Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 11

Journal of Personality Assessment

ISSN: 0022-3891 (Print) 1532-7752 (Online) Journal homepage: https://www.tandfonline.com/loi/hjpa20

What Lies Beyond the Superordinate Trait


Perfectionism Factors? The Perfectionistic Self-
Presentation and Perfectionism Cognitions
Inventory Versus the Big Three Perfectionism Scale
in Predicting Depression and Social Anxiety

Silvia Casale, Giulia Fioravanti, Laura Rugai, Gordon L. Flett & Paul L. Hewitt

To cite this article: Silvia Casale, Giulia Fioravanti, Laura Rugai, Gordon L. Flett & Paul L. Hewitt
(2019): What Lies Beyond the Superordinate Trait Perfectionism Factors? The Perfectionistic Self-
Presentation and Perfectionism Cognitions Inventory Versus the Big Three Perfectionism Scale in
Predicting Depression and Social Anxiety, Journal of Personality Assessment

To link to this article: https://doi.org/10.1080/00223891.2019.1573429

Published online: 25 Mar 2019.

Submit your article to this journal

View Crossmark data

Full Terms & Conditions of access and use can be found at


https://www.tandfonline.com/action/journalInformation?journalCode=hjpa20
JOURNAL OF PERSONALITY ASSESSMENT
https://doi.org/10.1080/00223891.2019.1573429

What Lies Beyond the Superordinate Trait Perfectionism Factors? The


Perfectionistic Self-Presentation and Perfectionism Cognitions Inventory Versus
the Big Three Perfectionism Scale in Predicting Depression and Social Anxiety
Silvia Casale1, Giulia Fioravanti1, Laura Rugai2, Gordon L. Flett3, and Paul L. Hewitt4
1
Department of Health Sciences, Psychology and Psychiatry Unit, University of Florence, Florence, Italy; 2Department of Experimental and
Clinical Medicine, University of Florence, Florence, Italy; 3Department of Psychology, York University, Toronto, Ontario, Canada; 4Department
of Psychology, University of British Columbia, Vancouver, British Columbia, Canada

ABSTRACT ARTICLE HISTORY


Extreme perfectionism has been linked with distress and dysfunction. This association is reflected Received 11 September 2018
by the recent development of the Big Three Perfectionism Scale (BTPS), which has superordinate Accepted 16 December 2018
trait-based scales that assess 3 broad elements—rigid, self-critical, and narcissistic perfectionism.
We examined psychometric features of the BTPS as well as the links between the BTPS and
indexes of distress. A sample of 602 undergraduates completed the BTPS, the Perfectionistic Self-
Presentation Scale, the Perfectionism Cognitions Inventory, and measures of social anxiety and
depression. Support was obtained for the psychometric qualities of the BTPS. All 3 superordinate
trait factors were associated with social anxiety and depression. Analyses also established that
rigid perfectionism, self-critical perfectionism, and narcissistic perfectionism are associated with
perfectionistic cognitions and perfectionistic self-presentation. In addition, the results of a series of
regression analyses established that perfectionistic self-presentation and perfectionistic cognitions
accounted for significant unique variance in distress beyond the variance attributable to rigid, nar-
cissistic, and self-critical perfectionism. Overall, our results suggest that the BTPS has significant
promise as a predictor of various forms of dysfunction, but the cognitive and self-presentational
aspects of the perfectionism construct are also uniquely relevant and not redundant with the
BTPS superordinate trait factors

Classic accounts of perfectionism have warned us that per- measure of perfectionism with elements that clearly are in
fectionism can be destructive (Blatt, 1995) and it tends to keeping with the emphasis on the role of perfectionism in
keeps people in turmoil (see Pacht, 1984). These warnings PD and dysfunction. We describe this new measure, the Big
have largely been confirmed by research that has linked per- Three Perfectionism Scale (BTPS), and its subscales that fit
fectionism with several negative outcomes. Much of this with conceptualizations of PD and dysfunction (i.e., rigid
research has focused on perfectionism and negative adjust- perfectionism, narcissistic perfectionism, and self-critical
ment outcomes such as suicidality (see, e.g., Smith et al., perfectionism). First, however, we provide an overview of
2018) but perfectionism has also been linked with a host of some earlier conceptualizations of perfectionism.
negative health outcomes (Molnar, Sirois, Flett, Janssen, &
Hewitt, 2018). Recent evidence also suggests that levels of
perfectionism might be on the rise (see Curran & Hill, Trait-based conceptualizations of perfectionism
2019) and a recent study found that about 3 in 10 adoles-
cents have some form of maladaptive perfectionism (Sironic The two most widely recognized and empirically supported
& Reeve, 2015). conceptualizations of perfectionism were independently pro-
Various authors have argued and researchers have dem- posed by Frost and colleagues (1990) and by Hewitt and
onstrated empirically that perfectionism is also linked with Flett (1991). Frost and associates developed the Frost
personality disorder (PD) and dysfunction (see Sherry, Multidimensional Perfectionism Scale (Frost MPS) to assess
Hewitt, Flett, Lee-Baggley, & Hall, 2007). Another recent six components of perfectionism (see Frost, Marten, Lahart,
study of trait perfectionism confirmed links with various & Rosenblate, 1990). The Frost MPS was designed originally
indexes of PD and it led the authors to conclude that perfec- to assess four personal aspects of perfectionism (i.e., per-
tionism is “one prominent target for refining PD sonal standards, concern over mistakes, doubts about
treatments” (Dimaggio et al., 2015, p. 328). The catalyst for actions, organization) and two familial aspects of perfection-
this investigation was the development of a new trait ism (i.e., unrealistic parental expectations and parental

CONTACT Silvia Casale silvia.casale@unifi.it Department of Health Sciences, Psychology and Psychiatry Unit, University of Florence, via di San Salvi 12,
Florence, Italy.
ß 2019 Taylor & Francis Group, LLC
2 CASALE ET AL.

