
sion (Teasdale & Barnard, 1993). Psychiatric was not obvious that a developmental per-
classifications have tended to draw sharp dis- spective would be useful because it was so
tinctions between depressive disorders on the uncommon for schizophrenic psychoses to be-
one hand and anxiety disorders on the other, gin in childhood.
despite the fact that, at an individual level, Nevertheless, it has become increasingly
there is very considerable overlap. Twin-study appreciated that a developmental perspective
findings indicate that both anxiety and depres- is needed. (See Cicchetti & Cannon, 1999, for
sive disorders probably reflect the same ge- an overview of the issues involved in relating
netic liability and that this may well reside, neurodevelopment to psychopathology.) Of-
to a considerable extent, in the temperamental ford and Cross (1969) pointed to the evidence
feature of neuroticism (Eley & Stevenson, that about half of schizophrenic psychoses
1999; Kendler, 1996). with an onset in adult life had been preceded
Although there is good reason for conclud- by substantial nonpsychotic abnormalities in
ing that adverse life experiences play a sub- childhood. In the 40-odd years since then, the
stantial role in the genesis of depressive disor- evidence that it is so has steadily mounted,
ders (see Rutter, in press-c), a caveat is with the impact from prospective longitudinal
necessary. Very little of the research is longi- studies of the general population being crucial
tudinal, and because, in adults, the life experi- (Done, Johnstone, Frith, Golding, Shep-
ences and the depressive symptomatology are herd, & Crow, 1991; Jones, Rodgers, Mur-
reported by the same informant, some of the ray, & Marmot, 1994). It is not just that social
association may reflect reporting bias. Also, and behavioral abnormalities may be evident
few of the studies have used genetically sensi- in childhood (although that is the case) but
tive designs, and it is relevant that genetic fac- also that there are developmental delays in a
tors play a role in the individual differences in substantial proportion of individuals who later
the likelihood of experiencing and of reacting develop schizophrenia. A neurodevelopmental
adversely to stress events, and that the genetic concept of schizophrenia has come to the fore
factors may also overlap with those that un- (Cannon & Murray, 1998; Harrison, 1997;
derlie depressive disorders (Kendler & Kar- Murray & Woodruff, 1995). At first, it was
kowski–Shuman, 1997; Thapar et al., 1998). thought that the neurodevelopmental risks
An immense amount has been learned were specific to schizophrenia. Recent find-
from the study of depressive disorders in ings have begun to cast doubt on whether that
adults. There is a growing body of research is, strictly speaking, so. It seems that rather
findings on comparable disorders occurring in similar risks may apply in the case of affec-
young people. Nevertheless, the adoption of a tive psychoses, although the findings are
developmental perspective has raised a num- stronger in the case of schizophrenia (Jones,
ber of important queries that have yet to be 1997). Genetic findings, like those in autism,
fully resolved. There is a need for more longi- have shown that the genetic liability extends
tudinal research that is explicitly focused on beyond schizophrenic psychoses to include
understanding the details of risk and protec- schizotypal and paranoid disorders (Kendler
tive mechanisms and that focuses on the less et al., 1995). The evidence is still in favor of

"  """     !      !


Developmental psychopathology: Concepts and challenges 281

a substantial degree of diagnostic specificity, ment at that early age. On the other hand,
but it is necessary to extend the concept of early brain injuries are perhaps more likely
schizophrenia beyond psychoses, to include than later ones to result in global intellectual
some forms of personality disorder. Precisely impairment (Vharga–Khadem et al., 1992).
how and where to draw the boundaries is, Also, adverse intellectual sequelae are more
however, still unclear. likely if the injury is associated with epi-
There is good evidence that genetic factors lepsy—suggesting that active brain dysfunc-
play a substantial role in schizophrenia (Mol- tion, rather than loss of function, may be criti-
din & Gottesman, 1997), and the notion that cal. The results of research are clear cut in
it is a psychogenic disorder has largely been indicating marked age-dependent variations in
abandoned (with good reason). Nevertheless, response to brain injury, but the differences
one of the most striking findings in recent lie more in the pattern of sequelae than in
years has been the evidence that schizophre- whether or not cognitive impairment occurs.
nia has a much increased rate among people Until recently, the available evidence has
of West Indian origin living in the United given no clear indication of gene–environ-
Kingdom. The rate is higher than that in the ment interactions in relation to intellectual de-
West Indies and is higher than in other ethnic velopment, but new findings have suggested
groups within the United Kingdom (McKen- that genetic influences are less when children
zie & Murray, 1999). Careful analysis of pu- are reared in less advantageous environments
tative causal influences indicates that some (Rowe, Jacobson, & van den Oord, 1999).
form of psychosocial stress or adversity that The possible importance of nature–nurture in-
impinges differentially on people of West In- terplay needs to be reconsidered.
dian background in the United Kingdom is In most psychological theorizing about in-
likely to be responsible. Whether or not this tellectual development, dietary factors have
form of schizophreniform disorder, in which played a negligible role. It has, therefore, been
psychosocial factors appear relatively strong, quite surprising that well-conducted research,
is different from other forms of schizophrenia both naturalistic and experimental, comparing
remains to be established. the effects of natural breast milk and formula
milk on the later intellectual development of
prematurely born children has shown substan-
Intellectual development
tial benefits of breast milk (Lucas, Morley, &
It used to be said that if you were going to Cole, 1998). It has been argued that some
have a head injury it would be better to have form of developmental programming may un-
it early because the effects were so much derlie the finding. The research is sound and
less—the so-called Kennard principle (1942). well designed, but, as with all forms of re-
Research evidence has shown that this is a search, independent replication with other re-
somewhat misleading oversimplification search groups is required before too much is
(Rutter, 1982, 1993). The age of the individ- built into this finding, however compelling
ual at the time of brain injury clearly is a most the evidence from one set of investigations.
important factor, but its effects are somewhat Moreover, the effect may not apply to full-
more complicated than the original principle term babies (Richards, Wadsworth, Rahimi–
suggested. Contrasted with the situation in Foroushani, Hardy, Kuh, & Paul, 1998).
adults, lateralization effects following brain Numerous studies over the years have
injuries in infancy are negligible. That is to shown that IQ scores in the first few years
say, unlike the situation in adults, left-brain of life are of negligible value for intellectual
injuries do not result in specific verbal defi- functioning in adolescence and adulthood.
cits, and right-brain injuries do not lead to cir- The findings had seemed to suggest a sharp
cumscribed visuospatial deficits. It seems that discontinuity between earlier and later intel-
either there is a transfer of functions across lectual functioning. There were many reasons
the hemisphere when one side is damaged, or why there should be a degree of discontinuity,
there is greater plasticity in brain develop- particularly because language skills become

"  """     !      !


282 M. Rutter and L. A. Sroufe

increasingly important in cognitive perfor- that are available for the assessment of psy-
mance during childhood. Also, animal studies chopathology in childhood and adolescence,
had shown that with maturation, different as well as in adult life (Shaffer, Lucas, &
parts of the brain may come to be responsible Richters, 1999). A rich assortment of ques-
for the same cognitive functions (Goldman– tionnaire, interview, observational, and psy-
Rakic, Issernoff, Schwartz, & Bugbee, 1983). chometric measures have been developed and
Nevertheless, it had seemed surprising that al- for many of them there is good evidence of
most no continuity was evident. About 20 their reliability and validity. It might well be
years ago, researchers began to adopt differ- thought that we have got as far as we can in
ent strategies for studying cognitive function- the field of measurement, but there are sub-
ing in infancy, recognizing that different skills stantial unresolved problems. In many re-
might be needed at this early age. The result spects, the most pervasive, and perhaps the
was the development of methods for attention most surprising, finding has been the poor
and habituation. These showed substantial in- agreement between parents, teachers, and
dividual differences that, contrary to findings children on almost all aspects of psychopath-
with developmental milestones of a traditional ology, and also of family experiences (Achen-
kind, did show modest continuities with intel- bach, McConaughy, & Howell, 1987;
lectual performance as assessed in the usual Elander & Rutter, 1996; Simonoff, Pickles,
ways in middle childhood (Bornstein & Sig- Meyer, Silberg, Maes, Loeber, Rutter, Hew-
man, 1986; Colombo, 1993; McCall & Car- itt, & Eaves, 1997).
riger, 1993). Many questions remain, and it There are many reasons why this might be
has not proved easy to define tests that are so. Thus, for example, children in rating their
applicable across more than quite narrow age own behavior may focus particularly on the
periods. Even so, the findings have been im- contrast between how they are feeling now (or
portant in showing that there are continuities what they are doing now) as compared with
as well as discontinuities in intellectual how things were some time ago. Parents, on
achievement and that the continuities are het- the other hand, are more likely to compare
erotypic. That is to say, although it seems children with their siblings or with other chil-
likely that the underlying function remains dren whom they know. Teachers, too, will
broadly the same over development, its sur- make comparisons with other children but
face manifestations differ markedly. will have a much larger sample of comparison
available to them. Inevitably, too, parents will
focus more on what they have perceived in
Looking Forward Into the 21st Century
the context of the home, whereas teachers will
As illustrated by this selective review of some do so in the context of the school, the situa-
key findings, considerable progress has been tional demands of the two settings being
made in understanding many of the features rather different.
presented by the domain of development psy- Evidence has accumulated that, with some
chopathology. Even so, it is equally apparent behaviors such as hyperactivity, parents may
that major research challenges remain. In this exaggerate the contrasts between siblings,
final section of the paper, we outline a few of leading to an artifactually low correlation be-
these key challenges. We make no attempt at tween ratings of the behavior of the twins or
being exhaustive in our coverage, but we do siblings (Simonoff, Pickles, Hervas, Silberg,
seek to highlight issues that have broad impli- Rutter, & Eaves, 1998). But the problem goes
cations beyond the specifics of a particular more broadly than that. It is not just that the
trait or disorder. agreement between informants is modest but
also that the correlates tend to be rather differ-
ent. Thus, for example, genetic influences on
Measurement issues
behavior as rated by children tend to be lower
The last few decades have seen major im- than as rated by parents (Hewitt, Silberg, Rut-
provements in the methods of measurement ter, Simonoff, Meyer, Maes, Pickles, Neale,

"  """     !      !


