Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 12

Received: 19 October 2022 | Accepted: 22 August 2023

DOI: 10.1111/ajsp.12582

bs_bs_banner Asian Journal of Social Psychology


REGULAR ARTICLE

The market mindset erodes social mindfulness

Huiwen Xiao | Ziqiang Xin

Department of Psychology, Renmin


University of China, Beijing, China Abstract
Previous studies have mainly explored the impacts of social mindfulness on
Correspondence
Ziqiang Xin, Department of Psychology, social behaviours (e.g., cooperation), little is known about crucial macro social
Renmin University of China, 59 environment factors that affect social mindfulness, like the market economy.
Zhongguancun Street, Haidian District, With the development of the market economy, people typically acquire the self-­
Beijing 100872, China.
Email: xinziqiang@sohu.com centred and rational market mindset which may suppress social mindfulness.
Therefore, the current study aimed to examine the causal relationship between
Funding information the market mindset and social mindfulness, as well as the psychological
Fundamental Research Funds for the
Central Universities and the Research mechanism behind the effect. In Study 1, participants whose market mindset was
Funds of Renmin University of China, activated by recalling market experiences showed lower social mindfulness than
Grant/Award Number: 22XNKJ01 those whose non-­market mindset was activated by recalling other experiences.
Studies 2 and 3 explored the mediation of perspective taking and state empathy
behind the detrimental effect. It was found that the relationship between the
market mindset and social mindfulness was mediated by perspective taking
rather than by state empathy. Together, the present three studies expand the
existing literature on the relationship between the market mindset and social
mindfulness and remind policymakers to pay attention to the adverse effects of
the market mindset on social mindfulness.

K EY WOR DS
market, market mindset, perspective taking, social mindfulness, state empathy

1 | I N T RODUC T ION China's market economy in the past decades, it provides


an ideal research site. Therefore, the current study aimed
Not all prosocial behaviours require great effort, and to explore how the market mindset affects individuals'
sometimes just leaving a choice to others is a sign of social mindfulness in the Chinese context.
benevolence. Social mindfulness is such a low-­cost be-
haviour, which refers to people being thoughtful of
others and considering others' needs and wishes before 1.1 | The market mindset and its potential
making a decision (Van Doesum et al., 2021). Prior re- effects on social mindfulness
search predominantly focused on the impact of social
mindfulness on social behaviours (e.g., cooperation, en- The market is universally acknowledged as an effec-
vironmental performance; Dou et al., 2018; Van Doesum tive mechanism for organizing production and distri-
et al., 2021), while neglecting crucial macro social envi- bution in an economy, with its influence permeating
ronment factors that affect social mindfulness, that is, various aspects of society. Many things that used to be
the market economy. Preceding studies demonstrated considered to hold non-­market value now have a price
that when people engaged in the market economy, they (Gneezy & Rustichini, 2000). In such a context, the mar-
usually acquired the market mindset (Zaki et al., 2021; ket mechanism promotes the maximization of benefits,
Zaleskiewicz et al., 2020). This mindset typically drives optimization of efficiency, competition, and the pursuit
them to pursue maximized self-­interest, potentially erod- of material interests (Xin & Liu, 2013), all of which are
ing social mindfulness. Due to the rapid development of internalized by individuals as their own market mindset.

© 2023 Asian Association of Social Psychology and John Wiley & Sons Australia, Ltd.

Asian J Soc Psychol. 2023;00:1–12.  wileyonlinelibrary.com/journal/ajsp | 1


1467839x, 0, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/ajsp.12582 by UFSC - Universidade Federal de Santa Catarina, Wiley Online Library on [27/11/2023]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
2 |   
bs_bs_banner Asian Journal of Social Psychology
XIAO and XIN

Such a mindset describes a special mental inclination 1.2 | The mediating roles of perspective
that encourages people to primarily care about how taking and state empathy
much they can get out of their investment and whether
the repayment will be of comparable value. For instance, To gain a deeper understanding of the causal relation-
certain studies revealed that exposing individuals to ship between the market mindset and social mindful-
market relations or reminding them of their shopping ness, it is crucial to explore the potential mediating role
experiences facilitated their proportional thinking, of empathy in this relationship. According to Hoffman's
competitiveness, and self-­ i nterest (Bauer et al., 2012; empathy theory (1984, 2001) and Batson et al.'s empathy-­
Zaleskiewicz et al., 2020). In other words, individuals' altruism hypothesis (2007), both cognitive and affective
participation in the market typically results in the ac- components, also known as perspective taking and state
quisition of a self-­interested and rational market mind- empathy, can serve as motivational factors that directly
set (Olga Kuzminska et al., 2022), which may ultimately induce prosocial behaviours. Additionally, Van Doesum
influence their prosocial behaviours. et al. (2013) have emphasized that perspective taking and
Social mindfulness is a common prosocial behaviour state empathy are core elements of social mindfulness.
in daily life that stands apart from other prosocial be- Specifically, perspective taking entails a cognitive aware-
haviours due to its low-­cost nature. It requires minimal ness of another person's internal states, whereas state
mental effort to consider options for both self and others empathy entails a vicarious affective reaction to another
and to forgo one's preferences (Van Doesum, et al., 2013; person. Previous literature has shown that perspective
Van Doesum, et al., 2017). Social mindfulness is typi- taking and state empathy not only differ in psychological
cally manifested in simple behaviours, such as leaving components but also have varying effects on prosocial
or limiting choices to others who share an interest in the behaviours (Belacchi & Farina, 2012; Kim et al., 2019;
outcome of an interdependent situation. For example, Singer, 2006). Thus, the current study aims to examine
consider a scenario where you are choosing between a the functions of the two empathy components (i.e., state
single piece of chocolate cake and two slices of straw- empathy and perspective taking) in the relationship be-
berry cake, both of which are available on a buffet table. tween market mindset and social mindfulness.
Your decision would clearly impact the person behind In marketplaces, the pursuit of self-­interest and com-
you. Depending on your choice, the person behind you petition is often prioritized (Werhane, 2019), leading in-
would either have access to only one option (strawberry dividuals to become more self-­c entred and less likely to
cake) or two (strawberry cake and chocolate cake). consider the perspective of others (low perspective tak-
As no studies have specifically investigated the cor- ing). Empirical literature has demonstrated that evoking
relation between the market mindset and social mind- the market mindset had a negative impact on individu-
fulness, the relevant literature offers two contrasting als' focus on others (Genschow et al., 2019), and it also
perspectives. One optimistic viewpoint is that the mar- decreased Fortune 100 managers' willingness to con-
ket mindset may potentially enhance social mindfulness. sider their consumers' viewpoints (Van Laer et al., 2013).
Studies indicated that exposure to market interactions in Given the above, engaging in market activities may re-
developing societies fostered the adoption of prosocial duce individuals' perspective taking. At the same time,
norms and encouraged friendly behaviours among indi- social mindfulness is regarded as a human capacity that
viduals (Baldassarri, 2020; Henrich et al., 2005; Henrich, needs the skill of perspective taking, so as to recognize
et al., 2010). Conversely, a pessimistic viewpoint is that the needs of others (Van Doesum, et al., 2013; Van Doe-
the market mindset may hinder social mindfulness. Ex- sum, et al.,2017). Consequently, individuals whose mar-
tensive research has established that priming individuals ket mindset is invoked may have reduced perspective
to contemplate money or engaging in market activities taking, which can weaken their social mindfulness.
leads to more unethical and individualistic behaviours Although individuals possess the skill to recognize
and less prosocial behaviours (Falk & Szech, 2013; Vohs the needs of others in social mindfulness, the actualiza-
et al., 2006; Zhang & Xin, 2019). By considering evidence tion of such behaviour remains uncertain. According
of the two conflicting views, we have found that there is to Van Doesum et al. (2013), state empathy is also a key
more evidence suggesting the negative effects of the mar- mechanism that determines whether individuals act
ket mindset (Xin et al., 2022; Xin & Yang, 2023). Also, with social mindfulness or not, suggesting that social
the majority of studies demonstrating the positive role mindfulness is closely associated with the ability to em-
of the market mindset in prosocial behaviour are pre- pathize. Additionally, empirical studies demonstrated
dominantly correlational rather than causal in nature, that exposure to money could induce the market mind-
thereby prohibiting the elimination of the influence of set, resulting in a decline in the capacity and readiness
other variables (Xin, 2019). Based on these, we make the of individuals to rely on emotional cues (Fiske, 1992;
following assumption: Fiske et al., 2019; Zaleskiewicz & Gasiorowska, 2017).
Moreover, this market mindset fostered an impersonal,
H1. The market mindset would erode social professional, and work-­ oriented attitude, leading to
mindfulness. a decrease in warmth among individuals (Caruso
1467839x, 0, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/ajsp.12582 by UFSC - Universidade Federal de Santa Catarina, Wiley Online Library on [27/11/2023]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
MARKET MINDSET ERODES SOCIAL MINDFULNESS
bs_bs_banner
  