criticism). Overall, four factors (i.e., excessive concern over perfectionism construct and trait perfectionism by introduc-
mistakes, doubts about actions, parental criticism, and par- ing a new conceptualization and measure of dispositional
ental expectations) have some obvious negative implications perfectionism, the BTPS, that assesses three higher order
in terms of vulnerability to various forms of distress, includ- global factors: rigid perfectionism, self-critical perfectionism,
ing depression and social anxiety. It should be noted that and narcissistic perfectionism. Rigid perfectionism refers to
these four Frost MPS subscales have often been combined in the belief that being perfect is essential and self-worth is
a composite factor typically called maladaptive evaluative based on absolute self-imposed standards. Rigid perfection-
concerns (e.g., Kawamura, Hunt, Frost, & DiBartolo, 2001). ism incorporates the self-oriented and personal standards
As Frost and his associates were constructing their meas- perfectionism found in earlier conceptualizations along with
ure, Hewitt and Flett (1990) also began exploring different the tendency to base one’s sense of self-worth on attaining
dimensions of perfectionism; this work resulted in the devel- personal perfectionism. The second broad BTPS factor, self-
opment of another multidimensional perfectionism measure critical perfectionism, consists of a combination of concern
that we refer to as the Hewitt and Flett Multidimensional over mistakes, doubts about action, self-criticism, and
Perfectionism Scale (see Hewitt & Flett, 1991, 2004). This socially prescribed perfectionism. It is clearly relevant to an
45-item inventory taps three trait dimensions—self-oriented, understanding of PD and dysfunction in terms of its
other-oriented, and socially prescribed perfectionism. emphasis on an abiding sense of the self as inadequate, wor-
Whereas self-oriented perfectionism entails a need to be per- thy of doubt, and unable to live up to high expectations.
fect and relentless striving for personal standards of perfec- Finally, the BTPS narcissistic perfectionism factor advances
tion, other-oriented perfectionism involves a demand that the growing literature on perfectionism and narcissism by
other people are perfect. Socially prescribed perfectionism is presenting the case that narcissism and perfectionism
the most deleterious trait component in that it is the dimen- become fused in some people. It is composed of four facets:
sion that is related most consistently to maladjustment (see other-oriented perfectionism, narcissistic hypercriticism (i.e.,
Hewitt & Flett, 1991). Socially prescribed perfectionism is harsh devaluation of others and their imperfections), entitle-
the degree to which perfection is seen as being demanded ment (i.e., a belief to be entitled to special treatment due to
by others. It can involve a sense of helplessness and hope- being perfect), and grandiosity (i.e., an idealized view of
lessness due to the sense that success will result in higher, oneself as superior or perfect). Flett and Hewitt (2016) noted
more extreme demands. that the BTPS should be useful in distinguishing neurotic
Hewitt and Flett added to their conceptualization of the perfectionists who are preoccupied with falling short of per-
perfectionism construct by creating the comprehensive fection from narcissistic perfectionists who are defensive but
model of perfectionism (see Hewitt, Flett, & Mikail, 2017). believe they are capable of being perfect.
This comprehensive model goes beyond trait perfectionism To our knowledge, the association between perfectionism
by also incorporating cognitive perfectionism and perfec- and symptoms of psychological distress has never been
tionistic self-presentation (PSP). These additional compo- investigated by adopting this new tripartite conceptualization
nents are described later. First, however, we describe the proposed by Smith, Saklofske, et al. (2016). Accordingly, one
BTPS and the conceptualization it is based on. overarching goal of this study was to evaluate the extent to
which rigid perfectionism, self-critical perfectionism, and
narcissistic perfectionism as assessed by the BTPS relate to
A new conceptualization of trait perfectionism indexes of psychological distress. However, we also sought
Some researchers argue that that the different forms, aspects, to examine how these broad traits relate to other compo-
and subordinate dimensions of perfectionism can be organ- nents of the perfectionism construct, which are
ized in two superordinate factors: perfectionistic strivings described next.
and perfectionistic concerns (see Cox, Enns, & Clara, 2002;
Dunkley, Blankstein, Halsall, Williams, & Winkworth, 2000).
Beyond trait perfectionism: Perfectionistic
Perfectionistic strivings—also called personal standards per-
cognitions and perfectionistic self-presentation
fectionism—capture forms, aspects, and subordinate dimen-
sions of perfectionism reflecting the tendency to demand Flett, Hewitt, Nepon, and Besser (2018) highlighted that
perfection of oneself and propensity to hold unrealistic high research and theory that focus solely on trait perfectionism
personal standards of performance. In contrast, perfectionis- could be missing key elements of the perfectionism con-
tic concerns—also called evaluative concerns perfection- struct that might be relevant for the explanation of psycho-
ism—involves traits reflecting concerns over making pathology development. Flett, Hewitt, Blankstein, and Gray
mistakes, the perception of others as demanding perfection, (1998) postulated the need to consider perfectionism from a
and feelings of discrepancy between one’s high standards cognitive perspective. They created the Perfectionism
and actual performance. Typically these two superordinate Cognitions Inventory (PCI) to assess individual differences
factors are captured by combining subscales from the meas- in automatic thoughts related to the need to be perfect and
ures developed by Hewitt and Flett and by Frost and falling short of perfection. The PCI reflects the premise that
his colleagues. perfectionists who sense a discrepancy between their actual
Recently, Smith, Saklofske, Stoeber, and Sherry (2016) self and the ideal self, or between their actual level of goal
sought to integrate recent advances concerning the attainment and high ideals will tend to experience automatic
PERFECTIONISM AND DISTRESS 3

