Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 32

Personality and Social Psychology Review Copyright ( 2001 by

2001, Vol. 5, No. 3, 242-273 Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.

Is There a Gender Difference in Strength of Sex Drive? Theoretical


Views, Conceptual Distinctions, and a Review of Relevant Evidence
Roy F. Baumeister, Kathleen R. Catanese, and Kathleen D. Vohs
Department of Psychology
Case Western Reserve University

The sex drive refers to the strength ofsexual motivation. Across many different studies
and measures, men have been shown to have more frequent and more intense sexual
desires than women, as reflected in spontaneous thoughts about sex, frequency and
variety of sexual fantasies, desired frequency of intercourse, desired number of part-
ners,masturbation, liking for various sexual practices, willingness toforego sex, initi-
ating versus refusing sex, making sacrifices for sex, and other measures. No contrary
findings (indicating stronger sexual motivation among women) were found. Hence we

conclude that the male sex drive is stronger than the female sex drive. The gender dif-
ference in sex drive should not be generalized to other constructs such as sexual or or-
gasmic capacity, enjoyment of sex, or extrinsically motivated sex.

If the world were designed for the primary goal of equality. If one gender generally experiences stronger
maximizing human happiness, the sexual Hastes of men or more frequent sexual desires, then this pattern is
and women would match up very closely. vv hat could likely to become incorporated into the society's con-
be more ideal than perfect attunement with one' s mate, ceptions of sex roles. Interaction patterns within ongo-
so that both people feel sexual desire at the same times, ing relationships, and perhaps outside of relationships
to the same degrees, and in the same ways? Yet there is too, will likely be shaped in recognition of the greater
ample evidence that romantic partners are sometimes desires of one gender. In contrast, if the two genders
out of synchrony with each other' s sexual wishes and are basically equal in sex drive, then negotiating a het-
feelings. The continuing market for sexual advice, sex erosexual relationship can focus on other issues.
therapy, couple counseling, and similar offerings is a A second set of implications that attend the question
testimony to the fact that many people are not perfectly of differential sex drive is concerned with reliance on
satisfied with their sex lives even within committed re- norms for self-understanding and self-evaluation. Sup-
lationships. Infidelity and divorce may also sometimes pose a particular woman desires sex more often than
reflect sexual dissatisfaction. her husband. If this is a typical pattern that character-
The focus of this article is on one potential source of izes most relationships, she should probably accept her
sexual disagreement. Specifically, in this article we re- greater desire as a standard fact of life, and certainly
view evidence pertaining to the question of whether there is no reason for her to engage in self-doubts or
men and women differ (on average) in the strength of guilt or to wonder what is wrong with her. In contrast,
sex drive. As we suggested, the ideal would be that the if the typical pattern is the opposite (greater desire
average sex drive would be equally strong in men and among husbands), then she may more appropriately
women, in which case individual variations would be wonder why her situation is different. Undoubtedly the
the only obstacle to marital harmony, and many people worst outcome is if a woman reaches a self-critical
could find a partner with a fairly precise match of sex- view based on a false understanding of what the actual
ual inclinations. Unfortunately, life may not conform norms and typical patterns are such that she thinks
to such ideals, and if one gender differs from the other something is wrong with her-even though her rela-
in average strength of sex drive, pervasive patterns of tionship in fact shows the same pattern that the major-
potential conflict could result. ity of couples experience. Improved public knowledge
The importance of the question of gender differ- would reduce such needless suffering.
ences in sex drive goes beyond utopian dreams of A third set of implications concerns a broader under-
standing of social exchange and interaction patterns in ro-
Requests for reprints should be sent to Roy F. Baumeister or mantic and sexual relationships. In any given
Kathleen Vohs, Department of Psychology, Case Western Reserve
University, 10900 Euclid Avenue, Cleveland, OH 44106-7123. relationship, whichever partner wants sex more is in a
E- mail: rfb2 @po.cwru.edu or kdv3@po.cwru.edu weaker position, insofar as greater desire creates depend-

242
GENDER DIFFERENCE IN SEX DRIVE?

ency on the partner (see Waller & Hill, 1938/195 1; also drive as opposed to the null hypothesis of no differ-
Baumeister & Tice, 2001). To the extent that sex can ence. The possibility that men have a stronger sex drive
serve as a medium for social exchange involving any was not considered. They concluded that future gener-
other social or material resources, it may influence the en- ations may recognize that women have an innately
tire relationship. Gender roles and marital equity will de- stronger sex drive than men, but at present they
pend on whether other resources are indeed exchanged in thought the limited evidence available favors the con-
a way to make up the imbalance in sexual interest. Thus, clusion that there is no gender difference in sex drive.
unequal sex drives may shape the broader marital rela- To summarize, these four leading textbooks either
tionship in ways that go far beyond the bedroom. avoid the issue of gender difference in sex drive or cau-
The question of whether men differ from women in tiously suggest that there is no difference. If a differ-
the average strength of sex drive is both immediate and ence were to emerge, these textbooks disagree as to
elusive. It is immediate in that almost every person can which gender will ultimately stand revealed as having
have some direct experience through marriage and other the stronger sex drive.
sexual relationships. It is elusive in that opinions differ The very breadth of the question about differences
widely. When we told people we were studying whether in sex drive makes it difficult for a single empirical
men and women differ in strength of sex drive, most peo- study to provide a conclusive answer. There is no sin-
ple responded by saying that the answer was obvi- gle optimal way to measure strength of sex drive, and
ous but when we cautiously asked them what the so multiple measures are needed. Nor is there any par-
obvious answer was, we heard all three possible answers ticular sample that is perfect, so multiple studies with
(i.e., men higher, women higher, no difference) endorsed. multiple samples would be helpful. Ideological interest
The existence of different opinions on this matter is in the question of differential sex drive also raises the
not confined to laypersons. Writings for the general possibility that experimenter effects (such as by asking
public have advocated polar opposite views, ranging questions in certain ways) could produce artifactual re-
from Acton's (1857) widely quoted assertion that "the sults in a single study. We think the only viable way to
majority of women (happily for society) are not very address the question of differential sex drive is in a
much troubled with sexual feeling of any kind" (p. broad literature review that examines many studies by
163) to Ehrenreich' s (1999) authoritative assertion that different investigators, using different methods and
woman, not man, is biologically destined to be "the measures, and with different samples. Convergence of
sexual powerhouse of the species" (p. 64). evidence across a range of studies would seemingly be
We also consulted leading textbooks on sexuality to required for drawing a firm conclusion. This article is
find whether any consensus existed on the topic about an attempt to provide such a review.
gender differences in sex drive, but the answer ap- To forestall possible misunderstandings or im-
peared to be no. Crooks and Baur (1999) dismissed the proper applications of our conclusions, it seems es-
view of stronger male sex drive as erroneous: "A sential to state that we do not see any obvious value
long-standing assumption in many Western societies is judgment regarding the desirability of a stronger ver-
the mistaken belief that women are inherently less sex- sus weaker sex drive. During the height of the "sexual
ually inclined than men" (p. 68). Thus, the textbook ac- revolution" in the 1970s, it was briefly fashionable to
knowledged a view that men have a stronger sex drive speak as if more sexual desire was always better, but
than women, but it rejected that view as having been the sobering aftermath of that period of sexual license
discredited. The possibility that the female sex drive is has presumably led to a more balanced view. Either
stronger than the male was not even considered in that extreme of sexual motivation can certainly be
work. Masters, Johnson, and Kolodny (1995) also ac- maladaptive and problematic, but there is a broad
knowledged that stereotypes exist, usually depicting middle range in which it is probably meaningless to
males as having more sexual desire than females, but speak of "better" or "worse," and we believe the aver-
the authors carefully avoided the question of whether age man and the average woman both fall within that
the stereotypes have any factual basis. Allgeier and range. Hence we do not think that any likely conclu-
Allgeier (2000) likewise acknowledged the existence sion about gender differences in sex drive would
of a stereotype that men have larger appetites for sex, mean that one gender was superior to the other.
but they too declined to say whether the stereotype had
any factual basis, and their treatment of gender differ-
ences in sexual arousability clearly favored the null hy- Theoretical and Operational
pothesis of no difference. The final textbook we Definitions
consulted was Hyde and DeLamater (1997), who in-
cluded a subsection entitled "Greater Sex Drive in Theoretical debates, value judgments, and general
Women?" As the title implies, it was concerned only confusion may arise from mixing or exchanging differ-
with the possibility that women have a stronger sex ent concepts so that seemingly opposite sides are not

243
BAUMEISTER, CATANESE, & VOHS

actually talking about the same thing. There are proba- be one who gets more enjoyment or pleasure out of
bly several relevant concepts that may well be fully in- sex. Probably the yield of pleasure varies considerably
dependent of each other, and so it is vital to make clear within persons across different situations and episodes
conceptual distinctions. (i.e., most people probably experience both relatively
As we understand the term, the sex drive refers to good and relatively bad sex at some point or other), but
the sexual motivation, usually focused on craving for there may still be individual differences in average de-
sexual activity and sexual pleasure. Desire is probably gree of enjoyment. Enjoyment is an outcome and is
the most precise rendition. A person with higher sex therefore conceptually quite different from sex drive,
drive would be one with more intense or more frequent although in general higher drive would plausibly be
desires, or both, for sex. To be sure, one can profitably correlated with higher enjoyment.
make finer distinctions. Hill (1997) observed that peo- Our primary focus is on the sex drive and thus the
ple's interest in sexual activity can have multiple strength of motivation. Operationally, we reason that
sources, such as desire for relief from stress, desire to higher sex drive will result in a broad variety of behav-
procreate, or enhancing feelings of power. Our analy- ioral manifestations because the relatively strong moti-
sis, however, focuses specifically on the desire for sex vation should produce more frequent and more intense
for its own sake, and therefore a desire for sex as a efforts to reach satisfaction. Extremely strong motiva-
means toward procreation or toward other nonsexual tions override (at least temporarily) other motivations,
goals is not part of our concept of sex drive. Strictly and the result is that behavior becomes almost entirely
speaking, desiring sex for the sake of distal goals organized by a supremely powerful motivation. In con-
would fall in the category of extrinsic motivation (see trast, a motivation that is extremely weak will yield
Deci, 1971; Lepper & Greene, 1978), whereas desiring precedence to every other motivation, and as a result it
sex for its own sake constitutes intrinsic motivation. will not produce much in the way of behavioral results.
We consider extrinsic motivations for sex briefly after Hence we have sought both subjective and objective
reviewing the evidence about intrinsic motivation. indexes of strength of sex drive.
It is also important to recognize that the term drive
does not necessarily refer to a biologically innate ten-
dency. One may speak of someone's drive in the sense Evidence: Strength of Sex Drive
of ambition within a particular job and presumably
much of that is acquired by socialization and experi- The sex drive is best understood in terms of the fre-
ence. In the history of psychology, the term drive was quency and intensity of desire. The question of gender
adopted as a way of avoiding the implication of innate- differences in sex drive thus refers to whether one gen-
ness that the term instinct entailed. To be sure, where bi- der desires sex more than the other.
ologically innate motivations exist, they undoubtedly The question of gender differences in sex drive may
contribute to the level of drive in particular individuals, be best approached by stepping away briefly from gen-
and most theorists probably regard some degree of sex- der and considering how two people of the same gen-
ual motivation as innate. Still, any findings of gender der would be observably different if their sex drives
differences in sex drive (motivation) should not auto- differed in strength. In other words, in what specific
matically be interpreted as reflecting innate differences ways would a woman with a strong sex drive differ
and especially not immutable differences. from a woman with a weak sex drive? On an a priori
Another relevant concept is sexual capacity. This re- basis, one would expect the difference in motivation to
fers to the maximum limit of sexual activity that some- be reflected in desired frequency of sex, desired variety
one can do. A person with higher sexual capacity would of sex acts and partners, frequency of fantasy, fre-
be one with greater ability to have more sex, such as quency of masturbation, number of partners, fre-
with more different partners, more often, or for a longer quency of thinking about sex, willingness to make
period of time. Drive and capacity are separate con- sacrifices in other spheres to obtain sex, and the like.
structs. Men are presumably as capable as women of di- We shall therefore examine evidence about gender dif-
eting, but they appear to be less driven to do so. ferences across this range of relevant variables. Proba-
Likewise, women are as capable as men of watching bly many single findings would be subject to
televised sports events, but they appear to be somewhat alternative explanations instead of pointing unambigu-
less motivated to do so. Any evidence of greater sexual ously to differential sex drive. It is therefore necessary
capacity by either gender would not therefore necessar- to find convergence across many studies and many
ily have any bearing on the question of differential sex operationalizations to rely on the rule of parsimony for
drive. Capacity is however important in its own right. drawing conclusions about the strength of sex drive.
A final concept is sexual enjoyment. This refers to Our strategy for locating sources was roughly as
the amount of pleasure that one derives from sexual ac- follows. As we have just mentioned, sex drive is not a
tivity. A person with higher sexual enjoyment would unitary variable with only one manifestation; rather, it

244
GENDER DIFFERENCE IN SEX DRIVE?

is our view that a variety of different behaviors, (1971) found that men reported more frequent
cognitions, and emotions, taken together, indicate sex- thoughts about sex than women. Laumann, Gagnon,
ual motivation. Thus, to locate sources for our review, Michael, and Michaels (1994) found that men think
we could not simply enter a few key words into a data- about sex more often than women. Over half the men
base and get the kind of data we were seeking. Our ini- in their national sample reported thinking about sex ev-
tial strategy was to read all abstracts of all articles in ery day, whereas only one fifth of the women reported
the Journal of Sex Research from the initial (1965) vol- thinking about sex that often.
ume up to the present and then all articles that offered Recent studies on uncontrolled and unwanted sexual
any measures of sexual motivation. The Archives of thoughts underscore the conclusion that the male sex
Sexual Behavior received similar treatment going back drive evokes more sexual thinking even if the person
to 1990. We then looked up sources cited in those arti- does not wish to have those thoughts. Byers, Purdon, and
cles, especially the more recent ones, that contributed Clark (1998) found that male college students reported
data regarding sexual motivation. more intrusive, unwanted, and even personally unaccept-
Subsequently, we went to the PsycINFO database able thoughts about sex than did college women (7.5 vs.
(American Psychological Association, 1967-2001) 5.6 out of 20 possible sexually intrusive thoughts listed).
and entered key words into the "keyword," "subject," Vanwesenbeeck, Bekker, and van Lenning (1998) devel-
and "abstract" search fields. Specifically, we searched oped a sexual compulsion scale with items such as "I
for sex (and its derivatives) and drive under keyword, think about sex more than I would like" and "I must fight
subject, and abstract search fields; we searched for sex- to keep my sexual thoughts and behavior under control."
ual and motiv (and its derivatives) in the subject and Men scored higher than women on this scale, indicating a
abstract search fields; we searched for sexual-desire as greater sense of being sexually driven.
a keyword and sexual and desire in the subject and ab- Data on spontaneous sexual arousal and desire reveal
stract search fields; and we searched for the letters libid one way in which men seem to have a higher sex drive.
(and its derivatives) under both the subject and abstract Beck, Bozman, and Qualtrough (1991) found that men
search fields. Our PsycINFO search yielded over 3,400 report more frequent sexual desire than women. Nearly
citations. A similar search in MEDLINE (National Li- all the men (91 %) but only half the women (52%) expe-
brary of Medicine, 1966-2001) yielded approximately rienced sexual desire several times a week or more.
2,000 citations. Their study also helped rule out the alternative explana-
The guidelines that we followed for inclusion or ex- tion that women find it more difficult than men to recog-
clusion of specific studies were as follows. We omitted nize sexual desire, because men and women endorsed
from our review research on nonhumans, dissertations essentially the same indicators of desire.
(however, if the dissertation was also published in a Likewise, a study by Knoth, Boyd, and Singer
refereed journal, it was included), and case studies. We (1988) concluded that the modal young man experi-
favored articles reporting normal populations, al- ences spontaneous sexual arousal several times per
though we also report some evidence on clinical popu- day, whereas the modal young women experiences it
lations, especially in connection with studies of the only a couple times per week. Eysenck (1971) likewise
physiological aspects of sexuality (and presenting found that men reported more frequent sexual desire
complaints of hypoactive sexual desire). Narrowly de- and more easily stimulated desire than women. Jones
fined subpopulations (e.g., people who are HIV posi- and Barlow (1990) had a sample of young heterosexual
tive, people with depression, paraplegics) were also adults monitor their sexual feelings for 7 days, and the
screened out. Last, we also omitted many studies that men had more than twice as many sexual urges per day
focused only on one gender, although there are data as the women (4.75 vs. 2.00, respectively). The differ-
(again, especially referring to the biological aspects of ence in internally generated fantasies was not signifi-
sexuality) that were applicable to our review. cant, but men had significantly more sexual fantasies
during masturbation than women.
It is possible that women sometimes do not know
Thoughts, Fantasies, and that they are sexually aroused, given the less salient na-
Spontaneous Arousal ture of female arousal as compared to male arousal. It
is thus conceivable that women are aroused more often
The person with the greater sex drive would proba- than they report, and therefore the true gap between the
bly think about sex more often than the person with the genders could be smaller than it appears. Then again,
lesser drive, just as the person with a greater motiva- the women in these studies were able to report that they
tional interest in money, children, or football will de- experienced spontaneous sexual arousal sometimes, so
vote more spontaneous thought to that topic than a less they are not entirely inept at sensing their own re-
interested person. Several studies have assessed fre- sponses. One might even suggest that the relative in-
quency of spontaneous thoughts about sex. Eysenck visibility of female sexual arousal causes women