Developmental psychopathology: Concepts and challenges 283

Loeber, Erickson, Kendler, Heath, Truett, are being found as risk correlates of one disor-
Reynolds, & Eaves, 1997; Eaves, Silberg, der may, in fact, be the risk factors for some
Meyer, Maes, Simonoff, Pickles, Rutter, other form of psychopathology with which it
Neale, Reynolds, Erickson, Heath, Loeber, happens to be associated), and because, if
Truett, & Hewitt, 1997; Eley & Stephenson, properly handled, the investigation of the
1999). Interest has also grown with respect to mechanisms underlying comorbidity may be
the ways in which ratings may be biased by highly informative on risk and protective fac-
the current mental state of the person making tors more generally.
the rating (such as the effects of parental de-
pression on the parental ratings Boyle & Pick-
Gender differences
les, 1997; Brewin, Andrews, & Gotlib, 1993).
The findings of such comparisons have been The existence of major gender differences in
informative, but it has to be said that the rates of particular forms of psychopathology
mechanisms involved in the similarities and has been well recognized for many years
differences among informants remain very (Earls, 1987; Eme, 1979, 1992), although
poorly understood. Their elucidation requires their meaning has been ill understood (Rutter,
systematic epidemiological–longitudinal stud- 1970; Gualteri & Hicks, 1985; Taylor & Rut-
ies focused on testing specific hypotheses ter, 1986). Thus, autism, attention-deficit hy-
about possible explanations. perkinetic disorder, developmental language
disorders, and delinquency all show a marked
male preponderance, whereas adolescence-on-
Comorbidity
set eating disorders (such as anorexia and bu-
After years of neglect, the issue of comorbid- limia nervosa) and depressive disorders in the
ity has become the “flavor of the moment,” years after childhood are both substantially
with the literature full of papers on its occur- more frequent in females. In all cases, it is
rence and patterning. Unfortunately, many of necessary to check that the gender differences
the studies are not as informative as they are not artifactual. For example, Zoccolillo
might be because the research has not been (1993) has suggested that conduct disorders
planned in a way that provides an adequate in girls are much more frequent than usually
test of competing mechanisms. The fact that appreciated but that the diagnostic threshold
supposedly separate forms of psychopathol- for a number of symptoms needs to be differ-
ogy frequently co-occur in the same individ- ent for girls than for boys. Likewise, Crick
ual has been well demonstrated in numerous (1996) has argued that much aggression in
epidemiological studies (Angold, Costello, & girls has been overlooked because it is of a
Erkanli, 1999; Caron & Rutter, 1991). The different form from that in boys. As both No-
key question is what the co-occurrence len–Hoeksema (1987) and Moffitt et al.
means; it is likely that some, perhaps much, (2000) have sought to demonstrate, it is possi-
is artifactual, deriving from mistaken assump- ble to postulate a range of different explana-
tions in diagnostic concepts and boundaries. tions and to undertake appropriate research
Alternatively, both may derive from the same tests to choose between competing hypothe-
set of intercorrelated risk factors. Or, yet ses. Clearly, there is a need to understand the
again, the presence of one form of psycho- processes underlying these quite marked gen-
pathology may, through its effects, constitute der differences. We do not even know
a risk mechanism for another form of psycho- whether the causes of gender differences are
pathology. There are numerous examples in the same across different disorders or whether
internal medicine of all of these (Rutter, quite different risk processes are operating.
1997a), and they provide useful models for On the face of it, it is not obvious that the
research strategies. It is crucially important to explanation for the male excess in delin-
determine the meanings of comorbidity, both quency and in autism would be the same.
because its presence is likely to confuse and However, it would be premature to entirely
distort the meaning of findings (because what reject the possibility of a unifying explana-

"  """     !      !


284 M. Rutter and L. A. Sroufe

tion. The point is that this major phenomenon, to obtain a better understanding of how they
gender differences in rates of specific psycho- are involved in the risk processes. For exam-
pathology, requires systematic study, and this ple, the evidence suggests that a depresso-
is likely to involve somewhat different re- genic attributional style may be more impor-
search strategies to those ordinarily employed. tant in the persistence or recurrence of
That is because the topic being investigated depression than in its original onset (Teas-
concerns differences between groups in level, dale & Barnard, 1993). In addition, there is an
rather than individual differences within a sin- equally pressing need to understand the con-
gle population. nections between brain and mind. The devel-
opment of functional brain-imaging tech-
niques, as represented at first by positron
Cognitive processing
emission tomography (PET scans) (Bremner,
The study of cognitive processing has consti- Innis, Ng, Staib, Salomon, Bronen, Duncan,
tuted one of the major growth areas in psy- Southwick, Krystal, Rich, Zubal, Dey,
chology over the last few decades. Two some- Soufer, & Charney, 1997), and more recently
what different phenomena seem to be by magnetic resonance imagery (MRI scans)
important in relation to psychopathology provide one possible way forward (Rugg,
(Rutter, 1987). On the one hand, there are dis- 1997; Fletcher, Happé, Frith, Baker, Dolan,
orders, such as autism, where the underlying Frackowiak, & Frith, 1995; Schultz, Gauthier,
problem seems to lie in the presence of cogni- Klin, Fulbright, Anderson, Volkmar, Skudlar-
tive deficits for particular kinds of mental pro- ski, Lacadie, Cohen, & Gore, 2000). In addi-
cesses. Similar arguments have been put in re- tion, however, studies of experiential effects
lation to schizophrenia (Frith, 1992) and on cognitive styles, and on their changing
antisocial behavior (Moffitt, 1993). On the manifestations with age, are needed.
other hand, some disorders seem to be charac- Sometimes functional imaging is “sold” as
terized by unusual styles of cognitive process- a means of seeing the brain in action, and of
ing, rather than deficits as such. That seems identifying the parts of the brain that are mal-
to apply, for example, to affective disorders functioning in the case of particular forms of
and their associated depressogenic attributions psychopathology. That is, however, a some-
(Teasdale & Barnard, 1993) and antisocial be- what misleading way of putting things. What
havior with its associated tendency to make the scans show is the part of the brain where
hostile attributions with respect to other peo- the main metabolic activity, as reflected in
ple’s actions (Dodge & Schwartz, 1997; blood flow, is taking part during particular
Coie & Dodge, 1997). In some cases, too, it cognitive tasks. That provides a most valuable
remains uncertain which type of cognitive means of differentiating among supposedly
feature applies. For example, attention deficits separate cognitive functions, and it should
have long been considered as fundamental in also be possible to determine whether the
hyperactivity disorders. However, despite ear- same brain areas are used to solve specific
lier views to the contrary, the evidence now cognitive tasks by normal individuals and by
suggests that the basis does not lie in any de- individuals with psychopathological disor-
fect in selective attention (Schachar, 1991). ders. There is no doubt that this linking of
There are associated cognitive impairments, brain function with the workings of the mind
although it is uncertain how far they show is going to increase our understanding of cog-
specificity, but also there seem to be more nitive and affective processing, but on its own
stylistic problems in terms of impulsivity and it will not identify brain pathology. It is im-
a difficulty in delaying gratification (Sonuga– portant to appreciate that changes in behavior
Barke, Taylor, Sembi, & Smith, 1992; Scha- brought about by psychological treatments
char, Tannock, Marriott, & Logan, 1995). In may also alter brain functioning as shown on
each of these cases, there is still a need to scans, as illustrated by the case of obsessive–
determine the degree of diagnostic specificity compulsive disorder (Baxter, Schwartz, Berg-
of the cognitive features, and there is a need man, Szuba, Guze, Mazziotla, Alazraki, Selin,

"  """     !      !


Developmental psychopathology: Concepts and challenges 285

Ferug, Munford, & Phelps, 1992). Neverthe- ations in affect regulation, and changes in
less, this is going to constitute one of the cognitive set or styles of cognitive processing.
highly important technologies that will help As discussed more fully elsewhere (Rutter
us understand brain–mind interrelationships. et al., 2000), there is also a major need to pro-
At present, there are few such studies in de- vide a much better understanding of environ-
velopmental journals, but we predict that the mental risk mechanisms. We know a good
situation is likely to change radically during deal about what are key risk indicators, but
the next decade. we know much less about the environmental
risk processes that they reflect. Research in
the past has made clear that it is easy to mis-
Nature–nurture interplay
take risk indicators and risk mechanisms, and
As already noted, with respect to the past, the that the theoretical policy implications are
study of the details of nature–nature interplay quite different according to which is which.
are likely to constitute another major growth For example, in relation to both childhood
area during the years ahead. The importance (Rutter, 1971b; Ferguson, Horwood, & Lyn-
of personal effects on the environment, and of skey, 1992) and adult life (Harris, Brown, &
gene–environment correlations and interac- Bifulco, 1986), it has become evident that pa-
tions, has been well demonstrated, but we still rental loss is not usually a major risk mecha-
lack adequate understanding of the role of nism. It is, however, a risk indicator because
these mechanisms in the emergence of psy- it tends to be associated with deficits in par-
chopathology. Thus, almost all of the research enting that do constitute something closer to
into person effects on the environment have a risk mechanism. Research in the field of di-
concerned very short-term effects (the study vorce and remarriage has also brought out the
by Maccoby & Jacklin, 1983, constituting one important point that it should not be consid-
of the few exceptions). Findings in the field ered as a time-limited event but rather as a
of antisocial behavior suggest that these may process in which the family interactions be-
be particularly important during the preschool fore and after the marriage breakup are as im-
years, and that the effects may be quite long portant as the breakup itself (Block, Block, &
lasting (O’Connor et al., 1998), but very few Gjerde, 1986; Cherlin, Furstenberg, Chase–
relevant data are available. The topic is a most Lansdale, Kiernan, Robins, Morrison, &
important one because it necessarily changes Teitler, 1991). Studies have begun to address
the way in which both genetic and environ- the possible mediating factors that are in-
mental risk mechanisms have to be thought volved in these risk processes operating over
about. If genetically influenced susceptibili- time (O’Connor, Thorpe, Dunn, Golding, &
ties operate indirectly, rather than directly, it the ALSPAC Study Team, 1999b), and more
may be that the search needs to be not for the research of this kind is clearly indicated
susceptibility genes for depressive disorder (Gottlieb, 1997).
but rather the susceptibility genes for reactiv-
ity to environmental stressors. Similarly, if
Heterotypic continuity
environmental effects are mainly evident in
genetically vulnerable individuals, we need to Several examples of heterotypic continuity
understand how individual characteristics have already been given. The social deficits
play a role in people’s responses to the envi- in adult life associated with severe develop-
ronment. mental disorders of receptive language consti-
It is also important that we determine what tute another (Howlin, Mawhood, & Rutter, in
it is that environmental effects do to the or- press). It is important to appreciate, however,
ganism that enables the sequelae to be carried that to establish heterotypic continuity, it is
forward to a later age. As already noted, not enough to show that one behavior follows
mechanisms to be considered are heteroge- another during development. Such consecu-
neous, spanning changes in brain structure, tive occurrence could occur for a whole vari-
changes in neuroendocrine function, alter- ety of reasons. Rather, it is crucial to show

"  """     !      !