Asian Journal of Social Psychology
| 3

et al., 2013; Vohs, 2015). Taken together, invoking the three studies not only expand the existing literature on
market mindset can impede individuals' willingness the relationship between the market mindset and social
to be socially mindful as it temporally reduces their mindfulness but also offer evidence to contribute to the
state empathy, consequently diminishing their ability ongoing debate about the impact of the market mindset
to infer the emotions of others. on prosocial behaviours.
The current study intended to explore not only the
mediating role of perspective taking and state empa-
thy but also the configuration in which they were in- 2 | ST U DY 1: T H E E F F EC T OF
volved in the relationship between the market mindset T H E M A R K ET M I N D SET ON
and social mindfulness. Based on this, two contrasting SOC I A L M I N DF U L N E S S
assumptions were formulated. The first assumption
was that perspective taking and state empathy run in 2.1 | Method
parallel, being two relatively independent mechanisms.
This assumption was grounded in the fact that some 2.1.1 | Participants
studies have found that individuals who observe oth-
ers' suffering activate brain regions associated with Because there were no relevant previous studies, we
both affective empathy and cognitive empathy (Mas- used a medium effect size to determine the sample size
ten et al., 2011). Thus, the two processes involved in in Study 1. G*power analysis recommended that 140
the relationship between the market mindset and so- participants were needed to achieve 80% power, using
cial mindfulness may be interrelated but relatively one-­way analysis of variance (ANOVA). A total of 179
independent. However, an alternative assumption participants were recruited for this experiment via the
suggested that the market mindset influences social TC lab system and online platforms. Sixteen participants
mindfulness sequentially through perspective taking were excluded because they did not answer the test ques-
and state empathy. According to the cognitive theory tions correctly or took too little time to finish the task
of emotions (Oatley & Johnson-­laird, 1987), a person's (less than 7 min). Thus, the final sample comprised 163
emotions are not solely generated by the stimulus itself, participants (94 women) with ages ranging from 18 to 35
but rather through a process of “stimulus-­evaluation-­ (Mage = 22.60, SD = 2.64).
emotion,” which implies that cognition plays a crucial
role. In other words, when an individual becomes self-­
centred and disregards others, they also become indif- 2.1.2 | Design
ferent, leading to a reduction in social mindfulness. In
summary, two competing predictions are proposed as A one-­way ANOVA design was used, with mindset ma-
follows: nipulation (market mindset vs. non-­market mindset vs.
blank) as the independent variable, and social mind-
H2a. Perspective taking and state empathy fulness measured in the SoMi paradigm (Van Doesum
would mediate the relationship between the et al., 2013) as the dependent variable. Participants were
market mindset and social mindfulness in a randomly assigned to three conditions, with 57 partici-
parallel manner. pants in the market mindset condition which was trig-
gered by recalling shopping experiences, 58 participants
H2b. Perspective taking and state empathy in the non-­market mindset condition which was trig-
would mediate the relationship between the gered by recalling watching TV experiences, and 48 par-
market mindset and social mindfulness in a ticipants in the blank condition.
sequential manner.

2.1.3 | Procedures
1.3 | The current research
After giving written informed consent, participants
The current study aimed to investigate how the mar- were informed that the experiment had four parts. In
ket mindset impacts social mindfulness, as well as the the first part, participants learned the rules regard-
psychological mechanism that underlies this effect. In ing the SoMi paradigm to enable them to perform
Studies 1 and 2, participants were asked to recall mar- the SoMi task immediately following the second part.
ket experiences to elicit the market mindset, enabling Then, in the second part, participants were randomly
us to explore the causal relationship between the mar- assigned to three conditions: market mindset condi-
ket mindset and social mindfulness. In Studies 2 and tion, non-­m arket mindset condition, and blank condi-
3, the mediating role of perspective taking and state tion. The manipulation was introduced by assigning
empathy in the relationship between the market mind- different recalling tasks for the first two conditions,
set and social mindfulness were further examined. The as adapted from Cohn et al. (2014). Participants in the
1467839x, 0, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/ajsp.12582 by UFSC - Universidade Federal de Santa Catarina, Wiley Online Library on [27/11/2023]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
4 |   
bs_bs_banner Asian Journal of Social Psychology
XIAO and XIN