245
BAUMEISTER, CATANESE, & VOHS

merely to guess and estimate their frequency of re- couples who had been married for over 20 years. He
sponse, and therefore perhaps they overestimate this found that "husbands continued to prefer intercourse
frequency, in which case the gender difference would more frequently than wives" (p. 274). In fact, wives
be even larger than these studies found. In any case, the consistently reported that they were quite satisfied
best current evidence suggests more frequent arousal with the amount of sex they had in their marriages, but
in men than in women. men on average wished for about a 50% increase. M.
Sexual fantasies are probably one of the best in- Brown and Auerback (1981) likewise found that a ma-
dexes of strength of sex drive because they are explic- jority of husbands (60%) but only a minority of wives
itly sexual and require conscious attention but are not (32%) said they would prefer to have sex more often. A
constrained by opportunities, social pressures, or other more recent study by Julien, Bouchard, Gagnon, and
external factors. A person may quietly enjoy a sexual Pomerleau (1992) found that husbands and wives
fantasy during a bus ride, a lecture, or a nap, for exam- agreed that the men were more sexually active and
ple. Moreover, it seems quite safe to assume that a per- frisky. Even more relevant, Julien et al. (1992) found
son with a high sex drive will have more frequent that men were more likely than women to report hav-
sexual fantasies than a person with a low sex drive. ing less sex in marriage than they wanted. With a sam-
Consistent with the view that fantasies are an index of ple of couples ages 51 to 61, Johannes and Avis (1997)
desire, Nutter and Condron (1983) found that women found that women were more likely than men to wish
suffering from chronically inhibited sexual desire re- for less frequent sex than they were having, whereas
ported less sexual fantasy than normal control women. husbands were more likely to wish for more frequent
Gender differences in sexual fantasy have been ex- sex than they were having. A study of elderly couples
amined in many studies. A review and meta-analysis in Sweden likewise found that men wanted more fre-
by Leitenberg and Henning (1995) concluded that men quent sex than women (Bergstrdm-Walan & Nielsen,
have more frequent and more varied fantasies than 1990). Indeed, the authors of that study concluded that
women. That is, men' s fantasies occur more often than "men are significantly more sexual than women, in all
women' s, include more different partners than ages and in all respects" (p. 289).
women's, and extend to a broader variety of sex acts Those findings refer to mature couples who are well
than women's (on an individual rather than a popula- into long-term relationships. One might expect that men
tion basis probably there is at least one woman who and women would be more similar early in relation-
has had any given fantasy). These differences in fan- ships. Yet data show that at the start of a relationship,
tasy suggest greater sex drive in men. men desire sex more than women. Abundant evidence
The variety in sex partners was the focus of a study confirms that men are ready for sex earlier in a relation-
by Ellis and Symons (1990). They asked people whether ship than women. In a large Australian sample, McCabe
they had had sex with over a thousand different partners (1987) found that the category of people who were in a
in their imagination. Given the relatively young age of committed relationship, who wanted to have sex, but
their sample (college students), a very active and highly who were not having sex, consisted almost entirely of
motivated imagination would presumably be necessary men. For example, among 25-year-olds, 28% of men
to achieve that high a tally. They found that men were but only 2% of women were in this category of "reluc-
four times more likely than women to report having tant virgins." Sprecher and Regan (1996) found that
imagined a thousand or more sex partners. men were more likely than women to cite partner un-
Thus, as compared with women, men think about willingness as the reason they were not having sex.
sex more often, report more frequent arousal, and have Driscoll and Davis (1971) found that men were more
more frequent and variable fantasies. These findings likely than women to list as a reasons for not having sex
would be most consistent with a view that men have a the fact that they could not talk their partner into doing
higher sex drive. so and that the decision was not theirs. Women are will-
ing to wait longer within a dating relationship, measured
either in terms of clock and calendar time or in terms of
Desired Frequency of Sex number of dates, before having sex (Buss & Schmitt,
1993; Cohen & Shotland, 1996; Sprecher, Barbee, &
A rather direct index of the strength of any drive is Schwartz, 1995). For example, Cohen and Shotland
how frequently the person feels the desire to satisfy it. found that men expected sex after about 8 dates,
Certainly one would expect that a person with a stron- whereas women expected it after about 12.
ger sex drive would desire sex more often than a person Thus, within heterosexual relationships, men want
with a relatively weak sex drive. Desired frequency of sex more than women at the start of a relationship, in
sex is therefore an important measure of sex drive. the middle of it, and after many years of it. Consistent
Many findings suggest that men want sex more fre- with that sweeping conclusion, McCabe (1987) found
quently than women. Ard (1977) reported a survey of that men in relationships (across the full sample and all

246
GENDER DIFFERENCE IN SEX DRIVE?

levels of relationship longevity) showed significantly but only a third of the gay men, were having sex less
more desire for intercourse than they were having, than once a month. Even that difference may be a sub-
whereas women had about what they wanted. stantial underestimate of the discrepancy in sexual ac-
This might seem exhaustive, but Baumeister and tivity: Blumstein and Schwartz reported that the gay
Bratslavsky (1999) proposed that there are theoretical men who had largely ceased having sex after 10 years
grounds to anticipate one possible exception. Spe- together were often having sex with other partners,
cifically, they proposed that there may be a phase during whereas the lesbians who had ceased having sex to-
which the relationship is blossoming in the sense that in- gether had generally not compensated for this deficit
timacy is rising rapidly and passion is high, and during by finding other sexual outlets. A lack of sexual desire
that brief interval the gap between men and women may and activity in women is reflected in the phrase "les-
dwindle or even disappear entirely. Thus, we do think bian bed death," (e.g., Iasenza, 2000) which has been
there remains the conceptual possibility that there is a coined to describe the low levels of sexual activity
brief phase in many relationships during which female among lesbians in long-term relationships.
sexual desire may approach the male desire in frequency Similar conclusions emerged from an earlier study
and intensity. Among 20-year-olds who had been dating by Bell and Weinberg (1978), which did not limit its
for about 2 years, Davies, Katz, and Jackson (1999) sample to people in committed relationships and is
found no mean gender difference in self-reported sexual thus a useful complement to the Blumstein and
desire, which does suggest that there may be a phase of Schwartz (1983) study. White homosexual men were
equal desire. Still, other evidence for that conclusion re- more likely than lesbians (47% vs. 32%) to report hav-
mains sparse, and the weight of evidence shows unmis- ing sex more than once per week. A similar difference
takably that at most points in relationships men want was found among gay Blacks (65% vs. 56%).
more sex than women. Thus, evidence from multiple sources indicates that
One reason that women may be less willing to en- men want sex more often than women. This appears to be
gage in sexual intercourse is because of the possibility true in both homosexual and heterosexual relationships
of becoming pregnant as a result. By biological neces- and at all ages and relationship stages. Table 1 summa-
sity, women are much more invested in pregnancy and, rizes the findings reviewed in this section. The pattern
thus, they may be reluctant to have sex because they would tentatively suggest higher sex drive in men.
recognize they will be the ones to suffer the conse-
quences. Thus, for heterosexual couples, women' s
weaker desire for sex could indicate cautiousness due
to the possibility of pregnancy. One way to examine Desired Number of Sex Partners
this hypothesis would be to consider sexual activity
that does not risk pregnancy, such as same-gender sex. Another possible sign of a high sex drive is desire for
Same-sex relationships provide relevant evidence many different partners. All else being equal, we would
about gender differences in sex drive. People in these expect a person with a stronger sex drive to want to have
relationships are clearly willing to defy social pres- sex with more different people than someone with a
sures favoring heterosexuality, at least to the extent of weaker sex drive. This is an important complement to
forming a committed same-gender relationship, and so the previous section, which focused on how often the
it is reasonable to conclude that people are not simply person desires to have sex with the same partner.
conforming to social prescriptions. Also, the Several studies have approached this question by
same-gender relationships are relatively free of the di- asking young adults how many sex partners they
rect influence of the opposite gender, and so it is possi- would ideally like to have over the rest of their lives if
ble to examine what sexual patterns look like when they were unconstrained by fear of disease, legal con-
they are set by only one gender. straints, possessive mates, and the like.
One large investigation that included a sizeable Surely it is possible that desire for multiple partners
sample of same-gender relationships was the study by might be a special case. One can argue that social pres-
Blumstein and Schwartz (1983). They found that gay sures and evolutionary contingencies shaped men to be
men had higher frequencies of sex than lesbians at all more inclined than women toward promiscuity. If the
stages of relationships. Within the first 2 years of a re- promiscuity data were to differ from the general patterns
lationship, for example, two thirds of the gay men but found with other measures, it might be necessary to dis-
only one third of the lesbians were in the maximum miss it as a nondiagnostic exception. However, if it re-
category of having sex three or more times per week flects the same pattern found with other variables, then
(the highest frequency category). After 10 years to- it seems reasonable to treat it as indicative of the larger
gether, 11% of the gay men but only 1% of the lesbians pattern. Certainly it seems reasonable on an a priori ba-
were still in that category of highly frequent sex. At the sis to predict that a woman with a high sex drive would
other extreme, after 10 years nearly half the lesbians, desire more partners than a woman with a low sex drive.

247
0) c0
0m Ct3 -50
0) .0)
-5 -
Ct
E 1<0
0t 0
Ct C0 ._
0
. 0CC
0 X 0
0CC.
0 ~CA CO
Ct 0 S 00
0) 0 0 I 2 0. 0) u-,-.
0 E u 0)
0 0O >x t .) ct U- CO x
0)
Cf
.
0)

0) 0 E,
0 'A
0
0
0)
0) 0 00
V0
Ct q c0 o X S
0 o -.

0t 21<
0) 0
0X =00)
*5 0)
._
0)
x
u
r. 0
0)
0

0o
OC FE
4. CO
u) R E
O Oo5) ._) Ct aC 0a)
08 < ^ 0~
u-
-OI
0) V)

a 0 C) . .. .

0):
0 ~0 0
00 0)
Q
01.0
0 0).
u au
._ r- ._
0 "
CO u 0) 50 .
O 1X C)
.
Su > ,I:)
._ 3 0 Q 0)0 .
o
0)0
,o u
0 0X
CV,
_: r
o
0r
00

0t
t
0 -"
;zm -C5
3S 1-O
0)
u
CO
0))
;O C)au
-0
C)
00
0)
0 ~Cf
C.,) 00
a) 0 00 .
0/~ U 0 4 . 0.0
00.5 '4 C) 0)
0)
00 00 00

VE
Cl a)
0.
0
C)
o0 11 0
Cl CO
.5C CC
wo
CC 11
11C eCl CC

11 'lI eC- 4~ z0
CC 0 0 CC 0

II kr) 0
11
0
0
0) C) OIN 0t
O
0m CC ll, 0
C-- CC O
11 CC) CO 11 C--

0 e-- 00C 0 A m CC
II C Ol
01 0-01 i -
00

Cl
0
o0
Cl *;E~-
CC a)
.. u E -
0) ,S

4 ou 3E2 Cl ,0 0): 0) 0
0) -0~ 11 il
r- 3 C 0 t
oll
.) l 000 0) 0)-
0 0
0: _0
O-0
0
u 0
Cu Q)
.I..
0
.50 .m .t
0) 0)
0)
&:
0
0
3 0
&
0)1

0h to
m0C 0o8
<C t0 .. .5
.S5 Q0 >o a) . 0)

4-
00 -. 01)
0 0
. 0 0
U 5- CtJ
.5 . CS 0
0-t Ct
0 3
a)
U:
S: 0) 0 O
00)0 -: .s 3C
0 0 CIC
C0)' 00. 00
01 0)
._E .5- . Cf
000) Cl4
U:C
._ a~
0
E
-. .
0
-- -.0.
0 tb- 3 OCC 0 0 Q
I..
5 o 0)
00
1<
Ln
I.5S
Cf 0)
.C-. CO Cf
COI
0~I 0)
z 0u
CO
0)
r

04)
C.
cl3
Ct
0)
w) CO
CC~ O.
I-
on
0) I._ c0
0)

Sz 3
00
6 1. 0

F* 00
o:
0
0
0
3
Ct
X o
,o
._

0) 0
0
._ u 30 u0 0

to 0
et ~-z .0Z
3 o- V Vo 0. 0 0t- 0:
0 * 0._0 0v,
0 0
=3 0 0 X
X t O 00 0
.0 0. .5
Ct s:0
CZ 0-00 0
la 0~ X 11
01 0t W .0
X 3 C-
00
) 0
; 0;
_m
O (A O 0. 0.0 S
Z.
0t
. 0
x 1.U
.

