Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 13

Applied Neuropsychology: Child

ISSN: (Print) (Online) Journal homepage: https://www.tandfonline.com/loi/hapc20

Osmotic Release Oral System-Methylphenidate


Hydrochloride (OROS-MPH) versus atomoxetine
on executive function improvement and clinical
effectiveness in ADHD: A randomized controlled
trial

Yasemin Taş Torun , Yasemen Işik Taner , Esra Güney & Elvan İseri

To cite this article: Yasemin Taş Torun , Yasemen Işik Taner , Esra Güney & Elvan İseri (2020):
Osmotic Release Oral System-Methylphenidate Hydrochloride (OROS-MPH) versus atomoxetine
on executive function improvement and clinical effectiveness in ADHD: A randomized controlled
trial, Applied Neuropsychology: Child, DOI: 10.1080/21622965.2020.1796667

To link to this article: https://doi.org/10.1080/21622965.2020.1796667

Published online: 06 Aug 2020.

Submit your article to this journal

Article views: 9

View related articles

View Crossmark data

Full Terms & Conditions of access and use can be found at


https://www.tandfonline.com/action/journalInformation?journalCode=hapc20
APPLIED NEUROPSYCHOLOGY: CHILD
https://doi.org/10.1080/21622965.2020.1796667

Osmotic Release Oral System-Methylphenidate Hydrochloride (OROS-MPH)


versus atomoxetine on executive function improvement and clinical
effectiveness in ADHD: A randomized controlled trial
Yasemin Taş Torun , Yasemen Işik Taner €ney
, Esra Gu , and Elvan _Iseri
Child and Adolescent Psychiatry Department, Gazi University Medical Faculty, Ankara, Turkey

ABSTRACT KEYWORDS
Objectives: The aim of this study to compare the clinic efficacy and effects of osmotic release Attention deficit; executive
oral system-methylphenidate and atomoxetine on executive function in children and adolescents functions; treatment
with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder by a open-label, prospective, randomized con-
trolled trial.
Methods: The study was performed by 95 cases between ages 6 and 12 years who were diag-
nosed as attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) and also 40 control individuals. In this
study, Conners’ Teacher Rating Scale (CTRS) was used in order to evaluate the efficacy of the treat-
ment. Executive functions were assessed by the performance-based neuropsychological tests and
ecological behavioral rating scales. Stroop test, cancellation test, and serial digit learning test were
applied to performance based neuropsychological tests. Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive
Function tests (BRIEFs) were used as behavioral assessment scales.
Results: Among the ADHD groups, a reduction of over 40% in the CTRS subtest scores used to
evaluate the efficacy of the treatment was considered to be an improvement, and no significant
difference was found for both drugs. Both Osmotic Release Oral System-Methylphenidate
Hydrochloride (OROS-MPH) and atomoxetine (ATX) significantly improved scores in neuropsycho-
logical tests.
Conclusion: Atomoxetine and OROS-MPH treatments have shown similar efficacy in clinical recov-
ery and improvement on executive functions. However, disturbances in executive functions
observed in children with ADHD are persistent despite treatment, when compared with the con-
trol group.

Introduction been clarified yet. There are numerous studies on the effects
of atomoxetine (ATX) (Adler et al., 2014; Brown et al.,
Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) is a neuro-
2011; Faraone et al., 2005; Gau & Shang, 2010; Maziade
psychiatric disorder characterized by attention deficit, hyper-
et al., 2009; Shang & Gau, 2012; Wehmeier et al., 2011) and
activity, and impulsivity symptoms that begin in childhood methylphenidate (Biederman et al., 2011; Coghill et al.,
(American Psychiatric Association [APA], 2013). Although 2014; Fallu et al., 2006; Karakaya et al., 2006; Kiriş et al.,
the diagnosis of ADHD is based on clinical signs of atten- 2013; Konrad et al., 2005; O’Driscoll et al., 2005; Yilmaz
tion deficit and/or hyperactivity/impulsivity, hundreds of et al., 2013; Zheng et al., 2015) on executive functions, two
experimental and theoretical studies have shown that neuro- of the most commonly used drugs used in ADHD treat-
psychological deficiencies, especially executive functions, ment, but comparative studies are fewer (Bedard et al., 2015;
play a role in this disorder (Barkley, 1997; Brown, 2006). Tasdelen et al., 2015; Ni et al., 2013; Yang et al., 2012; Yildiz
Executive function (EF) is an umbrella term that encom- et al., 2011). The results of one of these comparative studies,
passes the set of higher-order processes (such as inhibitory which included 26 patients aged 8–14 diagnosed with
control, working memory, and attentional flexibility) that ADHD and under treatment with Osmotic Release Oral
govern goal-directed action and adaptive responses to novel, System-Methylphenidate Hydrochloride (OROS-MPH) or
complex, or ambiguous situations (Yildiz et al., 2011). ATX, showed significant improvement in executive func-
Neuropsychological tests are widely used in research into tions compared to pretreatment in both drugs groups. The
executive functions in ADHD (Bakar et al., 2011; Homack & study also demonstrated that OROS-MPH treatment was
Riccio, 2004; Willcutt et al., 2005). Although it is known more effective in terms of variables such as perseveration
that there are many executive function impairments that are and interference resistance as a result of the neuropsycho-
closely related to the clinical presentation of ADHD, logical test battery (Wisconsin card sorting test, visual mem-
whether these impairments persist after treatment has not ory test, Stroop test) (Yildiz et al., 2011). In another study

CONTACT Yasemin Taş Torun ysmn.ts@gmail.com Child and Adolescent Psychiatry Department, Gazi University Medical Faculty, Ankara, Turkey.
ß 2020 Taylor & Francis Group, LLC
2 Y. TAŞ TORUN ET AL.

with a similar design including 33 ADHD patients aged psychiatric disorders as defined by Diagnostic and Statistical
7–12, the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test, the Visual Auditory Manual of Mental Disorders–Fifth Edition (DSM-5) (APA,
Number Sequence Test, and the Stroop test (VANS-B) were 2013). They were assessed by clinical interviews based on
performed before and after 20 weeks of followup. The con- DSM-5 and the Kiddy Schedule for Affective Disorders and
clusion was similar to other studies, where both drug groups Schizophrenia (K-SADS) (Kaufman et al., 1997). Patients
were observed to have increased test performance, but ATX with seizures, bipolar disorder, psychotic illness, mental
treatment was more effective in terms of the auditory, ver- retardation, pervasive developmental disorder, and those
bal, and written task performances measured in VANS-B who were taking concomitant psychoactive medications
(Tasdelen et al., 2015). Another study by Yang et al. (2012) were excluded from the study. Baseline measurements
compared the efficacy of OROS-MPH and ATX on executive included medical history, the Conners’ Teacher Rating Scale
functions in 142 children with ADHD aged 7–14. In this (CTRS), a neuropsychological tests battery (Stroop test,
study, Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function- Cancelation test, the Serial Digit Learning Test), and the
Teacher–Parents (BRIEF) scales, Rey Complex Figure Test, BRIEF. The CTRS, the neuropsychological test batteries, and
Digit Span Test, Tower of Hanoi Test, verbal fluency test, the BRIEF were repeated only at week 16, while other
Trail-Making Test, and Stroop test were used to evaluate assessments were repeated at weeks 4, 8, and 12. The exam-
executive functions. As a result, all performance-based tests iners and teachers who completed Conners were blind to
including working memory, inhibition, attention shifting, the study drug (ATX, OROS-MPH). This study was con-
verbal fluency, and planning were observed to be signifi- ducted in accordance with the ethical principles of the
cantly improved in the OROS-MPH group. In the ATX Declaration of Helsinki and the Good Clinical Practice
group, a significant improvement was observed in working guidelines established at the International Conference on
memory and inhibition skills. BRIEF evaluations showed sig- Harmonization. The study protocol was approved by the
nificant improvement compared to baseline in both drug Gazi University Ethics Committee (number: 25901600-549).
groups (Yang et al., 2012). Another study compared execu- In the patient group, a total of 100 patients were random-
tive functions using Cambridge Neuropsychological Test ized to receive ATX or OROS-MPH. Treatment was initi-
Automated Battery (CANTAB) before and after treatment ated at a standard specified dose (ATX: 0.5 mg/kg/day;
with IR-MPH and ATX in adult ADHD patients. The study OROS-MPH: 18 mg/day administered as a single morning
showed that both drugs had a curative effect on executive dose) for all patients and increased to 1.2 mg/kg/day for
functions after treatment, but ATX was more effective in ATX and 36–54 mg/day for OROS-MPH based on the
terms of spatial planning (Ni et al., 2013). A follow-up study reports of the parents and clinicians at weeks 4 and 8. Of
by Bedard et al. (2015) evaluated executive functions before the 100 subjects randomized, 95 completed the study. Three
and after treatment in adolescents with ADHD, it was con- subjects who were administered ATX and two subjects who
cluded that methylphenidate treatment was more effective were administered OROS-MPH discontinued the study.
than ATX in terms of measurements that were carried out
Nausea and vomiting were reported as the reasons for dis-
using continuous performance test.
continuation in the ATX group, and chest pain and palpita-
In conclusion, although the effects of drug therapies
tions were the reasons in the OROS-MPH group. The
applied in ADHD have been shown to have improvement
dosages in the ATX group (n ¼ 46) ranged from 18 to
on many executive functions, the mechanism of this activity,
60 mg/day (mean ¼ 1.18 mg/kg/day), and the dosages in the
which executive function is affected, and the individual dif-
OROS-MPH group (n ¼ 49) ranged from 18 to 54 mg/day
ferences regarding efficacy have not been elucidated.
(mean ¼ 0.94 mg/kg/day). After psychiatric examination, 40
The aim of this study was to compare the effects of ATX
healthy volunteers aged 6–12 were included in the study as
and OROS-MPH on executive functions and evaluate their
a control group. The controls were administered the same
clinical effectiveness using performance-based neuropsycho-
neuropsychological tests battery. This was a single-blind
logical tests and ecological behavioral rating scales.
randomized controlled trial study with only blind raters due
to the different appearance of the medications.
Methods The Turkish version of the Kiddy Schedule for Affective
Disorders and Schizophrenia Present and Lifetime Version
Study design and patients
(K-SADS-PL); this scale was developed by Kaufman et al.
This prospective, randomized, open-label, follow-up study (1997). Turkish validation and reliability studies were per-
compared the efficacy and effects of ATX and OROS-MPH formed by G€ okler et al. (2004). It allows the screening of
on executive functions in children and adolescents with psychiatric diagnoses in a wide spectrum. In this study, indi-
ADHD. The study duration was 18 weeks. All subjects vidual interviews with children and one of their parents
underwent a 1–2 week screening phase to determine the per- were made to complete the scale. Patients diagnosed with
sistence and severity of their diagnostic and eligibility crite- any psychiatric disorder other than ADHD by K-SADS-PL
ria at entry. The patients were randomly assigned to were excluded.
treatment with either ATX or OROS-MPH. All measure- CTRS: This scale was developed by Conners (Conners,
ments were performed by the two authors. The patients in 1969) and revised by Goyette et al. (1978). It was adapted
this study were 6–12 years old at their initial visit and met into Turkish, and its validation and reliability studies were
diagnostic criteria for ADHD and other comorbid performed by Dereboy et al. (2007). The scale contains
APPLIED NEUROPSYCHOLOGY: CHILD 3