thoughts that reflect perfectionistic themes (see Flett et al., symptoms (see, for a review, Smith, Sherry, et al., 2016).
1998). It is believed that perfectionists with high levels of Perfectionistic concerns confer vulnerability to depressive
perfectionism cognitions are especially susceptible to nega- symptoms through negative social situations (e.g., hostile
tive affect in the form of depression about failure to attain interactions), social cognitions (e.g., perceiving others as
perfection in the past, as well as in the form of anxiety uncaring), and maladaptive coping (e.g., avoidance), whereas
about the likelihood of failing to attain perfection in the perfectionistic strivings might confer vulnerability to depres-
future. The PCI has now been used in more than 40 studies sive symptoms in the presence of ego-involving achievement
with more than 50 samples. Collectively, these investigations stressors (Hewitt, Flett, & Ediger, 1996). Similarly, perfec-
have yielded extensive evidence suggesting that it taps a tionistic concerns and perfectionistic strivings have been
unique cognitive element of perfectionism that is not redun- found to be positively associated with social anxiety (e.g.,
dant with trait perfectionism, nor is it redundant with gen- Nepon, Flett, Hewitt, & Molnar, 2011) as social anxiety has
eral measures of negative automatic thoughts (also see Flett, been conceived as the doubt about one’s ability to achieve a
Hewitt, & Nepon, 2016). desired impression on others (Leary, 1983). In this study, we
Hewitt et al. (2003) also argued that whereas many per- examined the associations that the new proposed conceptu-
fectionists are focused on being perfect, a subset of perfec- alization of trait perfectionism had with measures of depres-
tionists are more heavily invested in seeming perfect. sion, social anxiety, and social phobia.
Whereas trait perfectionism reflects a dispositional need to Third, we examined the extent to which perfectionistic
be perfect, PSP reflects the need to appear perfect as well as automatic thoughts and PSP were associated with the three
the strategies employed to gain this aim. In other words, higher order global factors introduced by Smith, Saklofske,
PSP is a form of impression management that involves self- et al. (2016). This aspect of our investigation was designed,
presentational attempts to create an image of perfection in in part, to test and expand the BTPS nomological network.
public situations (Hewitt et al., 2003). PSP involves three It was anticipated that the pattern of associations would
distinct dimensions: perfectionistic self-promotion (i.e., pro- establish that there is a cognitive element to rigid perfection-
actively promoting a perfect image), nondisclosure of imper- ism and that rigid perfectionism and narcissistic perfection-
fections, and nondisplay of imperfections (avoiding verbal ism would be comparable to self-critical perfectionism in
disclosure of imperfections and concerns over behavioral terms of being associated with an interpersonal style of
displays of imperfection, respectively). Research on children, needing to seem perfect. Narcissistic perfectionists are
adolescents, and adults has shown that PSP is associated expected to actively promote a public image of perfection
uniquely with distress, disorder, and dysfunction in ways because their belief to be entitled to special treatment is a
that are not captured by trait measures of perfectionism and consequence of their belief to be perfect. Rigid perfectionism
other broad trait dimensions (see Hewitt et al., 2011; Hewitt is also expected to be associated with PSP dimensions
et al., 2003). For example, Sherry et al. (2007) found that all because the belief that being perfect is essential might lead
three facets of PSP were associated with dysregulation and to the need to actively promote one’s own perfection in
dissociality and these links went beyond the variance attrib- public situations as well as the avoidance of disclosure and
utable to broad trait dimensions representing the Five- demonstration of imperfections.
Factor Model. Finally, we tested our hypothesis that perfectionistic cog-
A key test with any new measure is to establish its incre- nitions and PSP dimensions would explain additional vari-
mental validity. Regarding the Perfectionistic Self- ance in predicting depression and social anxiety after
Presentation Scale (PSPS), Hewitt et al. (2003) demonstrated controlling for the three perfectionism higher order global
that this inventory went beyond the trait perfectionism factors. We sought to establish the need to go beyond mal-
adaptive, dysfunctional trait perfectionism by also assessing
dimensions postulated by Hewitt and Flett (1991) and Frost
perfectionistic cognitions and a potentially destructive self-
et al. (1990) in terms of being able to predict key outcomes.
presentational style.
Flett and Hewitt (2015) summarized extensive evidence
showing that PSP is particularly relevant when predicting
social anxiety and models of the link between perfectionism Method
and social anxiety should incorporate this focus on a need
to be perfect. Participants
A convenience sample of 602 undergraduates (51.16% female;
Goals of this study M age ¼ 22.21 years, SD ¼ 2.49) from the University of
Florence was recruited by two female assistants at the end of
This study had four main goals. First, we evaluated the psy- the lectures. The study was announced as an investigation
chometric characteristics of the BTPS in a sample of partici- about the association between certain dispositional factors and
pants of emerging adults from outside of North America. distress. Participants completed paper-and-pencil question-
Second, as noted earlier, we conducted one of the first tests naires in the classroom settings. No formative credits or
of the degree to which the three higher order global factors remunerative rewards were given. Study procedures were
in the BTPS relate to indexes of distress. Perfectionistic con- designed in accordance with the European research ethical
cerns and, to a lesser degree, perfectionistic strivings have guidelines and approved by the Director of the Department of
already shown to be positively associated with depressive Psychology. All the students identified themselves as White.
4 CASALE ET AL.

Table 1. Measures used in this study.


Authors Measure Construct Scales
Smith, Saklofske, Big Three Perfectionism Trait perfectionism Rigid Perfectionism (RP): Rigid insistence that one’s own performance
et al. (2016) Scale (BTPS) must be perfect
Self-Critical Perfectionism (SCP): Evaluative concerns perfectionism
Narcissistic Perfectionism (NP): Belief to be perfect and superior
to others
Hewitt et al. (2003) Perfectionistic Self-Presentation Perfectionistic self- Perfectionistic self-promotion: Proactive promotion of a perfect image
Scale (PSPS) presentation Nondisplay of imperfections: Concerns over behavioral display of
imperfections
Nondisclosure of imperfections: Avoidance of verbal disclosure of
imperfections
Flett et al. (1998) Perfectionism Cognition Perfectionism cognitions Perfectionism Cognition Inventory total score: Automatic thoughts
Inventory (PCI) related to the need to be perfect
Peter et al. (2012) Social Interaction Anxiety Distress in terms of anxiety SIAS–6 total score: Anxiety in social interactions
Scale (SIAS–6)
Peter et al. (2012) Social Phobia Scale (SPS–6) Distress in terms of SPS–6 Total Score: Anxiety in performance situations
social phobia
Radloff (1977) Center for Epidemiologic Studies Distress in terms of depres- CES–D total score: Current level of depressive symptoms
Depression Scale (CES–D) sive symptoms

Measures comprised of three subscales: perfectionistic self-promotion,


nondisplay of imperfection, and nondisclosure of imperfec-
The measures used in this study are briefly described in
tion. Perfectionistic self-promotion was captured with a 10-
Table 1.
item subscale (e.g., “I try always to present a picture of
perfection”); higher scores on this subscale indicated higher
Big Three Perfectionism Scale levels of perfectionistic self-presentational style characterized
The Italian version (Di Fabio, Saklofske, & Smith, in press) of by the need to brashly promote oneself as perfect to others.
the BTPS was used. The BTPS is a 45-item self-report ques- Nondisplay of imperfection was measured with 10-item sub-
tionnaire designed to measure three higher order global fac- scale (e.g., “It would be awful if I made a fool of myself in
tors (rigid perfectionism, self-critical perfectionism, front of others”), with higher scores on this subscale indicat-
narcissistic perfectionism) and 10 lower order perfectionism ing a higher level of perfectionistic self-presentational style
facets (self-oriented perfectionism and self-worth contingen- characterized by the need to avoid behavioral demonstra-
cies, loading on rigid perfectionism; concern over mistakes, tions of one’s imperfection. Nondisclosure of imperfection
doubts about actions, self-criticism, and socially prescribed was assessed with a 7-item subscale (e.g., “Admitting failure
perfectionism loading on self-critical perfectionism; other-ori- to others is the worst possible thing”); high scores on this
ented perfectionism, hypercriticism, grandiosity, and entitle- scale indicated high levels of perfectionistic self-presenta-
ment loading on narcissistic perfectionism). The Rigid tional style characterized by the need to avoid verbal disclo-
Perfectionism scale is composed of 10 items (e.g., “I strive to sures of one’s imperfection. Participants responded to the
be as perfect as possible”). Rigid perfectionism has two fac- items of the three subscales using a 7-point scale ranging
ets—self-oriented perfectionism and self-worth contingencies. from 1 (strongly disagree) to 7 (strongly agree). Evidence
The Self-Critical Perfectionism scale is composed of 18 items supports both the reliability and the validity of the PSPS
(e.g., “When I make a mistake, I feel like a failure”) that tap (Hewitt et al., 2003). In the Italian sample, the PSPS total
four facets of perfectionism typically regarded as maladap- score and PSPS scales showed adequate internal consistency
tive—concern over mistakes, doubts about actions, self-criti- reliability estimates, and both the dimensionality analyses
cism, and socially prescribed perfectionism. The third scale of and exploratory structural model supported the original
the BTPS is Narcissistic Perfectionism, which is composed of three-factor structure for PSPS items (Borroni et al., 2016).
17 items (e.g., “It bothers me when people don’t notice how
perfect I am”). Narcissistic perfectionism is made up of four
facets—other-oriented perfectionism, hypercriticism, entitle-
Perfectionism Cognition Inventory
ment, and grandiosity. Participants are instructed to respond
The PCI (Flett et al., 1998) is a 25-item measure that focuses
to each item using a 5-point rating scale ranging from 1
on the frequency of thoughts involving themes of perfection
(strongly disagree) to 5 (strongly agree). Higher scores indicate
and imperfection (i.e., the degree of cognitive activity) that
higher levels of the assessed dimension. Confirmatory factor
has occurred during the previous week: It is a measure that
analysis (CFA) supported the three-dimensional structure of
specifically identifies the automatic thoughts associated with
the BTPS. Additionally, the Italian version showed good
perfectionism and needing to be perfect (e.g., “I should be
internal consistency and validity (Di Fabio et al., in press).
perfect,” “Why can’t things be perfect?,” “No matter how
much I do, it’s never enough”). Participants responded to
Perfectionistic Self-Presentation Scale the items using a 5-point rating scale ranging from 0 (not at
PSP was assessed with the Italian version (Borroni et al., all) to 4 (all the time). Higher scores on this scale indicated
2016) of the PSPS (Hewitt et al., 2003), a 27-item measure higher levels of perfectionist thoughts.
PERFECTIONISM AND DISTRESS 5

