Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 21

The Journal of Special Education Apprenticeship

Volume 6 Number 1 Article 4

6-2017

Using Errorless Teaching to Teach Generalized Manding for


Information Using “How?”
Christopher Bloh
Kutztown University

Christopher Scagliotti
Kutztown University

Sarah Baugh
Kutztown University

Megan Sheenan
Kutztown University

Shane Silas
Kutztown University

See next page for additional authors

Follow this and additional works at: https://scholarworks.lib.csusb.edu/josea

Part of the Special Education and Teaching Commons

Recommended Citation
Bloh, Christopher; Scagliotti, Christopher; Baugh, Sarah; Sheenan, Megan; Silas, Shane; and Zulli, Nicole
(2017) "Using Errorless Teaching to Teach Generalized Manding for Information Using “How?”," The
Journal of Special Education Apprenticeship: Vol. 6 : No. 1 , Article 4.
Available at: https://scholarworks.lib.csusb.edu/josea/vol6/iss1/4

This Article is brought to you for free and open access by CSUSB ScholarWorks. It has been accepted for inclusion
in The Journal of Special Education Apprenticeship by an authorized editor of CSUSB ScholarWorks. For more
information, please contact scholarworks@csusb.edu.
Using Errorless Teaching to Teach Generalized Manding for Information Using
“How?”

Authors
Christopher Bloh, Christopher Scagliotti, Sarah Baugh, Megan Sheenan, Shane Silas, and Nicole Zulli

This article is available in The Journal of Special Education Apprenticeship: https://scholarworks.lib.csusb.edu/josea/


vol6/iss1/4
Vol. 6(1) June 2017
, 2016
Using Errorless Teaching to Teach Generalized Manding for
Information Using “How?”

Christopher Bloh, Christopher Scagliotti, Sarah Baugh, Megan


Sheenan, Shane Silas, and Nicole Zulli
Kutztown University

Five reinforcing activities were presented to and interrupted for two participants with
autism. An errorless teaching procedure was then introduced with two similar activities
prompting the participants to request information saying “How?” in order to resume the
activity. The dependent variable included both the cumulative number of times “How?”
occurred and number of times he used the acquired information to access his
reinforcer. Training was conducted across five clinicians to program for and determine
generalization across both activities and people. Results suggest that one participant’s
manding for information generalized across activities and clinicians, although his
utilizing the acquired information was not as apparent for 4 out of the 5 activities. The
second participant’s behavior suggested his manding to have generalized to 3 out of 5
activities but limited (2 out of 5) use of acquired information. A maintenance trial
conducted three weeks after the study’s conclusion indicated that the target behaviors
were maintained.
Keywords: errorless teaching, autism, manding, generalization

Using Errorless Teaching to motivating operation (MO) and specific


Teach Generalized Manding for reinforcement (Skinner, 1957; Michael,
Information Using “How?” 1982, 1988, 1993). An MO is an
Typically developing children environmental variable that alters the
often learn to make requests when saliency of a stimulus, e.g. manding for
prompted and in the presence of a water is under the control of a MO of
desired stimulus. As this ability, or being thirsty, reinforced by the
“manding,” becomes more consequence of being presented with
sophisticated, the speaker is able to water.
make this request when the stimulus is Simple mands can be
not present and without prompting. understood as mands for activities or
The mand is a verbal operant for which tangibles while more sophisticated
the response is under functional control mands for information are about
THE JOURNAL OF SPECIAL EDUCATION APPRENTICESHIP, 6(1) 2

activities or tangibles (Lechago, Carr, successful in teaching other participants


Grow, Love, & Almason, 2010). The to mand for information (Lechago et al.,
manded-for information could allow the 2010; Sundberg, Loeb, Hale, &
speaker to access a reinforcer (“Where Eigenheer, 2002).
can I get a pretzel?”), Lechago, Howell, The use of prompting to teach
Caccavale, & Peterson, 2013). Indeed, unknown tasks can be accomplished
most of the mands that sophisticated several ways. Prompts can be delivered
users of verbal behavior emit are those ‘least-to-most’ or ‘most-to-least’ (Libby,
for information. While typically Weiss, Bancroft, & Ahearn, 2008). In
developing children frequently mand for least-to-most prompting, the least
information with little or no direct invasive prompt, e.g. initial sound rather
teaching, those with autism may not than whole word, is typically delivered
acquire this capability. so that the participant can accomplish
A possibility for this deficit could the task. In this procedure, mistakes are
be attributed to the lack of reinforcing generally allowed but then corrected
value of the information requested with a prompt. In most-to-least
(Sundberg, Loeb, Hale, & Eigenheer, prompting, prompts are used to
2002). There may be a MO for the prevent, rather than to correct errors.
initial request for information but the This later method is commonly known
consequences may not reinforce further as “errorless teaching.” Errorless
attempts. Without direct instruction, teaching minimizes or eliminates the
children with autism frequently are probability of an incorrect choice by
unable to acquire an established using a zero-second delay (initially) to
questioning repertoire (Shillingsburg, prompt to the correct response
Valentino, Bowen, Bradley, & Zavatkay, (Terrace, 1963). Errorless teaching has
2011), been used to teach unknown skills to
Prompt and prompt fading adults with autism and intellectual
procedures have been used in direct disabilities (Jerome, Frantino, &
instruction to increase manding for Sturmey, 2007) and to increase visual
information for children with autism. discrimination skills and decrease
Manipulating MO, prompts and prompt avoidance behavior in children with
fading with vocal information regarding intellectual disabilities (Weeks &
the desired stimuli have evoked Gaylord-Ross, 1981).
manding for information in preschoolers Several studies have taught
(Endicott & Higbee, 2007). In this study, children with disabilities to mand for
MO were contrived by hiding preferred information; “which/when?,” (Shillings-
reinforcers and prompting (with burg et al., 2011), “who/where?,”
subsequent fading) the participants to (Sundberg et al., 2002), “what?,”
ask where they were or who had them. (Williams, Donley, & Keller, 2000) and
Whole word echoic prompting was used “who/which?,’ (Shillingsburg, Bowen,
to evoke the mand “Where is it?” when Valentino, & Peirce, 2014). However,
the reinforcer was hidden and “Who has teaching the mand “how” has not been
it?” when someone possessed it. Using investigated thoroughly (Shillingsburg &
whole word echoic prompting has been Valentino, 2011).
THE JOURNAL OF SPECIAL EDUCATION APPRENTICESHIP, 6(1) 3