market mindset condition answered eight questions words was used as an indicator of the market mindset.
regarding shopping to recall market experiences, for The assumption of homoscedasticity with Levene's test
example, “Please describe an online shopping experi- showed non-­significant differences across three condi-
ence that impressed you.” Similarly, participants in tions in the variances on the score of market-­related
the non-­m arket mindset condition also answered eight words, F(2, 95) = 1.36, p = 0.26, which met the premise
symmetrical questions regarding TV programs to re- of one-­way ANOVA. Then, a one-­way ANOVA showed
call watching TV experiences, such as “Please describe that the score of market-­ related words mentioned
a TV program that impressed you.” Participants in the in the market mindset condition (n = 32, M = 1.69,
blank condition did not perform any task before meas- SD = 1.03) was significantly higher than that in the non-­
uring the dependent variable. market mindset condition (n = 33, M = 1.03, SD = 0.88)
When participants completed the second part, and blank condition (n = 33, M = 1.24, SD = 0.83), F(2,
they were asked to perform the SoMi paradigm in the 95) = 4.56, p = 0.01, η2 = 0.09. Also, there was no signifi-
third part. In the SoMi paradigm, participants were cant difference between the non-­m arket mindset and
required to choose one among three or four objects in blank conditions. This result implied the manipulation
a series of different categories, such as apples, candy, of market mindset was successful.
f lowers, and so on. In each category, two or three of
the objects were entirely identical, and the third or
the fourth only differed in a single aspect (e.g., one 2.2 | Results
red apple and two green ones). The number of cate-
gories varied somewhat in each trial, but participants We examined skewness and kurtosis, as well as the
always had to keep in mind that they were engaging in assumption of homoscedasticity with Levene's test
the decision task together with strangers in a dyadic for social mindfulness. The results showed that the
interaction, and that they made their selection first absolute value of skewness and kurtosis was less than
and the other person was not able to choose the ob- 1 (Orcan, 2020). Further, there were no significant
ject they selected. Instructions were repeated in each differences across the three conditions in the vari-
SoMi trial. A total of 24 SoMi trials (12 target trials ance of social mindfulness, F(2, 160) = 0.76, p = 0.47.
and 12 control trials) were presented in random order This suggested that the data on social mindfulness
(Mischkowski et al., 2018; Van Doesum et al., 2016). was normally distributed and homogeneous, showing
Choosing one of the identical products would leave the data met the premise of one-­w ay ANOVA. Then,
the other person a chance to choose either the same or a one-­w ay ANOVA was performed on social mind-
a different product, and accordingly the choice was fulness, which showed a significant main effect be-
scored as socially mindful (1); by contrast, taking the tween conditions, F(2, 160) = 3.27, p = 0.04, η2 = 0.04.
unique product would limit the other person's choice, Notably, individuals who were in the market mind-
and was scored as socially unmindful (0). The final set condition (M = 0.31, SD = 0.22) showed a smaller
score was the proportion of socially mindful choices amount of social mindfulness than those who were in
across all target trials, which constituted the depen- the non-­m arket mindset (M = 0.41, SD = 0.25) and the
dent variable. Finally, the demographic characteris- blank conditions (M = 0.41, SD = 0.24). There was no
tics of the participants were collected in the fourth significant difference between the non-­m arket mind-
part. set and the blank conditions. Figure 1 displays all rel-
evant information.
The current study was the first attempt to explore
2.1.4 | Manipulation check the causal relationship between the market mindset
and social mindfulness, revealing that individuals
An additional 97 participants were recruited via the whose market mindset was activated through recalling
Credamo platform to perform a manipulation check on market experiences showed less social mindfulness.
the manipulated materials. None of these participants This implied that evoking the market mindset eroded
participated in the follow-­up experiments. After com- individuals' kindness. However, it is possible that par-
pleting the manipulated materials, participants were ticipants' emotional states, which were triggered by
presented with word stem completion tasks, in which the manipulated materials, could have confounded
they were instructed to add a character to each of the this causal relationship. Additionally, the underlying
following to form words: “花___”, “付___”, “消___”. A psychological mechanism behind the market mindset's
research assistant, who was not aware of the experi- suppression of social mindfulness was not yet clear.
ment's purpose, coded the participants' responses. If Therefore, Study 2 aimed to eliminate the impact of
the completed word was related to the market (e.g., 花 emotions on the relationship between the market mind-
钱 [expenditure], 付款 [payment], 消费 [consumption]), set and social mindfulness, and to explore the parallel
it was coded 1, otherwise 0. The total score of the three versus sequential roles of perspective taking and state
1467839x, 0, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/ajsp.12582 by UFSC - Universidade Federal de Santa Catarina, Wiley Online Library on [27/11/2023]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
MARKET MINDSET ERODES SOCIAL MINDFULNESS
bs_bs_banner
  
Asian Journal of Social Psychology
| 5

FIGURE 1 The means of social mindfulness across three conditions in Study 1. Error bars are ±1 SE.

empathy in the relationship between the market mind- on social mindfulness (Bradley & Lang, 1994). This
set and social mindfulness. consisted of asking participants to choose one of nine
expressions from unhappy to happy. After participants
completed the SoMi paradigm, they were asked to an-
3 | ST U DY 2 : T H E M E DI AT I NG swer two subscales of the Interpersonal Reactivity Index,
ROL E OF PE R SPE C T I V E TA K I NG which was slightly modified to assess the extent of state
A N D STAT E E M PAT H Y BET W E E N empathy and perspective taking when they chose objects
T H E M A R K ET M I N D SET A N D in the SoMi paradigm.
SOC I A L M I N DF U L N E S S

3.1 | Method 3.1.3 | Measures

3.1.1 | Participants 3.1.3.1 | State empathy


State empathy was measured by the adapted empathic
We used the medium effect size to determine the sam- concern subscale of the Interpersonal Reactivity Index
ple size ( f = 0.15). Based on a priori power analysis for a (Zhang et al., 2010). This has six items, two of which are
linear multiple regression test using G*power, a mini- reverse-­scored. An example item is “I was a little embar-
mum sample of 77 participants was suggested for 80% rassed when I thought the other person had no chance
power. A total of 136 participants were recruited via to choose the unique goods.” All items are rated on a
the TC lab system and online platforms. Seven par- 7-­point Likert scale ranging from 1 (strongly disagree)
ticipants were excluded because they did not answer to 7 (strongly agree), with higher scores demonstrating
the test questions correctly or took too little time. great state empathy after recoding the reverse items. In
Thus, the final sample comprised 129 participants (59 the current study, Cronbach's alpha coefficient for this
women) with ages ranging from 17 to 32 (Mage = 21.87, subscale was 0.89. Confirmatory factor analysis yielded
SD = 2.19). fit assessments of χ2/df = 2.62, CFI = 0.97, TLI = 0.95,
RMSEA = 0.11, and SRMR = 0.03, indicating that this
subscale had a good unitary-­factor structure.
3.1.2 | Design and procedures
3.1.3.2 | Perspective taking
Since individuals who were in the blank and non-­market Perspective taking was measured by the adapted per-
mindset conditions had a similar performance on social spective taking subscale of the Interpersonal Reactiv-
mindfulness in Study 1, we chose the non-­market mind- ity Index (Zhang et al., 2010). It has five items with no
set condition as a reference condition in Study 2. Similar reverse-­scoring items. An example item is “Before mak-
to the procedures of Study 1, participants were assigned ing a goods selection, I would take the position of both
into the market mindset (n = 67) or non-­market mindset sides.” All items are rated on a 7-­point Likert scale rang-
conditions (n = 62), and were then compared regarding ing from 1 (strongly disagree) to 7 (strongly agree), with
their social mindfulness. Differing from Study 1, partici- higher scores demonstrating a greater degree of perspec-
pants' emotions at the beginning and the end of the ex- tive taking. In the current study, the Cronbach's alpha
periment were measured by non-­verbal self-­assessment coefficient for this subscale was 0.93. The confirmatory
manikins with one item to rule out the effect of emotions factor analysis yielded fit assessments of χ2/df = 3.08,
1467839x, 0, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/ajsp.12582 by UFSC - Universidade Federal de Santa Catarina, Wiley Online Library on [27/11/2023]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
6 |   
bs_bs_banner Asian Journal of Social Psychology
XIAO and XIN

