2013 July Pre Reading With Letter and 3 Articles

You might also like

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 58

Name: Date:

Evidence-Based Upper Limb


Retraining after Stroke

2013

Workshop Pre-Reading

Prepared by presenters:

Annie McCluskey PhD MA DipCOT


Occupational Therapist

&
Karl Schurr MAppSc BAppSc MAPA
Physiotherapist

Address for correspondence:


PO Box 141, Regents Park NSW 2143, AUSTRALIA

Tel +61 2 9644 8217


Email: kschurr@bigpond.net.au
Annie McCluskey PhD MA DipCOT PO Box 141,
Occupational Therapist Regents Park,
NSW 2143 Australia
Karl Schurr BAppSc MAppSc (Physiotherapy)
Physiotherapist Ph 02 9644 8217
_____________________________________________________________________

Dear participant,

Evidence-Based Upper Limb Retraining after Stroke

1. Pre-Reading
Thankyou for your application to attend our workshop. Three articles of recommended pre-reading
are enclosed for your convenience. The articles provide background to task-specific training, motor
learning, structuring independent practice and long-term follow-up. Recent research – particularly
randomised controlled trials and systematic reviews - will be presented during the workshop, along
with a reference list.

We suggest that you take time to read and re-read the material, in order to gain maximum benefit
from the workshop. The more prepared you are for the workshop the more likely you are to apply
the information in practice. By early afternoon on Day 1, groups of therapists (n = 4) will assess and
train a new patient – a person you will not have seen before. Therefore, the more reading and
thinking you have done before the workshop, the more prepared you will be for this first clinical
session.

2. Anatomy Revision
Previous participants have also suggested that therapists coming to the workshop would benefit from
revising their upper limb anatomy.

3. Upper Limb Assessment Kit – Please bring to the Workshop


Please bring your own personal upper limb assessment kit to the workshop including such items as: a
knife, fork, flexible straws, Micropore tape, a ruler, stopwatch or timer, pen and pencil, Texta pens,
wooden tongue depressor, plastic picnic cup(s), plastic tweezers from wound dressing trays,
clicker/counter for recording repetitions. If you have access to a full-arm blow up airsplint, please
bring this to the workshop. These items may be required when you are working with a patient during
the workshop, and will become useful to you upon your return to work. Please label your kit. Please
also bring a name badge to wear, if possible.

4. Electrical Stimulation (ES) Machine & Mirror Box


If you have access to an ES machine at work, and a mirror box, please bring these items to the
workshop. Also please bring electrodes, fresh batteries, adhesive pads or gel.

We both look forward to meeting you soon. Please note the workshop starts at 8.00am both days.

Yours sincerely,

Annie McCluskey Karl Schurr


annie.mccluskey@sydney.edu.au kschurr@bigpond.net.au
ARTICLES FOR PRE-READING

* Distributed and copied for teaching and learning purposes. Not to


be copied for any other purpose nor for financial gain*

…………………………………………………………………………………………………………………………………….

McCluskey, A., Lannin, N.A., & Schurr, K. (2010). Optimising motor performance
following brain impairment. In M. Curtin, M. Molineux & J. Supyk-Mellson (Eds).
Occupational therapy and physical dysfunction: Enabling occupation (pp.579-606, 6th
ed). Edinburgh: Churchill Livingston.
(This chapter uses the tasks of eating and drinking to highlight features of reaching to grasp, and
postural adjustments required when during seated reaching. Common movement compensations are
explained, and photographic examples provided. Secondary musculoskeletal complications such as
contractures are discussed, along with factors which enhance motor learning and skill acquisition (for
example, goals and feedback). Evidence is reviewed for interventions aimed at improving strength in
paralysed or weak muscles (eg electrical stimulation, ES), postural control in sitting, control of force
generation, contracture management as well as interventions such as constraint induced movement
therapy (CIMT), mental practice, and dexterity training.

……………………………………….…………………………………………………………………………………………..

Barker, R.N., & Brauer, S.G. (2005). Upper limb recovery after stroke: The stroke
survivor’s perspective. Disability & Rehabilitation, 27(20), 1213-1223.
(A qualitative study conducted in Queensland, Australia. Methods for data collection included focus
groups and in-depth interviews with 19 people with stroke and 9 spouses. Several participants describe
their disappointment at the limited time therapists’ spent focussing on their arm versus their leg during
rehabilitation. Participants wanted to practice regularly and intensively, but often did not know how to
progress after they were discharged from therapy. Patient’s information post-discharge came mostly
from other people with stroke and those attending support groups, rather than from therapists. People
with minimal muscle activity particularly lacked expert advice. Strategies for longer-term follow up and
‘top-ups’ were needed to help participants continue on their upper limb recovery. Useful suggestions
are provided for restructuring out-patient and community rehabilitation services).

……………………………………….…………………………………………………………………………………………..
Birkenmeier, R.L., Prager, E.M., & Lang, C.E. (2010). Translating animal doses of task-
specific training to people with chronic stroke in 1-hour therapy sessions: A proof-of-
concept study. Neurorehabilitation and Neural Repair, 24(7), 620-635.
(This study challenged 15 people with chronic stroke (>6 months) to complete 300 or more repetitions of
upper limb task-specific training (3 tasks x 100 reps), three times a week for six weeks. The treatment
protocol was prescribed by an OT or PT, and involved functional tasks and practice of grasping,
moving/manipulating, then releasing an object. A frequently used task was ‘lifting cans on shelves’.
Examples of practice tasks and progressions are provided in an appendix to the paper. Therapists
documented the number of repetitions. For 13 participants, the average number of repetitions per
sessions was 322, and 97% of the possible sessions were attended. Action Research Arm Tests scores
improved an average of 8 points (on a 57-point scale) post-treatment and were maintained four weeks
later. The COPM was also used as a secondary outcome measure; COPM self-reported performance
changed by an average of 2.2 points (on a 10 point scale).
Citation: McCluskey, A., Lannin, N.A., &
Schurr, K. (2010). Optimising motor performance
following brain impairment (Chapter 37). In
Chapter M.Curtin, M. Molineux & J. Supyk (Eds.).
Thirty-Seven
37
Occupational therapy and physical dysfunction:
Enhancing occupation (pp 580-606, 6thed.).
Edinburgh: Churchill Livingstone.

Optimising motor performance


following brain impairment
Annie McCluskey, Natasha Lannin and Karl Schurr

CHAPTER CONTENTS Preventing and managing contractures . . . 599


Introduction . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 580 Improving reaching and postural control
in sitting . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 601
Essential skills, knowledge and attitudes
for improving motor performance . . . . . . 580 Future directions . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 602

Analysing movement . . . . . . . . . . . . . 580 Conclusion. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 603

Normal reaching to grasp . . . . . . . . . . 580 SUMMARY


Postural adjustments in sitting . . . . . . . 583
This chapter provides a framework for optimising
Focus on ‘positive’ versus ‘negative’
the motor performance of children and adults
impairments . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 585
with brain impairment. Conditions such as stroke,
Recognising contractures . . . . . . . . . . 585 traumatic brain injury, and cerebral palsy are
Recognising compensatory strategies . . . 587 mainly focused upon; however, the content can
be applied to people with other progressive
Hypothesising about compensatory neurological conditions, such as multiple
strategies . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 588 sclerosis. The occupations of eating and drinking
Teaching motor skills . . . . . . . . . . . . . 589 are used as examples throughout the chapter.
Skills and knowledge required by graduates are
The stages of motor learning . . . . . . . . 589
identified, including knowledge of motor
Making training task-specific . . . . . . . . 590 behaviour, the features of reaching to grasp and
Maximising practise . . . . . . . . . . . . . 590 postural adjustments required when reaching in
sitting. Common movement compensations are
Giving feedback . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 591 explained, followed by secondary musculoskeletal
Evaluating change in motor complications such as contracture, which need
performance . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 591 to be anticipated and managed. Factors which
enhance motor learning, skill acquisition and
Evidence-based intervention to improve
engagement are discussed, with implications for
upper-limb motor performance . . . . . . . . 593
therapists’ teaching skills. Finally, a summary is
Strength training for weak or paralysed provided of interventions to improve motor
muscles . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 594 performance after brain impairment. The best
currently available evidence is provided, from
Electrical stimulation. . . . . . . . . . . . . 594
systematic reviews and randomised trials.
Constraint-induced movement therapy . . . 594
Mental practice . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 595 KEY POINTS
Reducing muscle force during grasp . . . . 595 } Essential knowledge in neurological
Dexterity training. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 598 rehabilitation includes an understanding of

9khj_dU9^&)-UcW_d$_dZZ+-/ /%'+%(&&/(0'/0&(FC
SECTION FIVE Working with the individual

normal motor behaviour, muscle biology, and


skill acquisition.
Essential skills, knowledge
} Abnormal motor performance can be observed and attitudes for improving
during a task such as reaching for a cup, and
compared with expected performance.
motor performance
Hypotheses about the cause(s) of observed
movement differences can be made and To improve motor performance, therapists should
tested. think of themselves as ‘movement scientists’ (Carr
} Paralysis, weakness, and loss of dexterity et al 1987, Refshauge et al 2005). Just as occupa-
negatively affect upper-limb motor tional science refers to the science of occupation,
performance and occupational engagement. movement science refers to the science of
These impairments should be the focus of
movement.
the therapists’ attention rather than spasticity,
which is not correlated with reduced A movement scientist uses specialist knowledge
performance. from basic science (e.g. neuroplasticity, muscle
} Many people with brain impairment have biology), applied science (e.g. normal movement or
difficulty understanding instructions and motor control), education and adult learning (e.g.
feedback, and do not practise well. To help coaching strategies, feedback, and practise). This
people learn, each therapist needs to become knowledge is combined with critical appraisal of
an effective coach. systematic reviews and randomised controlled trials
} Motor performance should be remediated to inform intervention decisions. Valid and reliable
using evidence-based strategies including
task-specific training (as opposed to instruments are used to measure motor perfor-
generalised, non-specific training). mance, and determine the effectiveness of those
intervention decisions. Systematic reviews and ran-
domised controlled trials are critically appraised and
implications of those reviews and trials are used to
inform treatment decisions. Movement scientists
Introduction use this background knowledge to observe and
analyse movement, plan intervention, and evaluate
Some people are born with cerebral palsy; others the success (or failure) of intervention. The first step
may sustain a stroke or brain injury later in life. in this process involves analysing movement.
These types of brain impairment can lead to paral-
ysis and muscle weakness, and disrupt occupa-
tional engagement. Motor control is a term Analysing movement
commonly used in rehabilitation (Dickson 2002)
and refers to the complex neural mechanisms Movement analysis involves watching a person as
responsible for movements such as reaching- they attempt a task, then comparing the details of
to-grasp, and sitting-to-standing. Occupational that attempt with ‘normal’ movement. Therefore,
therapists and physiotherapists retrain motor therapists need to understand the biomechanics of
impairments which interfere with tasks such as normal movement, including kinematics and kinet-
sitting safely on the toilet or picking up a cup. The ics. The biomechanics of reaching to grasp a cup in
aim in this chapter is to encourage therapists to sitting will be described, to illustrate the process of
systematically observe, analyse and measure motor movement analysis. Reaching to grasp for a cup has
impairments and use targeted evidence-based been chosen because drinking is an everyday
interventions. occupation.
Participation in rehabilitation, and later in com-
munity occupations such as eating in a restaurant, Normal reaching to grasp
may be restricted by paralysis or contractures. These
impairments need to be actively managed. It is not The kinematics and kinetics of reaching to grasp
enough to teach a person how to compensate using have been described elsewhere (e.g. Jeannerod
one-handed techniques, or wait for recovery. 1984, 1986, Martenuik et al 1987, 1990, Wing et al
Instead, therapists need to proactively seek muscle 1986). Kinematics refers to the observable features
activity and anticipate secondary problems such as of a movement (i.e. the angular displacements, the
contractures. trajectory that body parts take during movement,

580

9khj_dU9^&)-UcW_d$_dZZ+.& /%'+%(&&/(0'/0&(FC
Optimising motor performance following brain impairment CHAPTER 37

velocity and acceleration). Kinematics are what we cup when it is grasped. Also, selection is made of
see. For example, we see increasing shoulder flexion the appropriate arm trajectory to transport the cup
and thumb abduction when a person reaches for a to an end point, and control the release of the cup.
cup (Figures 37.1A–C). While kinematics can be This process of reaching occurs with little or no
seen, the kinetics (or forces) that cause these dis- conscious thought. Final grasp is based on the intrin-
placements cannot be directly observed. sic properties of the cup, such as the shape, size and
When reaching for a cup, our brain automatically perceived fragility (e.g. a plastic cup versus a wine
selects the most appropriate hand trajectory (the glass) as well as extrinsic factors, such as distance
‘path’ our arm will take as it moves through space), from the object, and whether the person is sitting
decides when to begin forming the appropriate or standing. Hand shape and grasp position are
shape and how much grip force to use, based on selected early in reach. Normally, we produce a
experience and visual input. In addition, adaptations smooth trajectory, control forces at all joints
are made to disturbances and inertial forces on the involved, and resist disturbances to our grasp.

A B

5XVdaT"&  } Transport and pre-shaping of the hand during reaching to grasp a glass
These illustrations present the kinematics of reaching (i.e. what we see).
Figures 37.1A and 37.1B show the trajectory of the arm (the transport phase), and pre-shaping of the fingers and thumb.
As the hand is transported forwards, the shoulder moves into forward flexion, external rotation (enabling the hand and
thumb to reach the glass), elbow flexion then elbow extension.
Figure 37.1C shows wrist extension, and the forearm held midway between pronation and supination. As pre-shaping
occurs, the fingers are slightly flexed and rotated (at the metacarpal joints), producing pad-to-pad opposition in preparation
for contact with the glass. The thumb is abducted to make a space for the glass, but also rotated at the base of the
thumb, allowing pad-to-pad opposition.

581

9khj_dU9^&)-UcW_d$_dZZ+.' /%'+%(&&/(0'/0&)FC
SECTION FIVE Working with the individual

The timing and synchronisation of reaching


requires careful observation, particularly the timing
of the hand’s pre-shaping and transport, acceleration
and deceleration, and the size of hand aperture, if
abnormalities are to be identified. For example, we
know that in healthy adults, hand pre-shaping and
transport of the arm do not begin at exactly the same
time (van Vliet 1998). Rather, our arm begins to
move (particularly shoulder forward flexion and
external rotation) slightly before the thumb, fingers
and hand open to form the grasp shape.
Reaching to grasp in children has also been inves-
tigated (e.g. Zoia et al 2006). Even when object size A
and distance reached vary, 5-year-old children and
adults show more similarities than differences in
their reach (e.g. grasp formation and timing). The
major differences include longer movement dura-
tion and deceleration times in 5-year-olds compared
to adults, and a larger hand aperture (opening) in
children, particularly when visual feedback is
missing. Zoia and colleagues propose that younger
children may naturally compensate by reaching with
a larger ‘safety margin’ than necessary (hand aper-
ture) while developing reaching skills.
Finally, when adults reach for a cup which is close
(i.e. within 60% of arm’s length), there is minimal
hip flexion and trunk movement (Dean et al 1999a). B
When reaching for a cup or object further away (i.e.
100% or 140% of arm’s length), movement occurs
first at the trunk (via hip flexion). Hip and shoulder
flexion, and elbow extension all contribute to trans-
port the hand forwards. Trunk displacement via hip
flexion, and upper-arm movements are observed
earlier when people reach for objects further away.
The elbow does not fully extend at the end of reach,
except when this is the only way the object can be
reached (Figures 37.2A–C).
Therefore, it is normal, and biomechanically
more efficient, for people to move their trunk for-
wards to assist with arm placement during many
reaching tasks. Similarly, it is abnormal, and biome- C
chanically inefficient, for people to fully extend
their elbow when reaching for a cup which is close 5XVdaT"&! } Transporting the hand forward during
to them, unless that is the only way to accomplish seated reaching (cup within arms length then at 100% of
arms length)
the task.
In Figures 37.2A and 37.2B, there is minimal hip flexion and
In summary, in people without brain impairment, trunk displacement when this lady reaches for a cup which
the hand begins to make an appropriate hand shape is close and within arms length. Note also that her elbow
and aperture shortly after the upper arm begins to remains flexed even when grasping the cup.
move. During near reaching, the elbow remains In Figure 37.2C, the cup has been placed at arms length,
and on her affected side. Hip and shoulder flexion, and
flexed, with minimal hip and trunk movement. When elbow extension all help this lady to successfully transport
reaching for distant objects, the trunk moves first, her hand forward.
via hip flexion. These features are characteristic of

582

9khj_dU9^&)-UcW_d$_dZZ+.( /%'+%(&&/(0'/0&*FC
Optimising motor performance following brain impairment CHAPTER 37

many observed reaching tasks, not just reaching for us from falling. Reaching distance is significantly
a cup, and are consequently often called essential reduced when both feet are off the ground, and
components. becomes difficult even for healthy adults.
Reaching direction also influences leg muscle
activity. Reaching for a cup on the right side of the
Postural adjustments in sitting body results in increased right leg extensor activity
(Chari & Kirby 1986, Dean et al 1999b). Reaching
In the next section, we discuss adjustments required in front, or to the opposite side, results in increasing
to maintain upright sitting when reaching for a cup, leg extensor activity on the side opposite to the
and what to look for when analysing this task. We reaching arm (Dean et al 1999b). Absence of one
have intentionally chosen the term ‘postural adjust- leg (e.g. following amputation) also reduces the dis-
ments’ to encourage a shift in thinking (and therapy) tance that can be reached to that side (Chari &
away from muscles of the trunk, to muscles of the Kirby 1986).
lower limb. It is the leg, not the trunk, muscles that This research on normal reaching in sitting can be
prevent falling when a person reaches forward or to applied when training people who have difficulty
the side. In this section, we also discuss environmen- staying upright while reaching. For example, if a
tal factors such as base of support, reaching distance person is unable to generate sufficient leg extensor
and direction. Each of these factors can be manipu- force to prevent him/herself from falling forward
lated during analysis, to make seated reaching easier while reaching, they will need to learn to activate
or more challenging. their leg extensor muscles in order to be successful
When reaching for a cup in sitting, we anticipate at this task. Reaching forward will be easiest when
the effect that gravity will have on our body prior there is maximal thigh support, both feet are
to moving. We intuitively know, and can anticipate, on the floor and the chair/bed height is low. A per-
what will happen when we reach forwards, sideways son’s practise will be more successful if they are
or towards the floor because of the effect of gravity. asked to reach to a target within arm’s reach, so
Consequently, we adjust our bodies to maintain they can control their trunk movement before
balance and avoid falling. These adjustments are reaching further forward (i.e. beyond arms
required during dressing and toiletting. Our base of reach).
support, the direction and speed of reaching all Less muscle activity is required from the affected
influence reaching in sitting (Dean et al 1999a, leg extensors if a person reaches to the unaffected
1999b). side. Therefore, it is likely to be easier for a person
Our base of support comes from our feet and to practise reaching for a cup on their unaffected
thighs when we sit with both feet on the floor. side first. Task difficulty can be progressed by reach-
When reaching forwards beyond this base of support, ing further forward, reaching first to the unaffected
the leg muscles are critical for maintaining upright side, then to the front, then to the affected side. As
sitting (Chari & Kirby 1986, Dean et al 1999a, the person becomes more successful, the amount of
1999b). For example, when reaching for a cup at thigh support can be reduced and the seat height
140% of arm’s length, tibialis anterior contracts increased to increase the force required from
prior to anterior deltoid in the arm. Soleus and the legs.
biceps femoris muscles contract soon after, to Feedback during training also helps to increase
control the forward movement of our body mass learning. If a person is unable to push effectively on
(Dean et al 1999a, Crosbie et al 1995) (Figures their affected leg, they may need specific feedback
37.3A–F). about whether their leg muscles are working. Bath-
If thigh support is reduced when reaching for- room scales can be used to give feedback about the
wards, the contribution of the leg muscles increases force being generated through the affected leg (e.g.
to compensate (Dean et al 1999b). If both feet are weight in kilograms). Bathroom scales can also
off the floor (e.g. when sitting on a high bed or provide information about timing. For example, are
plinth), our base of support is reduced considerably. the leg muscles pushing at the appropriate time (i.e.
We can no longer push with our feet. We cannot anticipating the transfer of weight forward) to
make postural adjustments using the large muscles prevent the person falling? Systematic and persis-
which cross our knees and reach our feet. Instead, tent practise of reaching in this way has been
we have to rely on muscles around the hip to keep shown to improve reaching ability and the ability to

583

9khj_dU9^&)-UcW_d$_dZZ+.) /%'+%(&&/(0'/0&*FC
SECTION FIVE Working with the individual

5XVdaT"&" } Postural
adjustments required to stay
upright in sitting, when reaching
for a cup at distances greater
than arms length
This lady has been asked to reach
for, and pick up a cup on her
unaffected side, beyond arms
length. Her thighs and feet form her
base of support. She looks at the
object, begins to pre-shape her
hand, anticipates the effect that
gravity will have on her base of
support as she lifts her arm, then
transports her arm forwards. To
avoid falling forwards when lifting
her arm, she pushes with her feet
A (Figures 37.3A and 37.3B).
B
In Figure 37.3C, this lady is
reaching for a cup placed beyond
arms length, and on her affected
side. This task is difficult for her,
requiring greater leg extensor
activity from her left leg. If she does
not push through her left leg and
foot, she will fall forwards and to her
left.
Figure 37.3D illustrates her weight
shift forwards and to her left side.
Figure 37.3E shows a training
session which involves practice of
seated reaching. This lady is
practicing reaching for a cup placed
beyond arms length and to her
C D unaffected side. When her skill and
motor control improve, she will
practice placing the cup across to
the left side of the table. Her feet
are on the floor and her thighs well
supported. Electrical tape marks
correct foot position.
In Figure 37.3F, the seat height has
been raised, and this lady’s feet are
now off the floor. She cannot push
with her feet. Consequently, she is
unable to reach as far forward as
when her feet are one the floor. To
optimise successful reaching, the
base of support available to a
person needs to be considered and
E planned.