These aforementioned researchers clinicians and multiple exemplars of


implemented procedures to teach a activities to promote generalization.
child with autism to mand for Furthermore, limitations were also
information using “how” in order to suggested with treatment integrity and
obtain information to complete maintenance.
activities. Methods teaching Building upon Shillingsburg and
participants to mand “how” may not be Valentino’s (2011) work to teach a child
common in the literature because after with autism to mand for information
learning how to complete the using “how,” this study attempted to
activity/access the reinforcer, the teach two adolescent boys with autism
individual may no longer need to mand to generalize manding for information
“how” because the skill has been using “how.” The present study
learned. In the Shillingsburg and incorporated generalization procedures
Valentino (2011) study, MO were from Shillingsburg et al., (2014) with use
contrived to ensure that the of multiple clinicians and untrained
participant’s behavior could be tasks. What varies with the former
reinforced by “how.” Their initial mand study is that the present study utilized
training trials were conducted using errorless teaching for all mand training
errorless teaching, with the immediate trials (independent variable) and
prompting (zero second delay) of a generalization programming was
correct response. Thus, the implemented across clinicians, activities,
participant’s response was much more with a follow-up maintenance check.
likely to be correct but not independent. Furthermore, the current study
Subsequent mand training trials recorded the number of requests for
implemented a 5 second delay with information using “how” and the
necessary prompting for no response or number of times the acquired
an incorrect response. The later information was used to access the
procedure may have encouraged more reinforcer. Lastly, an internal measure
independence but allowed the learning of methodological fidelity was used to
of errors while not promoting fluency promote treatment integrity.
because of the latency between clinician
prompting and participant response. Methods
While the current literature Participants
provides programming for potential Melvin was a 12-year-old boy
generalization with manding for with autism and moderate intellectual
information (Carnett & Ingvarsson, disabilities. Both he and his brother, Bob
2016; Ingvarsson & Hollobaugh, 2010; (to be discussed later) participated in
Lechago et al., 2010; Taylor & Harris, this study. Melvin was diagnosed at the
1995; Shillingsburg, Bowen, Valentino, age of 18 months by a clinical
& Peirce, 2014), the single study psychologist. He attended an approved
teaching “how” (Shillingsburg & private school serving youths with
Valentino, 2011) has limited autism and other behavioral needs.
generalization potential. These last Anecdotally, Melvin was very vocal in
authors recommend using multiple that his mother reported that he
THE JOURNAL OF SPECIAL EDUCATION APPRENTICESHIP, 6(1) 4

habitually requests others to present attended the neighborhood high school,


him with activities that he was capable where he was in a self-contained Life
of accessing himself. He often whined Skills class. Anecdotally, Bob was not
for others to start a movie, access a very vocal, in that he gestured instead
website, etc. and tantrummed when his of vocalizing his needs, but used one or
request was not granted. After his two word requests occasionally. His
behavior escalated, he often did not mother described him as a ‘problem
attempt to access his reinforcer. Melvin solver.’ He attempted to find ways to
has had no direct instruction in verbal access his reinforcers rather than
behavior. request assistance from others. It
Bob, Melvin’s brother, was a 17- appeared that he chose not to interact
year-old young man with autism and with others besides his brother and
moderate intellectual disabilities. This mother. Please see Table 1 for the
information was provided by a recent participants’ VB results across selected
medical evaluation. Bob was diagnosed areas.
at the age of three by a psychiatrist. He

Table 1
VB-MAPP Results Across Selected Areas
________________________________________________________________________
Participant Mand Listener Response Motor Imitation Independent Play___
Melvin 5.0 8.0 3.5 6.0
Bob 5.0 7.0 3.0 6.0
________________________________________________________________________

Materials would be of functional use and high


Both participants, as reported by interest to their children. If either
their parents, watch preferred programs participant appeared unmotivated at
on computer screens and/or televisions the presentation of the activities
when alone or with others. Two (walking away, engaging in another
potentially reinforcing activities for each activity, or demonstrating escape
participant were identified by the behavior), then the contrived EO would
participants’ parents. For mand have been determined to be not
training, an essential component was sufficiently motivating. This did not
removed (Table 2) in order to teach him occur, as all novel stimuli appeared to
how to request information to complete motivate (physically gesturing/naming
the activity. For the generalized, activity, staring, emitting sounds and/or
nontraining activities, five were jumping up and down) the participants’
identified (Table 3). These activities engagement.
were also identified by the participants’
parents with the intention that they
THE JOURNAL OF SPECIAL EDUCATION APPRENTICESHIP, 6(1) 5