questions regarding the hyperactivity, attention deficit, and evaluate executive functions in ADHD before and
behavioral problems of children in school, and their teachers after treatment.
are asked to grade them. In this study, this scale was used to
evaluate the efficacy of treatment.
Stroop Test: This test was developed by Stroop (1935) Statistical analysis
and standardized for Turkish children by Kılıç et al. (2002). In this study, the data obtained from the participants were
The Stroop test measures selective attention (cards 2 and 4), analyzed using the SPSS 21 IBM software. The variables
focused attention (cards 1 and 3), and mental flexibility and were investigated using visual and analytical methods
the ability to inhibit a dominant response (card 5) by differ- (Kolmogorov–Simirnov/Shapiro Wilks test) to determine
ent subtests (Kılıç et al., 2002; MacLeod, 1992). Also, card 5 whether or not they are normally distributed. Descriptive
is believed to provide a measure of the individual’s ability to analyzes were used for sociodemographic characteristics of
inhibit stimulus-bound responses and deal with interference the groups. Mann–Whitney U and Kruskal Wallis analysis
(Homack & Riccio, 2004). The Stroop test is closely related methods were used to determine the differences in terms of
to the frontal lobe and many other cerebral regions. It pro- independent variables. Descriptives analyses were presented
vides information about several cognitive processes such as using means and standard deviations for pretreatment and
selective attention, response inhibition, interference control, posttreatment scale scores of Stroop, SDLT, and Cancelation
and input processing speed. Subtests among MPH, ATX, and Control. Levene test was
Cancelation Test (CT): This test was developed by used to assess the homogeneity of the variances. If the var-
Mesulam in 1985 and is used to measure sustained attention iances were homogeneoous, we used one-way analysis of
(Weintraub & Mesulam, 1985). CT has four subtests using variance (ANOVA); if not, we used Welch-ANOVA to
regular letters, regular shapes, irregular letters, and irregular determine the statistically significant results. An overall p-
shapes. In subtests that use letters, the letter “A” needs to be value of less than 0.05 was considered significant. When an
marked on the paper; and in subtests that use shapes, the overall significance was observed, pairwise post-hoc tests
same as all the figures shown by the examiner need to be were performed using the Tamhane T2 test. Then, for the
marked. Mesulam stated that a sensory component related treatment efficacy, we first determine the change in scale
to perceptual errors is a motor component related to the scores for MPH and ATX groups independently. Since the
detection of the stimuli and drive components that contain change of scale scores after treatment was not normally dis-
emotional features. Different studies have been shown that tributed, Wilcoxon nonparametric tests were conducted to
the test measures visual screening, response speed, and compare pretreatment and posttreatment scores. For
impulsivity. It was standardized for Turkish children by Wilcoxon analyzes, a p-value of less than 0.05 was consid-
Karakas (2004). ered to show statistically significant results.
Serial Digit Learning Test (SDLT): SDLT measures the
number of repetitions required to accurately say an index of
Results
nine numbers and therefore the learning ability. The stand-
ardization of SDLT to Turkish culture was carried out by Of the 135 participants, 95 were diagnosed with ADHD, and
Karakas (2004). the healthy group was comprised of 40 participants. A total
Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function of 49 children in the ADHD group received methylphenid-
(BRIEF): The BRIEF is an 86-item teacher and parent ques- ate treatment, while 46 children received ATX treatment.
tionnaire, with two summary index scores, an overall general There was no significant difference between the groups in
index score and eight scales intended to capture the basic terms of gender (X2 ¼ 0.379, p ¼ 0.827), hand preference
components of executive functioning (Gioia et al., 2000). It (X2 ¼ 2.271, p ¼ 0.321) or age (X2 ¼ 5.173, p ¼ 0.075). The
has eight subdomains: inhibit, shift, emotional control, initi- treatment doses used were found to be 0.94 mg/kg/day ± 0.2
ation, working memory, plan/organize, organization of in the OROS-MPH group and 1.18 mg/kg/day ± 0.08 in the
materials, and monitor. The inhibition, shift, and emotional ATX group.
control subdomains together result in an additional compos-
ite behavioral regulation index (BRI). The subdomains of
Pretreatment evaluation
initiation, working memory, plan/organize, organization of
materials, and monitor provide a composite metacognition Whether there was a difference in terms of CTRS pretreat-
index (MI). The BRI and MI results are also combined to ment scores in the ADHD group was determined using the
obtain an overall Global Executive Composite (GEC) score. Mann-Whitney U test, and no significant difference was
Reliability studies show high internal consistency and tes- observed between the groups (Table 1). The pretreatment
t–retest reliability (Gioia et al., 2002). Convergent validity performance-based neuropsychological test scores of chil-
was established with other measures of inattention, impul- dren with ADHD (OROS-Methylphenidate and ATX group)
sivity, and learning skills in clinical ADHD populations were compared to the control group, and the performance
(Isquith & Gioia, 2000). The standardization for the Turkish scores of the patients were found to be significantly lower
population was carried out by Bakar et al. (2011). In this than the control group. There was no significant difference
study, we administered the parent and teacher forms of the between the drug groups in terms of test performances
Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function (BRIEF) to (Tables 2 and 3). There was no significant difference
4 Y. TAŞ TORUN ET AL.