CFI = 0.98, TLI = 0.96, RMSEA = 0.13, and SRMR = 0.21, taking, the results revealed a significant effect of mind-
suggesting that this subscale had a good unitary-­factor set manipulation, t(127) = −3.35, p < 0.01, Cohen's d = 0.59.
structure. Also, individuals in whom the market mindset was ac-
To verify that the items measuring perspective-­taking tivated by market experiences were less likely to think
and state empathy loaded onto two distinct latent con- about others (M = 3.48, SD = 1.46) than those in whom
structs rather than a single construct, we conducted mea- the market mindset was activated by non-­market experi-
surement model analysis to compare a unitary-­factor ences (M = 4.35, SD = 1.50). Finally, when predicting state
model and a two-­factor model. It showed that compared empathy, the results showed a significant effect between
to the unitary-­ factor model (χ2/df = 7.72, CFI = 0.74, conditions, t(127) = −2.08, p = 0.04, Cohen's d = 0.37. Con-
TLI = 0.68, RMSEA = 0.23, SRMR = 0.11), the two-­factor cretely, individuals who were in the market mindset con-
model had better fit indicators (χ2/df = 3.14, CFI = 0.92, dition rated themselves lower in state empathy (M = 3.66,
TLI = 0.90, RMSEA = 0.13, SRMR = 0.09), suggesting that SD = 1.34) than individuals who were in the non-­market
the two subscales should be used separately. mindset condition (M = 4.15, SD = 1.34).
To exclude the impacts of emotions on the results,
independent-­samples t-­ tests were performed on emo-
3.2 | Results tions which were measured at the beginning and the end
of the experiment across the market mindset and non-­
3.2.1 | Differences in all variables for the market mindset conditions (the significance of Levene's
market and non-­market mindset conditions test was great than 0.05). The results revealed there was
a non-­ significant pretest emotion difference in both
We also examined skewness and kurtosis, as well as the conditions, t(127) = −0.06, p = 0.95, Cohen's d = 0.01: for
assumption of homoscedasticity with Levene's test for the market mindset condition, M = 6.61, SD = 1.38; for
social mindfulness, perspective taking, and state em- the non-­market mindset condition, M = 6.60, SD = 1.43.
pathy. The results showed that the absolute values of Simultaneously, there was a non-­ significant posttest
skewness and kurtosis for all variables were less than emotion difference in both conditions, t(127) = −0.07,
or equal to 1 (Orcan, 2020), suggesting that all variables p = 0.95, Cohen's d = 0.01: for the market mindset con-
were close to normal distribution. Levene's test based on dition, M = 6.43, SD = 1.61; for the non-­market mindset
medians revealed significant differences among mindset condition, M = 6.45, SD = 1.49. The results suggested the
conditions in the variance on social mindfulness, F(1, impact of the market mindset on social mindfulness was
127) = 16.57, p < 0.001, and non-­ significant differences not influenced by participants' current emotions.
among mindset conditions in the variance on perspective
taking, F(1, 127) = 0.43, p = 0.51, and state empathy, F(1,
127) = 0.41, p = 0.79. The results suggested that the data of 3.2.2 | Mediation analyses
social mindfulness was heterogeneous, whereas the data
for perspective taking and state empathy was homogene- We initially expected that individuals who recalled
ous. Therefore, a Welch's independent-­sample t-­test was market experiences showed less perspective taking and
performed for social mindfulness, and likewise, stand- state empathy (as the above t-­test results demonstrated).
ardized independent-­samples t-­tests were performed for Moreover, correlation analyses showed that perspec-
perspective taking and state empathy. tive taking and state empathy were positively correlated
First, a Welch's independent-­sample t-­test performed with social mindfulness (r = 0.39 and 0.24, respectively;
for social mindfulness showed a significant main ef- ps < 0.01), suggesting there may exist potential mediation
fect between conditions, t(105.86) = −2.20, p = 0.03, Co- models (Table 1). Accordingly, we specified two poten-
hen's d = 0.39. A smaller amount of social mindfulness tial mediation models using a path analysis framework,
was found in the market mindset condition (M = 0.31, estimated in Mplus (version 8) using maximum likeli-
SD = 0.22) than in the non-­ market mindset condition hood estimation: (a) a mediation model in which the pro-
(M = 0.42, SD = 0.33). Second, when predicting perspective pensity to rely on perspective taking and state empathy

TA BL E 1 Descriptive statistics and correlations in Study 2.

Variable M ± SD 1 2 3 4

1. Prime (0 = non-­market mindset, –­ 1


1 = market mindset)
2. Social mindfulness 0.37 ± 0.28 −0.19* 1
3. Perspective taking 19.49 ± 7.68 −0.29** 0.39** 1
4. State empathy 23.37 ± 8.14 −0.18* 0.24** 0.69** 1
*p < 0.05; **p < 0.01.
1467839x, 0, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/ajsp.12582 by UFSC - Universidade Federal de Santa Catarina, Wiley Online Library on [27/11/2023]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
MARKET MINDSET ERODES SOCIAL MINDFULNESS
bs_bs_banner
  
Asian Journal of Social Psychology
| 7

were parallel mediators; and (b) a mediation model in Study 2 replicated the findings of Study 1, and showed
which the propensity to rely on perspective taking and that two potential mediation models were not validated,
state empathy were tested in sequential order. in addition to showing that the psychological mechanism
The estimated parallel mediators of perspective tak- behind the detrimental effect of the market mindset on so-
ing and state empathy are shown in Figure 2. Mediation cial mindfulness was mediated only by perspective taking,
analysis indicated that evoking the market mindset had a rather than by state empathy. However, according to previ-
negative significant effect on perspective taking (β = −0.33, ous studies, it had been indicated that state empathy could
SE = 0.08, p < 0.001), which in turn affected social mind- mediate the relationship between the market mindset and
fulness significantly (β = 0.46, SE = 0.11, p < 0.001). In this high-­cost prosocial behaviours like trust (Olga Kuzminska
model, perspective taking played a fully mediating role et al., 2022). To further examine the role of state empathy
between the market mindset and social mindfulness. Also, in mediating between the market mindset and social mind-
the standardized mediating effect of perspective taking was fulness, we adopted a new tool to assess state empathy in
−0.15. Bootstrap analysis results showed that the indirect Study 3, as a replication of Study 2.
effect did not contain 0 between the upper and lower limits
of the 95% bootstrap confidence interval (95% CI [−0.28,
−0.08]). Furthermore, evoking the market mindset had a 4 | ST U DY 3 : R E -­V E R I F ICAT ION
significant effect on state empathy (β = −0.18, SE = 0.08, TOWA R D S T H E M E DI AT ION OF
p = 0.04), but state empathy had a non-­significant effect on PE R SPEC T I V E TA K I NG A N D
social mindfulness (β = −0.10, SE = 0.11, p = 0.35), implying STAT E E M PAT H Y
the mediation role of state empathy was not established.
Similarly, the sequential mediators of perspective tak- 4.1 | Method
ing and state empathy are shown in Figure 3. It is seen
that evoking the market mindset had a negative signif- 4.1.1 | Participants
icant effect on perspective taking (β = −0.33, SE = 0.08,
p < 0.001), which in turn significantly affected social In accordance with the procedure followed in Study 2, we
mindfulness (β = 0.46, SE = 0.11, p < 0.001) and state em- used a medium effect size to determine the sample size
pathy (β = 0.69, SE = 0.06, p < 0.001). However, evoking the (f = 0.15). A power analysis using G*power suggested a
market mindset had a non-­significant effect on state em- minimum sample of 77 participants for 80% power, using
pathy (β = −0.02, SE = 0.07, p = 0.82), and in addition, state a priori power analysis for a linear multiple regression test.
empathy had a non-­significant effect on social mindful- A total of 170 participants were recruited via the Credamo
ness (β = −0.10, SE = 0.11, p = 0.35). platform. Ten participants were excluded because they did