Table 2
Tasks Involved for Teaching the Mand, “How?”
Activity Contrived EO Errorless Teaching Participant Response
iPad muted program “How to make it work?” unmutes program
and shows how to unmute
TV remote unable to work “How to make it work?” manipulates remote
and shows how to use

Table 3
Generalized Manding
Activity Contrived EO Clinician response
Computer Can’t play computer Clinician shows how to manipulate mouse
iPad Paused program Clinician unpauses
Computer Can’t play computer Clinician connects mouse
Video game Can’t play game Clinician plays game
Computer Disconnected monitor Clinician connects monitor

Clinicians staircase. This staircase connected the


Five clinicians implemented the basement to the family’s living room.
procedures of this study: 2 female and 3 Throughout the study, some family
male. Four completed an introductory members, therapeutic support staff
course in Applied Behavior Analysis and (TSS), or friends were present in the
received training regarding intervention upstairs living room while each
procedures while the fifth was the participant was involved in the
university instructor. Schedules rotated basement. Background noise was not
so that the participants did not work consistent during the study and the
with the same clinician two days in a variations in ambient noise may have
row in order to program and observe allowed for unaccounted variables to
any generalization effects across people. interfere with participant performance.
Setting Response Measurement
All sessions took place in the The dependent variables were
family’s home and were conducted the independent cumulative responses
individually for each participant. The (“How?”) of the participants during
primary clinician sat or stood in full view nontraining scenarios and the
(within 5 to 7 feet) of the participant cumulative number of times that the
during the intervention. The secondary participant used that information to
clinician (for data collection purposes) access their reinforcer. A response was
was in the participant’s view but not as scored as independent if the participant
close as the primary (approximately 10- vocally manded “How?” within 5
15 feet). The topography of the room seconds of the clinician contriving the
was a furnished basement where a EO, e.g. from the time the video game
computer and television were by a wall was paused until the response of
approximately 12 feet opposite a “How?,” the time from the computer
THE JOURNAL OF SPECIAL EDUCATION APPRENTICESHIP, 6(1) 6

mouse was disabled to “How?,” etc. For multiplying by 100%. With a second
utilizing the information, a response observer being present for 74% of the
was scored as occurring if the trials, treatment integrity was 100%.
participant responded to the clinician Please see Appendix A.
prompting to use the acquired
information to resume the activity. Research Design
Interobserver Agreement An A-B design across conditions
A second observer was present (“How?” scenarios) was implemented.
during 74% of trials. Interobserver Baseline (A) was followed by the
agreement (IOA) was calculated by introduction of training (B) for two
dividing the number of agreements by conditions. Three additional
the sum of agreements and nontraining conditions (similar to
disagreements between the primary baseline, no treatment was
and secondary clinicians and multiplying implemented) were included to
by 100%. An agreement was defined as determine if trained skills had
both the primary and secondary generalized. If manding for information
clinicians agreeing that the participant’s occurred prior to training in any of the
response was a mand for information nontraining activities, training was not
using the word, “how.” A disagreement implemented for those activities. For
was defined as one clinician interpreting the five identified nontraining activities
the response differently than his/her for both participants, no manding for
counterpart. Please see Appendix B. information occurred prior to training.
IOA was 100%.
Treatment Integrity Experimental Procedures
Task analyses were completed Assessment.
for all training and nontraining The Verbal Behavior Milestones
procedures. Treatment integrity was Assessment and Placement Program
calculated through the use of the (VB-MAPP) (Sundberg, 2008) was used
Treatment Integrity Checklist to ensure across four areas; mand, listener
treatment fidelity. The secondary response, motor imitation, and
clinician checked “Yes” if s/he agreed independent play. The VB-MAPP
that the primary clinician-implemented attempts to provide a representative
task was accurate to the study’s sample of a participant’s existing verbal
outlined methods and “No” if there was skills. The levels of the instrument
deviance, i.e. too long of a delay during correspond to the verbal abilities of
errorless teaching, not capturing EO for typically developing at different ages;
the participant prior to presentations, Level 1 (0-18 months), Level 2 (18-30
providing a prompt during nontraining months), and Level 3 (30-48 months).
sessions, etc. Anything deviating from The selected VP-MAPP areas were
the stated methods was marked as “No” identified because of their relation to
by the secondary clinician. Treatment the methodological tasks involved. The
integrity was calculated by subtracting participants’ behavior was being trained
the “No” tasks from the total number of to request information (mand) to access
tasks available for that activity and information. They would potentially be
THE JOURNAL OF SPECIAL EDUCATION APPRENTICESHIP, 6(1) 7