c0t
co0
0 8o 0-.
0
50

0
0

00_
.0 0<
0
8.
0.. 0Q
-0
0
0;
S
03
00= 0
0
O00 0
M.C~
ONe

.,0
0:
M C., Co, Cr 00 0
r-- -4 0 00
~~~ r-. -.
.c
00 o
0
Ci

11 11 It 1C N O N
11 II 11 11
Z: m 11
. z 01 00Z Il 0- 1 0

0.
Cln
00 CS
0
.0
--4 r- 0
0t
rf. r-~ Cl .1

C .,c 00 ON 00

11 11 'Itt11
-6 C~, tl Cl 00
OC
NE
S4 l
S
q z
:. Ln 0
11 II11 0

.0-

u:
3 a-) M cn
0
C.)ct u ct
0
crct
C'n
X 0 E m
C .55 r- clt
Cl Cl It E
1N
*1 o It 0 0 l0 z O
.0
ON
ON
-O C
~ 0
tI3
cq 0 0._
1~ ~ ; 0
0 .5
r. 0 0
s: 0 .
0
0 o
0
0

C.0
0D
Ct
o. S 0n 0L

tfN~~._
0> 0o 0+
0
Cl
0k V.:
30 .&0 c

0 t.
U0 .
CA
Eo -Q .00
0t
0 :e
0=ec o 0
0 0 00
Cl 0:* z
V5
ON
-0
C' cL,
0

e
0

.jC000
0
.5; t

00
0 <
X S3 00
C4 r-
CA
,.0, 5036o N _

- X0
0- S
Ct
0 0 00
,; X

o
;
0: X 35 -o 0
00
0-0 O0 ._
b a ; .= . .

'u 3
0
C- 00 0t .0Q
0^.
1
Ct
w

0E :t -0
-
Q
00-
3 5 0 -
NA 0 an
5?U U: - 3.
r- 0. -
00

:U
._

u .t D
0- 00
0 ON 0 04
;- 0
x
.0
ON _0 ,
0.0
Ct
0: Uo
-

.5: _.
CL m .0
00
0
*O s. 00
0. <0
0: r
BAUMEISTER, CATANESE, & VOHS

Buss and Schmitt (1993) reported from several stud- students "Would you say you had sex if ... " and then
ies that men desired significantly more sex partners than presented a list of possible activities. Men and women
women did. In reporting how many sex partners men agreed very closely that vaginal and anal intercourse
and women would like to have over the next 2 years of constituted sex and that kissing did not, but they dis-
their lives, for example, the men were on average hop- agreed on the intermediate activities such as fellatio,
ing to have about 8 partners, whereas the women wanted cunnilingus, and manual stimulation of a partner's
approximately 1. Over the course of a lifetime, men genitals. Men were consistently more likely to rate
wanted around 18, whereas women desired 4 or 5. those activities as sex than women. This fits the view
Miller and Fishkin (1997) asked a sample of college stu- that men desired to count those activities as having had
dents how many sex partners they would like to have sex, which would serve the goal of enabling them to
over the entire rest of their lives if they were not con- think they had a higher number of sex partners.
strained by any factors such as disease or laws. The Desire for multiple partners can lead to extramarital
mean response by the women was that they would ide- or extradyadic activity. Most studies of extramarital
ally like to have 2.7 sex partners, whereas the men's activity find that men report more partners than
mean response was 64. Miller and Fishkin did not delete women, in both heterosexual and homosexual relation-
outliers from their sample, and in fact they noted that the ships (Cotton, 1975; Lawson, 1988; Spanier &
difference in means was almost entirely due to the skew: Margolis, 1983; Thompson, 1983). For example,
The median was 1 partner for both genders. Thus, large Spanier and Margolis found that 26% of the unfaithful
numbers of young men and women aspire to having husbands had had more than three extramarital part-
only 1 sex partner across a lifetime, but there is a minor- ners, as compared to only 5% of the unfaithful wives.
ity of promiscuously inclined men that is much larger Conversely, wives outnumbered husbands in the cate-
than the minority of promiscuously inclined women. gory of having only one extramarital partner (64% vs.
One may reject these studies as being merely hypo- 43%). The same conclusion emerges from studies of
thetical and insist on actual behavior. The same con- lesser infidelities, such as necking or petting with
clusion emerges: Men actually report significantly someone other than a steady dating partner: Men do
more sex partners than women, across all studies (e.g., this more than women (Hansen, 1987).
Janus & Janus, 1993; Laumann et al., 1994). Unfortu- Another consequence of a desire for multiple part-
nately this difference suffers from being logically im- ners would be engaging in sex with someone whom
possible, insofar as heterosexual intercourse involves one has just met. Herold and Mewhinney (1993) sur-
one man and one woman (so the mean tallies of part- veyed singles bar patrons, who presumably are already
ners should be equal). Several studies have sought to selected for interest in meeting new sex partners, but
explain this recurrent finding, and the answers con- even in that selected population they found that men
verge on motivated cognition: Some men, but fewer were more likely than women to have had sex with
women, tend to rely on estimating the number of sex someone they had met that same day. For example,
partners and hence round up, whereas women are more when asked whether they had ever engaged in any sex-
likely to rely on trying to enumerate all prior partners, ual activity beyond hugging and kissing with a person
which tends to lead to occasionally forgetting some who they had met the same day, 80% of men but only
partners and hence to producing an undercount (N. R. 59% of women said "yes." When asked about sexual
Brown & Sinclair, 1999; Wiederman, 1997). intercourse with someone they had met that same day,
We note too that median differences are plausible, 72% of the men as opposed to 49% of the women said
unlike mean differences. A few highly promiscuous "yes." The men were also significantly more likely to
women can have sex with many men. The median dif- express a desire and expectation to do so again. A quar-
ferences (e.g., Laumann et al., 1994) fit the view that ter (25%) of the men but only 2% of the women said
the promiscuously inclined minority of men is larger they always enjoyed casual sex.
than the promiscuously inclined minority of women. Similar findings are reported by Laumann et al.
In our view, the difference in the way people count sex (1994, p. 239), with a rigorous sample and thorough
partners is itself an indication that men want more than data, although the numbers overall are much lower
women. Men prefer an estimation strategy because it than what was found with patrons of singles bars.
tends to yield a high tally; women prefer an enumeration Laumann et al. found that men were more likely than
strategy because it yields a low tally. Choosing strategies women to report having sex with someone they had
in that way enables men to come up with higher numbers just recently met. They suggested that these discrepan-
than women, even though the means should be the same. cies resemble the gender difference in number of sex
If our interpretation of motivated cognitive strate- partners and represent reporting biases caused by a
gies is correct, it should be reflected in how people double standard. It is, however, logically possible that
count marginal cases. Sanders and Reinisch (1999) more men than women have had sex with someone
provided relevant data on this. They asked a sample of they just met, because a few women might do this with

250
GENDER DIFFERENCE IN SEX DRIVE?

many different men. Still, the relevant conclusion is have a high number of sex partners than women. Table
that men are more willing than women to have sex with 2 summarizes the findings reviewed in this section.
a new acquaintance.
Converging evidence of desire for multiple partners
may be obtained by examining homosexual activity, Masturbation
because those patterns are set by only one gender.
Again, the evidence consistently finds that men desire Differential masturbation is a rather clear and un-
more partners than women. The subculture of gay men ambiguous prediction that follows from any hypothe-
did briefly establish bathhouses and other institutions sis about differential sex drive. Masturbation involves
that allowed men to have sex with half a dozen or more gratification of one's sexual desires in a way that is
partners in a single evening. Even though lesbians are only minimally encumbered by opportunity con-
better able than gay men to engage in such promiscuity straints, social pressures, and broader concerns. A per-
(because of the lack of refractory period), lesbian com- son with a higher sex drive will probably masturbate
munities do not seem to have created any market for more than a person with a lower sex drive.
such institutionalized orgiastic behavior. Gender differences in masturbation are large and
Converging evidence comes from studies of consistent. Women and girls are less likely to mastur-
extradyadic sex partners. The data from Blumstein and bate than men and boys (Arafat & Cotton, 1974;
Schwartz (1983) are probably most relevant, because Asayama, 1975; Laumann et al., 1994; Sigusch &
they were collected after the sexual revolution had re- Schmidt, 1973), and some evidence indicates that
moved most traditional barriers to such activity, and males who masturbate do it more frequently than fe-
AIDS and other dangers had not yet surfaced to create males (Laumann et al., 1994; Sigusch & Schmidt,
new barriers. Like Lawson (1988), they found that hus- 1973). Jones and Barlow (1990) found, for example,
bands were more likely than wives to have a high number that 45 % of men but only 15 % of women reported mas-
of partners outside their primary relationship-even after turbating at least once per week. Meanwhile, nearly
controlling for the greater incidence of male than female half the women in their sample (47%) but only 16% of
infidelity. In other words, men were both more likely than the men said they had never masturbated. Arafat and
women to stray at all, and straying men were more likely Cotton (1974) found women and girls were almost four
than straying women to have multiple partners. times more likely than men and boys to say they never
Evidence about infidelity in same-gender relation- masturbated (39% vs. 11%). In a survey of German
ships is even more dramatic. Blumstein and Schwartz teenagers ages 16 to 17, Sigusch and Schmidt (1973)
(1983) found that, among people in committed rela- found that 80% of the boys, but only 25% of the girls,
tionships, gay men were far more likely than lesbians were engaged in masturbation during the past year, and
to have sex with someone other than their regular part- boys averaged five times per month as opposed to once
ner (82% vs. 28%). Among those who did experience per month for the girls.
sex with someone other than the partner, lesbians Indeed, in a meta-analysis of gender differences in
tended to have only 1 outside partner (53%), unlike gay sexual behavior, Oliver and Hyde (1993) found that
men (7%). The proportion of gay men who reported masturbation was the largest difference of all the vari-
having had over 20 outside partners during the rela- ables they examined, with men nearly a full standard
tionship was substantial (43%), but among lesbians it deviation higher than women, averaged across 26 dif-
was negligible (1%). Even in the moderately promis- ferent findings. Even past the age of 60, men mastur-
cuous category of having had between 6 and 20 part- bate more than women (Bergstrom-Walan & Nielsen,
ners, gay men outnumbered lesbians (30% vs. 4%). 1990). The differences are found in both incidence and
Again we look to Bell and Weinberg (1978) for frequency. In other words, women are more likely than
converging evidence with a sample that was not re- men to report never masturbating at all, and women
stricted to people in committed relationships. In a sam- who do masturbate do so less frequently than men who
ple of several hundred respondents, far more gay masturbate (Laumann et al., 1994).
White men (43%) than White lesbians (0%) reported Is it safe to infer level of sex drive from rates of
having had over 500 sex partners. Meanwhile, 58% of masturbation? Some have proposed that society dis-
White lesbians, but only 3% of gay White men, said proportionately discourages girls from masturbating,
their lifetime homosexual experience had included 9 or so that the gender difference in masturbation may re-
fewer partners. (The difference among Black respon- flect socialization. For example, they claim that soci-
dents was smaller.) Thus, again, men predominate at ety does not teach girls to masturbate or approve of
the high levels of sexual activity, whereas women pre- their doing so. We find these arguments dubious. Soci-
dominate at the low levels. ety has certainly expressed strong and consistent dis-
Again, these data point toward the conclusion of greater approval of masturbation by boys, and if anything the
sex drive in men. Men do appear much more motivated to pressures have been more severe on boys than girls.

251
CIA

t0 t0o0
t-0 0 =
a~0

0 ~ ~ ~ ~ ~ ~ ~~~~~~~0~~
CO CO 00 0 -~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~ 0.~~~t

kn 0 0

0) 0 0 ~~~0 50) 0 00 .0

CO .0 M~~ C10 CM -
>0 0) 0) ~~~~~~~~ ~~5 ~~-00
>.o 0~~ Oo ~ ~ - OoOCO

0 ~ ~ ~ ~ .)-.~~~~~~
~ ~ ~ U U 0000 0)

0.
.) ) 0,. 0

CV) C.) C.U. ) C) C


v00 00 0 0 0 0 0

C.)~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~r
c0)

4 C~

0)
0- .0
ON)tl

04) 00
C S- .08
0 0 0- 0
C%
00 Z
0
CIC
113 Oo 0
0
0 C_) C)
-0
C 0
0 ct
.0

0
C) r_
0 .
0
0
CA
._

0 Eo 00 :3
O
.

"I o
Ct
0 - c CA
a< c: 0 30
0. 0 0t .0
t 0
CA 0 .E cA
0. t) 0
C0
- 0 O. C. Cl4
S:0
0 C [.Q r 0
0
-0UQ I- ct

0~
.0 00: 0

t 0) ct ct 0
0 04
I
0 C =0 + 0).
_ 0
0 C -0
OC u u
V U U U < < - -- ~c 04
0.
00 z 0: 00

Cl I
00
ClI
_- 00
In 00 ON 00

CC
0-
&e, e, o 8
11 I

0
00

V) r tCI cq CCONCC- 1
11
,c
,,
-

4 6 O= r-
-

11 Z>
ON
-!
sv
CIA N
rn
00

1
00 Cl - &( I' 0~ 6 6 ,q -
0l
N I N Ot 'I 0 CIA C Cl 00 - O
07 \0 - &,*j zC

ar .5

n
cll
N1E
CC~ o~~~~~
Ln S EJ0& E 0
CC C

5
.- 0t
0 - 0Y
0 X 0 a
0w5
c.0S :
S -.
0
_ *:

0t 0
00.
00 000^
.5 . 0L 0.
0. v 0:
00
0 . ._
0 -.
00 V S
0 C
0

5:5 .0O
0 0 >.
0-0
z oO
:
0
0-
.0o6
0 Cl
OC I- 0 0.
.0
-0
0 0 O
.0 0 Ot
Cl 0
:5C._V,
0
.. 0
c

ao OO
0000::
0: ~o0
0
- 0 0

0 D~
S:
0- 0

55
0
0 00 0
0 0
- *0- 0-c
0 0.
.0CC;
-5 . .0
- Q0
0 .0o0
EE
Q)
;0 0 0. O .0
. .
.. .. .

0
z

0 r. O
00u ON
.
0 C
cl
cr.
I_
ON t CI ct
V) I 0000
.0 .-
0
0 ON
0
U
00 V:
0

cr
a
CL 1r-1 CO-CO
CC cr x
. 0

ct -5C/O CO

*0sC3
CO
C.

u
Z: =0
CO
0.+ x
0
CO
CA.
0;
Q.
14-
0 0 0
.U a) r-
0

(1 C) CO
Wj 0 .4
o0 0 U. c
CTJ CO
4 -zt-t
0
Ct. z Con =
._
CO 0

t
0
5ur 0It~I-0 CO-
CO7
ao
._
z c- -9
-
u
0
C11
t 05 -
CO0, Cto Of0
I- 0C
3
0
a
C- 00 a0
0
z0
0
r-
-V 0
6t
w 0D
CZ 0.
5w cc 0
.T* e e S 8 8 - cc- z It)ZIC)~~~~,I &
- Cl IC - Cl Y
)

m 00I m CA Iz00 0
C o o. Cs 11
0 c5
ClIIzl$
I
k)
vCI z cc- 0 IC Cl 't -t
CO
0
0

~ I c
- n N cc- C
c-it c-i \
l)
20
Cl cc-m a,,, cl 0 C
v)C Cl -
Cl Cl cc
CO cc
Il
0 cc
0-
It m t z m z ~t (, OC C
t

CO

-3
-0 -0 CO
0, -t
0 c
0 0

00
C-
_ U=b V~cr0r k
v)
._

0 _ O
=~~=-f -= zr
~~ I l) 00-

V-, C-
0.. DN
5_0 0 0 0
2 -t 0_
" ._
3
C O
,_ ON
0
E- 11
0

0;
CO
0
3t 11
zz
CO m
V
0.
0_
*-occ-v
0 _0
0,+ 3
-m o
0
CZ
C0

nzV #-0
-5
SE
04 0
CZ
z
Q
-3
Ct
o

5
71
GENDER DIFFERENCE IN SEX DRIVE?

For example, the warnings about blindness and insan- that the differences between genders have the same ba-
ity (as putative consequences of masturbation) were sis as the differences within gender.
mainly directed at young males, not females. The data on masturbation thus appear to be clear and
As to the feminist suggestion that society fails to consistent, and the gender difference is large. Men mas-
teach girls to masturbate, literally that may be true, but turbate more frequently than women, and the reasons
we do not think that society teaches boys to masturbate appear to be linked to desire for sexual gratification.
either. Arafat and Cotton (1974) found that half the Hence women's lesser masturbation points toward the
girls and more than half the boys (who masturbated) conclusion that they have a milder sex drive.
said they discovered it themselves. An equal number of
boys and girls learned about masturbation from friends
(30.9% of boys, 29.3% of girls) and siblings (2.6% of Willingness to Forego Sex
boys, 3.9% of girls). The high rates of self-taught mas-
turbation suggest that it is not a technically recondite, Another straightforward prediction is that the per-
esoteric practice. Anyone who wants to masturbate can son with the stronger drive will be more reluctant to do
probably figure out how to do it. without. Deprivation of whatever one desires will be
Moreover, the view that society uses guilt to pre- more painful for the person with the stronger desires,
vent girls from masturbating is questionable. Although almost by definition. With regard to sex, this means
guilt is reported by a significant minority of both male that a relatively mild sex drive should render a person
and female masturbators (see also Laumann et al., more willing than others to do without sex.
1994), it does not appear to be a very effective deter- The Kinsey studies (Kinsey, Pomeroy, & Martin,
rent. Undoubtedly the greatest guilt would presumably 1948; Kinsey, Pomeroy, Martin, & Gebhard, 1953)
be experienced by Catholic priests and nuns, for whom noted a relevant gender difference. They obtained rela-
masturbation is a violation of their most sacred vows of tively thorough sexual histories from a broad sample of
chastity. Yet apparently most priests do engage in mas- individuals. One of their major concepts was "total
turbation (e.g., Sipe, 1995, reported extensive inter- sexual outlet," which referred to all sexual activity (of-
views with many priests; Murphy, 1992, reported ten operationalized as total number of orgasms) in the
similar conclusions from survey data). If the guilt is person's life, from all sources. They found that some
not enough to deter priests, it is probably not a major women showed substantial fluctuations in total outlet.
barrier for other people. Thus, a woman might enjoy a full and active sex life
The only other possible objection in terms of guilt for a period, then lose her partner and have no sexual
would be that men and women have an equal desire to activity at all for some time, and then resume active sex
masturbate but guilt weighs more heavily on women with a new partner. Kinsey et al. (1953) observed that
than men. This is directly contradicted, however, by such discontinuities were almost never found among
Arafat and Cotton's (1974) finding that more males men. More recently, Leiblum and Rosen (1988) con-
(13%) than females (10%) reported feeling guilty after firmed that in-depth histories indicated that many
masturbation. By the same token, more males than fe- women seem to adapt easily to a complete absence of
males said they regarded their masturbatory activities sexual activity during long periods of involuntary ab-
as perverse (5% vs. 1%). Thus, if anything, guilt stinence, unlike men.
weighs more heavily on men. The total outlet measure is quite relevant to the issue
If not guilt, then what is the major deterrent to mastur- of total sex drive, because it combines all behavior rele-
bation? According to Arafat and Cotton's (1974) data, the vant to the sexual motivation and avoids the potential
most common reason for not masturbating was a lack of confusion that could stem from substituting one kind of
desire, and that was reported by more of the sexual gratification for another. The fact that women
nonmasturbating females (76%) than the nonmasturbating were more willing than men to do without sexual activ-
males (56%). Apparently it is not lack of social encourage- ity altogether supports the view that women are less
ment, but lack of personal interest that explains the lesser strongly motivated to find some sexual gratification
incidence of masturbation among females. The lack of per- consistently across time. When men lose one source of
sonal interest points to an explanation in terms of differ- sexual gratification, such as by breaking up with a regu-
ences in sex drive. lar sex partner, they apparently seek out a new one soon,
The view that masturbation is directly linked to or at least they step up the frequency of masturbation.
strength of sex drive is supported by further evidence. There are other signs that women are more willing
Within either gender, masturbation is related to strength than men to go without sexual gratification. As already
of sex drive and interest in sex, so that men with higher reported, women are slower to want sex within rela-
sex drives masturbate more than men with weaker ones, tionships, and they take longer after puberty to com-
and the same holds true for women (Abramson, 1973). mence masturbation and other sexual activity.
It is therefore most plausible and parsimonious to infer Moreover, the masturbation discrepancy is quite rele-

255
BAUMEISTER, CATANESE, & VOHS

vant because it avoids the alternative explanation that be attributed to the earlier maturation, and it would be
women are willing to forego intercourse out of fear of necessary to examine the time lag between puberty and
pregnancy rather than lack of desire: Masturbation car- onset of sexual behavior before drawing conclusions.
ries no risks of calamity (indeed, warnings of dire con- As it happens, though, most evidence indicates that
sequences of masturbation have been more frequently boys commence sexual interest and activity earlier
aimed at boys than girls), and the main reason reported than girls. Women start having sex at a later age than
by women and girls for not masturbating is a lack of men (Asayama, 1975; Laumann et al., 1994; Lewis,
desire (Arafat & Cotton, 1974). 1973; Wilson, 1975). For example, Asayama's inter-
Reasons for not having sex were the explicit focus of views with Japanese students during the late 1940s and
a study by Leigh (1989). Among men, fear of rejection 1950s found that half the boys had become quite inter-