584

9khj_dU9^&)-UcW_d$_dZZ+.* /%'+%(&&/(0'/0&,FC
Optimising motor performance following brain impairment CHAPTER 37

stand-up in people who have had a stroke in both impairments include abnormal postures, exagger-
acute (Dean et al 2007) and chronic settings (Dean ated proprioceptive reflexes producing spasticity,
& Shepherd 1997). and exaggerated cutaneous reflexes of the limbs
Before concluding this section, we want to argue producing flexion withdrawal spasms. ‘Negative’
strongly against the practice of ‘facilitating’ move- impairments include paralysis, weakness, loss of
ment. Training postural adjustments and sitting coordination and loss of dexterity. In practice, it is
balance by pushing a person in one direction (exter- the ‘negative’ impairments or characteristics, such
nal perturbations by the therapist) will result in very as paralysis, that most concern people with stroke
different muscle activation patterns compared to and other conditions, not spasticity and abnormal
self-generated movement (Forssberg & Hirschfield reflexes.
1994). The person cannot anticipate what direction Therapy textbooks (and many experienced prac-
or force of perturbation will be used by the thera- titioners), focus on the management of spasticity (a
pist, nor when these disturbances will occur. Train- ‘negative’ impairment), but provide less guidance to
ing postural adjustments by pushing a person from students and new graduates on strength or dexterity
side to side in sitting is unlikely to help them acti- training. However, research indicates that reducing
vate appropriate muscles necessary for self-gener- spasticity does not automatically improve perfor-
ated movement (for example, when cleaning mance (McLellan 1977, Neilson & McCaughey
themself on the toilet). Such ‘training’ strategies 1982). There appears to be little correlation between
may cause the person to become rigid and fearful of spasticity and function (Ada et al 2006b, O’Dwyer
moving during therapy, and should be avoided. The et al 1996, Sommerfield et al 2003). Thus, while
anticipatory muscle activity that occurs during acknowledging the presence of spasticity, we ques-
reaching while seating, and the possible intervention tion its emphasis. In this chapter, we provide exam-
implications and strategies, are presented in Table ples of strategies which focus on strength and
37.1. dexterity (‘positive’ impairments), which therapists
Training strategies should aim to mimic the are more likely to be able to influence.
normal sequence of muscle activity specific to the Here is a final note about analysing and labelling
task for which the person is being trained (see Table motor impairments. Therapists often use the term
37.1 for some examples). If a person is unable to ‘spasticity’ or ‘high tone’ to refer to stiff or tight
sit, the therapist will need to accurately analyse the muscles in the hand or arm, or to stiff joints. The
reasons why they cannot sit, then develop training cause of such stiffness or tightness needs to be
strategies which are specific to those difficulties. determined so that intervention can be planned
In summary, seated reach can be progressed by (Boyd & Ada 2001). Often what therapists describe
gradually increasing the distance, and changing the as spasticity or high tone is a shortening of muscles
direction of reach (i.e. to the affected side, then or contracture. Therapists need to be able to recog-
forwards, then to the contralateral or unaffected nise and diagnose a contracture, because muscle
side), decreasing the amount of thigh support and contractures, unlike spasticity, may be amenable to
increasing seat height. However, ‘tapping’ or pushing therapy.
a person off balance as part of training is unhelpful
and may interfere with their recovery. By systemati-
cally increasing the demands of a reaching task as Recognising contractures
suggested previously, people with neurological con-
ditions can learn to engage more successfully in A contracture can be recognised by loss of joint
occupations such as dressing and bathing. range and increased resistance to passive movement
at a joint (Ada & Canning 2005). Resistance to
movement is typically due to peripheral changes in
Focus on ‘positive’ versus muscle fibres and connective tissue (O’Dwyer et al
‘negative’ impairments 1996, Pandyan et al 2003), not to central nervous
system changes or spasticity. Animal studies show
Neurological conditions, such as stroke and cerebral that muscles shorten and lengthen in response to
palsy, lead to impairments which can be classified immobilisation. Animal muscles increase in length
as either ‘positive’ or ‘negative’ (Ada et al 2000, when immobilised in a lengthened position, and
Burke 1988, O’Dwyer et al 1996). ‘Positive’ decrease in length when immobilised in a shortened

585

9khj_dU9^&)-UcW_d$_dZZ+.+ /%'+%(&&/(0'/0&,FC
SECTION FIVE Working with the individual

Table 37.1 Summary of seated reaching without back support, feet on or off the floor

Reaching Implications for Possible training


forward in Anticipatory muscle activity intervention strategies
sitting without
back support, Leg/trunk Arm
feet on the floor
Within arm’s Back extensors Transport } Begin to train reaching Practice set-up
length Hip extensors Shoulder forward within arm’s length if } Sitting with back support:
flexion and person is unable to activate practise moving forward
external rotation hip extensors from (hip flexors) and back
Pre-shaping } Sit with back supported to to (hip extensors) the back
Wrist and finger minimise initial task support
extension, difficulty } Sit next to wall on the
radial deviation, } Provide trunk support if non-affected side for a
thumb abduction unable to sit without vertical cue
and opposition assistance
Greater than Hip extensors } Practise sitting on a stable Feedback:
arm’s length Knee extensors surface } Provide vertical cue for
Plantarflexors } Specific training of hip and sitting alignment (i.e. if the
knee extensor strength and person is falling towards
endurance on affected side their left, position them
} Maximise thigh support with a wall at their right
} Practise with feet supported side to provide a close
before training with feet vertical cue and feedback
unsupported when they begin to fall)
} Provide visual cue (e.g. line
Ipsilaterally (i.e. Ipsilateral } Train reaching to ipsilateral
on wall for appropriate
to the unaffected or (unaffected) side if person is unable to
shoulder position)
intact side) hip, knee and reach to contralateral side
} Place bathroom scales
ankle extensors } Gradually increase distance
under affected foot for
the person is attempting to
feedback about weight
reach ipsilaterally
bearing
} Gradually introduce
} Sitting on lower surface to
reaching across the midline
decrease extensor force
towards contralateral side
required
Contralaterally Contralateral } Ensure appropriate Progress difficulty by:
(i.e. to affected side) hip, knee and alignment of weight- T increasing time
ankle extensors bearing leg (i.e. knee over T increasing distance
foot, leg not abducted) from wall
} Ensure person begins to T decreasing thigh
use leg extensors in support
anticipation of weight T increasing height of
transference to affected surface
side T increasing distance
reached
T increasing contralateral
distance

586

9khj_dU9^&)-UcW_d$_dZZ+., /%'+%(&&/(0'/0&,FC
Optimising motor performance following brain impairment CHAPTER 37

Table 37.1 Summary of seated reaching without back support, feet on or off the floor—cont’d

Reaching forward Anticipatory Implications for intervention Possible training strategies


in sitting, without muscle activity
back support,
feet off the floor
Within arm’s length Back extensors } ?a^eXSTQPRZbd__^acXUcWT_aX\Pah Feedback:
Hip extensors purpose is to train reaching. } Provide vertical cue for sitting
alignment (i.e. if the person is
Greater than arm’s Hip extensors } Specific training of hip extensors if
falling towards their left side
length Back extensors unable to elicit hip extensor activity
position them with a wall at
} Maximise thigh support
their right side to give them
} Gradually and systematically
feedback when they begin to
increase the distance reached
fall)
} Practise with feet supported before
} Bathroom scales under the
training with feet unsupported
affected thigh for weight-
Ipsilaterally Ipsilateral hip extensors } 0bPQ^eTR^]RT]caPcT^]X_bX[PcTaP[ bearing feedback
hip extensors Progress difficulty by:
T increasing distance from wall
Contralaterally Contralateral hip } 2^]RT]caPcTcaPX]X]V^] T decreasing thigh support
extensors contralateral hip extensors T increasing distance reached
T increasing contralateral
distance

position, for example, in a plaster cast, by adding or able to open their hand wide enough to pick up a
losing sarcomeres, respectively (Tabary et al 1972, cup. Such contractures will need to be reversed
Williams & Goldspink 1978). A sarcomere is the using strategies such as positioning, passive stretch-
contractile part of a myofibril, within skeletal muscle ing and plaster casting. There is, however, still
(Gossman et al 1982). When immobilised in a short- uncertainty about the effectiveness of these inter-
ened position, muscles generate tension at a new, ventions, and how long stretches or positioning
shorter resting length (Herbert & Balnave 1993). need to be maintained per day to adequately
These structural changes lead to disorganisation reverse contracture. Nonetheless, it is unlikely
of connective tissue within a muscle (Goldspink & that short-duration stretch methods such as
Williams 1990), disrupting the synovial fluid, joint passive ranging of joints, will change tissue structure.
membrane and articular cartilage (Trudel et al Therefore, methods for applying long-duration
2003). Changes in the mechanical-elastic properties stretch are required, and are discussed later in this
of muscles and connective tissue limit joint range of chapter.
movement after stroke (Vattanaslip et al 2000), and
probably also after other neurological conditions.
This joint stiffness leads to resistance. Therapists Recognising compensatory
often (inappropriately) call this resistance to move- strategies
ment ‘high tone’ or ‘spasticity’.
While it remains unknown how long contractures When analysing motor and occupational perfor-
take to develop after brain impairment, there is no mance, therapists need to be able to recognise com-
doubt that they do develop. Contractures are unde- pensations. Compensatory strategies are movement
sirable for many reasons, including their effect on solutions that allow a person to compensate for loss
occupational performance. For example, a person of normal muscle activity (Carr & Shepherd 1989).
with contractures of the wrist and finger flexor Compensations may be caused by a muscle contrac-
muscles will be unable to extend their wrist to ture, muscle weakness or both. For example, a
achieve normal grasp and release. Nor will they be person who cannot successfully reach forward to

587

9khj_dU9^&)-UcW_d$_dZZ+.- /%'+%(&&/(0'/0&,FC
SECTION FIVE Working with the individual

grasp a cup may use hip flexion and/or shoulder


abduction to compensate for poor shoulder flexion.
In previous years, these patterns of muscle contrac-
tion were called ‘abnormal synergies’, and believed
to be a normal stage of recovery after stroke or brain
injury. However, there is no neurophysiological
explanation for these synergies. Rather, this combi-
nation of muscle activity appears to be more biome-
chanically advantageous to the person who cannot
activate weak or paralysed muscles (Carr & Shep-
herd 1989).
The problem with compensations is that they
become learned. The more a person practises using
these inappropriate movement solutions, the more A
difficult it becomes to change behaviour. In the long
term, compensatory strategies are inefficient and
inflexible. Therefore, therapists need to help people
to contract their muscles more appropriately when
reaching. When observing a person reach to grasp a
cup, we also need to compare what we see (kine-
matics) with our knowledge of normal movement.
For example, when pre-shaping to reach for a cup
within arm’s reach, we need to determine if a person
is opening their hand and abducting their thumb at
the beginning of reaching. Thumb abduction and
metacarpo-phalangeal extension of the fingers are
essential, and result in a web space large enough to
accommodate the cup. Typically, people who have B
difficulty abducting their thumb, and/or extending
their fingers and wrist will compensate by extending
5XVdaT"&# } Normal pre-shaping while reaching for
their thumb, pronating their forearm and/or abduct- a cup and commonly observed compensations
ing their shoulder (Carr & Shepherd 1987, 2003) Figure 37.4A illustrates normal pre-shaping during reaching,
(Figures 37.4A–B). These strategies may lead to suc- with the thumb abducted and opposed, and the person’s
cessful contact with a cup, but are inefficient and wrist extended ready to grasp the cup.
However, in Figure 37.4B the person is compensating
compensatory.
during reaching for poor control of thumb abduction (a
When a person transports their arm towards a missing essential component). Instead, they are extending
cup which is nearby (i.e. within arm’s reach), we their thumb and pronating their forearm (both are
watch to see if they are using their shoulder flexors compensations) to try to grasp the cup.
and external rotators, without using excessive shoul-
der elevation, internal rotation or abduction. The
latter three compensatory movements may suggest
weakness or paralysis of the person’s shoulder flexion and trunk movement may be compensations
flexors and/or external rotators. Alternatively, these for weak shoulder flexors.
shoulder movements may be helping to compensate
for poor control of forearm, wrist, thumb or finger
muscles. For a full discussion and analysis, see Carr Hypothesising about
and Shepherd (1987). compensatory strategies
When reaching in sitting, it is normal to flex at
the hips to reach distances at arm’s length or greater The final step in the process of analysing movement
(Dean et al 1999a). However, it is not normal to use is developing then testing the movement hypothe-
hip flexion when reaching for an object such as a cup ses. Developing a hypothesis (or hypotheses) about
which is very close to the body. In that case, hip a person’s movement compensations will help us to

588

9khj_dU9^&)-UcW_d$_dZZ+.. /%'+%(&&/(0'/0&-FC
Optimising motor performance following brain impairment CHAPTER 37

plan treatment. One hypothesis might be that a muscle force required. Taping a light polystyrene
person’s shoulder muscles are paralysed or too weak cup into their hand will also decrease task demands
to lift the limb up against gravity, to reach for a cup. and eliminate the need to pre-shape the hand. The
This hypothesis can be tested by assessing muscle person can then concentrate on transporting the cup
strength (i.e. palpating the muscle belly during a rather than worrying about pre-shaping. Each move-
movement attempt). If a person cannot easily reach ment hypothesis can be tested in turn.
forwards, two key muscles to check are anterior Assuming we have correctly analysed the person’s
deltoid (a shoulder flexor) and infraspinatus (an movement problems, identified the missing essential
external rotator). If these muscles are weak, components and compensations, and tested our
strengthening exercises will be required. In the hypotheses, the next step is to design a programme
absence of a muscle contraction, we might use elec- to improve motor performance.
trical stimulation (Ada & Foongchomcheay 2002,
Pomeroy et al 2006) and/or mental practise to elicit
muscle contractions (see Bell & Murray 2005, Braun Teaching motor skills
et al 2006).
A second hypothesis might be that muscles such People with brain impairment often have difficulty
as the internal rotators, elbow, wrist and finger understanding instructions, using feedback, remem-
flexors are short and/or stiff due to contractures. bering their practice and learning motor skills.
The opposing muscles may be incapable of generat- Therefore, therapists need to develop critical teach-
ing the necessary force to lift the arm, extend the ing skills and become effective coaches. We need to
wrist or open the hand. This hypothesis can be understand how motor learning progresses, provide
tested by manually checking the available range of training that is task-specific, ensure learners practise
external rotation; forward flexion; elbow, wrist and well and often, and provide useful, timely feedback.
finger extension; and thumb abduction. Loss of Each of these factors will influence learning.
range at any one of these joints will change the
person’s ability to reach for an object such as a cup.
A third possible hypothesis might be that the The stages of motor learning
person is using excessive muscle force to achieve the
task (i.e. to pick up the cup). They may be using too There is considerable literature on motor learning.
many muscles, too much force, or both. A group of The Fitts and Posner model of motor learning
muscles such as the finger and wrist flexors may be (1967) is most often used to inform rehabilitation
overactive and contract with excessive force when practice. The three stages described by Fitts and
movement is attempted. Overactivity may occur Posner are: (1) the verbal-cognitive stage; (2) the
(e.g. all the muscles of the arm switch on with motor stage; and (3) the autonomous stage. In the
effort) to help compensate for weakness in other first stage, learners rely on verbal feedback and
muscle groups, such as the shoulder flexors. We can external environmental information to achieve goals
test our hypothesis by setting up the practice task and understand the demands of a task (Haggard
to minimise effort. For example, the person could 2001, Prinz 1997). In the second stage, the focus is
practise reaching with their arm supported on a on the quality of movement, mass practice (Mastos
table, and a sheet of paper or cloth under their hand et al 2007) and decreasing mistakes (McNevin et al
to reduce friction. 2000). Finally, in the third stage the learner is able
A fourth hypothesis might be that the task and/ to perform the task with less cognitive effort, cope
or environmental set-up are too challenging. The more effectively with distractions and draw on their
cup may be positioned too far in front or to the side problem-solving skills when performing the task in
for the person to grasp without compensating. Or novel situations (Mastos et al 2007, McNevin et al
the table may be too high. We can test these hypoth- 2000). At each stage, learners need timely feedback
eses by placing the cup closer or lowering the table. about performance and goal achievement, and
Another example is changing the person’s position, increasing amounts of practise (Lee & White 1990,
allowing muscles to contract more easily (e.g. McNevin et al 2000, Shea et al 2000).
moving the person from a sitting to lying position). Using our previous training example of reaching
This change will lessen the effect of gravity on their for a cup in sitting, a goal might be for the person
weak shoulder flexors, and decrease the amount of to sit upright for 30 seconds without falling to the

589

9khj_dU9^&)-UcW_d$_dZZ+./ /%'+%(&&/(0'/0&-FC
SECTION FIVE Working with the individual

affected side. In the first stage of learning, they may out to control a computer game (Sietsema et al
require continual feedback about pushing with their 1993), and demonstrated improved dexterity when
affected leg, to avoid falling to their affected side. engaging in kitchen activities versus table-top
In the second stage, they may recognise when they therapy activities (Neistadt 1994).
are beginning to fall, make an attempt to prevent The bottom line is that people learn what they
this but require occasional assistance or prompting. practise. If a person wants to learn to drink from a
In the third stage, they can sit without assistance, cup, they should practise reaching for and transport-
conduct a concurrent conversation and reach forward ing a cup, not a plastic shape or cone that vaguely
to pick up a cup of water with their non-affected resembles a cup. Early training might involve sliding
hand without falling to the affected side. If practise or placing a lightweight plastic cup forwards on a
tasks are too demanding in the early stages of learn- low table, with the cup taped into the person’s hand
ing, the person may be unable to achieve the goal. if they have no active hand movement. Advanced
For example, asking the person to reach to their dexterity training might involve moving and manipu-
contralateral (affected) side before they can sit lating objects of interest, such as garments, eye-
upright for 5 seconds would be unrealistic. glasses, cutlery and writing implements, not beans
or plastic counters. Training should replicate the
skill or task that a person wants to learn. Valuable
Making training task-specific time should not be wasted on non-specific
practise.
The terms ‘task-specific training’, ‘task-related prac-
tise’ and ‘specificity of training’ are used in the lit-
erature (e.g. Blennerhassett & Dite 2004, Dean & Maximising practise
Shepherd 1997, Michaelsen et al 2006, van der Lee
et al 2001). These terms refer to therapy involving More time spent practising leads to improved per-
intentional practise of a specific movement, action formance across many skill areas including chess
or task, versus repetition of non-specific tasks (Charness 1981), typing (Ericsson 2004), sports
(Bayona et al 2005) such as lifting your arm up high performance (Helsen-Starkes & Hodges 1998) and
for no reason, touching your head or nose (in playing musical instruments (Ericsson 2004,
response to an instruction by a professional) or Lehmann & Ericsson 1996). In a study involving
stacking cones instead of practising reaching for a 20-year-old violinists (Ericsson 2004), the best per-
cup. Examples of task-specific training include prac- formers, as judged by conservatory teachers aver-
tise of pen or cutlery manipulation to improve aged 10,000 hours of practise during their lives. The
writing and eating respectively, or picking up and second-best performers averaged 7,500 hours, the
transporting a cup to improve the occupation of next-best, 5,000 hours and so forth.
drinking. In the early stages of motor recovery, when A similar commitment to practise is required by
a person cannot hold objects, implements can be learners with acquired brain impairment and their
taped into the affected hand (e.g. a fork or cup), or therapists, if motor performance is to improve. In a
placed in front to encourage reaching. randomised controlled trial evaluating the effect of
Research shows that we learn (or relearn) motor seated reaching (Dean & Shepherd 1997), people
skills through engagement in tasks and activities. For with stroke each performed 2,970 reaches beyond
example, when people practised reaching to pick up arm’s length during a 2-week training period. Par-
a pen and write their name, compared to pretending ticipants in the intervention group could reach
or imagining themselves picking up a pen to write, further and faster than the control group after task-
the quality of reach and grasp improved significantly specific training, and improved their ability to
(Wu et al 1994). Although that writing study stand-up.
involved healthy adults, the implication is that Repetition of practise tasks is also integral to
people need to practise real-life tasks, not simulate learning. Multiple repetitions of a movement can
them. Studies involving adults with brain injury also significantly improve upper limb strength in people
demonstrate the importance of using real-life tasks following stroke. In one study, people with stroke
for motor training (Neistadt 1994, Sietsema et al practised repetitive finger flexion and extension
1993). For instance, people with a brain injury pro- against resistance, twice daily for 15-minute periods
duced a greater range of movement when reaching (Butefisch et al 1995). This task-specific practise led

590

9khj_dU9^&)-UcW_d$_dZZ+/& /%'+%(&&/(0'/0&-FC
Optimising motor performance following brain impairment CHAPTER 37

to greater grip strength, improved contraction veloc- knowledge of performance and knowledge of results
ities and peak acceleration in the extensor muscles, (Kilduski & Rice 2003).
compared to a control group receiving standard Knowledge of performance refers to information
Bobath therapy. Another study by Feys and col- about the movement process or attempt, for example
leagues (1998, 2004) included adults with minimal ‘Your wrist was flexed’ or ‘You kept your elbow
arm function after stroke, who were required to close to your body’. Knowledge of results refers to
protract and retract their affected shoulder while information about the movement outcome, for
seated in a rocking chair, for 30 minutes daily over example ‘You picked up the cup’. Knowledge of
6 weeks. This practise was intended to help improve results is usually obvious to the person performing
forward reaching. The treatment had greatest effect the task; they either achieved the desired outcome
in people with more severe motor deficits. Improve- or they did not. The bigger problem is how to change
ments in upper-limb motor function were retained the next attempt. This is where the therapist as
at 5 years’ follow-up. coach can provide knowledge about performance,
Mass practise and multiple repetitions are also and suggest ways to make the next attempt more
features of constraint-induced movement therapy successful.
(CIMT; see Morris et al 1997 for a summary). Extrinsic feedback can be very helpful to learn-
CIMT involves restraining the unimpaired arm and ers, particularly corrections that need to be made,
hand to encourage intensive practise of tasks using and features to focus on during subsequent attempts
the affected arm. Although CIMT studies require (Kernodle & Carlton 1992). The timing of feedback
participants to practise for up to 6 hours a day, the is important, relative to performance. Concurrent
amount of practise required to improve function knowledge of results – that is, feedback provided
remains unknown. However, we do know that a during performance – appears to impede motor
minimum dose of 16 hours additional practise is learning (Annett 1959). Talking during performance
needed to improve motor outcome following stroke, may be distracting (although motivational com-
according to a recent systematic review (Kwakkel ments such as ‘Keep going’ or ‘That’s right’ can be
2006). helpful). However, feedback given after task com-
Finally, practice which involves lots of repetitions pletion has a positive influence on motor learning
but no transfer of learning may limit skill develop- (Adams 1971, Bilodeau 1966, Newell 1976). The
ment. For example, using a fork with a built-up amount and frequency of extrinsic feedback also
handle to repeatedly pick up pieces of soft bread affects performance. Intermittent feedback is more
will not enable a person to eat a meal successfully effective than constant feedback (Ho & Shea 1978,
in a restaurant with a normal fork. Motor learning Winstein & Schmidt 1990), whereas too much feed-
theory suggests that people improve their perfor- back may negatively influence learning (van Vliet &
mance by practising in a variety of situations, and Wulf 2006). People appear to benefit from watching
experiencing errors during learning. In order to others demonstrate a task, and then receive feed-
move beyond the first two stages of motor learning, back about their movement outcome (Reo & Mercer
people need to practise in different settings, with 2004). This form of modelling seems to be most
different movement parameters (for example, forks effective when presented at intervals during skill
with different handles, and different foods). Increas- acquisition, before practise commences, and at
ing demands in this way helps learners to problem various intervals thereafter.
solve and fathom the rules underlying task perfor-
mance (Schmidt & Lee 1999). Evaluating change in motor
performance
Giving feedback
Therapists need to re-evaluate motor (and occupa-
Accurate feedback is critical to the teaching and tional) performance at regular intervals. Objective
learning of motor skills. Feedback can be provided measures need to be taken before and during train-
by the task itself (intrinsic feedback), or by an ing. Ideally, a review of performance and goals will
outside source such as the therapist, biofeedback occur at every session. Performance can be mea-
device or timer (extrinsic feedback). Extrinsic feed- sured using complex or simple equipment. For
back has been further classified into two types: example, to determine if a person with sitting

591

9khj_dU9^&)-UcW_d$_dZZ+/' /%'+%(&&/(0'/0&-FC
SECTION FIVE Working with the individual

balance problems is weight-bearing equally through videotaped clients, and discussed his training pro-
both legs, a therapist may use bathroom scales. grammes with peers. He has organised fortnightly
Other simple measures of performance include the peer review sessions where staff observe each other
number of movement repetitions per session, the conducting a therapy session, and provide feedback
number of correctly performed movements versus about analysis and teaching skills. Leo regularly
those performed with compensations, and distance attends rehabilitation conferences as he feels they
reached. ‘are a great pick-me-up’. More recently, Leo
If performance is not changing, the problem may increased his knowledge and skills by commencing
lie with the therapist rather than the person with a major research project, a randomised controlled
trial of task-specific training, as part of a Masters
brain impairment. Common reasons for lack of
degree.
improvement include unclear instructions, feedback
Here, Leo gives an example of a lady, Mary, he
and goals. If instructions are unclear, off-target, or
saw recently following her stroke. He describes her
too detailed, the person with stroke or brain injury
motor control problems and compensations, and the
may not understand the expected goal. Similarly, if upper-limb training programme provided over several
verbal feedback is unclear (or even absent), the months to improve occupational performance. Mary
person may not understand how to alter their next could not use her affected arm much when
movement attempt to achieve success. engaging in daily occupations. She could not hold or
In addition to carefully considering the words we transport objects such as a cup or a knife during
use to explain and correct movement attempts, the meals.
task chosen to elicit a movement attempt is also
important. If the task is too difficult (or too easy) Mary
progress may not be seen. If our initial movement “I saw Mary recently. She had recovery of
hypothesis was wrong, we miss the main cause of a some muscles in her arm, but a lot of
movement problem, and change in performance overactivity, many compensations and little
may not be seen. When re-measurement of perfor- control in her hand. For example, when
mance shows little or no progress, we need to reas- attempting to reach forwards to grasp a cup,
sess the person’s abilities before hypothesising again she elevated her shoulder and abducted her
about the possible causes of their movement prob- arm, clenched her fingers, flexed her elbow,
lems. If the movement hypotheses are correct, and moved her whole body forwards instead of
therapists can then reflect critically on their teaching just her arm and hand. She compensated for
skills. Alternatively, if a different movement hypoth- poor shoulder flexion, loss of external rotation
esis is made new training strategies and an interven- and thumb adduction by using every muscle
tion plan will be needed. Therapists should not possible in her arm. It was hard work.
underestimate the importance of re-measuring per-
formance, reflecting on their teaching skills, and, Training sessions targeted Mary’s shoulder
above all, persisting and expecting to see improved flexors in a lying position, which reduced the
motor performance at every session. effect of gravity. We focused on the anterior
Practice Scenario 37.1 shows how Leo, an occu- deltoid muscle. Mary was asked to rest her
hand on her forehead with the elbow flexed,
pational therapist, developed his teaching and analy-
and control her anterior deltoid in that position.
sis skills, and applied evidence-based practice in
When she could hold her arm there, she
adult rehabilitation.
started sliding her hand back from her forehead
to the pillow and the crown of her head, to
control anterior deltoid in lying, then reaching
higher to the wall to touch a marker. It was too
Practice Scenario 37.1 Leo hard in sitting. She couldn’t lift her arm up
against gravity without compensating. Other
and Mary practice tasks focused on her shoulder external
Leo is an experienced occupational therapist working rotation, elbow, wrist and finger extension and
in a large district hospital in rural Australia. He has thumb abduction. We pieced each component
over 10 years experience in adult neurological reha- together, then eventually began working on
bilitation. Leo is dedicated to developing his skills. He functional reaching in a seated position (see
has attended upper-limb motor training workshops, Figure 37.5).

