Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 13

Journal of Business Research 160 (2023) 113804

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Journal of Business Research


journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/jbusres

Does polarizing personality matter in influencer marketing? Evidence


from Instagram
Mohammadali Koorank Beheshti a, 1, *, Mahesh Gopinath b, Sama Ashouri b, Saeed Zal c
a
Marist College, Poughkeepsie, NY, USA
b
Old Dominion University, Norfolk, VA, USA
c
Southern Connecticut State University, New Haven, CT, USA

A R T I C L E I N F O A B S T R A C T

Keywords: Social media platforms have become more polarizing with the emergence of polarizing influencers. This research
Influencer marketing investigates how polarizing influencers can improve the effectiveness of brand-posts with the help of three ex­
Defensive motivated reasoning periments and field-data from Instagram. The results of the first experiment suggest that the polarizing nature of
Group polarization
the communication source triggers defensive motivated reasoning among fans, even when the message being
Source characteristics
communicated is non-polarizing. This, in turn, has downstream consequences on post engagement and purchase
intention. Analysis of 779 brandposts of Instagram influencers suggests that the polarization effect on post
engagement is stronger for mega (vs. macro) influencers. By exploring the role of motivated reasoning, this
research expands our understanding of the factors that drive consumers to engage with brand content on social
media. The findings suggest that marketers can take advantage of the existing polarization among online users
regarding polarizing influencers to enhance the effectiveness of their brand communication.

1. Introduction researchers have devoted considerable attention to identifying the traits


that influencers—as the source of communication—have, to exert in­
Influencer marketing is growing, and marketers are already fluence over their followers (Ki, Cuevas, Chong, & Lim, 2020). For
employing social media influencers in their online communication mix example, authenticity (Audrezet, de Kerviler, & Moulard, 2020), inspi­
or considering working with them in the future (Campbell & Farrell, ration, enjoyability (Ki et al., 2020), attractiveness, intimacy (Yuan &
2020) as an important component of the firms’ digital marketing strat­ Lou, 2020), perceived credibility (Yuan & Lou, 2020), prestige and
egy (Hughes, Swaminathan, & Brooks, 2019). Influencer marketing is informativeness (Ki & Kim, 2019) have been found to influence the
becoming so popular that media agencies often refer to the use of social effectiveness of influencers. A recent development in social media is
media influencers when asked about their vision for the future of social group polarization. This research explores this important concept in
media marketing (Voorveld, 2019). Social media influencers are people social media by studying how polarization—as a characteristic of the
known to have large social networks of followers (Abidin, 2016), with influencer—affects their effectiveness in marketing communication.
an ability to influence (Dhanesh & Duthler, 2019) and monetize (Abidin, Over the last few decades, American society has become more
2016) their followers. Social media influencers have been segmented biased, polarized, and sorted (Guerra, Meira, Cardie, & Kleinberg,
based on their number of followers into micro influencers (<100,000), 2013). Existing research in social sciences shows that this polarization is
macro influencers (more than 100,000 and<1 million), and mega not restricted only to political contexts but also to other settings such as
influencers (more than 1 million) (Enke & Borchers, 2019; Voorveld, non-political social media conversations (Garibay, Mantzaris, Rajabi, &
2019). Some mega influencers are indeed well-known celebrities who Taylor, 2019). Contentious social media conversations have been fueled
are active on social media. However, not all celebrities are active on by an increasing number of polarizing influencers, ranging from mega
social media to be known as social media influencer (Taillon, Mueller, influencers such as Kim Kardashian to less popular influencers such as
Kowalczyk, & Jones, 2020). Regardless of their number of followers, Trisha Paytas. For instance, although for a lot of people Kim Kardashian

* Corresponding author.
E-mail addresses: mohammadali.koorankbeheshti@marist.edu (M. Koorank Beheshti), mgopinat@odu.edu (M. Gopinath), sashouri@odu.edu (S. Ashouri), zals1@
southernct.edu (S. Zal).
1
Koorank Beheshti is the last name.

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jbusres.2023.113804
Received 2 November 2021; Received in revised form 21 February 2023; Accepted 23 February 2023
Available online 2 March 2023
0148-2963/© 2023 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.
M. Koorank Beheshti et al. Journal of Business Research 160 (2023) 113804

represents the “idiocy of being famous for doing nothing” (Taylor, 2. Literature review and hypothesis development
2017), her fans love and appreciate her as an “ingenious marketing
mind” (Sankara, 2017). Justin Bieber is another well-known example of 2.1. Group polarization
a polarizing influencer. While his fans strongly support and appreciate
him for having “a heart of gold” (Na, 2019), to his opponents, he is an Academic marketing research on this topic has primarily focused on
egocentric person who does not follow social norms and is mean to studying polarizing brands (Ramírez et al., 2019), which are defined as
others (Sisodiya, 2016). These kinds of disagreements among social brands that receive a wide range of attitudes ranging from love to hate
media users over polarizing influencers sometimes become so intense (Jayasimha & Billore, 2015). This stream of research has found that
that they lead to online “battles” between opposing groups. As an brand polarization leads to a clear separation between those who love
example, Justin Bieber’s song “Baby” posted on YouTube received 12 and those who hate the brand (Ramírez et al., 2019). Each group’s
million “dislikes” and 18 million “likes”, which made this post the sec­ members clearly separate themselves from others who have exactly the
ond most “disliked” video in the history of YouTube. opposite feelings toward the same brand (Popp, Germelmann, & Jung,
Although the effect of polarization on consumers and social media 2016). While proponents (brand lovers) demonstrate strong positive
users has received considerable attention from brand love/hate litera­ attitudes and behaviors such as high levels of brand recognition and
ture (Ramírez, Veloutsou, & Morgan-Thomas, 2019) and political psy­ loyalty (Wolter, Brach, Cronin, & Bonn, 2016), opponents show extreme
chology researchers (e.g., Guerra et al., 2013), the marketing field is yet negative attitudes and behaviors such as willingness to harm the brand
to examine in detail how brand communication outcomes may differ (Dalakas & Melancon, 2012).
when brand endorsement comes from an influencer with a high (vs. low) Consistent with the literature on polarizing brands, we define
degree of polarization. Polarizing influencers differ from controversial polarizing influencers as a type of influencer who instigates a wide range
celebrities who received considerable attention from marketing of both negative and positive attitudes towards themselves (Winter,
scholars. Research on controversial celebrities often focuses on instances 2011) among social media users. For example, while a group of people
where a celebrity is embroiled in a scandal or misconduct (Carrillat, may extremely love a polarizing influencer, others may extremely hate
O’Rourke, & Plourde, 2019). However, a polarizing influencer divides the influencer. Therefore, polarizing influencers on social media divide
the audience into two opposing groups because of his/her specific life­ their audiences into two opposing groups. This phenomenon is called
style, communication style, partisanship, or opinions. This paucity of group polarization (Iyengar & Westwood, 2015). These subgroups have
knowledge on the marketing effect of polarizing influencers represents conflicting and contrasting positions, goals, or viewpoints, with only a
an important research gap, given the significant marketing expenditures small number holding a neutral or intermediate position (Isenberg,
dedicated to influencer marketing (Campbell & Farrell, 2020) and the 1986). The presence of homogeneity within a group and heterogeneity
increasingly polarizing nature of social media dialogues (Garibay et al., among groups (Clark, 2009), strong feelings such as in-group favoritism
2019). and out-group hatred (Iyengar & Westwood, 2015), and a tendency for
This research makes a number of contributions to marketing litera­ in-group conformity are among the other characteristics of polarized
ture and practice. First, we contribute to existing research on the impact contexts (Ramírez et al., 2019).
of influencer traits on influencer marketing effectiveness. Previous The polarizing nature of social media platforms intensifies attitude
research in this area has often focused on positive influencer personality polarization among both proponents and opponents of polarizing dis­
traits such as authenticity (Audrezet et al., 2020) and inspiration (Ki cussions (Yarchi, Baden, & Kligler-Vilenchik, 2021). This attitude po­
et al., 2020). The current research introduces a personality trait, po­ larization can affect individuals’ judgments and evaluations of
larization, that is not always seen as positive, but can be surprisingly information (Lau, Andersen, Ditonto, Kleinberg, & Redlawsk, 2017). In
effective in brand endorsements by influencers. Second, previous the presence of two competing arguments, such as on polarized social
research in social psychology shows that people engage in defensive media, people tend to rate the quality and value of supporting argu­
motivated reasoning when evaluating a polarizing message (e.g., Hart, ments more favorably than counterarguments (Asker & Dinas, 2019;
Nisbet, & Shanahan, 2011). We extend such findings to suggest that even Han & Federico, 2018). Influencers are employed by marketers to
when a message itself is not polarizing, the polarizing nature of the persuade their fans in favor of the brands (Ki & Kim, 2019). So, it is
communication source (in this case, influencers) can trigger defensive important to study how this biased processing of information affects the
motivated reasoning. In doing so, we enrich the motivated reasoning persuasive power of influencers who have a high degree of polarization
literature and suggest additional factors that may be at work. Third, compared to those who have a low degree of polarization.
previous research on social media influencers primarily focused on the
fans of the influencers when they studied the persuasiveness of influ­ 2.2. Group polarization and defensive motivated reasoning
encers on their followers. The followers of an influencer can like, dislike,
or be indifferent to them. This important differentiation was not made in According to motivated reasoning theory (Kunda, 1990), consumers
prior works. The effect of an influencer on supporters, opponents and are motivated to process information and make judgments in either a
neutrals is distinctly different. The last two experiments of this research biased or unbiased manner (Einwiller, Fedorikhin, Johnson, & Kamins,
add more nuance to the understanding of the effectiveness of influencers 2006; Mazursky & Ganzach, 1998). The biased information processing
with a high (vs. low) degree of polarization by studying their impact on manner has been conceptualized as defensive motivated reasoning
opponents and neutrals. Finally, the findings from this research offer (Kunda, 1990). Balcetis and Dunning (2006) showed that, as a result of
valuable insights to marketing practitioners, suggesting an opportunity this process, people are likely to interpret an uncertain situation in a way
to leverage disagreements among social media users over polarizing that is consistent with their desired outcome. Linking motivated
influencers. Our experiments show that the fans of influencers with a reasoning to consumers’ evaluation of a product, MacInnis and De Mello
high (vs. low) degree of polarization evaluate the influencers’ argument (2005, p. 6) defined defensive motivated reasoning as “a desire to think
in favor of a brand more positively. The resulting support from the fans about and evaluate information in a way that supports a particular
of polarizing influencers and the subsequent favorable behavioral out­ directional conclusion.” They further concluded that the activation of
comes can make such influencers an effective choice for persuasive defensive motivated reasoning may prevent consumers from making an
brand communications. objective evaluation of a product and lead to a biased evaluation of the
product quality. Consistent with MacInnis and De Mello (2005), we
define defensive motivated reasoning as a desire to evaluate information
in a way that supports a particular partisan stance and adherence to that
stance against opponents.