286 M. Rutter and L. A. Sroufe

that the different behaviors thought to repre- Bishop, Eley, Oliver, Price, Purcell, Steven-
sent heterotypic continuity have comparable son, & Plomin, 1998). So far, the findings,
correlates. Very little research of that kind has from a twin study, apply only to 2 years of
been undertaken. Genetic research strategies age (which is rather too early to determine
would be informative. Thus, for example, is language disorders), and the sample was bi-
there the same genetic liability for the mea- ased in certain key respects. Nevertheless, the
sures of attention and habituation in infancy findings are provocative in showing that ge-
and for the measures of general intelligence netic influences for the extremes were much
in later childhood and adolescence? Similar stronger than for variations within the normal
questions arise with respect to the neurodevel- range. The comparison of so-called individual
opmental precursors of schizophrenia. Do heritability and group heritability (the latter
they constitute a diagnostically nonspecific referring to the difference between the ex-
risk factor or are they indexing a specific ge- tremes and the normal variation) provides a
netic liability to schizophrenia? Again, geneti- means of determining whether the differences
cally sensitive designs would provide an an- are statistically significant (DeFries & Fulker,
swer. Of course, the correlates to be examined 1985). So far, there has been very little sys-
should include psychosocial risk factors as tematic research testing for continuities and
well as genetic ones. Thus, with respect to the discontinuities across the range of behavioral
links between a lack of early selective attach- variation, and there are substantial practical
ments and a pattern of indiscriminate friendli- problems in view of the very large numbers
ness later, as shown by adoptees from Roma- that are required to do this. Nevertheless, it
nia, it is necessary to ask whether the does remain an important research priority for
correlates are similar. That is, is there evi- the future.
dence not only of within-individual continuity
between the two, but are the correlates simi-
Developmental programming
lar? The fact that both show the same associa-
tion with duration of privation begins to pro- Animal studies have provided strong evidence
vide evidence of heterotypic continuity, but for the existence of certain forms of develop-
much more needs to be done. The negative mental programming by which environmental
findings with respect to disruptive behavior input influences brain structure and develop-
show that disproof is possible. Thus, although ment. This is best established with respect to
hyperactivity–inattention in Romanian adopt- the role of visual input in the development of
ees was associated with the duration of priva- the occipital brain area underlying later visual
tion, disruptive behavior was not (Kreppner function (Blakemore, 1991). The practical im-
et al., 2000) indicating that it probably had portance of the phenomenon is shown in hu-
somewhat different origins. man studies by the evidence that an uncor-
rected squint in early childhood leads to a
later loss of binocular vision. Hormonal influ-
Continuities and discontinuities between
ences in animals have also been shown to af-
normal variations and disorder
fect the sexual differentiation of the brain
Similar issues apply to the study of continui- (Gorski, 1996). In the fields of biology and
ties and discontinuities between normal varia- internal medicine, there are also parallels with
tions in behavior and disorder that might re- respect to the development of immunity and
flect an extreme on the same dimension. The responses to diet (Bock & Whelan, 1991). In
need, again, is to determine whether the corre- the field of psychopathology, there is a possi-
lates are the same or different. The example ble parallel in the case of the effects of pro-
of severe mental retardation has already been found early social privation on the later devel-
given with respect to discontinuity. Recent opment of close confiding friendships and
findings suggest that the same might apply love relationships. We have already noted the
with respect to normal variations in language extent to which such social deficits seem to
and severe language delay (Dale, Simonoff, persist in spite of a radical change for the bet-

"  """     !      !


Developmental psychopathology: Concepts and challenges 287

ter in the environment. The concept of devel- lessons for the understanding of both normal
opmental programming, at least as extended development and disorder. For example, they
more broadly, remains a controversial one, have shown the importance of a pathways ap-
and much has still to be learned about the bio- proach to causal processes that recognizes
logical processes that it reflects. The issues, both direct and indirect effects, and which ac-
however, are of obvious importance, and fur- cepts that a single risk factor may have di-
ther research into the phenomenon is much verse consequences and that a single disorder
needed. outcome may arise by a variety of routes. It
is likely that, with full understanding of the
mechanisms involved, these complexities will
Therapeutic mechanisms in treatment reduce to a limited set of unifying processes,
but the striving for parsimony needs to be
Studies of treatment, as such, are not a central
tempered by a frank appreciation of the com-
feature of developmental psychopathology,
plexities that have to be explained. Similarly,
but an understanding of the mechanisms in-
developmental psychopathology approaches
volved in psychotherapeutic efficacy are, be-
have underlined the importance of consider-
cause of the light they may throw on develop-
ing both continuities and discontinuities in de-
mental processing generally (Rutter et al.,
velopment and of examining age-indexed
submitted). The work of Clark and his col-
variations in vulnerability to specific influ-
leagues in the field of panic disorders pro-
ences and of variations in onset and life
vides a good example of what can be done in
course as indices of psychopathological heter-
this connection (Clark, 1996; Clark & Fair-
ogeneity. Concepts of risk and protective
burn, 1997), and the studies of Patterson and
mechanisms, of resilience, of nature–nurture
his colleagues have begun to do the same in
and person–environment interplay, of the
the field of antisocial disorders (Forgatch &
cognitive and affective processing of experi-
DeGarmo, 1999). The effects of attachment-
ences, of dimensional risk and protective pro-
based interventions are similarly informative
cesses and psychopathological outcomes, and
(van IJzendoorn, 1995). The field is, however,
of the interplay between different domains of
very underexplored up to now. The compari-
development have all proved informative. But
son of experimental and control groups will
much work lies ahead. Developmental psy-
not, on its own, give this information. What
chopathology arose out of a recognition of the
is needed is the demonstration that, within the
value of combining developmental and clini-
treated group, there is a systematic dose–res-
cal perspectives but also out of an apprecia-
ponse relationship between changes in the
tion of the limitations of the grand theories of
postulated mediated mechanism and changes
the day. There is a continuing need to remain
in the target feature of psychopathology.
sceptical about the new evangelisms that have
Much greater use of this research stratagem
come to take their place, but equally the im-
should be made.
perative must be to replace doubt with pro-
grammatic research that truly tests competing
Conclusions hypotheses and which has the potential of
providing a real understanding of the range of
Developmental psychopathology perspectives causal processes as they apply across the span
have already provided many useful leads and of behavioral and developmental variation.

References
Achenbach, T. M. (1974). Developmental psychopathol- Achenbach, T. M., McConaughy, S. H., & Howell,
ogy. New York: Ronald Press. C. T. (1987). Child/adolescent behavioral and emo-
Achenbach, T. (in press). Assessment of psychopathol- tional problems: Implications of cross-informant cor-
ogy. In M. Lewis, A. Sameroff, & S. Miller (Eds.), relations for situational specificity. Psychological
Handbook of developmental psychopathology. New Bulletin, 101, 212–232.
York: Plenum. Acheson, D. (1998). Great Britain independent enquiry

"  """     !      !