Social Interaction Anxiety and Social Phobia Scale Three hierarchical regression analyses were conducted to
The Social Interaction Anxiety (SIAS–6) and the Social Phobia determine if PSP and perfectionistic cognitions (Step 2) pre-
Scale (SPS–6; Peters, Sunderland, Andrews, Rapee, & Mattick, dict social interaction anxiety, social phobia, and depressive
2012) were used to measure social anxiety. Whereas the symptoms over and above perfectionism traits as assessed by
SIAS–6 measures anxiety experienced while initiating or main- the BTPS (Step 1). For all the statistical tests we assumed a
taining social interactions (e.g., “I have difficulty talking with 5% significance level.
other people”), the SPS–6 assesses anxiety experienced in per-
formance situations in which the individual might be scruti-
Results
nized (e.g., “I get nervous that people are staring at me as I
walk down the street”). Participants responded to the items of Construct validity: Confirmatory factor analysis
the two subscales using a 5-point Likert scale ranging from 0
CFA revealed that the original factor structure of the BTPS
(not at all characteristic or true of me) to 4 (extremely character-
(10-first order factors and three global factors) produced a
istic or true of me). Higher scores on the SIAS–6 scale indicated
good fit to these data, v2(932) ¼ 3684.88, p < .001, RMSEA ¼
higher levels of social interaction anxiety and higher scores on
.07, 90% CI [.060, .072], CFI ¼ .96, SRMR ¼ .07. The path dia-
the SPS–6 scale indicated higher levels of social phobia. The
gram and the standardized path coefficients are shown in
Social Interaction Anxiety Scale and Social Phobia Scale–12
Figure 1. Inspection of factor loadings showed that these were
showed adequate psychometric properties (Peters et al., 2012).
high (i.e., > 50) for all items, all of which were significant at
the .001 level as well as the estimated correlations among
Center for Epidemiologic Studies Depression Scale errors. Factor loadings of first-order factors ranged from .76 to
The Italian version (Fava, 1983) of the Center for .96 for self-oriented perfectionism; from .64 to .98 for self-
Epidemiologic Studies Depression Scale (CES–D; Radloff, worth contingencies; from .85 to .96 for concern over mistakes;
1977) is a 20-item self-report scale that measures current from .87 to .99 for doubts about action; from .91 to .99 for
level of depressive symptomatology, with emphasis on the self-criticism; from .53 to .86 for socially prescribed perfection-
affective component (e.g., “I felt hopeful about the future”). ism; from .58 to .76 for other-oriented perfectionism; from .59
Participants respond to the CES–D by rating each item in to .67 for hypercriticism; from .62 to .75 for entitlement; and
terms of the frequency that each mood or symptom from .54 to .56 for grandiosity. Factor loadings for second-
occurred “during the past week” on a 4-point Likert scale, order factors ranged from .98 to .99 for rigid perfectionism;
ranging from 0 (none of the time) to 3 (most of the time). from .65 to .95 for self-critical perfectionism; and from .78 to
Higher scores indicate higher levels of depressed mood. .91 for narcissistic perfectionism. Factor loadings ranged from
The Italian versions of the PCI and the SIAS–6 and SPS–6 .53 to .99 for all facets. This outcome attests to the psychomet-
were obtained using a back-translation method in which one ric properties of the BTPS.
bilingual translator translated the test from the source lan-
guage to the target language (Italian). A second translator,
Correlations between the three higher order factors and
without having seen the original test, translated the new ver-
indexes of depression and social anxiety
sion of the test back to the source language. The original and
the back-translated versions of the tests were then compared BTPS descriptive statistics and zero-order correlations with
and judgments were made about their equivalence. depression and social anxiety are shown in Table 2. All three
BTPS scales were associated with depression, social anxiety,
and social phobia, but robust links were found between
Data analyses
these distress measures and self-critical perfectionism, with
The structure of the BTPS was examined using CFA. The correlations ranging from .48 to .56. The correlations linking
criteria for assessing overall model fit were mainly based on the three BTPS scores with the measures of depression and
practical fit measures: the ratio of chi-square to its degree of social anxiety are revealing given the paucity of research on
freedom (S–B v2/df), the comparative fit index (CFI; Bentler, these dimensions and measures of psychological distress.
1995), the Tucker–Lewis Index (TLI; Tucker & Lewis, 1973), Parenthetically, it should be noted that the BTPS concern
and the root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA; over mistakes subscale and the doubts about action subscale
Steiger, 1990). For the ratio of chi-square to its degrees of were the lower order perfectionism facets with the most
freedom (S–Bv2/df), values less than 3 were considered to robust associations with depression, social interaction anx-
reflect fair fit (Kline, 2005). We considered CFI and TLI val- iety, and social phobia (see Table S.1).
ues of .90 and above to reflect fair fit (Bentler, 1995). For
the RMSEA, values equal to or less than .08 were considered
Correlations between perfectionistic automatic thoughts
to reflect adequate fit (Browne & Cudeck, 1993).
and perfectionistic self-presentation with the three
Pearson’s correlations were used to examine the associa-
higher order BTPS factors
tions between the higher order perfectionism traits and
indexes of distress (i.e., social interaction anxiety, social pho- Scores on the PCI were correlated robustly with scores on
bia, and depressive symptoms) as well as with perfectionistic the BTPS measures of rigid perfectionism and self-critical
automatic thoughts and PSP. perfectionism, and, to a lesser extent, with narcissistic
6 CASALE ET AL.