was the main reason given for avoiding sex. Women, ested in sex by age 15 and 90% had by age 19, whereas
however, reported that they avoided sex because a lack only 30% to 40% of the girls had become interested by
of interest and enjoyment. The significantly greater allu- age 18. Over a third of the boys had masturbated by age
sion by women than men to a lack of sexual interest and 15 and over 80% had done so by age 21, whereas by
enjoyment fits the view that women have a weaker sex age 21 only 12% of the women had masturbated.
drive, as Leigh (1989) herself acknowledged. Asayama concluded that the development of sexual in-
Another very instructive case concerns clerical vows terest "among females is rather slow while for males it
of celibacy. This is especially important because there is is quite rapid" (p. 95). With an American sample,
no question of separate values or double standard: Lewis (1973) found that half (52%) the boys but only
Among Catholic Christian clergy, both men and women 16% of the girls reported having sex by the age of 17.
take profoundly important and sacred vows to forego all Even though girls pass through puberty earlier than
sexual gratification throughout life. The single standard boys, they report experiencing sexual arousal later, and
of absolute purity is thus clear to both priests and nuns. in fact in multiple samples all the boys reported their
Yet the evidence suggests that nuns are far more suc- first experiences of arousal prior to the age of 13,
cessful than priests at achieving that ideal. whereas most girls reported their first experience after
Clerical celibacy among both genders was studied that age (Knoth et al., 1988). Girls start having sexual
by Murphy (1992) using a questionnaire survey and a fantasies later than boys (Leitenberg & Henning, 1995),
sample of several hundred. Her results suggest signifi- and they are slower even to develop interest in sex
cantly greater success at celibacy among female than (Asayama, 1975). Most studies find that boys begin
male Catholic clergy. More male clergy (62%) than fe- masturbating earlier than girls (Kinsey et al., 1953), al-
male clergy (49%) reported having been sexually ac- though recent data suggest that the discrepancy may be
tive since they took their vows of celibacy. Among the dwindling (Leitenberg, Detzer, & Srebnik, 1993; Smith,
sexually active, the men had had more partners than the Rosenthal, & Reichler, 1996). In a national survey, girls
women. Thus, 24% of the sexually active men, but reported a later onset of sexual activity than boys
hardly any of the sexually active women (3%) reported (Leigh, Morrison, Trocki, & Temple, 1994).
having had more than five partners since taking their A survey of nearly 2,000 seventh- and eighth-grade
vows. The men were more likely than the women to students found that the girls were less likely to have
emphasize the orgasm as the most important part of the had sex than the boys, and in fact the boys reported
experience (20% vs. 2%). The women were more about twice as many sexual experiences as the girls
likely than the men to have terminated the sexual rela- (De Gaston, Weed, & Jensen, 1996). The girls were
tionship (i.e., the women might just lapse once or also more likely than the boys to believe that sexual
briefly whereas many men would continue violating urges can be controlled, which suggests that the sub-
their vows). All these findings suggest that women find jective intensity of desire was greater among the boys.
it easier than men to live without sexual gratification. Thus, the gender difference in onset of sexual inter-
est and activity suggests greater sexual interest on the
part of boys. The earlier sexual maturation of girls is
Emergence of Sexual Desire consistent with the general pattern of greater sexual ca-
pacity among girls. These differences underscore the
If all else were equal, one would expect the person need to maintain a careful conceptual distinction be-
with the higher sex drive to begin sexual activity earlier tween sex drive and sexual capacity. Taken together,
than the person with the lesser drive. Unfortunately for they point all the more strongly to the conclusion of
the purposes of comparison, all else is not equal, and greater sex drive in males. Thus, sexual interest ap-
there appear to be significant gender differences in sex- pears very soon after puberty for males, whereas sex-
ual readiness: Girls pass through puberty and achieve ual interest is relatively slow to awaken in females.
sexual maturity earlier than boys. Hence, if girls com- It must be acknowledged that emergence of sexual
menced all sexual activities earlier than boys, this could desire could be affected by a variety of factors. Be-

256
GENDER DIFFERENCE IN SEX DRIVE?

cause an erect penis is more obvious than a lubricated Refusal rates outside of relationships do differ by gen-
vagina, males may find it easier to recognize their own der. Probably the best data were provided by Clark and
sexual arousal, and this could contribute to earlier Hatfield (1989), who used an experimental procedure to
commencement of sexual activity. Hence, we do not investigate responses to sexual offers. Both men and
regard the findings on emergence of sexual desire to be women were approached by a moderately attractive, op-
strong or conclusive, although they do provide some posite-sex confederate and invited to have sexual inter-
convergence with other patterns. course that evening. Women's refusal rate was 100%
across two studies, whereas only 25% of the men refused.
A different approach was taken by LaPlante,
Seeking Versus Avoiding, Initiating McCormick, and Brannigan (1980). They developed a
Versus Refusing list of 19 strategies for obtaining or avoiding sex, and
then they surveyed respondents of both genders as to
Spontaneous initiation of goal-directed behavior is whether each strategy was more typical of men or
widely accepted as an indication of the strength of any women. Both genders said all 10 of the strategies for
motivation. Therefore, all else being equal, one would obtaining sex were more typical of men than women.
predict that a person with a stronger sex drive would Conversely, both genders said that all 9 strategies for
initiate more sexual activity than a person with a avoiding sex were more typical of women than men.
weaker sex drive. To be sure, differences in initiative Clearly, these participants recognized initiating sex as
are not unambiguous, but there is some justification for typical of men and refusing sex as typical of women.
interpreting them as indications of motivation. These differences may be questioned on the basis of
Hurlbert (1991) found that women with higher levels social prescriptions. It might be argued that society has
of sexual desire were more likely to initiate sex, and in- assigned men the role of initiating sex and women the
deed sexual assertiveness was correlated with a num- role of refusing it. That argument raises a substantial
ber of measures indicating high sex drive. For this chicken-and-egg problem: Did society originally pre-
reason the prediction that stronger motivation would scribe initiative to men for arbitrary reasons, or did the
lead to more initiative is hard to dismiss, and it is there- greater male initiative cause society to incorporate sexual
fore worth examining the evidence. If nothing else, a initiative into the male role? Our own suspicion is that the
lack of differential initiative would speak against a hy- causal arrow points both ways, so that cultural influence
pothesis of differential drive. tends to reify and intensify the preexisting differences.
Women initiate sex less often than men. A diary
study by O'Sullivan and Byers (1992) found that men
initiated sex about twice as often as women, although Liking for Various Sexual Practices
there was no significant difference in considering initi-
ating sex. M. Brown and Auerback (1981) found that A person with a higher sex drive would presumably
men initiated it three times as often as women during the be drawn to engage in a broader variety of sexual prac-
1st year of marriage, although the difference dwindled tices than a person with a weaker drive, although one
in later years. Byers and Heinlein (1989) found that over could certainly imagine cases in which individuals
a 1-week period, men initiated sex about twice as often have very intense but narrowly focused sex drives.
as women. Differences in sexual initiative may help ex- Thus, the number of sexual practices that people find
plain the differential rates of sex in gay male versus les- appealing can be used as another potential source of
bian relationships (Blumstein & Schwartz, 1983). converging evidence.
Refusal rates and patterns do not show a consistent Fewer sexual practices appeal to women than men.
gender difference, and some studies have concluded Laumann et al. (1994) offered their respondents a list
that men and women refuse about equally (see of 14 sexual practices and asked whether they found
Blumstein & Schwartz, 1983). When a difference does each of them appealing. They reported only percent-
emerge, however, it indicates that women are more ages, not significance tests, but these were extremely
likely to refuse. M. Brown and Auerback (1981) found consistent: On 13 of the 14 practices, a higher percent-
that wives refused sex more often than husbands, and age of men than women rated the activity as appealing,
indeed seven times as many husbands as wives were and the 14th showed no difference ("being forced by a
able to claim that they had never turned down their sex partner" was rated as appealing by less than 1 % of
spouse's request for sex. Thus it appears that there is both men and women). The index summarizing the
some inconsistency as to whether there is any differ- number of appealing practices yielded, not surpris-
ence in refusal rates once a couple has begun having ingly, an overall significant finding that men liked
sex. (As already noted, however, many relationships more activities than women.
do pass through a phase in which the man desires sex Although the list used by Laumann et al. (1994) con-
whereas the woman refuses; McCabe, 1987.) tained practices that are favored by people in the

257
BAUMEISTER, CATANESE, & VOHS

so-called normal range of sexual activity, such as watch- Some might object that most pornography is more
ing a partner undress, receiving oral sex, or stimulating geared toward men than women. There are two reasons
the anus with a finger, a similar conclusion emerges from to dismiss this objection, however. First, several studies
examination of unusual or deviant variations of sexuality. have found that women do have strong physiological re-
Nearly all the paraphilias appeal to men more than sponses to pornography and experience levels of sexual
women (American Psychiatric Association, 1994). arousal that compare with men's (e.g., Fisher & Byrne,
Even though a majority of married couples today 1978; also Heiman, 1977). It is thus clear that currently
practice both fellatio and cunnilingus, women find available pornography is amply arousing to women.
these activities (especially fellatio) less appealing than Once they see it, they like it and find it stimulating-but
men. For example, Laumann et al. (1994) found that women are simply not sufficiently motivated to seek out
45% of men but only 29% of women said receiving that kind of stimulation as often as men.
oral sex was very appealing, and a similar discrepancy The other reason to dismiss the argument of gender
was found for giving oral sex (34% of men, 17% of bias in the sex industry is that if the market existed for a
women). This difference caused the researchers to special, female-targeted pornography, it is highly
speculate that some women perform such acts more likely that someone would have been willing and eager
out of a sense of obligation than genuine desire to make the millions of dollars that it would represent.
(Laumann et al., 1994, p. 157). In actual fact, the sex industry has tried repeatedly to
The greater appeal of multiple practices to men than reach out to women, but it has repeatedly failed
women is not confined to the modem United States. A (Abramson & Pinkerton, 1995). Playgirl was intro-
study of elderly people in Sweden, which is supposedly duced to the market with considerable hoopla in the
a very egalitarian and sexually liberated culture, found 1970s, but the appeal of seeing nude men did not sus-
that men liked the full span of sexual activities more tain enough sales to make it successful (let alone even
than women (Bergstrbm-Walan & Nielsen, 1990). approaching the success of Playboy), and so it shifted
away from male nudity as a major selling point. Viva,
which alone among the female-targeted magazines
Sacrificing Resources to Get Sex featured pictures of male genitals, closed down after 3
years. The market was simply not there-unlike the
Another test of sex drive is what the person is will- male market for pictures of nude women, which has
ing to sacrifice to gain sexual gratification. As with any sustained an assortment of magazines for decades.
motivation, people with a higher drive should be will- Another instructive category is the purchase of sexual
ing to make more material and pragmatic sacrifices aids and devices: There too one expends money to obtain
than people with a low or weak drive. With gender dif- sexual pleasure. Both men and women find such pur-
ferences, one must keep in mind that men have gener- chases embarrassing, but men are more willing to swal-
ally had more money and other resources than women, low their pride and make such a purchase (Laumann et
so men are capable of expending more. Still, the gap al., 1994). This is true even though what is available for
has narrowed substantially in recent years, and cer- women (vibrators) seems superior to anything available
tainly the cost of some sexual products is not large to men (see C. M. Davis, Blank, Lin, & Bonillas, 1996).
enough to make them unattainable for most people. The vibrator is thus a further counterargument to the view
In any case, it is clear that men spend a great deal that the sex industry is only aimed at men.
more money on sexual products than women. Men have Extramarital sex constitutes a risk factor for divorce
paid women for sex throughout most of history and (e.g., Blumstein & Schwartz, 1983), and it seems rea-
across many different cultures, but the pattern of women sonable to assume that most adulterers recognize that
paying men for sex has been considerably less common they are putting their marriage at risk. As already noted,
and in many contexts nonexistent (e.g., Elias, Bullough, research has consistently found that men are more will-
Elias, & Brewer, 1998). Even in societies where there ing than women to have extramarital sex, which sug-
have certainly been enough rich women to be able to gests that they are more willing to risk their marriage. In
pay for sex, the practice has been rare or nonexistent. fact, the difference may be even larger than it appears,
The same is true for pornography. Men spend con- because there is some evidence that women only have
siderably more money on pornography and erotica extramarital affairs when they are dissatisfied with their
than women do, as all studies have shown (e.g., marriage (e.g., Lawson, 1988)-in which case they are
Laumann et al., 1994). To be sure, pornography is only not risking something they value as highly. The most
one kind of sexual stimulus. A lack of interest in por- relevant measure is whether people will put a happy,
nography does not alone signify a lack of interest in solid marriage at risk to have extramarital sex, and men
sex. Women's magazines in recent years do offer in- seem far more willing to do that.
formation about sex, presumably reflecting a market- One could examine the expenditure of other re-
place in which women will pay for such information. sources for the sake of sex. We do not know of system-

258
GENDER DIFFERENCE IN SEX DRIVE?

atic studies, but we predict that the same conclusion ies find that males are more opposed to homosexuality
would emerge. Jeopardizing one' s career for the sake of than females, although a meta-analysis (Oliver &
a sexual indulgence would be one relevant measure. To Hyde, 1993) concluded that no systematic difference
our knowledge, female politicians and leaders have not existed. Some findings of lesser male tolerance may
had their careers endangered by sex scandals almost at have been driven partly by the fact that the term homo-
all, whereas such scandals have damaged and even sexuality connotes male homosexuality to many peo-
ended careers by male politicians. It is conceivable that ple. Whitley (1988) found that both men and women
an anti-male bias in the media leads to greater exposure were more permissive toward homosexuality of the
of men than women, but we think that the American other gender but more opposed to their own gender's
press is sufficiently free and competitive that it would be homosexuality (see also Herek & Capitanio, 1999). Of
eager to publicize sexual misdeeds by eminent, power- course, these data mainly reflect the attitudes of hetero-
ful women if it could find evidence of them. sexuals, because gay men and lesbians are certainly
Surely the relative shortage of female leaders and much more favorable and permissive in their attitudes
politicians creates a serious base-rate confound, and so toward same-sex activity. That discrepancy, however,
the greater number of male-centered scandals could be makes attitudes toward homosexuality seem irrelevant
an artifact of the greater number of male leaders. As to the question of differences in sex drive: There is no
women increasingly move into political leadership motivational reason to have a favorable attitude toward
roles, time will tell whether they are as willing as men to an activity that falls outside one's own sexual interests.
jeopardize their careers for the sake of short-term sexual In other words, men' s opposition to male homosexual-
gratifications. Given the weight of other evidence re- ity is not inconsistent with men having a high drive to-
viewed here, we predict that they will not, on average. ward heterosexual sex.1
Differences in attitudes toward pornography, pros-
titution, extramarital sex, and other sexual activities fit
Favorable Attitudes Toward Sex the same pattern of greater favorability in male than fe-
male attitudes. We think that multiple factors may in-
Attitudes and motivations are generally understood trude into and confound such attitudes, so we do not
to be linked. Thus, having a motivated interest in some regard them as conclusive signs of greater male sex
outcome generally produces more favorable attitudes drive. Women might oppose prostitution, for example,
toward it, as well as strengthening the attitude-behav- out of fear that their boyfriends and husbands will con-
ior link (Crano, 1995; Sivacek & Crano, 1982). The tract diseases from them and infect the women. Then
link should be especially strong with regard to sex, in again, the men run the more direct risk of infection
which attitudes range from the restrictive and disap- from prostitutes, and so one might have predicted from
proving ones that oppose many forms of sexual activ- a medical risk standpoint that men would be more op-
ity to the opposite of highly permissive, liberal posed to prostitution. In any case, these differences in
attitudes that permit and even encourage sexual activ- permissiveness are consistent with the view that males
ity. The person with the higher sex drive would be mo- have a stronger sex drive, but the ambiguities and alter-
tivated to espouse more favorable attitudes toward sex. native explanations render them unable to support
Findings are consistent across a broad range of sex- clear conclusions by themselves.
ual attitudes: Women have less permissive attitudes to- A highly specific and relevant set of attitudes con-
ward sex than men. Although they are equal on some cerns liking for sex organs. One would predict that a
things, generally women are more critical of promiscu- person with a high sex drive would have more favor-
ity, premarital sex, extramarital sex, and various other able attitudes toward sex organs than a person with
sexual activities (Laumann et al., 1994; Oliver & lower sex drive.
Hyde, 1993; Sprecher, 1989; Wilson, 1975). Some of Attitudes toward sex organs were assessed by
these attitudes, most notably favoring casual sex, pro- Reinholtz and Muehlenhard (1995). They found that
duce gender differences that meet the statistical criteria men held more favorable opinions of their own sex or-
to be called large differences (Oliver & Hyde, 1993). gans (i.e., their penises) than women held toward theirs
Casual sex is conceptually important because it repre- (i.e., their vaginas). One might object that these judg-
sents the opportunity to obtain sexual gratification ments are confounded by the physical nature of the or-
without a high degree of effort, commitment, or invest-
ment, and therefore people with a high desire for sex-
ual gratification would be expected to be most There is also evidence that men who most strongly oppose homo-
favorable toward such opportunities. Apparently, most sexuality are also aroused by it, suggesting that opposition to homo-
sexuality may be a kind of reaction formation designed to avoid ac-
of those people are men. knowledging one's own desires (see Adams, Wright, & Lohr, 1996).