288 M. Rutter and L. A. Sroufe

into inequalities in health [Report]. London: and adult disease (Ciba Foundation Symposium No.
HMSO. 156, pp. 129–146). Chichester, UK: Wiley.
Aguilar, B., Sroufe, L. A., Egeland, B., & Carlson, E. Block, J. H., Block, J., & Gjerde, P. F. (1986). The per-
(2000). Distinguishing the early-onset/persistent and sonality of children prior to divorce: A prospective
adolescence-onset antisocial behavior types: From study. Child Development, 57, 827–840.
birth to 16 years. Development and Psychopathology, Bock, G. R., & Whelan, J. (1991). The childhood envi-
12, 109–132. ronment and adult disease. Chichester, UK: Wiley.
Ainsworth, M. D. S., Blehar, M. C., Waters, E., & Wall, Bohman, M. (1996). Predisposition to criminality: Swed-
S. (1978). Patterns of attachment: A psychological ish adoption studies in retrospect. In G. R. Bock &
study of the Strange Situation. Hillsdale, NJ: Erl- J. A. Goode (Eds.), Genetics of criminal and anti-
baum. social behaviour (Ciba Foundation Symposium No.
Anderson, A. M. (1982). The great Japanese IQ increase. 194, pp. 99–114). Chichester, UK: Wiley.
Nature, 297, 180–181. Bornstein, M. H., & Sigman, M. D. (1986). Continuity in
Angold, A., Costello, E., & Erkanli, A. (1999). Comor- mental development from infancy. Child Develop-
bidity. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, ment, 57, 251–274.
40, 55–87. Bowlby, J. (1969). Attachment and loss: Vol. 1. Attach-
Bailey, A., Palferman, S., Heavey, L., & Le Couteur, A. ment. London: Hogarth Press.
(1998). Autism: The phenotype in relatives. Journal Bowlby, J. (1973). Attachment and loss: Vol. 2. Separa-
of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 28, 369– tion, anxiety and anger. London: Hogarth Press.
392. Bowlby, J. (1988). A secure base. New York: Basic
Baltes, P. B., Staudinger, U. M., & Lindenberger, U. Books.
(1999). Lifespan psychology: Theory and application Boyle, M. H., & Pickles, A. (1997). Maternal depressive
to intellectual functioning. Annual Review of Psychol- symptoms and ratings of emotional disorder symp-
ogy, 50, 471–507. toms in children and adolescents. Journal of Child
Barker, D. J. P. (1998). Mothers, babies, and health in Psychology and Psychiatry, 38, 981–992.
later life. London: Churchill Livingstone. Bremner, J. D., Innis, R. B., Ng, C. K., Staib, L. H., Salo-
Baron–Cohen, S. (1995). Mindblindness: An essay on au- mon, R. M., Bronen, R. A., Duncan, J., Southwick,
tism and theory of mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. S. M., Krystal, J. H., Rich, D., Zubal, G., Dey, H.,
Baron–Cohen, S., Leslie, A., & Frith, U. (1985). Does Soufer, R., & Charney, D. S. (1997). Positron emis-
the autistic child have a “theory of mind”? Cognition, sion tomography measurement of cerebral metabolic
21, 37–46. correlates of yohimbine administration in combat-re-
Baumrind, D. (1993). The average expectable environ- lated posttraumatic stress disorder. Archives of Gen-
ment is not good enough: A response to Scarr. Child eral Psychiatry, 54, 246–254.
Development, 64, 1299–1317. Bretherton, I. (1995). A communication perspective on
Baxter, L. R., Schwartz, J. M., Bergman, K. S., Szuba, attachment relationships and internal working models.
M. P., Guze, B. H., Mazziotla, J. C., Alazraki, A., Commentary. Monographs of the Society for Re-
Selin, C. E., Ferug, H.-K., Munford, P., & Phelps, search in Child Development 60 (2–3, Serial No.
M. E. (1992). Caudate glucose metabolic rate changes 244), 310–329.
with both drug and behavior therapy for obsessive– Bretherton, I., & Waters, E. (1985). Growing points of
compulsive disorder. Archives of General Psychiatry, attachment theory and research. Monographs of the
49, 681–689. Society for Research in Child Development 50 (1–2,
Bebbington, P. (1996). The origins of sex differences in
Serial No. 209).
depressive disorders: Bridging the gap. International
Brewin, C. R., Andrews, B., & Gotlib, I. H. (1993). Psy-
Review of Psychiatry, 8, 295–332.
chopathology and early experience: A reappraisal of
Bell, R. Q. (1968). A reinterpretation of the direction of
retrospective reports. Psychological Bulletin, 113,
effects in studies of socialization. Psychological Re-
82–98.
view, 75, 81–95.
Bronfenbrenner, U., & Ceci, S. J. (1994). Nature-nurture
Bell, R. Q., & Chapman, M. (1986). Child effects in stud-
ies using experimental or brief longitudinal ap- reconceptualization in developmental perspective: A
proaches to socialization. Developmental Psychology, bioecological model. Psychological Review, 101,
22, 595–603. 568–586.
Belsky, J., & Nezworski, T. (1988). Clinical implications Brown, G. W., & Harris, T. O. (1978). The social origins
of attachment. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. of depression: A study of psychiatric disorder in
Benoit, D., Parker, K. C. H., & Zeanah, C. H. (1997). women. London: Tavistock.
Mothers’ representations of their infants assessed pre- Brown, G. W., & Harris, T. O. (Eds.). (1989). Life events
natally: Stability and association with infants’ attach- and illness. New York: Guilford Press.
ment classifications. Journal of Child Psychology and Brown, G. W., Harris, T. O., & Hepworth, C. (1994).
Psychiatry, 38, 307–313. Life events and “endogenous” depression: A puzzle
Biederman, J., Faraone, S., Milberger, S., Guite, J., Meck, re-examined. Archives of General Psychiatry, 51,
E., Chen, L., Mennin, D., Marrs, A., Ouellette, C., 525–534.
Moore, P., Spencer, T., Norman, D., Wilens, T., Brown, R., Hobson, R. P., & Lee, A. (1997). Are there
Kraus, I., & Perrin, J. (1996). A prospective 4-year autistic-like features in congenitally blind children.
follow-up study of attention-deficit hyperactivity and Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 38,
related disorders. Archives of General Psychiatry, 53, 693–703.
437–446. Bryant, P. E. (1974). Perception and understanding in
Blakemore, C. (1991). Sensitive and vulnerable periods young children. London: Methuen.
in the development of the visual system. In G. R. Cadoret, R. J. (1985). Genes, environment and their inter-
Bock & J. Whelan (Eds.), The childhood environment action in the development of psychopathology. In T.

"  """     !      !


Developmental psychopathology: Concepts and challenges 289

Sakai & T. Tsuboi (Eds.), Genetic aspects of human self-regulatory structures of the mind. Development
behavior (pp. 165–175). Tokyo: Igaku–Shoin. and Psychopathology, 6, 533–549.
Cadoret, R. J., Yates, W. R., Troughton, E., Woodworth, Clark, D. M. (1996). Panic disorder: From theory to ther-
G., & Stewart, M. A. (1995). Genetic–environmental apy. In P. M. Salkovskis (Ed.), Frontiers in cognitive
interaction in the genesis of aggressivity and conduct therapy (pp. 318–344). New York: Guilford Press.
disorders. Archives of General Psychiatry, 52, 916– Clark, D. M., & Fairburn, C. G. (1997). The science and
924. practice of cognitive behaviour therapy. Oxford: Ox-
Cannon, M., & Murray, R. (1998). Neonatal origins of ford University Press.
schizophrenia. Archives of Disease in Childhood, 78, Coie, J. D., & Dodge, K. A. (1997). Aggression and anti-
1–3. social behavior. In W. Damon & N. Eisenberg (Eds.),
Cantwell, D. P., & Rutter, M. (1994). Classification: Con- Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 3: Social, emo-
ceptual issues and substantive findings. In M. Rutter, tional, and personality development (5th ed., pp.
E. Taylor, & L. Hersov (Eds.), Child and adolescent 779–862). New York: Wiley.
psychiatry: Modern approaches (3rd ed., pp. 3–21). Colombo, J. (1993). Infant cognition: Predicting later in-
Oxford: Blackwell Scientific. tellectual functioning. Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
Carlson, E., Jacobvitz, D., & Sroufe, L. A. (1995). A de- Crick, N. B. (1996). The role of overt aggression, rela-
velopmental investigation of inattentiveness and hy- tional aggression, and prosocial behaviour in the pre-
peractivity. Child Development, 66, 37–54. diction of children’s future social adjustment. Child
Caron, C., & Rutter, M. (1991). Comorbidity in child Development, 67, 2317–2327.
psychopathology. Concepts, issues and research strat- Dale, P. S., Simonoff, E., Bishop, D. V. M., Eley, T. C.,
egies. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, Oliver, B., Price, T. S., Purcell, S., Stevenson, J., &
32, 1063–1080. Plomin, R. (1998). Genetic influence on language de-
Caspi, A., Lynam, D., Moffit, T. E., & Silva, P. A. lay in two-year-old children. Nature Neuroscience, 1,
(1993). Unraveling girls’ delinquency: Biological, 324–328.
dispositional, and contextual contributions to adoles- DeFries, J. C., & Fulker, D. W. (1985). Multiple regres-
cent misbehavior. Developmental Psychology, 29, sion analysis of twin data. Behavior Genetics, 15,
19–30. 467–473.
Caspi, A., & Moffitt, T. E. (1991). Individual differences De Ribaupierre, A. (Ed.). (1989). Transition mechanisms
are accentuated during periods of social change: The in child development: The longitudinal perspective.
sample case of girls at puberty. Journal of Personality Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
and Social Psychology, 61, 157–168. Dodge, K. A., Bates, J. E., & Pettit, G. S. (1990). Mecha-
Caspi, A., & Moffitt, T. E. (1993). When do individual nisms in the cycle of violence. Science, 250, 1678–
differences matter? A paradoxical theory of personal- 1683.
ity coherence. Psychological Inquiry, 4, 247–271. Dodge, K. A., Pettit, G. S., Bates, J. E., & Valente, E.
Caspi, A., & Moffitt, T. E. (1995). The continuity of mal- (1995). Social information-processing patterns par-
adaptive behavior: From description to understanding tially mediate the effect of early physical abuse on
the study of antisocial behavior. In D. Cicchetti & D. later conduct problems. Journal of Abnormal Psy-
Cohen (Eds.), Developmental psychopathology (Vol. chology, 104, 632–643.
2, pp. 472–511). New York: Wiley. Dodge, K. A., & Schwartz, D. (1997). Social information
Cassidy, J., & Shaver, P. R. (Eds.). (1999). Handbook of processing mechanisms in aggressive behavior. In D.
attachment: Theory, research, and clinical applica- Stoff, J. Breilling, & J. D. Maser (Eds.), Handbook of
tions. New York: Guilford Press. antisocial behavior (pp. 171–180). New York: Wiley.
Cherlin, A. J., Furstenberg, F. F., Jr., Chase–Lansdale, Done, D. J., Johnstone, E. C., Frith, C. D., Golding, J.,
P. L., Kiernan, K. E., Robins, P. K., Morrison, Shepherd, P. M., & Crow, T. J. (1991). Complications
D. R., & Teitler, J. O. (1991). Longitudinal studies of of pregnancy and delivery in relation to psychosis in
effects of divorce on children in Great Britain and the adult life. British Medical Journal, 302, 1576–1580.
United States. Science, 252, 1386–1389. Duggal, S., Carlson, E., Sroufe, L. A., & Egeland, B. (in
Chisholm, K. (1998). A three-year follow-up of attach- press). Depressive symptomatology in children and
ment and indiscriminate friendliness in children adolescents. Development and Psychopathology.
adopted from Romanian orphanages. Child Develop- Dunn, J. (1988). The beginning of social understanding.
ment, 69, 1092–1106. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Cicchetti, D., & Cannon, T. D. (1999). Neurodevelop- Dunn, J. (1996). Children’s relationships: Bridging the
mental processes in the ontogenesis and epigenesis of divide between cognitive and social development.
psychopathology [Editorial]. Development and Psy- The Emanuel Miller Memorial Lecture 1995. Journal
chopathology, 11, 375–393. of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 37, 507–518.
Cicchetti, D., & Rogosch, F. (1996). Equifinality and Earls, F. (1987). Sex differences in psychiatric disorders:
multifinality in developmental psychopathology. De- Origins and developmental influences. Psychiatric
velopment and Psychopathology, 8, 597–600. Developments, 1, 1–23.
Cicchetti, D., & Sroufe, L. A. (1978). An organizational Eaves, L. J., Silberg, J. L., Meyer, J. M., Maes, H. H.,
view of affect: Illustration from the study of Down Simonoff, E., Pickles, A., Rutter, M., Neale, M. C.,
syndrome infants. In M. Lewis & L. Rosenblum Reynolds, C. A., Erickson, M. T., Heath, A. C.,
(Eds.), The development of affect (pp. 309–350). New Loeber, R., Truett, K. R., & Hewitt, J. K. (1997). Ge-
York: Plenum Press. netics and developmental psychopathology: 2. The
Cicchetti, D., & Toth, S. (1995). Developmental psychol- main effects of genes and environment on behavioral
ogy and disorders of affect. In D. Cicchetti & D. Co- problems in the Virginia twin study of adolescent be-
hen (Eds.), Developmental psychopathology (Vol. 2, havioral development. Journal of Child Psychology
pp. 369–420). New York: Wiley. and Psychiatry, 38, 965–980.
Cicchetti, D., & Tucker, D. (1994). Development and Elander, J., & Rutter, M. (1996). Use and development of

"  """     !      !