able to respond to and apply this immediately prompted (errorless


information (listener response) to teaching = zero second delay) with the
access reinforcers. Although not being clinician saying “How?,” once per trial.
specifically trained in motor imitation, if When the participant echoed “How?,”
they were able to physically imitate the then the clinician provided verbal (told
clinicians (motor imitation), they may into which port to plug the unconnected
have been able to access the reinforcers mouse, which button to push, etc.) and
without requesting information. gestural prompts (demonstrated by
Independent play was assessed to pointing) to resume the activity. This
understand the participants’ occurred on every training trial during
ability/duration to engage in reinforcers the study with no transfer (fading
without the mediation of others. prompts) taking place. An identical
According to the selected VB-MAPP procedure was then implemented for
areas, both participants’ manding and the second reinforcing teaching activity.
motor imitation abilities were at the After the two training sessions were
beginner level (Level 1), while listener presented to each participant (one for
responding and independent play were each activity) in random order, the five
assessed to be at the intermediate level nontraining sessions commenced and
(Level 2). Please see the following table data collected. Nothing else was done
for results. to increase the probability of an echoic
Baseline. Baseline data were response: No words were pronounced
collected across the two training with a special emphasis or different
activities. Before any training sessions tone of voice. The contrived EO was
commenced, the participants were intended to be the only stimuli
presented with their respective occasioning the mand for information.
reinforcing activities (Table 2) that Nontraining Session. Similar to
required information to fully access, but the training procedure, the clinician
none was provided, to determine if they engaged in the preferred activities
were able to request information to (Table 3), in full view of the participants.
access it or physically enable the After the participant was motivated to
activities. Each activity was presented participate, the activity was interrupted
once per trial in a random order. (contrived EO) for which information
Training began after there were no was needed to resume/access it. As
responses for four consecutive trials of previously stated, a response was
baseline across all activities at the scored as independent if the participant
beginning of the study. vocally manded “How?” within 5
Training Session. The clinician seconds of the clinician contriving the
engaged in the preferred activity (Table EO, e.g. from the time the video game
2), in full view of the participant. After was paused until the response of
the participant demonstrated that he “How?,” the time from the computer
was motivated to participate as mouse was disabled to “How?,” etc. He
previously noted, he was presented was then vocally and gesturally
with access to the activity (handing him presented with the information needed
the iPad and remote accordingly) and to access his reinforcer. A latency of
THE JOURNAL OF SPECIAL EDUCATION APPRENTICESHIP, 6(1) 8

responding greater than 5 seconds or activity to be resumed but not how this
none at all was not scored as study’s methods instructed him. He
independent. If a non-independent would mand “Play bowling, please.
response was scored, then that specific Show the movie, etc.” As delineated in
activity was terminated. For utilizing the Methods section, these requests
the information, a response was scored were not granted. Data for the training
as independent if the participant trials are not displayed, as the target
responded to the clinician prompting to behavior occurred on 100% of trials, due
use the acquired information to resume to errorless teaching. This behavior
the activity, e.g. plugged in the cannot be considered as independent,
computer mouse, turned the video however, as zero-second delay
game back on, etc. Trials ended when prompting was used.
all five nontraining activities were Despite mastery criteria not
offered to the participant, regardless of being met for the two activities
whether or not they manded for manipulating the computer mice for
information regarding them. Mastery Bob, the training sessions were
criteria were independent responses terminated. These two activities
scored across all nontraining activities required that Bob directly come in close
three times consecutively. proximity to the clinician(s), down from
Maintenance. Approximately the stairs, and sit at the computer at the
three weeks after mastery criteria had opposite wall. Across the 34 trials, Bob
been met for manding “How?,” seldom ventured off the steps: He stood
maintenance data were collected. With at the base of the steps approximately
no additional training being provided, 2-3 times and for durations under a
the participants were presented with minute. Consequently, his sitting at the
the nontraining activities to determine if computer and manipulating the
they have retained the skills necessary unfamiliar and unconnected mice was
to access them. One trial was not possible. Thus, potential
conducted for each of the five activities. generalization to these two untrained
activities could not be determined. The
Results three activities where generalization did
As the data indicate (please see occur were able to be completed at a
Figures 1 and 2), Bob achieved mastery distance from the clinician(s). The iPad
criteria for 3 out of 5 nontraining was brought to him on the stairs where
activities for manding for information he manded for information and used
while Melvin mastered all five. For that information to resume the activity.
applying the manded-for information, The video game responses occurred in a
data suggest that Bob engaged in this similar fashion. For the computer
behavior (albeit at a slower rate of monitor being disconnected, Bob
acquisition) for 2 out of 3. Melvin’s data watched the monitor and manded for
suggest that he applied the manded-for information from afar. Being at a
information to access 4 out of the 5 distance, he was not in a position to
nontraining activities. Throughout the utilize this acquired information.
study, Melvin would mand for an
THE JOURNAL OF SPECIAL EDUCATION APPRENTICESHIP, 6(1) 9

Independent "How?" mands


Maintenance
20 Baseline Mand Training Accessed reinforcer using acquired
15 information
10
5 iPad Video
0 Training

20
15
10
TV Remote
5
0
Training

20
15
10
5 Unknown
Cumulative Responses

0 Computer Mouse

20
15
10
5 iPad Volume
0 Disabled

20
15
10
5
Disconnected
0
Computer Mouse

20
15
10
5 Video Game
0 Disabled

20
15
10
5 Computer Monitor
0 Disconnected
1 3 5 7 9 11 13 15 17 19 21 23 25 27 29 31 33 34