592

9khj_dU9^&)-UcW_d$_dZZ+/( /%'+%(&&/(0'/0&-FC
Optimising motor performance following brain impairment CHAPTER 37

beginning, a 14 point change. With a


combination of task–specific training,
persistence on both our parts, objective
measurement, intensive practice and
feedback, Mary achieved improved hand
function. Without this persistence and practice,
I don’t think she would have achieved this
outcome.”

Ensuring enough practice by people with a stroke


or brain injury is always a challenge: to help ensure
individuals spend plenty of time each day practising,
Leo uses typed practice records with imported digital
photographs. The rehabilitation team runs a cross-
disciplinary upper-limb group several times a week,
where people with stroke or brain injury follow their
own practice programme with co-learners, and
5XVdaT"&$ } Practice of essential components supervision from therapists. Therapy assistants and
required for reaching (forward flexion and external relatives also help supervise individual practice after
rotation) and drinking from a cup this has been documented with instructions, goals
Since her stroke, this woman has had limited
and illustrations by the therapist. Family members are
opportunities to engage in occupations such as drinking
from a cup with her dominant right hand. She has weak involved in helping with practice as early as possible,
shoulder flexors and external rotators, and cannot open because of the limited time available for one-to-one
her thumb or fingers to pre-shape correctly. The therapy.
occupational therapist is helping her to practise shoulder
flexion and external rotation – essential components of
reaching – while also maintaining wrist extension and
forearm supination.
In this photograph, she is sliding the cup forwards while
Evidence-based intervention to
staying inside the black lines (electrical tape stuck to the improve upper-limb motor
table). The practice environment encourages external
rotation, wrist extension and supination, and discourages performance
compensations such as internal rotation and abduction.
Two drinking straws have been applied to her arm, one to
the inner elbow and another on to the back of her wrist.
There are several reasons why a person may be
These straws act as visual cues, reminding her to unable to reach for, grasp and drink from a cup.
maintain shoulder external rotation (the straw stays in Different causes will require different intervention/s.
contact with the wooden block) and wrist extension (her A person with a paralysed arm, who is unable to
knuckles stay in contact with the flexible straw). She is elicit any muscle contractions, will require interven-
also learning to monitor her own performance, so that she
can practise alone outside of therapy sessions. Notice the
tion targeting muscle strength. A person with a con-
timer near the therapist’s right hand, to record practice tracture will require intervention to lengthen, and
time and repetitions then strengthen, their weak overstretched muscles.
In this chapter, we have emphasised the impor-
tance of identifying the correct motor control
problem, in order to select appropriate, targeted
Mary practised for about two hours a day for
intervention. A number of interventions have now
three months (unsupervised for some of the
time); then one hour daily for another three
been tested in randomised trials, and the collective
months; then about three hours a week for the findings synthesised in systematic reviews. It is the
last three months. It took 36 weeks or six effective interventions we highlight here. Where
months before she had a functional grasp and interventions and training strategies have not been
release. In the first six weeks she completed tested using more rigorous research, we will indicate
12,810 repetitions, with an average of 427 reps if we are relying on lower level evidence or personal
per session or 85 per exercise. After 36 weeks, experience.
she achieved a score of 16/57 on the Action In adult rehabilitation, interventions that improve
Research Arm Test, compared to 2/57 at the performance of upper-limb motor control appear to

593

9khj_dU9^&)-UcW_d$_dZZ+/) /%'+%(&&/(0'/0&.FC
SECTION FIVE Working with the individual

involve greater intensity of practise and repetitions Examples of practice tasks aimed at increasing
(Kwakkel 2006, van der Lee et al 2001), and use muscle strength are shown in Figures 37.6–37.9.
task-specific training strategies to improve strength
(Ada et al 2006b). However, the evidence is still Electrical stimulation
scattered and limited. By definition, more intense
practise and repetitions requires active involvement Electrical stimulation should be provided to people
of the learner. One of the biggest challenges in reha- with no palpable muscle activity after a stroke. Ada
bilitation (and this is not limited to motor rehabilita- and Foongchomcheay (2002) conducted a meta-
tion) is how to increase the amount of practise a analysis involving four trials of electrical stimulation
person completes in the hospital and community. to prevent subluxation early after stroke (average 17
For a thoughtful discussion of the feasibility of pro- days post-stroke), involving a total of 145 subjects.
viding intensive practise, see Kwakkel (2006). It is Electrical stimulation reduced subluxation by an
important that people spend as much time as pos- average of 6.5 mm, but had no worthwhile effect on
sible practising. One hour of therapy doing 100 rep- reducing pain or improving functional recovery. No
etitions is better than 1 hour of therapy doing 10 or clinically important differences were found when
20 repetitions. Keeping a record of repetitions com- stimulation was applied later (60 days or more post-
pleted during each therapy session is one way of stroke), based on meta-analysis of data from three
ensuring practise intensity. Such records provide an randomised trials.
insight into just how much practise is needed before More recently, Pomeroy and colleagues (2006)
people acquire particular skills. examined the effect of electrical stimulation on
upper- and lower-limb motor recovery. A total of
Strength training for weak or 24 randomised trials were included, involving 888
participants from 9 days to 4 years post stroke.
paralysed muscles When compared to no treatment, function was
improved (statistically significant difference between
Some individuals may be unable to elicit a muscle groups) on the Box and Block tests, motor reaction
contraction due to paralysis or weakness. They need time and isometric muscle torque. Another explor-
practise which helps them elicit a single muscle atory study involving nine people with chronic
contraction then gradually increase the duration and stroke who had moderate-to-severe motor weakness
strength of contractions. There is a growing body of and intact sensation reported improved motor func-
evidence demonstrating that muscle strengthening tion after only 2 hours of electrical stimulation to
and repetitions improve strength and function the sensory nerves of the hand (Wu et al 2006). In
without any increase in spasticity (e.g. Ada et al summary, electrical stimulation is being used
2006a, Butefisch et al 1995). increasingly in adult neurological rehabilitation,
One of the few randomised trials targeting very although further research is still needed, including
weak muscles has already been highlighted (Feys research involving children.
et al 1998). These researchers recruited 100 people
early after stroke, and provided a novel training
strategy to improve shoulder control. Participants Constraint-induced movement
were seated in a rocking chair with their affected therapy
arm in an airsplint, and practised protraction and
retraction for 30 minutes daily for 6 weeks. The Constraint-induced movement therapy (CIMT) is a
median score on the Action Research Arm Test relatively new and popular treatment aimed at
(ARAT) was 0 at baseline for control and experi- increasing use of the affected hand after stroke,
mental groups (total maximum score  57). At the brain injury, and cerebral palsy in children and
5-year follow-up, the mean difference in improve- adults. Typically, a splint or mitt and a sling are used
ments between groups was 17 points on the ARAT to restrain the unaffected hand for up to 6 hours a
(Feys et al 2004). As previously noted, greater gains day, for 2 weeks, during which time the person
were seen in people with severe motor deficits at intensively practises using their hand. The aim is to
baseline. While there was a wide range and variabil- discourage use of the ‘good’ hand and greater use of
ity in responses, the mean effect in this very weak the affected hand. For a detailed description of a
population was large. CIMT protocol, see Morris and colleagues (1997).

594

9khj_dU9^&)-UcW_d$_dZZ+/* /%'+%(&&/(0'/0&.FC
Optimising motor performance following brain impairment CHAPTER 37

Goal one: To lift your wrist back to straight. Hold for 10 seconds
CIMT for paediatric populations has been studied
x 20 repeats x 1 day less. A small number of randomised controlled trials
with low methodological quality and small sample
Wrist sizes have been reported (e.g. Taub et al 2004, Willis
et al 2002). Consequently, the effect remains uncer-
tain (Hoare et al 2007, see Wallen 2007 for an
overview and key references). Readers should follow
Table Hold for 10 sec the work of Eliasson and colleagues in Sweden. They
describe a programme of CIMT, which involves
shorter periods of restraint using a mitt, for 2 hours
Instructions over 8 weeks with adolescents who had cerebral
1. Tape straw on forearm palsy (Eliasson et al 2003). More recently, they
2. Hand on table; elbow straight evaluated the effect with young children with
3. Let wrist drop down so fingers hang over edge of table cerebral palsy using a non-randomised study design
4. Keep fingers straight
5. Bring your hand back to straight by moving your wrist
(Eliasson et al 2005).

Critical features Mental practice


4+$,-%evenly
4$(" +,,-+$"#--#+)."#).- Mental practice and imagery have been used to
4#.') ,()-')/ ).-0+,02!+)'#(
promote motor recovery. This type of practice is
* Record number correct/number of attempts used routinely in sports training to improve skill
acquisition. In rehabilitation, a person with stroke
Date No. correct or brain injury might mentally rehearse the task of
17/7 14/20 picking up a cup and imagine the transport and pre-
17/7 15/20 shaping actions, without physically attempting the
26/7 21/25 actions (Bell & Murray 2005). A recent systematic
30/7 9–10 review (Braun et al 2006) summarised the effects
7–10
02/8 20/25
of several studies in stroke rehabilitation but again
03/8 22/25 found underpowered studies with small sample
sizes. No definite conclusions could be reached, but
this intervention is promising for people with no
5XVdaT"&% } Wrist extension
Wrist extension is essential for most activities involving active movement. Interested therapists will need to
reaching, such as picking up a cup to drink. The page devise their own script and tape to give to interested
shown above is taken from a practice book and illustrates people. For an example see Dijkerman and col-
the wrist extension exercise, where the goal is to lift the leagues (2004).
wrist back to the ‘straight’ position and hold for 10 seconds,
and to repeat this exercise 20 times in one session.
Reducing muscle force
The collective research in stroke rehabilitation
during grasp
(14 randomised trials and four systematic reviews)
shows a moderate effect of CIMT on upper-limb Some individuals turn on too many muscles, or the
motor performance, as measured by the Action wrong muscles, when reaching for and grasping
Research Arm Test (Hakkennes & Keating 2005). objects. This is a normal response to skill acquisition
However, all participants have had active wrist and (not spasticity) in most cases. Until we have mas-
finger extension at study commencement. We do tered or refined a new skill, we will try a range of
not yet know if this intervention can drive recovery strategies, and use too many muscles. Therefore,
in people with a paralysed arm, as occurred in early one aim in therapy is to reduce effort, and help the
studies involving monkeys. Currently in adult stroke person focus on the muscle actions required for task
rehabilitation, CIMT is used with small cohorts of performance.
individuals but may have the potential to drive Changing the demands of a task and the environ-
recovery if used more widely. ment can reduce effort. For example, asking a

595

9khj_dU9^&)-UcW_d$_dZZ+/+ /%'+%(&&/(0'/0&.FC
SECTION FIVE Working with the individual

Goal one: Keep texter pentip touching the X mark for 5 seconds
x 3 times in a row
Goal two: Draw a line with the texter 5 cm up the wall
x 3 times in a row

Instructions
1. Stick the paper onto the wall with tape (X mark at hip height).
2. Stand beside the poster with pen in hand.
3. Rotate the pen out so the pentip touches the X mark.
keep shoulder rotated out.
4. Hold for 5 seconds, rest and repeat.
5. Try drawing a line up the wall – no further than 5 cm initially

Check
/%%!.%$))*'$ $+'()%+'()+""%'
bend your trunk.
/##')%')+ "&')  $
/&-%*'"%+()' )"$)$.

25 cm
20 cm Start here
15 cm
10 cm 5 cm
x with pen

5XVdaT"&& } Practice for shoulder external rotation and forward


flexion in standing
Both external rotation and shoulder forward flexion are essential for
transporting the arm and hand forwards to reach for a cup or telephone. While
this man is using extra muscle force to hold the pen (increased finger flexion),
his response is typical of new skill acquisition, and is not a concern to the
therapist.
The first goal (Goal 1: Keep the texta pentip touching the X mark on the paper
for 5 seconds) demands a sustained contraction of his external rotators
combined with full supination. Without some external rotation, the goal cannot
be achieved (except by trunk rotation). The second goal (Goal 2: Draw a line
5 cm up the wall) demands sustained external rotation and shoulder flexion

596

9khj_dU9^&)-UcW_d$_dZZ+/, /%'+%(&&/(0'/0&/FC
Optimising motor performance following brain impairment CHAPTER 37

A B

5XVdaT"&' } Practice to improve fork control


This lady cannot sustain flexion of her fourth and fifth digits around a fork handle when trying to pick up food. When she
tries to use her fork, the handle rotates and she loses her grasp. Part-practice has been devised to help improve flexion of
her ring and little fingers around a fork handle. Figures 37.8A and 37.8B show her setting up the practice. She has been
asked to hold a coin between plastic tweezers for 5 seconds. This task sustains her attention. She gets feedback instantly
if her grasp weakens, because the coin drops onto the table.
Figure 37.8C shows her still holding the tweezers and coin (coin no longer visible), flexing her wrist and fingers, and
pressing the index finger down on the end of a spoon. She finds it much more challenging to keep her fourth and fifth
digits flexed in this position while her index finger is extended, as it needs to be while using a fork. Again, she receives
instant feedback if her grasp weakens because the coin drops out of the tweezers – feedback which would not be
provided by a standard fork.
Figure 37.8D illustrates how the tweezers and fork handle can be taped together, to enable fork practice to progress. This
lady can continue her practice with the coin held between plastic tweezers, and learn to transport small pieces of soft
vegetable or bread squares from plate to plate, without dropping the coin

597

9khj_dU9^&)-UcW_d$_dZZ+/- /%'+%(&&/(0'/0''FC
SECTION FIVE Working with the individual

Goal: After 10 minutes stretch, to move your forearm over so that ‘When you next reach forwards for the cup,
the cup touches the blue ‘blob’, hold for 10 seconds x 30 slide rather than lift your hand. Watch your
repeats hand and keep it the same shape as the cup.
Notice if your fingers and thumb are closing as
Stretching you reach. If they are, see if you can keep your
position fingers and thumb ‘soft’ as you reach.

or:

‘This time, when you close your fingers around


the polystyrene cup, don’t press so hard. Try
not to squash or deform the cup. If you press
too hard, the water will come up above the
marked line. Just use light pressure on the sides
Instructions of the cup’.
1. Sitting with your right arm supported on a table, elbow straight
2. Tape a cup into your hand so that your thumb web space is
stretched Dexterity training
3. Use your left hand to take your arm over and hold it
in a stretch
4. Take your left hand off, hold the cup on the ‘blob’ Some individuals can grasp and pick up objects, such
for 10 seconds as a cup, knife or fork, but cannot manipulate them.
5. Come back so that the cup is upright Training of advanced hand function involves more
6. Move the cup back so that the lip touches the ‘blob’ than cutting up slices of bread or repeatedly copying
lines of writing. Careful analysis enables therapists
Date No. correct to determine which components of skilled perfor-
10/7 0/30 mance are missing or altered, in comparison to
21/7 0/30 normal manipulation. This stage of analysis and
27/7 0/20 training is most interesting but demands careful
30/7 3/30
03/8 5/30 observation and problem solving. Tasks requiring
advanced skill performance (and analysis) include
handwriting, use of cutlery or chopsticks, putting on
5XVdaT"&( } Supination practice jewellery, applying contact lenses, tying shoes, doing
Practice book showing the exercise (Forearm Supination)
and goal, in the learners own words: ‘After 10 minutes up buttons, using a screwdriver or mobile phone.
stretch, to move your forearm over so that the cup touches With small objects, training of grip force during
the blue ‘blob’, hold for 10 seconds’. Additional instructions lift-off and manipulation will be required, with rep-
have also been added, and a section for recording practice etitions and feedback. Healthy adults typically apply
attempts
a force slightly higher than the minimum required
in order to prevent object slippage (Nowak &
person to lift a light plastic cup off the table instead Hermsdorfer 2003). However, people with chronic
of a glass, or slide rather than lift a cup along the stroke and intact sensation (n  10) have been shown
table will help to reduce effort. If a person is unable to apply significantly greater mean grip forces (q
to grasp while reaching, taping a cup into their hand 39%) at lift-off compared to healthy adults (Quaney
will reduce the task demands and help the person et al 2005). The participants with stroke used a
to concentrate on specific features of reaching. If too greater safety margin. Blennerhassett and colleagues
much force is used, use a disposable polystyrene cup (2006) reported different findings for 45 people
which deforms easily when grasped, to give the with stroke and 45 healthy adults, who were able to
person feedback about their force production pick up a pen lid concealed from view, using a pinch
(Figures 37.10 and 37.11). grip. These authors reported prolonged time and
Different instructions may also help the person excessive grip force prior to commencing the lift in
to become more self-aware, and learn to use some half the people with stroke, as well as fluctuating
muscles more and others with less force. For forces and extreme slowness, but excessive safety
example: margins were not present in all cases.

598

9khj_dU9^&)-UcW_d$_dZZ+/. /%'+%(&&/(0'/0''FC
Optimising motor performance following brain impairment CHAPTER 37

A B

5XVdaT"&  } Practice to decrease finger and wrist flexion force while transporting a cup to drink or while
carrying liquid
The person has been asked to gently press the side of the polystyrene cup, and move the cup edge between the two lines
on the wooden stick (Figure 37.10A).
When the short-term goal has been achieved, the person can progress to transporting the cup of liquid up onto a box,
stand up while holding the cup, and, finally, walk while carrying the cup.
Short-term goal: Press the cup inwards 1 cm to the second pen mark, release and repeat 3 times.
Medium-term goal: In sitting, maintain the round shape of the cup (see Figure 37.10B) and lift onto a 5 cm box.
Medium-term goal: Maintain the round shape of the cup (Figure 37.10B) while standing up and sitting down 5 times from a
45 cm chair.
Long-term goal: Carry a full cup of water 3 times, from the kitchen to the dining room table, without spilling any liquid

The combined message for therapists from these present, but also anticipate contractures that may
studies is that people with stroke typically have dif- develop in the future. Loss of shoulder external
ficulty preparing a suitable grip force and using the rotation range of movement is common after stroke.
normal feed-forward mechanisms. Impaired sensa- In one study (n  25), people with stroke experi-
tion is likely to compound these problems. However, enced an average of 30% loss of external rotation
training strategies are likely to be similar for people (Andrews & Bohannon 1989), with some experienc-
with and without sensory impairment. Training ing a loss of up to 60% of their range. This loss of
needs to involve task-specific practice, with many range correlates with shoulder pain (Bohannon
repetitions and feedback. If a person has difficulty 1988) and impacts on occupational performance,
using a knife, fork or pen, they need to engage in particularly self-care tasks. Therefore, it is impor-
part-practise with these utensils. For example, tant for therapists to anticipate and prevent
picking up an object precisely without spinning or contractures.
rotating the handle, cutting food and writing all Muscle stretching has become the main interven-
require appropriate force production and accurate tion for managing muscle-length changes and con-
opposition of the forces of the thumb and fingers to tracture. Animal studies suggest that muscle
be successful. See Figure 37.12 as one example. stretches need to be sustained for more than a few
minutes to reverse length-related changes from
immobilisation (Goldspink & Williams 1990,
Preventing and managing Williams 1990). However, it is not possible or eco-
contractures nomical for stretches to be provided manually by
therapists for extended periods. Instead, stretches
During the therapy planning process, therapists need to be integrated into routine positioning pro-
need to not only consider the contractures that are grammes. For example, shoulder extensors and

599

9khj_dU9^&)-UcW_d$_dZZ+// /%'+%(&&/(0'/0'(FC
SECTION FIVE Working with the individual

5XVdaT"&  } Practice to modulate finger and


thumb flexion force while holding a plastic bottle
which deforms easily
The person has been asked to gently press the sides of the
plastic bottle, and control the water levels between the two
black lines on the tube. Too much pressure causes a jet of
water to shoot out the top, which gives immediate
feedback to the learner about the amount of force being
generated. The practice demands attention for successful
performance.
To construct the training device, first drill a hole in the top
of a plastic bottle cap. The hole should be just large B
enough to accommodate the suction tubing. Insert tubing
down through the hole, fill the bottle with water and seal
the unit tightly with the screw top. If necessary, seal the unit 5XVdaT"& ! } Practice of pen rotation
with tape to prevent air escaping. This practice aims to improve pen control and handwriting.
Short-term goal: In sitting, push water up and down The short-term goal is to rotate the pen/pencil 10 times in
between the two black lines 5 times, without water 30 seconds by the end of one week. The medium-term
escaping from the tube. goal is to rotate the pen/pencil 10 times in 20 seconds by
Medium-term goal: In sitting, keep the water level with the the end of 2 weeks.
upper black line and lift the bottle up onto a 5 cm box, 5 Instructions remind the person to:
times, without water escaping from the tube } a^[[cWT_T]_T]RX[P !cda]X]TPRWSXaTRcX^]
} PX\c^R^eTacWT]d]R^eTaP_T]\PaZP[^]VcWTQPaaT[
of the pen (see Figures 37.12A and 37.12B)
} P[[^fcWT_T]_T]RX[c^aTbcPVPX]bccWTfTQb_PRTfWX[T
practicing
internal rotator muscles can easily be stretched by } dbTcWT\XSS[T ]VTac^aTPSYdbc_T]_^bXcX^]fWT]
positioning the arm on a table, out to the side in necessary
supine in bed or on the floor (Figure 37.13). Range } Pe^XSdbX]VcWT^cWTaWP]Sc^WT[_
of movement can be monitored with simple mea- } PX\c^_aPRcXbTU^a$\X]dcTbg"cX\TbSPX[h $\X]dcTb
daily)
sures as illustrated.
} cah]^cc^W^[ScWT_T]cXVWc[h
More sustained stretches can be applied with } _aPRcXbTfXcWSXUUTaT]c_T]b_T]RX[bc^WT[_VT]TaP[XbT
serial casts to immobilise joints in their stretched this skill
position. Serial casts should only be considered for
more severe contractures of the elbow, wrist or
fingers because they impede motor training and may
reinforce learned non-use. Studies investigating the
effect of hand splinting to prevent contracture after
stroke and brain injury have shown no difference in

600

9khj_dU9^&)-UcW_d$_dZZ,&& /%'+%(&&/(0'/0')FC
Optimising motor performance following brain impairment CHAPTER 37

Britton 2005) or found no difference following


stretch/positioning.
There is, therefore, still uncertainty about
whether stretch interventions are effective in the
long term and, if they are, how long stretches should
be held for and how often they should be adminis-
tered. Animal research suggests that if stretches are
being used, they should be sustained for as long as
practically feasible (Harvey 2008). It may be that
stretches in humans need to be administered for
many hours a day, rather than minutes, and contin-
ued indefinitely in people with minimal motor
recovery.
Shoulder strapping appears to reduce shoulder
pain but not function. A Cochrane systematic review
5XVdaT"& " } Shoulder stretch to increase external (Ada et al 2005b) found that shoulder strapping
rotation for reaching with adhesive tape delayed the onset of shoulder
This is a gravity-assisted stretch aimed at lengthening the
right internal rotator muscles and increasing the range of
pain but did not reduce pain, improve function or
available external rotation. Limited external rotation is very contracture development. A more recent ran-
common after stroke and reduces reaching performance domised controlled trial post stroke (n  33) con-
during daily occupations such as eating and drinking. firmed the effect of strapping (Griffin & Bernhardt
This stretch can be done independently for up to 30 2006). These authors reported a mean of 26 pain-
minutes daily, lying on the bed or floor. Some people find
this stretch more comfortable if a pillow or rolled-up towel is free days for the intervention group compared to 19
placed under the humerus, as they can relax into the pain-free days in a placebo controlled group, and 16
stretch more easily. in the control group.
In this figure, this man’s goal was to relax both shoulders Shoulder slings and supports have not been well
during the stretch so that his right elbow was 20 cm above
researched despite their frequent use in practise
the floor (comparable with his unaffected left shoulder). At
the beginning of his outpatient therapy sessions, his right (Ada et al 2005b,c). Current expert opinion is that
elbow was 38 cm above the floor due to shortening of his external supports such as wheelchair and chair
internal rotators. attachments are needed to support the weight of the
arm (Foongchomcheay et al 2005). Triangular ban-
dages and collar and cuff slings may help to protect
the arm during transfers but do not physically reduce
wrist extensibility compared to controls (no splint), a joint subluxation. Electrical stimulation shows
even when splints were worn for many hours over more promise as an intervention than slings and sup-
4 weeks (Lannin et al 2003, 2007) and 3 months ports; however, ultimately individuals need active
(Harvey et al 2006). A recent Cochrane review also motor training, in supine and side lying to help them
concluded that hand splinting had no effect on func- learn to switch on paralysed muscles around the
tion, finger or wirst flexibility or pain, although this shoulder and upper arm.
conclusion was based on a small number of trials
(Tyson & Kent 2009).
Although anecdotal evidence suggests that con- Improving reaching and postural
tractures are a common secondary complication control in sitting
after brain impairment, evidence that long-duration
stretch lengthens shortened muscles or maintains Limited research has been conducted on sitting
their length is limited (Harvey et al 2002). Two balance training. A Cochrane review (Pollock et al
trials involving adults with stroke (Ada et al 2005a, 2003) highlighted the need for task-specific training
de Jong et al 2006) reported that shoulder and arm strategies instead of generalised ‘approaches’ which
positioning to promote stretch slowed down, but have been used in the past. Task-specific training
did not prevent, contracture development. Other strategies have been shown to be effective in ran-
studies involving stroke populations were either domised trials involving people with stroke (Dean &
underpowered (e.g. Dean et al 2000, Turton & Shepherd 1997, Dean et al 2007). These studies,