290 M. Rutter and L. A. Sroufe

the Rutter parents’ and teachers’ scales. International 2. Internalizing and externalizing expressions of dys-
Journal of Methods in Psychiatry, 6, 63–78. function (pp. 225–264). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Elder, G. H., Jr. (1986). Military times and turning points Garmezy, N. (1974). The study of competence in children
in men’s lives. Developmental Psychology, 22, 233– at risk for severe psychopathology. In E. Anthony &
245. C. Koupernik (Eds.), The child in his family (Vol. 3,
Eley, T. C., & Stevenson, J. (1999) Using genetic analy- pp. 77–97). New York: Wiley.
ses to clarify the distinction between depressive and Gelman, R., & Baillargeon, R. (1983). A review of some
anxious symptoms in children. Journal of Abnormal Piagetian concepts. In J. H. Flavell & E. M. Markman
Child Psychology, 27, 105–114. (Eds.), Mussen’s handbook of child psychology: Vol.
Emde, R., & Spicer, P. (2000). Experience in the midst 3. Cognitive development (4th ed., pp. 167–230).
of variation. Development and Psychopathology, 12, New York: Wiley.
313–331. Goldberg, S. (1997). Attachment and childhood behavior
Eme, R. F. (1979). Sex differences in childhood psycho- problems in normal, at-risk, and clinical samples. In
pathology: A review. Psychological Bulletin, 86, L. Atkinson & K. J. Zucker (Eds.), Attachment and
574–595. psychopathology (pp. 171–195). New York: Guilford
Eme, R. F. (1992). Selective female affliction in develop- Press.
ment of disorders of childhood: A literature review. Goldman–Rakic, R. C., Issernoff, A., Schwartz, M. L., &
Journal of Clinical Child Psychology, 21, 354–364. Bugbee, N. M. (1983).The neurobiology of cognitive
Erikson, E. H. (1950). Childhood and society. New York: development. In M. M. Haith & J. J. Campass (Eds.),
W.W. Norton. Mussen’s handbook of child psychology: Vol 2. In-
Farrington, D. P., Barnes, G. C., & Lambert, S. (1996). fancy and developmental psychobiology (4th ed., pp.
The concentration of offending in families. Legal and 281–344). New York: Wiley.
Criminological Psychology, 1, 47–63. Goodman, R. (1991). Growing together and growing
Farrington, D., Loeber, R., & van Kammen, W. B. apart: The non-genetic forces on children in the same
(1990). Long-term outcome of hyperactivity–impul- family. In P. McGuffin & R. Murray (Eds.), The new
sivity–attention deficit problems in childhood. In genetics of mental illness (pp. 212–224). Oxford:
L. N. Robins & M. Rutter (Eds.), Straight and devi- Heinemann Medical.
ous pathways from childhood to adulthood. Cam- Goodman, R. (1994). Brain development. In M. Rutter &
bridge: Cambridge University Press. D. F. Hay (Eds.), Development through life: A hand-
Fergusson, D. M., & Horwood, L. J. (1993). The struc- book for clinicians (pp. 49–78). Oxford: Blackwell
ture, stability and correlations of the trait components Scientific.
of conduct disorder, attention deficit and anxiety/ Gorski, R. A. (1996). Gonadal hormones and the organi-
withdrawal reports. Journal of Child Psychology and zation of brain structure and function. In D. Magnus-
Psychiatry, 34, 749–766. son (Ed.), The lifespan development of individuals:
Ferguson, D. M., Horwood, L. J., & Lynskey, M. T. Behavioral, neurobiological, and psychosocial per-
(1992). Family change, parental discord and early of- spectives (pp. 315–340). New York: Cambridge Uni-
fending. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, versity Press.
33, 1059–1075. Gottlieb, G. (1997). Synthesizing nature–nurture: Prena-
Fletcher, P., Happé, F., Frith, U., Baker, S., Dolan, R., tal roots of instinctive behavior. Mahwah, NJ: Erl-
Frackowiak, R., & Frith, C. (1995). Other minds in baum.
the brain: A functional imaging study of “theory of Gottlieb, G., Wahlsten, D., & Lickliter, R. (1998). The
mind” in story comprehension. Cognition, 57, 109– significance of biology for human development: A
128. developmental psychobiological systems view. In W.
Flynn, J. R. (1987). Massive IQ gains in 14 nations: What Damon & R. M. Lerner (Eds.), Handbook of child
IQ tests really measure. Psychological Bulletin, 101, psychology: Vol 1. Theoretical models of human de-
171–191. velopment (5th ed., pp. 233–273). Toronto: Wiley.
Fonagy, P., Steele, M., Steele, H., Higgitt, A., & Target, Griffith, E. M., Pennington, B. R., Wehner, E. A., & Rog-
M. (1994). The Emanuel Miller Memorial Lecture ers, S. J. (1999). Executive functions in young chil-
1992. The theory and practice of resilience. Journal dren with autism. Child Development, 70, 817–832.
of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 35, 231–257. Gualteri, T., & Hicks, R. E. (1985). An immunoreactive
Forgatch, M. S., & DeGarmo, D. S. (1999). Parenting theory of selective males affliction. Behavioral and
through change: An effective prevention program for Brain Sciences, 8, 427–477.
single mothers. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Happé, F. G. E. (1994). An advanced test of theory of
Psychology, 67, 711–724. mind: Understanding of story characters’ thoughts
Freud, S. (1953). Three essays on the theory of sexuality. and feelings by able, autistic, mentally handicapped
In J. Strachey (Ed. and Trans.), The standard edition and normal children and adults. Journal of Autism
of the complete works of Sigmund Freud (Vol. 7, pp. and Developmental Disorders, 24, 129–154.
125–143). London: Hogarth Press. (Original work Happé, F. G. E. (1996). Studying weak central coherence
published 1905) at low levels: Children with autism do not succumb
Frith, C. D. (1992). The cognitive neuropsychology of to visual illusions: A research note. Journal of Child
schizophrenia. Hove, UK: Erlbaum. Psychology and Psychiatry, 37, 873–877.
Frith, U. (1989). Autism: Explaining the enigma. Oxford: Harrington, R., Fudge, H., Rutter, M., Pickles, A., & Hill,
Blackwell. J. (1990). Adult outcome of childhood and adolescent
Garber, J., Quiggle, N. L., Panak, W., & Dodge, K. A. depression. I. Psychiatric status. Archives of General
(1991). Aggression and depression in children: Co- Psychiatry, 47, 465–473.
morbidity, specificity, and social cognitive process- Harrington, R., Rutter, M., Weissman, M., Fudge, H.,
ing. In D. Cicchetti & S. L. Toth (Eds.), Rochester Groothues, C., Bredenkamp, D., Pickles, A., Rende,
Symposium on Developmental Psychopathology: Vol. R., & Wickramaratne, P. (1997). Psychiatric disorders

"  """     !      !


Developmental psychopathology: Concepts and challenges 291

in the relatives of depressed probands. I. Comparison adult schizophrenia in the British 1946 birth cohort.
of prepubertal, adolescent and early adult onset cases. Lancet, 344, 1398–1402.
Journal of Affective Disorders, 42, 9–22. Judd, L. L. (1997). The clinical course of unipolar major
Harris, D. B. (Ed.) (1957). The concept of development: depressive disorders. Archives of General Psychiatry,
An issue in the study of human behavior. Minneapo- 54, 989–991.
lis/St. Paul, MN: University of Minnesota Press. Kagan, J. (1981). The second year: The emergence of
Harris, T., Brown, G. W., & Bifulco, A. (1986). Loss of self-awareness. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University
parent in childhood and adult psychiatric disorder: Press.
The role of lack of adequate parental care. Psycholog- Kendler, K. S. (1996). Major depression and generalised
ical Medicine, 16, 641–659. anxiety disorder. Same genes, (partly) different envi-
Harris, T., Brown, G. W., & Bifulco, A. (1990). Loss of ronments—revisited. British Journal of Psychiatry,
parent in childhood and adult psychiatric disorder: A 168(Suppl. 30), 68–75.
tentative overall model. Development and Psycho- Kendler, K. S., & Karkowski–Shuman, L. (1997). Stress-
pathology, 2, 311–328. ful life events and genetic liability to major depres-
Harrison, P. J. (1997). Schizophrenia: A disorder of neu- sion: Genetic control of exposure to the environment?
rodevelopment? Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 7, Psychological Medicine, 27, 539–547.
285–289. Kendler, K. S., Kessler, R. C., Walters, E. E., MacLean,
Hazan, C., & Shaver, P. R. (1994). Attachment as an or- C., Neale, M. C., Heath, A. C., & Eaves, L. J. (1995).
ganizational framework for research on close relation- Stressful life events, genetic liability, and onset of an
ships. Psychological Inquiry, 5, 1–22. episode of major depression in women. American
Hennessey, J. W., & Levine, S. (1979). Stress, arousal, Journal of Psychiatry, 152, 833–842.
and the pituitary–adrenal system: A psychoendocrine Kendler, K. S., Neale, M. C., Prescott, C. A., Kessler,
hypothesis. In J. M. Sprague & A. N. Epstein (Eds.), R. C., Heath, A. C., Corey, L. A., & Eaves, L. J.
Progress in psychobiology and physiological psychol- (1996). Childhood parental loss and alcoholism in
ogy (pp. 133–178). New York: Academic Press. women: A causal analysis using a twin-family design.
Hermelin, B., & O’Connor, N. (1970). Psychological ex- Psychological Medicine, 26, 79–95.
periments with autistic children. Oxford: Pergamon Kennard, M. A. (1942). Cortical reorganization of motor
Press. function. Archives of Neurology and Psychiatry, 48,
Hetherington, E. M. (1989). Coping with family transi- 227–240.
tions: Winners, losers, and survivors. Child Develop- Kreppner, J., O’Connor, T., Rutter, M., & the English
ment, 60, 1–14. and Romanian Adoptees Study Team. (2000). Is inat-
Hetherington, E. M., & Clingempeel, W. G. (1992). Cop- tention/overactivity a deprivation disorder? Manu-
ing with marital transitions: A family systems per- script submitted for publication.
spective. Monographs of the Society for Research in Krueger, R. F., Moffitt, T. E., Caspi, A. C., & Bleske,
Child Development, 57(Serial No. 227), 1–242. A. (1998). Assortative mating for antisocial behavior:
Hetherington, E. M., Lerner, R. M., & Perlmutter, M. Developmental and methodological implications. Be-
(Eds.). (1988). Child development in life-span per- haviour Genetics, 28, 173–186.
spective. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Lahey, B. B., Waldman, I. D., & McBurnett, K. (1999).
Hetherington, E. M., & Stanley–Hagan, M. M. (1997). The development of antisocial behavior: An integra-
The effects of divorce on fathers and their children. tive causal model. Journal of Child Psychology and
In M. E. Lamb (Ed.), The role of the father in child Psychiatry, 40, 669–682.
Lane, N. E., Bloch, D. A., Jones, H. H., Marshall,
development (pp. 191–211). New York: Wiley.
W. H., Wood, P. D., & Fries, J. F. (1986). Long-dis-
Hewitt, J. K., Silberg, J. L., Rutter, M., Simonoff, E.,
tance running, bone density, and osteoarthritis. Jour-
Meyer, J. M., Maes, H., Pickles, A., Neale, M. C.,
nal of the American Medical Association, 255, 1147–
Loeber, R., Erickson, M. T., Kendler, K. S., Heath,
1151.
A. C., Truett, K. R., Reynolds, C. A., & Eaves, L. J.
Lane, N. E., Bloch, D. A., Wood, P. D., & Fries, J. F.
(1997). Genetics and developmental psychopathol-
(1987). Aging, long-distance running and the devel-
goy: 1. Phenotypic assessemnt in the Virginia twin opment of musculo-skeletal disability: A controlled
study of adolescent behavioral development. Journal study. American Journal of Medicine, 82, 722–780.
of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 38, 943–963. Laub, J. H., Nagin, D. S., & Sampson, R. J. (1998). Tra-
Hinde, R. A. (1997). Relationships: A dialectical ap- jectories of change in criminal offending: Good mar-
proach. Hove, UK: Psychology Press. riages and the desistance process. American Sociolog-
Hodges, J., & Tizard, B. (1989a). IQ and behavioural ad- ical Review, 63, 225–238.
justment of ex-institutional adolescents. Journal of Lewinsohn, P. M., Rohde, P., & Seeley, J. R. (1998).
Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 30, 53–75. Major depressive disorder in older adolescents: Prev-
Hodges, J., & Tizard, B. (1989b). Social and family rela- alence, risk factors, and clinical implications. Clinical
tionships of ex-institutional adolescents. Journal of Psychology Review, 18, 765–794.
Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 30, 77–97. Liu, D., Diorio, J., Tannenbaum, B., Caldji, C., Francis,
Howlin, P., Mawhood, L., & Rutter, M. (in press). Au- D., Freedman, A., Sharma, S., Pearson, D., Plotsky,
tism and developmental receptive language disor- P., & Meaney, M. J. (1997). Maternal care, hippo-
der—A follow-up comparison in early adult life. II. campal glucocorticoid receptors, and hypothalamic–
Social, behavioural and psychiatric outcomes. Journal pituitary–adrenal responses to stress. Science, 277,
of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 1659–1662.
Jones, P. (1997). The early origins of schizophrenia. Brit- Loeber, R., & Hay, D. (1997). Key issues in the develop-
ish Medical Bulletin, 53, 135–155. ment of aggression and violence from childhood to
Jones, P. B., Rodgers, B., Murray, R. M., & Marmot, early adulthood. Annual Review of Psychology, 48,
M. G. (1994). Child developmental risk factors for 371–410.