2
M. Koorank Beheshti et al. Journal of Business Research 160 (2023) 113804

Research often states that individuals’ defensive motivated et al., 2018). Second, the presence of an opposing group against polar­
reasoning is triggered when facing a polarizing message or issue, leading izing influencers motivates the influencers’ fans to express their support
them to come up with a conclusion consistent with their commitments by engaging with their posts on social media. This engagement helps
including social identities, beliefs, group membership, or partisan users differentiate themselves from the opposing group, which in turn
stances (Kahan, 2012; Levendusky, 2013) since polarized opinions are can help them clarify and articulate their self-views.
highly biased (Mouw & Sobel, 2001). In particular, it is well established H2: Fans of influencers with a high (vs. low) degree of polarization engage
that in the presence of a competing group (out-group), people show more with the influencer’s brand-post.
strong in-group biases (Nicholson, 2012). For example, Hickman and
Ward (2013) assert that supporters of polarizing brands express strong 2.4. Attitude and purchase intention
acceptance toward in-group members and strong rejection toward de­
tractors. According to self-categorization theory, when group polariza­ Attitude towards the promoted product and purchase intention (PI)
tion occurs, people tend to conform to the norms and positions of the are two important outcomes for brands collaborating with social media
group to which they belong. Rather than the average position of the influencers (Reinikainen, Tan, Luoma-aho, & Salo, 2021). As per the
group, this norm is the prototypical position of the group. This typical ABC [Affect, Behavior, Cognition] model, attitudes have three founda­
position of the group can be its initial attitude, one of its extreme atti­ tions: affect or emotion, behavior, and cognitions. The cognitive foun­
tudes, or a view of the influential members of the group (Abrams, dation of attitudes could originate from active information processing
Wetherell, Cochrane, Hogg, & Turner, 1990). Self-categorization theory (Breckler, 1984). In the previous sections, we argued that fans of
further states that the influence of a view depends on interpersonal in­ influencers with a high (vs. low) degree of polarization engage in more
fluences among group members, such that more powerful members have defensive motivated reasoning and evaluate the argument of the influ­
a greater influence (Friedkin, 1999). encer to be more valid and compelling. This suggests that fans of
Combining the discussions above, the presence of an opponent group influencers with a high (vs. low) degree of polarization are more
should trigger fans of an influencer to engage intensely in defensive confident in the product information that the influencer is communi­
motivated reasoning to conform to the argument of the influencer, who cating. This biased information processing should affect fans’ attitudes
is the prime member of the group, even when the message itself is not through the cognitive foundation. Moreover, we discussed that the
polarizing. This biased information processing will help the supporters presence (vs. absence) of a strong out-group motivates fans to conform
of polarizing influencers to strengthen their bonds with other in-group with the influencer’s recommendations and strengthen their bonds with
members and support the position of the group against the out-group. in-group members. Both the higher level of confidence in the influ­
Therefore: encer’s suggestions and the greater tendency to conform with in-group
H1: When evaluating an influencer’s argument, fans of influencers with a members can increase fans’ intention to purchase the product pro­
high (vs. low) degree of polarization engage in more defensive motivated moted by the influencer, who is the prime member of the group.
reasoning. H3: Fans of influencers with a high (vs. low) degree of polarization
generate a more favorable attitude towards the promoted product.
2.3. Group polarization and post engagement H4: Fans of influencers with a high (vs. low) degree of polarization
generate a higher purchase intention towards the promoted product.
Our key dependent variable for the experimental studies and field
study is post engagement. Previous research shows that social media 2.5. The mediating role of defensive motivated reasoning
users are more likely to engage with an influencer’s post if they have a
good reason to do so (Ki & Kim, 2019). For example, people are more Previous research in both marketing and political psychology sug­
motivated to engage with a post when they perceive socialization ben­ gests that activation of defensive motivated reasoning leads to down­
efits (Carlson & Dreher, 2018; Jahn & Kunz, 2012). Research also sug­ stream attitude and behavioral consequences. Bolsen and Palm (2019)
gests that the strength of social ties is a good reason for consumers to discuss that the concept of motivated reasoning has been widely used by
respond positively to brand communications on social media (e.g., Shan political science researchers to explain how people respond to infor­
& King, 2015; Hayes, Wesselmann, & Carr, 2018). Besides, social media mation, and to explain suboptimal behavior. Engagement in motivated
users value and enjoy connecting, interacting, and engaging with other reasoning has also been shown to affect consumers’ purchase and con­
like-minded consumers (Hall-Phillips, Park, Chung, Anaza, & Rathod, sumption. For example, de Mello and MacInnis (2005) showed that
2016). motivated reasoning drives consumption, as consumers buy products or
According to the tenets of structural anthropology (Strauss, 1974), services to achieve goals that they developed through the motivated
the inherent structure of human thought is based on binary opposites (e. reasoning mechanism. In another study, Park, Sohi, and Marquardt
g., hot–cold, male–female) because binary opposites help organize (1997) concluded that motivated reasoning plays an important role in
worldviews. For example, previous research has shown that as products consumers’ consideration of a vendor. Additionally, the activation of
become more polarizing, they serve as greater signifiers due to the defensive motivated reasoning translates into approval of the influ­
strong and differentiated opinions they generate (Rozenkrants, Wheeler, encer’s argument. In the previous section, we discussed that this higher
& Shiv, 2017). This is because polarizing products imply the presence of approval through engagement in bias processing of information affects
two opposing camps of opinions, and as a result, they lend themselves individuals’ attitude towards the promoted product. On the other hand,
naturally to signifying one’s characteristics. Moreover, research in­ social media users show their approval of a post or content by liking and
dicates that expressing an opinion that differentiates one from a group of commenting on the post. Hence, we expect that the effect of polarization
people can help individuals clarify and articulate their self-views on post engagement, attitude and purchase intention will be mediated
(Schultz, Kleine, & Kernan, 1989). For example, Rooderkerk and Pau­ through defensive motivated reasoning.
wels (2016) found that LinkedIn users are more likely to comment on the H5: The effect of polarization degree on (a) post engagement, (b) atti­
controversial contents that differentiate them from others. Similarly, tude, and (c) purchase intention is mediated by defensive motivated
consumers have more reasons and are more motivated to engage with reasoning.
the content posted by an influencer with a high (vs. low) degree of po­
larization even when the post itself is not polarizing. First, engaging with 2.6. Need for closure and defensive motivated reasoning
posts from polarizing influencers provides socialization benefits for
online users, as they have the opportunity to strengthen ties and interact Personal need for closure (NFC) refers to the chronic or situational
with other like-minded consumers (Hall-Phillips et al., 2016; Hayes desire to hold a definite opinion (rather than uncertainty or doubt) in

3
M. Koorank Beheshti et al. Journal of Business Research 160 (2023) 113804

order to avoid confusion, ambiguity, or inconsistency (Kardes, Cronley, 2.7. Influencer type as a boundary condition to the effect of polarizing
Kellaris, & Posavac, 2004). Previous research has extensively identified influencers on post engagement
personal need for closure as a factor that affects individuals’ judgment
and information processing (e.g., Luttig, 2018; Nan & Daily, 2015). Previous research has shown that consumer response to influencer
Therefore, the effect of influencers with a high (vs. low) degree of po­ marketing may differ based on the influencer type, categorized based on
larization on triggering defensive motivated reasoning should depend the size of their followers. For example, Wissman (2018) findings sug­
on individuals’ level of need for closure. People high in NFC tend not to gest that the lesser-known influencers are more effective for influencer
change their minds after forming an opinion (Lee, Lalwani, & Wang, marketing practices than mega influencers since social media users
2020). Moreover, they tend to reach a judgment as quickly as possible perceive the first group as more authentic and trustworthy. Dhanik
and are inclined to maintain that judgment for as long as they can (2016) argues that influencers with a small group of followers can be
(Kardes et al., 2004) because they value certainty, predictability, and more effective than mega influencers in brand communication since
reaffirming information (Webster & Kruglanski, 1994). In fact, in­ they have a greater personal connection with their followers and higher
dividuals high in NFC tend to neglect belief-inconsistent information engagement rates. Campbell and Farrell (2020) discussed that macro
and come up with a conclusion that supports their current beliefs, values influencers have a stronger tie with their fans than mega influencers.
and partisanships (Kardes et al., 2004). The lesser-known (vs. mega) influencers connect to their audiences in
Prior research indicates that as individuals’ NFC increase, their specific ways that heavily draw on intimacy (Berryman & Kavka, 2017;
biases in favor of in-groups (Shah, Kruglanski, & Thompson, 1998) and Raun, 2018). The stronger tie between the lesser-known (vs. mega)
against out-groups increase as well (Brandt & Reyna, 2010). This sug­ influencers and their followers suggest that, in the presence of a strong
gests that driven by their biases and strong ties with a particular influ­ opposing group, fans of macro influencers are more likely to provide
encer, fans high in NFC are motivated to evaluate an influencer’s greater support for the influencer than fans of mega influencers. Given
argument in a biased manner to reaffirm their support for the influencer. that social media users often express their opinions and support through
Individuals with high NFC will be upset by an inconsistency because it engagement with influencers’ posts, we hypothesize that:
involves “a lowering of subjective confidence,” which undermines the H7: The effect of polarization degree on post engagement (H2) is
certainty they crave (Kruglanski, 2004, p. 185). In contrast, those low in moderated by influencer type such that this effect is stronger for the macro
NFC who do not have the motivation to seek certainty will not be con­ (vs. mega) influencers.
cerned about inconsistency. Rather, what is important to them is the Fig. 1 presents the graphical summary of the hypotheses.
value of the outcome of the inconsistency (Kruglanski, 2004). The
presence (vs. absence) of a strong opposing group for an influencer can
attach a negative value to the inconsistency resulting from not con­ 2.8. Overview of the studies
forming with the recommendations made by the prime member of the
group, jeopardizing the position of the fan as a committed in-group We predicted that proponents of the influencers with a high (vs. low)
member. Therefore, we expect to see a stronger effect of polarizing degree of polarization engage in more defensive motivated reasoning
influencers on triggering defensive motivated reasoning in individuals when evaluating their arguments in favor of a brand and respond more
low (vs. high) in NFC, who generally tend not to engage in biased in­ favorably to the brand-post. Study 1 examines the effect of influencer
formation processing unless that there is a negative value associated polarization degree on the fans’ evaluations of the brand-related argu­
with the outcome of the inconsistency. ment for a wide range of influencers. This study also tests the moder­
H6: The effect of polarization degree on defensive motivated reasoning ating role of need for closure. In Study 2, brand-post information of 83
(H1) is negatively moderated by NFC, such that this effect is stronger for influencers from Instagram was used to examine the role of influencer
individuals low (vs. high) in NFC. type as a boundary condition to the effect of polarization on post
engagement rate. Study 2 also tests the external validity of the findings
of Study 1. Finally, studies 3a and 3b expand on the findings of study 2
by investigating how opponents and neutrals respond differently to the
posts of influencers with a high (vs. low) degree of polarization.