The main exception to the greater permissiveness of This line of reasoning would support the view that men have stronger
males is in attitudes toward homosexuality. Some stud- desires hence the stronger opposition.

259
BAUMEISTER, CATANESE, & VOHS

gans themselves, such as if the penis were inherently Other therapy studies have confirmed that lack of li-
more lovable than the vagina, but this interpretation is bido is more common among women than men. Hawton
contradicted by additional findings: Men rated their and Catalan (1986) tallied 154 cases presenting for sex
girlfriends' vaginas more favorably than the women therapy. The problem of "impaired sexual interest" was
rated their boyfriends' penises. Thus, men rated both the most common problem (58%) among female pa-
their own and their partners' sex organs more favor- tients but the least common problem (4%) among male
ably than women rated them. patients. Rosen and Leiblum (1989, p. 21) confirmed
that the diagnosis of inhibited desire is more commonly
given to women than men. Leiblum and Rosen (1988)
Prevalence of Low Sexual Desire reported elsewhere that their clinical observations sup-
ported the conclusion that "men have a more insistent
An examination of sexual dysfunctions and their and constant sexual appetite" (p. 13) than women. Clini-
consequences within interpersonal relationships also cal observations by Kaplan (1979) likewise led to the
supports the hypothesis that men have stronger sex conclusion that sexual desire is more consistently strong
drives than women. We have suggested that the optimal and less easily stifled in men than in women.
strength of sex drive would be intermediate, in the sense A study of sexual dysfunctions in Denmark
of being neither too high nor too low. If women are on (Ventegodt, 1998) confirmed the patterns of problematic
average toward the lower end of that intermediate range, sexual desire found in North American studies. Among
and if both genders are normally distributed around their women, one of the most frequently reported sexual prob-
respective means, then women should be more vulnera- lems (11% vs. 3% of men) was decreased sexual desire,
ble than men to pathological or problematic patterns of whereas among men the lack of a suitable and willing sex
very low (inadequate) sexual desire. Furthermore, if one partner was the most common complaint.
assumes that both men and women will experience peri- Clinical cases of low or absent sexual desire suggest
ods of low sexual desire in life, these should be more biological factors may be relevant. A study comparing
problematic to the degree that one's partner wants and 15 women with lifelong absence of sex drive versus a
expects more sexual activity. If men have stronger sex control group of women with normal sex drive re-
drives, then they are more likely than women to be dis- vealed that the only endocrine measure to distinguish
traught when their partner loses interest in sex, and so between the groups was bloodstream levels of free tes-
marital conflict (leading to presenting for therapy) tosterone, which were significantly lower in the patient
should be more likely to focus on lack of sexual interest sample (Riley & Riley, 2000).
in the wife than in the husband. In other words, cases in Meanwhile, an attempt to assess the frequency of
which one person does not want to have sex should be various sexual dysfunctions in normal (nontherapy)
more distressing to the partner who has a high sex drive, couples likewise found that reports of lack of sexual
as opposed to a low one. desire or sexual interest were more common among
Hypoactive sexual desire disorder (HSDD) is offi- women (35%) than men (16%; Frank, Anderson, &
cially defined by the American Psychiatric Associa- Rubinstein, 1978). This finding helps rule out the po-
tion (1994) as constantly low or absent desire for tential confound that the difference in therapeutic pre-
sexual activity or sexual fantasies, a condition that is sentation is due to the greater willingness of women to
distressing to the person and is not caused by a medical come forward rather than the greater prevalence of
or substance abuse disorder. A recent review of hypoactive sexuality. In other words, the same pattern
hypoactive sexual disorder (Beck, 1995) highlighted is found both in therapy clinics and outside of them:
differences in the prevalence of HSDD diagnoses as a More couples struggle with low sexual desire in the
function of gender. Significantly more women than woman than in the man.
men are diagnosed with HSDD, consistent with the Similarly, a study of the frequency of disagree-
view that women are more vulnerable to problems of ments caused by one partner being reluctant to engage
low sexual desire. Beck (1995) noted that low sexual in sexual activity revealed consensus among both male
motivation is among the most common complaints in and female participants that female sexual reluctance
sex therapy. A study of over 900 clients who were be- was a far more common source of disagreement than
ing seen for a variety of sexual dysfunctions confirmed male reluctance (O'Sullivan & Byers, 1995). One
the frequency of the complaint, with 65% of all clients study (Byers & Lewis, 1988) found that at least once a
being diagnosed with HSDD. More germane to this month 47% of heterosexual couples disagree with their
analysis, 81% of those diagnosed with HSDD were partner about sex. In this study, it was always the case
women (475 women out of 588). Thus, women ap- that the man wanted to engage in a particular sexual be-
peared to be more vulnerable than men to the problem havior and his partner did not. Paired with the data on
of low sexual desire by a rate of about four to one hypoactive sexual desire, these studies suggests not
(Segraves & Segraves, 1991). only that women are more likely to be the reluctant

260
GENDER DIFFERENCE IN SEX DRIVE?

partner but that there is conflict caused by the discrep- been reported by MacPhee, Johnson, and van der Veer
ancy between men's and women' s desired level of sex- (1995). These findings suggest that low sexual desire in
ual activity. Perhaps when the man is the reluctant the woman becomes a problem at the level of the couple.
partner, the woman does not become upset by the pros- Low sexual desire in the man has not received commen-
pect of less sexual activity. surate attention as a cause of couple adjustment prob-
A study on marital adjustment and sexual desire lems, most likely either because it is a less common
(Trudel, Landry, & Larose, 1997) also found that the occurrence or is less troublesome to the partner.
woman was significantly more likely than the man to In sum, women are more likely than men to report a
report low sexual desire. Moreover, this study revealed serious or pathological lack of sexual desire, and cou-
that low sexual motivation was related to marital ad- ples have more conflicts and problems because of a fe-
justment problems. Beck (1995) also reported that male than a male lack of sexual desire. If these findings
hyposexuality is strongly related to poor dyadic adjust- were isolated, they might be interpreted to mean that
ment (e.g., Stuart, Hammond, & Pett, 1987; Trudel, female sexuality is more vulnerable (than male sexual-
Boulos, & Matte, 1993). A study of sexuality and rela- ity) to being interrupted by stress or other situational
tionship indexes found that sexual desire discrepancies factors. They are however consistent with the view that
(the difference between each partner's level of sexual women have on average less sexual desire to start with,
desire) predicted overall relationship satisfaction and so more women than men will fall into the spec-
(Davies, Katz, & Jackson, 1999). A discrepancy in trum of very low sex drive-indeed too low, as defined
which the woman has lower sexual desire than the man either by themselves or their partners.
appeared to be especially problematic, with these
women reporting lower relationship and sexual satis-
faction than women who matched or exceeded their Self-Rated Sex Drive
partner's level of sexual desire.
Buss (1989) hypothesized that differences in male Last, we turn to the direct reports and classifications
and female sexual strategies would lead to specific types about sex drive. Mercer and Kohn (1979) included
of conflict between a sexually active heterosexual cou- items asking people to rate the strength of their sex
ple. He proposed and found that women become upset drive. Women rated their sexual urges as less strong
about men's strategy of sexual assertiveness (e.g., want- than men rated men's. Although one may question
ing sex sooner after meeting, wanting more frequent whether people have an accurate basis for comparing
sex, being more persistent to have sex, and wanting their own feelings against those of others, the results
more partners than the woman), whereas men become do point toward stronger sex drives in men. In studies
upset about women's sexual strategy of sexual restraint of sexual desire among healthy people, men report
(e.g., withholding sex, having lower desire for sex, and higher levels of sexual interest than women, regardless
needing certain conditions to be met before engaging in of age. For instance, Beck et al. (1991) found this pat-
sexual activity). Buss (1989) showed that not only do tern among college students, Pfeiffer, Verwoerdt, and
these problems arise between men and women on a gen- Davis (1972) found this pattern among middle-aged
eral level, they also operate within couples to predict men and women, and Bretschneider and McCoy
both marital and sexual satisfaction. (1988) found gender differences in sexual desire in
A nonclinical sample of never married college stu- people ages 80 to 102. A study (Mehrabian &
dents by Carroll, Volk, and Hyde (1985) provided con- Stanton-Mohr, 1985) on emotions, sexual desire, and
verging evidence. Far more women (19%) than men gender found uniformly greater sexual motivation
(2%) claimed that they never felt that they wanted or among males than females across all emotional states.
needed sex. Moreover, even when sexual tension or We have also already mentioned Leigh's (1989) study,
desire was felt, it was apparently less intense among which examined reasons for having and for not having sex.
the women. Most of the men (80%) but only 25% of Women were more likely to cite lack of interest and enjoy-
the women said they preferred to have sex as a way of ment as a reason for not having sex (Leigh, 1989).
releasing sexual tension when they did feel it. Instead, Adolescent sexual patterns were studied by
women said they preferred to engage in vigorous phys- Buzwell and Rosenthal (1996). They classified their
ical activity (50%) or even just watch television (20%). sample of high school students into five categories,
Therapies for sexual desire problems have focused on ranked in order of increasing sex drive and experience:
the difference between men's and women's sexual moti- sexually naive, sexually unassured, sexually compe-
vation. Hurlbert and his colleagues (Hurlbert, White, tent, sexually adventurous, and sexually driven. The
Powell, & Apt, 1993) studied couples in which the lowest category (i.e., sexually naive) was defined by
woman has hypoactive sexual desire and found evidence showing minimal or no sexual activity. Girls outnum-
that including couples in sex therapy treatment is more bered boys three to one in this category. In contrast, the
effective than a women-only format. Similar results have second highest category of sexually adventurous peo-

261
BAUMEISTER, CATANESE, & VOHS

ple was defined by being highly comfortable with sex, men' s anyhow. Women clearly have a greater overall
highly confident about it, and highly active in explor- capacity for sex than men.
ing sex. It was populated mainly (85%) by boys. The Enjoyment is considerably more difficult to assess
extreme high category of sexually driven individuals than either drive or capacity. Clearly there is consider-
was defined by seeming to be obsessed with sex, and it able variation within a single person in terms of how
was composed almost entirely (97%) of boys. In sum, much enjoyment one gets from sex, and these individ-
girls clustered at the low end of sexual desire and activ- ual fluctuations are likely to be more substantial and
ity, whereas boys predominated at the high end. meaningful than any difference between the genders
Converging evidence from another culture was pro- that is based on very broad averages. Moreover, there
vided by Useche, Villegas, and Alzate (1990), who is no reason to assume that the within-person variation
surveyed high school students in Colombia. The young is the same across gender, and indeed Baumeister
men reported more intense and more frequent sexual (2000) concluded that women exhibit significantly
desire than the young women. higher within-person variance in many sexual vari-
Parallel results were found with homosexuals by ables. We think it quite likely that women's enjoyment
Bell and Weinberg (1978), although when data were of sex varies more than men's, so that women's maxi-
tabulated by race there were some racial differences mum enjoyment may exceed that of men, even if on
and the small number of Black respondents did not average men enjoy it more than women.
show clear gender differences. Among White respon- Orgasm may be taken as one measure of sexual en-
dents, however, gay males were more likely than lesbi- joyment, but it is admittedly crude and incomplete, and
ans (37% vs. 24%) to be found in the maximum certainly many people report enjoying sex without or-
category of high sexual interest. Meanwhile, lesbians gasm. Even if one does use it as an index of enjoyment,
were more likely than gay males (42% vs. 22%) to be however, the results are mixed. Women are more likely
found in the low sexual interest category. than men to experience multiple orgasms during a single
In the preceding section, we cited evidence that copulation, and women are also more likely than men to
couples seeking therapy often conform to the pattern experience no orgasm during a single copulation (e.g.,
that the woman is less interested in sex than the man is Laumann et al., 1994). Thus, women's capacity for sex-
(and the reverse is relatively rare). Converging evi- ual enjoyment may have a higher peak than men's, but
dence from normal middle-aged adults with partners, across all sexual acts women's average level of sexual
Johannes and Avis (1997) found that men had higher enjoyment may be lower than men' s. Put more simply,
levels of sexual desire than women. These findings women can occasionally enjoy sex more than men can,
also indicate that self-reported sex drive is consistently whereas men actually do enjoy sex more consistently
higher in men than women. than women do. Also, men probably experience more
orgasms than women over a lifetime, especially if mas-
turbation is included in the tally. Given the current state
of knowledge, we regard the question of which gender
Evidence: Other Constructs enjoys sex more as unanswerable.
A third concept is extrinsic motivation. That is, moti-
Our focus has been on the strength of the sex drive, vations can be distinguished into intrinsic and extrinsic
which we defined as intrinsic motivation to engage in (Deci, 1971; Lepper, Greene, & Nisbett, 1973; Lepper
sex. To avoid overgeneralization of our findings, we & Greene, 1978). Intrinsic motivation is defined as de-
briefly consider several other constructs (sexual capac- siring to perform an activity for the sake of the direct sat-
ity, enjoyment, and extrinsic motivation) that might isfaction of performing the activity itself. In contrast,
seem to be related to the sex drive but that will not nec- extrinsic motivation involves wanting to perform an ac-
essarily yield similar conclusions. tivity for the sake of some consequence that it may
The first of these is sexual capacity. By virtue of the bring. In other words, with extrinsic motivation the ac-
very biological structure of the sex organs, women tivity is a means to an end, whereas with an intrinsic mo-
have superior capacity to men. Women can copulate tivation the activity is an end in itself.
with more consecutive partners than men, can copulate Applied to sex, the extrinsic motivation concept en-
for a longer period of time, and can achieve more or- tails that people may desire to engage in sex, not for the
gasms during a single session than men can. We can sake of enjoying the sexual activity itself, but because
think of no aspect of sex in which men's capacity for it serves as a means toward a desired end. Although ex-
sexual performance matches or exceeds women' s, trinsic motivations do not attest to the existence of in
other than the reliable incidence of orgasm. Orgasm is, intrinsic drive and hence are not directly relevant to the
however, arguably an index of pleasure rather than question of drive strength, they can be quite powerful
sexual performance per se, and indeed the potential or- and effective at causing behavior. Extrinsic motivation
gasmic capacity of women undoubtedly exceeds may be especially relevant to female sexual motivation

262
GENDER DIFFERENCE IN SEX DRIVE?

because of the plasticity of the female sex drive. Based their own love was the second most important reason.
on a broad literature review, Baumeister (2000) argued Julien et al. (1992) found that husbands and wives
that the female sex drive is more responsive than the agreed that sexual initiatives by husbands tended to
male drive to situation and cultural influences, and a stem from an internal (thus intrinsic) need, whereas the
greater susceptibility to extrinsic motivators would be wives' initiatives were perceived as motivated by love.
a plausible extension of that pattern. Thus, both genders perceive husbands as (intrinsically)
Having a child would be one extrinsic goal of sex. It is motivated to have sex for its own sake, whereas women
plausible that the desire for a child sometimes motivates are seen as desiring sex as a means to obtain love.
women to desire sex. Janus and Janus (1993) found that One might also consider money as a relevant extrin-
men were slightly more eager than women to report sic motivation, especially insofar as it has been featured
wanting to have a baby regardless of whether they mar- in research on extrinsic motivation from the very earli-
ried or remained single, and also more likely than women est studies (see Deci, 1971). Prostitutes obtain money by
to agree that parenthood is the highest human attainment. means of engaging in sexual activity, and the desire for
Still, these findings do not rule out the hypothesis that money can produce relatively high levels of sexual mo-
women actually engage in sex for the extrinsic goal of tivation. It is difficult to draw firm conclusions about
having a baby more often (or more eagerly) than men. gender differences in financial motivations for sex,
Another extrinsic motivation would be to create or however, because the opportunities for men to obtain
maintain a long-term relationship. It is a familiar obser- money from sex are much more limited than for women,
vation that women require a relationship context for and conversely, in many societies men have had far
sexual activity more than men do. We have already pre- more diverse ways to make money (apart from prostitu-
sented findings that men are more favorable than tion) than women. Still, we concede the greater partici-
women toward engaging in casual sex, defined as sex pation in prostitution by women than men could be
without a relationship context (Oliver & Hyde, 1993). interpreted as de facto evidence of higher extrinsic moti-
Herold and Mewhinney (1993) found that male patrons vation for sexual activity.
of a singles bar were more favorable than female patrons In conclusion, in this section we have considered
toward casual sex. Carroll, Volk, and Hyde (1985) several constructs that might seem part of the sex drive
found that women reported substantially lower desire but that deserve to be distinguished conceptually. Our
than men for sexual intercourse in the absence of emo- conclusions about gender differences in strength of sex
tional intimacy. Regan and Berscheid (1996) found that drive should not be generalized to these other con-
more women than men (35% vs. 13%) described love structs. First, sexual capacity is not the same as sex
and emotional intimacy as important goals of sexual de- drive, and women may well have a greater sexual ca-
sire, whereas men were more likely than women (70% pacity than men, in the sense of being able to copulate
vs. 43%) to say that the sexual activity itself was the with more partners or for a longer period of time with-
goal of sexual desire. These results are consistent with out interruption and in the sense of being able to have
the view that men are more intrinsically and women more orgasms on any given occasion. Second, sexual
more extrinsically motivated in sex: Male desire aims at enjoyment is not the same as sex drive. Although pre-
the sexual activity itself, whereas female desire aims be- cise comparisons seem difficult if not impossible, we
yond it toward other outcomes and consequences. speculate based on limited evidence that women's en-
Extramarital sex is arguably an especially good in- joyment of sex is more variable than men' s, with prob-