F I G U R E 2 The parallel mediating role of perspective taking and state empathy after excluding the impacts of control variables in Study 2.
*p < 0.05, ***p < 0.001.

F I G U R E 3 The sequential mediating role of perspective taking and state empathy after excluding the impacts of control variables in Study
2. ***p < 0.001.
1467839x, 0, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/ajsp.12582 by UFSC - Universidade Federal de Santa Catarina, Wiley Online Library on [27/11/2023]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
8 |   
bs_bs_banner Asian Journal of Social Psychology
XIAO and XIN

not answer the test questions correctly or took too little/ RMSEA = 0.07, SRMR = 0.03), suggesting that the two
long time (less than 7 min, more than 25 min). Thus, the subscales could be used separately.
final sample comprised 160 participants (107 women) with
ages ranging from 19 to 57 (Mage = 31.29, SD = 8.41).
4.2 | Results

4.1.2 | Design and procedures 4.2.1 | Manipulation check

Similar to the procedures of Study 2, participants were We invited a research assistant who did not know the in-
assigned to the market mindset condition (n = 81) or the tention of this experiment to code the responses of partici-
non-­market mindset condition (n = 79), and then com- pants to the word stem completion tasks. If the completed
pared their social mindfulness. After participants com- word was related to the market, it would be coded 1, oth-
pleted the SoMi paradigm, they were asked to complete erwise 0. The total score of the three words was used as an
word stem completion tasks to check the manipulation indicator of the market mindset. The assumption of ho-
effect (“花___”, “付___”, “消___”), as in Study 1. Finally, moscedasticity with Levene's test showed non-­significant
they were required to answer two adapted subscales of differences across the two conditions in the variances of
perspective taking and state empathy. the scores of market-­related words, F(1, 158) = 0.56, p = 0.46,
which met the premise of the independent t-­test. Then,
an independent t-­test showed that the score of market-­
4.1.3 | Measures related words mentioned in the market mindset condition
(M = 1.83, SD = 0.95) was significantly higher than that in
4.1.3.1 | State empathy the non-­ market mindset condition (M = 1.01, SD = 0.93),
State empathy was measured by the adapted Empathy t(158) = 5.50, p = 0.01, implying a successful manipulation.
Concern Scales (Batson et al., 1987). We programmed a
story in which the participants had just selected a com-
modity with another person in the SoMi paradigm. Then, 4.2.2 | Mediation analyses
participants were asked to evaluate the degree of their
affective experience across four affective adjectives (sym- The correlation analyses showed that perspective taking
pathy, pity, soft-­hearted, and concerned). All items are and state empathy were positively correlated with social
rated on a 7-­point Likert scale ranging from 1 (strongly mindfulness (r = 0.39 and 0.29, respectively; ps < 0.01),
disagree) to 7 (strongly agree), with higher scores demon- whereas they were negatively correlated with the prime
strating greater state empathy. In the current study, the condition (r = −0.18 and −0.22, respectively; ps < 0.05).
Cronbach's alpha coefficient for this subscale was 0.90. This suggested there may exist potential mediation mod-
els (Table 2).
4.1.3.2 | Perspective taking Accordingly, we tested the parallel mediators of per-
The scale which measured perspective taking was the spective taking and state empathy in the relationship
same as in Study 2. In the current study, the Cronbach's between market mindset and social mindfulness using
alpha coefficient for this subscale was 0.95. a path analysis framework. The estimated mediation
To confirm whether the items measuring perspec- model is shown in Figure 4. Mediation analysis indicated
tive taking and state empathy loaded onto one or two that evoking the market mindset had a negative signif-
distinct latent constructs, we conducted measurement icant effect on perspective taking (β = −0.17, SE = 0.08,
model analysis to compare the unitary-­factor model and p = 0.03), which in turn affected social mindfulness sig-
the two-­factor model. It showed that compared to the nificantly (β = 0.36, SE = 0.10, p < 0.001). In this model,
unitary-­factor model (χ2/df = 2.93, CFI = 0.97, TLI = 0.95, perspective taking played a fully mediating role between
RMSEA = 0.11, SRMR = 0.04), the two-­factor model had the market mindset and social mindfulness. Also, the
better fit indicators (χ2/df = 1.73, CFI = 99, TLI = 0.98, standardized mediating effect of perspective taking was

TA BL E 2 Descriptive statistics and correlations in Study 3.

Variable M ± SD 1 2 3 4

1. Prime (0 = non-­market mindset, 1 = market –­ 1


mindset)
2. Social mindfulness 0.36 ± 0.25 −0.16* 1
3. Perspective taking 3.77 ± 1.58 −0.18* 0.39** 1
4. State empathy 3.75 ± 1.50 −0.22** 0.29** 0.77** 1
*p < 0.05; **p < 0.01.
1467839x, 0, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/ajsp.12582 by UFSC - Universidade Federal de Santa Catarina, Wiley Online Library on [27/11/2023]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
MARKET MINDSET ERODES SOCIAL MINDFULNESS
bs_bs_banner
  
Asian Journal of Social Psychology
| 9

F I G U R E 4 The parallel role model of perspective taking and state empathy after excluding the impacts of control variables in Study 3.
*p < 0.05, **p < 0.01, ***p < 0.001.