601

9khj_dU9^&)-UcW_d$_dZZ,&' /%'+%(&&/(0'/0'*FC
SECTION FIVE Working with the individual

which have already been discussed, involved reach- leg extensors). Table 37.2 presents a detailed analy-
ing in sitting to the affected side at distances greater sis and training suggestions.
than arm’s length. The ‘sham’ training or control
group received an equivalent amount of reaching
practise but within arm’s reach. After 2 weeks’
Future directions
training participants in the intervention group all
improved in the distance and speed with which they These are exciting times. New technologies and
could reach in comparison to the control group. In more rigorous methodologies provide increasing
addition, the increased weight-bearing practise support for theories of movement rehabilitation. We
improved the participants’ ability to stand up. know that the earlier rehabilitation begins, the
General principles for training reaching in sitting better the recovery from conditions such as stroke
include the following: minimise the effort required and brain injury. Greater intensity of treatment
to sit; check that both feet are on the ground; translates into better outcomes. Gains in motor
provide good thigh support to maximise the base of control and recovery continue for many years. Ther-
support; limit the distance reached initially; intro- apists are moving away from one-on-one, hands-on
duce forward reaching, and reaching to the unaf- therapy and making better use of circuit and group
fected side before reaching across to the affected training programmes. And as rehabilitation research
side which requires maximum leg extensor muscle continues to grow, so too will the evidence on which
control; if unable to activate the leg extensor muscles therapists base their practice.
on the affected side, the person will be unable to The need for increased intensity of practice has
control movement onto the affected side; help led to the testing of two more novel rehabilitation
people learn to activate their leg extensor muscles, techniques, CIMT and robotics. Reinkensmeyer
and anticipate the need to turn those muscles on and colleagues (2004) report that robotic therapy
prior to beginning to moving towards that side; as allows for some of the labour-intensive training
the person improves their ability to turn their leg tasks performed currently by therapists to be per-
extensor muscles on, task difficulty can be increased. formed by automated devices, thereby providing
This can be done by reaching across to the affected people with greater access to therapy. As the evi-
side, increasing the distance reached, decreasing the dence grows in support of more intensive therapy,
amount of thigh support and increasing the chair constraint and robotics will be used more often
height (which increases the force required from the because they can increase practise and may lead

Table 37.2 Summary of motor control problems affecting the upper limb and seated reaching, and possible interventions for people
with neurological conditions

Motor control problem Possible interventions and evidence from key studies
Eliciting movement in paralysed } Repetitive contractions and practice of shoulder protraction in sitting (Feys et al 1998,
muscles 2004)
} Electrical stimulation of the wrist extensor and forearm (Powell et al 1999), and
shoulder muscles (Ada & Foongchomcheay 2002)
} Mental practice (Braun et al 2006, Dijkerman et al 2004)
} Mirror box therapy (Altschuler et al 1999, Yavuzer et al 2008)
Increasing force generation or } 4[TRcaXRP[bcX\d[PcX^]^UcWTfaXbcTgcT]b^aP]SU^aTPa\?^fT[[TcP[ (((P]S
strength in weak muscles shoulder muscles (Ada & Foongchomcheay 2002, Pomeroy et al 2006)
Decreasing force in overactive } AT_TcXcXeTR^]caPRcX^]bP]S_aPRcXRTfaXbcP]SU^aTPa\\dbR[Tb1dcT bRWTcP[ (($
muscles
Increasing dexterity, speed and } 2^]bcaPX]cX]SdRTS\^eT\T]ccWTaP_h4[XPbb^]TcP[!$7PZZT]]TbTcP[!$
control } Task-related training in groups (Blennerhassett et al 2004)
Preventing or reversing muscle } BcaTcRWX]VX]cTa]P[a^cPc^abX]bd_X]TU^a"\X]dcTbSPX[h\PhWT[_c^STRaTPbT[^bb
contractures ^UTgcTa]P[a^cPcX^]aP]VT^U\^eT\T]c0SPTcP[!$

602

9khj_dU9^&)-UcW_d$_dZZ,&( /%'+%(&&/(0'/0'*FC
Optimising motor performance following brain impairment CHAPTER 37

to greater motor recovery. Mirror box therapy is and adults with brain impairment. The content is
another intervention which is supported by two necessarily impairment-focused because much of
randomised trials (Altschuler et al, 1999; Yavuzer upper-limb rehabilitation, particularly in hospital
et al, 2008). Like CIMT, this intervention allows settings, focuses on eliciting muscle activity and
independent practice by people with stroke. With strength training prior to return of functional
technologies improving all the time, it is not possible grasp. At this stage, therapists need to remind
to predict what advances will become routine prac- themselves and the people they work with of the
tice in the future. The important message is, there- occupational goals of training, for example, eating
fore, to remain abreast of current scientific evidence. a meal with family members using cutlery in both
hands. Once a person has an effective grasp, can
hold and manipulate objects, tasks and goals are
Conclusion more obvious. While the overall goal of occupa-
tional therapy is to increase engagement in occu-
This chapter has focused on the process of analys- pations, we cannot and should not ignore
ing and retraining motor performance in children impairment-focused intervention.

References
Ada, L. & Canning, C. (2005). controlled trial. Archives of Physical Bell, A. R. & Murray, B. J. (2005).
Changing the way we view the Medicine and Rehabilitation, 86, Improvement in upper limb motor
contribution of motor impairments 230–234. performance following stroke: The
to physical disability after stroke. Ada, L., Dorsch, S. & Canning, C. use of mental practice. British
In: K. Refshauge, L. Ada & E. Ellis (2006a). Strengthening Journal of Occupational Therapy,
(Eds). Science-based rehabilitation: interventions to increase strength 67, 501–507.
theories into practice. (pp. 87–106). and improve activity after stroke: a Bilodeau, I. M. (1966). Information
Edinburgh, Elsevier, systematic review. Australian feedback. In E. A. Bilodeau (Ed.).
Butterworth-Heinemann. Journal of Physiotherapy, 52, Acquisition of skill. (pp. 225–296).
Ada, L., Canning, C. & Dwyer, T. 241–248. New York, Academic Press.
(2000). Effect of muscle length on Ada, L., O’Dwyer, N. & O’Neill, E. Blennerhassett, J. & Dite, W. (2004).
strength and dexterity after stroke. (2006b). Relation between Additional task-related practice
Clinical Rehabilitation, 14, 55–61. spasticity, weakness and contracture improves mobility and upper limb
Ada, L. & Foongchomcheay, A. of the elbow flexors and upper limb function early after stroke: a
(2002). Efficacy of electrical activity after stroke: an randomised controlled trial.
stimulation in preventing or observational study. Disability and Australian Journal of Physiotherapy,
reducing subluxation of the Rehabilitation, 28, 891–897. 50, 219–224.
shoulder after stroke: a meta- Adams, J. A. (1971). A closed-loop Blennerhassett, J. M., Carey, L. M. &
analysis. Australian Journal of theory of motor learning. Journal of Matyas, T. A. (2006). Grip force
Physiotherapy, 48, 257–267. Motor Behavior, 3, 111–150. regulation during pinch grip lifts
Ada, L., Foongchomcheay, A. & Altschuler, E. L., Wisdom, S. B. & under somatosensory guidance:
Canning, C. (2005a). Supportive Stone, L. (1999). Rehabilitation of comparison between people with
devices for preventing and treating hemiparesis after stroke with a stroke and healthy controls.
subluxation of the shoulder after mirror. Lancet, 353, 2035–2036. Archives of Physical Medicine and
stroke. The Cochrane Database of Andrews, A. W. & Bohannon, R. W. Rehabilitation, 87, 418–429.
Systematic Reviews Issue 1. (1989). Decreased range of motion Bohannon, R. W. (1988). Relationship
Ada, L., Foongchomcheay, A. & on paretic side after stroke. between shoulder pain and selected
Canning, C. (2005b). Use of Physical Therapy, 69, 768–722. variables in patients with
devices to prevent subluxation of Annett, J. (1959). Learning a pressure hemiplegia. Clinical Rehabilitation,
the shoulder after stroke. under conditions of immediate and 2, 111–117.
Physiotherapy Research delayed knowledge of results. Boyd, R. N. & Ada, L. (2001).
International, 10, 134–145. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Physiotherapy management of
Ada, L., Goddard, E., McCully, J., Psychology, 11, 3–15. spasticity. In M. P. Barnes & G. R.
Stavrinos, T. & Bampton, J. Johnson (Eds). Upper motor
Bayona, N. A., Bitensky, J., Salter, K.
(2005c). Thirty minutes of neurone syndrome and spasticity.
& Teasell, R. (2005). The role of
positioning reduces the (pp. 96–103). Cambridge,
task-specific training in
development of external rotation Cambridge University Press.
rehabilitation therapies. Topics in
but not flexion contracture in the Stroke Rehabilitation, 12, Braun, S. M., Beurskens, A. J., Borm,
shoulder after stroke: a randomised 58–65. P. J. et al (2006). The effects of

603

9khj_dU9^&)-UcW_d$_dZZ,&) /%'+%(&&/(0'/0'*FC
SECTION FIVE Working with the individual

mental practice in stroke controlled trial. Stroke, 28, Feys, H. M., de Weerdt, W. J., Selz,
rehabilitation: a systematic review. 722–728. B. E. et al (1998). Effect of a
Archives of Physical Medicine and Dean, C., Shepherd, R. & Adam, R. therapeutic intervention for the
Rehabilitation, 87, 842–852. (1999a) Sitting balance I: trunk and hemiplegic upper limb in the acute
Burke, D. (1988). Spasticity as an arm coordination and the phase after stroke: a single-blind
adaptation to pyramidal tract contribution of the lower limbs randomized multicentre trial.
injury. Advances in Neurology, 47, during self-paced reaching in sitting. Stroke, 29, 785–792.
401–423. Gait and Posture, 10, 135–146. Feys, H. M., de Weerdt, W. J.,
Butefisch, C., Hummelsheim, H., Dean, C., Shepherd, R. & Adam, R. Verbeke, G. et al (2004). Early and
Denzler, P. et al. (1995). Repetitive (1999b). Sitting balance II: reach repetitive stimulation of the arm
training of isolated movements direction and thigh support affect can substantially improve the
improves the outcome of motor the contribution of the lower limbs long-term outcome after stroke: a
rehabilitation of the centrally when reaching beyond arm’s length 5-year follow-up study of a
paretic hand. Journal of the in sitting. Gait and Posture, 10, randomized trial. Stroke, 35,
Neurological Sciences, 130, 59–68. 147–153. 924–929.
Carr, J. H. & Shepherd, R. B. (1987). Dean, C., Katrak, P. & Mackey, F. Fitts, P. M. & Posner, M. I. (1967).
A motor relearning programme for (2000). Examination of shoulder Human performance. Belmont, CA,
stroke. (2nd ed). Oxford, positioning after stroke: a Brooks/Cole.
Heinemann Medical. randomized controlled pilot trial. Foongchomcheay, A., Ada, L. &
Carr, J. H. & Shepherd, R. B. (1989). Australian Journal of Physiotherapy, Canning, C. (2005). Use of devices
A motor learning model for stroke 46, 35–40. to prevent subluxation of the
rehabilitation. Physiotherapy, 75(7), Dean, C., Channon, E. & Hall, J. shoulder after stroke. Physiotherapy
372–380. (2007). Sitting training early after Research International, 10(3),
Carr, J. H., Shepherd, R. B., Gordon, stroke improves sitting ability and 134–145.
J. et al (1987). Movement science: quality and carries over to standing Forssberg, H. & Hirschfeld, H. (1994).
foundations for physical therapy in up but not to walking: a Postural adjustments in sitting
rehabilitation. Rockville, MD, randomised controlled trial. humans following external
Aspen. Australian Journal of Physiotherapy, perturbations: muscle activity and
Carr, J. H. & Shepherd, R. B. (2003). 53, 97–102. kinematics. Experimental Brain
Reaching and manipulation. In: Dickson, M. (2002). Rehabilitation of Research, 97, 515–527.
Stroke rehabilitation: guidelines for motor control following stroke: Goldspink, G. & Williams, P. (1990).
exercise and training to optimize searching the evidence. British Muscle fibre and connective tissue
motor performance. (pp. 159–206). Journal of Occupational Therapy, changes associated with use and
London, Butterworth-Heinemann. 65, 269–274. disuse. In L. Ada & C. Canning
Chari, V. R. & Kirby, R. L. (1986). Djikerman, H. C., Letswaart, M., (Eds.) Key issues in neurological
Lower-limb influence on sitting Johnston, M. et al. (2004). Does physiotherapy. (pp. 197–218).
balance while reaching forward. motor imagery training improve Oxford, Butterworth-Heinemann.
Archives of Physical Medicine and hand function in chronic stroke Gossman, M. R., Sahrmann, S. A. &
Rehabilitation, 67, 73–733. patients? A pilot study. Clinical Rose, S. J. (1982). Review of
Charness, N. (1981). Search in chess: Rehabilitation, 18, 538–549. length associated changes in muscle.
age and skill differences. Journal of Eliasson, A. C., Bonnier, B. & Physical Therapy, 62, 1799–1808.
Experimental Psychology: Human Krumlinde-Sundholm, L. (2003). Griffin, A. & Bernhardt, J. (2006).
Perception and Performance, 7, Clinical experience of constraint Strapping the hemiplegic shoulder
467–476. induced movement therapy in prevents development of pain
Crosbie, J., Shepherd, R. & Squire, T. adolescents with hemiplegic during rehabilitation: a randomized
(1995). Postural and voluntary cerebral palsy – A day camp model. controlled trial. Clinical
movement during reaching in Developmental Medicine and Child Rehabilitation, 20, 287–295.
sitting: the role of the lower limbs. Neurology, 45, 357–359. Haggard, P. (2001). The psychology of
Journal of Human Movement Eliasson, A. C., Krumlinde-Sundholm, action. British Journal of
Studies, 28, 103–126. L., Shaw, K. et al (2005). Effects Psychology, 92, 113–128.
de Jong, L. D., Nieuwboer, A. & of constraint-induced movement Hakkennes, S. & Keating, J. L. (2005).
Aufdemkampe, G. (2006). therapy in young children with Constraint-induced movement
Contracture preventive positioning hemiplegic cerebral palsy: An therapy following stroke: A
of the hemiplegic arm in subacute adapted model. Developmental systematic review of randomised
stroke patients: a pilot randomized Medicine and Child Neurology, 47, controlled trials. Australian
controlled trial. Clinical 266–275. Journal of Physiotherapy, 51,
Rehabilitation, 20, 656–667. Ericsson, K. A. (2004). Deliberate 221–231.
Dean, C. & Shepherd, R. (1997). Task practice and the acquisition and Harvey, L. (2008). Management of
related training improves maintenance of expert performance spinal cord injuries: a guide for
performance of seated reaching in medicine and related domains. physiotherapists. United States,
tasks after stroke: a randomised Academic Medicine, 79, S70–S81. Butterworth Heinemann.

604

9khj_dU9^&)-UcW_d$_dZZ,&* /%'+%(&&/(0'/0'+FC
Optimising motor performance following brain impairment CHAPTER 37

Harvey, L., Herbert, R. & Crosbie, J. Issues for consideration. Disability Morris, D. M., Crago, J. E., Deluca, S.
(2002). Does stretching induce and Rehabilitation, 28, 823–830. et al (1997). Constraint induced
lasting increases in joint ROM? A Lannin, N. A., Horsley, S. A., Herbert, movement therapy for motor
systematic review. Physiotherapy R. et al (2003). Hand splinting in recovery after stroke.
Research International, 7, 1–13. the functional position after brain Neurorehabilitation, 9, 29–43.
Harvey, L., de Jong, I., Goehl, G. & impairment: A randomised Neilson, P. & McCaughey, J. (1982).
Mardwedel, S. (2006). Twelve controlled trial. Archives of Physical Self-regulation of spasm in cerebral
weeks of nightly stretch does not Medicine and Rehabilitation, 84, palsy. Journal of Neurology,
reduce thumb web-space 297–302. Neurosurgery and Psychiatry, 45,
contractures in people with a Lannin, N. A., Cusick, A., McCluskey, 320–330.
neurological condition: a A. & Herbert, R. (2007). Effects of Neistadt, M. (1994). The effect of
randomised controlled trial. The splinting on wrist contracture different treatment activities on
Australian Journal of Physiotherapy. following stroke: a randomised functional fine motor coordination
52, 251–258. controlled trial. Stroke, 38, in adults with brain injury.
Helsen-Starkes, J. L. & Hodges, N. J. 111–116. American Journal of Occupational
(1998). Team sports and the theory Lee, T. D. & White, M. A. (1990). Therapy, 48(10), 877–882.
of deliberate practice. Journal of Influence of an unskilled model’s Newell, K. M. (1976). Knowledge of
Sport and Exercise Psychology, 20, practice schedule on observational results and motor learning. Exercise
12–34. motor learning. Human Movement and Sport Sciences Reviews, 4,
Herbert, R. B. & Balnave, R. J. (1993). Science, 9, 349–367. 195–228.
The effect of position of Lehmann, A. C. & Ericsson, K.A. Nowak, D. A. & Hermsdorfer, J.
immobilisation on the resting (1996). Music performance without (2003). Selective deficits of grip
length, resting stiffness and weight preparation: Structure acquisition force during object manipulation in
of rabbit soleus muscle. Journal of of expert sight reading. patients with reduced sensibility of
Orthopaedic Research, 11, Psychomusicology, 15, 1–29. the grasping digits. Neuroscience
358–366. Martenuik, R. G., Mackenzie, C. L., Research, 47, 65–72.
Ho, L. & Shea, J. B. (1978). Effects of Jeannerod, M. et al (1987). O’Dwyer, N. J., Ada, L. & Neilson, P.
relative frequency of knowledge of Constraints on human arm D. (1996). Spasticity and muscle
results on retention of a motor movement trajectories. Canadian contracture following stroke. Brain,
skill. Perceptual and Motor Skills, Journal of Physiotherapy, 4, 119, 1737–1749.
46, 859–866. 365–378. Pandyan, A. D., Cameron, M., Powell,
Hoare, B., Imms, C., Carey, L. & Martenuik, R. G., Leavitt, J. L., J. et al (2003). Contractures in the
Wasiak, J. (2007). Constraint- Mackenzie, C. L. et al (1990). post-stroke wrist: a pilot study of
induced movement therapy in the Functional relationships between its time course of development and
treatment of the upper limb in grasp and transport components in its association with upper limb
children with hemiplegic cerebral a prehension task. Human recovery. Clinical Rehabilitation,
palsy: a Cochrane systematic Movement Science, 9, 149–176. 17(1), 88–95.
review. Clinical Rehabilitation, 21, Mastos, M., Miller, K., Eliasson, A. C. Pollock, A., Baer, G., Pomeroy, V.
675–685. et al (2007). Goal-directed training: et al (2003). Physiotherapy treatment
Jeannerod, M. (1984). The timing of linking theories of treatment to approaches for postural control and
natural prehension movements. clinical practice for improved lower limb function following stroke.
Journal of Motor Behaviour, 26, functional activities in daily life. The Cochrane Database of Systematic
235–254. Clinical Rehabilitation, 21, Reviews, Issue 2.
Jeannerod, M. (1986). The formation 47–55. Pomeroy, V. M., King, L., Pollock, A.
of finger grip during prehension: a McLellan, D. L. (1977). Co-contraction et al (2006). Electrostimulation for
cortically mediated visuomotor and stretch reflexes in spasticity promoting recovery of movement
pattern. Behaviour and Brain during treatment with Baclofen. or functional ability after stroke.
Research, 19, 99–116. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, The Cochrane Database of
Kernodle, M. W. & Carlton, L. G. and Psychiatry, 40, 30–38. Systematic Reviews, Issue 2.
(1992). Information feedback and McNevin, N. H., Wulf, G. & Carlson, Powell, J., Pandyan, D., Granat, M.
the learning of multiple-degree-of- C. (2000). Effects of attentional et al (1999). Electrical stimulation
freedom activities. Journal of Motor focus, self-control, and dyad of wrist extensors on poststroke
Behaviour, 24, 187–196. training on motor learning: hemiplegia. Stroke, 30, 1384–1389.
Kilduski, N. C. & Rice, M. S. (2003). implications for physical Prinz, W. (1997). Perception and
Qualitative and quantitative rehabilitation. Physical Therapy, 80, action planning. European Journal of
knowledge of results: Effects on 373–385. Cognitive Psychology, 9, 129–154.
motor learning. American Journal of Michaelsen, S. M., Dannenbaum, R. & Quaney, B. M., Perera, S., Maletsky,
Occupational Therapy, 57, Levin, M. F. (2006). Task-specific R., Luchies, C. W. & Nudo, R. J.
329–336. training with trunk restraint on arm (2005). Impaired grip force
Kwakkel, G. (2006). Impact of recovery in stroke. Stroke, 27, modulation in the ipsilesional hand
intensity of practice after stroke: 186–192. after unilateral middle cerebral

605

9khj_dU9^&)-UcW_d$_dZZ,&+ /%'+%(&&/(0'/0'+FC
SECTION FIVE Working with the individual

artery stroke. Neurorehabilitation impairment. Pediatrics, 113, Williams, P. (1990). Use of


and Neural Repair, 19, 338–349. 305–312. intermittent stretch in the
Refshauge, K. M., Ada, L. & Ellis, E. Trudel, G., Himori, K., Goudreau, L. prevention of serial sarcomere loss
(Eds) (2005). Science-based et al (2003). Measurement of in immobilised muscle. Annals of
rehabilitation: theories into practice. articular cartilage surface Rheumatological Disease, 49,
Edinburgh, Elsevier, irregularity in rat knee contracture. 316–317.
Butterworth-Heinemann. Journal of Rheumatology, 30, Williams, P. E. & Goldspink, G.
Reinkensmeyer, D. J., Emken, J. L. & 2218–2225. (1978) Changes in sarcomere
Cramer, S. C. (2004). Robotics, Turton, A. & Britton, E. (2005). A length and physiological properties
motor learning, and neurologic pilot randomized controlled trial of in immobilized muscle. Journal of
recovery. Annual Review of a daily muscle stretch regime to Anatomy, 127, 459–468.
Biomedical Engineering, 6, prevent contractures in the arm Willis, J. K., Morello, A., Davie, A.
497–525. after stroke. Clinical Rehabilitation, et al (2002). Forced use treatment
Reo, J. A. & Mercer, V. S. (2004). 19, 600–612. of childhood hemiparesis.
Effects of live, videotaped, or Tyson, S. & Kent, R. (2009). Orthotic Pediatrics, 110, 94–96.
written instruction on learning an devices after stroke and other Wing, A. M., Turton, A., Fraser, C.
upper-extremity exercise program. non-progressive brain lesions. The (1986). Grasp size and accuracy of
Physical Therapy, 84, 622–633. Cochrane Database of Systematic approach in reaching. Journal of
Schmidt, R. A. & Lee, T. D. (1999). Reviews, Issue 1, Art. No.: Motor Behaviour, 18, 81–260.
Motor control and learning. A CD003694. DOI: Winstein, C. J. & Schmidt, R. A.
behavioral emphasis. Human 003610.001002/14651858. (1990). Reduced frequency of
Kinetics Champaign, IL. CD14003694.pub14651852. knowledge of result enhances
Shea, C. H., Wright, D. L., Wulf, G. van der Lee, J., Snels, I. A. K., motor skill learning. Journal of
et al (2000). Physical and Beckerman, H. et al (2001). Experimental Psychology, 16,
observational practice afford unique Exercise therapy for arm function 677–691.
learning opportunities. Journal of in stroke patients: A systematic Wu, C. Y., Trombly, C. A. & Lin,
Motor Behavior, 32, 27–36. review of randomised controlled K. C. (1994). The relationship
Sietsema, J. M., Nelson, D. L., trials. Clinical Rehabilitation, 15, between occupational form and
Mulder, R. M. et al (1993). The 20–31. occupational performance: A
use of a frame to promote arm van Vliet, P. M. (1998). An kinematic perspective. American
reach in persons with traumatic investigation of reaching movements Journal of Occupational Therapy,
brain injury. American Journal of following stroke. PhD thesis, 48, 679–688.
Occupational Therapy, 47(1), University of Nottingham, Wu, C. W., Seo, H. J. & Cohen, L. G.
19–24. Nottingham. (2006). Influence of electric
Sommerfield, D. K., Eek, E. U. B., van Vliet, P. M. & Wulf, G. (2006). somatosensory stimulation on
Svensson, A. K. et al (2003). Extrinsic feedback for motor paretic-hand function in chronic
Spasticity after stroke: its learning after stroke: what is the stroke. Archives of Physical
occurrence and association with evidence? Disability and Medicine and Rehabilitation
motor impairments and activity Rehabilitation, 28, 831–840. Medicine, 87, 351–357.
limitations. Stroke, 35, 134–140. Vattanasilp, W., Ada, L. & Crosbie, J. Yavuzer, G., Selles, R., Sezer, N. et al
Tabary, J. C., Tabary, J. C., Tardieu, (2000). Contribution of thixotropy, (2008). Mirror therapy improves
C. et al (1972) Physiological and spasticity and contracture to ankle hand function in subacute stroke: A
structural changes in the cat’s stiffness after stroke. Journal of randomized controlled trial.
soleus muscle due to Neurology, Neurosurgery and Archives of Physical Medicine and
immobilization at different lengths Psychiatry, 69, 34–39. Rehabilitation, 89(3), 393–398.
by plaster case. Journal of Wallen, M., Ziviani, J., Herbert, R., Zoia, S., Pezzetta, E., Blason, L. et al
Physiology (London), 224, 231–244. Evans, R. & Novak, I. (2007). (2006). A comparison of the
Taub, E., Landesman Ramey, S., Modified constraint-induced reach-to-grasp movement between
DeLuca, S. et al (2004). Efficacy of therapy for children with children and adults: a kinematic
constraint-induced movement hemiplegic cerebral palsy: A study. Developmental
therapy for children with cerebral feasibility study. Developmental Neuropsychology, 30(2),
palsy with symmetric motor Neurorehabilitation, Oct 15, 1–10. 719–738.