Fig. 1. Research model.

4
M. Koorank Beheshti et al. Journal of Business Research 160 (2023) 113804

3. Study 1 “favorable-non-favorable” (Cronbach’s alpha = 0.96).


Purchase intention: Following (Till & Busler, 2000), purchase
Study 1 aims to investigate the impact of polarizing influencers on intention was measured by asking participants the following question:
triggering defensive motivated reasoning, and in turn, the effectiveness “How likely is it that you would consider purchasing this product?” on a
of the brand-post (post engagement, attitude, and purchase intention). seven-point scale (1 = Definitely would not, 7 = Definitely would).
This study also examines how individuals’ need for closure score may
moderate this polarization effect. 3.3. Convergent and divergent validity of the measurements

3.1. Sample and study design To test divergent and convergent validity of the multi-item con­
structs, we ran CFA on need for closure, defensive motivated reasoning,
Participants were recruited through Prolific.co and screened to be attitude and post engagement. For each construct, the average variance
active social media users from USA and fluent in English. Following the extracted (AVE) was greater than the recommended threshold level of
recommendation of Arndt, Ford, Babin, and Luong (2022), multiple 0.5 (Fornell & Larcker, 1981), supporting the convergent validity of the
attention check criteria were employed to ensure data quality (web measures. Following Fornell and Larcker (1981), we measured
appendix). After removing low-quality data, the final sample consisted discriminant validity by examining whether the square root of the AVEs
of 382 participants (Mean age = 36.11, 57 % female). At the beginning of of each construct were greater than its correlations with other con­
the study, participants were asked to write the name of a social media structs. Table 1 displays that the square root AVEs of each construct are
influencer that they strongly support and are a big fan of. The influ­ greater than the correlation they share with other constructs. This sup­
encer’s name was automatically embedded in the following scenario: ports the discriminant validity of the measures.
“Imagine that you are scrolling down your social media app and seeing a
promotional content posted by [influencer]. In this post, [influencer] is 3.4. Results and discussion
sharing his/her experience about a new product he/she just tried. The
influencer explains how amazing the experience was and how this product Regressing defensive motivated reasoning on polarization degree,
works better than the similar products in the market. At the end of the NFC (mean-centered), and their interaction showed a significant posi­
post, [influencer] encourages you to try this new product.” tive coefficient for polarization degree (b = 0.08, SE = 0.04, t = 2.15, p
= 0.03). This suggests that respondents engage in more defensive
The scenario has been developed based on the real, common social motivated reasoning when evaluating arguments made by influencers
media posts in which influencers promote a product or a brand to their with a high (vs. low) degree of polarization (H1 is supported). The re­
followers. A non-polarizing message was used in the scenario, to remove sults also revealed a significant positive coefficient (b = 0.08, SE = 0.03,
the message effect. To verify the realism of the scenario, at the end of the t = 2.30, p = 0.02) for the interaction between NFC and polarization
survey, participants were asked to rate how realistic the scenario degree. This result is in the opposite direction of our hypothesis and
appeared to them (1 = Not at all realistic to 7 = Very realistic). The suggests that the polarization effect on defensive motivated reasoning is
result of the one-sample t-test confirmed that the scenario was realistic, stronger on individuals high (vs. low) in NFC who naturally tend to
with a mean score significantly above the scale midpoint of 4 (Mean = engage in defensive motivated reasoning (Fig. 2). However, this finding
5.28, t = 69.80, p < 0.001). can be explained by the literature on attitude strength. Leeper and
Slothuus (2014, p.143) explicate that the “goal to defend attitudes is
3.2. Measurements proportionate to the strength of those attitudes, with stronger attitu­
des—those held to be more personally important or, perhaps, held with
Polarization degree: To avoid any confounding effect, the polariza­ greater certainty—demanding need for defense and weaker attitudes
tion degree was measured at the end of the questionnaire. After all other producing lower defensive motivation”. Since reaffirming judgments,
measurement questions, participants were provided with the following and supporting current beliefs, values, and partisanships (Kardes et al.,
definition of polarizing influencers. “A type of social media influencer 2004) are strong attitudes for individuals high (vs. low) in NFC, they
that one group of people strongly like and support, while another group engage in more defensive motivated reasoning in a polarized setting to
of people strongly dislike and oppose.” Then, they answered the avoid inconsistency which is an important value for those high (vs. low)
following question on a seven-point scale (1 = Not polarizing at all, 7 = in need for closure. Therefore, fans high (vs. low) in need for closure
Very polarizing): Based on the definition above, to what extent do you engage in a more biased processing of information when evaluating the
think that [influencer] is a polarizing influencer? arguments of polarizing influencers. To best of our knowledge, the
Defensive motivated reasoning: Following prior research (Han & relationship between attitude strength and defensive motivations has
Federico, 2018; Pyszczynski, Greenberg, & Holt, 1985; Wyer & Frey, not been studied in the context of polarization. This could be an inter­
1983), defensive motivated reasoning was measured by asking partici­ esting area for future research.
pants to what extent they think the argument made by the influencer is
(a) valid and (b) compelling on a seven-point scale (1 = Not at all valid/
Table 1
compelling, 7 = Extremely valid/compelling) (r = 0.79). Convergent and discriminant validity. Italicized diagonal elements are the
Need for closure. A 15-item scale from Roets and Van Hiel (2011) square root of AVE for each construct.
was used to measure NFC on a 7-point scale where 1 = Strongly disagree
Attitude Motivated Reasoning NFC Engagement
and 7 = Strongly agree (Cronbach’s alpha = 0.88).
Engagement: Post engagement intention was measured using the Attitude 0.97
Motivated Reasoning 0.81 0.94
following items adapted from Mirbagheri and Najmi (2019): 1) how
NFC 0.06 0.10 0.75
likely is it that you would comment on this post? 2) How likely is it that Engagement 0.61 0.59 − 0.01 0.90
you would share this post? 3) How likely is it that you would “Like” this
post? and 4) How likely is it that you would follow the posts related to
this product? These items were measured on a 7-point scale anchored at To test H2, H3 and H4, we regressed post engagement, attitude, and
1 = Very unlikely and 7 = Very likely. (Cronbach’s alpha = 0.88).
Attitude. Attitude toward the featured product was measured using
the following three adjective pairs (Stevenson, Bruner, & Kumar, 2000)
on a 7-point scale: 1) “good-bad”; 2) “positive–negative”, and 3)