−0.06. Bootstrap analysis results showed that the indi- participating in market activities, which leads to the
rect effect did not contain 0 between the upper and lower development of the market mindset. Such a market
limits of the 95% bootstrap CI (95% CI = [−0.16, −0.01]). mindset emphasizes competition, rationality, logical
Furthermore, evoking the market mindset had a signifi- thinking, efficiency, and equal exchange, often leading
cant effect on state empathy (β = −0.21, SE = 0.08, p < 0.01), individuals to prioritize self-­interest as the appropri-
but state empathy had a non-­significant effect on social ate behaviour (Fosgaard et al., 2017) and ultimately re-
mindfulness (β = −0.02, SE = 0.10, p = 0.82), implying the sulting in individuals behaving unethically or reducing
mediation role of state empathy was not established. trust, cooperation, etc. (Brandts & Riedl, 2020; Falk &
Study 3 replicated the findings of Study 2. Compared Szech, 2013; Vohs et al., 2006; Xin & Liu, 2013; Zhang
to Study 2, it added a manipulation check, as well as & Xin, 2019). Combined with previous research, our
new tools to measure state empathy. The results showed results found that evoking the market mindset not only
that our manipulation check was successful. Meanwhile, hurts prosocial behaviours that require significant ef-
it supported once again the view that perspective tak- fort (Bartling et al., 2014; Kirchler et al., 2015), but also
ing was the underlying mechanism between the mar- inhibits low-­c ost prosocial behaviours such as social
ket mindset and social mindfulness, rather than state mindfulness (Van Doesum, et al., 2013; Van Doesum,
empathy. et al., 2017).

5 | DI SC US SION 5.2 | Psychological mechanism of the market


mindset's influence on social mindfulness
We designed three experiments to examine the influ-
ence of the market mindset on social mindfulness, as More strikingly, the present research proposed per-
well as to explore the underlying psychological mecha- spective taking as the underlying mechanism for the
nism. It was found that individuals who recalled mar- negative impacts of the market mindset on social mind-
ket experiences showed less social mindfulness than fulness. As noted by Van Doesum et al. (2021), social
those who recalled watching TV experiences (Stud- mindfulness is a manifestation of interpersonal be-
ies 1 and 2) or those in the blank condition (Study 1). nevolence that necessitates fundamental perspective
Furthermore, Studies 2 and 3 demonstrated that per- taking. This form of perspective taking underscores
spective taking was the underlying mechanism in the an individual's ability to recognize the needs of others,
relationship between the market mindset and social which is regarded as a skill. Theoretically, exposure to
mindfulness, rather than state empathy. market practices tends to decrease reliance on others
and increase self-­c entredness while diminishing sensi-
tivity to the perspectives of others (Vohs et al., 2008;
5.1 | Relationship between the market Wang et al., 2020). Similarly, perspective taking is criti-
mindset and social mindfulness cal to prosocial behaviours (Bensalah et al., 2016; Os-
wald, 2002). Therefore, when people's market mindset
The present study is inconsistent with the optimistic is activated through recalling market experiences, it
perspective, but tends to concur with the pessimis- blocks them from thinking from the viewpoint of oth-
tic view, supporting the assumption that evoking the ers, indicating that evoking the market mindset coun-
market mindset has a negative impact on individu- ters the capability of perspective taking.
als' social mindfulness towards others. In fact, as the Additionally, the findings revealed that evoking the
marketization process advances, more people are market mindset can decrease state empathy, but this
1467839x, 0, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/ajsp.12582 by UFSC - Universidade Federal de Santa Catarina, Wiley Online Library on [27/11/2023]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
10 |   
bs_bs_banner Asian Journal of Social Psychology
XIAO and XIN

feeling does not influence social mindfulness. On the 6 | CONC LUSION


one hand, research has shown that the market mindset
can prompt individuals to interact with others in a more The three studies of the current contribution explored
indifferent manner (Vohs, 2015; Wang et al., 2020), since the impact of the market mindset on social mindfulness
the nature of the market accentuates logic, competency, and its underlying psychological mechanism. Results re-
and self-­interest over emotions (Gasiorowska & Zales- vealed that individuals who were activating the market
kiewicz, 2021). On the other hand, the current study did mindset showed less social mindfulness. Furthermore,
not find a correlation between state empathy and so- perspective taking, rather than state empathy, played a
cial mindfulness in the mediation model. These results mediating role in the relationship between the market
seem to be in contrast to those of previous studies (Olga mindset and social mindfulness. In summary, the mar-
Kuzminska et al., 2022; Van Doesum et al., 2013). This ket mindset erodes people's social mindfulness via per-
may be due to the fact that compared with high-­c ost spective taking.
prosocial behaviour, low-­c ost prosocial behaviour re-
quires less emotional involvement and a more cognitive AU T HOR C ON T R I BU T ION S
role. In other words, it indicates that cognitive empathy Huiwen Xiao: Conceptualization; data curation; formal
(perspective taking) has a more significant influence analysis; investigation; writing –­original draft. Ziqiang
on social mindfulness than affective empathy (state Xin: Conceptualization; formal analysis; funding acqui-
empathy). sition; methodology; project administration; supervi-
sion; validation; writing –­review and editing.

5.3 | Theoretical and practical implications F U N DI NG I N F OR M AT ION


This paper was supported by the Fundamental Research
As a theoretical contribution, the current study ex- Funds for the Central Universities and the Research
tended the literature on the relationship between the Funds of Renmin University of China (no. 22XNKJ01).
market mindset and social mindfulness, supporting the
idea of the negative consequences of the market mind- C ON F L IC T OF I N T E R E ST STAT E M E N T
set while refuting views of its positive effects. From a The authors have no conflicts of interest.
practical perspective, the present three studies remind
policymakers to pay attention to the adverse effects of DATA AVA I L A B I L I T Y STAT E M E N T
marketization on low-­c ost prosocial behaviour. Tradi- The data are available at our OSF page (https://osf.io/
tional Chinese culture has always emphasized the im- ck7dv/?view_only=b17b7e466d304ee5a748d8b422cefac5)
portance of “performing good deeds, no matter how
small they may be.” However, this belief has been un- E T H IC S STAT E M E N T
dermined with the development of the market economy. The all studies were approved by the Institutional Re-
Therefore, policymakers should take proactive meas- view Board of Department of Psychology, Renmin Uni-
ures to mitigate the negative effects of the market mind- versity of China.
set. Furthermore, since perspective taking serves as a
psychological mechanism, it is crucial to develop and R E S E A RC H M AT E R I A L S STAT E M E N T
implement perspective taking intervention programs Research materials can be made available upon request
(Cigala et al., 2015) to counteract the negative effects of from the corresponding author.
the market mindset.
ORC I D
Huiwen Xiao https://orcid.org/0000-0003-4312-978X
5.4 | Limitations and future directions Ziqiang Xin https://orcid.org/0000-0001-9983-2322