"  """     !      !


292 M. Rutter and L. A. Sroufe

Lucas, A. R., Beard, C. M., O’Fallon, W. M., & Kurland, Murray, R. M., & Woodruff, P. W. R. (1995). Develop-
L. T. (1991). Fifty-year trends in the incidence of an- mental insanity or dementia praecox: A new perspec-
orexia nervosa in Rochester, Minnesota: A popula- tive on an old debate. Neurological Psychiatric Brain
tion-based study. American Journal of Psychiatry. Research, 3, 167–176.
148, 917–922. Nagin, D. S., Farrington, D. P., & Moffitt, T. E. (1995).
Lucas, A., Morley, R., & Cole, T. J. (1998). Randomised Life-course trajectories of different types of offend-
trial of early diet in preterm babies and later intelli- ers. Criminology, 33, 111–139.
gence quotient. British Medical Journal, 317, 1481– Nagin, D. S., & Tremblay, R. E. (1999). Trajectories of
1487. boys’ physical aggression, opposition, and hyperac-
Luthar, S. S., Cicchetti, D., & Becker, B. (in press). The tivity on the path to physically violent and nonviolent
construct of resilience: A critical evaluation and juvenile delinquency. Child Development, 70, 1181–
guidelines for future work. Child Development. 1196.
Maccoby, E. E., & Jacklin, C. N. (1983). The “person” Nathanielsz, P. W. (1999). Life in the womb: The origin
characteristics of children and the family as environ- of health and disease. Ithaca, NY: Promethean Press.
ment. In D. Magnusson & V. L. Allen (Eds.), Human Nielsen, S. (1990). The epidemiology of anorexia nervosa
development: An international perspective (pp. 76– in Denmark from 1973 to 1987: A nationwide register
91). New York: Academic Press. study of psychiatric admission. Acta Psychiatrica
Main, M., Kaplan, N., & Cassidy, J. (1985). Security in Scandinavica, 81, 507–514.
infancy, childhood and adulthood: A move to the Nolen–Hoeksema, S. (1987). Sex differences in unipolar
level of representation. Monographs of the Society for depression: Evidence and theory. Psychological Bul-
Research in Child Development, 50(Serial No. 209), letin, 101, 259–282.
66–106. O’Brien, J. T. (1997). The “glucocorticoid cascade” hy-
Main, M., & Solomon, J. (1986). Discovery of a new, pothesis in man: Prolonged stress may cause perma-
insecure–disorganized/disoriented attachment pattern. nent brain damage. British Journal of Psychiatry, 170,
In M. Yogman & T. B. Brazelton (Eds.), Affective 199–201.
development in infancy (pp. 95–124). Norwood, NJ: O’Connor, N., & Hermelin, B. (1978). Seeing and hear-
Ablex. ing and space and time. London: Academic Press.
Marmot, M., & Wilkinson, R. G. (Eds.). (1999). Social O’Connor, T., Bredenkamp, D., Rutter, M., & the English
determinants of health. Oxford: Oxford University and Romanian Adoptees Study Team. (1999). Attach-
Press. ment disturbances and disorders in children exposed
Marvin, R., & O’Connor, T. G. (1999, April). The forma- to early severe deprivation. Infant Mental Health
tion of parent–child attachment following privation. Journal, 20, 10–29.
Paper presented at the Biennial Meeting of the Soci- O’Connor, T. G., Deater–Deckard, K., Fulker, D., Rutter,
ety for Research in Child Development, Albuquer- M., & Plomin, R. (1998). Genotype–environment
que, NM. correlations in late childhood and early adolescence:
Masten, A., & Curtis, W. J. (2000). Integrating compe- Antisocial behavioral problems and coercive parent-
tence and psychopathology. Development and Psy- ing. Developmental Psychology, 34, 970–981.
chopathology, 12, 529–550. O’Connor, T. G., Rutter, M., & the English and Roma-
McCall, R. B., & Carriger, M. S. (1993). A meta-analysis nian Adoptees Study Team. (in press). Attachment
of infant habituation and recognition memory perfor- disorder behavior following early severe deprivation:
mance as predictors of later IQ. Child Development, Extension and longitudinal follow-up. Journal of the
64, 57–79. American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychi-
McKenzie, K., & Murray, R. M. (1999). Risk factors for atry.
psychosis in the UK Afro-Caribbean population. In O’Connor, T. G., Thorpe, K., Dunn, J., Golding, J., & the
D. Bhugra & V. Bahl (Eds.), Ethnicity: An agenda ALSPAC Study Team. (1999). Parental divorce and
for mental health (pp. 48–59). London: Gaskell. adjustment in adulthood: Findings from a community
McKeown, T. (1976). The role of medicine: Dream, mi- sample. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry,
rage or nemesis? London: Nuffield Provincial Hospi- 40, 777–789.
tals Trust. Offord, D. R., & Cross, L. A. (1969). Behavioural antece-
Michel, G. F., & Moore, C. L. (1995). Developmental dants of adult schizophrenia. Archives of General
psychobiology: An interdisciplinary science. Cam- Psychiatry, 21, 267–283.
bridge, MA: MIT Press. Patterson, G. R. (1995). Coercion as a basis for early age
Moffitt, T. E. (1993). Adolescence-limited and life- of onset for arrest. In J. McCord (Ed.), Coercion and
course-persistent antisocial behavior: A develop- punishment in long-term perspectives (pp. 81–105).
mental taxonomy. Psychological Review, 100, 674– Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
701. Patterson, G. R., & Yoerger, K. (1997). A developmental
Moffitt, T. E., Caspi, A., Rutter, M., & Silva, P. A. model for late-onset delinquency. In R. Dienstbier &
(2000). Sorting out sex differences in antisocial be- D. W. Osgood (Eds.), The Nebraska Symposium on
havior: Findings from the first two decades of the Motivation: Vol. 44. Motivation and delinquency (pp.
Dunedin longitudinal study. Manuscript submitted for 110–177). Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press.
publication. Pawlby, S. J., Mills, A., & Quinton, D. (1997). Vulnera-
Moldin, S. O., & Gottesman, I. I. (1997). At issue: Genes, ble adolescent girls: Opposite sex relationships. Jour-
experience, and chance in schizophrenia—Position- nal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 38, 909–
ing for the 21st century. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 23, 920.
547–561. Pawlby, S. J., Mills, A., Taylor, A., & Quinton, D.
Molenaar, P. C. M., Boomsma, D. I., & Dolan, C. V. (1997). Adolescent friendships mediating childhood
(1993). A third source of developmental differences. adversity and adult outcome. Journal of Adolescence,
Behavior Genetics, 23, 519–524. 20, 633–644.

"  """     !      !