5
M. Koorank Beheshti et al. Journal of Business Research 160 (2023) 113804

research (e.g., Leal-Rodríguez, Ariza-Montes, Roldán, & Leal-Millán,


2014; Sheehan, Jin, & Gottlieb, 2020).
The analyses confirmed H5a, H5b and H5c by revealing a significant
indirect effect of polarization degree on post engagement (b = 0.05, SE
= 0.02; CI95 = [0.003, 0.099]), attitude (b = 0.05, SE = 0.03; CI95 =
[0.003, 0.101]) and purchase intention (b = 0.06, SE = 0.03; CI95 =
[0.002, 0.126]) through defensive motivated reasoning. The results also
suggested that need for closure positively moderates the observed in­
direct effect of defensive motivated reasoning such that the indirect
effect of polarization on consumer responses is significant for the par­
ticipants with high (=7) NFC score (post engagement: b = 0.17, SE =
0.07; CI95 = [0.03, 0.30]; attitude: b = 0.18, SE = 0.07; CI95 = [0.05,
0.30]; purchase intention: b = 0.22, SE = 0.08; CI95 = [0.06, 0.38]),
while this effect is not significant for the participants with low (=1) NFC
score (post engagement: b = − 0.13, SE = 0.10; CI95 = [− 0.33, 0.07];
attitude: b = − 0.14, SE = 0.01; CI95 = [− 0.34, 0.08]; Purchase inten­
Fig. 2. Effect of polarization on defensive motivated reasoning as a function of tion: b = − 0.17, SE = 0.13; CI95 = [− 0.41, 0.10]) (see Table 2).
need for closure. The findings of this study support the argument that polarizing
influencers can trigger defensive motivated reasoning in their followers,
purchase intention on polarization degree. The results supported H2 and even when communicating a non-polarizing message. This, in turn,
H4 by showing a significant positive effect of polarization on post encourages them to engage more with the brand-post and generates a
engagement2 (b = 0.20, SE = 0.04, t = 4.60, p < 0.01) and purchase higher purchase intention.
intention (b = 0.15, SE = 0.05, t = 2.97, p < 0.001) (H2 and H4 are
supported). H3 is not supported as polarization did not have a significant 4. Study 2
effect on attitude (b = 0.04, SE = 0.03, t = 1.28, p = 0.20).
Support for H2 and H4 indicates that polarization has significant The purpose of Study 2 is twofold: (1) to address the limitations of
effect on post engagement and purchase intention while lack of support the previous experimental design study and (2) to examine whether the
for H3 shows that polarization does not have an effect on attitude. Here polarization effect on post engagement varies based on the number of
we are seeing behavioral changes without change in attitude. An followers of an influencer. In the first study, the respondents were
explanation could be that under conditions of polarization, the sup­ restricted to the fans of influencers. Study 2 addresses this limitation by
porters of the polarizing influencers engage in behavior consistent with studying the engagement rate of the actual Instagram posts that all
their partisanship even without attitude change. The relationship be­ Instagram users are able to engage with. Besides, this study tests the
tween attitude and behavior is conditional and has been questioned robustness and external validity of our findings in study 1 by comparing
from very early on (Fazio & Zanna, 1978; Regan & Fazio, 1977). Our the social media users’ responses to brand-posts of a wide array of
finding is consistent with research in cognitive psychology that has polarizing and non-polarizing influencers.
demonstrated the existence of two fundamentally different motivational
goals that structure how individuals respond to information (Kunda,
1990). These are accuracy-oriented and goal-oriented motivations. 4.1. Pretest and sample
Goal-oriented motivations prompt people to respond to information in a
way that supports their preexisting commitments to particular beliefs, In a pretest, 209 undergraduate students (Mean age = 23.68, 64 %
groups, or outcomes. The findings of this study suggest that the higher female) from a public university in the United States were asked to write
intention to engage with the influencer post and to purchase the pro­ the name of either a polarizing or a non-polarizing Instagram influencer
moted product is occurring through goal-oriented motivations. This (participants were randomly assigned to one of the conditions). This
could be an interesting area for future research. gave us 141 influencers. Using Phantombuster.com, we extracted in­
Moderated mediation test. Following Hayes and Preacher (2014), formation from Instagram posts, including the number of likes and
three moderated mediation tests were conducted [PROCESS Model 7 comments for the publicly visible posts of the 141 influencers from 10/
with 5000 iterations] with polarization degree as the independent var­ 10/2021 to 01/10/2022. Two of the authors carefully reviewed the
iable, NFC as the moderator, defensive motivated reasoning as the extracted posts and identified 819 brand-posts from 88 influencers as
mediator, and attitude, post engagement and purchase intention as the relevant for the analysis [53 of the influencers did not have any publicly
dependent variables. The procedure developed by Hayes and Preacher available brand-posts in that period].
(2014) generates a sampling distribution from an existent sample to Using the Instagram direct message tool, another pretest was con­
conduct a rigorous bootstrap test. Conditional process analysis is ducted to categorize influencers as polarizing or non-polarizing. We
required for this study as the effect of the independent variable (degree
of polarization) on the mediator (defensive motivated reasoning) is Table 2
conditional on the level of the moderator (need for closure). Conditional The effect of polarization on post engagement and purchase intention through
defensive motivated reasoning as a mediator on different values of moderator.
process analysis is superior to conventional tests of moderated media­
tion (Baron & Kenny, 1986), as the procedure developed by Hayes and Direct effect Indirect effect Total effect
Preacher (2014) utilizes the bootstrapping technique to calculate “path” Outcome: Post engagement (Y)
effects and offers a confidence interval for that effect. The path effect is Need for closure = low 0.15 (0.04)* − 0.13 (0.10) 0.02 (0.11)
considered statistically significant when 0 is not included in the confi­ Need for closure = high 0.15 (0.04)* 0.17 (0.07)* 0.32 (0.08)*
Outcome: Purchase intention (Y)
dence interval. This procedure has been widely used in marketing
Need for closure = low 0.09 (0.04)* − 0.17 (0.13) − 0.08 (0.13)
Need for closure = high 0.09 (0.04)* 0.22 (0.08)* 0.31 (0.10)*
Outcome: attitude (Y)
2 Need for closure = low − 0.01 (0.02) − 0.14 (0.11) − 0.15 (0.11)
We also ran two similar regressions of “liking” and “commenting” on degree
Need for closure = high − 0.01 (0.02) 0.18 (0.07)* 0.17 (0.07)*
of polarization. The regression produced similar results as the main analysis
using engagement as the dependent variable. *Statistically significant under 95 % confidence interval.

6
M. Koorank Beheshti et al. Journal of Business Research 160 (2023) 113804

asked 10 followers of each of the influencers to answer the following influencers.


question. “In your opinion, among the people who know [influencer The second ANCOVA, which controlled for influencer effect, used
name], what percentage of them support (vs. oppose vs. are indifferent comment rate as the dependent variable and polarization condition,
towards) her/him”. The respondents were instructed that the sum of the influencer type, and their interaction as independent variables. The re­
allocated points should be 100 for each influencer. From each respon­ sults indicated a significant interaction effect on the comment rate (F =
dent, we obtained an estimate of the percentage of people who support 3.89, p = 0.049). In contrast with what we expected, this significant
and oppose the influencers. We calculated the supporter (S) to opponent interaction effect suggests that the polarization effect on comment rate is
(O) ratio [S/O]3 for each influencer for each participant. The ratio of the significantly stronger for mega influencers than for macro influencers
supporters to opponents was averaged across the sample for each of the (H7 is not supported). A potential explanation for the stronger effect of
influencers. Then, the median (2.17) of this ratio was calculated for all polarization on mega (vs. macro) influencers’ comment rates is that the
88 influencers. Influencers with an S to O ratio greater than 2.17 were disagreements over polarizing mega influencers among online users are
labeled as non-polarizing influencers, and those with a ratio lower than more serious and intense than the disagreements over the macro influ­
2.17 were coded as polarizing influencers. encers. Moreover, as implied in their name, a larger group of social
To examine the influencer type as a boundary condition to the media users follow mega influencers and interact with their content.
observed effect of the polarizing (vs. non-polarizing) influencers on post This means that engaging with polarizing mega (vs. macro) influencers
engagement rate, the influencers were categorized based on their offers a higher opportunity for social media users to interact with other
number of followers. The first group includes 5 micro influencers with like-minded users and to separate themselves from the opposing group,
more than 10,000 and <100,000 followers. The second group includes which provides them with a socialization benefit. As discussed in the
24 influencers with more than 100,000 and <1 million followers (macro hypothesis development section, social media users are more motivated
influencers). The last group includes 59 influencers with 1 million or to engage with a post when they see socialization benefits (Carlson &
more followers (mega influencers). Since only a few micro influencers Dreher, 2018; Jahn & Kunz, 2012). Future research is recommended to
were included in the sample, the analysis was limited to the comparison further explore the link between polarization and socialization benefit.
of macro and mega influencers. This gave us a final sample size of 779 Finally, the analysis suggested that for macro influencers, the
posts from 83 influencers. Following Hughes et al. (2019), engagement comment rate of polarizing influencers is 22 % less than the comment
was operationalized with two variables: like and comment. The like rate rate of non-polarizing influencers. However, among mega influencers,
was calculated as (# of likes) * 100/(# of the influencer’s followers), the comment rate of the polarizing influencers is 173 % greater than the
and the comment rate was calculated as (# of comments) * 100/(# of non-polarizing influencers (Fig. 4). This is an important finding indi­
the influencer’s followers). cating the strong effect of polarization on boosting the comment rate of
mega influencers.

4.2. Results and discussion 5. Study 3

Two analyses of covariances (ANCOVA) were performed, controlling Study 1 showed that fans of influencers with a high (vs. low) degree
for influencer effect. The first ANCOVA was conducted with like rate as of polarization are more likely to engage with the influencer. Study 2
the dependent variable and polarization condition, influencer type, and provided evidence from Instagram that polarizing influencers enjoy a
their interaction as the independent variables. As expected, the results higher engagement rate than non-polarizing influencers. One may argue
showed that the like rate was significantly higher (F = 37.18, p < 0.001) that the additional comments on the polarizing influencers’ post might
for polarizing influencers (mean = 3. 80 %) than for non-polarizing ones have come from the opponents or neutrals. To address this concern,
(mean = 1.92 %) (H2 is supported). Consistent with the previous liter­ studies 3a (opponents) and 3b (neutrals) were designed to examine how
ature, the results showed that the like rate is significantly (F = 9.62, p < these two groups respond to the brand-posts of influencers with a high
0.001) higher for the macro (mean = 3.38) influencers than the mega (vs. low) degree of polarization.
influencers (mean = 2.33) (Fig. 3). The interaction effect was not sig­ Previous research asserts that the opposing groups (supporters and
nificant suggesting that the effect of polarization on like rate does not opponents) tend to clearly differentiate themselves from each other in a
significantly differ between the polarizing and non-polarizing polarized setting (Popp et al., 2016). We also know from previous
research that individuals tend to evaluate information in a way that is
consistent with their partisanship and group adherence (MacInnis & De
Mello, 2005). Thus, we expect that opponents of influencers with a high
(vs. low) degree of polarization engage in more defensive motivated
reasoning in the opposite direction of the supporters. This suggests that

Fig. 3. Like rate as the function of polarization and influencer type.

3
In order to get infinite numbers, denominators equal to 0 were changed to
1. Fig. 4. Comment rate as the function of polarization and influencer type.

7
M. Koorank Beheshti et al. Journal of Business Research 160 (2023) 113804

opponents of the highly polarizing influencers will evaluate the influ­ [− 0.040, − 0.001]; Purchase intention: b = − 0.03, SE = 0.02; CI95 % =
encer’s argument less valid and compelling than opponents of influ­ [− 0.073, − 0.002]). Besides, the results suggested a significant negative
encers with a lower degree of polarization. Thus, we expect that indirect effect of polarization on attitude through defensive motivated
opponents respond less favorably to the brand-posts of the influencers reasoning (b = − 0.06, SE = 0.03; CI95 % = [− 0.13, − 0.007]).
with a high (vs. low) degree of polarization as a result of evaluating the
influencer’s argument to be less valid and compelling. However, we do 5.2. Study 3b
not expect to see a meaningful effect of polarization on neutrals as they
are indifferent towards the influencer. As the name implies, the neutrals 5.2.1. Study design and procedure
align neither with the supporters nor the opponents. Study 3b replicated the previous study with neutrals. After removing
low quality data, the final sample consisted of 116 participants (Mean
5.1. Study 3a age = 33.09, 47 % female) recruited from Prolific. Participants were
asked to write the name of a social media influencer that they are
5.1.1. Study design and procedure indifferent towards. Similar to the previous experiments, the influencer
Study 3a investigates the effect of influencer polarization degree on name was automatically embedded in the scenario. As in studies 1 and
the opponents’ engagement in defensive motivated reasoning and the 3a, the result of the one-sample t-test on the realism question
consequent outcome variables. Similar to study 1, participants were acknowledged the realism of the scenario (Mean = 5.54, t = 40.43, p <
recruited from Prolific.co. After removing low-quality responses, the 0.001).
final sample for Study 3a included 210 participants (Mean age = 31.82;
45 % female). At the beginning of the study, participants were instructed 5.2.2. Results and discussion
to provide the name of an influencer that they strongly dislike and We conducted similar analyses as previous study to test the effect of
oppose. The influencer name was automatically embedded in the polarization on neutral social media users. As expected, no significant
scenario. effect of polarization was observed. The results shed additional light on
The scenario and measurements used in Study 3a were identical to the findings of our previous studies by showing that, similar to the op­
those used in Study 1, with the exception that Study 3a did not include a ponents, neutrals do not tend to comment more on the brand-posts of
measurement for need for closure. Similar to Study 1, the result of the influencers with a high (vs. low) degree of polarization. This suggests
one-sample t-test confirmed the realism of the scenario by showing that that the additional comments posted on the polarizing (vs. non-
the mean of the realism item is significantly above the scale midpoint of polarizing) influencers’ brand-posts on Instagram (Study 2) are com­
4, mean = 5.12, t = 45.09, p < 0.001. ing from the supporters of the polarizing influencers.