Figure 1. Big Three Perfectionism Scale (BTPS): Items and factors loadings from the confirmatory factor analysis. Note. SCP ¼ Self-Critical Perfectionism; RP ¼ Rigid
Perfectionism; NP ¼ Narcissistic Perfectionism; SOP ¼ Self-Oriented Perfectionism; SWC ¼ Self-Worth Contingencies; COM ¼ concern over mistakes; DAA ¼ Doubts
About Action; SC ¼ Self-criticism; SPP ¼ Socially Prescribed Perfectionism; OOP ¼ Other-Oriented Perfectionism; HC ¼ Hypercriticism; ENT ¼ Entitlement;
GRAN ¼ Grandiosity; C1 to C4 ¼ Concern over mistakes Items 1 to 4; D1 to D5 ¼ Doubts About Actions Items 1 to 5; S1 to S4 ¼ Self-criticism Items 1 to 4; SP1 to
SP4 ¼ Socially-Prescribed Perfectionism Items 1 to 4; SO1 to SO5 ¼ Self-Oriented Perfectionism Items 1 to 5; SW1 to SW5¼ Self-Worth Contingencies Items 1 to 5;
O1 to O5 ¼ Other-Oriented Perfectionism Items 1 to 5; H1 to H4 ¼ Hypercriticism Items 1 to 4; E1 to E4 ¼ Entitlement Items 1 to 4; G1 to G4 ¼ Grandiosity Items
1 to 4.

Table 2. Descriptive statistics and zero-order correlations among BTPS higher order factors, PSPS, PCI, and indexes of distress.
Variables M SD a 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10
BTPS: Higher order
1. Rigid perfectionism 21.91 8.88 .91 —
2. Self-critical perfectionism 44.75 14.14 .93 .67 —
3. Narcissistic perfectionism 33.61 10.50 .89 .60 .48 —
PSPS
4. Self-promotion 35.05 12.02 .85 .68 .54 .50 —
5. Nondisplay 37.54 11.95 .84 .46 .61 .34 .67 —
6. Nondisclosure 21.82 7.38 .71 .38 .44 .29 .50 .52 —
7. PCI 40.53 18.53 .92 .62 .61 .45 .53 .46 .38 —
8. SIAS–6 4.58 4.34 .79 .29 .48 .26 .33 .46 .38 .33 —
9. SPS–6 4.99 4.72 .83 .22 .47 .19 .28 .43 .34 .33 .60 —
10. CES–D 19.24 11.26 .92 .29 .56 .25 .28 .35 .29 .47 .40 .36 —
Note. N ¼ 602. BTPS ¼ Big Three Perfectionism Scale; PSPS ¼ Perfectionistic Self-Presentation Scale; PCI ¼ Perfectionism Cognitions Inventory; Self-promo-
tion ¼ perfectionistic self-promotion; Nondisplay ¼ nondisplay of imperfections; Nondisclosure ¼ nondisclosure of imperfections; SIAS–6 ¼ Social Interaction

Anxiety Scale; SPS–6 ¼ Social Phobia Scale; CES–D ¼ Center for Epidemiologic Studies Depression Scale.
p < .01.

perfectionism (Table 2). Considering the 10 lower order per- regressions, we checked for multicollinearity. There was no
fectionism facets, the PCI was found to be strongly corre- indication of multicollinearity, as tolerance statistics were
lated with self-criticism and self-oriented perfectionism (for well above .2 and variance influence factors were well below
detailed information see Table S.1). 10 (see Bowerman & O’Connell, 1990).
Analyses of the PSPS facets showed that perfectionistic The regression results are displayed in Table 3. Although
self-promotion was associated with all BTPS measures, and the first set of BTPS trait predictors accounted for substan-
its strongest association was with rigid perfectionism. In tial variance in all instances, the inclusion of the PSP dimen-
contrast, the self-critical perfectionism measure was associ- sions and the PCI scales (Step 2) accounted for an
ated most robustly with the need to avoid imperfections and additional statistically significant amount of variance.
the need to avoid disclosing imperfections. Regarding the Regarding social interaction anxiety scores, the second block
lower order perfectionism dimensions, perfectionistic self- of predictors accounted for an additional 7% of the variance
promotion was associated most strongly with self-oriented beyond the 23% already explained by the three BTPS meas-
perfectionism; perfectionistic nondisclosure was correlated ures; significant individual predictors were the PSPS nondis-
play and nondisclosure facets (see Table 3).
robustly with scores on the concern over mistakes
As for the other anxiety measure, social phobia was asso-
BTPS subscale.
ciated with high levels of rigid perfectionism, self-critical
perfectionism, nondisplay and nondisclosure of imperfec-
Prediction of depression and social anxiety scores tion, and perfectionistic cognitions. The three higher order
perfectionism dimensions explained 23% of the variance in
Next, a series of hierarchical regression analyses was con- social phobia scores and the inclusion of other dimensions
ducted. First, however, before running the hierarchical
PERFECTIONISM AND DISTRESS 7

Table 3. Hierarchical regression analyses predicting social interaction anxiety, social phobia, and depression total scores.
SIAS–6 social CES–D
interaction anxiety SAS–6 social phobia depressive symptoms
Predictors
DR2 b sr2 DR2 b sr2 DR2 B sr2
Step 1 .23 .23 .33
BTPS: Higher order
Rigid perfectionism –.09 –.06 –.19 –.13 –.18 –.12
Self-critical perfectionism .51 .37 .58 .43 .66 .49
Narcissistic perfectionism .07 .06 .03 .03 .04 .05
Step 2 .07 .05 .05
BTPS: Higher order
Rigid perfectionism –.14 –.08 –.25 –.15 –.27 –.16
Self-critical perfectionism .32 .20 .40 .25 .56 .35
Narcissistic perfectionism .05 .04 .01 .01 .01 .01
PSPS
Self- promotion –.03 –.02 –.02 –.00 –.01 –.01
Nondisplay .23 .15 .19 .12 –.03 –.02
Nondisclosure .15 .13 .12 .09 .06 .04
PCI .04 .03 .12 .08 .29 .21
Total R2 .30 .29 .38
Note. N ¼ 602. SIAS–6 ¼ Social Interaction Anxiety Scale; SPS–6 ¼ Social Phobia Scale; CES–D ¼ Center for Epidemiologic Studies Depression Scale; BTPS ¼ Big

Three Perfectionism

Scale; PSPS ¼ Perfectionistic Self-Presentational Scale; PCI ¼ Perfectionism Cognitions Inventory; sr ¼ squared correlations.

p < .05. p < .01. p <.001.