Trials
Figure 1. Bob’s cumulative number of mands for information using “How?” and
number of times he applied that information to access that reinforcer for each activity.
THE JOURNAL OF SPECIAL EDUCATION APPRENTICESHIP, 6(1) 10

Baseline Mand Training Independent "How?" mands Maintenance


20 Accessed reinforcer using acquired information
15
10 iPad Video
5
0 Training

20
15
10 TV Remote
5 Training
0

20
15
10
5 Unknown
0 Computer Mouse
Cumulative Responses

20
15
10 iPad Volume
5 Disabled
0

20
15
10 Disconnected
5 Computer Mouse
0

20
15
10
Video Game
5
0 Disabled

20
15
10
5 Computer Monitor
0 Disconnected
1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 19 20 21 22 23 24 25 26 27 28 29 30 31 32

Trials

Figure 2. Melvin’s cumulative number of mands for information using “How?” and the
number of times he applied that information to access that reinforcer for each activity.
THE JOURNAL OF SPECIAL EDUCATION APPRENTICESHIP, 6(1) 11

Discussion more receptive and engaging with the


General Discussion activities when the clinician was at a
Before mastery criteria were distance. Originally, mastery criteria
met, Melvin often tacted the contrived were identified when independent
aspect of the activities during the trials, responses were scored across
i.e. “They’re broken!” and “It doesn’t nontraining activities three times
work!” Although it was beyond the consecutively. However, Bob did not
procedures of this study, future efforts approach (place himself in physical
could employ a tact-to-mand transfer proximity, 2-3 feet, to access the
procedure across operants to teach activities) two of the nontraining
manding for information. A similar activities. As previously noted, more
echoic-to-mand transfer procedure pairing may address this obstacle to
could be considered for Bob, as he instruction. Additionally, it is possible
frequently echoed the vocal stimuli of that baseline may have extinguished
the activities. responding in that repeated
Bob’s mother also reported that presentations of the reinforcing activity
he frequently sits on the steps when his with no delivery of that activity may
father works on the computer. Bob’s have reduced the effectiveness of that
unwillingness/inability to work in close and similar activities as a reinforcer. In
proximity and/or previous history of any within-subject design, however,
staying on the stairs when adults were baseline should not be disregarded.
in the basement may have Future studies could ensure that the
compromised the ability to determine experimental activities have a history of
generalization effects. As previously reinforcement prior to withholding
mentioned, Bob often manded for reinforcement during baseline, though a
information to resume the computer historically dense schedule of
activities with the mice from afar but reinforcement may decrease the
did not attempt to manipulate them. saliency as a reinforcer. Perhaps pairing
Future studies could expand the the clinician’s presence with the
operational definition of the target participants’ reinforcers may encourage
behavior to observe more sensitive compliance despite reinforcement being
generalization effects. withheld during baseline.
As per the participants’ mother, Melvin did behave as his mother
“Bob tended to be a problem solver and described before the commencement of
Melvin repeatedly vocalized his needs the study in that he did vocalize for
that which he could obtain himself.” stimuli that he may have been able to
Her perceptions did appear to typify the access himself. As previously
behavior of the participants. As mentioned, if his responses differed
evidenced by shying away or cringing from the operational definition of the
when closely approached, Bob did not target behavior, “How?” it was not
seem to want to participate with the reinforced. While some activities saw
clinician-presented reinforcers. He was more robust responding (iPad and
THE JOURNAL OF SPECIAL EDUCATION APPRENTICESHIP, 6(1) 12

computer monitor), his responses were achieving 60%, it can be believed that
generally consistent suggesting his the target behavior was generalized
previous tact-mand “It’s broken” was across people. The schedules of the
extinguished for these activities and the clinicians rotated so that the
target behavior of manding for participants did not work with the same
information increased. Lastly, it clinician two days consecutively to
appeared that Melvin’s utilization of the program for generalization across
manded for information increased as he people. Figures 1 and 2 suggest that
attempted on his last two trials to generalization across clinicians occurred
problem-solve and resume the for both participants because of the
interrupted activities. While manding semi-consistent responding. In other
“How?” before the clinician’s prompts, words, the same clinician implemented
he independently attempted to activate the experimental procedures every 4-5
the unfamiliar mouse and connect the trials. If the participants manded for
disabled mouse. information only to certain clinicians,
For Melvin, 3 of the 5 their data should have suggested an
nontraining activities reflected increase and then a plateau equivalent
consistent manding-for information to clinician rotation. While there were
(albeit late responding for the unknown some plateaus in vocalizing “How?,” it
computer mouse). While it can be appears that (disabled video game for
supposed that some activities were Bob in trials 19-21 and 25-27 for Melvin)
more salient than others, another there were no trends which might
possible explanation for the variation in suggest greater responding to a
responding was competing reinforcers. particular clinician. There may not be
Melvin engaged in self-stimulating enough use of the acquired information
behavior with string, lint, etc. On those responding data to determine to make
trials where he had possession of these any claims about generalization effect.
articles and was engaging in self- Recording generalization probes
stimulation, it was more challenging to across multiple items and contexts is a
capture MO. To ensure that strength of this study, as well a
appropriate levels of MO are present in conducting maintenance probes. As
order to conduct this type of training previously mentioned, the is a paucity
and better encourage consistent of research regarding the generalized
responding, competing/distracting manding for information. Maintenance
reinforcing activities should be probes also allowed the conclusion as to
eliminated or minimized. whether the manding will be
There appeared to be no trend maintained post intervention.
with any individual clinician potentially Adequate IOA and treatment integrity
affecting the responding of the checks also strengthened reliability and
participants. The participants validity.
responded or did not respond Establishing Operation
comparatively across clinicians. As EO was determined by making
Melvin achieved mastery criteria for eye contact, motioning towards, and/or
100% of the activities with Bob repeating the name of the reinforcing
THE JOURNAL OF SPECIAL EDUCATION APPRENTICESHIP, 6(1) 13