5.1.2. Results and discussion 6. General discussion


Consistent with our expectation, a regression of defensive motivated
reasoning on degree of polarization revealed a significant negative co­ To briefly review our results, Study 1 showed that fans of influencers
efficient of polarization (b = − 0.11, SE = 0.05, t = − 2.25, P = 0.03). with a high (vs. low) degree of polarization engage in more defensive
This suggests that —similar to the supporters of the polarizing influen­ motivated reasoning when evaluating an influencer’s argument in favor
cers—opponents of influencers with a high (vs. low) degree of polari­ of a brand. This, in turn, leads to a higher intention to purchase the
zation engage in more defensive motivated reasoning and evaluate their promoted product and to engage with the brand-post. However, we did
arguments as less valid and compelling. We conducted similar analyses not observe a significant effect of polarization degree on attitude toward
with post engagement, attitude and PI as the dependent variables to the promoted product. The results also suggested that the effect of po­
compare the behavioral responses of the opponents to the influencers larization on defensive motivated reasoning is stronger for those high
with a high (vs. low) degree of polarization. The results showed that (vs. low) in need for closure. Study 2 validated the external validity of
opponents of the influencers with a higher (vs. lower) degree of polar­ these findings by showing that polarizing (vs. non-polarizing) Instagram
ization significantly engage4 less with the brand-post (b = − 0.04, SE = influencers enjoy a higher engagement rate. The polarization effect was
0.02, t = − 2. 07, P = 0.04) and have a significantly lower intention to stronger for mega (vs. macro) influencers. Consistent with previous
purchase the promoted product (b = − 0.09, SE = 0.03, t = − 2.66, P < research on influencer marketing (e.g., Jin, Muqaddam, & Ryu, 2019),
0.01). Similar to study 1, the effect of degree of polarization on attitude our results showed that Instagram users engage more with the brand-
was not significant (b = − 0.08, SE = 0.05, t = − 1.54, P = 0.12). These posts of macro (vs. mega) influencers. Studies 3a and 3b revealed that
results shed light on the findings of Study 2 by disclosing that influencers although the opponents of the influencers with a high (vs. low) degree of
with a high (vs. low) degree of polarization do not receive significantly polarization respond less favorably to the influencer brand-post, neutral
more comments from their opponents when they use a non-polarizing social media users do not respond differently to the promotional content
message to promote a brand. We will elaborate on this finding in Sec­ of influencers with a high (vs. low) degree of polarization.
tion 6.1.
Mediation test. The following test of indirect effect was conducted to 6.1. Theoretical contribution
test the indirect effect of polarization through defensive motivated
reasoning on post engagement, attitude, and purchase intention: po­ This research makes several contributions to marketing literature.
larization degree → defensive motivated reasoning → dependent vari­ First, by introducing polarizing personality as an important source
able (PROCESS Model 4 with 5000 iterations). The analyses showed that characteristic of influencer effectiveness in brand communication, this
the observed polarization effect on post engagement and purchase work contributes to the research on the effect of influencer traits on
intention was mediated by participants’ engagement in defensive influencer marketing effectiveness (e.g., Audrezet et al., 2020; Ki et al.,
motivated reasoning (post engagement: b = − 0.02, SE = 0.01; CI95 % = 2020; Yuan & Lou, 2020). This is an important source characteristic
needed to be studied because, although social media platforms are
intensely polarized (Garibay et al., 2019), no research has examined yet
4
Similar to Study 1, we ran two similar regressions of “liking” and “com­ how group polarization on social media affects influencer effectiveness
menting” on degree of polarization. The regression produced similar results as in brand communication.
the main analysis. This suggests that opponents of the influencers with a high This research focused on understanding how and why fans of influ­
(vs. low) degree of polarization are not motivated to comment more on the encers with a high (vs. low) degree of polarization may respond differ­
influencer brand post. ently to influencer brand-post. The first experiment showed that fans of

8
M. Koorank Beheshti et al. Journal of Business Research 160 (2023) 113804

influencers with a high (vs. low) degree of polarization respond more research suggesting that when a brand becomes polarizing, the oppo­
favorably to their brand-posts by expressing a higher intention to engage nents become more intense in their opposition (Ramírez et al., 2019).
with the brand-post and a higher purchase intention. This finding is in Indeed, group polarization drives individuals to automatically reject
line with the research on polarizing brands indicating that polarizing messages made by the out-groups (Lodge & Taber, 2005). One important
brands help their supporters to find like-minded individuals and to fulfill finding of Study 3a is that opponents of polarizing influencers are not
their desires to be considered as an in-group member and interact with motivated to comment on the influencer post when the message is not
other supporters (Badrinarayanan & Sierra, 2018; Ramírez et al., 2019). polarizing. This finding contributes to the research on polarizing brands
Rozenkrants et al. (2017) declared that choosing polarizing products is and polarization on social media. Previous research states that oppo­
considered to be self-expressive and helps individuals to better express nents are motivated to express their negative comments on social media
their taste and personality. Engaging with a brand-post of polarizing when the topic of the discussion is polarizing, such as polarizing brands
influencers and purchasing the product promoted by them might be or political discussions. However, we found that opponents of polarizing
another way for social media users to connect with other like-minded influencers are not motivated to express negative opinions when the
individuals and to express their social identity. It is well-established influencer communicates a non-polarizing message to promote a brand.
that we, as human beings, tend to use binary opposites (here sup­ A possible explanation is that if the opponents want to attack non-
porters vs. opponents) to organize the world around us (Strauss, 1974). polarizing posts of polarizing influencers consistently, they need to
This categorization based on binary opposites can become so extreme put a lot of effort and time into their opposition. Study 3b indicated that
that consumers think of the polarizing brand as “a part of their real self” neutrals are not influenced by the polarized setting and do not respond
(Ramírez et al., 2019, p. 624). The higher level of support for polarizing differently to the arguments made by influencers with a high (vs. low)
influencers through liking and commenting, as well as the higher pur­ degree of polarization. Research on group polarization often studies the
chase intention toward the products promoted by influencers with a effect of polarization on one of the two opposing groups. Results of Study
high (vs. low) degree of polarization, are consistent with research on 3b contribute to this line of research by exploring how polarization af­
self-categorization theory that group polarization happens on the basis fects behavioral responses of the moderate group. The important take­
of a person’s conformity with the group (Abrams et al., 1990). In this away from studies 3a and 3b is that opponents and neutrals do not tend
case, the influencer has a prime position in the group, and when po­ to engage with the posts of influencers with a high (vs. low) degree of
larization happens, he/she can have an undue influence on the members polarization when the content is not polarizing. These findings suggest
of the group. that the higher comment rate of the polarizing influencers on Instagram
Using motivated reasoning theory, we were able to explain the (study 2) is coming from the supporters of the polarizing influencers.
higher persuasive power of the influencers with a high (vs. low) degree
of polarization. We showed that presence (vs. absence) of an opposing 6.2. Managerial implication
group motivates fans of the influencer to engage in biased information
processing, when exposed to an argument of the influencer. Although Our findings offer valuable insights to marketing practitioners on
individuals’ engagement in defensive motivated reasoning when eval­ how to utilize polarizing influencers in a positive way. First, marketers
uating a polarizing message has been widely confirmed in a broad array can achieve a higher engagement rate on social media by including
of polarizing contexts, such as climate change (Hart et al., 2011) and polarizing influencers in their brand communication mix. The results of
political discussions (Baum & Groeling, 2008), this research showed that Study 1 suggest that fans of influencers with a high degree of polariza­
the polarizing nature of the communication sources (in this case, tion (vs. low) are more motivated to support and conform to the influ­
influencers) triggers defensive motivated reasoning even when encers’ recommended brands. As we discussed earlier, supporters of
communicating a message which itself is not at all polarizing. polarizing influencers tend to be demonstrative of their support for the
Using field-data from Instagram, the second study contributes influencer to strengthen their bonds with the in-group members. This
directly to our knowledge about factors driving social media users’ can help increase the effectiveness of the marketing campaigns aiming at
engagement with brand-posts of influencers. A stream of research on creating buzz and gaining publicity on social media.
social media influencers focused on the role of the audience on influ­ Marketers are often concerned with the low engagement rates of
encer effectiveness and acknowledged the effect of positive comments of mega influencers who are known to charge over $250,000 per Instagram
followers on influencer effectiveness (e.g., Reinikainen, Munnukka, post (Santora, 2021). However, our field-data results offer valuable in­
Maity, & Luoma-aho, 2020; Rihl & Wegener, 2019). Reinikainen et al. sights for marketers about the value of mega influencers who are
(2020) studied the role of audience comments in the effectiveness of polarizing. Findings of this study showed that the comment rate of
YouTube influencers and found that influencers with active followers polarizing mega influencers is 171 % greater than the comment rate of
are perceived as more credible. This work makes a contribution by non-polarizing mega influencers. This suggests that marketers interested
suggesting that opposition of a group of audience—which normally is in working with well-known influencers will benefit more by including
considered negative—will, in fact, benefit the influencer by making the polarizing influencers in their promotional strategy.
fans more active through liking and commenting. Study 2′ s findings also The first experiment showed that fans of influencers with a high (vs.
contribute to the emerging body of research on the effectiveness of low) degree of polarization evaluate the influencer’s argument more
influencers based on their number of followers. Previous research em­ positively, when encouraged to try a new product. As a result, fans of
phasizes the higher effectiveness and engagement rate of the lesser- polarizing influencers generated a higher purchase intention towards
known influencers compared to mega influencers (e.g., Jin et al., the promoted product. Therefore, influencer with a high (vs. low) degree
2019). Our analysis of 779 brand-posts of 83 Instagram influencers in­ of polarization can be more effective in advertisements to persuade their
troduces polarization as an important boundary condition to the effect of fans to try a product. Brands aiming to promote a recently launched
influencer type on engagement rate by suggesting that, on average, product or hoping to attract a new customer base can benefit from this
polarizing mega influencers enjoy a higher engagement rate than macro finding.
influencers. Although there are important advantages to utilizing polarizing
Studies 3a and 3b widen the scope of the research by studying influencers in brand communications, marketers should be aware of a
whether and how opponents and neutrals respond differently to the possible downside of utilizing polarizing influencers to promote a brand
brand-posts of influencers with a high (vs. low) degree of polarization. or a product. Although our findings showed that the opponents are not
We observed that opponents of the influencers with a high (vs. low) motivated to comment on the brand-posts of polarizing influencers
degree of polarization engaged in more defensive motivated reasoning when the message itself is not polarizing, brands should be aware
in the opposite direction of the fans. This is consistent with previous that—similar to the case of polarizing brands (Ramírez et al., 2019)—