606

9khj_dU9^&)-UcW_d$_dZZ,&, /%'+%(&&/(0'/0'+FC
Disability and Rehabilitation, 2005; 27(20): 1213 – 1223

RESEARCH ARTICLE

Upper limb recovery after stroke: The stroke survivors’ perspective

R.N. BARKER & S.G. BRAUER

Division of Physiotherapy, School of Health & Rehabilitation Sciences, University of Queensland, Brisbane, Queensland,
Australia

Accepted February 2005


Disabil Rehabil Downloaded from informahealthcare.com by University of Sydney

Abstract
Purpose. This study investigated stroke survivors’ perspective of upper limb recovery after stroke. The aim was to determine
factors other than medical diagnosis and co-morbidities that contribute to recovery. The objectives were to explore how
stroke survivors define recovery, identify factors they believe influence recovery and determine strategies used to maximize
upper limb recovery.
Method. A qualitative study consisting of three focus groups and two in-depth interviews was conducted with stroke
survivors (n = 19) and spouses (n = 9) in metropolitan, regional and rural Queensland, Australia. Data were analysed using
principles of grounded theory.
For personal use only.

Results. Stroke survivors maximize upper limb recovery by ‘keeping the door open’ a process of continuing to hope for and
work towards improvement amidst adjusting to life with stroke. They achieve this by ‘hanging in there’, ‘drawing on support
from others’, ‘getting going and keeping going with exercise’, and ‘finding out how to keep moving ahead’.
Conclusions. This study provides valuable insight into the personal experience of upper limb recovery after stroke. It
highlights the need to develop training strategies that match the needs and aspirations of stroke survivors and that place no
time limits on recovery. It reinforces the benefits of stroke support groups and advocates their incorporation into stroke
recovery services. These findings can be used to guide both the development and evaluation of stroke survivor centred upper
limb training programmes.

Keywords: Stroke survivors’ perspective, upper limb, recovery

Any attempts to improve upper limb recovery after


Background
stroke across the population must be investigated
Upper limb recovery after stroke is unacceptably within the context of the current health climate. Over
poor with only 50% of stroke survivors likely to the last decade an increasing number of people are
regain some functional use [1] compared with 82% surviving stroke but with rising levels of disability [8].
who could expect to walk independently again [2]. Greater demand is being placed on rehabilitation
This disparity has been attributed to minimal time services at a time when cost containment is leading to
spent on the upper limb during rehabilitation [3], the reduction in hospital length of stay. With time
lack of spontaneous use of the arm for function and available for upper limb training rapidly diminishing,
the complexity of upper limb function necessitating the search for effective and efficient strategies to
greater recovery of motor control to achieve function maximize upper limb recovery has become more
[4]. Although allocation of stroke recovery services pressing.
have been traditionally based on the belief that As the first step, consideration must be given
recovery occurs within the first three months and is to factors known to contribute to recovery.
complete by twelve months [5], further improvement Previous studies have shown that the site and
has been shown to occur with intervention beyond severity of the lesion [9] and the impact of co-
that period [6,7]. morbidities [10] is associated with recovery of the

Correspondence: Ruth Barker, Division of Physiotherapy, School of Health and Rehabilitation Sciences, University of Queensland, Qld 4072, Australia.
Tel: 61 7 33652275; Fax: 61 7 33652775. E-mail: r.barker@shrs.uq.edu.au
ISSN 0963-8288 print/ISSN 1464-5165 online ª 2005 Taylor & Francis
DOI: 10.1080/09638280500075717
1214 R.N. Barker & S.G. Brauer

arm and hand after stroke. Upper limb training


Methodology
programmes involving intensive and repetitive task
related practice [7,11] have been shown to A qualitative research design was chosen for this
improve recovery. What has not been investigated study. Three focus groups (n = 19 stroke survivors +
is the extent to which stroke survivors want to or 9 spouses) and two in-depth interviews (n = 2 stroke
are able to use these interventions [12] that survivors) were used to gather the raw data over a six
demand time, energy and long-term commitment. month period. The data were collected, analysed and
With no studies on upper limb recovery from compared with the literature using principles of
stroke survivors’ point of view, little is known grounded theory [17] for the purpose of gaining
about what upper limb deficits mean to indivi- insight, not to generate theory.
dual stroke survivors, or their differing beliefs and The nature of the research question and the
behaviours with regards to upper limb recovery. decision to collaborate with two stroke groups,
Clearly the perspective of stroke survivors is determined the method. Principles of grounded
essential in building a comprehensive framework theory were considered most appropriate for explor-
to promote upper limb recovery and in ensuring ing the experience from stroke survivors’ point of
adherence to demanding rehabilitation routines. view and for discovering the methods used by them
This study arose out of the belief that stroke to promote recovery. Face to face forums were
Disabil Rehabil Downloaded from informahealthcare.com by University of Sydney

survivors had a story to tell and that much could preferred as a more natural setting for conversation
be learnt from their experience. Explanations as with the advantage that non-verbal and spontaneous
to why some individuals experience an unexpect- reactions of participants are witnessed. Focus group
edly good recovery compared with others could interviews were used to collect information from
reveal mechanisms used by stroke survivors to various points of view and to take advantage of the
enhance recovery. Stroke survivors views on richness of group data. In-depth interviews were
current health service provision could provide used to complement this method by providing more
insight into why some aspects of rehabilitation are personal and individual responses that would not be
For personal use only.

more acceptable than others and highlight a influenced by the group or could not be drawn out in
broader range of issues that need to be con- a group, due to the personal nature or lack of
sidered in planning of services [13]. Their approval from the group. They also offered the
experience of transitions of care from the acute opportunity to address or challenge issues or points
admission through rehabilitation, discharge and that had either not been mentioned or not discussed
cessation of services may assist in anticipating in sufficient depth. The number of focus groups and
needs and tailoring services to meet them [14]. in-depth interviews conducted was determined by
Stroke survivors’ perspective casts light on long- saturation of data, deemed to be the point where
term recovery to which rehabilitation staff are nothing more was added to enhance or distinguish
rarely exposed and to which scant attention is emerging concepts.
given in the literature [15]. Ultimately, training
programmes developed will apply to stroke
Sampling process
survivors, and will most likely require them to
adopt new practices and take on new responsi- Initial sampling was purposive and self selected in an
bilities [16] as well as greater responsibility as attempt to include those from whom most could be
demand for services increases. Their continued learnt and to ensure a wide range of opinions.
participation is essential to ensure strategies Participants had experienced stroke affecting the arm
developed will be acceptable, relevant and acces- from 3 months to 13 years previously. Following the
sible to stroke survivors within the context of initial focus group interview, thematic sampling [17]
their lives. was used to broaden and challenge emerging
In an effort to provide fresh insights and a new categories, with participants selected according to
perspective for the development and delivery of rural, regional or metropolitan residence, self re-
effective training strategies, this paper reports the ported good and bad recovery, functional level,
results of a qualitative study investigating stroke absence or presence of a spouse or carer, time since
survivor’s perspective on upper limb recovery. The stroke, arm affected, age and gender. Finally,
aim of the study was to determine factors other than participants for two in-depth interviews were selected
medical diagnosis and co-morbidities that contribute on the basis of a self reported bad recovery of the arm
to recovery of the upper limb after stroke. The but not the leg and a good recovery of the arm, but
objectives were to explore how stroke survivors not the leg. The demographics of study participants
define recovery, identify factors they believe influ- are summarized in Table I.
ence recovery and lastly, determine strategies used to Participants were recruited using a network app-
maximize upper limb recovery. roach via two stroke support groups in Queensland.
Stroke survivors’ perspective on recovery 1215

Table I. Demographics of study participants.

ID Interview Age* Years** Since Stroke Gender Location Arm affected Arm function Spouse present

1 Focus group 1 66 3.5 M Rural L Nil + spouse


2 Focus group 1 67 1.5 M Rural R Nil + spouse
3 Focus group 1 75 2.5 M Rural L Nil + spouse
4 Focus group 1 55 7 M Rural R Nil + spouse
5 Focus group 1 71 10 M Rural R Nil + spouse
6 Focus group 1 72 10 M Rural R Some + spouse
7 Focus group 1 61 6 M Rural L Nil + spouse
8 Focus group 2 79 4.5 F Regional L Functional
9 Focus group 2 71 13 F Regional L Functional
10 Focus group 2 71 2.5 F Regional R Nil + spouse
11 Focus group 3 59 3 M Metro L Functional
12 Focus group 3 54 4.5 F Metro L Nil
13 Focus group 3 59 5 M Metro L Functional + spouse
14 Focus group 3 42 4.5 M Metro R Some
15 Focus group 3 62 1.5 M Metro R Some
16 Focus group 3 62 6.5 M Metro L Functional
17 Focus group 3 42 4 F Metro L Some
Disabil Rehabil Downloaded from informahealthcare.com by University of Sydney

18 In-depth 61 3 F Metro L Nil


19 In-depth 82 .4 F Metro L Functional

*Mean age * 64 yrs, age range 42 – 82 yrs.


**Mean time since stroke* 4.9 yrs, range 3 months – 13 yrs.

Members were advised of the specific criteria for the questions was developed to frame the information to
For personal use only.

first and each subsequent interview as well as the be gathered and to encourage participants to con-
time, date and venue. Those members and others tribute information in their own words. There was no
known to them who fitted the criteria were invited to strict adherence to the style and type of questioning
attend. This method of recruitment led to all beyond these four key questions with probes used to
participants being members of one of four stroke follow or challenge themes that were emerging.
support groups in Queensland. It also resulted in These questions are included in Table II.
attendance by not only those who had themselves
had a stroke but also by their spouse or partner,
Procedure for focus group and in-depth
particularly those with aphasia. Based on their belief
interviews
that as a couple they were stroke survivors and that
their differing perspectives and roles were both Each focus group interview consisted of stroke
critical in stroke recovery, no differentiation was survivors as participants, the one researcher as group
made between the views expressed. As the researcher leader and an experienced scribe who managed
had worked as a physiotherapist with stroke survivors taping of the session and took notes to record the
in metropolitan, rural and remote areas of Australia group dynamics and non-verbal language. Firstly,
some members of both stroke support groups had the study purpose and process was explained and
previous knowledge of the researcher. While this interview questions distributed. Participants were
could have led to reluctance to criticize it appeared to encouraged to immediately write down their an-
enhance the contribution made with participants swers, to capture spontaneous responses and those
holding the belief that the study offered real potential that had not yet been shaped by the group process.
for change. Each participant was then invited to tell their
personal story of stroke and their reasons for
attending. This was designed to build rapport and
Collection of data
trust that individual experiences would be valued and
Each focus group and in-depth interview was held on to allow presentation of information of particular
one occasion in the homes of participants and importance to participants. The questions specific to
conducted by one researcher. The length varied with the aims of the study followed with additional probes
earlier focus group interviews taking over ninety used to ensure reflection on their own experience
minutes and the final in-depth interview taking only and that of others with whom they had come in
thirty minutes. All interviews were taped once contact. All interviews were drawn to a close with the
consent had been given by all participants. A semi- group leader summing up the discussion and asking
structured format based on a set of open ended each participant to add to or dispute what had been
1216 R.N. Barker & S.G. Brauer

Table II. Focus group questions.

1. What is recovery?
Probes included: What is a good recovery? What is a bad recovery?
Think about someone who had a ‘good recovery’ and explain why.
Think about someone who has had a ‘bad recovery’ and explain why.
Do you consider yourself to have had a good or bad recovery and why?

2. What factors influence recovery?’


Probes included: How do you think we can influence recovery?
How can we enhance recovery? What are the obstacles to recovery?
How could your recovery have been better or worse? What can you do to influence your recovery? What can others do to influence your
recovery?

3. How do you think we can maximise recovery?


Probes included: How can we maximise recovery with the same resources we have now? How could we do it differently to get a better
result?
Disabil Rehabil Downloaded from informahealthcare.com by University of Sydney

said and add his or her final comments. Written and tapes transcribed verbatim, focus groups and
responses to the focus group questions were col- interview transcripts were explored by a process of
lected and included as data. The group leader and reading and re-reading. On the first reading tran-
scribe debriefed after the session documenting what scripts were read in their entirety to acquire a sense
might be considered the ‘group data’. of the whole. On the second reading, using line by
In each focus group, participants consistently line analysis, themes, patterns or concepts were
encouraged each other to share their unique story, identified and listed, being mindful of core themes or
showing respect and sensitivity particularly when the patterns emerging. Those that predominated were
For personal use only.

personal impact of the experience resulted in tentatively placed into four (4) conceptual categories.
expression of strong emotions. However concern The third reading involved returning to the data to
for the feelings of others could limit the depth of check the ‘fit’ of the categories, pursuing patterns
discussion particularly with regards to the attitudes both consistent and inconsistent with the categories
and behaviour of those with a good recovery defined. The original categories were modified to
compared with those with a bad recovery. For this more effectively represent or ‘fit’ the emergent
reason the in-depth interviews provided the oppor- patterns in the data. The analysis then involved
tunity to gain greater depth in this regard. specifying the properties of each category, noting the
The same order and content was used to conduct conditions under which it arose, relating how it
the in-depth interviews but with less structure changed, describing its consequences and ultimately
imposed on the interview. Participants were encour- specifying its relationship to the other categories.
aged to talk with questions raised only when a Finally, a core category was identified as the central
particular aspect was not covered or clarification was phenomenon underpinning, linking and integrating
required. Consistent with the purpose of the inter- the emergent categories and accounting for varia-
view, probes were more personal and often related to bility in the data.
the interviewee and others known to them. Whilst limited attention was given to the literature
Ethical approval was obtained from the University for the initial conception of the study, specific review
of Queensland Behavioural and Social Sciences of the literature only occurred after commitment to
Ethical Review Committee. All participants con- the categories and to the main analytical message, in
sented to taping of interviews, having been informed order to guard against preconceived categories or
in writing and verbally that all tapes would be ideas emerging from the literature. This review was
transcribed verbatim and that confidentiality in undertaken according to the four (4) categories and
reporting of study findings was assured. the core category. As a result, concepts were not
borrowed from the literature, rather the categories
that emerged influenced the selection and ordering of
Data analysis
relevant literature. Concepts that appeared repeatedly
Analysis occurred continuously throughout the in the literature, were used to check the data to
study. During data collection, naı̈ve interpretation discover whether or not they applied in this context.
followed immediately after each interview to ensure In order to verify and extend the emerging
that what had been learnt could be used to extend framework [18] member checking occurred through-
questioning, seek further clarification or challenge in out the study. Due to the geographical spread of
the following interview. Once all data were collected study participants, the researcher reported back to
Stroke survivors’ perspective on recovery 1217

participants when they were attending stroke group . . .some use . . . to hold something . . ...I just want to hold
meetings. This occurred on five occasions, twice with the nail and hammer it in . . .you have to have two
each of the two main stroke groups and once in the hands. . .. . .woodwork . . . in the shed . . .
final stages, during a meeting with representatives of
both rural and metropolitan focus groups in atten- A bad recovery was to lose hope, forget about
dance. This led to small additions or modifications even trying to use the arm and to find no way
to content at each stage as well as the renaming of around or substitute for what had been lost. A
conceptual categories in the final stage, reinforcing good recovery was to have some return of
the personal rather than clinical nature of the movement and feeling, use of the hand, to do
experience. During the period of analysis the what you want to do and to get on with your life
researcher also interacted extensively with stroke again, believing there could always be further
survivors involved in a larger research project of improvement. Interestingly, participants believed
which this study was a part. Peer review was recovery only came to an end if the stroke
undertaken with an experienced qualitative research- survivor ‘gave up’.
er and coder checking with a novice researcher who
had an acquired disability. Over the length of the
Keeping the door open
study, findings were presented to clinical experts and
Disabil Rehabil Downloaded from informahealthcare.com by University of Sydney

to a number of professional audiences. The core conceptual category, identified as the key
process used by stroke survivors to maximise upper
limb recovery, is described as ‘keeping the door
Results
open’, a process of ‘continuing along in life hoping
Upper limb recovery was viewed by participants as a for and working towards improvement’. Participants
critical but neglected issue, with most participants emphasized the importance of not placing time limits
feeling the magnitude of their loss was poorly under- on recovery, and of remaining open to future
stood or appreciated. Expressions of disappointment, possibilities, better services, more answers or further
For personal use only.

frustration and anger prevailed in this regard. spontaneous recovery. They frequently supported
this optimistic and open ended approach by citing
It’s a big deal to be able to use your arm again. I think most their own or others experience of progress many
of the doctors think it’s not. It’s a big deal to be able to use years after stroke.
your arm again psychologically as well physically.
Even years later things are still changing as long as you
Participants frequently spoke about the experience of haven’t accepted this is all over.
stroke in general, but expressed the enormous losses
associated with arm impairment, with many reduced Participants highlighted the reality of striving for
to tears during the discussion. recovery amidst adjusting to the stroke event and
stroke consequences, navigating rehabilitation
It would almost be easier if the arms came back. You could sit services while getting back on with their life
in a wheelchair, at least you could do something. When the again as the consequences of stroke endure.
leg comes back the only thing you learn to do is walk. But the Efforts were directed at not only ‘keeping the
number of things you can do with an arm ..
door open’ but also preventing it from closing
against the pressure of others, such as doctors
Their definitions of recovery were often intensely who may insist there is no hope, or from within
personal, conveying sentiments such as a sense of themselves, such as a sense of hopelessness.
hope, a familiar identity, valued activities and lifestyle While this sometimes required the stroke survi-
choices. When asked individually what recovery vor to be a ‘maverick’, at other times or in
meant to them, participant’s responses varied con- other cases, a gentler approach was needed
siderably as is evident in the following examples. whereby keeping an open mind and a flexible
arm allowed future recovery to remain a
possibility.
. . .some hope for recovery. . . a sign at least to say recovery Overall, the processes used by stroke survivors in
was possible . . .. . .
‘keeping the door open’ are described as ‘hanging in
there’, ‘drawing on support’, ‘getting going and
. . .this was the hand that worked all the time and it
doesn’t respond. I was a right-handed man. What I have to keeping going with exercise’ and ‘finding out how to
do is control that hand . . .. . . keep moving ahead’. As the four conceptual cate-
gories which emerged from the data, these processes
. . .become independent and that summarises it . . . . you are interrelated and frequently occur simultaneously
don’t want to be a burden (Figure 1).
1218 R.N. Barker & S.G. Brauer
Disabil Rehabil Downloaded from informahealthcare.com by University of Sydney
For personal use only.

Figure 1. Processes used by stroke survivors to maximise upper limb recovery.

go’. Depression commonly overcame them, allow-


Hanging in there
ing hope for recovery to fade. Negative or
The expression ‘hanging in there’ was used by dismissive attitudes from others created or con-
participants to accentuate the personal commitment founded the desire to ‘give up’. Alternatively, this
required to persist and not give up. The importance fired the motivation of others who used such an
of keeping hope alive and maintaining a strong attitude as a driving force to recover, just to prove
reason to recover was highlighted. them wrong.

. . .It’s what comes from inside. What you bring out of Because that doctor knocked me so far down with what she
yourself. said I fought her, not me.