dex of the desire for sex without a relationship, because ably a lower mean enjoyment but a greater maximum
the person already has a relationship (by definition), enjoyment. Third, the sex drive refers to intrinsic moti-
and the extramarital activity can involve either a tem- vation, but extrinsic motivation can also be quite pow-
porary fling or an emotionally intense relationship. erful, and the two are distinct. It seems quite plausible
Spanier and Margolis (1983) surveyed people who had that women surpass men in extrinsically motivated
engaged in extramarital sex about their most recent ex- sex, such as in engaging in sex to obtain money or con-
perience. Unfaithful men far outnumbered women in struct a relationship, but ambiguities in the evidence
the category of one night stands (29% vs. 5%), whereas make a firm conclusion impossible at present.
unfaithful women outnumbered men in the category of
long-term love relationships (41% vs. 1 1%).
The use of sex to obtain love is related to the motive Discussion
to maintain a relationship. The most common reason
that women reported for initiating sexual activity was to We have surveyed a broad range of available evi-
receive love and intimacy (M. Brown & Auerback, dence on the relative strength of sex drive, defined in
1981). For men, the most common reason was to obtain terms of sexual motivation. The evidence was exten-
a release of sexual tension, which suggests an intrinsic sive, methodologically diverse, and consistent. By all
motivation. Both genders said that the wish to express measures, men have a stronger sex drive than women.

263
BAUMEISTER, CATANESE, & VOHS

Men think about sex more often, experience more fre- Third, men generally rate their sexual desires as stron-
quent sexual arousal, have more frequent and varied ger than women rate women's desires.
fantasies, desire sex more often, desire more partners, Hence we conclude that the data indicate gender
masturbate more, want sex sooner, are less able or will- differences in both frequency and intensity of sexual
ing to live without sexual gratification, initiate more and desires, although we reiterate that the data are more
refuse less sex, expend more resources and make more conclusive with regard to frequency. Either frequency
sacrifices for sex, desire and enjoy a broader variety of or intensity alone would be sufficient to conclude that a
sexual practices, have more favorable and permissive gender difference in sex drive exists, however, and so
attitudes toward most sexual activities, have fewer com- the broad conclusion is not in doubt.
plaints about low sex drive in themselves (but more In general, then, the weight of evidence points
about their partners), and rate their sex drives as stronger strongly and unmistakably toward the conclusion that
than women. There were no measures that showed the male sex drive is stronger than the female. It is clear
women having stronger drives than men. that men experience more frequent sexual desires, and
At the outset, we noted that the concept of sex drive it seems apparent (though less certain) that men experi-
encompasses both frequency and intensity of sexual ence more intense sexual desires. Any conception of
desires. The bulk of the evidence we have reviewed re- sexual desire or drive that does not recognize fre-
fers to frequency: Men clearly have more frequent sex- quency of feelings of desire, range and frequency of
ual desires and desire more frequent sex. The evidence fantasy, preferred number of partners, ease of arousal,
on intensity is less conclusive, however, although part preferred frequency of sex, time and money invested,
of that may simply reflect the greater difficulty of mea- masturbation, desire for variety of activities, and the
suring intensity than frequency. Still, if one concedes rest of the variables covered previously would have to
that men have more frequent sexual desires, is there be so peculiarly constructed as to be almost incoherent.
any clear evidence of a difference in intensity? We do not wish this conclusion to be misunderstood
Many sexual behaviors are measured two ways, fre- or to be misused for ideological purposes. The differ-
quency and incidence. Frequency refers to how often ence in sex drive does not mean that women do not en-
the person engages in the activity, whereas incidence joy sex, nor does it mean that women do not desire sex.
refers to the proportion of the sample that has ever en- It certainly does not mean that women should not de-
gaged in it. One could interpret the frequency measures sire sex or that they should feel guilty over sexual de-
as indicative of the frequency of desire, whereas inci- sire or pleasure. There is also substantial variance
dence reflects the intensity. By that reasoning, the evi- within individuals, and factors such as stress or sleep
dence for a gender difference in intensity is not far deprivation could certainly contribute to changes in
behind the difference in frequency. For example, inci- frequency or intensity of sex drive within the same per-
dence of masturbation is significantly and substan- son. Our conclusion is merely that on average men de-
tially higher among men than women, across many sire sex more strongly and more frequently than
studies (Oliver & Hyde, 1993). women. Also, we reiterate that we do not see having a
There are several other findings that suggest men stronger sex drive as in any way better than having a
have more intense sexual desires, although we con- milder one indeed, either extreme of sexual desire
tinue to regard the difference in frequency as better can be problematic for both individual and society.
supported. Men like a greater variety of sexual prac- Furthermore, the difference in sex drive should not be
tices and activities than women, a difference that does used to justify coercive or exploitative behavior either.
not reduce easily to a difference in frequency of desire. Men may not be able to prevent themselves from desir-
The greater reluctance of men to live without sex, even ing sex under many circumstances, but they can pre-
when personal values and community support strongly vent themselves from acting on those desires. By the
encourage celibacy (as among priests), suggests more same token, women may not want sex as much as men,
intense desires. Men thus have less success than but they can refrain from exploiting men's dependency
women at restraining their sexual desires, although like that arises from the difference in sex drive.
all failures in self-control there is ambiguity as to The concept of sex drive, if defined in a fairly pre-
whether stronger desires or weaker inner control is re- cise manner, can be effectively distinguished from
sponsible. Men are less likely than women to assert other concepts, and the greater male sex drive does not
that their sexual desires can be controlled. entail greater male sexuality in those other terms. We
Three findings point most strongly toward a differ- suggested that women's capacity for sex may be
ence in intensity. First, men make more sacrifices than greater than men's in the sense that women are capable
women to obtain sex, and sacrificing one resource for of engaging in more sex and having more orgasms than
another is a reasonable operationalization of the inten- men. We concluded that there is no definite answer to
sity of motivation for obtaining sex. Second, women the question of whether men enjoy sex more than
report lack of sexual interest or desire more than men. women. We also suggested that women may surpass

264
GENDER DIFFERENCE IN SEX DRIVE?

men in extrinsically motivated sex, although a definite information in their published reports) so that it will
conclusion is elusive in that sphere too. For present become possible to forge statistically precise estimates
purposes, the crucial point is that the greater male sex of the gender difference in sex drive, including its size
drive does not entail greater capacity, greater enjoy- and shape as well as how the difference varies across
ment, or greater extrinsic motivation. acts and circumstances. For example, it remains possi-
Another limitation would be that these data are only ble that the gender difference in sex drive dwindles or
broad averages. It is not safe to conclude that in all mar- even effectively disappears during the blossoming of
riages the husband will want sex more than the wife. passionate love that attends rapidly rising intimacy, so
Certainly there are some men with quite low sex drives that many couples will have the temporary experience
and some women with quite high ones. Miller and of equal sexual desire (see Baumeister & Bratslavsky,
Fishkin (1997), for example, found that the median 1999)-but when that phase ends, the partners return
number of desired sex partners was the same (1.0) for to their baseline level of sex drive, and the gender dif-
both genders even though the means were wildly differ- ference reemerges, possibly to the acute surprise of the
ent (2.7 vs. 64.0). Sexual conflicts of interest will tend to couple. Further research is needed to support or invali-
take the form of men wanting more, earlier, and more date such theoretically important possible exceptions
varied sex than their female partners, but undoubtedly and other variations in the discrepancy.
there will be some cases in which the woman's sexual
desires are stronger than her male partner's.
The findings of Miller and Fishkin (1997) raise the Are Differences Rooted in Biology?
possible objection that the consistent findings of
greater male sex drive may be produced by a small mi- Most of the data we have reviewed pertain to the
nority of men or women who skew the data. In other late 20th century and to modern, Western cultures. It is
words, a small number of sexually unresponsive, unin- clear that during that time, men in Western cultures
terested, and unmotivated women could bring down have had a stronger sex drive than women. Yet does
the mean for all women or a small number of that mean the difference is biologically innate? Obvi-
hyperresponsive, hyperaroused, and hypersexual men ously, human biology has changed relatively little dur-
could bring up the mean for all men. The preponder- ing recent centuries and, as reviewed subsequently,
ance of men and women would conceivably have there is increasing evidence for the role of hormones in
roughly equivalent sex drives, in that view. determining human sexual behaviors and motivations.
However, several lines of evidence speak against Our review of the literature indicated that role of an-
the view that a small minority drives the gender differ- drogens (e.g., testosterone) was crucial in producing sex
ence in sex drive. First, data on masturbation-an area drive. We focused on the androgens for several reasons.
that shows some of the strongest and most consistent First, scientists' interest in the effects of testosterone
gender differences (Oliver & Hyde, 1993)-reveal that have yielded a wealth of data on its effects. Second, tes-
the vast majority of men (84%), but only about half of tosterone is one of the primary organizational and
women, engage in masturbation (Jones & Barlow, activational hormones that differentiates men and
1990). Similarly, when asked if they masturbated at women. Although both women and men have natural
least once a year, 80% of boys but only 25% of girls re- supplies of testosterone in their bloodstream, the
ported at least yearly masturbation (Sigusch & amount of testosterone varies significantly between the
Schmidt, 1973). Prevalence data like these are espe- genders. On average, men's blood testosterone levels
cially relevant because each person is allowed to be are 1,000 nanograms per deciliter, whereas women's
counted only once and in a binary fashion; accord- blood testosterone levels are only one seventh or one
ingly, extreme minorities cannot taint the overall re- eighth of this amount (see Dabbs, 2000; Mazur &
sults. Second, research by Byers and Lewis (1988) on Booth, 1998). Postmenopausal women have especially
disagreements about sex has found that almost half of low levels of testosterone (regardless of whether meno-
all heterosexual couples disagree about sex, and every pause occurs naturally or as a result of surgical proce-
single disagreement involved the man desiring some dures). Most commonly, surgically induced menopause
sexual activity that his female partner did not. Like- is the result of an oophorectomy (i.e., removal of the
wise, men and women agree that female reluctance ovaries and adrenals) or hysterectomy (i.e., removal of
about sex is much more likely to occur than male sex- the uterus). Third and perhaps most germane to this
ual reluctance (O'Sullivan & Byers, 1995). Thus, it analysis, evidence from the animal and human litera-
does not appear that the majority of heterosexual cou- tures suggests that androgens are responsible for active
ples are evenly matched in sex drive. initiation of sexual activity (i.e., proceptivity), whereas
Nevertheless, we exhort future researchers to attend estrogens are responsible for passive acceptance of sex-
carefully to the statistical distribution patterns in stud- ual activity (i.e., receptivity; Beach, 1976; De Jonge &
ies on gender and sex drive (and to provide thorough Van de Poll, 1984; Sherwin, 1988).

265
BAUMEISTER, CATANESE, & VOHS

In a recent study, researchers found that high doses women corroborates our conclusion that testosterone
of testosterone given to oophorectomized women via a is an important determinant of sexual drive.
skin patch (i.e., transdermally) improved sexual func- Despite our conclusion that androgens (especially
tioning and sexual motivation relative to receiving a pla- testosterone) are a major component of the biological
cebo (Shifren et al., 2000). Frequency of sexual activity, basis of sexual motivations, we also found many stud-
reports of pleasure and orgasm, sexual arousal, and sex- ies that failed to find a significant association. We
ual desires and thoughts were most strongly affected by found as many studies reporting a significant relation
testosterone therapy. For example, the percentage of between testosterone levels in men and women and
women reporting sexual fantasies at least once a week sexual appetites (e.g., Bancroft, Sanders, Davidson, &
was 12% at baseline, 10% for the placebo group, and Warner, 1983; Dabbs & Mohammed, 1992; Sherwin,
24% for women in the high testosterone group. Simi- Gelfand, & Brender, 1985; Udry, Billy, Morris, Groff,
larly, masturbation at least once a week was reported by & Raj, 1985; Van Goozen, Wiegant, Endert, Helmond,
3% of women at baseline, 5% of women receiving the & Van de Poll, 1997) as we found studies reporting no
placebo, and 10% of women receiving high testosterone relation (e.g., Alexander & Sherwin, 1991; Alexander,
treatment. The percentage of women reporting sexual Sherwin, Bancroft, & Davidson, 1990; Galyer,
intercourse at least once a week also increased with tes- Conaglen, Hare, & Conaglen, 1999; Kirchengast,
tosterone treatment (23% of women at baseline vs. 35% Hartmann, Gruber, & Huber, 1996). Indeed, we won-
of women during placebo treatment vs. 41 % during high der how many studies that found no association be-
testosterone treatment). tween androgens and sexuality were never published.
A study of 35 female-to-male transsexuals and 15 Still, when effects are found, the direction is consis-
male-to-female transsexuals also supports the impact tent: Higher testosterone is linked to higher sex drive.
of androgens on sex drive. In a longitudinal design that We found no studies in which higher testosterone pre-
tested patients before and 3 months postoperatively, dicted lesser sex drive.
Van Goozen, Cohen-Kettenis, Gooren, Frijda, & Van Even within a given study, the strength of the link
de Poll (1995) found a decrease in sexual interest and between sexuality and testosterone varies as a function
arousability among the male-to-female transsexuals, how testosterone is measured and which component of
who were administered anti-androgens and estrogens. sexual desire or functioning is assessed. Some re-
In contrast, the female-to-male transsexuals, who were searchers focus on total testosterone, which is com-
administered testosterone, reported heightened sexual prised of testosterone bound to globulin, testosterone
interest and arousability. These data highlight the im- bound to albumin, unbound testosterone, and free tes-
portance of testosterone in producing meaningful tosterone, whereas other researchers focus solely on
changes in sexual arousal and interest, even over a rela- free testosterone, which is considered the physiologi-
tively short time. cally active portion (see Alexander et al., 1990). In ad-
Differences in naturally occurring testosterone lev- dition, researchers emphasize different facets of
els distinguish between two subtypes of lesbians, sexuality, which vary in their relation to specific mea-
"butch" versus "femme" (Singh, Vidaurri, Zambarano, sures of testosterone (Sherwin, 1988). For instance, in
& Dabbs, 1999). Lesbians classify a butch lesbian (as a study of healthy young women (Van Goozen et al.,
opposed to being a femme lesbian) by body build and 1997), free testosterone was highly correlated with
weight, gait, style of dress, and attitude, with butches sexual interest, frequency of intercourse, and fre-
being bigger in size, more active, and less pretty than quency of orgasm, but total testosterone was correlated
femmes (Loulan, 1990). Degree of self-rated with frequency of masturbation. In a study of healthy
butchness was predicted by testosterone level (P3 = .51; young men (Alexander & Sherwin, 1991), free testos-
see similar findings by Pearcey, Dochert, & Dabbs, terone was positively correlated with level of reported
1996), a relation that was statistically significant even sexual arousal after listening to an audiotape depicting
after controlling for related factors such as body an erotic heterosexual scene. However, free testoster-
weight and age. Additional data from this study re- one failed to predict attentional bias for sexual stimuli
vealed that butch lesbians were involved in more sex- during a dichotic listening task (during which the
ual relationships in the past 2 years, reported less desire erotic tape was played), a response that could be con-
to give birth but reported more desire to raise children sidered an indicator of sexual interest or motivation.
relative to femme lesbians, and were less likely to Although it is well known that sex drive and sexual
adopt a submissive style of sexual participation. Butch functioning are influenced by social factors (see
lesbians also reported greater enjoyment of erotica rel- Baumeister & Tice, 2001), there is some suggestion that
ative to heterosexual women. In discussing their find- the role of biology is moderated by social factors more for
ings, Singh et al. (1999) related the style of butch women than for men. In a longitudinal study of over 200
lesbians to sexual behaviors typical of men; their find- teen and preteen girls (Halpern, Udry, & Suchindran,
ings regarding higher testosterone levels among these 1997), both level of testosterone and change in testoster-

266
GENDER DIFFERENCE IN SEX DRIVE?

one predicted reports of first act of sexual intercourse. Sherwin et al., 1985; Shifren, Nahum, & Mazer, 1998) and
However, religious service attendance moderated the tes- overall decreased well-being (Nathorst-Boos, von
tosterone/sexual-intercourse link, such that more fre- Schoultz, & Carlstrom, 1993). Researchers have thus con-
quent attendance attenuated the association. cluded that a lack of sexual interest and activity is the result
A recent review of the literature on sexual desire and of insufficient androgen production, although these latter
hormones (Regan, 1999) concluded that there exists a examples may be cases in which androgen threshold theo-
threshold effect, suggesting that testosterone levels ries apply.
above a certain critical level do not yield additional in- There may also exist a biological basis for gender
creases in sexual behavior. Moreover, there are sugges- differences in sex drive through differences in genital
tions that this threshold effect applies more to women, size, a factor hypothesized to subsequently influence
who are sensitive to smaller amounts of testosterone sexual motivation through learning experiences
(see Dabbs, 2000) and who are influenced more by so- (Baldwin & Baldwin, 1997). Differences in the size of
cial factors than men. For example, investigations of the penis versus the size of the clitoris is likely to be a
hormonal and social influences on sexuality among primary reason that one's own sexual arousal is much
boys and girls (Udry et al., 1985; Udry, Talbert, & Mor- more apparent to a man than to woman (Knoth et al.,
ris, 1986) found that among boys, the best predictors for 1988). Hence, a review of biosocial explanations for
autosexual and partnered sexual activity were levels of differences in sex drive as a function of gender con-
testosterone. Among girls there were correlations be- cludes that as a consequence of women's decreased
tween testosterone and sexual activity, but the strongest awareness of their sexual responsiveness, they may be-
predictors were social factors such as peer group inter- come less interested in and less able to enjoy the plea-
actions and the sexual activities of close friends. surable aspects of sex.