in the second block predicted an additional 5% of prescribed perfectionism, self-criticism) that have been
explained variance. shown to be highly maladaptive in many studies. What
In terms of predicting levels of depression, the final might be considered surprising is that in terms of the zero-
model explained 38% of the variance in depressive symp- order correlations, small but significant links were found
toms scores. It was found that the PCI accounted for signifi- between rigid perfectionism and all of the distress measures
cant unique variance beyond the significant variance in (.21 < r < .30). This is intriguing in that rigid perfectionism
CES–D scores accounted for by rigid perfectionism and self- includes an assessment of self-oriented perfectionism, and
critical perfectionism. there have been some suggestions in the past that self-ori-
ented perfectionism is actually adaptive. In this regard, the
results of our regression analyses are illustrative because
Discussion when rigid perfectionism was considered along with other
Our study provided strong support for the soundness of the perfectionism measures, the regression results showed that
BTPS from a psychometric point of view. The proposed rigid perfectionism was associated with less distress. Perhaps
model provided a good fit to the data and high levels of this outcome reflects the strong links that self-criticism has
with self-oriented perfectionism and the self-worth contin-
internal consistency were detected. The fit of the model with
gency. Clearly, it would be difficult at the conceptual level
three higher order global factors and 10 lower order perfec-
to make the case that a dimension conceptualized as “rigid
tionism facets was good and very similar to that found by
perfectionism” would be adaptive given the established role
Smith, Saklofske, et al. (2016). Moreover, bivariate correla-
of rigidity as an indicator of personality dysfunction and
tions were in line with expectations, thus providing support-
our zero-order correlations. However, these results are very
ive evidence of the validity of the BTPS in addition to the
much in keeping with statements about the “perils of parti-
findings already provided by Smith, Saklofske, et al. (2016).
aling” and concerns that have been expressed about how
Taken together, these results confirm that this new measure statistical procedures could provide a misleading view of the
might be an informative assessment of multidimensional true nature of perfectionism dimensions (for a discussion,
perfectionism and it seems well-suited to further investiga- see Hill, 2014; Stoeber & Gaudreau, 2017).
tion of perfectionism and PD given the explicit BTPS As was anticipated, both cognitive perfectionism and PSP
emphasis on rigid perfectionism and narcissistic perfection- were from moderately to strongly associated with the three
ism. Our results indicated that the three broad trait factors BTPS broad trait dimensions. The link that was established
are substantially intercorrelated but not to the extent that between perfectionistic cognitions and self-critical perfec-
they are redundant with each other. However, the relation- tionism was particularly robust (r ¼ .61) and it is very
ship between self-critical perfectionism and rigid perfection- much in keeping with accounts that emphasize the self-crit-
ism was quite high and this overlap merits further scrutiny ical tendencies of people who frequently experience perfec-
in future research both at empirical and conceptual levels. tionistic thoughts (see Flett et al., 1998). Our results
Distress was associated at the bivariate level with all three underscore the need to keep in mind that people with ele-
BTPS trait measures, but the most robust associations were vated levels of self-critical perfectionism are prone to experi-
found with the measure of self-critical perfectionism (.46 < r ence frequent ruminations about needing to be perfect and
< .57). This is not surprising given that the self-critical per- falling short of ideals and goals. These ruminations translate
fectionism factor is comprised of trait perfectionism facets into a tendency for the self-critical perfectionist to be chron-
(i.e., concern over mistakes, doubts about actions, socially ically focused on mistakes and shortfalls and her or his
8 CASALE ET AL.

inability to live up to extreme standards. Related research automatic thoughts. Similarly, the lack of an emphasis on
has indicated that high PCI scorers not only are self-critical perfectionistic automatic thoughts could prove particularly
and self-blaming, but also have a heightened response to problematic for cognitively based treatment interventions.
perceived criticism, and an absence of positive self-talk Although this article is based on variable-centered
(Flett, Hewitt, Whelan, & Martin, 2007). As a consequence, research, it is important to briefly consider the implications
it is not surprising that this element of the perfectionism of our results from a person-centered perspective. Consider,
construct is not conducive to positive mental health. for instance, the role that perfectionism plays in suicide. A
The correlational results also established that there is a recent comprehensive meta-analysis by Smith et al. (2018)
substantial cognitive side to narcissistic perfectionism as showed unequivocally that various trait perfectionism
assessed by the BTPS. At present, there have been few dimensions are implicated in suicidal thoughts and attempts.
attempts to consider the narcissism–perfectionism link from Unfortunately, this meta-analysis, although highly revealing,
a cognitive perspective. It is possible that narcissistic perfec- was limited because there has not been extensive research
tionism involves a more substantial tendency to focus on thus far on the role of perfectionism cognitions and PSP in
grandiose thoughts reflecting the attainment of perfection- suicidality. Our current results indicate that cognitive perfec-
ism and cognitive images of glory and superiority rather tionism and PSP play a unique role in psychological distress.
than not achieving perfection, and grandiose forms of day- When it comes to the risk of suicide, some people likely are
dreaming and mind-wandering about achieving per- in great psychological pain and are ruminating frequently
fect success. about their imperfections and failures, yet on the surface,
The moderate-to-strong correlations involving the facets they seem to be high functioning in ways that are synonym-
of PSP provided a revealing overall picture. The PSPS non- ous with PSP. Unfortunately, some people might present a
display of imperfections facet had its strongest association “front” that masks a highly negative internal dialogue and is
with self-critical perfectionism (r ¼ .61). In contrast, when very convincing to the point that no one realizes their level
it comes to narcissistic perfectionism, the strongest associ- of risk until it is too late. We have included these observa-
ation was with the PSPS self-promotion facet (r ¼ .50). To tions to underscore the fact that there are practical conse-
some extent, this can be considered evidence of the validity quences and considerations that follow from the failure to
of this new measure of narcissistic perfectionism given past go beyond trait perfectionism and take an extended view of
research that has linked perfectionistic self-promotion with the perfectionism construct.
trait narcissism (see Flett, Sherry, Hewitt, & Nepon, 2014). Clearly, these findings must be viewed within some key
Past research on PSP has shown that people with a tendency limitations. Of course, it is always possible that self-report
to portray themselves as flawless are highly invested in the data have been influenced by response bias, and it is import-
dysfunctional pursuit of self-image goals (Nepon, Flett, & ant for future research to revisit the issues addressed in this
Hewitt, 2016) and this link between self-promotion and nar- study by including informants’ ratings of perfectionism and
cissism can prove to be quite self-defeating in terms of ali- adjustment. This seems especially important when consider-
enating people and creating the kinds of social ing someone who has an extreme form of narcissistic perfec-
disconnection that have become linked with perfectionism tionism and who might lack the self-insight to be aware of
(see Hewitt et al., 2017). adjustment problems. Second, it is evident that these results
Overall, perhaps the most revealing results from this are cross-sectional and causal assumptions cannot be made.
investigation came from statistical tests showing that both Longitudinal research is needed and given that there is evi-
PSP and perfectionism cognitions are not redundant with dence that both trait perfectionism and cognitive perfection-
broad trait dimensions of perfectionism. The general pattern ism are associated with heightened stress reactivity (Flett,
of results suggested that the more defensive aspects of PSP Nepon, Hewitt, & Fitzgerald, 2016), future research would
were able to account for unique variance in social anxiety be particularly revealing if trait perfectionism, perfectionistic
outcomes, beyond the variance already accounted for by automatic thoughts, and PSP were compared and contrasted
rigid perfectionism, self-critical perfectionism, and narcissis- within the context of life stress experiences. The results
tic perfectionism. This is in keeping with initial evidence described in this study should also be examined in cross-cul-
showing that PSP plays a unique role in social anxiety in tural research that tests the generalizability of these findings.
both adolescents and adults (Flett, Coulter, & Hewitt, 2012; Finally, gender differences in the relationship between per-
Hewitt et al., 2003) and supports the call for a greater focus fectionism dimensions and distress should be examined by
on PSP in models of social anxiety and social phobia (see future research. Although gender differences have not been
Flett & Hewitt, 2014). By extension, PSP merits much more tested often in the perfectionism literature, those few avail-
consideration in conceptual models of proneness to distress able studies have yielded evidence that perfectionism might
and PD and dysfunction. Similarly, it was found that perfec- play a greater role in women’s experience of distress, espe-
tionistic cognitions accounted for unique variance in depres- cially in terms of depression (Besser, Flett, & Hewitt, 2010),
sion scores beyond the variance already accounted for by perhaps because presentation of the self is more relevant for
rigid perfectionism, self-critical perfectionism, and narcissis- females (see Pliner, Chaiken, & Flett, 1990).
tic perfectionism. Clearly, these results suggest that models In summary, this study yielded results that illustrated the
of the link between perfectionism and depression are incom- usefulness of the BTPS measure, but also showed the need
plete if they do not include an emphasis on perfectionistic to go beyond trait perfectionism measures. Most notably, we
PERFECTIONISM AND DISTRESS 9