. . .You have to take responsibility for yourself, you have


Maintaining hope and a sense of humour and being
got to work at it. Don’t ever give up.
surrounded by others who were positive and
encouraging helped to overcome obstacles. Many
The constant reminder of the losses and the advocated setting small goals to ensure small
frustration of repeated failure overwhelmed those successes could be celebrated along the way.
with severe paresis. Not knowing what to do or
how to get help eroded the will to continue. . . . you can’t look from nothing to everything you have to
Feeling scared of doing harm especially where pain take it step by step. As each piece comes back you practise
was present led to a loss of confidence to ‘have a until you have encouragement to try something else.
Stroke survivors’ perspective on recovery 1219

One stroke group routinely reported successes abandoned. Rather than accepting negative predic-
during their meetings so that individuals were tions, many strongly advised not to listen to health
acknowledged for their achievement, others were professionals because ‘they are not always right’.
encouraged and the whole group celebrated. Working hard was seen as a way of making it easier
Hanging in there through the highs and the lows, and worthwhile for therapists to be supportive. Being
often with support from others, provided the stability proactive in seeking support was advocated including
necessary for getting going and keeping going with ‘knocking on doors’ or seeking complementary
exercise and the impetus for finding out how to keep health medicines once traditional avenues were
moving ahead. exhausted.
Stroke survivors drew on the support of others so
that they were able to hang in there, gain help with
Drawing on support from others
getting going and keeping going with exercise and as
Participants believed they could not hang in there a source of information on how to keep moving
and achieve a good recovery of their arm without ahead.
support from others. This included their spouse,
family, friends and community, other stroke survi-
Getting going and keeping going with exercise
vors and health professionals who together provided
Disabil Rehabil Downloaded from informahealthcare.com by University of Sydney

physical, emotional and social support. Exercising was seen as the means to physical
recovery. Emphasis was placed on starting soon after
Support has been the biggest thing for me. I come to the stroke stroke onset, practicing regularly, intensively, appro-
group, I don’t have to try to explain what happened .. they priately and continually and using the arm in
have been there and done that. If I have a problem I can talk everyday tasks. To not exercise was to do nothing
to anyone of these carers and they know where I am coming
and without exercise recovery would not occur.
from and even if they can’t help me at least I know they
understand.
If you sit in a chair and do nothing you definitely won’t
For personal use only.

improve. There is no might about it you definitely won’t. At


A number of participants were unable to imagine least doing something you’ve got a sporting change of it
having an effect.
how recovery was possible for those who did not have
a spouse. In contrast, some who were on their own
felt they were forced to do more for themselves and However many participants did not exercise in the
fared better than those whose spouse was fearful and manner advocated. Although most persisted with
did too much or pushed too much. As with all some form of stretching, they felt they did not know
participants, they looked to the stroke group for how to exercise or how to progress, a fact they
support, appreciating the camaraderie, humour and attributed to insufficient attention to the arm during
information that was shared, as well as the benefits of rehabilitation. Many also believed they did not have
both giving and receiving support. However, indivi- enough movement to work with and therefore had
duals who would not help themselves could be seen nothing they could do to practise.
as a burden to the group.
Participants also looked to health professionals for . . ..I have got no use so I can’t improve what I’ve got if I
support, believing they had the knowledge, authority haven’t got anything.
and responsibility for promoting recovery of their
arm. The ‘therapy’, the training, guidance and the A question repeatedly posed was whether the arm
encouragement provided were frequently credited would have recovered as well as the leg, had it been
with the positive outcomes made. ‘worked on’ in the same manner as the leg. Many felt
that in the beginning they had no movement in either
. . ..The rehab is critical. If you haven’t got it to start with their leg or arm, but that extreme effort was used to
you don’t know where you are going. Those are the people get the leg going and to start walking. This was not
who sort of set you up and get you going. the case with the arm. Consistent with this reasoning,
the final interview participant believed her arm was
. . ..The courage the physiotherapist gave me is what I
better than her leg because it had been ‘worked on
needed. That he had faith in me.
from the start’ while she was immediately assigned to
a wheelchair on what proved to be a faulty assump-
Conversely, many resented the attitude of health tion that she would never walk again.
professionals and the power of their predictions. While a few participants found some therapists
Being told they would never use their arm again ‘useless’, all believed access to suitably skilled
shattered hopes and often limited or precluded them therapy services could help them to get going with
from therapy, leaving them feeling disadvantaged or exercise. However, keeping going with exercise was
1220 R.N. Barker & S.G. Brauer

also recognized as an issue that needed to be they needed it. Not being able to access services
addressed. Therapists teaching stroke survivors how beyond a prescribed period or over a certain quota
to exercise independently and how to progress was was a barrier. Those from a rural area felt the lack of
seen as a priority. Yet even with the best knowledge, services with any expertise in stroke recovery was a
maintaining the motivation to exercise over a long major disadvantage. Written material in the book-
period of time was highlighted as a problem. shop, library or via the internet was found to be
sparse and inadequate for their needs.
. . .Its difficult to remain positive and willing to do the Participants recommended upper limb training
exercises everyday. It really takes a lot of dedication and based on a self-help principle with spouses or carers
determination. . .. because other things come into play. involved at least until the stroke survivor could
absorb the information they needed. Traditional one
Participants agreed that attendance at therapy, one to to one therapy was valued, as were group-training
one or in classes, helped maintain the motivation to sessions particularly if held within their stroke group
keep going. While some incorporated practice into environment. Other resources such as stroke survivor
everyday tasks, it was recognized as a strategy not friendly pamphlets, books, videos and websites were
available to those with severe paresis. considered necessary to consolidate and broaden
their understanding. Rather than receiving all ser-
Disabil Rehabil Downloaded from informahealthcare.com by University of Sydney

. . .Once I got home all the exercises they had been showing vices in the earlier stages of recovery, participants
me were so damn boring I tried incorporating the exercises believed a more staggered arrangement would allow
into the daily things I did so that . . .they became extensions of them to access information and guidance when they
the exercises. So they were happening whether I wanted them
were ready and as it became relevant to them in their
to happen or not.
life. An annual review by a visiting team of stroke
experts was requested for the stroke survivor and
Participants were in no doubt that ‘getting going and carer from a rural area.
keeping going with exercise’ was the physical means By finding out how to keep moving ahead stroke
For personal use only.

to keeping the door open for recovery. Achieving this survivors were able to continue working for recovery.
was dependent on their ability to persist, on support This involved drawing on the support of others to
from others for encouragement, as well as therapy gain the necessary information and guidance that
and advice on how to get started and how to would help to build the confidence to hang in there
progress. and keep going with exercise. In this way, partici-
pants reinforced that together, these processes
enabled the stroke survivor to keep the door open
Finding out how to keep moving ahead
for recovery.
This involved pursuing information and guidance,
with emphasis on demonstration and feedback.
Discussion
Participants believed that professionals working
within rehabilitation services were the most qualified This study explores stroke survivors’ perspective on
to provide this. However in practice, other stroke upper limb recovery and the strategies believed to
survivors and the stroke support groups had played maximize recovery. It provides a retrospective view
this role. Participants who felt confident in moving of stroke survivors with 3 months to 13 years
ahead attributed it to their sense of responsibility and experience living with stroke. The findings portray
the teaching and encouragement of therapists at an optimistic open ended approach to upper limb
some point along the way. Equally important was the recovery that presumes the value of the stroke
knowledge that the ‘door was open’ for them to survivor’s contribution to their own recovery and
return to a rehabilitation service for feedback and explores the potential for more meaningful interac-
further guidance when they needed it. tion with stroke recovery services to enhance upper
limb recovery.
They give you feedback. And that’s what you need. You need The outcomes of this study offer new and valuable
that feedback. I myself I am striving to get better and better insight into upper limb recovery after stroke that has
all the time but you need their help. implications for stroke recovery services. However
methodological limitations need to be considered to
In contrast, most participants felt ill equipped to place the findings within proper perspective. Firstly,
continue moving ahead. It was common to have as all study participants volunteered to attend and
difficulty understanding and remembering informa- were members of stroke support groups, the findings
tion given in the early stages, to find little relevance may reflect the views of those for whom upper limb
in the information they were given and to experience recovery is a priority, the views that result from
frustration over failed attempts to acquire help when attendance at a stroke group or the views of the type
Stroke survivors’ perspective on recovery 1221

of person who chooses to attend a stroke group. identifying a recovery path [24] or trajectory [19]
Stroke survivors who can not or did not choose to that did not have an endpoint. While the findings of
attend a stroke support group or for whom upper this study are similar to this latter view, emphasis is
limb recovery is not a priority may have had different placed on recovery as part of life’s path, whereby
views. Not to be forgotten however, is that as recovery is incorporated into stroke survivors lives,
members of stroke support groups with experience rather than a distinct recovery path. As participants
of a variety of rehabilitation services across both were members of stroke groups with up to 13 years
metropolitan and rural areas, participants’ contribu- experience, this may represent a view of recovery that
tion will have been influenced by not only their own has evolved over time and become part of a stroke
recovery, but also the recovery of a large number of group ethos that is embraced by newcomers. It may
other stroke survivors with whom they have inter- also reflect the unique experience of upper limb
acted. In addition, a follow-up study has been recovery where priority is often only given after some
conducted with a larger and more representative adjustment to stroke and resumption of life has
sample of stroke survivors. occurred.
Secondly, the interpretation will have been influ- Clearly, the processes used to keep the door open
enced by the background and perspective of both the for recovery echo a self-management and self-
researchers and stroke survivors who volunteered to improvement approach, specifically tailored to pro-
Disabil Rehabil Downloaded from informahealthcare.com by University of Sydney

participate. The researchers had witnessed stroke gression. Based on the goals and aspirations of stroke
survivors who had made unexpected recoveries, survivors, attention is given to the psychological,
regardless of the services received, leading to the social, emotional, physical and educational require-
belief that personal factors have a role in recovery. ments to achieve these goals. While this perspective
Collaboration on this study came about because of a represents those who chose to attend a stroke
shared belief that stroke survivors had much to support group or self help group, it highlights the
contribute and that the potential for upper limb value of stroke support groups as a positive mechan-
recovery had not yet been exploited. The researchers ism for promoting recovery, through which existing
For personal use only.

believe that this led to a positive outlook within each services could work.
interview, rigorous questioning by the researchers What is most striking about the findings of this and
and open, honest and forthright opinions from previous studies [19,21,25] is the mismatch between
participants. stroke survivors’ view and their experience of stroke
As with previous studies, the stroke survivors in recovery services. Rather than a long-term view with
this sample defined recovery according to ‘what no time limit placed on recovery, the services they
matters to them’ and that frequently this was not received reflect an historical viewpoint that recovery
what mattered to health professionals [19 – 21]. occurs within a finite period. Emphasis on mobility
Similarly, the stressors and challenges involved in seemed to represent rehabilitation priorities devel-
pursuing recovery were mediated by the process of oped prior to the advent of motorized wheelchairs,
adjusting to life with stroke and the persistence of the mechanical hoists and disabled access that have
consequences of stroke into future life [22]. In this enabled greater inclusion in the community. Even
study, stroke survivors attempted to maximise upper though intensive and repetitive task related practice
limb recovery by ‘keeping the door open’ a process is known to promote upper limb recovery, incon-
that allowed them to continue in life hoping for and sistent attention was given to the upper limb during
working towards improvement while remaining open rehabilitation. The inequity in intensity of exercise
to future possibilities. This involved ‘hanging in for the upper limb compared with the lower limb
there’, ‘drawing on support of others’, ‘getting going appeared to leave stroke survivors with the sense that
and keeping going with exercise’ and ‘finding out their potential for upper limb recovery had not really
how to keep moving ahead’. It is important to note been explored. The difficulties participants experi-
that these practices existed on a continuum with enced in ‘finding out how to keep moving ahead’ is
some people engaging in them more consistently likely to also reflect the belief that recovery occurs
than others, an important consideration given the within a finite period as well as the inability of shorter
heterogeneous nature of stroke, stroke survivors and term services to address changing needs and goals as
their experience of upper limb recovery. recovery progresses.
This view of recovery differs from previous studies
which have looked at stroke recovery in general,
Clinical implications for upper limb training
within the first two years after stroke. Some studies
have described a recovery path with an endpoint This perspective of upper limb recovery provides
where personal goals were achieved [23] or con- some insight into the experience of upper limb
versely when nothing more could be done [21]. recovery and an appreciation of the processes used
Others reported a long term view of recovery, by stroke survivors to maximize recovery. In an effort
1222 R.N. Barker & S.G. Brauer

to support stroke survivors in their endeavour, the development and delivery of upper limb training
outcomes of this study highlight the need to match programmes.
services to the individual aspirations of stroke
survivors and to assist them to continue working
Acknowledgements
for recovery amidst adjusting to life with stroke.
Keeping the door open for recovery implies We would like to acknowledge support given to this
removing time limits placed on recovery by health project by the members and friends of the Lockyer
professionals and stroke rehabilitation services [26] Valley Stroke Support Group and the Acacia Ridge
as well as providing services to promote recovery Young Stroke Group who participated in the focus
from stroke onset until life ends, as previously group and in-depth interviews and provided feed-
suggested [19,23,27]. Accommodating the obvious back on the analysis as it proceeded. Not to be
budgetary limitations will no doubt demand forgotten is Miriam Trevis, the group scribe and
creative solutions such as an annual quota of services observer who provided her valuable skills during data
that can be accessed by stroke survivors when collection and transcription of tapes. We are grateful
difficulties are encountered or further direction for the financial support provided by the Toowoom-
required. A variety of services could address not ba Hospital Foundation with the award of the 2001
only the varying stages of recovery but also the needs Infront Outback Research Grant and to the War
Disabil Rehabil Downloaded from informahealthcare.com by University of Sydney

of individuals. In particular, the provision of an Widows Guild Queensland Inc., for the award of the
annual review by an expert stroke recovery team is Jessie Mary Vasey Scholarship. Thanks also go to Dr
indicated. Alun Williams for assistance in project planning, Dr
Ideally service delivery would benefit from a self- Samantha Bursnall, Dr Glenys Carlson and Scott
management and self improvement approach that Kenny for their assistance and advice during data
would prepare and guide stroke survivors through analysis and Dr Pim Kuipers, Liz Logan and Toby
the psychological, social, physical and educational Gill for helpful comments and editing in the final
demands for recovery. Staged self-management stages.
For personal use only.

training programmes and user friendly information


resources that interface with mainstream rehabilita-
tion services could reinforce and extend the lessons
learnt. References
In an effort to thoroughly explore the potential for 1. Broeks JG, Lankhorst GJ, Rumping K, Prevo AJH. The long-
upper limb recovery, therapy services need to term outcome of arm function after stroke: results of a
consistently implement upper limb training in a follow-up study. Disability and Rehabilitation 1999;21:
manner shown to promote recovery and of similar 357 – 364.
intensity as applied to the lower limb. In the face of 2. Kwakkel G, Kollen BJ, Wagenaar RC. Therapy impact on
functional recovery in stroke rehabilitation: A critical review
severe upper limb paresis and the difficulties of the literature. Physiotherapy 1999;13:457 – 470.
associated with compliance to exercise in the long 3. Goldie P, Matyas T, Kinsella G. Movement rehabilitation
term, new and innovative interventions and services following stroke. Department of Health Housing and Com-
may be required. munity Services: Victoria, 1992.
4. Feys HM, De Weerdt WJ, Selz BE, Steck GAC, Spichiger R,
Finally, the benefits of stroke support groups need
Vereeck LE, et al. Effect of a therapeutic intervention for the
to be explored and perhaps formally incorporated hemiplegic upper limb in the acute phase after stroke - A
into stroke recovery services. These findings suggest single-blind, randomized, controlled multicenter trial. Stroke
they offer a tangible resource for support of more 1998;29: 785 – 792.
recent stroke survivors as well as a discreet group that 5. Jorgensen HS, Nakayama H, Raaschou HO, Vivelarsen J,
existing services could work with to help maximize Stoier M, Olsen TS. Outcome and Time-Course of Recovery
in Stroke .2. Time-Course of Recovery - the Copenhagen
recovery in the long term. Stroke Study. Archives of physical medicine and rehabilita-
tion 1995;76: 406 – 412.
6. Dean C, Shepherd R. Task-related training improves
Conclusion performance of seated reaching tasks after stroke - A
This stroke survivors’ perspective on upper limb randomized controlled trial. Stroke 1997;28:722 – 728.
7. Taub E, Miller NE, Novack TA, Cook EW, Flemming WC,
recovery highlights a long-term view of recovery Nepomuceno CS, et al. Technique to improve chronic motor
described as ‘keeping the door open’. To achieve deficit after stroke. Archives of physical medicine and
this, stroke survivors are involved in ‘hanging in rehabilitation 1993;74:347 – 354.
there’ while ‘drawing on support from others’, 8. Australia’s Health 2000. AIHW Cat. No. 19 ed. The seventh
‘getting going and keeping going with exercise’ and biennial health report of the Australian Institute of Health
and Welfare. Canberra: Australian Institute of Health and
‘finding out how to keep moving ahead’. In order to Welfare, 2000.
support stroke survivors in this endeavour, the 9. Shelton F, Reding MJ. Effect of lesion location on upper
findings of this study can be used to guide the limb motor recovery after stroke. Stroke 2001;32:107 – 112.
Stroke survivors’ perspective on recovery 1223

10. McLean DE. Medical complications experiences by a cohort 19. Burton CR. Living with stroke: a phenomenological study.
of stroke survivors during inpatient, tertiary-level stroke Journal of Advanced Nursing 2000;32:301 – 309.
rehabilitation. Archives of physical medicine and rehabilita- 20. Lawler J, Dowswell G, Hearn J, Forster A, Young J.
tion 2004;85:466 – 469. Recovering from a stroke: a qualitative investigation of the
11. Butefisch C, Hummelsheim H, Denzler P, Mauritz KH. role of goal setting in late stroke recovery. Journal of Advanced
Repetitive training of isolated movements improves the Nursing 1999;30:401 – 409.
outcome of motor rehabilitation of the centrally paretic hand. 21. Dowswell G, Lawler J, Dowswell T, Young J, Forster A,
Journal of the Neurological Sciences 1995;130:59 – 68. Hearn J. Investigating recovery from stroke: a qualitative
12. White G. Consumer participation in disability research: the study. Journal of Clinical Nursing 2000;9:507 – 515.
golden rule as a guide for ethical practice. Rehabilitation 22. Burton CR. Rethinking stroke rehabilitation: the Corbin and
Psychology 2002;47:438 – 446. Strauss chronic illness trajectory framework. Journal of
13. Bendz M. Rules of relevance. Social Science and Medicine Advanced Nursing 2000;32:595 – 602.
2000;51:713 – 723. 23. Doolittle ND. The experience of recovery following lacunar
14. Hafsteinsdottir TB, Grypdonck M. Being a stroke patient: a stroke. Rehabilitation Nursing 1992;17:122 – 125.
review of the literature. Journal of Advanced Nursing 24. Folden SL. Managing the effects of stroke: the first months.
1997;26:580 – 588. Rehabilitation Nursing Research 1994;Fall:79 – 85.
15. Kwakkel G, Wagenaar RC, Twisk JWR, Lankhorst GJ, 25. O’Connell B, Hanna B, Penney W, Pearce J, Owen M,
Koetsier JC. Intensity of leg and arm training after primary Warelow P. Recovery after stroke: A qualitative perspective.
middle- cerebral-artery stroke: a randomised trial. Lancet Journal of Clinical Practice 2001;21:120 – 125.
1999;354:191 – 196. 26. Page SJ, Bach-y-Rita, P. Reconsidering the motor recovery
Disabil Rehabil Downloaded from informahealthcare.com by University of Sydney

16. Holman H, Lorig K. Patients as partners in managing chronic plateau in stroke rehabilitation. Archives of Physical Medicine
disease. British Medical Journal 2000;320:526 – 27. and Rehabilitation 2004;85:1377 – 1381.
17. Strauss A, Corbin J. Basics of qualitative research: techniques and 27. Sabari JS, Meisler J, Silver KHC. Reflections upon rehabilita-
procedures for developing grounded theory. 2nd ed. Thousand tion by members of a community based stroke club. Disability
Oaks: Sage Publications, 1998. and Rehabilitation 2000;22:330 – 336.
18. Miles MB, Huberman MA. Qualitative data analysis. 2nd ed.
Thousand Oaks, California: Sage Publications, 1994.
For personal use only.
Research Articles
Neurorehabilitation and

Translating Animal Doses of Task-Specific Neural Repair


24(7) 620–635
© The Author(s) 2010
Training to People With Chronic Stroke Reprints and permission: http://www.
sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.nav

in 1-Hour Therapy Sessions: DOI: 10.1177/1545968310361957


http://nnr.sagepub.com

A Proof-of-Concept Study

Rebecca L. Birkenmeier, MSOT1, Eliza M. Prager1,


and Catherine E. Lang, PhD1

Abstract
Objective. The purposes of this study were to (1) examine the feasibility of translating high-repetition doses of upper-
extremity (UE) task-specific training to people with stroke within the confines of the current outpatient delivery system
of 1-hour therapy sessions and (2) to gather preliminary data regarding the potential benefit of this intensity of training.
Methods. A total of 15 patients with chronic ( 6 months) UE paresis caused by stroke underwent 3 weeks of baseline
assessments followed by 6 weeks of the high-repetition intervention (3 sessions/wk for 6 weeks). During each 1-hour session,
participants were challenged to complete 300 or more repetitions of UE functional task training (3 tasks 100 repetitions).
Assessments during and after the intervention were used to measure feasibility and potential benefit. Results. For the
13 participants completing the intervention, the average number of repetitions per session was 322. The percentage of
sessions attended was 97%. Participant ratings of pain and fatigue were low. Action Research Arm test scores improved an
average of 8 points during the intervention and were maintained at the 1-month follow-up. Secondary measures of activity
and participation increased, but the measure of impairment did not. Conclusions. It is feasible to deliver hundreds of repeti-
tions of task-specific training to people with stroke in 1-hour therapy sessions. Preliminary outcome data suggest that this
intervention may be beneficial for some people with stroke.

Keywords
stroke, UE, task-specific training, function, translational research

Introduction that hundreds of repetitions of task-specific practice may be


required to optimize function poststroke.
Converging evidence suggests that task-specific practice may The reality of stroke rehabilitation is that there is limited
be the best way to promote functional recovery after stroke.1,2 task-specific practice taking place.12,13 In a small pilot study
Repeated practice of challenging movement tasks results in and a larger, multisite study, we observed that most UE prac-
larger brain representations of the practiced movement.3,4 tice during rehabilitation was devoted to basic exercises,
These findings and others from the neuroscience literature5,6 including both active and passive movements. Substantially
indicate that extended, task-specific practice is critical for less practice was devoted to movements in functional, task-
producing lasting changes in motor system networks, motor specific contexts. Task-specific UE training occurred in only
learning, and motor function. Paradigms designed to investi- 51% of the therapy sessions that involved UE rehabilitation,
gate neural adaptations in animal models often require “sub- and the average number of repetitions of task-specific training
jects” to engage in hundreds of daily repetitions of functionally was 32.13 Thus, the current dose of task-specific practice
important upper-extremity (UE) task practice.3,7,8 Studies
designed to investigate motor learning in humans also involve
large amounts of practice, although usually for fewer sessions. 1
Washington University, St. Louis, MO, USA
The numbers vary, but different studies have used ranges
Corresponding Author:
between 300 and 800 repetitions per session.9-11 Although the Catherine Lang, Program in Physical Therapy, Washington University,
definitive number of repetitions needed for optimal human 4444 Forest Park, Campus Box 8502, Saint Louis, MO 63108, USA
learning is unknown, these paradigms collectively suggest Email: langc@wustl.edu

Downloaded from nnr.sagepub.com at SSRT-Tech Serv on November 4, 2011


Birkenmeier et al 621

provided during stroke rehabilitation is an order of magnitude


lower than what is currently provided in animal models of
stroke and in human motor learning studies.
This proof-of-concept study was an attempt to translate
the high-repetition doses of task-specific training from animal
models to people with stroke. Constraint-induced movement
therapy (CIMT) was one of the first successful translations
Figure 1. Schematic of experimental design illustrating
of a rehabilitation intervention, from deafferented, nonhuman the time course of assessments and treatment; abbreviated
primates to people with stroke. CIMT has been found to be weekly assessments included the Action Research Arm Test
beneficial for those with mild to moderate motor deficits and grip strength
later14-17 but not more immediately after stroke.18,19 Three
components of CIMT include the following: constraint of the
unaffected upper limb, a behavioral agreement (transfer pack- Participants
age), and high doses of movement training.20,21 The high
doses of movement training, counted in minutes and hours, People with chronic UE paresis poststroke were recruited from
not repetitions, are provided in 3- to 6-hour therapy sessions. the St Louis metropolitan area via the Cognitive Rehabilitation
Because CIMT has 3 components, the specific benefit of the Research Group Stroke Registry, from local outpatient stroke
movement training is unclear. Additionally, the requirement rehabilitation clinics, and from the community. Inclusion
of multihour therapy sessions makes it challenging to imple- criteria for participation in the study were as follows: (1) clini-
ment these high doses in routine clinical practice.22 cal diagnosis of stroke, meeting ICD-9 criteria; (2) time since
The purposes of this study were to (1) examine the feasibil- stroke 6 months; (3) sufficient cognitive ability to participate,
ity of translating high-repetition doses of UE task-specific as indicated by scores of 0 to 1 on the Questions and Com-
training to people with stroke within the confines of the current mands items of the National Institutes of Health Stroke Scale
outpatient delivery system of 1-hour therapy sessions and (NIHSS); and (4) unilateral UE paresis, as indicated by a score
(2) to gather preliminary data regarding the potential benefit of 1 to 3 on the NIHSS Arm item. Exclusion criteria for par-
of these doses. We started this line of investigation in people ticipation in the study were as follows: (1) severe hemineglect,
with chronic stroke (operationally defined as 6 months) as indicated by a score of 2 on the NIHSS Extinction and
because their motor deficits and UE function are relatively Inattention item; (2) inability to follow 2-step commands;
stable.23-25 We hypothesized that people could repeatedly (3) history or current diagnoses of any other neurological or
achieve 300 repetitions per session without inducing pain psychiatric conditions; (4) concurrent participation in other
or significant fatigue and that performing these high-repetition UE stroke treatments (eg, Botox); or (5) nonavailability of the
doses would improve UE function. We chose a target of 300 participant for assessment or treatment sessions.
or more repetitions because 300 is the lower end of the num- The number of participants enrolled was chosen based on a
ber of repetitions achieved in the animal and human studies reasonable number to make an assessment of feasibility and an
cited above. Furthermore, we believed that this was likely the a priori power estimate using a repeated-measures analysis of
upper limit of what could be achieved in 1 hour and that it variance (ANOVA). Based on the parameters entered, estimated
would still make the practiced movements sufficiently chal- effect sizes ranged from 0.63 to 2.0, with potential participant
lenging to an individual’s motor capacity. numbers ranging from 8 to 44. During the 1-year period of the
study, 27 people were screened, and 15 were enrolled to test
feasibility and to obtain preliminary estimates of the effect size
Methods of the intervention.
This proof-of-concept study was a within-participant,
repeated-measures design (Figure 1). Patients participated in
3 baseline assessments, 1 week apart prior to starting the Measures
intervention. The intervention was 1 h/d, 3 d/wk for 6 weeks General characteristics (Table 1) of all participants were col-
(18 total sessions). During the intervention, brief weekly lected for descriptive purposes. Feasibility measures were
assessments were used to gather data on the time course of assessed at each treatment session by the therapist providing
changes. Postintervention assessments occurred at the end of treatment. Measures to assess preliminary benefits were
the intervention and 1 month later. The study was approved assessed at baseline, during the intervention, and after the
by the Washington University Human Research Protection intervention (Figure 1) by the treating therapist or by another
Office, and all participants signed an informed consent docu- trained assessor. Because all participants received the interven-
ment prior to participating. tion, none of the assessors was blinded with respect to the