Developmental psychopathology: Concepts and challenges 293

Pennington, B. F., & Ozonoff, S. (1996). Executive func- (Eds.), The child in his family (pp. 165–196). New
tions and developmental psychopathology. Journal of York: Wiley.
Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 37, 51–87. Rutter, M. (1971a). Normal psychosexual development.
Perris, C. (1992). Bipolar–unipolar distinction. In E. S. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 11,
Paykel (Ed.), Handbook of affective disorders (pp. 259–283.
57–75). London: Churchill Livingstone. Rutter, M. (1971b). Parent–child separation: Psychologi-
Petersen, A. C. (1993). Creating adolescents: The role of cal effects on the children. Journal of Child Psychol-
context and process in developmental trajectories. ogy and Psychiatry, 12, 233–260.
Journal of Research on Adolescence, 3, 1–18. Rutter, M. (1972). Childhood schizophrenia reconsidered.
Piaget, J. (1970). Piaget’s theory. In P. H. Mussen (Ed.), Journal of Autism and Child Schizophrenia, 2, 315–
Carmichael’s handbook of child development (pp. 337.
703–732). New York: Wiley. Rutter, M. (1981). Maternal deprivation reassessed (2nd
Plaisted, K. C., Swettenham, J., & Rees, L. (1999). Chil- ed.). Harmondsworth, Middlesex, UK: Penguin.
dren with autism show local precedence in a divided Rutter, M. (1982). Developmental neuropsychiatry: Con-
attention task and global precedence in a selective at- cepts, issues and problems. Journal of Clinical Neu-
tention task. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychi- ropsychology, 4, 91–115.
atry, 40, 733–742. Rutter, M. (1983). Cognitive deficits in the pathogenesis
Plomin, R., & Rutter, M. (1998). Child development, mo- of autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychia-
lecular genetics and what to do with genes once they try, 24, 513–531.
are found. Child Development, 69, 1223–1242. Rutter, M. (1984). Psychopathology and development: II.
Popper, K. R. (1972). Objective knowledge: An evolution- Childhood experiences and personality development.
ary approach. Oxford: Oxford University Press. Australian and New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry,
Post, R. (1992). Transduction of psychosocial stress into 18, 314–327.
neurobiology of recurrent affective disorder. Ameri- Rutter, M. (1986a). Child psychiatry: The interface be-
can Journal of Psychiatry, 149, 999–1010. tween clinical and developmental research. Psycho-
Quinton, D., Pickles, A., Maughan, B., & Rutter, M. logical Medicine, 16, 151–169.
(1993). Partners, peers and pathways: Assortative Rutter, M. (1986b). The developmental psychopathology
pairing and continuities in conduct disorder. Develop- of depression: Issues and perspectives. In M. Rutter,
ment and Psychopathology, 5, 763–783. C. Izard, & P. Read (Eds.), Depression in young peo-
Quinton, D., & Rutter, M. (1988). Parenting breakdown: ple: Developmental and clinical perspectives (pp. 3–
The making and breaking of inter-generational links. 30). New York : Guilford Press.
Aldershot, UK: Avebury. Rutter, M. (1987). The role of cognition in child develop-
Richards, M., Wadsworth, M., Rahimi–Foroushani, A., ment and disorder. British Journal of Medical Psy-
Hardy, R., Kuh, D., & Paul, A. (1998). Infant nutri- chology, 60, 1–16.
tion and cognitive development in the first offspring Rutter, M. (1989a). Psychiatric disorder in parents as a
of a national UK birth cohort. Developmental Medi- risk factor for children. In D. Shaffer, I. Philips,
cine and Child Neurology, 40, 163–167. N. B. Enzer, M. M. Silverman, & V. Anthony (Eds.),
Richters, J., & Cicchetti, D. (1993). Mark Twain meets Prevention of mental disorders, alcohol and other
DSM III-R: Conduct disorder, development and the drug use in children and adolescents (OSAP Preven-
concept of harmful dysfunction. Development and tion Monograph No. 2, pp. 157–189). Rockville, MD:
Psychopathology, 5, 5–29. Office for Substance Abuse Prevention, U.S. Depart-
Robins, L. N. (1993). Vietnam veterans’ rapid recovery ment of Health and Social Services.
from heroin addiction: A fluke or normal expectation? Rutter, M. (1989b). Pathways from childhood to adult
Addiction, 88, 1041–1054. life. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 30,
Robins, L. N., Davis, D. H., & Wish, E. (1977). Detecting 23–51.
predictors of rare events: Demographic family and Rutter, M. (1989c). Age as an ambiguous variable in de-
personal deviance as predictors of stages in the pro- velopmental research: Some epidemiological consid-
gression toward narcotic addiction. In J. S. Strauss, erations from developmental psychopathology. Inter-
H. M. Babigian, & M. Roff (Eds.), The origins and national Journal of Behavioral Development, 12,
course of psychopathology (pp. 379–406). New York: 1–34.
Plenum Press. Rutter, M (1990). Psychosocial resilience and protective
Rose, S. (1995). The rise of neurogenetic determinism. mechanisms. In J. Rolf, A. Masten, D. Cicchetti, K.
Nature, 373, 380–382. Neuchterlein, & S. Weintraub (Eds.), Risk and pro-
Rowe, D. C., Jacobson, K. C., & van den Oord, tective factors in the development of psychopathology
J. C. G. (1999). Genetic and environmental influences (pp. 181–214). New York: Cambridge University
on vocabulary IQ: Parental education level as a mod- Press.
erator. Child Development, 70, 1151–1162. Rutter, M. (1993). An overview of paediatric neuropsy-
Roy, P., Rutter, M., & Pickles, A. (2000a). Institutional chiatry. Educational and Child Psychology,
care: Risk from family background or pattern of rear- 10(Suppl.), 4–11.
ing? Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 41, Rutter, M. (1995). Clinical implications of attachment
139–149. concepts: Retrospect and prospect. Journal of Child
Roy, P., Rutter, M., & Pickles, A. (2000b). Institutional Psychology and Psychiatry, 36, 549–571.
care: Associations between hyperactivity and attach- Rutter, M. (1996). Transitions and turning points in de-
ment problems. Manuscript submitted for publication. velopmental psychopathology: As applied to the age
Rugg, M. D. (Ed.). (1997). Cognitive neuroscience. span between childhood and mid-adulthood. Interna-
Hove, UK: Psychology Press. tional Journal of Behavioral Development, 19, 603–
Rutter, M. (1970). Sex differences in children’s response 626.
to family stress. In E. J. Anthony & C. Koupernik Rutter, M (1997a). Comorbidity: Concepts, claims and

"  """     !      !


294 M. Rutter and L. A. Sroufe

choices. Criminal Behaviour and Mental Health, 7, Testing hypotheses on specific environmental risk
265–285. mechanisms for psychopathology. Manuscript submit-
Rutter, M. (1997b). Nature–nurture integration: The ex- ted for publication.
ample of antisocial behavior. American Psychologist, Rutter, M., & Plomin, R. (1997). Opportunities for psy-
52, 390–398. chiatry from genetic findings. British Journal of Psy-
Rutter, M. (in press-a). Resilience reconsidered: Concep- chiatry, 171, 209–219.
tual considerations and empirical findings. In J. P. Rutter, M., & Rutter, M. (1993). Developing minds: Chal-
Shonkoff & S. J. Meisels (Eds.), Handbook of early lenge and continuity across the life span. Harmondsw-
childhood intervention. New York: Cambridge Uni- orth, Middlesex, & New York: Penguin Books.
versity Press. Rutter, M. R., & Quinton, D. (1984). Parental psychiatric
Rutter, M. (in press-b). Family influences on behavior disorder: Effects on children. Psychological Medi-
and development: Challenges for the future. In J. cine, 14, 853–880.
McHale & W. Grolnick (Eds.), Retrospect and pros- Rutter, M., & Shaffer, D. (1980). DSM-III: A step for-
pect in the psychological study of families. Hillsdale, ward or back in terms of the classification of child
NJ: Erlbaum. psychiatric disorders? Journal of the American Acad-
Rutter, M. (2000a). Genetic studies of autism: From the emy of Child Psychiatry, 19, 371–394.
1970s into the millennium. Journal of Abnormal Rutter, M., Silberg, J., O’Connor, T., & Simonoff, E.
Child Psychology. (1999a). Genetics and child psychiatry: I Advances in
Rutter, M. (2000b). Psychosocial influences: Critiques, quantitative and molecular genetics. Journal of Child
findings, and research needs. Development and Psy- Psychology and Psychiatry, 40, 3–18.
chopathology, 12, 375–405. Rutter, M., Silberg, J., O’Connor, T., & Simonoff, E.
Rutter, M., Andersen–Wood, L., Beckett, C., Breden- (1999b). Genetics and child psychiatry: II Empirical
kamp, D., Castle, J., Groothues, C., Kreppner, J., research findings. Journal of Child Psychology and
Keaveney, L., Lord, C., O’Connor, T. G., & the En- Psychiatry, 40, 19–55.
glish and Romanian Adoptees Study Team. (1999). Rutter, M., Simonoff, E., & Plomin, R. (1996). Genetic
Quasi-autistic patterns following severe early global influences on mild mental retardation: Concepts, find-
privation. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychia- ings and research implications. Journal of Biosocial
try, 40, 537–549. Science, 28, 509–526.
Rutter, M., Bailey, A., Simonoff, E., & Pickles, A. Rutter, M., & Smith, D. J. (Eds.). (1995). Psychosocial
(1997). Genetic influences and autism. In D. J. Co- disorders in young people: Time trends and their
hen & F. R. Volkmar (Eds.), Handbook of autism and causes. Chichester, UK: Wiley.
pervasive developmental disorders (pp. 370–387). Sameroff, A. J. (2000). Developmental systems and psy-
New York: Wiley. chopathology. Development and Psychopathology,
Rutter, M., Champion, L., Quinton, D., Maughan, B., & 12, 297–312.
Pickles, A. (1995). Understanding individual differ- Sampson, R. J., & Laub, J. H. (1996). Socioeconomic
ences in environmental risk exposure. In P. Moen, achievement in the life course of disadvantaged men:
G. H. Elder, Jr., & K. Lüscher (Eds.), Examining lives Military service as a turning point, circa 1940–1965.
in context: Perspectives on the ecology of human de- American Sociological Review, 61, 347–367.
velopment (pp. 61–93). Washington, DC: American Schachar, R. (1991). Childhood hyperactivity. Journal of
Psychological Association. Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 32, 155–191
Rutter, M., Dunn, J., Plomin, R., Simonoff, E., Pickles, Schachar, R., Tannock, R., Marriott, M., & Logan, G.
A., Maughan, B., Ormel, J., Meyer, J., & Eaves, L. (1995). Deficient inhibitory control in attention deficit
(1997). Integrating nature and nurture: Implications of hyperactivity disorder. Journal of Abnormal Child
person-environment correlations and interactions for Psychology, 23, 411–437.
developmental psychology. Development and Psy- Schultz, R. T., Gauthier, I., Klin, A., Fulbright, R. K.,
chopathology, 9, 335–364. Anderson, A. W., Volkmar, F., Skudlarski, P., Laca-
Rutter, M., Giller, H., & Hagell, A. (1998). Antisocial die, C., Cohen, D. J., & Gore. J. C. (2000). Abnormal
behavior by young people. New York: Cambridge ventral temporal cortical activity during face discrimi-
University Press. nation among individuals with autism and Asperger
Rutter, M., Maughan, B., Meyer, J., Pickles, A., Silberg, syndrome. Archives of General Psychiatry, 57, 331–
J., Simonoff, E., & Taylor, E. (1997). Heterogeneity 340.
of antisocial behavior: Causes, continuities and con- Shaffer, D., Lucas, C. P., & Richters, J. E. (Eds.). (1999).
sequences. In R. Dienstbier & D. W. Osgood (Eds.), Diagnostic assessment in child and adolescent psy-
Nebraska Symposium on Motivation: Vol 44. Motiva- chopathology. New York: Guilford Press.
tion and delinquency (pp. 45–118). Lincoln: Univer- Sigman, M., & Ruskin, E. (1999). Continuity and change
sity of Nebraska Press. in the social competence of children with autism,
Rutter, M., & O’Connor, T. G. (1999). Implications of Down syndrome, and developmental delays. Mono-
attachment theory for child care policies. In J. Cas- graphs of the Society for Research in Child Develop-
sidy & P. R. Shaver (Eds.), Handbook of attachment: ment, 64(Serial No. 256), v–vi, 1–114.
Theory, research and clinical applications (pp. 823– Silberg, J., Pickles, A., Rutter, M., Hewitt, J., Simonoff,
844). New York: Guilford Press. E., Maes, H., Carbonneau, R., Murrelle, L., Foley,
Rutter, M., O’Connor, T., Beckett, C., Castle, J., Croft, D., & Eaves, L. (1999). The influence of genetic fac-
C., Groothues, C., Kreppner, J., & the English and tors and life stress on depression among adolescent
Romanian Adoptees Study Team. (in press). Recov- girls. Archives of General Psychiatry, 56, 225–232.
ery and deficit following profound early deprivation. Silberg, J., Rutter, M., Neale, K., & Eaves, L. (in press).
In P. Selman (Ed.), Edited collection on intercountry Genetic moderation of environmental risk for depres-
adoption. London: BAAF. sion and anxiety in adolescent girls. British Journal
Rutter, M., Pickles, A., Murray, R., & Eaves, L. (2000). of Psychiatry.