established that rigid perfectionism, self-critical perfection- Clinical, Developmental, and Social Perspectives, 3rd ed. (pp.
ism, and narcissistic perfectionism are associated with social 159–187). London, UK: Elsevier.
Flett, G. L., & Hewitt, P. L. (2015). Measures of perfectionism. In G.
anxiety and depression, but it is also the case that cognitive
Boyle, D. Saklofske, & G. Matthews (Eds.), Measures of Personality
perfectionism and PSP account for social anxiety and and Social Psychological Constructs (Vol. 2, pp. 595–618). London,
depression in ways that go beyond broad trait perfectionism UK: Academic Press.
dimensions. Given that the perfectionism field has been Flett, G. L., & Hewitt, P. L. (2016). Still measuring perfectionism after
dominated by research from a trait perspective, our results all these years: Reflections and an introduction to the Special Issue
on advances in the assessment of perfectionism. Journal of
represent an illustration of the benefits of a more compre-
Psychoeducational Assessment, 34(7), 615–619. doi:10.1177/
hensive and balanced approach that supplements this focus 0734282916651540
with programmatic research and theory on aspects of the Flett, G. L., Hewitt, P. L., Blankstein, K. R., & Gray, L. (1998).
construct such as PSP and cognitive perfectionism. Psychological distress and the frequency of perfectionistic thinking.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 75(5), 1363–1381. doi:
10.1037//0022-3514.75.5.1363
References Flett, G. L., Hewitt, P. L., & Nepon, T. (2016). Perfectionism, worry,
and rumination in health and mental health: A review and a con-
Bentler, P. M. (1995). EQS structural equations program manual. ceptual framework for a cognitive theory of perfectionism. In F. M.
Encino, CA: Multivariate Software. Sirois & D. S. Molnar (Eds.), Perfectionism, Health, and Well-being
Besser, A., Flett, G. L., & Hewitt, P. L. (2010). Perfectionistic self-pres- (pp. 121–156). New York, NY: Springer.
entation and trait perfectionism in social problem-solving ability Flett, G. L., Hewitt, P. L., Nepon, T., & Besser, A. (2018).
and depressive symptoms. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 40, Perfectionism cognition theory: The cognitive side of perfectionism.
2123–2156. In J. Stoeber (Ed.), The Psychology of Perfectionism: Theory,
Blatt, S. J. (1995). The destructiveness of perfectionism. Implications Research, Applications (pp. 89–110). London, UK: Routledge.
for the treatment of depression. The American Psychologist, 50(12), Flett, G. L., Hewitt, P. L., Whelan, T., & Martin, T. R. (2007). The
1003–1020. Perfecionism Cognitions Inventory: Psychometric properties and
Borroni, S., Bortolla, R., Lombardi, L. M., Somma, A., Maffei, C., & associations with distress and deficits in cognitive self-management.
Fossati, A. (2016). The Italian version of perfectionistic self-presen- Journal of Rational-Emotive & Cognitive-Behavior Therapy, 25(4),
tation scale: Psychometric proprieties and its associations with 255–277. doi:10.1007/s10942-007-0055-4
pathological narcissism and adult attachment in an adult non clin- Flett, G. L., Nepon, T., Hewitt, P. L., & Fitzgerald, K. (2016).
ical sample. Personality and Mental Health, 10(2), 130–141. doi: Perfectionism, components of stress reactivity, and depressive symp-
10.1002/pmh.1328 toms. Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment, 38(4),
Bowerman, B. L., & O’Connell, R. T. (1990). Linear Statistical Models: 645–654. doi:10.1007/s10862-016-9554-x
An Applied Approach (2nd ed.) Belmont, CA: Duxbury Classic. Flett, G. L., Sherry, S. B., Hewitt, P. L., & Nepon, T. (2014).
Browne, M. W., & Cudeck, R. (1993). Alternative ways of assessing Understanding the narcissistic perfectionists among us: Grandiosity,
model fit. In K. A. Bollen & Long J. S. (Eds.), Testing structural vulnerability, and the quest for the perfect self. In A. Besser (Ed.),
equation models (pp. 136–162). Newbury Park, CA: Sage Publishing. Handbook of the psychology of narcissism: Diverse perspectives (pp.
Cox, B. J., Enns, M. W., & Clara, I. P. (2002). The multidimensional 43–66). New York, NY: Nova Science Publishers.
structure of perfectionism in clinically distressed and college student Frost, R. O., Marten, P., Lahart, C., & Rosenblate, R. (1990). The
samples. Psychological Assessment, 14(3), 365–373. doi:10.1037// dimensions of perfectionism. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 14(5),
1040-3590.14.3.365 449–468. doi:10.1007/BF01172967
Curran, T., & Hill, A. P. (2019). Perfectionism is increasing over time: Hewitt, P. L., Blasberg, J. S., Flett, G. L., Besser, A., Sherry, S. B.,
A meta-analysis of birth cohort differences from 1989 to 2016. Caelian, C., … Birch, S. (2011). Perfectionistic self-presentation in
Psychological Bulletin. 154(4), 410–429. doi:10.10137/bul0000138 children and adolescents: Development and validation of the
Di Fabio, A., Saklofske, D., & Smith, M. (in press). Big-Three Perfectionistic Self-Presentation–Junior form. Psychological
Perfectionism Scale Short Form (BTPS-SF): Primo contributo alla Assessment, 23(1), 125–142. doi:10.1037/a0021147
validazione della versione Italiana [Big-Three Perfectionism Scale Hewitt, E. L., & Flett, G. L. (1990). Perfectionism and depression: A
Short Form (BTPS-SF): First contribution to the validation of the multidimensional analysis. Journal of Social Behavior and
Italian version]. Counseling: Giornale Italiano di Ricerca e Personality, 5, 423–438.
Applicazioni. Hewitt, P. L., & Flett, G. L. (1991). The multidimensional perfectionism
Dimaggio, G., Lysaker, P. H., Calarco, T., Pedone, R., Marsigli, N., scale: Reliability, validity, and psychometric properties in psychiatric
Riccardi, I., … Paviglianiti, A. (2015). Perfectionism and personality samples. Psychological Assessment, 3(3), 464–468.
disorders as predictors of symptoms and interpersonal problems. Hewitt, P. L., & Flett, G. L. (2004). Multidimensional Perfectionism
American Journal of Psychotherapy, 69(3), 317–330. doi:10.1176/ Scale (MPS): Technical manual. Toronto, Canada: Multi-Health
appi.psychotherapy.2015.69.3.317 Systems.
Dunkley, D. M., Blankstein, K. R., Halsall, J., Williams, M., & Hewitt, P. L., Flett, G. L., & Ediger, E. (1996). Perfectionism and
Winkworth, G. (2000). The relation between perfectionism and dis- depression: Longitudinal assessment of a specific vulnerability
tress: Hassles, coping, and perceived social support as mediators and hypothesis. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 105(2), 276–280. doi:
moderators. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 47(4), 437–453. doi: 10.1037//0021-843X.105.2.276
10.1037//0022-0167.47.4.437 Hewitt, P. L., Flett, G. L., & Mikail, S. F. (2017). Perfectionism: A rela-
Fava, G. A. (1983). Assessing depressive symptoms across cultures: tional approach to conceptualization, assessment, and treatment. New
Italian validation of the CES-D self-rating scale. Journal of Clinical York, NY: Guilford.
Psychology, 39(2), 249–251. doi:10.1002/1097-4679(198303)39: Hewitt, P. L., Flett, G. L., Sherry, S. B., Habke, M., Parkin, M., Lam,
2<249::AID-JCLP2270390218>3.0.CO;2-Y R. W., … Stein, M. B. (2003). The interpersonal expression of per-
Flett, G. L., Coulter, L., & Hewitt, P. L. (2012). The perfectionistic self- fection: Perfectionistic self-presentation and psychological distress.
presentation scale—Junior form. Canadian Journal of School Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84(6), 1303–1325. doi:
Psychology, 27(2), 136–149. doi:10.1177/0829573511431406 10.1037/0022-3514.84.6.1303
Flett, G. L., & Hewitt, P. L. (2014). Perfectionism and perfectionistic Hill, A. P. (2014). Perfectionistic strivings and the perils of partialling.
self-presentation in social anxiety: Implications for assessment and International Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 12(4),
treatment. In S. Hofmann & P. DiBartolo (Eds.), Social anxiety: 302–315. doi:10.1080/1612197X.2014.919602
10 CASALE ET AL.