There are several limitations in this study that can be ad- R EF ER ENCE S
dressed by future research. First, although we have con- Baldassarri, D. (2020). Market integration accounts for local variation
firmed a causal relationship between the market mindset in generalized altruism in a nationwide lost-­letter experiment.
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United
and social mindfulness through recalling shopping expe-
States of America, 117(6), 2858–2863. https://doi.org/10.1073/
riences, the ecological validity needs verification. Fur- pnas.18199​34117
ther research could investigate the relationship between Bartling, B., Weber, R. A., & Yao, L. (2014). Do markets erode social
the market mindset and social mindfulness via behav- responsibility? Quarterly Journal of Economics, 130(1), 219–266.
ioural priming or by exposing participants to a real mar- https://doi.org/10.1093/qje/qju031
Batson, C. D., Eklund, J. H., Chermok, V. L., Hoyt, J. L., & Ortiz,
ketplace. Second, no intervention that counteracts the
B. G. (2007). An additional antecedent of empathic concern:
deleterious effects of the market mindset on social mind- Valuing the welfare of the person in need. Journal of Personality
fulness was examined in this research. Therefore, future and Social Psychology, 93(1), 65–74. https://doi.org/10.1037/002
studies should strive to find efficacious solutions. 2-­3514.93.1.65
1467839x, 0, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/ajsp.12582 by UFSC - Universidade Federal de Santa Catarina, Wiley Online Library on [27/11/2023]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
MARKET MINDSET ERODES SOCIAL MINDFULNESS
bs_bs_banner
  
Asian Journal of Social Psychology
| 11

Batson, C. D., Fultz, J., & Schoenrade, P. A. (1987). Distress and em- societies. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 28(6), 795–815. https://
pathy: Two qualitatively distinct vicarious emotions with dif- doi.org/10.1017/S0140​525X0​5000142
ferent motivational consequences. Journal of Personality, 55(1), Henrich, J., Ensminger, J., McElreath, R., Barr, A., Barrett, C.,
19–39. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-­6494.1987.tb004​26.x Bolyanatz, A., Cardenas, J. C., Gurven, M., Gwako, E., Henrich,
Bauer, M. A., Wilkie, J. E. B., Kim, J. K., & Bodenhausen, G. V. N., Lesorogol, C., Marlowe, F., Tracer, D., & Ziker, J. (2010).
(2012). Cuing consumerism: Situational materialism undermines Markets, religion, community size, and the evolution of fair-
personal and social well-­b eing. Psychological Science, 23(5), 517– ness and punishment. Science, 327(5972), 1480–1484. https://doi.
523. https://doi.org/10.1177/09567​97611​429579 org/10.1126/scien​c e.1182238
Belacchi, C., & Farina, E. (2012). Feeling and thinking of others: Hoffman, M. L. (1984). Interaction of affect and cognition in empathy.
Affective and cognitive empathy and emotion comprehension in In C. E. Izard, J. Kagan, & R. B. Zajonc (Eds.), Emotions, cogni-
prosocial/hostile preschoolers. Aggressive Behavior, 38(2), 150– tion, and behavior (pp. 103–131). Cambridge University Press.
165. https://doi.org/10.1002/ab.21415 Hoffman, M. L. (2001). Empathy and moral development: Implications
Bensalah, L., Caillies, S., & Anduze, M. (2016). Links among cogni- for caring and justice. Cambridge University Press.
tive empathy, theory of mind, and affective perspective taking by Kim, E. K., You, S., & Knox, J. (2019). The mediating effect of
young children. The Journal of Genetic Psychology, 177(1), 17–31. empathy on the relation between child self-­expressiveness in
https://doi.org/10.1080/00221​325.2015.1106438 family and prosocial behaviors. Journal of Child and Family
Bradley, M. M., & Lang, P. J. (1994). Measuring emotion: The self-­ Studies, 29(2), 1572–1581. https://doi.org/10.1007/s1082​6 -­019-­
assessment manikin and the semantic differential. Journal of 01676​-­2
Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 25(1), 49–59. Kirchler, M., Huber, J., Stefan, M., & Sutter, M. (2015). Market de-
https://doi.org/10.1016/0005-­7916(94)90063​-­9 sign and moral behavior. Management Science, 62(9), 2615–2625.
Brandts, J., & Riedl, A. (2020). Market interaction and efficient co- https://doi.org/10.1287/mnsc.2015.2246
operation. European Economic Review, 121, 103318. https://doi. Masten, C. L., Morelli, S. A., & Eisenberger, N. I. (2011). An fMRI in-
org/10.1016/j.euroe​c orev.2019.103318 vestigation of empathy for ‘social pain’ and subsequent prosocial
Caruso, E. M., Vohs, K. D., Baxter, B., & Waytz, A. (2013). Mere expo- behavior. NeuroImage, 55(1), 381–388. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
sure to money increases endorsement of free-­market systems and neuro​i mage.2010.11.060
social inequality. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, Mischkowski, D., Thielmann, I., & Glöckner, A. (2018). Think it
142(2), 301–306. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0029288 through before making a choice? Processing mode does not
Cigala, A., Mori, A., & Fangareggi, F. (2015). Learning others' point influence social mindfulness. Journal of Experimental Social
of view: Perspective taking and prosocial behaviour in pre- Psychology, 74, 85–97. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2017.09.001
schoolers. Early Child Development and Care, 185(8), 1199–1215. Oatley, K., & Johnson-­laird, P. N. (1987). Towards a cognitive the-
https://doi.org/10.1080/03004​430.2014.987272 ory of emotions. Cognition and Emotion, 1(1), 29–50. https://doi.
Cohn, A., Fehr, E., & Maréchal, M. A. (2014). Business culture and org/10.1080/02699​93870​8408362
dishonesty in the banking industry. Nature, 516(7529), 86–89. Olga Kuzminska, A., Gasiorowska, A., & Zaleskiewicz, T. (2022).
https://doi.org/10.1038/natur​e13977 Market hinders interpersonal trust: The exposure to market
Dou, K., Wang, Y. J., Li, J. B., Li, J. J., & Nie, Y. G. (2018). Perceiving relationships makes people trust less through elevated propor-
high social mindfulness during interpersonal interaction tional thinking and reduced state empathy. Quarterly Journal
promotes cooperative behaviours. Asian Journal of Social of Experimental Psychology, 76(8), 1830–1851. https://doi.
Psychology, 21(1), 97–106. https://doi.org/10.1111/ajsp.12210 org/10.1177/17470​21822​1126416
Falk, A., & Szech, N. (2013). Morals and markets. Science, 340(6133), Orcan, F. (2020). Parametric or non-­parametric: Skewness to test normal-
707–711. https://doi.org/10.1126/scien​c e.1231566 ity for mean comparison. International Journal of Assessment Tools
Fiske, A. P. (1992). The four elementary forms of sociality: Framework in Education, 7(2), 255–265. https://doi.org/10.21449/​ijate.656077
for a unified theory of social relations. Psychological Review, Oswald, P. A. (2002). The interactive effects of affective demeanor,
99(4), 689–723. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-­295X.99.4.689 cognitive processes, and perspective-­t aking focus on helping be-
Fiske, A. P., Seibt, B., & Schubert, T. (2019). The sudden devotion havior. The Journal of Social Psychology, 142(1), 120–132. https://
emotion: Kama muta and the cultural practices whose func- doi.org/10.1080/00224​54020​9603890
tion is to evoke it. Emotion Review, 11(1), 74–86. https://doi. Singer, T. (2006). The neuronal basis and ontogeny of empathy and
org/10.1177/17540​73917​723167 mind reading: Review of literature and implications for future
Fosgaard, M. R., Fosgaard, T. R., & Foss, N. J. (2017). Consumer or research. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 30(6), 855–
citizen? Prosocial behaviors in markets and non-­markets. Social 863. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubi​orev.2006.06.011
Choice and Welfare, 49(2), 231–253. https://doi.org/10.1007/s0035​ Van Doesum, N. J., Murphy, R. O., Gallucci, M., Aharonov-­Majar, E.,
5-­017-­1058-­4 Athenstaedt, U., Au, W. T., Bai, L., Böhm, R., Bovina, I., Buchan,
Gasiorowska, A., & Zaleskiewicz, T. (2021). Trading in search of N. R., Chen, X. P., Dumont, K. B., Engelmann, J. B., Eriksson,
structure: Market relationships as a compensatory control tool. K., Euh, H., Fiedler, S., Friesen, J., Gächter, S., Garcia, C., …
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 120(2), 300–334. Van Lange, P. A. M. (2021). Social mindfulness and prosociality
https://doi.org/10.1037/pspi0​0 00246 vary across the globe. Proceedings of the National Academy of
Genschow, O., Schuler, J., Cracco, E., Brass, M., & Wänke, M. Sciences of the United States of America, 118(35), e2023846118.
(2019). The effect of money priming on self-­ focus in the https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.20238​46118
imitation-­inhibition task: A registered report. Experimental Van Doesum, N. J., Tybur, J. M., & Van Lange, P. A. M. (2017).
Psychology, 66(6), 423–436. https://doi.org/10.1027/1618-­3169/ Class impressions: Higher social class elicits lower prosociality.
a000466 Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 68, 11–20. https://doi.
Gneezy, U., & Rustichini, A. (2000). Pay enough or don't pay at all. org/10.1016/j.jesp.2016.06.001
The Quarterly Journal of Economics, 115(3), 791–810. https://doi. Van Doesum, N. J., Van Lange, D. A. W., & Van Lange, P. A. M.
org/10.1162/00335​53005​54917 (2013). Social mindfulness: Skill and will to navigate the social
Henrich, J., Boyd, R., Bowles, S., Camerer, C., Fehr, E., Gintis, H., world. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 105(1), 86–
McElreath, R., Alvard, M., Barr, A., Ensminger, J., Henrich, 103. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0032540
N. S., Hill, K., Gil-­ W hite, F., Gurven, M., Marlowe, F. W., Van Doesum, N. J., Van Prooijen, J. W., Verburgh, L., & Lange, P. A.
Patton, J. Q., & Tracer, D. (2005). “Economic man” in cross-­ M. V. (2016). Social hostility in soccer and beyond. PLoS One,
cultural perspective: Behavioral experiments in 15 small-­scale 11(4), e0153577. https://doi.org/10.1371/journ​al.pone.0153577
1467839x, 0, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/ajsp.12582 by UFSC - Universidade Federal de Santa Catarina, Wiley Online Library on [27/11/2023]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
12 |   
bs_bs_banner Asian Journal of Social Psychology
XIAO and XIN