Table 1. Pretreatment scores of CTRS. Based on this result, it can be concluded that both drugs
Sequence had similar efficacy on ADHD core symptoms, attention
Subtests Group N Mean sum U p
deficit, hyperactivity, and behavioral problems.
CTRS-hyperactivity OROS-MPH 49 50.43 2471 1008 0.373
Atomoxetine 46 45.41 2089
This study adopted two types of EF measures, perform-
CTRS-inattention OROS-MPH 49 47.94 2349 1124 0.982 ance-based neuropsychological tests and ecological behavioral
Atomoxetine 46 48.07 2211 rating scales. The performance-based neuropsychological tests
CTRS-behavior problems OROS-MPH 49 49.47 2424 1055 0.590
Atomoxetine 46 46.43 2136
were more specific for the components of EF, although they
Notes: CTRS ¼ Conners’ Teacher Rating Scale; OROS-MPH ¼ Osmotic Release
might not reflect EF in real life, while the rating scales had
Oral System-Methylphenidate Hydrochloride. good ecological validity. Brown (2006) and Barkley & Fischer
(2011), suggested that a behavioral rating scale might be a
between the ADHD groups—except in four subtests in the more sensitive indicator (Barkley & Fischer, 2011; Brown,
teacher evaluation form—in terms of the BRIEF pretreat- 2006). The combined use of performance based neuropsycho-
ment scores (Mann–Whitney U test) (Table 4). logical tests and rating scales for EF can provide a more com-
prehensive view of medication effects on EF performance at
various levels.
Post-treatment evaluation In this study, Stroop, CT and SDLT were applied to per-
formance-based neuropsychological tests. BRIEF was used as
Among the ADHD groups, the reduction of over 40% in the
a behavioral assessment scale. The results imply that both
CTRS subtest scores was considered to be an improvement,
OROS-MPH and ATX could improve EF in children with
but no significant difference was found between the drug
ADHD. In both drug groups, when the Stroop test perform-
groups (Table 5).
ances before and after the treatment were evaluated separ-
Both drug groups showed significantly improved scores
ately, it was observed that, after the treatment, they
in performance-based neuropsychological tests. When the
completed the test in less time and made less errors and
post-treatment performance of the ADHD group was com-
corrections. When the differences between the groups were
pared with the control group, it was observed that both
evaluated after the treatment, it was observed that both drug
drug groups performed worse than the control group except
groups completed the tests in a similar time to the control
for the time points in the Stroop test (Table 2).
group, except for the first card. It can be thought that this
When the BRIEF pre and post-treatment scores were
difference may be due to the remember effect of the test,
evaluated in the ADHD group, there was an improvement
which is one of the exercise cards. When the number of cor-
in many scores in both drug groups (Table 6) but no signifi-
rections and errors was observed, it was seen that there was
cant difference between the groups (Table 7).
no difference between each drug group and that there were
more errors and corrections in both drug groups than in the
Discussion control group (MPH ¼ ATX > control). In the literature,
there has been only one study that included a control group
Although there are comparative studies regarding the treat- with ATX and methylphenidate drug groups. Similarly to
ment effectiveness of ATX and psychostimulant agents, our study, an improvement was found in interference resist-
comparative data are limited regarding executive functions. ance in both drug groups after treatment, and the control
In the present study, we aimed to compare the effects of group showed better performance than the drug groups
ATX and OROS-MPH on executive functions and evaluate (Yang et al., 2012). When all findings were evaluated, Stroop
their clinical effectiveness. test performances were found to be significantly impaired in
In this study, clinical efficacy was evaluated with the ADHD, evaluating variables such as the ability to maintain
results of CTRS in accordance with many studies in the lit- set-up against disruptive effect, selective attention, and psy-
erature (Çetin et al., 2015; Gao et al., 2006). In some studies chomotor velocity. After treatment, an increase in test per-
that comparing the efficacy of pharmacological treatment in formance was observed in both groups. However, despite
ADHD, methylphenidate treatment was found to be more this increase, the ADHD group and the control group
effective than ATX treatment (Newcorn et al., 2008; Yildiz showed similar performance on the psychomotor speed after
et al., 2011), but ATX was found to be more effective in treatment, while the ADHD group showed worse perform-
fewer studies (Prasad et al., 2007). In some follow-up stud- ance on ability to maintain set-up against disruptive effect
ies, it was concluded that both drugs showed similar clinical and selective attention compared to the control group. This
efficacy (Çetin et al., 2015; Kratochvil et al., 2002; Su et al., finding can be interpreted that the ATX and methylphenid-
2016). In the present study, similar to the other studies in ate treatments are beneficial for impulsivity but not enough
the field, CTRS was applied at the end of the 4-month fol- control when there is a disturbing factor.
low-up period, before and after treatment, and there was a The cancelation test measures cognitive functions such as
significant decrease in attention deficit, hyperactivity, and visual spatial perception, visual screening, continuous atten-
behavioral problems subscale scores in both methylphenidate tion, and response rate, especially in the parietal region
and ATX groups after treatment. In addition, no significant (Kılıç et al., 2002). When the performance of the groups was
difference was found in terms of the three subscale scores compared before the treatment, the children diagnosed with
when the clinical efficacy was 40% or more in CTRS scores. ADHD completed the subtests with regular letters, regular
Table 2. The comparison of pretreatment and posttreatment scores of STROOP and SDLT among groups and the statistics of treatment efficacy.
Pretreatment scores Postreatment Treatment efficacy
c c
MPH ATX Control Statistics MPH ATX Control Statistics Statistics for MPHd Statistics for ATXd
b b
S1- time 18.1 ± 12.6 15.9 ± 5.2 14.9 ± 3.5 F ¼ 1.78, p ¼ 0.174 13.5 ± 4.0 12.1 ± 3.0 14.9 ± 3.5 F 5 6.1, Z 5 24.15, p < 0.001 Z 5 24.86,
p 5 0.003, p < 0.001
ATX < Control
S1-error 0.0 ± 0.0 0.0 ± 0.0 0.0 ± 0.0 – 0.02 ± 0.14 0.0 ± 00 0.0 ± 0.0 – Za ¼ 1.00, p ¼ 0.31 Zc ¼ 0.00, p ¼ 1.0
S1-correction 0.08 ± 0.2 0.06 ± 0.2 0.02 ± 0.1 F ¼ 0.64, p ¼ 0.52 0.0 ± 0.0 0.04 ± 0.2 0.02 ± 0.1 – Zb 5 22.0, p 5 0.04 Zb ¼ 0.44, p ¼ 0.65
S2-time 19.4 ± 9.9 17.9 ± 6.1 16.9 ± 4.0 F ¼ 1.46, p ¼ 0.23 17.5 ± 6.4 20.2 ± 8.2 16.9 ± 4.0 F ¼ 2.91, p ¼ 0.06 Zb ¼ 1.11, p ¼ 0.26 Zb ¼ 0.82, p ¼ 0.40
S2-error 0.04 ± 0.1 0.0 ± 0.0 0.0 ± 0.0 – 0.02 ± 0.1 0.04 ± 0.2 0.0 ± 0.0 – Zb ¼ 0.57, p ¼ 0.56 Za ¼ 1.41, p ¼ 0.15
S2-correction 0.5 ± 1.1 0.1 ± 0.6 0.02 ± 0.1 F* 5 5.44, p 5 0.006, 0.1 ± 0.3 0.1 ± 0.4 0.02 ± 0.1 F ¼ 2.93, p ¼ 0.059 Zb 5 22.11, p 5 0.03 Zc ¼ 0.00, p ¼ 1.0
MPH > Control
S3-time 24.0 ± 7.8 23.2 ± 6.6 22.7 ± 5.8 F ¼ 0.38, p ¼ 0.67 21.5 ± 7.8 20.4 ± 7.2 22.7 ± 5.8 F ¼ 1.19, p ¼ 0.30 Zb ¼ 1.79, p ¼ 0.07 Zb 5 21.86, p 5 0.06
S3-error 0.1 ± 0.6 0.02 ± 0.14 0.0 ± 0.0 – 0.04 ± 0.1 0.1 ± 0.3 0.0 ± 0.0 – Zb ¼ 1.0, p ¼ 0.31 Za ¼ 1.41, p ¼ 0.15
S3-correction 1.2 ± 1.1 1.1 ± 1.4 0.07 ± 0.2 F* 5 34.18, p < 0.001, 0.4 ± 0.6 0.4 ± 0.9 0.07 ± 0.2 F* 5 7.91, p 5 0.001, Zb 5 -3.72, p < 0.001 Zb 5 2.99,
MPH > Control, MPH > Control, p 5 0.003
p < 0.001; p 5 0.005;
ATX > Control, p < 0.001 ATX > Control,
p 5 0.03
S4-time 41.1 ± 16.8 37.1 ± 8.9 30.1 ± 5.4 F* 5 15.78, p < 0.001, 29.5 ± 11.1 27.2 ± 10.4 30.1 ± 5.4 F ¼ 1.29, p ¼ 0.27 Zb 5 24.58, Zb 5 24.12,
MPH > Control, p < 0.001 p < 0.001
p < 0.001;
ATX > Control, p < 0.001
S4-error 0.2 ± 0.6 0.1 ± 0.4 0.0 ± 0.0 – 0.08 ± 0.3 0.2 ± 0.8 0.0 ± 0.0 – Zb ¼ 1.14, p ¼ 0.25 Za ¼ 0.32, p ¼ 0.74
S4-correction 3.2 ± 2.7 3.5 ± 2.8 0.1 ± 0.4 F*5 15.78, p < 0.001, 0.9 ± 1.2 1.0 ± 1.5 0.1 ± 0.4 F* 5 14.51, p < 0.001, Zb 5 24.28, Zb 5 24.59,
MPH > Control, MPH > Control, p < 0.001 p < 0.001
p < 0.001; p < 0.001;
ATX > Control, p < 0.001 ATX > Control,
p < 0.001
S5-time 58.0 ± 18.2 52.8 ± 17.6 39.6 ± 5.9 F* 5 30.14, p < 0.001, 42.5 ± 16.1 39.9 ± 12.0 39.6 ± 5.9 F ¼ 0.68, p ¼ 0.50 Zb 5 24.73, Zb 5 23.78,
MPH > Control, p < 0.001 p < 0.001
p < 0.001;
ATX > Control, p < 0.001
S5-error 2.9 ± 3.7 2.8 ± 3.1 0.0 ± 0.0 – 0.2 ± 0.5 0.2 ± 0.4 0.0 ± 0.0 – Zb 5 24.70, Zb 5 24.65,
p < 0.001 p < 0.001
S5-correction 5.8 ± 3.2 6.2 ± 2.6 0.3 ± 0.6 F* 5 16.54, p < 0.001, 1.8 ± 1.8 1.6 ± 1.6 0.3 ± 0.6 F* 5 22.67, p < 0.001, Zb 5 25.68, p < 0.001 Zb 5 25.68,
MPH > Control, MPH > Control, p < 0.001
p < 0.001; p < 0.001;
ATX > Control, p < 0.001 ATX > Control,
p < 0.001
SDLT 5.2 ± 6.6 6.6 ± 6.4 18.0 ± 2.0 F* 5 130.2, p < 0.001, 17.7 ± 5.1 14.3 ± 6.2 18.0 ± 2.0 F* 5 6.97, p 5 0.002, Za 5 25.91, p < 0.001 Zb 5 25.62,
MPH < Control, MPH > ATX, p 5 0.01; p < 0.001
p < 0.001; Control > ATX,
ATX < Control, p < 0.001 p < 0.001
Notes: SDLT ¼ Serial Digit Learning Test; MPH ¼ methylphenidate hydrochloride; ATX ¼ atomoxetine.
Statisticsc: One-Way analysis of variance (ANOVA) or Welch ANOVA was used to compare parameters between MPH, ATX, and control groups. When an overall significance was observed, pairwise pos-hoc tests were per-
formed using Tamhane’s test. Statistics value were indicated as F for One-Way ANOVA, and F for Welch ANOVA.
Statisticsd: The Wilcoxon test was used to compare the treatment efficacy by using posttreatment-pretreatment scores for MPH and ATX group, separately. Za: Z value based on negative ranks; Zb: Z value based on
positive ranks; Zc: Z value based on the sum of negative ranks equals the sum of positive ranks.
p < 0.05 is marked as bold.
APPLIED NEUROPSYCHOLOGY: CHILD
5
Table 3. The comparison of pretreatment and posttreatment scores of cancelation subtests among groups and the statistics of treatment efficacy. 6
Pretreatment scores Postreatment scores Treatment efficacy
a a
MPH ATX Control Statistics MPH ATX Control Statistics Statistics for MPHb Statistics for ATXb
b b
RL-marked 54.2 ± 5.9 57.0 ± 4.3 59.7 ± 0.7 59.2 ± 1.8 59.4 ± 1.0 59.7 ± 0.7 F ¼ 2.23, p ¼ 0.11 Z ¼ 0.63, p ¼ 0.52 Z 5 22.05, p 5 0.04
F* 5 28.3, p < 0.001, MPH < Control, p <
0.001;ATX < Control, p < 0.001; MPH < ATX,
p 5 0.02
RL- skipped 5.7 ± 5.9 2.9 ± 4.3 0.2 ± 0.7 0.7 ± 1.8 0.5 ± 1.0 0.2 ± 0.7 F ¼ 2.23, p ¼ 0.11 Zb 5 25.21, p < 0.001 Zb 5 23.78, p < 0.001
F* 5 28.3, p < 0.001, MPH > Control, p <
Y. TAŞ TORUN ET AL.