9
M. Koorank Beheshti et al. Journal of Business Research 160 (2023) 113804

there is a presence of a strong base of opponents, who are ready to attack Current literature on group polarization effect in social psychology
and criticize the influencer and probably everything associated with that suggests that supporters of politicians who are polarizing (vs. non-
influencer. Hence, it is even more important for brands working with polarizing) express more intense emotions (Asker & Dinas, 2019).
polarizing influencers to deliver what they promised because they are Future research may investigate if this is the same for the influencers
perpetually under the watchful eyes of the opponents. with a high (vs. low) degree of polarization. It may be worthwhile to
The findings of this research have implications for social media explore whether the possible intense emotion towards the polarizing
influencers. The results suggested that the presence of an opponent influencer will transfer to the promoted brand.
group energizes the fans of the influencer. Fans of influencers with a In the conceptual part, we discussed how polarization can affect the
high (vs. low) degree of polarization are (1) more receptive to their tendency of individuals to conform to the prototypical idea of the group
suggestions, (2) evaluate their arguments more positively, and (3) and to strengthen their bonds with other members of the in-group,
engage more with their brand-posts on social media. Therefore, instead which will lead to a higher engagement in defensive motivated
of trying to satisfy the opponent group, polarizing influencers may reasoning. Future research is encouraged to elaborate more on this
consider strengthening their ties with their fans to benefit more from the psychological process by measuring the intensity of variables such as
advantages of group polarization on social media. level of attachment and social identity.
Influencers should note that being polarizing is different from pro­ This work used a non-polarizing message with a general product for
moting hateful toxic content, a technique that is used by some social the experiments. It will be interesting to study how supporters, oppo­
media platforms to generate a higher engagement rate and thus increase nents and neutrals respond to the brand-post of the influencers with a
the time users spend on the platform. The Whistleblower Frances Hau­ high (vs. low) degree of polarization, when promoting a polarizing (vs.
gen disclosed “how Facebook’s algorithm fosters anger to drive non-polarizing) product/brand or using a polarizing (vs. non-polarizing)
engagement, how it favors high-profile users, and how Instagram made message. Field-data was obtained for this study only from one social
nearly one-third of teenage girls who use it feel worse about their media platform, Instagram. Further research can explore the effective­
bodies” (De Guzman, 2021). She added that this was done with the ness of polarizing influencers on other social media platforms such as
intention to create “division, extremism, and polarization” in order to TikTok, Twitch, Twitter, and Facebook so as to examine the generaliz­
increase the engagement rate of Facebook users. This research ac­ ability of the findings, given the demographic differences among these
knowledges the benefits of polarization for both influencers and brands. platforms. Another limitation of Study 2 is that we limited our analysis
However, distributing hateful content that leads to division and to macro and mega influencers. Future research is encouraged to extend
extremism creates serious negative consequences on social media users this study to micro and nano influencers.
including anxiety, self-rejection, depression, and eating-related medical An interesting topic for future research is to categorize polarizing
disorders (Leets, 2002). This is a practice that is to be highly discouraged influencers based on the reason for polarization and explore how it could
to both protect the vulnerable social media users and to preserve the affect the likelihood of followers to engage with the influencer’s post on
credibility of influencers. social media. For example, an influencer could be polarizing because of
competing with another athlete (Cristiano Ronaldo), initiating cam­
6.3. Limitation and future research paigns against global warming (Greta Gutenberg), or engaging in social/
political discussions (Ellen DeGeneres).
This work has a few limitations that could be avenues for future Research on group polarization suggests that exposure to opinions of
research. We studied one psychological path, defensive motivated other social media users may affect the polarization effect on defensive
reasoning, to explore the effect of polarization on influencer marketing motivated reasoning. For example, prior research suggests that in­
outcomes. Although the total effect of polarization degree on attitude dividuals will be more biased about the polarizing topic when exposing
was not significant, our results suggested the presence of a significant to the same side opinions (e.g., Sia, Tan, & Wei, 2002). However, other
positive indirect effect of polarization on attitude. This implies that the studies suggest that exposure to the opponents’ opinion makes in­
indirect effect of polarization degree through other potential mediators dividuals more polarized and biased in their primary attitude and belief
on attitude may be in the negative direction, resulting in a non- (e.g., Bail et al., 2018). Usually, Instagram and other social media
significant total effect. Future research is recommended to investigate platforms show one or two comments posted by others to the users,
other cognitive and/or emotional routes such as self-categorization when they see the post. Future research is encouraged to study how
tendency, self-concept clarity and empathy toward favorite influencers exposure to comments of other users on the influencers’ post affects the
to further explore the indirect effect of polarization degree on attitude observed polarization effects.
and other marketing outcomes through other potential mediators. This research can have cross-cultural implications. When polariza­
This research investigated the effect of polarizing influencers on tion happens in social media, people are motivated to conform to the
three important communication outcome variables, namely post policies ideas and arguments made by members of their own in-group­
engagement, attitude, and purchase intention. It would be worthwhile to —and oppose ideas generated by out-groups. Three of the Hofstede’s
explore the effect of polarization on other outcome variables such as cultural dimensions (Hofstede, 2011) may have an effect on this phe­
attitude towards the brand and brand loyalty, as long-term relationships nomenon. In the context of individualism/collectivism, the support for
with influencers are becoming more common (Reinikainen et al., 2021). in-group members is usually stronger in collectivistic cultures. However,
Future research is needed to investigate the possibility of an alter­ differentiating the self through supporting a divisive idea is more
native explanation for the observed polarization effect. There is a pos­ encouraged in an individualistic culture. If we consider power distance
sibility that fans of influencers with a high (vs. low) degree of dimension, it is conceivable that cultures having high power distance
polarization are people who in general are more receptive to influ­ will hesitate to publicly oppose influencers. As a result, the opposition to
encers’ suggestions. This means that social media users, who in general polarizing influencers is expected to be attenuated. Uncertainty avoid­
engage more with influencers’ posts and are more willing to purchase ance dimension can be expected to have an effect on need for closure.
their recommended products, prefer to support influencers with a high Therefore, the level of uncertainty avoidance may have an effect similar
(vs. low) degree of polarization. Conversely, it is possible that in­ to the need for closure on polarization, as discussed in Study 1. These are
dividuals, who do not like to conform completely to the influencers’ interesting future directions for cross-cultural studies.
opinions, prefer influencers with a low (vs. high) degree of polarization.
Therefore, the causality could be reversed, as those who like to accept CRediT authorship contribution statement
and conform to influencers’ opinions prefer to support polarizing
influencers. Mohammadali Koorank Beheshti: Visualization, Validation,