stimulus. Thus, there was no preferred activity and in the presence of


measurement of intensity for EO, only the relevant MO, it should reinforce
whether it was present or not. The asking the “how” question. The fact
participants may have been more or less that the participants often did not
motivated on some trials but the engage in the activity after responding
methods of this study were not sensitive “how” casts some doubt on whether
enough to reflect this variable. It could these activities were reinforcing or
be expected in this situation that the whether the participants were
target behavior would be more likely to motivated to access the activities at the
occur if the participant was motivated time. Although "how" responding
to respond. However, motivation is not began to be emitted by both
an ‘all or nothing’ variable. Future participants, it can’t be determined if
studies could consider measuring EO on the response is under the control of the
a Likert scale, e.g. 0 = did not look at EO for information to access the
activity, 1 = looked at activity, 2 = activity. Indeed, the nature of the
looked and motioned to activity, 3 = “how” mand for information suggests
looked, motioned, and walked to that its consequence should over time
activity, etc. Those trials where abolish the relevant EO. This should be
manding for information and/or true unless the tasks are constantly
applying that information does not changing. One would expect the
occur while motivation is scored to be participants to learn how to solve the
high should reevaluate the methods. problem themselves (e.g., connect the
Conversely, if the target behavior occurs computer mouse without being told
while the utilized Likert scale suggests how) over repeated trials. When the
low motivation should also reevaluate participants know how to solve the
the methods. Melvin’s responses were problem, the relevant EO is no longer in
staggered in that 3 of the 5 activities place, and they should no longer mand
reflect consistent incorrect or no “how.” It is possible that the vocal
responding preceded and followed by response “How?” was more
correct manding for information. This appropriately classified as an echoic
may be related to competing reinforcers under partial contextual control of the
or lack of appropriate EO for interruption. Future efforts could
responding. provide opportunities to engage in
It is a possibility that the “how” activities in which the information is not
responses were under discriminative needed and observe if the "how"
control rather than EO, i.e. the stimuli, response is not emitted during those
training setting, therapist, or some trials. This could provide support for EO
other stimulus came to evoke control of the responses. Furthermore,
responding. If the responses were it is possible that the participants simply
actually functioning as mands for needed more trials to learn to engage in
information, that “information” should the responses independently).
serve a dual function: It should set the The activities were not covert as
occasion for the response (e.g., enabling to when they were going to be
the iPad) that enables access to the interrupted (contrived EO). Perhaps
THE JOURNAL OF SPECIAL EDUCATION APPRENTICESHIP, 6(1) 14

when the participants saw the activity addressed by providing the


was going to be interrupted, it may have corresponding type of prompt.
served as an additional discriminative Limitations and Future Research
stimulus to request information. A consideration for future
However, this could hardly be studies should be the availability of the
considered independent, as there would reinforcers. During the parent interview
be multiple stimuli controlling the before the commencement of the study
response. Additionally, it may not be to determine potential reinforcers,
functional in an applied setting. Future computer activities were identified as
studies could consider being overt with especially salient for both participants.
the contrived EO and transferring to While this did appear to be accurate,
covert presentations which could both participants had non-contingent
possibly promote both acquisition and access before and after every trial.
encourage independence. Thus, a participant could essentially not
Language Assessment Association respond, wait for the clinician to leave,
How did the participants and indicate to the attending staff or
respond relative to their VB-MAPP family member what he wanted access
(Sundberg, 2008) subtest assessments? to the computer. If the participants
The study’s methods attempted to have were denied access to the computer,
the participants acquire the ability to the acquisition of the target behavior
mand for information and investigate may have occurred and more quickly.
the application of that information and Perhaps the results may have
did not individually instruct across been different if a formal preference
operants. Thus, relating participant assessment was used to identify the
performance to their subtest tasks, as opposed to anecdotal input
assessments is speculative. The limited from the participants’ parents. Having
outcomes could be a function of the these data on whether the identified
participants’ limited mand repertoires activities functioned as reinforcers
(Level 1, VB-MAPP). According to the would better support their
VB-MAPP, manding for information consideration for mand training.
exceeds what can be expected from As previously stated, when
Level-1 mand performance. It could be teaching “How?” mands for
reasonable to estimate that the higher a information, the MO should be lost
VB-MAPP assessment, the more likely a when the participant has the
participant would acquire the target information. In other words, once the
behavior. Since both Melvin and Bob’s information was given, the activities
assessment suggested that their Lister should no longer be “nontraining”
Responding ability was higher than their activities. Treatment Integrity (see
Motor Imitation, vocal prompts may Appendix B) suggests that the outlined
have been more likely to encourage experimental methods were
acquisition. Future research could implemented with fidelity (100%). Why
conduct similar assessments and ensure didn’t “How?” responding plateau and
that areas of strength (Listener the use of the acquired information to
Responding versus Motor Imitation) be resume the activity increase? As the
THE JOURNAL OF SPECIAL EDUCATION APPRENTICESHIP, 6(1) 15