0.001; ATX > Control, p < 0.001; MPH > ATX,


p 5 0.02
RL-incorrect 0.5 ± 2.1 0.2 ± 1.6 0.0 ± 0.0 0.02 ± 0.1 0.0 ± 0.0 0.0 ± 0.0 – Zb ¼ 1.73, p ¼ 0.08 Zb ¼ 1.34, p ¼ 0.163

RL-error 6.3 ± 6.6 3.2 ± 4.9 0.2 ± 0.7 0.6 ± 1.6 0.5 ± 1.0 0.2 ± 0.7 F ¼ 1.64, p ¼ 0.20 Zb 5 25.23, p < 0.001 Zb 5 23.84, p < 0.001
F* 5 27.2, p < 0.001, MPH > Control, p <
0.001; ATX > Control, p < 0.001; MPH > ATX,
p 5 0.03
RL-time 221.0 ± 110.0 173.6 ± 72.7 203.0 ± 23.7 188.0 ± 74.4 170.1 ± 39.8 203.0 ± 23.7 F* 5 11.1, p < 0.001, Zb ¼ 1.48, p ¼ 0.13 Za ¼ 0.33, p ¼ 0.73
F* 5 4.17, p 5 0.001, Control > ATX, p 5 ATX < Control, p < 0.001
0.03; MPH > ATX, p 5 0.04
RS-marked 54.7 ± 7.0 57.6 ± 3.7 59.7 ± 0.6 59.3 ± 1.3 59.4 ± 1.8 59.7 ± 0.6 F ¼ 1.01, p ¼ 0.36 Za 5 25.10, p < 0.001 Za 5 23.40, p 5 0.001
F* 5 18.2, p < 0.001, MPH < Control, p <
0.001; ATX < Control, p 5 0.002;
MPH < ATX, p 5 0.04
RS-skipped 5.2 ± 7.0 2.3 ± 3.7 0.2 ± 0.6 0.6 ± 1.3 0.5 ± 1.8 0.2 ± 0.6 F ¼ 1.01, p ¼ 0.36 Zb 5 25.10, p < 0.001 Zb 5 23.40, p 5 0.001
F* 5 18.2, p < 0.001, MPH > Control, p <
0.001; ATX > Control, p 5 0.002;
MPH > ATX, p 5 0.02
RS-incorrect 1.3 ± 4.1 0.7 ± 1.8 0.0 ± 0.0 0.02 ± 0.1 0.04 ± 0.2 0.0 ± 0.0 F ¼ 0.54, p ¼ 0.57 Zb 5 23.89, p < 0.001 Zb 5 23.22, p 5 0.001

RS-error 6.5 ± 8.4 3.1 ± 4.8 0.2 ± 0.6 0.6 ± 1.3 0.5 ± 1.9 0.2 ± 0.6 F ¼ 1.04, p ¼ 0.35 Zb 5 25.36, p < 0.001 Zb 5 23.82, p < 0.001
F* 5 20.9, p < 0.001, MPH > Control, p <
0.001; ATX > Control, p < 0.001; MPH > ATX,
p 5 0.04
RS-time 196.8 ± 81.6 190.3 ± 78.6 192.0 ± 67.9 178.0 ± 49.8 167.0 ± 40.2 192.0 ± 67.9 F ¼ 3.04, p ¼ 0.051 Zb ¼ 1.24, p ¼ 0.21 Zb ¼ 1.26, p ¼ 0.20
F ¼ 0.25, p ¼ 0.77
IRL-marked 53.5 ± 6.6 55.1 ± 4.7 59.6 ± 0.8 58.0 ± 4.3 58.7 ± 2.5 59.6 ± 0.8 F* 5 4.79, p 5 0.01, Za 5 24.53, p < 0.001 Za 5 24.48, p < 0.001
F* 5 37.8, p < 0.001, MPH < Control, p < MPH < Control,
0.001; ATX < Control, p < 0.001 p 5 0.05;
IRL-skipped 6.4 ± 7.1 4.5 ± 4.8 0.4 ± 0.8 1.8 ± 4.2 1.1 ± 2.4 0.4 ± 0.8 F* 5 4.28, p 5 0.01, Zb 5 24.49, p < 0.001 Zb 5 24.29, p < 0.001
F* 5 32.5, p < 0.001, MPH > Control, p < MPH > Control,
0.001; ATX > Control, p < 0.001 p 5 0.05
IRL-incorrect 0.4 ± 1.8 0.3 ± 1.3 0.0 ± 0.0 0.02 ± 0.1 0.2 ± 1.3 0.0 ± 0.0 – Zb ¼ 1.93, p ¼ 0.053 Zb ¼ 1.44, p ¼ 0.14

IRL-error 6.8 ± 7.5 4.9 ± 5.1 0.4 ± 0.8 1.8 ± 4.2 1.3 ± 3.4 0.4 ± 0.8
F* 5 4.10, p 5 0.02, Zb 5 24.71, p < 0.001 Zb 5 24.48, p < 0.001
F* 5 34.3, p < 0.001, MPH > Control, p < MPH > Control,
0.001; ATX > Control, p < 0.001 p 5 0.06
IRL-time 245.3 ± 301.8 194.4 ± 89.2 214.4 ± 19.1 181.6 ± 53.6 191.3 ± 64.8 214.4 ± 19.1 F* 5 9.54, p < 0.001, Zb ¼ 1.61, p ¼ 0.10 Zb ¼ 0.06, p ¼ 0.95
F ¼ 1.3, p ¼ 0.26 MPH < Control,
p 5 0.001
IRS-marked 54.2 ± 6.9 56.5 ± 4.5 59.6 ± 0.9 58.4 ± 2.8 59.1 ± 2.4 59.6 ± 0.9 F* 5 4.13, p 5 0.02, Za 5 24.28, p < 0.001 Za 5 24.39, p < 0.001
F* 5 23.4, p < 0.001, MPH < Control, p < MPH < Control,
0.001; ATX < Control, p < 0.001 p 5 0.02
(continued)
APPLIED NEUROPSYCHOLOGY: CHILD 7