Kawamura, K. Y., Hunt, S. L., Frost, R. O., & DiBartolo, P. M. (2001). Sherry, S. B., Hewitt, P. L., Flett, G. L., Lee-Baggley, D. L., & Hall,
Perfectionism, anxiety, and depression: are the relationships inde- P. A. (2007). Trait perfectionism and perfectionistic self-presentation
pendent? Cognitive Therapy and Research, 25(3), 291–301. doi: in personality pathology. Personality and Individual Differences,
10.1023/A:1010736529013 42(3), 477–490. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2006.07.026
Kline, R. B. (2005). Principles and Practice of Structural Equation Sironic, A., & Reeve, R. A. (2015). A combined analysis of the frost
Modeling (2nd Ed.). New York, NY: Guilford Press. multidimensional perfectionism scale (FMPS), child and adolescent
Leary, M. R. (1983). Social anxiousness: The construct and its measure- perfectionism scale (CAPS), and almost perfect scale-revised
ment. Journal of Personality Assessment, 47(1), 66–75. doi:10.1207/
(APSR): Different perfectionist profiles in adolescent high school
s15327752jpa4701_8
students. Psychological Assessment, 27(4), 1471–1483. doi:10.1037/
Molnar, D. S., Sirois, F. M., Flett, G. L., Janssen, W. F., & Hewitt, P. L.
(2018). Perfectionism and health: The roles of health behaviors and pas0000137
stress-related processes. In J. Stoeber (Ed.), The Psychology of Smith, M. M., Saklofske, D. H., Stoeber, J., & Sherry, S. B. (2016). The
Perfectionism: Theory, Research, Applications (pp. 200–221). London, big three perfectionism scale: A new measure of perfectionism.
UK: Routledge. Journal of Psychoeducational Assessment, 34(7), 670–687. doi:
Nepon, T., Flett, G. L., & Hewitt, P. L. (2016). Self-image goals in trait 10.1177/0734282916651539
perfectionism and perfectionistic self-presentation: Toward a broader Smith, M. M., Sherry, S. B., Chen, S., Saklofske, D. H., Mushquash,
understanding of the drives and motives of perfectionists. Self and C., Flett, G. L., & Hewitt, P. L. (2018). The perniciousness of per-
Identity, 15(6), 683–706. doi:10.1080/15298868.2016.1197847 fectionism: A meta-analytic review of the perfectionism-suicide
Nepon, T., Flett, G. L., Hewitt, P. L., & Molnar, D. S. (2011). relationship. Journal of Personality, 86(3), 522–542. doi:10.1111/
Perfectionism, negative social feedback, and interpersonal rumin- jopy.12333
ation in depression and social anxiety. Canadian Journal of Smith, M. M., Sherry, S. B., Rnic, K., Saklofske, D. H., Enns, M., &
Behavioural Science, 43(4), 297–308. doi:10.1037/a0025032 Gralnick, T. (2016). Are perfectionism dimensions vulnerability fac-
Pacht, A. R. (1984). Reflections on perfection. American Psychologist, tors for depressive symptoms after controlling for neuroticism? A
39(4), 386–390. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.39.4.386 meta-analysis of 10 longitudinal studies. European Journal of
Peters, L., Sunderland, M., Andrews, G., Rapee, R. M., & Mattick, R. P.
Personality, 30(2), 201–212. doi:10.1002/per.2053
(2012). Development of a short form social interaction anxiety
Steiger, J. H. (1990). Structural model evaluation and modification: An
(SIAS) and social phobia scale (SPS) using nonparametric item
interval estimation approach. Multivariate Behavioral Research,
response theory: The SIAS-6 and the SPS-6. Psychological
Assessment, 24(1), 66–76. doi:10.1037/a0024544 25(2), 173–180. doi:10.1207/s15327906mbr2502_4
Pliner, P., Chaiken, S., & Flett, G. L. (1990). Gender differences in con- Stoeber, J., & Gaudreau, P. (2017). The advantages of partialling perfec-
cern with body and weight and physical appearance over the life- tionistic strivings and perfectionistic concerns: Critical issues and
span. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 16(2), 263–273. doi: recommendations. Personality and Individual Differences, 104,
10.1177/0146167290162007 379–386. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2016.08.039
Radloff, L. S. (1977). The CES-D Scale: A self-report depression scale Tucker, L. R., & Lewis, C. (1973). A reliability coefficient for maximum
for research in the general population. Applied Psychological likelihood factor analysis. Psychometrika, 38(1), 1–10. doi:10.1007/
Measurement, 1(3), 385–401. doi:10.1177/014662167700100306 BF02291170

You might also like