However, some researchers argue that a lack of A final point about biology and sexual motivation,
clarity on the effects of hormones or physiological fac- and one that was mentioned briefly earlier, is that there
tors on sexual functioning among women reflect resis- may be evolutionary reasons for men to have a stronger
tance to systematic research on the topic, as well as sex drive than women. Sexual strategies theory (see
inadequate funding (e.g., Bartlik, Kaplan, Kaminetsky, Buss, 1998; Buss & Schmitt, 1993) proposes that be-
Roentsch, & Goldberg, 1999). Thus, future research cause the investment costs of sexual activity are so
may illuminate more precisely the role of biology in much higher for women than men, women should be
women's sexual motivation. sexually more selective and less promiscuous than men.
Certainly there is more to the biological aspects of sex- We think this argument also extends to differences in
ual motivation than androgens. Increasingly researchers sex drive such that higher costs of sex should promote a
are examining neurological correlates of sexual desire (al- weaker motivation for sex. Consider the potential con-
beit mainly in animals). A review of the relations among sequences of a strong sex drive in a woman. She may
neuroanatomical, neurochemical, and neuroendocrine sys- engage in frequent and wide-ranging sexual practices
tems and sexual responses (Pfaus, 1999) emphasizes how with many sexual partners and, as a result, she is likely
these systems work in conjunction with hormone levels to to become pregnant. Becoming pregnant then requires
influence partner preference, erections, orgasm, sexual de- at least a 9 month commitment for the woman, whereas
sire, and sexual satisfaction (see also Meston & Frohlich, these same behaviors and consequence require from a
2000). Research has also revealed that peaks and troughs man only as much time as he chooses to commit. In-
in sexual desire can be linked to changes in estrogen levels deed, research suggests that the primary messages com-
within menstrual cycle phase and as influenced by oral municated by parents to their daughters about sex
contraceptives (e.g., Adams, Gold, & Burt, 1978). How- involve menstruation and pregnancy (Roberts, Kline, &
ever, a study (Sherwin et al., 1985) of surgically Gagnon, 1978). Thus, given differential costs to engag-
postmenopausal women receiving high doses of testoster- ing in sexual activity, there may be evolutionary reasons
one, alone or in conjunction with estrogen, indicated this to promote different sexual motivations in men and
led to increased sexual desire, fantasies, and sexual arousal women. Moreover, women may want sex for reasons
significantly more than women receiving only estrogen or outside of the sexual activity itself, such as wanting sex
a placebo. A similar effect was found among women re- to foster feelings of love for each other, to gain material
ceiving either therapy with testosterone and estrogen or es- resources, or to become pregnant.
trogen alone. Women whose therapy included testosterone
in addition to estrogen showed increased sexual activity,
satisfaction, pleasure, and orgasm frequency (S. R. Davis, Cultural Influence
McCloud, Strauss, & Burger, 1995). Last, surgically in-
duced postmenopausal women who receive estrogen ther- The possibility of cultural influence must be ac-
apy continue to report depressed sexual desire, activity, knowledged, and cultures have certainly changed. Some
and pleasure (Nathorst-Boos & von Schoultz, 1992; recent authors have begun to look at the accumulating

267
BAUMEISTER, CATANESE, & VOHS

evidence and yet they draw very different conclusions. not male Indians preferred sex partners, but the fact
Schwartz and Rutter (1998) considered evidence from that they are willing to have sex with them at all, de-
the National Health and Social Life Survey (Laumann et spite the vileness of the reputation of Untouchables, is
al., 1994) and the large "American couples" survey an indication of men' s strong desire for sex.
(Blumstein & Schwartz, 1983) and repeatedly came We noted in our review of the evidence that a study
down on the side of social constructionist explanations, from the Netherlands (Vanwesenbeeck et al., 1998) also
such as by noting that the sexual revolution seems to supports our conclusion of a weaker sex drive among
have brought about changes in female sexuality that re- women. This large-scale study of several hundred univer-
vealed how social pressures could override biology. sity students in the Netherlands found gender differences
Rigorous data from other cultures are difficult to in attitudes about sex. Women scored significantly higher
find, given cultural mores about permissiveness of talk in a factor called Anxious Insecurity (comprised of items
about sex. Thus, it is perhaps not surprising that the ma- such as "I don't know how to deal with sex"), a difference
jority of the studies cited in this article were drawn from that was highly significant among both single (p = .02)
an American sample. Indeed, if the null hypothesis that and partnered (p = .002) women. Among women, this
there are no differences in sex drive between men and variable was positively correlated with Sexual Anxiety
women were correct, it should have been more observ- (sample item "Sexual desires make me worry") and neg-
able in responses by people of the United States, given atively correlated with Sexual Sensation Seeking. Men,
this culture' s emphasis on sex and sexuality. conversely, scored significantly higher on the factors
Support for our main conclusion of differences in sex Sexual Compulsion (sample item "I find it difficult to
drive comes from an impressive survey of 890 Indian control my sexual thoughts and actions") and Sexual
schoolchildren ages 10 to 17 (Tikoo, 1997). On almost all Sensation Seeking (sample item "I want to try out un-
statements regarding sexual activity in various situations, usual sexual things"). These data converge with data
girls were more likely than boys to say that a given sexual from American samples to indicate that men have a stron-
behavior was not appropriate. For instance, disagreement ger sex drive than women. Given the highly permissive
with the statement, "it is OK for a boyfriend and a girl- attitudes about sex in the Netherlands, which would al-
friend to have sex" was higher among girls (51%) than low women to freely express their sexual desires, these
boys (34%). More girls than boys also disagreed more data argue against a confirmation of the null hypothesis
with the statements "it is OK for good friends to have of no differences in sex drive as a function of gender.
sex," (51% vs. 42%) and "sex is OK before marriage" A survey of 198 university students in Colombia
(51% vs. 43%). Self-reports of behavioral data also sug- also documented differences in sex drive (Alzate &
gest lower sex drive among Indian girls, with 88% of the Villegas, 1994). Although conducted to assess whether
girls saying they had never been sexually attracted to a sexual practices have changed since an awareness of
boy and only 60% of boys saying they had never been the AIDS virus, data on frequency and intensity of sex-
sexually attracted to a girl. In addition, twice as many ual desire showed that men have a stronger sex drive
boys than girls reported masturbating. than women. Thus, a South American sample of young
One objection to these data would be that perhaps the men and women in college, participants who are com-
students were merely repeating what they had been taught parable to those included in North American studies, is
by their culture. However, there is evidence to refute this similarly supportive of our conclusions.
interpretation, suggesting instead that sexual experience Another source of cross-cultural information is anthro-
strengthened the differences in reports of sex drive. There pological studies of gender and sexuality. Although none
was a significant difference in the endorsement of the explicitly considered whether men and women differ in
statements "boys like sex more than girls" and "girls want sex drive, these writings suggest that men have greater sex-
love more than sex" as a function of age category, such that ual motivation. Gutmann's (1997) review of masculinity
students in grades 10 to 12 (who are approximately 15-17 notes that in many cultures, maleness is often connected to
years old) agreed more with these statements than did stu- genitalia. One anthropologist wrote the phrase "men who,
dents in grades 6 to 9 (who are approximately 10-14 years as naturally promiscuous, ... " (Weeks, 1985, p. 159) al-
old). Although these data were never correlated with sex- most as a stated fact. According to Gutmann (1997), some
ual experience, older students did report more sexual expe- ethnographers assume that there is a stronger male sex
riences and higher frequency of sexual intercourse, drive and subsequently focus on the mechanisms (e.g.,
behaviors that may have given them great and direct infor- power inequalities and male domination) responsible for
mation with which to respond to these statements. this difference (see, e.g., Godelier, 1986). A review of pas-
In India, male-but not female-members of the sionate love and, specifically, sexual desire (Hatfield &
dominant culture will engage in sexual relations with Rapson, 1993) concluded that there exists much more sim-
Untouchables, despite the fact that any association ilarity than differences in patterns of sexual desire among
with Untouchables is perceived as polluting, contami- major cultural groups. However, the cross-cultural study
nating, and defiling (Mahar, 1972). Untouchables are of male versus female sexuality has been neglected, as two

268
GENDER DIFFERENCE IN SEX DRIVE?

anthropologists noted in stating that " ... [flor the study of clusion that a sex difference exists. The late 20th century
erotics ... cross-cultural data are still too impoverished and followed the sexual revolution(s) of the 20th century,
decontextualized to truly compare masculinity and femi- which are widely recognized as having liberated female
ninity, sexual excitement, and fantasy constructs" (Herdt sexuality from cultural suppression (at least to a substantial
& Stoller, 1990, pp. 352-353). Despite this, the available extent). Any culture-based differences in sex drive should
anthropological and cross-cultural evidence suggests that be smaller, not larger, in the late 20th century samples than
men have a stronger sex drive than women. in what studies of different cultures or eras would find.
Given our brief review of the evidence regarding the in- So what can we conclude about whether the differ-
fluence of biology and culture, we find ourselves leaning ence in sex drive reflects innate, biological patterns? We
toward the interactionist argument. We acknowledge that think the evidence indicates that both cultural forces and
it is famously difficult to provide unambiguous evidence biological tendencies are involved. Evidence of histori-
that nature rather than culture is the sole determinant of be- cal and cultural variation in the degree of difference in
havior. No adult human being has escaped the influence of sex drive points toward culture as playing a role. On the
culture and socialization, and so it generally requires a leap other hand, the finding of greater male sex drive does
of faith to ascribe some pattern to nature rather than cul- not seem limited to particular historical periods or cul-
ture. Certainly most of the evidence we have provided tural circumstances (although admittedly the vast ma-
about sexual behaviors and feelings is based on people jority of the available evidence is based on Western or
who have been influenced by culture. Nonetheless, there Westernized cultures), and it is found even when culture
was not one definitive source of cross-cultural data to indi- encourages female sexuality. At present we regard it as
cate that women had a stronger sex drive than men. In fact, highly unlikely that either nature or culture will emerge
cross-cultural data-even from countries with permissive as solely responsible for the difference.
sexual attitudes such as the Netherlands-supported our Still, that conclusion goes beyond the scope of this arti-
conclusion that women possess a weaker sex drive. cle. Our goal has been to establish that a reliable gender dif-
However, we think it fair to assume that there has been ference in sex drive exists because existing opinion does not
significant influence by culture and society aimed at sup- apparently subscribe to any such view. Once the existence
pressing female sexuality. A critical review of a broad as- of the difference is established, a logical next step for re-
sortment of evidence does indicate that culture has exerted search is to begin to ascertain how much of that difference is
some strong and one-sided efforts toward this end (see attributable to biology and how much to socialization.
Baumeister & Twenge, 2001). So to some degree the rela-
tive mildness of female sexuality reflects cultural stifling, Concluding Remarks
as Schwartz and Rutter (1998) and others have suggested.
Then again, even where cultural stifling has not seem- All the evidence we have reviewed points toward the
ingly had much influence, there do remain substantial dif- conclusion that men desire sex more than women. Al-
ferences in sex drive that may be linked to biological though some of the findings were more methodologi-
differences between men and women. As noted in the sec- cally rigorous than others, the unanimous convergence
tion on differences in sex drive, several findings indicate across all measures and findings increases confidence.
that women have less frequent or intense sexual desires We did not find a single study, on any of nearly a dozen
than men even when cultural pressures do not selectively different measures, that found women had a stronger
constrain female sexuality. Women have been encouraged sex drive than men. We think that the combined quan-
to want sex within marriage, but they still want less than tity, quality, diversity, and convergence of the evidence
men. The culture's attempts to stamp out masturbation render the conclusion indisputable.
were directed primarily at young men, not young women, Turning to the causes of gender differences in sex
and if cultural programming could succeed we would ex- drive, it would be premature to declare that a substantial
pect that men would masturbate less than women, but the part of the gender difference in sex drive is biologically
reverse is true. Catholic clergy all subscribe to the same, innate, but we think the evidence is pointing in that di-
single standard of sexual purity, but men find it much more rection (not least because of the apparent consistency of
difficult to live up to this ideal than women. the difference). Biological processes, including the sub-
The best bet is therefore probably that society's in- stantial gender difference in testosterone, have been im-
fluence may have at various times tried to increase or plicated as determining sex drive. Although most
reduce the gender difference in sex drive, but it did not findings pertain to modern America, a smattering of
likely reverse the natural order of things, nor did it cre- findings from other cultures continues to depict the male
ate the difference out of nothing. Probably gender dif- sex drive as stronger. Cultural influences have sought to
ferences in sex drive reflect a combination of natural stifle some aspects of female sexuality, but we found the
and cultural influences. difference in sex drive even in sexual spheres (such as
Moreover, the emphasis in this review on relatively re- marital sex) where culture has supported and encour-
cent data should have biased the findings against the con- aged female sexual desire, so stifling should not be rele-

269
BAUMEISTER, CATANESE, & VOHS

vant. Personally we would like to believe that culture Adams, D. M., Gold, A. R., & Burt, A. D. (1978). Rose in female-ini-
and socialization could be modified so as to make the fe- tiated sexual activity at ovulation and its suppression by oral
contraceptives. New England Journal of Medicine, 299,
male sex drive precisely the same as the male sex drive 1145-1150.
(because that would seemingly foster more harmonious Adams, H. E., Wright, L. W., & Lohr, B. A. (1996). Is homophobia
relationships), but our review of the literature does not associated with homosexual arousal? Journal ofAbnormal Psy-
offer much encouragement to that view. Certainly any- chology, 105, 440-445.
one seeking to advocate that view of total cultural rela- Alexander, G. M., & Sherwin, B. B. (1991). The association between
testosterone, sexual arousal, and selection attention for erotic
tivity faces a substantial burden of proof. stimuli in men. Hormones and Behavior, 25, 367-381.
Regardless of whether the gender difference in sex Alexander, G. M., Sherwin, B. B., Bancroft, J., & Davidson, D. W.
drive is universal or is a product of modern Western (1990). Testosterone and sexual behavior in oral contraceptive
culture, it is important for understanding close rela- users and nonusers: A prospective study. Hormones and Behav-
tionships and sexual behavior today. According to the ior, 24, 388-402.
Allgeier, E. R., & Allgeier, A. R. (2000). Sexual interactions (5th
principle of least interest (Waller & Hill, 1938/1951), ed.). Boston: Houghton Mifflin.
social interactions will be shaped by the "act that the Alzate, H., & Villegas, M. (1994). Sexual behavior of unmarried Co-
person who wants something more than the other is in a lumbian university students in 1990. Journal of Sex Education
dependent position and will usually have to offer the and Therapy, 20, 287-298.
other some inducements. Hence many male-female American Psychiatric Association. (1994). Diagnostic and statistical
romantic interactions will take the form of the man of- manual ofmental disorders (4th ed.). Washington, DC: Author.
American Psychological Association. (1967-2001). PsycINFO [On-
fering the woman some resources (commitment, flat- line]. Washington, DC: Author [Producer]. Ovid Technologies
tery, food, entertainment, money, companionship) to [Distributor].
induce her to commence a sexual relationship. Arafat, I. S., & Cotton, W. L. (1974). Masturbation practices of males
Several eminent authors have recently discussed and and females. Journal of Sex Research, 10, 293-307.
deplored the relatively poor state of theorizing in the study Ard, B. N. (1977). Sex in lasting marriages: A longitudinal study.
of sexuality (see Weis, 1998; also DeLamater & Hyde, Journal of Sex Research, 13, 274-285.
Asayama, S. (1975). Adolescent sex development and adult sex be-
1998). We think that the field's lack of consensus about havior in Japan. Journal of Sex Research, 11, 91-112.
gender differences in sex drive may have been a consider- Baldwin, J. D., & Baldwin, J. I. (1997). Gender differences in sexual
able obstacle to building theory. Indeed, some authors interest. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 26, 181-210.
have hinted that there has been pressure to assume men Bancroft, J., Sanders, D., Davidson, D., & Warner, P. (1983). Mood,
and women have equal sexual motivations (e.g., Leigh, sexuality, hormones, and the menstrual cycle. III: Sexuality and
the role of androgens. Psychosomatic Medicine, 45, 509-516.
1989). The assumption of equality would have steered the- Bartlik, B., Kaplan, P., Kaminetsky, J., Roentsch, G., & Goldberg, J.
ory in certain directions, such as forcing theorists to invoke (1999). Medications with the potential to enhance sexual
ulterior (e.g., political) explanations for men's greater in- responsivity in women. Psychiatric Annals, 29, 46-52.
terest in pornography, prostitution, and the like. Even anal- Baumeister, R. F. (2000). Gender differences in erotic plasticity: The
yses of marriage may have had to regard men's sexual female sex drive as socially flexible and responsive. Psycholog-
demands on their wives as rooted in nonsexual motives, to ical Bulletin, 126, 347-374.
Baumeister, R. F., & Bratslavsky, E. (1999). Passion, intimacy, and
explain the differential sexual behavior without violating time: Passionate love as a function of change in intimacy. Per-
the (false) principle of equal sex drive. Although political sonality and Social Psychology Review, 3, 49-67.
factors can certainly influence sexual behavior, we suggest Baumeister, R. F., & Tice, D. M. (2001). The social dimension of sex.
that the political motives are likely to interact with and de- Boston: Allyn & Bacon.
velop around the basic gender difference in sexual motiva- Baumeister, R. F., & Twenge, J. M. (2001). The cultural suppression
tion. More generally, the point that men want sex more offemale sexuality. Who is the proximal cause? Manuscript
submitted for publication.
than women is likely to become a focal issue in how most Beach, F. A. (1976). Sexual attractivity, proceptivity, and receptivity
couples negotiate their sex lives, from first dates to losing in female mammals. Hormones and Behavior, 70, 105-138.
virginity to multidecade marriages. An accurate, empiri- Beck, J. G. (1995). Hypoactive sexual desire disorder: An overview.
cally grounded appreciation of the gender difference in sex Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 63, 919-927.
drive should be a helpful part of the foundation for advanc- Beck, J. G., Bozman, A. W., & Qualtrough, T. (1991). The experi-