previous paragraph stated, the A limitation of this study was the


participants had access to nontraining simultaneous initiation for both training
activities after the clinicians finished the activities of the independent variable.
sessions. Perhaps the session/clinician Multiple treatment interference
was negatively reinforcing in that the (Cooper, Herron, & Heward, 2007) may
participants responded “How?” to finish have occurred for a participant’s
the sessions. Considering that “How?” behavior being influenced by the effects
was acquired (in most scenarios) before of one or the other training procedures.
utilizing any information, it is a Because both variations of the IV were
possibility. Future research could commenced simultaneously, the more
produce the manipulation without its effective (if either) training activity
being caused by the clinician, e.g. cannot be determined. The IV (training
programming the iPad to lock after a activities) was not implemented during
minute of inactivity, TV to sleep after baseline. As in errorless teaching, a zero
one minute, etc. This method could be second delay prompt was
more naturalistic, less contrived, and simultaneously provided for both
not be attributed to the clinician. training scenarios of the IV. Thus, it
Furthermore, when the TV sleeps during cannot be determined which scenario of
viewing, the clinician could initiate the the IV (iPad or TV remote) could have
trial by saying, “Oh, Melvin, the TV is been more effective. Displaying the
asleep. Turn it back on.” target behavior occurred on 100% of
Melvin occasionally manded trials for the IV, as can be expected.
appropriate requests “Play bowling, Because whole-word prompting was
please. Show the movie, etc.,” but used, this very method of errorless
these were not reinforced. Considering teaching encourages correct responses.
that Melvin’s manding was assessed to As previously mentioned, in multiple
be at Level 1 of the VB-MAPP, future baseline designs, the independent
studies could provide a prompt when a variable is not applied to the next
functionally and socially appropriate activity until the previous setting has
non-target mand was emitted. changed. Future research could delay
Future efforts could allow for the commencement of additional
more pairing the clinicians with the training activities (IV) to determine
preferred stimuli of the participants. treatment effects of those already
Bob may have been capable in begun. Additionally, future efforts
displaying this generalized skill but implementing errorless teaching should
unwilling. Impaired social interaction is fade prompts. A zero second delay was
a significant characteristic of autism. used across all trials. To encourage
Whether it is deficient or absent, independence, a transfer procedure
increasing a person with autism’s ability where prompts are systematically
to interact with others is a common delayed to 1, 2, 3 seconds, etc. to none
component of instruction. While being provided, could be implemented.
generalization training methods were The data suggest that the
paramount, more pairing may have participants’ behavior may not have
remedied this potential limitation. been as reinforced by the secondary
THE JOURNAL OF SPECIAL EDUCATION APPRENTICESHIP, 6(1) 16

reinforcer of information as much as the indiscriminately used vocal, gestural, or


resumption of the reinforcing activity a combination of both prompts in
itself. It could be that the “how” response to the participants’ manding
response became part of a chain whose “How?.” The participants may have
terminal reinforcer was access to the responded better to one type. Perhaps
reinforcer, and possibly have functioned those trials where the participants
as a mand for the activity, rather than utilized the manded-for information
for information. For Melvin, the data from the clinician’s vocal prompt may
indicate that he utilized the manded-for have suggested more independence
information to access 4 out of 5 of the than those requiring a gestural one.
mastered nontraining activities. For the Additionally, a directive (mand) may
fifth, he did not utilize the manded-for have been better to give to the
information at all suggesting that he participant after requesting “How?”
may not have been manding for rather than labeling (tacting) how to
information but requesting the activity access the activity. In other words, tell
itself. The results are similar for Bob in him to do it rather than tell him what to
that of the three nontraining activities do. Future efforts should utilize
mastered, he utilized the manded-for consistent levels of prompting and fade
information for only two. Perhaps they accordingly.
may not have understood the While there are potential
information provided or did not have methodological improvements to be
the listeners’ repertoire to use the made, this study has potential value for
information. Future research could only clinicians and teachers to teach people
provide gestural and/or vocal prompts with autism to request and apply
on how to access the reinforcers rather information. Teaching participants how
than directly enabling the resumption of to apply manded-for information could
the reinforcing activity. Furthermore, potentially increase independence.
the manded for information should be Going beyond the two participants with
easy to utilize by ensuring that a autism in the current study, this type of
participant possesses the prerequisite self-sufficiency training has utility for
skills necessary for task completion. those dependent on others for adaptive
Melvin did not utilize the information to behavior. In teaching “how?” to access
plug in the disconnected computer information and directly reinforcing that
monitor because he may not have behavior with that information,
possessed the fine motor skills. independence could be encouraged
Another limitation with this across people with disabilities and
study was the inconsistency with levels settings.
of prompting. Clinicians

References Analysis of Verbal Behavior, 2, 233-


Carnett, A., & Invarsson, E.T. (2016). 241.
Teaching a child with autism to Catania, A.C. (1998). Learning, 4th ed.
mand for answers to questions using Upper Saddle River, NJ: Prentice
a speech-generated device. The Hall.
THE JOURNAL OF SPECIAL EDUCATION APPRENTICESHIP, 6(1) 17