"  """     !      !


Developmental psychopathology: Concepts and challenges 295

Simonoff, E., Pickles, A., Hervas, A., Silberg, J. L., Rut- Taylor, E. (1999). Developmental neuropsychopathology
ter, M., & Eaves, L. (1998). Genetic influences on of attention deficit and impulsiveness. Development
childhood hyperactivity: Contrast effects imply pa- and Psychopathology, 11, 607–629.
rental rating bias, not sibling interaction. Psychologi- Taylor, E., & Rutter, M. (1986). Sex differences in neuro-
cal Medicine, 28, 825–837. developmental and psychiatric disorders: One expla-
Simonoff, E., Pickles, A., Meyer, J. J., Silberg, J. L., nation or many? [Commentary]. Behavioural and
Maes, H. H., Loeber, R., Rutter, M., Hewitt, J. K., & Brain Sciences, 8, 460–461.
Eaves, L. J. (1997). The Virginia twin study of ado- Taylor, E., Sandberg, S., Thorley, G., & Giles, S. (1991).
lescent behavioral development: Influences of age, The epidemiology of childhood hyperactivity. Oxford:
gender and impairment on rates of disorder. Archives Oxford University Press.
of General Psychiatry, 54, 801–808. Teasdale, J. D., & Barnard, P. J. (1993). Affect, cognition,
Sonuga–Barke, E. J. S., Taylor, E., Sembi, S., & Smith, and change: Re-modelling depressive thought. Hove,
J. (1992). Hyperactivity and delay aversion: I. The UK: Erlbaum.
effect of delay on choice. Journal of Child Psychol- Thapar, A., Harold, G., & McGuffin, P. (1998). Life
ogy and Psychiatry, 33, 387–398. events and depressive symptoms in childhood—
Sroufe, L. A. (1983). Infant–caregiver attachment and Shared genes or shared adversity? A research note.
patterns of adaptation in the preschool: The roots of Journal of Child Psychology & Psychiatry & Allied
maladaptation and competence. In M. Perlmutter Disciplines, 39, 1153–1158.
(Ed.), Minnesota Symposia in Child Psychology: Vol. Thapar, A., & McGuffin, P. (1994). A twin study of de-
16. Development and policy concerning children with pressive symptoms in childhood. British Journal of
special needs (pp. 41–83). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Psychiatry, 165, 259–265.
Sroufe, L. A. (1988). The role of infant–caregiver attach- Tizard, J. (1975). Race and IQ: The limits of probability.
ments in development. In J. Belsky & T. Nezworski New Behavior, 1, 6–9.
(Eds.), Clinical implications of attachment (pp. 18– Townsend, P., & Davidson, N. (Eds.). (1992). Inequali-
38). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. ties in health: The Black report. London: Penguin.
Sroufe, L. A. (1989). Pathways to adaptation and malad- Tremblay, R. E., Japel, C., Pérusse, D., Boivin, M., Zoc-
aptation: Psychopathology as developmental devia- colillo, M., Montplaisir, J., & McDuff, P. (in press).
tion. In D. Cicchetti (Ed.), Rochester Symposium on The search for the age of “onset” of physical aggres-
Developmental Psychopathology (Vol. 1, pp. 13–40). sion: Rousseau and Bandura revisited. Criminal Be-
Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. havior & Mental Health.
Sroufe, L. A. (1996). Emotional development: The organ- van IJzendoorn, M. H. (1995). Adult attachment repre-
ization of emotional life in the early years. New York: sentations, parental responsiveness, and infant attach-
Cambridge University Press. ment: A meta-analysis on the predictive validity of
Sroufe, L. A. (1997). Psychopathology as an outcome of the Adult Attachment Interview. Psychological Bulle-
development. Development and Psychopathology, 9, tin, 117, 387–403.
251–268. van IJzendoorn, M. H., Schuengel, C., & Bakermans–
Sroufe, L. A. (in press). From infant attachment to ado- Kranenburg, M. J. (1999). Disorganized attachment in
lescent autonomy: Longitudinal data on the role of early childhood: Meta-analysis of precursors, con-
parents in development. In J. Borkowskis, S. Ra- comitants and sequelae. Development and Psycho-
mey, & M. Bristol–Power (Eds.), Parenting and your pathology, 11, 225–249.
child’s world. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Vargha–Khadem, F., Isaacs, E., van der Werf, S., Robb,
Sroufe, L. A., Egeland, B., & Carlson, E. (1999). One
S., & Wilson, J. (1992). Development of intelligence
social world: The integrated development of parent–
and memory in children with hemiplegic cerebral
child and peer relationships. In W. A. Collins & B.
palsy: The deleterious consequences of early seizures.
Laursen (Eds.), Relationships as developmental con-
Brain, 115, 315–329.
text: The 30th Minnesota Symposium on Child Psy-
Vondra, J. I., & Barnett, D. (Eds.). (1999). Atypical at-
chology. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
tachment in infancy and early childhood among chil-
Sroufe, L. A., & Fleeson, J. (1988). The coherence of fam-
ily relationships. In R. A. Hinde & J. Stevenson–Hinde dren at developmental risk. Monographs of the Soci-
(Eds.), Relationships within families: Mutual influences ety for Research in Child Development, 64(Serial No.
(pp. 27–47). Oxford: Oxford University Press. 258).
Sroufe, L. A., Fox, N., & Pancake, V. (1983). Attachment Wahlsten, D. K., & Gottlieb, G. (1997).The invalid sepa-
and dependency in developmental perspective. Child ration of effects of nature and nurture: Lessons from
Development, 55, 17–29. animal experimentation. In R. J. Sternberg & E. L.
Sroufe, L. A., & Rutter, M. R. (1984). The domain of Grigorenko (Eds.), Intelligence, heredity and environ-
developmental psychopathology. Child Development, ment (pp. 163–192). Cambridge: Cambridge Univer-
55, 17–29. sity Press.
Stattin, H., & Magnusson, D. (1990). Pubertal matura- Waters, E., Vaughan, B. E., Posada, G., & Kondo–Ike-
tion in female development. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. mura, K. (Eds.). (1995). Caregiving, cultural and cog-
Suess, G. J., Grossmann, K. E., & Sroufe, L. A. (1992). nitive perspectives on secure-base behavior and work-
Effects of infant attachment to mother and father on ing models: New growing points of attachment theory
quality of adaptation in preschool: From dyadic to in- and research. Monographs of the Society for Research
dividual organization of self. International Journal of in Child Development, 60(Serial No. 244).
Behavioral Development, 15, 43–66. Weissman, M. M., Wolk, S., Goldstein, R. B., Moreau,
Taylor, E. (1994). Syndromes of attention deficit and over- D., Adams, P., Greenwald, S., Klier, C. M., Ryan,
activity. In M. Rutter, E. Taylor, & L. Hersov (Eds.), N. D., Dahl, R. E., & Wickramaratne, P. (1999). De-
Child and adolescent psychiatry: Modern approaches pressed adolescents grown up. Journal of the Ameri-
(3rd ed., pp. 285–308). Oxford: Blackwell Scientific. can Medical Association, 281, 1707–1713.

"  """     !      !


296 M. Rutter and L. A. Sroufe

Wilkinson, R. G. (1996). Unhealthy societies: The afflic- children adopted from institutions. Journal of Devel-
tions of inequality. London: Routledge. opmental and Behavioral Pediatrics.
Wohlwill, J. (1970). The age variable in psychological Zoccolillo, M. (1993). Gender and the development of
research. Psychological Review, 77, 49–64. conduct disorder. Development and Psychopathology,
Wohlwill, J. (1973). The study of behavioral develop- 5, 65–78.
ment. New York: Academic Press. Zoccolillo, M., Pickles, A., Quinton, D., & Rutter, M.
Zeanah, C. H. (1996). Beyond insecurity: A reconceptu- (1992). The outcome of childhood conduct disorder:
alization of attachment disorders of infancy. Journal Implications for defining adult personality disorder
of Consulting & Clinical Psychology, 64, 42–52. and conduct disorder. Psychological Medicine, 22,
Zeanah, C.H. (in press). Disturbances of attachment in 971–986.

"  """     !      !

You might also like