Statisticsd: The Wilcoxon test was used to compare the treatment efficacy by using posttreatment-pretreatment scores for MPH and ATX group, separately. Za: Z value based on negative ranks; Zb: Z value based on posi-
Statisticsc: One-Way analysis of variance (ANOVA) or Welch ANOVA was used to compare parameters between MPH, ATX and control groups. When an overall significance was observed, pairwase pos-hoc tests were per-
figures and irregular letters in similar time with their healthy
Z 5 24.28, p < 0.001 Z 5 24.39, p < 0.001

Zb 5 24.48, p < 0.001 Zb 5 24.22, p < 0.001

Za ¼ 1.92, p ¼ 0.054
Zb 5 23.10, p 5 0.002 Zb ¼ 1.31, p ¼ 0.18
Statistics for ATXb

peers. However, it was observed that the subtest with irregu-


lar shapes was completed in a shorter time than the control
group (p < 0.05). There are studies in the literature reporting
that children with ADHD perform faster in the case of presen-
Treatment efficacy

tation of stimulants in an irregular and unfamiliar manner


b

(Landau et al., 1999). In addition, it was observed that chil-


dren with ADHD had less target markings, more targets and
Za ¼ 1.32, p ¼ 0.18
Statistics for MPHb

more wrong targets than the control group in all subtests


except the one with regular letters (p < 0.05). This finding is
consistent with other studies in the literature (Kılıç et al.,
2007; Soysal, 2007). These findings suggest that ADHD is
associated with impaired cognitive functions such as visual
b

motor sensing, visual selectivity, selection of mental activity,


suppression of inappropriate, and sustained attention. The
situation of marking false stimuli at CT is described as
162.7 ± 47.9 166.2 ± 39.0 195.6 ± 21.9 F* 5 15.0, p < 0.001,
F* 5 4.13, p 5 0.02,

F* 5 4.05, p 5 0.02,

“impulsivity,” one of the core symptoms of ADHD (Kılıç


a

MPH > Control,

MPH > Control,

MPH < Control,


Statistics

et al., 2007). In the ADHD group, the response rate was


p < 0.001
p 5 0.02

p 5 0.02

higher, but the total number of errors was also higher. It can
be thought that individuals diagnosed with ADHD are more
Postreatment scores

aggressive in daily life, but are more likely to be accident-


prone and clumsy individuals. When the differences between


0.4 ± 0.9

0.0 ± 0.0

0.4 ± 0.9
Control

the groups were evaluated after the treatment, it was observed


that the drug groups completed the subtests in a shorter time
compared to the control group. While there was no significant
difference between the groups in terms of the marked, skipped
0.8 ± 2.4

0.2 ± 0.7

1.1 ± 3.9
ATX

and mismarked targets, there was no significant difference


between the groups in terms of the regular/irregular letters
formed using Tamhane’s test. Statistics value were indicated as F for One-Way ANOVA, and F for Welch ANOVA.

sub-tests and the regular shapes subtest. However, the ATX


1.5 ± 2.8

0.0 ± 0.0

1.5 ± 2.8

group showed similar performance with respect to the num-


MPH

ber of targets marked in the irregular shapes subtest and the


number of skipped targets. When all the findings were eval-
uated, it was observed that children with ADHD performed
F* 5 23.4, p < 0.001, MPH > Control, p <

F* 5 24.4, p < 0.001, MPH > Control, p <

F* 5 28.9, p < 0.001, MPH < Control, p <

differently than their healthy peers in CT, which evaluated


tive ranks; Zc: Z value based on the sum of negative ranks equals the sum of positive ranks.

cognitive functions such as visual spatial perception, visual


screening, continuous attention and response rate. After the
0.001; ATX > Control, p < 0.001

0.001; ATX > Control, p < 0.001

0.001; ATX < Control, p < 0.001

treatment, visual spatial perception, visual screening, and per-


a
Statistics

forming tasks that require constant attention were observed to


be performed better than before. This increase in performance
was more prominent in the ATX group. When response rates
and mismarked targets were evaluated, it was concluded that
Pretreatment scores

Notes: MPH ¼ methylphenidate hydrochloride; ATX ¼ atomoxetine.

they made quick decisions similar to the ones before the treat-
ment, and their impulsivity persisted despite the treatment.
When the SDLT scores used to measure short-term

146.9 ± 58.3 195.6 ± 21.9

memory capacity and learning ability were compared, it was


0.4 ± 0.9

0.0 ± 0.0

0.4 ± 0.9
Control

observed that the pretreatment SDLT performances in both


methylphenidate and ATX groups were lower than in the
control group (p < 0.05). In the study, it was determined
3.4 ± 4.5

0.3 ± 1.7

3.8 ± 5.3

that children with ADHD had significant impairment in


ATX

short-term memory and learning capacities compared to


their healthy peers. This finding is consistent with the
p < 0.05 is marked as bold.

results of a study by Erdogan Bakar (2007) with 154 ADHD


136.3 ± 42.9
5.7 ± 6.9

0.6 ± 1.9

6.4 ± 7.4
MPH

patients and 73 healthy participants. The memory space


Table 3. Continued.

tests, including the arrays of digits, can be used as a meas-


urement tool for short-term memory capacity, but also as a
IRS-incorrect
IRS-skipped

measurement tool for attention. In ADHD, the lower per-


IRS-error

IRS-time

formance than the control group in sensitive memory subt-


ests is considered as a condition related to working memory
8 Y. TAŞ TORUN ET AL.

Table 4. Pretreament BRIEF scores.


Subtests Group N Mean Sequence sum U p
Inhibit (BRIEF-Teacher) OROS-MPH 49 41.19 2018.5 793.5 0.013
Atomoxetine 46 55.25 2541.5
Shift (BRIEF-Teacher) OROS-MPH 49 44.80 2195.0 970.0 0.241
Atomoxetine 46 51.41 2365.0
Emotional Control (BRIEF-Teacher) OROS-MPH 49 44.60 2185.5 960.5 0.214
Atomoxetine 46 51.62 2374.5
BRI (BRIEF-Teacher) OROS-MPH 49 42.33 2074.0 849.0 0.038
Atomoxetine 46 54.04 2486.0
Initiation (BRIEF-Teacher) OROS-MPH 49 42.01 2058.5 833.5 0.027
Atomoxetine 46 54.38 2501.5
Working Memory (BRIEF-Teacher) OROS-MPH 49 42.58 2086.5 861.5 0.047
Atomoxetine 46 53.77 2473.5
Plan/Organize (BRIEF-Teacher) OROS-MPH 49 47.19 2312.5 1087.5 0.768
Atomoxetine 46 48.86 2247.5
Organization of materials (BRIEF-Teacher) OROS-MPH 49 45.93 2250.5 1025.5 0.448
Atomoxetine 46 50.21 2309.5
Monitor (BRIEF-Teacher) OROS-MPH 49 48.49 2376.0 1103.0 0.858
Atomoxetine 46 47.48 2184.0
MI (BRIEF-Teacher) OROS-MPH 49 44.07 2159.5 934.5 0.152
Atomoxetine 46 52.18 2400.5
GEC (BRIEF-Teacher) OROS-MPH 49 43.38 2125.5 900.5 0.092
Atomoxetine 46 52.92 2434.5
Inhibit (BRIEF-Parents) OROS-MPH 49 51.49 2523.0 956.0 0.201
Atomoxetine 46 44.28 2037.0
Shift (BRIEF-Parents) OROS-MPH 49 49.95 2447.5 1031.5 0.474
Atomoxetine 46 45.92 2112.5
Emotional Control (BRIEF-Parents) OROS-MPH 49 50.46 2472.5 1006.5 0.368
Atomoxetine 46 45.38 2087.5
BRI (BRIEF-Parents) OROS-MPH 49 51.16 2507.0 972.0 0.248
Atomoxetine 46 44.63 2053.0
Initiation (BRIEF-Parents) OROS-MPH 49 46.72 2289.5 1064.5 0.640
Atomoxetine 46 49.36 2270.5
Working Memory (BRIEF-Parents) OROS-MPH 49 50.63 2481.0 998.0 0.335
Atomoxetine 46 45.20 2079.0
Plan/Organize (BRIEF-Parents) OROS-MPH 49 48.49 2376.0 1103.0 0.858
Atomoxetine 46 46 47.48
Organization of materials (BRIEF-Parents) OROS-MPH 49 49 53.26 869.5 0.054
Atomoxetine 46 46 42.4
Monitor (BRIEF-Parents) OROS-MPH 49 49 48.54 1100.5 0.843
Atomoxetine 46 46 47.42
MI (BRIEF-Parents) OROS-MPH 49 49 49.85 1036.5 0.500
Atomoxetine 46 46 46.03
GEC (BRIEF-Parents) OROS-MPH 49 49 50.89 985.5 0.292
Atomoxetine 46 46 44.92
Notes: BRIEF ¼ Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function; OROS-MPH ¼ Osmotic Release Oral System-Methylphenidate Hydrochloride; BRI ¼ behavioral regu-
lation index; MI ¼ metacognition index; GEC ¼ Global Executive Composite.
p < 0.05.