10
M. Koorank Beheshti et al. Journal of Business Research 160 (2023) 113804

Supervision, Project administration, Methodology, Investigation, Brandt, M. J., & Reyna, C. (2010). The role of prejudice and the need for closure in
religious fundamentalism. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 36(5), 715–725.
Formal analysis, Data curation, Conceptualization, Writing – original
https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167210366306
draft, Writing – review & editing. Mahesh Gopinath: Supervision, Breckler, S. J. (1984). Empirical validation of affect, behavior, and cognition as distinct
Project administration, Methodology, Data curation, Conceptualization, components of attitude. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 47(6),
Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. Sama Ashouri: 1191–1205. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.47.6.1191
Campbell, C., & Farrell, J. R. (2020). More than meets the eye: The functional
Methodology, Formal analysis, Data curation, Writing – original draft, components underlying influencer marketing. Business Horizons, 63(4), 469–479.
Writing – review & editing. Saeed Zal: Formal analysis, Writing – https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bushor.2020.03.003
original draft. Carlson, B., & Dreher, T. (2018). Introduction: Indigenous innovation in social media.
Media International Australia, 169(1), 16–20. https://doi.org/10.1177/
1329878X18803798
Carrillat, F. A., O’Rourke, A. M., & Plourde, C. (2019). Celebrity endorsement in the
Declaration of Competing Interest world of luxury fashion–when controversy can be beneficial. Journal of Marketing
Management, 35(13–14), 1193–1213. https://doi.org/10.1080/
The authors declare that they have no known competing financial 0267257X.2019.1634632
Clark, T. S. (2009). Measuring ideological polarization on the united states supreme
interests or personal relationships that could have appeared to influence
court. Political Research Quarterly, 62(1), 146–157. https://doi.org/10.1177/
the work reported in this paper. 1065912908314652
Dalakas, V., & Melancon, J. P. (2012). Fan identification, Schadenfreude toward hated
rivals, and the mediating effects of Importance of Winning Index (IWIN). Journal of
Data availability
Services Marketing, 26(1), 51–59. https://doi.org/10.1108/08876041211199724
De Guzman, C. (2021). The Facebook whistleblower revealed herself on 60 minutes. Here’s
Data will be made available on request. what you need to know. Retrieved from https://time.com/6103645/facebook-whistle
blower-frances-haugen/. Accessed April 12, 2022.
de Mello, G. E., & MacInnis, D. J. (2005). Why and how consumers hope: Motivated
Acknowledgment reasoning and the marketplace. In Inside consumption (pp. 66–88). Routledge.
Dhanesh, G. S., & Duthler, G. (2019). Relationship management through social media
influencers: Effects of followers’ awareness of paid endorsement. Public Relations
We would like to express our special thanks to Dr. Yuping Liu- Review, 45(3), Article 101765. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pubrev.2019.03.002
Thompkins for her invaluable feedback and comments on this Dhanik, T. (2016). Micro, not macro: Rethinking influencer marketing. Retrieved from htt
ps://adage.com/article/digitalnext/micro-macro-influencer-marketing-kim-karda
research. We also thank the Associate Editor Dr. Yogesh Dwivedi and shian/307118. Accessed September 10, 2021.
four anonymous reviewers for their support of this research and their Einwiller, S. A., Fedorikhin, A., Johnson, A. R., & Kamins, M. A. (2006). Enough is
constructive comments and suggestions. enough! When identification no longer prevents negative corporate associations.
Journal of the Academy of Marketing Science, 34(2), 185–194. https://doi.org/
This research did not receive any specific grant from funding
10.1177/0092070305284983
agencies in the public, commercial, or not-for-profit sectors. Enke, N., & Borchers, N. S. (2019). Social media influencers in strategic communication:
A conceptual framework of strategic social media influencer communication.
International Journal of Strategic Communication, 13(4), 261–277. https://doi.org/
Appendix A. Supplementary material 10.1080/1553118X.2019.1620234
Fazio, R. H., & Zanna, M. P. (1978). Attitudinal qualities relating to the strength of the
Supplementary data to this article can be found online at https://doi. attitude-behavior relationship. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 14(4),
398–408. https://doi.org/10.1016/0022-1031(78)90035-5
org/10.1016/j.jbusres.2023.113804.
Fornell, C., & Larcker, D. F. (1981). Structural equation models with unobservable
variables and measurement error. Algebra and Statistics, 18(3), 382–388. https://doi.
References org/10.1177/002224378101800313
Friedkin, N. E. (1999). Choice shift and group polarization. American Sociological Review,
64(6), 856–875. https://doi.org/10.2307/2657407
Abidin, C. (2016). Visibility labour: Engaging with Influencers’ fashion brands and#
Garibay, I., Mantzaris, A. V., Rajabi, A., & Taylor, C. E. (2019). Polarization in social
OOTD advertorial campaigns on Instagram. Media International Australia, 161(1),
media assists influencers to become more influential: Analysis and two inoculation
86–100. https://doi.org/10.1177/1329878X16665177
strategies. Scientific Reports, 9(1), 1–9. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-019-55178-
Abrams, D., Wetherell, M., Cochrane, S., Hogg, M. A., & Turner, J. C. (1990). Knowing
8
what to think by knowing who you are: Self-categorization and the nature of norm
Guerra, P. C., Meira Jr, W., Cardie, C., & Kleinberg, R. (2013, June). A measure of
formation, conformity and group polarization. British Journal of Social Psychology, 29
polarization on social media networks based on community boundaries. In Seventh
(2), 97–119. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-8309.1990.tb00892.x
international AAAI conference on weblogs and social media, 7(1), 215–224. Retrieved
Arndt, A. D., Ford, J. B., Babin, B. J., & Luong, V. (2022). Collecting samples from online
from https://ojs.aaai.org/index.php/ICWSM/article/view/14421.
services: How to use screeners to improve data quality. International Journal of
Hall-Phillips, A., Park, J., Chung, T. L., Anaza, N. A., & Rathod, S. R. (2016). I (heart)
Research in Marketing, 39(1), 117–133.
social ventures: Identification and social media engagement. Journal of Business
Asker, D., & Dinas, E. (2019). Thinking fast and furious: Emotional intensity and opinion
Research, 69(2), 484–491. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jbusres.2015.05.005
polarization in online media. Public Opinion Quarterly, 83(3), 487–509. https://doi.
Han, J., & Federico, C. M. (2018). The polarizing effect of news framing: Comparing the
org/10.1093/poq/nfz042
mediating roles of motivated reasoning, self-stereotyping, and intergroup animus.
Audrezet, A., de Kerviler, G., & Moulard, J. G. (2020). Authenticity under threat: When
Journal of Communication, 68(4), 685–711. https://doi.org/10.1093/joc/jqy025
social media influencers need to go beyond self-presentation. Journal of Business
Hart, P. S., Nisbet, E. C., & Shanahan, J. E. (2011). Environmental values and the social
Research, 117, 557–569. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jbusres.2018.07.008
amplification of risk: An examination of how environmental values and media use
Badrinarayanan, V., & Sierra, J. J. (2018). Inferred social approval and brand tribalism: A
influence predispositions for public engagement in wildlife management decision
tale of two communities. Journal of Product & Brand Management, 27(4), 363–374.
making. Society and Natural Resources, 24(3), 276–291. https://doi.org/10.1080/
https://doi.org/10.1108/JPBM-10-2017-1597
08941920802676464
Bail, C. A., Argyle, L. P., Brown, T. W., Bumpus, J. P., Chen, H., Hunzaker, M. F., …
Hayes, R. A., Wesselmann, E. D., & Carr, C. T. (2018). When nobody “likes” you:
Volfovsky, A. (2018). Exposure to opposing views on social media can increase
Perceived ostracism through paralinguistic digital affordances within social media.
political polarization. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 115(37),
Social Media+ Society, 4(3), 1–12. doi: 10.1177/2056305118800309.
9216–9221. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1804840115
Hayes, A. F., & Preacher, K. J. (2014). Statistical mediation analysis with a
Balcetis, E., & Dunning, D. (2006). See what you want to see: Motivational influences on
multicategorical independent variable. British Journal of Mathematical and Statistical
visual perception. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 91(4), 612–625.
Psychology, 67(3), 451–470. https://doi.org/10.1111/bmsp.12028
https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.91.4.612
Hickman, T. M., & Ward, J. C. (2013). Implications of brand communities for rival
Baron, R. M., & Kenny, D. A. (1986). The moderator–mediator variable distinction in
brands: Negative brand ratings, negative stereotyping of their consumers and
social psychological research: Conceptual, strategic, and statistical considerations.
negative word-of-mouth. Journal of Brand Management, 20(6), 501–517. https://doi.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51(6), 1173.
org/10.1057/bm.2012.57
Baum, M. A., & Groeling, T. (2008). New media and the polarization of American
Hofstede, G. (2011). Dimensionalizing cultures: The Hofstede model in context. Online
political discourse. Political Communication, 25(4), 345–365. https://doi.org/
readings in Psychology and Culture, 2(1), 2307-0919. doi: 10.9707/2307-0919.1014.
10.1080/10584600802426965
Hughes, C., Swaminathan, V., & Brooks, G. (2019). Driving brand engagement through
Berryman, R., & Kavka, M. (2017). ‘I guess a lot of people see me as a big sister or a
online social influencers: An empirical investigation of sponsored blogging
friend’: The role of intimacy in the celebrification of beauty vloggers. Journal of
campaigns. Journal of Marketing, 83(5), 78-96. doi: 10.1177 %
Gender Studies, 26(3), 307–320. https://doi.org/10.1080/09589236.2017.1288611
2F0022242919854374.
Bolsen, T., & Palm, R. (2019). Motivated reasoning and political decision making. In
Oxford research encyclopedia of politics. doi: 10.1093/acrefore/
9780190228637.013.923.