Van Laer, T., de Ruyter, K., & Cox, D. (2013). A walk in custom- Xin, Z., & Yang, Z. (2023). Provincial marketization and individual
ers' shoes: How attentional bias modification affects own- financial literacy in China. Journal of Neuroscience, Psychology,
ership of integrity-­ v iolating social media posts. Journal of and Economics, 16(2), 60–79. https://doi.org/10.1037/npe00​0 0173
Interactive Marketing, 27(1), 14–27. https://doi.org/10.1016/j. Zaki, J., Neumann, E., & Baltiansky, D. (2021). Market cogni-
intmar.2012.09.002 tion: How exchange norms alter social experience. Current
Vohs, K. D. (2015). Money priming can change people's thoughts, feel- Directions in Psychological Science, 30(3), 236–241. https://doi.
ings, motivations, and behaviors: An update on 10 years of ex- org/10.1177/09637​21421​995492
periments. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 144(4), Zaleskiewicz, T., & Gasiorowska, A. (2017). The psychological con-
e86–e93. https://doi.org/10.1037/xge00​0 0091 sequences of money for economic and social relationships. In C.
Vohs, K. D., Mead, N. L., & Goode, M. R. (2006). The psychological V. Jansson-­Boyd & M. J. Zawisza (Eds.), The international hand-
consequences of money. Science, 314(5802), 1154–1156. https:// book of consumer psychology (pp. 312–326). Taylor & Francis
doi.org/10.1126/scien​c e.1132491 Routledge.
Vohs, K. D., Mead, N. L., & Goode, M. R. (2008). Merely activating Zaleskiewicz, T., Gasiorowska, A., Kuzminska, A., Korotusz, P., &
the concept of money changes personal and interpersonal behav- Tomczak, P. (2020). Market mindset impacts moral decisions:
ior. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 17(3), 208–212. The exposure to market relationships makes moral choices more
https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-­8721.2008.00576.x utilitarian by means of proportional thinking. European Journal
Wang, X., Chen, Z., & Krumhuber, E. G. (2020). Money: An integrated of Social Psychology, 50(7), 1500–1522. https://doi.org/10.1002/
review and synthesis from a psychological perspective. Review of ejsp.2701
General Psychology, 24(2), 172–190. https://doi.org/10.1177/10892​ Zhang, F., Dong, Y., & Wang, K. (2010). Reliability and validity of
68020​905316 the Chinese version of the Interpersonal Reactivity Index-­C.
Werhane, P. H. (2019). The role of self-­interest in Adam Smith's Chinese Journal of Clinical Psychology, 18(2), 155–157. https://doi.
wealth of nations. In D. J. Bevan, R. W. Wolfe, & P. H. org/10.16128/​j.cnki.1005-­3611.2010.02.019
Werhane (Eds.), Systems thinking and moral imagination: Zhang, Y., & Xin, Z. (2019). Rule comes first: The influences of market
Rethinking business ethics with Patricia Werhane (pp. 271–280). attributes on interpersonal trust in the marketization process.
Springer International Publishing. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-­ Journal of Social Issues, 75(1), 286–313. https://doi.org/10.1111/
3-­319-­89797​- ­4 _15 josi.12306
Xin, Z. (2019). Marketization and interpersonal trust decline in China.
Advances in Psychological Science, 27(12), 1951–1966. https://doi.
org/10.3724/SP.J.1042.2019.01951
Xin, Z., Liu, F., & Mu, H. Y. (2022). The instrumental use of sa-
How to cite this article: Xiao, H., & Xin, Z. (2023).
cred values in advertising and its influence. Journalism &
Communication, 29(2), 97–128. The market mindset erodes social mindfulness.
Xin, Z., & Liu, G. (2013). Homo economicus belief inhibits trust. Asian Journal of Social Psychology, 00, 1–12.
PLoS One, 8(10), e76671. https://doi.org/10.1371/journ​a l.​ https://doi.org/10.1111/ajsp.12582
pone.0076671

You might also like