problems (Kaplan et al., 1998). When the post-treatment Table 5. Post-treatment improvement of CTRS sub-test scores in two
SDLT scores were compared with the pretreatment scores, drug groups.
both drug groups showed significant improvement com- OROS-MPH Atomoxetine
CTRS N (%) N (%) p
pared to pretreatment levels. In addition, there was no dif-
Hyperactivity HYPERACTIVITY 24 (49) 22 (47.8) 0.910
ference between the methylphenidate and ATX groups in Inattention 34 (69.4) 29 (63) 0.513
terms of pretreatment SDLT performance, but higher per- Behavior problems 27 (55.1) 27 (58.7) 0.724
formance was found in the methylphenidate group com- Notes: CTRS ¼ Conners’ Teacher Rating Scale; OROS-MPH ¼ Osmotic Release
pared to the ATX group. The performance of the Oral System-Methylphenidate Hydrochloride.
methylphenidate group was better than the control group,
but the performance of the ATX group was significantly ATX treatment was more effective in auditory-verbal-written
lower than the control group (MPH > control > ATX). tasks; however, no control group was included in the study
There is no study comparing SDLT performance before and (Tasdelen et al., 2015). In a study by Yang et al. (2012), the
after ADHD treatment in the literature. However, the task effects of ATX and methylphenidate treatments on WISC-R
used in the SDLT reveals the sequences of numbers used in number sequence subtest performance were evaluated, and
the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale–Revised (WISC-R) both drugs showed significant improvement in the number
test and Visual Auditory Number Sequence Test (VANST) sequence performance test compared to the pretreatment
(Kaplan et al., 1998). In a comparative study by Tasdelen levels. It was concluded that the performances of both drug
et al. (2015) on the effect of ATX and methylphenidate groups after treatment were similar to the healthy control
treatments on VANST performance, both drugs were found group. In addition, after treatment, no significant difference
to increase test performance similarly. It was concluded that was found between the two drug groups (Yang et al., 2012).
APPLIED NEUROPSYCHOLOGY: CHILD 9

Table 6. The comparison of pretreatment and posttreatment scores of BRIEF among MPH and ATX groups and the statistics of treatment efficacy.
MPH ATX
Treatment efficacy
BRIEF subtests Pretreatment Posttreatment Treatment efficacy statistics Pretreatment Posttreatment statistics
Teacher
 Inhibit 22.9 ± 5.6 (24) 20.0 ± 4.9 (20) Zb 5 22.89, p 5 0.004 25.7 ± 4.0 (26.5) 20.5 ± 5.8 (20) Zb 5 24.34, p < 0.001
 Shift 19.4 ± 4.0 (20) 17.4 ± 3.3 (17) Zb 5 22.68, p 5 0.007 20.4 ± 3.2 (20) 17.6 ± 3.3 (18) Zb 5 24.20, p < 0.001
 Emotional Control 18.4 ± 4.9 (19) 16.9 ± 4.4 (18) Zb 5 22.34, p 5 0.01 19.7 ± 4.7 (19.5) 16.8 ± 4.1 (16) Zb 5 23.67, p < 0.001
 BRI 60.8 ± 11.6 (61) 54.4 ± 9.2 (53) Zb 5 23.63, p < 0.001 65.9 ± 8.7 (68) 54.9 ± 8.4 (55.5) Zb 5 25.06, p < 0.001
 Initiation 16.8 ± 3.2 (17) 15.2 ± 3.3 (15) Zb 5 22.46, p 5 0.01 18.1 ± 3.4 (19) 15.0 ± 3.3 (15) Zb 5 24.71, p < 0.001
 Working Memory 23.4 ± 4.1 (24) 20.7 ± 3.9 (21) Zb 5 23.95, p < 0.001 24.9 ± 4.2 (26) 20.4 ± 3.7 (21) Zb 5 25.09, p < 0.001
 Plan/Organize 23.7 ± 3.7 (23) 21.2 ± 2.9 (21) Zb 5 23.46, p 5 0.001 23.7 ± 4.3 (24) 20.3 ± 3.7 (20) Zb 5 24.40, p < 0.001
 Organization of materials 15.3 ± 4.4 (17) 15.1 ± 3.7 (15) Zb ¼ 0.20, p ¼ 0.83 16.3 ± 3.3 (16) 16.0 ± 3.6 (16) Zb ¼ 0.27, p ¼ 0.78
 Monitor 21.3 ± 4.4 (23) 19.8 ± 4.4 (19) Zb 5 22.39, p 5 0.001 21.4 ± 4.3 (21.5) 18.6 ± 3.5 (18.5) Zb 5 23.95, p < 0.001
 MI 100.6 ± 15.7 (102) 92.1 ± 11.4 (91) Zb 5 23.57, p < 0.001 104.7 ± 14.6 (107.5) 90.4 ± 10.5 (88) Zb 5 25.01, p < 0.001
 GEC 161.4 ± 24.2 (164) 146.6 ± 18.2 (147) Zb 5 23.03, p < 0.001 170.7 ± 20.5 (173.5) 145.4 ± 14.5 (147) Zb 5 25.34, p < 0.001
Parents
 Inhibit 21.6 ± 4.8 (23) 19.4 ± 3.7 (19) Zb 5 23.03, p 5 0.002 20.9 ± 4.3 (21) 19.3 ± 3.1 (19) Zb 5 22.02, p 5 0.04
 Shift 17.1 ± 3.3 (18) 14.7 ± 3.6 (15) Zb 5 24.01, p < 0.001 16.8 ± 3.3 (17.5) 15.4 ± 2.8 (16) Zb 5 22.28, p 5 0.02
 Emotional Control 21.6 ± 4.6 (22) 19.8 ± 4.1 (19) Zb 5 21.99, p 5 0.004 20.9 ± 5.1(20) 18.9 ± 3.7 (19.5) Zb 5 22.15, p 5 0.03
 BRI 60.5 ± 10.7 (62) 53.6 ± 9.4 (54) Zb 5 23.66, p < 0.001 58.7 ± 9.8 (59) 53.7 ± 6.6 (54) Zb 5 22.70, p 5 0.007
 Initiation 17.6 ± 3.2 (18) 16.4 ± 3.1 (17) Zb 5 22.50, p 5 0.01 17.8 ± 3.8 (18) 15.8 ± 2.8 (16) Zb 5 22.70, p 5 0.007
 Working Memory 22.7 ± 3.6 (23) 20.20 ± 3.6 (20) Zb 5 23.72, p < 0.001 22.0 ± 4.1 (22) 19.0 ± 2.9 (19) Zb 5 23.34, p 5 0.001
 Plan/Organize 27.5 ± 4.2 (28) 25.7 ± 5.1 (26) Zb 5 21.97, p 5 0.04 27.2 ± 5.4 (27.5) 24.7 ± 4.9 (24) Zb 5 22.24, p 5 0.02
 Organization of materials 13.9 ± 2.8 (14) 13.2 ± 3.0 (14) Zb ¼ 1.61, p ¼ 0.10 12.5 ± 3.4 (12) 12.4 ± 2.9 (12) Zb ¼ 0.50, p ¼ 0.61
 Monitor 19.1 ± 3.5 (20) 16.5 ± 3.6 (17) Zb 5 23.89, p < 0.001 18.8 ± 3.9 (20) 16.9 ± 2.9 (17) Zb 5 22.66, p 5 0.008
 MI 100.9 ± 14.6 (102) 92.0 ± 12.7 (93) Zb 5 23.53, p < 0.001 98.5 ± 16.8 (101) 88.9 ± 11.5 (89) Zb 5 23.20, p 5 0.001
 GEC 161.5 ± 23.9 (166) 145.7 ± 19.0 (148) Zb 5 24.01, p < 0.001 157.2 ± 24.5 (164) 142.6 ± 15.8 (143) Zb 5 23.57, p < 0.001
Notes: BRIEF ¼ Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function; MPH ¼ methylphenidate hydrochloride; ATX ¼ atomoxetine; BRI ¼ behavioral regulation index;
MI ¼ metacognition index; GEC ¼ Global Executive Composite.
Statistics: The Wilcoxon test was used to compare the treatment efficacy by using posttreatment-pretreatment scores for MPH and ATX group, separately. Za: Z
value based on negative ranks, Zb: Z value based on positive ranks, Zc: Z value based on the sum of negative ranks equals the sum of positive ranks.
p < 0.05 is marked as bold.