Downloaded from nnr.sagepub.com at SSRT-Tech Serv on November 4, 2011


622 Neurorehabilitation and Neural Repair 24(7)

Table 1. Participant Characteristics

Time Dominant Other Stroke-


Age Poststroke Side Side Baseline Related
Participant (years) Gender (months) Affected Affected ARAT Scorea Spasticityb Deficitsc

R001 47 F 36 L N 3 4 Somatosensation
R002 50 M 19 L N 12 2
R003 75 M 12 L N 8 2
R004 55 M 60 R Y 3 4
R005 44 F 6 R Y 38 1
R006 44 F 36 R Y 4 2 Aphasia, Somatosensation
R007 55 F 120 L Y 20 3
R008 28 F 48 R Y 43 0 Aphasia, Emotionally labile
R009 57 M 18 L N 9 3
R010 50 F 48 R Y 40 0 Aphasia
R011 65 F 36 L N 27 2 Ataxia
R012 56 M 57 R Y 20 4
R013 57 F 36 L N 15 1
R014 90 M 48 L N 22 4 Somatosensation
R015 33 M 22 R Y 20 1 Aphasia
Mean/% 54 53% F 40 47% R 53% Y 19 2.2
Abbreviation: ARAT, Action Research Arm Test.
a
Maximum (normal) score 57; value is mean of the 3 baseline scores.
b
Spasticity assessed with the Modified Ashworth Scale52 at the elbow; normal 0, with higher scores indicating greater spasticity.
c
Additional motor and nonmotor deficits as documented from the National Institutes of Health Stroke Scale, clinical examination, and/or medical
records.

intervention. All assessments were videotaped, and videotapes Ratings of fatigue were used to assess tolerability and side
were reviewed periodically by all assessors together to check effects of the intervention. The Stanford Fatigue Visual
agreement on grading criteria. Numeric Scale (http://patienteducation.stanford.edu/research/
Measures of feasibility. The number of repetitions of task- vnsfatigue.html) was used to determine the presence and
specific training was recorded for each treatment session and severity of mental and physical fatigue at the beginning and
was the primary measure of feasibility. For any given task, a end of each treatment session. This scale was chosen because
single repetition was operationally defined as reaching to, it was the only one that considered mental and physical
grasping, moving or manipulating, and releasing an object. fatigue, both of which we thought might be important in this
The percentage of sessions attended was used as a measure high-dose intervention. Although reliability and validity of
of compliance with the intervention. The number of treatment the Stanford scale have not been published, the psychometric
sessions attended was divided by the number of possible treat- properties of general numeric rating scales are well estab-
ment sessions (18) and expressed as a percentage. The duration lished.27,28 Fatigue ratings presented in the Results section are
of each treatment session was tracked to compare the dose of the change scores for each treatment session—that is, the rat-
treatment provided here with previous and ongoing UE stroke ing at the end of the session minus the rating at the beginning
rehabilitation studies that report dose in minutes. Rest breaks of the session.
longer than 1 minute and breaks between tasks were not Measures of preliminary benefit. The Action Research Arm
counted as part of the duration. Although each session was Test (ARAT) was the primary measure used to assess benefit
scheduled for 60 minutes, the duration was recorded as the of the intervention.29 The ARAT was chosen as the primary
number of minutes that the participant was performing task- measure because it (1) has a low testing burden, (2) has strong
specific practice. psychometric properties in people with stroke,25,30,31 and (3)
Ratings of pain in the UE were used to assess side effects of is widely used in UE rehabilitation trials around the world.
the intervention. The Wong-Baker FACES Pain Rating Scale26 Grip strength, measured on a Jamar hydraulic hand-held
was used to determine the presence and severity of pain at the dynamometer,32 was used as a secondary measure to capture
beginning and end of each treatment session. Participant ratings changes in UE impairment.33,34 Three measurements were taken
were an overall measure of UE pain and no pain at individual at each assessment, and the average of the 3 measurements,
joints. Pain ratings presented in the Results section are the change reported in kilograms, was used to represent grip strength.
scores for each treatment session—that is, the rating at the end Two subscales of the Stroke Impact Scale (SIS) were used
of the session minus the rating at the beginning of the session. to capture self-perceived UE function in everyday life, outside

Downloaded from nnr.sagepub.com at SSRT-Tech Serv on November 4, 2011


Birkenmeier et al 623

of the clinic or laboratory.35-37 The SIS-Hand and SIS-ADL across people with stroke. Additionally, being given a choice
(Activities of Daily Living) subscales were used. The 2 SIS of tasks may enhance motivation and participation in rehabili-
subscales were administered by face-to-face interview at tation.41,42 The general target number of repetitions of task-
baseline and postintervention assessments. specific training was 300 per session, from practice of 3
The Canadian Occupational Performance Measure (COPM) specific tasks each session. Three tasks per session ( 100
was used for 2 purposes: as a goal setting tool to aid in choos- repetitions per task) were selected to allow for variability in
ing appropriate tasks to practice during the intervention and task practice and to avoid the boredom that might come from
as an individualized measure of potential treatment benefit. performing 300 repetitions of a single task.
The COPM is designed to detect changes in a patient’s self- Using information from the baseline assessments, selected
perception of their own occupational performance over time.38 tasks were graded to match the motor capabilities of the par-
It is a structured interview that assesses patient-specific areas ticipants. The job of the therapist was to grade tasks such that
of concern and progress in 3 domains: self-care, productivity, they challenged, but not overwhelmed or underwhelmed, the
and leisure. The interview results in a list of activities the motor capabilities of each participant. In other words, we did
individual wants, needs, or is expected to perform.38,39 The not want participants simply repeating tasks that they were
COPM’s 10-point rating scale is then used to indicate the already skilled at performing nor did we want them to repeat-
importance, performance, and satisfaction of these everyday edly fail at a task. Guiding principles for delivering the inter-
activities. At the postintervention assessments, participants vention were derived from the animal rehabilitation paradigms
were asked to repeat the rating of performance and satisfac- and motor learning literature and are provided in detail in the
tion. On both scales, higher scores indicate better perfor- Appendix. An example of a frequently used task was “lifting
mance and greater satisfaction with performance on specific, cans on shelves.” This task simulated the real-world activity
individualized activities. of storing and retrieving objects on shelves, such as putting
away groceries. This task could be graded by (1) changing the
size, weight, or shape of the cans; (2) changing the height of
Treatment Protocol the shelf; (3) changing the location of the shelves with respect
The intervention was delivered in 1-hour treatment sessions to the participant; and/or (4) changing the depth of the cans
by an occupational or physical therapist or by an occupational on the shelves. Algorithms were developed to determine when
therapy student supervised by one of the licensed therapists. to progress a task, that is, grade up or grade down, and when
All therapists were trained and monitored to ensure fidelity to to switch tasks. During each session, the treating therapist
the protocol. Task selection, task grading, and task progression documented the tasks and specific grading levels, the number
were discussed and documented for each participant by the of repetitions, and participant ratings of pain and fatigue. More
treating therapists. The principal investigator reviewed the information and examples regarding task selection, grading,
selected tasks, the grading of each task, and the decisions to and progression are provided in the Appendix.
change tasks or progress to a more challenging version of the
same task on a weekly basis.
The high-repetition intervention consisted of supervised, Statistical Analyses
massed practice of functional daily tasks, which were appro- Statistica version 6.1 software (StatSoft Inc, Tulsa, OK) was
priately graded and progressed for each participant. Most used for all analyses, and the criterion for statistical signifi-
functional UE tasks require 4 essential movement compo- cance was set at P .05. Data were found to be normally
nents: reaching for, grasping, moving/manipulating, and then distributed. Means, standard deviations (SDs), and 95% con-
releasing an object. What varies across the repertoire of daily fidence intervals (CIs) were generated for the feasibility data.
UE tasks is how the combinations of the components are For the number of repetitions, calculated averages excluded
strung together and the specifics of the component (eg, direc- the first week of treatment (first 3 sessions) because we
tion of reach, type of grasp, manipulative forces required). We expected that participants would require a few sessions to build
provided progressive training of these essential components up to the 300 repetition target. Repeated-measures ANOVAs
through repeated practice of various tasks, with the desired were used on the repetitions, pain, and fatigue data to evaluate
goal of building the participant’s capacity to perform a mul- if they increased over the course of the intervention. Our
titude of UE functions. a priori criteria for determining if the intervention was feasible
Participants were given the COPM to assist in determining were that the average number of repetitions per session would
activities of interest and specific tasks to address during treat- be 300 and that average percentage of sessions attended would
ment sessions. An individualized approach to task selection40 be 85%. We anticipated that the major consequence of
and not a general one (all participants do the same tasks) was increased pain or fatigue ratings would be a reduction in the
selected because severity of paresis and personal interests vary number of repetitions completed or sessions attended.

Downloaded from nnr.sagepub.com at SSRT-Tech Serv on November 4, 2011


624 Neurorehabilitation and Neural Repair 24(7)

Figure 2. Number of repetitions achieved at each session for the group (A) and for 2 individual participants (B-D); group values are
means and standard errors

Within-participant, repeated-measures ANOVAs were used Feasibility Data


to determine if the intervention provided benefit to participants.
Planned contrasts were as follows: (1) comparison of baseline Two participants dropped out: R001 and R004. R001 com-
assessments to evaluate the stability of the initial motor and pleted 4 treatment sessions prior to withdrawing for personal
functional deficits, (2) comparison of the baseline assessments reasons. R004 completed 8 treatment sessions before with-
and the first postintervention assessment to evaluate benefit, drawing. He achieved 300 or more repetitions by the fourth
and (3) comparison of the first and second postintervention treatment session but then reported painful leg cramps; over-
assessments to evaluate any persisting benefit. Because base- flow contractions in his affected lower extremity were noted
line scores were not significantly different, baseline scores for during UE task-specific practice. The increased activation of
each variable were averaged and used in further statistical lower-extremity muscles could have contributed to the leg
analyses. Comparisons with only 2 time points were done with cramps, so he was withdrawn. Data are presented from the
paired t tests. Spearman and Pearson correlation coefficients 13 participants who completed the intervention.
were used to explore relationships between the feasibility and After the first 3 sessions, the average number of repetitions
outcome data. per session was 322 (95% CI 285-358). Figure 2 shows
group and representative, individual data for the number of
repetitions. As a group (Figure 2A), the number of repetitions
Results increased over the course of the intervention (main effect of
Fifteen participants were enrolled, and 13 completed the study. time, F17,136 7.72, P .0001). Figure 2B shows an example
Table 1 provides descriptive information about the sample. of someone who achieved nearly 300 repetitions right away and
The sample was heterogeneous with respect to age, time since then fluctuated around 400 repetitions per session. Figure 2C
stroke, initial UE functional limitations (as measured by the is an example of someone who took much longer to achieve
ARAT), and the presence of deficits in other domains. the target number of repetitions. The total repetitions during

Downloaded from nnr.sagepub.com at SSRT-Tech Serv on November 4, 2011


Birkenmeier et al 625

F17,102 1.26, P .23; fatigue, F17,102 0.80, P .69). No


participant reported missing sessions because of pain or fatigue.

Preliminary Outcomes
ARAT data (Table 2) showed a significant repeated-measures
effect overall (F4,48 14.19; P .0001). Initial deficits were
stable, as indicated by no differences between the baseline
ARAT scores (F2,24 0.26; P .77). ARAT scores were higher
after the intervention (t12 3.66; P .003). The average change
score from baseline to the first postintervention assessment
was 8 points (95% CI 4-12 points). No differences were
found between the first and second postintervention assess-
ments (t12 0.23; P .82), indicating that the benefit of the
intervention was largely retained 1 month later. The secondary
measures showed similar results (Table 2), except for grip
strength, which did not change after the intervention.
Figure 4 shows the time course of ARAT changes during
the intervention. On average, ARAT scores increased steadily
during the 6-week intervention (Figure 4A). Exploratory
analyses indicated that improvement during the intervention
may be moderately related to both the initial deficit (baseline
ARAT vs change in ARAT: 0.64; P .02) and the doses of
training received (change in ARAT vs total repetitions: r 0.46;
P .10). The relationship between improvement and the dose
of training received is illustrated in Figure 4B. The dose of
training received was not related to the initial deficit (total
repetitions vs initial ARAT: r 0.14; P .65).

Discussion
Those with chronic UE paresis were able to achieve 300
Figure 3. Participant ratings of change in pain (A) and fatigue
repetitions of task-specific UE training without inducing pain
(B), calculated from numeric scale ratings reported before
and after each treatment session; values are means standard or substantial fatigue. Participating in this intervention resulted
deviations; negative values indicate that pain and fatigue in improved UE function as measured by the ARAT. Improve-
diminished at the end of a treatment session compared with the ments were also seen on self-report measures of UE activity
beginning of that treatment session and participation but not on the measure of UE impairment
(grip strength).

the intervention ranged from 3849 to 7568 with a mean of


5476 1088 (SD). The average duration of treatment was 47 Feasibility of the Intervention
3 minutes out of the scheduled 60-minute session (78% of Our main finding was that the high-repetition intervention was
the scheduled time). feasible in 1-hour therapy sessions. Numbers of repetitions
The percentage of sessions attended was 97% (95% CI attained and the time course of achieving them varied across
94-100). Ratings of pain (Figure 3A) were low, with an average participants and within sessions. A common observation was
of 0.3 (95% CI 0.2 to 0.9) out of 10. Ratings of fatigue that more repetitions were achieved earlier in a session. The
(Figure 3B) were somewhat higher and more variable across moderate increase in fatigue at the end of each session was
participants and sessions, with an average of 1.9 (95% CI appropriate, indicating that participation required both mental
0.9-2.8) out of 10. We recorded numerous instances where and physical effort. Our study builds on earlier literature (see
participant ratings of pain and fatigue decreased from the Introduction section) to show that it is feasible to achieve high-
beginning to the end of the session as represented by negative repetition doses of task-specific training in 1-hour sessions. In
change scores. Neither pain nor fatigue ratings increased hindsight, we wondered if we could have asked for even higher
over the course of the intervention (main effects of time: pain, numbers of repetitions in each session. Achieving even higher

Downloaded from nnr.sagepub.com at SSRT-Tech Serv on November 4, 2011


626 Neurorehabilitation and Neural Repair 24(7)

Table 2. Outcome Data (n 13)a

Measureb Baseline 1 Baseline 2 Baseline 3 Post 1 Post 2

ARAT 21.1 3.6 21.4 3.5 21.7 3.3 29.2 4.8c 29.5 4.7c
Grip strength (kg) 14.0 1.9 12.7 1.1 12.0 1.2 14.5 1.5 15.4 1.8
SIS, Hand subscale 45 8 40 9 — 48 8d 49 8d
SIS, ADL subscale 70 6 67 5 — 78 4c 78 3c
COPM, performance 3.3 0.5 — — 5.5 0.6c 5.4 0.6c
COPM, satisfaction 2.8 0.4 — — 5.3 0.7c 5.4 0.8c
Abbreviations: ARAT, Action Research Arm test; SIS, Stroke Impact Scale; ADL, Activities of Daily Living; COPM, Canadian Occupational
Performance Measure.
a
Values are means standard error. For measures taken multiple times during the baseline, no significant differences between baseline assessments
were found. No significant differences were found between the first and second post-intervention assessments.
b
ARAT, 0- to 57-point scale; SIS, 0- to 100-point scales; COPM, 0- to 10-point scales. Higher numbers indicate better results on all scales.
c
P .05 for difference between baseline and postintervention assessments.
d
P .08 for difference between baseline and postintervention assessments.

above 350 repetitions in 1 hour would have required us to make


the practiced tasks easier. This would mean that we would be
more likely to have participants repeat tasks they could already
do quickly instead of challenging their motor capacity. It is
unknown how the number of repetitions performed here com-
pares to the number of repetitions achieved with the modified
form of CIMT43-46 because dose is reported only in time sched-
uled for therapy. Our feasibility results indicate that it is pos-
sible for rehabilitation clinicians to move from providing
minimal amounts of task-specific training12,13 to hundreds of
repetitions within the current outpatient service delivery model.
Our sample contained individuals with deficits from stroke
other than paresis (Table 1): 3 had diminished somatosensa-
tion, 4 had aphasia, and 1 had ataxia in addition to their UE
paresis. The sample was therefore consistent with the clinical
presentation of stroke, where the majority of patients experi-
ence deficits in multiple domains. The manner in which the
intervention was delivered was modified to fit the needs of
people with the other deficits. For example, for those with
aphasia, conversation was limited during treatment sessions
so as to not distract the participant. Paper and a black marker
were provided so that the participant could write down any
statements he or she was having difficulty verbalizing. The
finding that this intervention was successfully applied to
individuals with deficits beyond paresis enhances its clinical
utility.

Preliminary Benefit of the High-Repetition


Dose Intervention
Figure 4. A. Time course of changes in the Action Research
Arm Test (ARAT) for the group; values are means and standard Those participating in this intervention improved their UE
errors. B. Scatterplot of total number of repetitions of task- function as measured by clinical tests of UE activity and
specific training versus change in ARAT score participation. The average 8-point change seen here on the
ARAT was larger than a 4-point minimal detectable change
for this measure47 and larger than its 6-point minimal clini-
numbers in 1-hour sessions is unlikely for 2 reasons. First, cally important change identified in people with chronic
participants were fatigued at the end of the session. Many stroke17 but smaller than its 12- to 17-point change estimate
provided feedback that they could not continue to work this in people with acute stroke.48 The magnitude of improvement
hard for another hour. And second, achieving targets much seen here is similar to the magnitude of improvement in the

Downloaded from nnr.sagepub.com at SSRT-Tech Serv on November 4, 2011


Birkenmeier et al 627

experimental groups of other enhanced, task-specific, or motor deficits, with lesser impaired participants performing
CIMT studies in people with chronic stroke.14,15,17,45,49 As better, and the dose of training provided. Further studies are
with other studies, some participants did not respond to the needed to examine optimal dosing for people with stroke.
intervention, and this may be partially related to the severity
of their initial motor deficits. Note that the 2 participants who
dropped out had the most severe initial deficits (Table 1). Appendix
From these preliminary data, we can only surmise that this Treatment Protocol for High-Repetition Task-Specific
high-repetition intervention is likely no worse than other Intervention
interventions tested.
We propose that the main utility of this intervention is as The high-repetition intervention consisted of supervised,
a vehicle for investigating the dose–response relationship massed practice of functional daily tasks, which were appro-
between movement practice and functional improvements. priately graded and progressed for each participant. Most
The dose–response relationship is a critical issue facing functional UE tasks require 4 essential movement compo-
neurorehabilitation clinicians and researchers.50-52 Under- nents: reaching for, grasping, moving/manipulating, and then
standing the dose–response relationship is important as new releasing an object. What vary across the repertoire of daily
treatments are emerging, particularly for treatments where a UE tasks are how the combinations of the components are
novel agent (eg, drug, robot, cortical stimulation, transcranial strung together and the specifics of the component (eg, direc-
magnetic stimulation) is paired with physical practice. The tion of reach, type of grasp, manipulative forces required).
number of repetitions is a useful way to quantify dose, either We provided progressive training of these essential compo-
session-by-session or in total, as is done in animal models of nents through repeated practice of various tasks, with the
stroke. The correlation analysis suggests that more repetitions desired goal of building the participant’s capacity to perform
may result in better outcomes. The time profiles of ARAT a multitude of UE functions.
data indicate that patients were improving over the course of Guiding Principles for Providing Treatment. Principles for how
the intervention. Because the duration was set at 6 weeks for the treatment was implemented were developed prior to the
practical reasons, we do not know if continuing the interven- study and are summarized in Table A1. Information on motor
tion, thereby achieving larger total doses, would have resulted learning principles and animal model paradigms are primarily
in greater functional improvements. Further studies are needed derived from recent reviews and chapters.
to map the dose–response relationship poststroke and the Matching Participant Goals With Specific Tasks. Participants
potential modifiers of this relationship. were given the COPM to assist in determining activities of
Several limitations should be considered when interpreting interest and specific tasks to address during treatment sessions.
our data. First, we had a small sample, which limits the gen- They were encouraged to select activities/tasks that included
eralizability of the findings. Second, the sample was hetero- a primary UE component. For instance, an individual who
geneous. Average data could have masked important findings chose ballroom dancing as a goal was encouraged to identify
in specific subgroups, if there had been a sufficient number another goal because ballroom dancing is primarily a lower-
of participants. Third, assessments were not blinded, which extremity activity. Using the goals selected by the participant
likely introduced bias into our measurement, particularly for on the COPM in addition to the subscale scores on the ARAT,
the assessments of benefit. Because of the pilot nature of the the study therapist was able to choose related treatment activi-
project and amount of available funding, we were unable to ties even if the participant was unable to perform the whole
use a blinded rater. Fourth, we did not have a control group. task selected as a goal. Every attempt was made to perform
Although the baseline data indicate that the motor function the whole activity as a treatment activity. In cases where this
was stable prior to the intervention, the improvements may was impossible, a component of the whole task was chosen.
have been a result of participating in any therapy versus par- Examples of goals identified by participants and the tasks
ticipating in the specific intervention tested here. chosen to address those goals are provided in Table A2.
Grading Tasks to Challenge Motor Capabilities. Using informa-
tion from the baseline assessments, selected tasks were graded
Conclusions to match the motor capabilities of the participant. The job of
This is the first attempt to translate the high-repetition doses the therapist was to grade tasks such that they challenged, but
from animal models to people with stroke in 1-hour therapy not overwhelmed or underwhelmed, the motor capabilities of
sessions. Our data indicate that it is possible to achieve 300 each participant. In other words, we did not want participants
repetitions of task-specific UE training without inducing pain simply repeating tasks that they were already skilled at per-
or substantial fatigue. Preliminary outcome data suggest that forming nor did we want them to repeatedly fail at a task.
this intervention may be beneficial for some people with stroke.
The benefit of the intervention may be a function of initial (continued)

Downloaded from nnr.sagepub.com at SSRT-Tech Serv on November 4, 2011


628 Neurorehabilitation and Neural Repair 24(7)

Appendix (continued)
Table A1. Guiding Principles for the High-Repetition Intervention

Insights and Implementation in Animal


Principle Models and Motor Learning Studies Implementation in Our Protocol

Practice of a movement results in Animals practice purposeful movements: Each task incorporates the essential
improvement in that movement reach–grasp–retrieve components of reach, grasp, move/manipulate,
and release
Large amounts of practice are Animals perform hundreds of repetitions The target number of repetitions is 300 per
required to truly master a motor daily for up to 3 months session
skill To date, animal studies have not Setting the target as instead of allows us
determined an optimal number of daily flexibility to see how much participants can be
repetitions challenged.
Brain reorganization continues for a short Duration of the intervention (for future studies)
while after behavior plateaus should extend 1 to 2 weeks beyond the
anticipated behavioral plateau
Learning requires solving the motor Brain reorganization occurs with learning Tasks have grades of increasing difficulty. Rules
problem and not rote repetition of and not simply repetition for progressing to more difficult grades
overlearned tasks are designed to continually challenge the
participant’s motor capabilities
Learning does not occur in the Animals have clear intrinsic feedback on Tasks have clear goals so participants can easily
absence of feedback each trial about knowledge of results (ie, determine knowledge of results
eat the food pellet vs not eat)
Intrinsic feedback is optimal for Participants are given summary feedback on
promoting self-learning and knowledge of results (number of repetitions
generalization achieved) at the end of each task
Optimal learning occurs with Animals are food deprived, and the task is Participants help select tasks for practice to
high levels of motivation and to retrieve food, creating very high levels increase engagement and motivation
engagement of motivation and engagement Tasks can be changed each week to minimize
boredom
Participants practice 3 tasks each session to
minimize boredom
Variable practice conditions Animals practice a single task under limited Essential movement components stay the same,
are optimal for learning and variable conditions (eg, changing well but contexts of the components change
generalization sizes, well locations) Variation is accomplished across tasks (eg,
practice of 3 tasks, change 1 task weekly) and
within tasks (eg, vary objects, location, weight,
speed, accuracy, etc)
Within-session, massed practice Animals continually perform their The environment is set up to allow continuous
promotes learning better than movement task throughout the session practice
within-session, distributed practice Participants are given encouragement by the
therapist to continue practicing
Rest breaks are only provided at the request of
the participants
Random practice of several tasks Animals only practice 1 task, so this is not In the current protocol, task practice is done
results in better learning than an issue in blocks for simplicity. Future studies are
blocked practice of the same tasks needed to address this issue
in healthy adults
This principle is often tested as
randomization of small blocks of
trials of up to 3 tasks
Practice of a whole task results in Animals practice the whole task of Basic underlying movement components of
better learning than practice of retrieving and eating a pellet reach, grasp, move/manipulate, and release
parts of the task, unless the task represent a whole sequence of movements as
can be broken down into clearly performed in the real world
separable components