Cooper, J.O., Heron, T.E., & Heward, Michael, J. (1993). Establishing


W.L. (2007). Applied Behavior operations. The Behavior Analyst, 16,
Analysis, 2nd ed. Upper Saddle River, 191-206.
NJ: Prentice Hall. Shillingsburg, M.A., Bowen, C.N.,
Endicott, K., & Higbee, T.S. (2007). Valentino, A.L., & Peirce, L.E. (2014).
Contriving motivating operations to Mands for information using “who”
evoke mands for information in and “which” in the presence of
preschoolers with autism. Research establishing and abolishing
in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 1, operations. Journal of Applied
210-217. Behavior Analysis, 47, 136-150.
Jerome, J., Frantino, E.P., & Sturmey, P. Shillingsburg, M.A., Valentino, A.L.,
(2007). The effects of errorless (2011). Teaching a child with autism
learning and backward chaining on to mand for information using
the acquisition of Internet skills in “how?” The Analysis of Verbal
adults with developmental Behavior, 27, 179-184.
disabilities. Journal of Applied Shillingsburg, M.A., Valentino, A.L.,
Behavior Analysis, 40, 185-189. Bowen, C.N., Bradley, D., &
Lechago, S.A., Carr, J.E., Grow, L.L., Love, Zavatkay, D. (2011). Teaching
J.R., & Almason, S.E. (2010). Mands children with autism to request
for information generalize across information. Research in Autism
establishing operations. Journal of Spectrum Disorders, 5, 670-679.
Applied Behavior Analysis, 43, 381- Skinner, B.F. (1957). Verbal behavior.
395. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.
Lechago, S.A., Howell, A., Caccavale, Sundberg, M., Loeb, M., Hale, L., &
M.N., & Peterson, C.W. (2013). Eigenheer, P. (2002). Contriving
Teaching “how?” mand-for- establishing operations to teach
information frames to children with mands for information. The Analysis
autism. Journal of Applied Behavior of Verbal Behavior, 18, 15-29.
Analysis, 46, 1-11. Sundberg, M.L. (2008). Verbal Behavior
Libby, M.E., Weiss, J.S., Bancroft, S., & Milestones Assessment and
Ahearn, W.H. (2008). A comparison Placement Program. Concord, CA:
of most-to-least and least-to-most AVB Press.
prompting on the acquisition of Tayor, B.A., & Harris, S.L. (1995).
solitary play skills. Behavior Analysis Teaching children with autism to
in Practice, 1(1), 37-43. seek information: Acquisition of
Michael, J. (1982). Distinguishing novel information and
between discriminative and generalization of responding.
motivational functions of stimuli. Journal of the Experimental Analysis
Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 28, 3-14.
of Behavior, 37, 149-155. Terrace, H. (1963). Discrimination
Michael, J. (1988). Establishing learning with and without "errors."
operations and the mand. The Journal of the Experimental Analysis
Analysis of Verbal Behavior, 6, 3-9. of Behavior, 6, 1-27.
THE JOURNAL OF SPECIAL EDUCATION APPRENTICESHIP, 6(1) 18

Weeks, M., & Gaylord-Ross, R. (1981). Williams, G., Donley, C.R., & Keller, J.W.
Task difficulty and aberrant behavior (2000). Teaching children with
in severely handicapped students. autism to ask questions about
Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, hidden objects. Journal of Applied
14, 449-463. Behavior Analysis, 33, 627-630.

Author Contact Information:


Christopher Bloh, bloh@kutztown.edu
Christopher Scagliotti, cscag529@live.kutztown.edu
Sarah Baugh, sbaug011@live.kutztown.edu
Megan Sheenan, megansheenan4@outlook.com
Shane Silas, bigdrummershane@yahoo.com
Nicole Zulli, nzull567@live.kutztown.edu
THE JOURNAL OF SPECIAL EDUCATION APPRENTICESHIP, 6(1) 19

Appendix A
Treatment Integrity Form
I. Teaching Procedures Yes No
1. Was MO captured for teaching task #1? Was the setting manipulated to
ensure that the participant was motivated to engage in the activity as
evidenced by reaching for or looking at item/activity?
2. For teaching task #1, was errorless teaching employed (zero second
prompt)? iPad manipulation
3. Was MO captured for teaching task #2? Was the setting manipulated to
ensure that the participant was motivated to engage in the activity as
evidenced by reaching for or looking at item/activity?
4. For teaching task #2, was errorless teaching employed (zero second
prompt)? TV remote incapacitated
II. Generalized Procedures Yes No
1. Was MO captured for generalized procedure #1? Was the setting
manipulated to ensure that the participant was motivated to engage in the
activity, as evidenced by reaching for or looking at item/activity? unfamiliar
mouse
2. Was MO captured for generalized procedure #2? iPad manipulation

3. Was MO captured for generalized procedure #3? unconnected mouse

4. Was MO captured for generalized procedure #4? unfamiliar video game


controller
5. Was MO captured for generalized procedure #5? unconnected computer
monitor

Appendix B
Interobserver Agreement
Generalized Tasks Yes No
1. Successfully manipulated unfamiliar computer mouse

2. Successfully manipulated iPad

3. Successfully connected mouse to computer

4. Successfully manipulated unknown video game controller

5. Successfully powered computer monitor

You might also like