SDLT is also a learning test as it measures the number of study comparing the results of self-report BRIEF subscale
repeat sequences for the correct repetition of a given num- scores and performance-based neuropsychological tests on
ber of sequences, as well as measuring the short-term mem- adult ADHD patients, it was concluded that the only pre-
ory, similar to the WISC-R number sequence subtest and dictive subtest for performance-based tests was material
VANS. From this, it can be said that ATX and methylphen- organization (Grane et al., 2014). In this study, the continu-
idate treatments have an improving effect on both short- ity in the material organization subscale scores, which
term memory and learning ability compared to similar study express the ability to keep the related parts of the environ-
results, and this effect is observed more with methylphenid- ment in a systematic way, was noteworthy in both drug
ate treatment. groups, in both teacher and parent notifications.
In this study, the results of the BRIEF test were used as a The present study had several limitations. First, subgroups
behavior evaluation scale. When the scale scores were com- of ADHD were not evaluated in the study. Improvement in
pared separately in the two drug groups before and after the executive functions may vary in different subtypes of ADHD.
treatment, all BRIEF subscale scores except the suppression, Second, we did not include a group that received placebo;
initiation, working memory, and behavioral regulation index therefore, there might have been bias in our results regarding
were similar in both groups. Considering the change in a potential placebo effect when we estimated the effect of
scores before and after treatment in ADHD groups, there each medication. Another limition of present study is not
was a significant decrease in all the scale scores except for having measure of impairment. Also, the study was only
parents/teacher material organizing scores in both methyl- 18 weeks in duration; study results may not reflect the long-
phenidate and ATX groups. Significant decreases were term effects of treatment on executive functions.
observed in the upper cognition index and global executive At the same time, this study had several strengths. The
scores in both BRIEF-parents and BRIEF-teacher forms major strengths of this study were that this was the first
(Maziade et al., 2009). According to the results of a com- head-to-head study comparing OROS-methylphenidate and
parative study on ATX and methylphenidate by Yang et al. ATX directly in children with ADHD, including a healthy
(2012) with OROS-MPH and ATX groups, there was a control group. An additional strength in this study, was that
decrease in all the parents/teacher scale scores, except for the effects of ATX and OROS-MPH on executive functions
the initiation scores in the parents scale. Also, no significant were evaluated using both performance-based neuropsycho-
difference was observed between the two drug groups (Yang logical tests and ecological behavioral rating scales.
et al., 2012). In a study by Adler et al. (2014), there was a In conclusion, the present study suggests that ATX and
significant decrease in self-efficacy based on the initiation, OROS-MPH treatments have shown similar efficacy in clin-
material organization, set change, and emotion control sub- ical recovery and improvement on executive functions.
scale scores in adult ADHD cases after ATX treatment. In a However, disturbances in executive functions observed in
10 Y. TAŞ TORUN ET AL.

Table 7. Post-treament BRIEF scores.


Subtests Group N Mean Sequence sum U p
Inhibit (BRIEF-Teacher) OROS-MPH 49 47.83 2343.5 1118.5 0.949
Atomoxetine 46 48.18 2216.5
Shift (BRIEF-Teacher) OROS-MPH 49 46.69 2288 1063.0 0.632
Atomoxetine 46 49.39 2272
Emotional Control (BRIEF-Teacher) OROS-MPH 49 48.7 2386.5 1092.5 0.796
Atomoxetine 46 47.25 2173.5
BRI (BRIEF-Teacher) OROS-MPH 49 47.29 2317 1092.0 0.794
Atomoxetine 46 48.76 2243
Initiation (BRIEF-Teacher) OROS-MPH 49 48.62 2382.5 1096.5 0.820
Atomoxetine 46 47.34 2177.5
Working Memory (BRIEF-Teacher) OROS-MPH 49 48.76 2389 1090.0 0.781
Atomoxetine 46 47.2 2171
Plan/Organize (BRIEF-Teacher) OROS-MPH 49 51.34 2515.5 963.5 0.220
Atomoxetine 46 44.45 2044.5
Organization of materials (BRIEF-Teacher) OROS-MPH 49 44.69 2190 965.0 0.225
Atomoxetine 46 51.52 2370
Monitor (BRIEF-Teacher) OROS-MPH 49 52.02 2549 930.0 0.141
Atomoxetine 46 43.72 2011
MI (BRIEF-Teacher) OROS-MPH 49 50.03 2451.5 1027.5 0.458
Atomoxetine 46 45.84 2108.5
GEC (BRIEF-Teacher) OROS-MPH 49 48.34 2368.5 1110.5 0.902
Atomoxetine 46 47.64 2191.5
Inhibit (BRIEF-Parents) OROS-MPH 49 48.89 2395.5 1083.5 0.744
Atomoxetine 46 47.05 2164.5
Shift (BRIEF-Parents) OROS-MPH 49 45.45 2227 1002.0 0.349
Atomoxetine 46 50.72 2333
Emotional Control (BRIEF-Parents) OROS-MPH 49 50.07 2403.5 980.5 0.348
Atomoxetine 46 44.82 2061.5
BRI (BRIEF-Parents) OROS-MPH 49 48.1 2357 1122.0 0.970
Atomoxetine 46 47.89 2203
Initiation (BRIEF-Parents) OROS-MPH 49 50.73 2486 993.0 0.316
Atomoxetine 46 45.09 2074
Working Memory (BRIEF-Parents) OROS-MPH 49 52.11 2553.5 925.5 0.131
Atomoxetine 46 43.62 2006.5
Plan/Organize (BRIEF-Parents) OROS-MPH 49 50.72 2485.5 993.5 0.319
Atomoxetine 46 45.1 2074.5
Organization of materials (BRIEF-Parents) OROS-MPH 49 52.11 2553.5 925.5 0.131
Atomoxetine 46 43.62 2006.5
Monitor (BRIEF-Parents) OROS-MPH 49 46.49 2278 1053.0 0.580
Atomoxetine 46 49.61 2282
MI (BRIEF-Parents) OROS-MPH 49 51.23 2510.5 968.5 0.238
Atomoxetine 46 44.55 2049.5
GEC (BRIEF-Parents) OROS-MPH 49 50.05 2452.5 1026.5 0.454
Atomoxetine 46 45.82 2107.5
Notes: BRIEF ¼ Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function; OROS-MPH ¼ Osmotic Release Oral System-Methylphenidate Hydrochloride;
BRI ¼ behavioral regulation index; MI ¼ metacognition index; GEC ¼ Global Executive Composite.

children with ADHD are persistent despite treatment, when References


compared with the control group.
Adler, L., Tanaka, Y., Williams, D., Trzepacz, P. T., Goto, T., Allen,
A. J., & Upadhyaya, H. P. (2014). Executive function in adults with
attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder during treatment with atom-
oxetine in a randomized, placebo-controlled, withdrawal study.
Acknowledgment Journal of Clinical Psychopharmacology, 34(4), 461–466.
We would like to thank the children and their families for participating American Psychiatric Association (APA). (2013). Diagnostic and statis-
in the study on a voluntary basis. The cost of the study has been paid tical manual of mental disorders (5th ed.). American Psychiatric
by the authors. Association.
Bakar, E. E. (2007). Dikkat Eksiklig i Hiperaktivite Bozuklug unun Alt
€ unt€
Tiplerine Ait Bilişsel Or€ uler. Doktora Tezi.
Bakar, E. E., Taner, Y. I., Soysal, A. S., Karakas, S., & Turgay, A.
(2011). Behavioral rating inventory and laboratory tests measure dif-
Disclosure statement ferent aspects of executive functioning in boys: A validity study.
No potential conflict of interest was reported by the author(s). Klinik Psikofarmakoloji B€
ulteni [Bulletin of Clinical
Psychopharmacology], 21(4), 302–316. https://doi.org/10.5455/bcp.
20111004014003
ORCID Barkley, R. A. (1997). Behavioral inhibition, sustained attention, and
executive functions: Constructing a unifying theory of ADHD.
Yasemin Taş Torun http://orcid.org/0000-0002-4922-7594 Psychological Bulletin, 121(1), 65–94. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-
Yasemen Işik Taner http://orcid.org/0000-0001-8560-5161 2909.121.1.65
Esra G€uney http://orcid.org/0000-0002-4043-8301 Barkley, R. A., & Fischer, M. (2011). Predicting impairment in major
_
Elvan Iseri http://orcid.org/0000-0002-1809-5867 life activities and occupational functioning in hyperactive children
APPLIED NEUROPSYCHOLOGY: CHILD 11