11
M. Koorank Beheshti et al. Journal of Business Research 160 (2023) 113804

Isenberg, D. J. (1986). Group polarization: A critical review and meta-analysis. Journal of Pyszczynski, T., Greenberg, J., & Holt, K. (1985). Maintaining consistency between self-
Personality and Social Psychology, 50(6), 1141–1151. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022- serving beliefs and available data: A bias in information evaluation. Personality and
3514.50.6.1141 Social Psychology Bulletin, 11(2), 179–190. doi: 10.1177 %2F0146167285112006.
Iyengar, S., & Westwood, S. J. (2015). Fear and loathing across party lines: New evidence Ramírez, S. A. O., Veloutsou, C., & Morgan-Thomas, A. (2019). I hate what you love:
on group polarization. American Journal of Political Science, 59(3), 690–707. https:// Brand polarization and negativity towards brands as an opportunity for brand
doi.org/10.1111/ajps.12152 management. Journal of Product & Brand Management, 28(5), 614–632. https://doi.
Jahn, B., & Kunz, W. (2012). How to transform consumers into fans of your brand. org/10.1108/JPBM-03-2018-1811
Journal of Service Management., 23(3), 344–361. https://doi.org/10.1108/ Raun, T. (2018). Capitalizing intimacy: New subcultural forms of micro-celebrity
09564231211248444 strategies and affective labour on YouTube. Convergence, 24(1), 99–113. doi:
Jayasimha, K. R., & Billore, A. (2015, June). Polarizing brands: An investigation in 10.1177 %2F1354856517736983.
tourism context. In 5th AHTMM conference (pp. 105–108). Regan, D. T., & Fazio, R. (1977). On the consistency between attitudes and behavior:
Jin, S. V., Muqaddam, A., & Ryu, E. (2019). Instafamous and social media influencer Look to the method of attitude formation. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology,
marketing. Marketing Intelligence & Planning, 37(5), 567–579. https://doi.org/ 13(1), 28–45. https://doi.org/10.1016/0022-1031(77)90011-7
10.1108/MIP-09-2018-0375 Reinikainen, H., Munnukka, J., Maity, D., & Luoma-aho, V. (2020). ‘You really are a
Kahan, D. M. (2012). Ideology, motivated reasoning, and cognitive reflection: An great big sister’–parasocial relationships, credibility, and the moderating role of
experimental study. Judgment and Decision making, 8, 407–424. https://doi.org/ audience comments in influencer marketing. Journal of Marketing Management, 36
10.2139/ssrn.2182588 (3–4), 279–298. https://doi.org/10.1080/0267257X.2019.1708781
Kardes, F. R., Cronley, M. L., Kellaris, J. J., & Posavac, S. S. (2004). The role of selective Reinikainen, H., Tan, T. M., Luoma-aho, V., & Salo, J. (2021). Making and breaking
information processing in price-quality inference. Journal of Consumer Research, 31 relationships on social media: The impacts of brand and influencer betrayals.
(2), 368–374. https://doi.org/10.1086/422115 Technological Forecasting and Social Change, 171, Article 120990. https://doi.org/
Ki, C. W. C., Cuevas, L. M., Chong, S. M., & Lim, H. (2020). Influencer marketing: Social 10.1016/j.techfore.2021.120990
media influencers as human brands attaching to followers and yielding positive Rihl, A., & Wegener, C. (2019). YouTube celebrities and parasocial interaction: Using
marketing results by fulfilling needs. Journal of Retailing and Consumer Services, 55, feedback channels in mediatized relationships. Convergence, 25(3), 554–566. doi:
102–133. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jretconser.2020.102133 10.1177 %2F1354856517736976.
Ki, C. W. C., & Kim, Y. K. (2019). The mechanism by which social media influencers Roets, A., & Van Hiel, A. (2011). Item selection and validation of a brief, 15-item version
persuade consumers: The role of consumers’ desire to mimic. Psychology & of the Need for Closure Scale. Personality and Individual Differences, 50(1), 90–94.
Marketing, 36(10), 905–922. https://doi.org/10.1002/mar.21244 https://doi.org/10.1348/014466610X491567
Kruglanski, A. W. (2004). The quest for the gist: On challenges of going abstract in social Rooderkerk, R. P., & Pauwels, K. H. (2016). No comment?! The drivers of reactions to
and personality psychology. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 8(2), 156–163. online posts in professional groups. Journal of Interactive Marketing, 35, 1–15.
https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327957pspr0802_9 https://doi.org/10.1016/j.intmar.2015.12.003
Kunda, Z. (1990). The case for motivated reasoning. Psychological Bulletin, 108(3), Rozenkrants, B., Wheeler, S. C., & Shiv, B. (2017). Self-expression cues in product rating
480–498. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.108.3.480 distributions: When people prefer polarizing products. Journal of Consumer Research,
Lau, R. R., Andersen, D. J., Ditonto, T. M., Kleinberg, M. S., & Redlawsk, D. P. (2017). 44(4), 759–777. https://doi.org/10.1093/jcr/ucx067
Effect of media environment diversity and advertising tone on information search, Sankara, P. (2017). Why do people hate Kim Kardashian so much? Retrieved from https://
selective exposure, and affective polarization. Political Behavior, 39(1), 231–255. www.quora.com/Why-do-people-hate-Kim-Kardashian-so-much/answer/Puga-Sa
https://doi.org/10.1007/s11109-016-9354-8 nkara. Accessed September 10, 2021.
Leal-Rodríguez, A. L., Ariza-Montes, J. A., Roldán, J. L., & Leal-Millán, A. G. (2014). Santora, J. (2021). 100 Influencer marketing statistics for 2021. Retrieved from https://in
Absorptive capacity, innovation and cultural barriers: A conditional mediation fluencermarketinghub.com/influencer-marketing-statistics. Accessed September 10,
model. Journal of Business Research, 67(5), 763–768. https://doi.org/10.1016/j. 2021.
jbusres.2013.11.041 Schultz, S. E., Kleine, R. E., & Kernan, J. B. (1989). ‘These are a few of my favorite
Lee, H., Lalwani, A. K., & Wang, J. J. (2020). Price no object!: The impact of power things’: Toward an explication of attachment as a consumer behavior construct.
distance belief on consumers’ price sensitivity. Journal of Marketing, 84(6), 113–129. Advances in Consumer Research, 16(1), 359–366.
https://doi.org/10.1177/0022242920929718 Shah, J. Y., Kruglanski, A. W., & Thompson, E. P. (1998). Membership has its (epistemic)
Leeper, T. J., & Slothuus, R. (2014). Political parties, motivated reasoning, and public rewards: Need for closure effects on in-group bias. Journal of Personality and Social
opinion formation. Political Psychology, 35, 129–156. https://doi.org/10.1111/ Psychology, 75(2), 383–393. https://doi.org/10.1037//0022-3514.75.2.383
pops.12164 Shan, Y., & King, K. W. (2015). The effects of interpersonal tie strength and subjective
Leets, L. (2002). Experiencing hate speech: Perceptions and responses to anti-semitism norms on consumers’ brand-related eWOM referral intentions. Journal of Interactive
and antigay speech. Journal of social issues, 58(2), 341–361. https://doi.org/ Advertising, 15(1), 16–27. https://doi.org/10.1080/15252019.2015.1016636
10.1111/1540-4560.00264 Sheehan, B., Jin, H. S., & Gottlieb, U. (2020). Customer service chatbots:
Levendusky, M. S. (2013). Why do partisan media polarize viewers? American Journal of Anthropomorphism and adoption. Journal of Business Research, 115, 14–24. https://
Political Science, 57(3), 611–623. https://doi.org/10.1111/ajps.12008 doi.org/10.1016/j.jbusres.2020.04.030
Lodge, M., & Taber, C. S. (2005). The automaticity of affect for political leaders, groups, Sia, C. L., Tan, B. C., & Wei, K. K. (2002). Group polarization and computer-mediated
and issues: An experimental test of the hot cognition hypothesis. Political Psychology, communication: Effects of communication cues, social presence, and anonymity.
26(3), 455–482. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9221.2005.00426.x Information Systems Research, 13(1), 70–90. https://doi.org/10.1287/isre.13.1.70.92
Luttig, M. D. (2018). The “Prejudiced Personality” and the origins of partisan strength, Sisodiya, A. (2016). Do you like Justin Bieber? Why/Why not? Retrieved from https:
affective polarization, and partisan sorting. Political Psychology, 39, 239–256. //www.quora.com/Do-you-like-Justin-Bieber-Why-Why-not/answer/Yu-Na-71.
https://doi.org/10.1111/pops.12484 Accessed September 10, 2021.
MacInnis, D. J., & De Mello, G. E. (2005). The concept of hope and its relevance to Stevenson, J. S., Bruner, G. C., & Kumar, A. (2000). Webpage background and viewer
product evaluation and choice. Journal of Marketing, 69(1), 1–14. https://doi.org/ attitudes. Journal of Advertising Research, 40(1–2), 29–34. https://doi.org/10.2501/
10.1509/jmkg.69.1.1.55513 JAR-40-1-2-29-34
Mazursky, D., & Ganzach, Y. (1998). Does involvement moderate time-dependent biases Strauss, C. L. (1974). Structural anthropology. Person & Law, 1, 571.
in consumer multiattribute judgment? Journal of Business Research, 41(2), 95–103. Taillon, B. J., Mueller, S. M., Kowalczyk, C. M., & Jones, D. N. (2020). Understanding the
https://doi.org/10.1016/S0148-2963(97)00016-7 relationships between social media influencers and their followers: The moderating
Mirbagheri, S., & Najmi, M. (2019). Consumers’ engagement with social media role of closeness. Journal of Product & Brand Management, 26(6), 767–782. https://
activation campaigns: Construct conceptualization and scale development. doi.org/10.1108/JPBM-03-2019-2292
Psychology & marketing, 36(4), 376–394. https://doi.org/10.1002/mar.21185 Taylor, J. (2017). Why do people hate Kim Kardashian so much? Retrieved from https
Mouw, T., & Sobel, M. E. (2001). Culture wars and opinion polarization: The case of ://www.quora.com/Why-do-people-hate-Kim-Kardashian-so-much/answer/Jordan
abortion. American Journal of Sociology, 106(4), 913–943. https://doi.org/10.1086/ -Taylor-5. Accessed September 10, 2021.
320294 Till, B. D., & Busler, M. (2000). The match-up hypothesis: Physical attractiveness,
Na, Y. (2019). Do you like Justin Bieber? Why/Why not? Retrieved from https://www.quor expertise, and the role of fit on brand attitude, purchase intent and brand beliefs.
a.com/Do-you-like-Justin-Bieber-Why-Why-not/answer/Yu-Na-71. Accessed Journal of Advertising, 29(3), 1–13. https://doi.org/10.1080/
September 10, 2021. 00913367.2000.10673613
Nan, X., & Daily, K. (2015). Biased assimilation and need for closure: Examining the Voorveld, H. A. (2019). Brand communication in social media: A research agenda.
effects of mixed blogs on vaccine-related beliefs. Journal of Health Communication, 20 Journal of Advertising, 48(1), 14–26. https://doi.org/10.1080/
(4), 462–471. https://doi.org/10.1080/10810730.2014.989343 00913367.2019.1588808
Nicholson, S. P. (2012). Polarizing cues. American Journal of Political Science, 56(1), Webster, D. M., & Kruglanski, A. W. (1994). Individual differences in need for cognitive
52–66. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1540-5907.2011.00541.x closure. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67(6), 1049–1062. https://doi.
Park, J. Y., Sohi, R. S., & Marquardt, R. (1997). The role of motivated reasoning in org/10.1037/0022- 3514.67.6.1049
vendor consideration. Psychology & Marketing, 14(6), 585–600. https://doi.org/ Winter, D. G. (2011). Philosopher-king or polarizing politician? A personality profile of
10.1002/(SICI)1520-6793(199709)14:6<585::AID-MAR4>3.0.CO;2-3 Barack Obama. Political Psychology, 32(6), 1059–1081. https://doi.org/10.1111/
Popp, B., Germelmann, C. C., & Jung, B. (2016). We love to hate them! Social media- j.1467- 9221.2011.00852.x
based anti-brand communities in professional football. International Journal of Sports Wissman, B. (2018). Micro-influencers: The marketing force of the future. Retrieved from htt
Marketing and Sponsorship, 17(4), 349–367. https://doi.org/10.1108/IJSMS-11- ps://www.forbes.com/sites/barrettwissman/2018/03/02/micro-influencers-th
2016-018 e-marketing-force- of-the-future. Accessed September 10, 2021.

12
M. Koorank Beheshti et al. Journal of Business Research 160 (2023) 113804

Wolter, J. S., Brach, S., Cronin, J. J., Jr, & Bonn, M. (2016). Symbolic drivers of include social media and digital marketing, consumer to consumer interaction, brand to
consumer–brand identification and disidentification. Journal of Business Research, 69 consumer interaction, and cross-cultural advertising.
(2), 785–793. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jbusres.2015.07.011
Wyer, R. S., Jr, & Frey, D. (1983). The effects of feedback about self and others on the
Mahesh Gopinath is an associate professor of marketing at Old Dominion University. He
recall and judgments of feedback-relevant information. Journal of Experimental Social
earned his PhD in marketing from University of Michigan. His research interests include
Psychology, 19(6), 540–559. https://doi.org/10.1016/0022-1031(83)90015-X
Consumer behavior, effects of emotions on decision making, persuasive communication,
Yarchi, M., Baden, C., & Kligler-Vilenchik, N. (2021). Political polarization on the digital
advertising and cognitive elaboration.
sphere: A cross-platform, over-time analysis of interactional, positional, and affective
polarization on social media. Political Communication, 38(1–2), 98–139. https://doi.
org/10.1080/10584609.2020.1785067 Sama Ashouri is a PhD student in marketing at Old Dominion University. Her research
Yuan, S., & Lou, C. (2020). How social media influencers foster relationships with interests include digital marketing, advertising and brand communication.
followers: The roles of source credibility and fairness in parasocial relationship and
product interest. Journal of Interactive Advertising, 20(2), 133–147. https://doi.org/
Saeed Zal is an assistant professor of marketing at Southern Connecticut State University.
10.1080/15252019.2020.1769514
He earned his PhD in marketing from Old Dominion University. His research interests
include service marketing and retailing.
Mohammadali Koorank Beheshti is an assistant professor of marketing at Marist Col­
lege. He earned his PhD in marketing from Old Dominion University. His research interests

13

You might also like