ence of sexual desire: Psychological correlates in a college sam-
ing theory about human sexuality and relationships. ple. Journal of Sex Research, 28, 443-456.
Bell, A. P., & Weinberg, M. S. (1978). Homosexualities: A study of di-
References versity among men and women. New York: Simon & Schuster.
Bergstrom-Walan, M.-B., & Nielsen, H. H. (1990). Sexual expres-
Abramson, P. R. (1973). The relationship of the frequency of mastur- sion among 60-80-year-old men and women: A sample from
bation to several aspects of personality and social behavior. Stockholm, Sweden. Journal of Sex Research, 27, 289-295.
Journal of Sex Research, 9, 132-142. Blumstein, P., & Schwartz, P. (1983). American couples. New York:
Abramson, P. R., & Pinkerton, S. D. (1995). With pleasure: Thoughts on Simon & Schuster.
the nature ofhuman sexuality. New York: Oxford University Press. Bretschneider, J. G., & McCoy, N. L. (1988). Sexual interest and be-
Acton, W. (1857). Thefunctions and disorders of the reproductive organs havior in healthy 80- to 102-year olds. Archives of Sexual Be-
in youth, in adult age, and in advanced life. London: John Churchill. havior, 17, 109- 130.

270
GENDER DIFFERENCE IN SEX DRIVE?

Brown, M., & Auerback, A. (1981). Communication patterns in initi- DeLamater, J. D., & Hyde, J. S. (1998). Essentialism vs. social
ation of marital sex. Medical Aspects of Human Sexuality, 15, constructionism in the study of human sexuality. Journal ofSex
105-117. Research, 35, 10-18.
Brown, N. R., & Sinclair, R. C. (1999). Estimating number of lifetime Driscoll, R. H., & Davis, K. E. (1971). Sexual restraints: A compari-
sexual partners: Men and women do it differently. Journal of son of perceived and self-reported reasons for college students.
Sex Research, 36, 292-297. Journal of Sex Research, 7, 253-262.
Buss, D. M. (1989). Conflict between the sexes: Strategic interfer- Ehrenreich, B. (1999, March 8). The real truth about the female.
ence and the evocation of anger and upset. Journal of Personal- Time, 153(9), 57-71.
ity and Social Psychology, 56, 735-747. Elias, J. E., Bullough, V. L., Elias, V., & Brewer, G. (1998). Prostitu-
Buss, D. M. (1998). Sexual strategies theory: Historical origins and tion: On whores, hustlers, and johns. New York: Prometheus.
current status. Journal of Sex Research, 35, 19-31. Ellis, B. J., & Symons, D. (1990). Sex differences in sexual fantasy:
Buss, D. M., & Schmitt, D. P. (1993). Sexual strategies theory: A An evolutionary psychological approach. Journal of Sex Re-
contextual evolutionary analysis of human mating. Psychologi- search, 27, 527-555.
cal Review, 100, 204-232. Eysenck, H. J. (1971). Masculinity-femininity, personality and sex-
Buzwell, S., & Rosenthal, D. (1996). Constructing a sexual self: Ado- ual attitudes. Journal of Sex Research, 7, 83-88.
lescents' sexual self-perceptions and sexual risk-taking. Jour- Fisher, W. A., & Byrne, D. (1978). Sex differences in response to
nal of Research on Adolescence, 6, 489-513. erotica: Love versus lust. Journal ofPersonality and Social Psy-
Byers, E. S., & Heinlein, L. (1989). Predicting initiations and refusals chology, 36, 117- 125.
of sexual activities in married and cohabiting heterosexual cou- Frank, E., Anderson, C., & Rubinstein, D. (1978). Frequency of sex-
ples. Journal of Sex Research, 26, 210-231. ual dysfunction in "normal" couples. New England Journal of
Byers, E. S., & Lewis, K. (1988). Dating couples' disagreements over Medicine, 299, 111-115.
the desired level of sexual intimacy. Journal of Sex Research, Galyer, K. T., Conaglen, H. M., Hare, A., & Conaglen, J. V. (1999).
24, 15-29. The effect of gynecological surgery on sexual desire. Journal of
Byers, E. S., Purdon, C., & Clark, D. A. (1998). Sexual intrusive thoughts Sex and Marital Therapy, 25, 81-88.
of college students. Journal of Sex Research, 35, 359-369. Godelier, M. (1986). The making ofgreat men: Male domination and
Carroll, J. L., Volk, K. D., & Hyde, J. S. (1985). Differences between power among the New Guinea Baruya. Cambridge, England:
males and females in motives for engaging in sexual inter- Cambridge University Press.
course. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 14, 131-139. Gutmann, M. C. (1997). Trafficking in men: The anthropology of
Clark, R. D., & Hatfield, E. (1989). Gender differences in receptivity masculinity. Annual Review ofAnthropology, 26, 385-409.
to sexual offers. Journal ofPsychology and Human Sexuality, 2, Halpern, C. T., Udry, J. R., & Suchindran, C. (1997). Testosterone
39-55. predicts initiation of coitus in adolescent females. Psychoso-
Cohen, L. L., & Shotland, R. L. (1996). Timing of first sexual inter- matic Medicine, 59, 161 -171.
course in a relationship: Expectations, experiences, and percep- Hansen, G. L. (1987). Extradyadic relations during courtship. Jour-
tions of others. Journal of Sex Research, 33, 291-299. nal of Sex Research, 23, 382-390.
Cotton, W. L. (1975). Social and sexual relationships of lesbians. Hatfield, E., & Rapson, R. L. (1993). Historical and cross-cultural
Journal of Sex Research, 11, 139-148. perspectives on passionate love and sexual desire. Annual Re-
Crano, W. D. (1995). Attitude strength and vested interest. In R. Petty view of Sex Research, 4, 67-97.
& J. Krosnick (Eds.), Attitude strength: Antecedents and conse- Hawton, K., & Catalan, J. (1986). Prognostic factors in sex therapy.
quences (pp. 131-157). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Asso- Behaviour Research and Therapy, 24, 377-385.
ciates, Inc. Heiman, J. R. (1977). A psychophysiological exploration of sexual
Crooks, R., & Baur, K. (1999). Our sexuality. Pacific Grove, CA: arousal patterns in females and males. Psychophysiology, 14,
Brooks/Cole. 266-274.
Dabbs, J. M., Jr. (2000). Heroes, rogues, and lovers: Testosterone Herdt, G., & Stoller, R. J. (1990). Intimate communications: Erotics
and behavior. New York: McGraw-Hill. and the study ofculture. New York: Columbia University Press.
Dabbs, J. M., Jr., & Mohammed, S. (1992). Male and female salivary Herek, G. M., & Capitanio, J. P. (1999). Sex differences in how het-
testosterone concentrations before and after sexual activity. erosexuals think about Lesbians and gay men: Evidence from
Physiology and Behavior, 52, 195- 197. survey context effects. Journal of Sex Research, 36, 348-360.
Davies, S., Katz, J., & Jackson, J. L. (1999). Sexual desire discrepan- Herold, E. S., & Mewhinney, D.-M. K. (1993). Gender differences in
cies: Effects on sexual and relationship satisfaction in heterosex- casual sex and AIDS prevention: A survey of dating bars. Jour-
ual dating couples. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 28, 553-567. nal of Sex Research, 30, 36-42.
Davis, C. M., Blank, J., Lin, H.-Y., & Bonillas, C. (1996). Character- Hill, C. A. (1997). The distinctiveness of sexual motives in relation to
istics of vibrator use among women. Journal of Sex Research, sexual desire and desirable partner attributes. Journal of Sex Re-
33, 313-320. search, 34, 139-153.
Davis, S. R., McCloud, P., Strauss, B. J. G., & Burger, H. (1995). Tes- Hurlbert, D. F. (1991). The role of assertiveness in female sexuality:
tosterone enhances estradiol' s effects on postmenopausal bone A comparative study between sexually assertive and sexually
density and sexuality. Maturitas, 21, 227-236. nonassertive women. Journal of Sex and Marital Therapy, 17,
Deci, E. L. (1971). Effects of externally mediated rewards on intrin- 183-190.
sic motivation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, Hurlbert, D. F., White, C., Powell, D. & Apt, C. (1993). Orgasm con-
18, 105-115. sistency training in the treatment of women reporting
De Gaston, J. F., Weed, S., & Jensen, L. (1996). Understanding gen- hypoactive sexual desire: An outcome comparison of
der differences in adolescent sexuality. Adolescence, 31, women-only groups and couples-only groups. Journal of Be-
217-231. havior Therapy & Experimental Psychiatry, 24, 3-13.
De Jonge, F. H., & Van de Poll, N. E. (1984). Relationships between Hyde, J. S., & DeLamater, J. (1997). Understanding human sexuality
sexual and aggressive behavior in male and female rats. In G. J. (6th ed.). Boston: McGraw-Hill.
de Bries, J. P. C. de Bruin, H. B. M. Uylings, & M. A. Corner Iasenza, S. (2000). Lesbian sexuality post-Stonewall to post-modern-
(Eds.), Sex differences in the brain B Progress in brain research ism: Putting the "lesbian bed death" concept to bed. Journal of
(Vol. 61, pp. 283-302). Amsterdam: Elsevier. Sex Education and Therapy, 25, 59-69.

271
BAUMEISTER, CATANESE, & VOHS

Janus, S. S., & Janus, C. L. (1993). The Janus report on sexual behav- Masters, W. H., Johnson, V. E., & Kolodny, R. C. (1995). Human
ior. New York: Wiley. sexuality (5th ed.). New York: HarperCollins.
Johannes, C. B., & Avis, N. E. (1997). Gender differences in sexual Mazur, A., & Booth, A. (1998). Testosterone and dominance in men.
activity among mid-aged adults in Massachusetts. Maturitas, Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 21, 353-397.
26, 175-184. McCabe, P. (1987). Desired and experienced levels of premarital af-
Jones, J. C., & Barlow, D. H. (1990). Self-reported frequency of sex- fection and sexual intercourse during dating. Journal of Sex Re-
ual urges, fantasies, and masturbatory fantasies in heterosexual search, 23, 23-33.
males and females. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 19, 269-279. Mehrabian, A., & Stanton-Mohr, L. (1985). Effects of emotional
Julien, D., Bouchard, C., Gagnon, M., & Pomerleau, A. (1992). In- state on sexual desire and sexual dysfunction. Motivation and
siders' views of marital sex: A dyadic analysis. Journal of Sex Emotion, 9, 315-330.
Research, 29, 343-360. Mercer, G. W., & Kohn, P. M. (1979). Gender difference in the inte-
Kaplan, H. S. (1979). Disorders of sexual desire. New York: Brun- gration of conservatism, sex urge, and sexual behaviors among
ner/Mazel. college students. Journal of Sex Research, 15, 129-142.
Kinsey, A. C., Pomeroy, W. B., & Martin, C. E. (1948). Sexual be- Meston, C. M., & Frohlich, P. F. (2000). The neurobiology of sexual
havior in the human male. Philadelphia: Saunders. function. Archives of General Psychiatry, 57, 1012-1030.
Kinsey, A. C., Pomeroy, W. B., Martin, C. E., & Gebhard, P. H. Miller, L. C., & Fishkin, S. A. (1997). On the dynamics of human
(1953). Sexual behavior in the human female. Philadelphia: bonding and reproductive success: Seeking windows on the
Saunders. adapted-for human-environmental interface. In J. Simpson & D.
Kirchengast, S., Hartmann, B., Gruber, D., & Huber, J. (1996). De- Kenrick (Eds.), Evolutionary social psychology (pp. 197-235).
creased sexual interest and its relationship to body build in Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.
postmenopausal women. Maturitas, 23, 63-71. Murphy, S. (1992). A delicate dance: Sexuality, celibacy, and relation-
Knoth, R., Boyd, K., & Singer, B. (1988). Empirical tests of sexual ships among Catholic clergy and religious. New York: Crossroad.
selection theory: Predictions of sex differences in onset, inten- Nathorst-Boos, J., & von Schoultz, B. (1992). Psychological reactions
sity, and time course of sexual arousal. Journal of Sex Research, and sexual life after hysterectomy with and without oophorectomy.
24, 73-89. Gynecological and Obstetrician Investment, 34, 97-101.
LaPlante, M. N., McCormick, N., & Brannigan, G. G. (1980). Living Nathorst-Boos, J., von Schoultz, B., & Carlstrom, K. (1993). Elective
the sexual script: College students' views of influence in sexual ovarian removal and estrogen replacement therapy: Effects on
encounters. Journal of Sex Research, 16, 338-355. sexual life, psychological well-being and androgen status. Jour-
Laumann, E. O., Gagnon, J. H., Michael, R. T., & Michaels, S. nal ofPsychosomatic Obstetrics and Gynecology, 14, 283-293.
(1994). The social organization of sexuality: Sexualpractices in National Library of Medicine. (1966-2001). MEDLINE [Online].
the United States. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. Bethesda, MD: National Library of Medicine.
Lawson, A. (1988). Adultery: An analysis of love and betrayal. New Nutter, D. E., & Condron, M. K. (1983). Sexual fantasy and activity
York: Basic Books. patterns of females with inhibited sexual desire versus normal
Leiblum, S. R., & Rosen, R. C. (1988). Changing perspectives on controls. Journal of Sex and Marital Therapy, 9, 276-282.
sexual desire. In S. Leiblum & R. Rosen (Eds.), Sexual desire Oliver, M. B., & Hyde, J. S. (1993). Gender differences in sexuality:
disorders (pp. 1-20). New York: Guilford. A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 114, 29-51.
Leigh, B. C. (1989). Reasons for having and avoiding sex: Gender, O'Sullivan, L., & Byers, E. S. (1992). College students' incorpora-
sexual orientation, and relationship to sexual behavior. Journal tion of intiator and restrictor roles in sexual dating interactions.
of Sex Research, 26, 199-209. Journal of Sex Research, 29, 435-446.
Leigh, B. C., Morrison, D. M., Trocki, K., & Temple, M. T. (1994). O'Sullivan, L. F., & Byers, E. S. (1995). Gender differences in re-
Sexual behavior of American adolescents: Results from a U.S. sponse to discrepancies in desired level of sexual intimacy.
national survey. Journal of Adolescent Health, 15, 117-125. Journal of Psychology and Human Sexuality, 8, 49-67.
Leitenberg, H., Detzer, M. J., & Srebnik, D. (1993). Gender differ- Pearcey, S. M., Dochert, K. J., & Dabbs, J. M. (1996). Testosterone
ences in masturbation and the relation of masturbation experi- and sex role identification in lesbian couples. Physiology and
ence in preadolescence and/or early adolescence to sexual be- Behavior, 60, 1033-1035.
havior and sexual adjustment in young adulthood. Archives of Pfaus, J. G. (1999). Neurobiology of sexual behavior. Current Opin-
Sexual Behavior, 22, 87-98. ion in Neurobiology, 9, 751-758.
Leitenberg, H., & Henning, K. (1995). Sexual fantasy. Psychological Pfeiffer, E., Verwoerdt, A., & Davis, G. (1972). Sexual behavior in
Bulletin, 117, 469-496. middle life. American Journal Psychiatry, 128, 1262-1267.
Lepper, M. R., & Greene, D. (1978). (Eds.). The hidden costs of re- Regan, P. C. (1999). Hormonal correlates and causes of sexual desire:
ward. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc. A review. Canadian Journal of Sexuality, 8, 1-16.
Lepper, M. R., Greene, D., & Nisbett, R. E. (1973). Undermining Regan, P. C., & Berscheid, E. (1996). Beliefs about the state, goals,
children's intrinsic interest with extrinsic rewards: A test of the and objects of sexual desire. Journal of Sex and Marital Ther-
overjustification hypothesis. Journal of Personality and Social apy, 22, 110-120.
Psychology, 28, 129-137. Reinholtz, R. K., & Muehlenhard, C. L. (1995). Genital perceptions
Lewis, R. A. (1973). Parents and peers: Socialization agents in the and sexual activity in a college population. Journal of Sex Re-
coital behavior of young adults. Journal of Sex Research, 9, search, 32, 155-165.
156-170. Riley, A., & Riley, E. (2000). Controlled studies on women present-
Loulan, J. (1990). Lesbian erotic dance: Butch, femme, androgyny, ing with sexual drive disorder: I. Endocrine status. Journal of
and other rhythms. Duluth, MN: Spinsters Book Co. Sex and Marital Therapy, 26, 269-283.
MacPhee, D. C., Johnson, S. M., & van der Veer, M. M. C. (1995). Roberts, E. J., Kline, D., & Gagnon, J. (1978). Family life and sexual
Low sexual desire in women: The effects of marital therapy. learning (Vol. 1). Cambridge, MA: Population Education.
Journal of Sex and Marital Therapy, 21, 159-182. Rosen, R. C., & Leiblum, S. R. (1989). Assessment and treatment of
Mahar, J. M. (1972). Agents of Dharma in a north Indian village. In J. desire disorders. In R. Rosen & S. Leiblum (Eds.), Principles
M. Mahar (Ed.), The Untouchables in contemporary India (pp. and practice of sex therapy (2nd ed., pp. 19-47). New York:
17-35). Tucson: University of Arizona Press. Guilford.

272
GENDER DIFFERENCE IN SEX DRIVE?

Sanders, S. A., & Reinisch, J. M. (1999). Would you say you "had Stuart, F., Hammond, D., & Pett, M. (1987). Inhibited sexual desire in
sex" if ... ? Journal of the American Medical Association, 281, women. Archives ofSexual Behavior, 16, 91-106.
275-277. Thompson, A. P. (1983). Extramarital sex: A review of the research
Schwartz, P., & Rutter, V. (1998). The genderof sexuality. Thousand literature. Journal ofSex Research, 19, 1-22.
Oaks, CA: Pine Forge Press. Tikoo, M. (1997). Sexual attitudes and behaviors of school students
Segraves, K., & Segraves, R. T. (1991). Hypoactive sexual desire dis- (grades 6-12) in India. Journal of Sex Research, 34, 77-84.
order: Prevalence and comorbidity in 906 subjects. Journal of Trudel, G., Boulos, L., & Matte, B. (1993). Dyadic adjustment in cou-
Sex and Marital Therapy, 17, 55-58. ples with hypoactive sexual desire. Journal of Sex Education
Sherwin, B. (1988). A comparative analysis of the role of androgen in and Therapy, 19, 31-36.
human male and female sexual behavior: Behavioral specific-
Trudel, G., Landry, L., & Larose, Y. (1997). Low sexual desire: The
ity, critical thresholds, and sensitivity. Psychobiology, 16,
role of anxiety, depression and marital adjustment. Sexual and
416-425.
Marital Therapy, 12, 95-99.
Sherwin, B. B., Gelfand, M. M., & Brender, W. (1985). Androgen en-
hances sexual motivation in females: A prospective, crossover Udry, J. R., Billy, J. 0. G., Morris, N. M., Groff, T. R., & Raj, M. H.
study of sex steroid administration in the surgical menopause. (1985). Serum androgenic hormones motivate sexual behavior
Psychosomatic Medicine, 47, 339-35 1. in adolescent boys. Fertility and Sterility, 43, 90-94.
Shifren, J. L., Braunstein, G. D., Simon, J. A., Casson, P. R., Buster, J. Udry, J. R., Talbert, L. M., & Morris, N. M. (1986). Biosocial founda-
E., Redmond, G. P., Burki, R. E., Ginsburg, E. S., Rosen, R. C., tions for adolescent female sexuality. Demography, 23,
Leiblum, S. R., Jones, K. P., Daugherty, C. A., Caramelli, K. E., 217-229.
& Mazer, N. A. (2000). Transdermal testosterone treatment in Useche, B., Villegas, M., & Alzate, H. (1990). Sexual behavior of
women with impaired sexual function after oophorectomy. New Colombian high school students. Adolescence, 25, 291-304.
England Journal of Medicine, 343, 682-688. Van Goozen, S. H. M., Cohen-Kettenis, P. T., Gooren, L. J. G.,
Shifren, J. L., Nahum, R., & Mazer, N. A. (1998). Incidence of sexual Frijda, N. H., & Van de Poll, N. E. (1995). Gender differences in
dysfunction in surgically menopausal women. Menopause, 5, behaviour: Activating effects of cross-sex hormones.
189-190. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 20, 343-363.
Sigusch, V., & Schmidt, G. (1973). Teenage boys and girls in West Van Goozen, S. H. M., Wiegant, V. M., Endert, E., Helmond, F. A., &
Germany. Journal of Sex Research, 9, 107-123. Van de Poll, N. E. (1997). Psychoendocrinological assessment
Singh, D., Vidaurri, M., Zambarano, R. J., & Dabbs, J. M. (1999). of the menstrual cycle: The relationship between hormones,
Lesbian erotic role identification: Behavioral, morphological, sexuality, and mood. Archives ofSexual Behavior, 26, 359-383.
and hormonal correlates. Journal ofPersonality and Social Psy- Vanwesenbeeck, I., Bekker, M., & van Lenning, A. (1998). Gender
chology, 76, 1035-1049. attitudes, sexual meanings, and interactional patterns in hetero-
Sipe, A. W. R. (1995). Sex priests, and power: Anatomy of a crisis. sexual encounters among college students in the Netherlands.
New York: Brunner/Mazel. Journal of Sex Research, 35, 317-327.
Sivacek, J., & Crano, W. D. (1982). Vested interest as a moderator of Ventegodt, S. (1998). Sex and the quality of life in Denmark. Ar-
attitude-behavior consistency. Journal of Personality and So- chives of Sexual Behavior, 27, 295-307.
cial Psychology, 43, 210-221. Waller, W., & Hill, R. (1951). The family: A dynamic interpretation.
Smith, A. M. A., Rosenthal, D. A., & Reichler, H. (1996). New York: Dryden. (Original work published in 1938)
Highschoolers's masturbatory practices: Their relationship to
sexual intercourse and personal characteristics. Psychological Weeks, J. (1985). Sexuality and its discontents: Meanings, myths,
Reports, 79, 499-509. and modern sexualities. London: Routledge & Kegan Paul.
Spanier, G. P., & Margolis, R. L. (1983). Marital separation and ex- Weis, D. L. (1998). The use of theory in sexuality research. Journal of
tramarital sexual behavior. Journal ofSexResearch, 19, 23-48. Sex Research, 35, 1-9.
Sprecher, S. (1989). Premarital sexual standards for different catego- Whitley, B. E. (1988). Sex differences in heterosexuals' attitudes to-
ries of individuals. Journal ofSex Research, 26, 232-248. ward homosexuals. Journal of Sex Research, 24, 287-291.
Sprecher, S., Barbee, A., & Schwartz, P. (1995). "Was it good for Wiederman, M. W. (1997). The truth must be in here somewhere: Ex-
you, too?": Gender differences in first sexual experiences. Jour- amining the gender discrepancy in self-reported lifetime num-
nal of Sex Research, 32, 3-15. ber of sex partners. Journal of Sex Research, 34, 375-386.
Sprecher, S., & Regan, P. C. (1996). College virgins: How men and Wilson, W. C. (1975). The distribution of selected sexual attitudes
women perceive their sexual status. Journal of Sex Research, and behaviors among the adult population of the United States.
33, 3-15. Journal of Sex Research, 11, 46-64.

273

You might also like