(continued)

Downloaded from nnr.sagepub.com at SSRT-Tech Serv on November 4, 2011


Birkenmeier et al 629

Appendix (continued)
Table A2. Examples of Goals and the Tasks Chosen to Address Them

Goal Tasks Chosen to Address the Goal Rationale for Chosen Task

Handwriting Practice writing; incrementally write Handwriting is necessary for signing documents and work duties
faster to improve quality and accuracy
Typing on a Practice typing Typing is often necessary for work, including communication
keyboard (eg, E-mail)
Fishing Picking up fishing lures; sorting a tackle Fishing is an important leisure activity for many individuals.
box Manipulating fishing-related equipment addresses finger dexterity
Playing games Playing Connect Four; dealing cards Playing social games is an important leisure activity for many
individuals. Different games that are of interest to the participant
can be selected to address motor impairments of the proximal
and/or distal upper extremity
Folding towels Folding towels or washcloths Folding is a bilateral activity and an integral part of many household
tasks. Grasping and releasing clothing is important for dressing as well
Stacking and Lift/remove cans onto shelves This task mimics many common daily movements such as unloading
removing cans on a grocery bag and getting out craft/hobby supplies. This task can
shelves be graded to address proximal and distal motor impairments
Sorting silverware Pick up silverware and sort into Silverware is used at most meals. This activity addresses grasp and
container release of objects
Managing and Pick up a variety of coins and place in a Manipulation of money is an everyday task and addresses finger
manipulating coins metal piggy bank with slotted top dexterity
Scrapbooking Cutting paper with scissors Scrapbooking is a growing leisure activity, involving cutting, pasting/
gluing, and manipulating different sized papers and photographs.
The variety of movements required can be structured to address
proximal and/or distal motor impairments

Following the selection of tasks, the participant attempts 3. Changing the weight of task materials
to perform each activity. For example, 1 participant had a. Heavy objects
decreased shoulder range of motion but was able to use her b. Light-weight objects
hand to pick up small objects with decreased coordination. 4. Changing the size of objects
Because she enjoyed playing games with her grandchildren, a. Use large items.
the task of playing Connect Four was selected. During the first b. Use small items (eg, small buttons vs large
attempts at this task, the participant was seated with the Con- buttons).
nect Four grid placed directly in front of her, on dycem (to 5. Using adaptive equipment/materials
prevent slipping). As she continued to improve, the grid posi- a. Use dycem to prevent an item from moving.
tion was moved further away from the participant to challenge b. Allow therapist to hold items in order to sta-
shoulder range of motion. These changes to the activity did bilize task materials.
not occur until the criteria detailed in the Progressing Tasks c. Increase the grip of objects used in tasks (eg,
section (below) were met. use cylindrical foam to increase the size of a
Several universal ways to grade tasks were adopted: pencil/pen for handwriting).
d. Other adaptive equipment may be used as long
1. Physical position of the participant it encourages the performance of the selected
a. Sitting task with the affected UE.
b. Standing
2. Changing the position of task materials.
Keeping Track of the Tasks, Task Grading, and Number of
a. Change the height of task materials.
Repetitions. The solution for keeping an accurate count of the
b. Change the depth/change distance of reach
tasks, task grading, and the number of repetitions in each task
(move task materials closer or further away).
was for the study therapist to record repetitions on an
c. Place task materials midline/right/left of the
materials. (continued)

Downloaded from nnr.sagepub.com at SSRT-Tech Serv on November 4, 2011


630 Neurorehabilitation and Neural Repair 24(7)

Appendix (continued) movement. Again, this activity was related to a goal selected
on the COPM.
easy-to-use form we developed. The form had a separate page
for each task. At the top of the page, there were fields to write Example Tasks and Ways to Grade Them
in the specific task and how it was graded that session. On the
rest of the page, there were numbers from 1 to 150, arranged Activity: writing
in 6 rows of 25. At the start of the session, the therapist filled
in information about the task and grading. During the session, Materials
the therapist made a slash mark through each number as he or 1. Paper
she watched the participant perform each repetition. Whenever 2. Pen
possible, we grouped items used for a given task into groups 3. Pencil
of 5 or 10 to make counting the repetitions task easier. If the 4. Dry erase board
therapist was unsure if all repetitions were counted, the vid- 5. Cylindrical foam
eotapes were reviewed to determine this.
Progressing Tasks. The following rules were used to Method
determine when to progress a task, that is, grade up or grade 1. Patient sits at table with paper and writing utensil
down. Generally, tasks were graded up (made more diffi- of choice at midline.
cult) if the participant had successfully achieved 100 or 2. Patient practices free writing signature or words/
more repetitions in less than 15 minutes for a given task on sentences/paragraphs.
2 occasions. Additionally, if a participant achieved 100 3. Patient can fill out forms similar to those used at
repetitions at the 15-minute mark, the task was graded up work/school.
for an additional few minutes to challenge the participant.
After this occurred on 2 or more occasions, the graded-up Grading
version of the task was adopted and used until the partici-
pant achieved 100 repetitions. Tasks were graded down 1. Increase task difficulty by increasing the number
(made easier) if the participant was unable to achieve 50 of words/components of written work (ie, phone
repetitions of a task within 15 minutes. Tasks were also number, address, form letter).
graded down within the 15 minutes if the participant began 2. Decrease task difficulty by using built-up writing
exhibiting extreme fatigue or was unable to perform the utensil to aid with grip.
task. For example, 1 participant, who was diabetic, expe- 3. Decrease task difficulty by using dry erase board
rienced low blood sugar during 1 treatment session and was and writing large letters, then progress to writing
not performing the task as well as was typical. After provid- on a pad of paper.
ing her with the appropriate food, her tasks were graded Repetition description
down to accommodate her performance level for the rest
of that session. On other occasions, a few participants 1 Repetition Completed signature (pick up pen–
experienced fatigue from not sleeping well and did not write–release pen), or
perform at typical levels. On these occasions, the task was 1 Repetition 1 Word.
graded down as well for that particular session. For both
instances, in the next treatment session, the task was Activity: typing on a keyboard
returned to the pre-event level. Materials
Changing to New, Different Tasks. If a previous activity was
no longer challenging or if the participant desired to do a 1. Computer/Keyboard
different activity, 1 new task could be selected at the begin- 2. Mouse
ning of each week. In cases where the participant was able 3. Typing program (to challenge speed/accuracy)
to perform the whole activity without difficulty (100 repeti-
tions in 15 minutes on 2 occasions), the study therapist was Method
permitted to choose another activity to further challenge the
1. Patient sits at computer table with both hands on
participant, again relying on the identified COPM goals. On
the keyboard.
rare occasions, the participant stated that he or she did not
2. Patient practices typing with both hands using a
like a treatment activity. The therapist then evaluated the
typing program.
treatment activity to determine which movements were being
addressed and selected another activity to work on the same (continued)

Downloaded from nnr.sagepub.com at SSRT-Tech Serv on November 4, 2011


Birkenmeier et al 631

Appendix (continued) Activity: playing games (Connect Four)

Grading Materials

1. Connect Four game


1. Increase task difficulty by typing longer words/ 2. Table
sentences or paragraph.
2. Decrease task difficulty by typing single letters.
Method
3. Increase task difficulty by typing memos dictated
from third party.
4. Increase task difficulty by using a speed/dexterity 1. The Connect Four game board is placed on the
typing program. table at patient’s midline. Checkers are placed on
the table on the patient’s affected side.
Repetition description 2. Patient will be instructed to place checkers in the
Connect Four grid.
1 Repetition 1 Word typed, or
1 Repetition 1 Letter typed Grading

Activity: fishing (sorting tackle box) 1. Increase or decrease task difficulty by changing
the position (depth) of the grid.
Materials 2. Increase or decrease task difficulty by changing
the position of the checkers.
1. 10 Fishing lures 3. Increase or decrease task difficulty by changing
2. Various sized bobbers the height of the table.
3. Fishing weights 4. Increase or decrease task difficulty by changing
4. Tackle box the patient positioning (sitting vs standing).
Decrease task difficulty by placing the grid on
Method dycem to stabilize.

1. Tackle box is placed at patient’s midline.


Repetition description
2. Fishing weights, bobbers, and fishing lures are
placed on the affected side.
3. Patient is instructed to pick up items and place in 1 Repetition 1 Connect Four checker picked up and
the tackle box. released in grid.
4. Patient is instructed to pick up items 1 at a time.
5. Variation: patient can remove specific items from Activity: folding towels
the tackle box and place on the table.
Materials
Grading
1. 20 Wash cloths
1. Increase or decrease task difficulty by increasing 2. 10 Hand towels
or decreasing the size of the items in the tackle 3. 10 Bath towels
box.
2. Increase or decrease task difficulty by moving the
Method
tackle box closer or farther away from the patient.
1. Patient will sit or stand at the table.
Repetition description
2. Patient will fold the towels at midline while sitting
or standing.
1 Repetition Reach, grasp, release 1 fishing item
3. All towels should be folded in half and then in half
into the tackle box, or
again using bilateral UEs.
1 Repetition Reach, grasp, release 1 fishing item
from box back onto table. (continued)

Downloaded from nnr.sagepub.com at SSRT-Tech Serv on November 4, 2011


632 Neurorehabilitation and Neural Repair 24(7)

Appendix (continued) 4. Increase or decrease task difficulty by changing the


weight of objects (light–empty vs heavy–full cans).
Grading 5. Decrease task difficulty by beginning with the
patient just moving objects from the shelf to the
1. Increase task difficulty by alternating the location counter (downward).
of the towel piles to necessitate reach to facilitate 6. Decrease task difficulty by stabilizing objects with
goal movements. therapist assistance while patient is attempting grasp.
2. Decrease task difficulty by decreasing the number
of “folds” necessary to complete the folding task. Repetition description
3. Increase or decrease task difficulty by changing
the number of towels to be folded. 1 Repetition Reach, grasp, release 1 can onto shelf, or
4. Increase or decrease task difficulty by changing 1 Repetition Reach, grasp, release 1 can onto
the size of the towels. countertop.

Repetition description Task: sorting silverware

1 Repetition Towel is folded in half and then in half Materials


again.
1. Fork, spoons, knives (5 of each), standard stainless
Note: therapist unfolds towels in order for the patient to steel
fold again. 2. Fork, spoons, knives (5 of each), standard plastic
3. Sorted utensil container
Activity: lifting cans onto shelves (organizing kitchen shelves) 4. Table/Kitchen counter

Materials Method

1. Twenty 16-ounce cans 1. Patient will sit or stand at the counter, with utensils
2. Countertop with overhead cabinet containing 3 placed on the affected side and sorter placed on
shelves or the unaffected side.
3. Stacking shelves to simulate overhead cabinet 2. Patient will pick up 1 utensil at a time and place
in the correct slot.
Method
Grading
1. Therapist will place all materials on the counter.
2. Patient will stand at the countertop with kitchen 1. Decrease task difficulty by beginning with plastic
shelves directly in front of the body. utensils before changing to stainless steel.
3. Objects will be placed on the lowest shelf. 2. Increase or decrease task difficulty by increasing
4. Patient will be instructed to lift all objects onto the or decreasing the number of utensils to be sorted.
shelf one by one. 3. Increase task difficulty by alternating the position
5. When the patient has lifted all objects onto the of the utensil sorter to necessitate reach to facilitate
shelf, all objects must then be returned one by one goal movements.
to the counter. 4. Increase or decrease task difficulty by standing or
sitting.
Grading 5. Decrease task difficulty by using built-up utensils
to aid with grip.
1. Decrease task difficulty by using bilateral UEs to
complete task. Unaffected UE must be used as a Repetition description
gross assist.
2. Increase or decrease task difficulty by changing
1 Repetition Reach, grasp, release 1 utensil into
the height of the shelf.
container.
3. Increase or decrease task difficulty by changing
placement (depth) of items placed on the shelf. (continued)

Downloaded from nnr.sagepub.com at SSRT-Tech Serv on November 4, 2011


Birkenmeier et al 633

Appendix (continued) Grading

Task: managing and manipulating coins 1. Increase or decrease task difficulty by increasing
or decreasing the size of the paper.
Materials 2. Increase or decrease task difficulty by increasing
or decreasing the thickness of the paper.
1. Various coins (pennies, nickels, dimes, quarters) 3. Decrease task difficulty by beginning with plastic
2. Coin bank loop scissors (childproof scissors) and progress to
standard scissors.
Method 4. Increase task difficulty by changing the type of
line to cut (straight, zigzag, wavy lines).
1. Patient will sit or stand at the table with the coin
bank placed at midline. Repetition description
2. Patient will pick up coins 1 at a time with the
affected UE and place in the slot on the top of the 1 Repetition Cutting 1 line.
coin bank.
Declaration of Conflicting Interests
Grading
The authors declared no conflicts of interest with respect to the
1. Increase task difficulty by alternating the position authorship and/or publication of this article.
of the coin bank to necessitate reach and facilitate
goal movements. Funding
2. Increase task difficulty by rotating coin bank slot, Support for this study was provided by HealthSouth Corporation,
challenging wrist movements. the Missouri Physical Therapy Association, NIH K01 HD047669
3. Increase or decrease task difficulty by standing or (CEL), and NIH TL1 RR024994 (EMP).
sitting.
4. Decrease task difficulty by picking up coins and References
placing in another container (nonbank). 1. Teasell R, Foley N, Salter K, Boghal S, Jutai J, Speechley M.
5. Decrease task difficulty by using dycem to stabi- Evidence-Based Review of Stroke Rehabilitation. London,
lize coins (prevent slipping off table). Canada: University of Western Ontario; 2008.
2. Shepherd RB. Exercise and training to optimize functional
Repetition description motor performance in stroke: driving neural reorganization?
Neural Plast. 2001;8:121-129.
1 Repetition Reach, grasp, release 1 coin into the 3. Nudo RJ, Milliken GW, Jenkins WM, Merzenich MM. Use-
coin bank. dependent alterations of movement representations in primary
motor cortex of adult squirrel monkeys. J Neurosci. 1996;16:
Task: scrapbooking (cutting paper) 785-807.
4. Karni A, Meyer G, Jezzard P, Adams MM, Turner R,
Materials Ungerleider LG. Functional MRI evidence for adult motor
cortex plasticity during motor skill learning. Nature. 1995;377:
1. Scissors 155-158.
2. Scrapbooking paper 5. Nudo RJ, Plautz EJ, Frost SB. Role of adaptive plasticity in
recovery of function after damage to motor cortex. Muscle
Method Nerve. 2001;24:1000-1019.
6. Kleim JA, Jones TA. Principles of experience-dependent neural
1. Therapist will need to create (and photocopy) sheet plasticity: implications for rehabilitation after brain damage.
with lines prior to treatment time. J Speech Lang Hear Res. 2008;51:S225-S239.
2. Therapist will place paper in front of the patient 7. Nudo RJ, Milliken GW. Reorganization of movement repre-
at midline. sentations in primary motor cortex following focal ischemic
3. Scissors are to be placed on the patient’s affected infarcts in adult squirrel monkeys. J Neurophysiol. 1996;75:
side within reach. 2144-2149.
4. Patient will be instructed to cut paper. 8. Kleim JA, Barbay S, Nudo RJ. Functional reorganization of the
5. Patient may use the unaffected UE to assist with rat motor cortex following motor skill learning. J Neurophysiol.
the task. 1998;80:3321-3325.

Downloaded from nnr.sagepub.com at SSRT-Tech Serv on November 4, 2011


634 Neurorehabilitation and Neural Repair 24(7)

9. Carey JR, Kimberley TJ, Lewis SM, et al. Analysis of FMRI 24. Duncan PW, Lai SM, Keighley J. Defining post-stroke recovery:
and finger tracking training in subjects with chronic stroke. implications for design and interpretation of drug trials. Neuro-
Brain. 2002;125:773-788. pharmacology. 2000;39:835-841.
10. Boyd L, Winstein C. Explicit information interferes with 25. Beebe JA, Lang CE. Relationships and responsiveness of six
implicit motor learning of both continuous and discrete move- upper extremity function tests during the first six months of
ment tasks after stroke. J Neurol Phys Ther. 2006;30:46-57; recovery after stroke. J Neurol Phys Ther. 2009;33:96-103.
discussion 58-59. 26. Stuppy DJ. The FACES pain scale: reliability and validity with
11. Fine MS, Thoroughman KA. Motor adaptation to single mature adults. Appl Nurs Res. 1998;11:84-89.
force pulses: sensitive to direction but insensitive to within- 27. Williamson A, Hoggart B. Pain: a review of three commonly
movement pulse placement and magnitude. J Neurophysiol. used pain rating scales. J Clin Nurs. 2005;14:798-804.
2006;96:710-720. 28. Ritter PL, Gonzalez VM, Laurent DD, Lorig KR. Measure-
12. Lang CE, MacDonald JR, Gnip C. Counting repetitions: an ment of pain using the visual numeric scale. J Rheumatol.
observational study of outpatient day treatment for people with 2006;33:574-580.
hemiparesis. J Neurol Phys Ther. 2007;31:1-8. 29. Lyle RC. A performance test for assessment of upper limb
13. Lang CE, MacDonald JR, Reisman DS, et al. Observation of function in physical rehabilitation treatment and research. Int
amounts of movement practice provided during stroke reha- J Rehabil Res. 1981;4:483-492.
bilitation. Arch Phys Med Rehabil. 2009;90:1692-1698. 30. Yozbatiran N, Der-Yeghiaian L, Cramer SC. A standardized
14. Taub E, Miller NE, Novack TA, et al. Technique to improve approach to performing the action research arm test. Neurore-
chronic motor deficit after stroke. Arch Phys Med Rehabil. habil Neural Repair. 2008;22:78-90.
1993;74:347-354. 31. van der Lee JH, De Groot V, Beckerman H, Wagenaar RC,
15. Taub E, Uswatte G, King DK, Morris D, Crago JE, Chatterjee A. Lankhorst GJ, Bouter LM. The intra- and interrater reliability
A placebo-controlled trial of constraint-induced movement of the action research arm test: a practical test of upper extrem-
therapy for upper extremity after stroke. Stroke. 2006;37: ity function in patients with stroke. Arch Phys Med Rehabil.
1045-1049. 2001;82:14-19.
16. Wolf SL, Winstein CJ, Miller JP, et al. Effect of constraint- 32. Schmidt RT, Toews JV. Grip strength as measured by the Jamar
induced movement therapy on upper extremity function 3 to dynamometer. Arch Phys Med Rehabil. 1970;51:321-327.
9 months after stroke: the EXCITE randomized clinical trial. 33. Mathiowetz V, Kashman N, Volland G, Weber K, Dowe M,
JAMA. 2006;296:2095-2104. Rogers S. Grip and pinch strength: normative data for adults.
17. van der Lee JH, Wagenaar RC, Lankhorst GJ, Vogelaar TW, Arch Phys Med Rehabil. 1985;66:69-74.
Deville WL, Bouter LM. Forced use of the upper extremity in 34. Boissy P, Bourbonnais D, Carlotti MM, Gravel D, Arsenault BA.
chronic stroke patients: results from a single-blind randomized Maximal grip force in chronic stroke subjects and its rela-
clinical trial. Stroke. 1999;30:2369-2375. tionship to global upper extremity function. Clin Rehabil.
18. Dromerick AW, Lang CE, Birkenmeier RL, et al. Very early 1999;13:354-362.
constraint-induced movement during stroke rehabilitation 35. Duncan PW, Wallace D, Lai SM, Johnson D, Embretson S,
(VECTORS): A single-center RCT. Neurology. 2009;73:195-201. Laster LJ. The stroke impact scale version 2.0: evaluation of
19. Boake C, Noser EA, Ro T, et al. Constraint-induced move- reliability, validity, and sensitivity to change. Stroke. 1999;
ment therapy during early stroke rehabilitation. Neurorehabil 30:2131-2140.
Neural Repair. 2007;21:14-24. 36. Lai SM, Studenski S, Duncan PW, Perera S. Persisting con-
20. Winstein CJ, Miller JP, Blanton S, et al. Methods for a multisite sequences of stroke measured by the Stroke Impact Scale.
randomized trial to investigate the effect of constraint-induced Stroke. 2002;33:1840-1844.
movement therapy in improving upper extremity function 37. Duncan PW, Lai SM, Tyler D, Perera S, Reker DM,
among adults recovering from a cerebrovascular stroke. Neu- Studenski S. Evaluation of proxy responses to the Stroke Impact
rorehabil Neural Repair. 2003;17:137-152. Scale. Stroke. 2002;33:2593-2599.
21. Taub E, Uswatte G, Pidikiti R. Constraint-induced movement 38. Law M, Baptiste S, McColl M, Opzoomer A, Polatajko H,
therapy: a new family of techniques with broad application to Pollock N. The Canadian occupational performance measure:
physical rehabilitation—a clinical review. J Rehabil Res Dev. an outcome measure for occupational therapy. Can J Occup
1999;36:237-251. Ther. 1990;57:82-87.
22. Page SJ, Levine P, Sisto S, Bond Q, Johnston MV. Stroke 39. Dedding C, Cardol M, Eyssen IC, Dekker J, Beelen A. Validity
patients’ and therapists’ opinions of constraint-induced move- of the Canadian occupational performance measure: a client-
ment therapy. Clin Rehabil. 2002;16:55-60. centred outcome measurement. Clin Rehabil. 2004;18:660-667.
23. Jorgensen HS, Nakayama H, Raaschou HO, Vive-Larsen J, 40. Higgins J, Salbach NM, Wood-Dauphinee S, Richards CL,
Stoier M, Olsen TS. Outcome and time course of recovery Cote R, Mayo NE. The effect of a task-oriented intervention
in stroke. Part ii: Time course of recovery. The Copenhagen on arm function in people with stroke: a randomized controlled
stroke study. Arch Phys Med Rehabil. 1995;76:406-412. trial. Clin Rehabil. 2006;20:296-310.

Downloaded from nnr.sagepub.com at SSRT-Tech Serv on November 4, 2011


Birkenmeier et al 635

41. Patten C, Dozono J, Schmidt S, Jue M, Lum P. Combined quality of life in stroke. Neurorehabil Neural Repair. 2009;
functional task practice and dynamic high intensity resistance 23:160-165.
training promotes recovery of upper-extremity motor function 47. Lin JH, Hsu MJ, Sheu CF, et al. Psychometric comparisons of
in post-stroke hemiparesis: a case study. J Neurol Phys Ther. 4 measures for assessing upper-extremity function in people
2006;30:99-115. with stroke. Phys Ther. 2009;89:840-850.
42. Salbach NM, Mayo NE, Wood-Dauphinee S, Hanley JA, 48. Lang CE, Edwards DF, Birkenmeier RL, Dromerick AW.
Richards CL, Cote R. A task-orientated intervention enhances Estimating minimal clinically important differences of upper-
walking distance and speed in the first year post stroke: a ran- extremity measures early after stroke. Arch Phys Med Rehabil.
domized controlled trial. Clin Rehabil. 2004;18:509-519. 2008;89:1693-1700.
43. Page SJ, Levine P, Leonard A, Szaflarski JP, Kissela BM. 49. Ross LF, Harvey LA, Lannin NA. Do people with acquired
Modified constraint-induced therapy in chronic stroke: results brain impairment benefit from additional therapy specifi-
of a single-blinded randomized controlled trial. Phys Ther. cally directed at the hand? A randomized controlled trial. Clin
2008;88:333-340. Rehabil. 2009;23:492-503.
44. Page SJ, Levine P, Leonard AC. Modified constraint-induced 50. Platz T, van Kaick S, Mehrholz J, Leidner O, Eickhof C,
therapy in acute stroke: a randomized controlled pilot study. Pohl M. Best conventional therapy versus modular impairment-
Neurorehabil Neural Repair. 2005;19:27-32. oriented training for arm paresis after stroke: a single-blind,
45. Page SJ, Sisto S, Levine P, McGrath RE. Efficacy of modi- multicenter randomized controlled trial. Neurorehabil Neural
fied constraint-induced movement therapy in chronic stroke: Repair. 2009;23:706-716.
a single-blinded randomized controlled trial. Arch Phys Med 51. Wade D. Control in rehabilitation research. Clin Rehabil. 2009;
Rehabil. 2004;85:14-18. 23:675-680.
46. Lin K-C, Wu C, Liu J, Chen Y, Hsu C-J. Constraint- 52. Dobkin BH. Rehabilitation and functional neuroimaging dose-
induced therapy versus dose-matched control intervention response trajectories for clinical trials. Neurorehabil Neural
to improve motor ability, basic/extended daily functions, and Repair. 2005;19:276-282.

Downloaded from nnr.sagepub.com at SSRT-Tech Serv on November 4, 2011

You might also like