as adults: Self-reported executive function (EF) deficits versus EF Neuropsychology, 8(4), 249–257. https://doi.org/10.1076/chin.8.4.249.
tests. Developmental Neuropsychology, 36(2), 137–161. https://doi. 13513
org/10.1080/87565641.2010.549877 G€ €
okler, B., Unal, F., Pehlivant€urk, B., K€ult€
ur, E. Ç., Akdemir, D., &
Bedard, A. C. V., Stein, M. A., Halperin, J. M., Krone, B., Rajwan, E., Taner, Y. (2004). Reliability and validity of schedule for affective
& Newcorn, J. H. (2015). Differential impact of methylphenidate disorders and Schizophrenia for school age children-present and
and atomoxetine on sustained attention in youth with lifetime version-Turkish version (K-SADS-PL-T)[in Turkish]. Turk
attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Journal of Child Psychology Journal of Child Adolescence Mental Health, 11, 109–116.
and Psychiatry, 56(1), 40–48. https://doi.org/10.1111/jcpp.12272 Goyette, C. H., Conners, C. K., & Ulrich, R. F. (1978). Normative data
Biederman, J., Mick, E., Fried, R., Wilner, N., Spencer, T. J., & on revised Conners parent and teacher rating scales. Journal of
Faraone, S. V. (2011). Are stimulants effective in the treatment of Abnormal Child Psychology, 6(2), 221–236. https://doi.org/10.1007/
executive function deficits? Results from a randomized double blind BF00919127
study of OROS-methylphenidate in adults with ADHD. European Grane, V. A., Endestad, T., Pinto, A. F., & Solbakk, A. K. (2014).
Neuropsychopharmacology: The Journal of the European College of Attentional control and subjective executive function in treatment-
Neuropsychopharmacology, 21(7), 508–515. https://doi.org/10.1016/j. naive adults with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. PLOS One,
euroneuro.2010.11.005 9(12), e115227. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0115227
Brown, T. E. (2006). Executive functions and attention deficit hyper- Homack, S., & Riccio, C. A. (2004). A meta-analysis of the sensitivity
activity disorder: Implications of two conflicting views. International and specificity of the Stroop Color and Word Test with children.
Journal of Disability, Development and Education, 53(1), 35–46. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 19(6), 725–743. https://doi.org/
https://doi.org/10.1080/10349120500510024 10.1016/j.acn.2003.09.003
Brown, T. E., Holdnack, J., Saylor, K., Adler, L., Spencer, T., Williams, Isquith, P. K., & Gioia, G. A. (2000). BRIEF predictions of ADHD:
D. W., Padival, A. K., Schuh, K., Trzepacz, P. T., & Kelsey, D. clinical utility of the Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive
(2011). Effect of atomoxetine on executive function impairments in Function for detecting ADHD subtypes in children. Archives of
adults with ADHD. Journal of Attention Disorders, 15(2), 130–138. Clinical Neuropsychology, 15(8), 780–781. https://doi.org/10.1093/
https://doi.org/10.1177/1087054709356165 arclin/15.8.780a
Çetin, F. H., Torun, Y. T., & Taner, Y. I. (2015). Dikkat Eksikligi Kaplan, B. J., Dewey, D., Crawford, S. G., & Fisher, G. C. (1998).
Hiperaktivite Bozuklugu Tedavisinde Atomoksetin ve Osmotik Deficits in long-term memory are not characteristic of ADHD.
Salinimli Metilfenidat: Etkinlik ve Yan Etki Profilinin Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 20(4),
Degerlendirildigi Alti Aylik Izlem Çlismasi [Atomoxetine Versus 518–528. https://doi.org/10.1076/jcen.20.4.518.1477
Oros Methylphenidate in Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder: Karakas, S. (2004). Handbook of BILNOT Battery: Studies of research
A Six-Month Follow Up Study for Efficacy and Adverse Effects]. and development for neuropsychological tests. Dizayn Ofset.
T€urkiye Klinikleri. Tip Bilimleri Dergisi, 35(2), 88. Karakaya, I., Yıldız, O. € & Şişmanlar, Ş. (2006). Metilfenidatın Dikkat
Coghill, D., Seth, S., Pedroso, S., Usala, T., Currie, J., & Gagliano, A. Eksikligi Hiperaktivite Bozuklugu olan çocuklarda dikkat ve
(2014). Effects of methylphenidate on cognitive functions in children y€ur€ut€ uc€ €zerine etkisi: Bir olgu serisi. Çocuk ve Gençlik Ruh
u işlevler u
and adolescents with ADHD: Evidence from a systematic review Sag lıg ı Dergisi, 13(69), 75.
and a meta-analysis. Biological Psychiatry, 76(8), 603–615. https:// Kaufman, J., Birmaher, B., Brent, D., Rao, U., Flynn, C., Moreci, P.,
doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2013.10.005 Williamson, D., & Ryan, N. (1997). Schedule for affective disorders
Conners, C. K. (1969). A teacher rating scale for use in drug studies and schizophrenia for school-age children-present and lifetime ver-
with children. The American Journal of Psychiatry, 126(6), 884–888. sion (K-SADS-PL): Initial reliability and validity data. Journal of the
https://doi.org/10.1176/ajp.126.6.884 American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 36(7),
Dereboy, C., Senol, S., Sener, S., & Dereboy, F. (2007). Validation of 980–988. https://doi.org/10.1097/00004583-199707000-00021
the Turkish versions of the short-form Conners’ teacher and parent Kılıç, B. G., Irak, M., Koçkar, A. I., _ Şener, Ş., & Karakaş, S. (2002).
rating scales. Turk psikiyatri dergisi [Turkish Journal of Psychiatry], _
Işaretleme Testi T€ urk Formu’nun 6-11 Yaş Grubu Çocuklarda
18(1), 48–58. Standardizasyon Çalışması. Klinik Psikiyatri Dergisi, 5(4), 213–228.
Fallu, A., Richard, C., Prinzo, R., & Binder, C. (2006). Does OROS- Kılıç, B., Koçkar, A., Irak, M., Şener, Ş., & Karakaş, S. (2002). The
methylphenidate improve core symptoms and deficits in executive standardization study of the Stroop test TBAG form in children
function? Results of an open-label trial in adults with attention def- between 6 and 11 years of age. Turkish Journal of Child Adolescent
icit hyperactivity disorder. Current Medical Research and Opinion, Mental Health, 9(2), 86–99.
22(12), 2557–2566. https://doi.org/10.1185/030079906X154132 Kiliç, B. G., Sener, S., Koçkar, A. I., & Karakaş, S. (2007).
Faraone, S. V., Biederman, J., Spencer, T., Michelson, D., Adler, L., Multicomponent attention deficits in attention deficit hyperactivity
Reimherr, F., & Seidman, L. (2005). Atomoxetine and Stroop task disorder. Psychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences, 61(2), 142–148.
performance in adult attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Journal https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1440-1819.2007.01629.x
of Child and Adolescent Psychopharmacology, 15(4), 664–670. https:// Kiriş, N., Tahiroglu, A. Y., Avci, A., Herg€ € Altunbaşak, Ş., &
uner, O.,
doi.org/10.1089/cap.2005.15.664 Karakaş, S. (2013). Dikkat Eksikligi Hiperaktivite Bozuklugu Olan
Gao, H., Zhao, Y., Levine, L., & Allen, A. (2006). Determining cut- Çocuklarda Metilfenidatın N€oropsikolojik Işlevler _ €
Uzerine Etkisi.
points for clinically meaningful improvement: A receiver operating Turkiye Klinikleri Journal of Medical Sciences, 33(3), 797–805.
characteristic approach. In Scientific Proceedings of the 53rd Annual https://doi.org/10.5336/medsci.2012-31505
Meeting of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Konrad, K., G€ unther, T., Heinzel-Gutenbrunner, M., & Herpertz-
Psychiatry. American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. Dahlmann, B. (2005). Clinical evaluation of subjective and objective
Gau, S. S. F., & Shang, C. Y. (2010). Improvement of executive func- changes in motor activity and attention in children with attention-
tions in boys with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: An open- deficit/hyperactivity disorder in a double-blind methylphenidate
label follow-up study with once-daily atomoxetine. The International trial. Journal of Child and Adolescent Psychopharmacology, 15(2),
Journal of Neuropsychopharmacology, 13(2), 243–256. https://doi.org/ 180–190. https://doi.org/10.1089/cap.2005.15.180
10.1017/S1461145709990836 Kratochvil, C. J., Heiligenstein, J. H., Dittmann, R., Spencer, T. J.,
Gioia, G. A., Isquith, P. K., & Guy, S. C. (2000). Construct validity of Biederman, J., Wernicke, J., Newcorn, J. H., Casat, C., Milton, D., &
the behavior rating inventory of executive function (BRIEF). Journal Michelson, D. (2002). Atomoxetine and methylphenidate treatment
of the International Neuropsychological Society, 6, 139. in children with ADHD: A prospective, randomized, open-label trial.
Gioia, G. A., Isquith, P. K., Retzlaff, P. D., & Espy, K. A. (2002). Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent
Confirmatory factor analysis of the Behavior Rating Inventory of Psychiatry, 41(7), 776–784. https://doi.org/10.1097/00004583-
Executive Function (BRIEF) in a clinical sample. Child 200207000-00008
12 Y. TAŞ TORUN ET AL.

Landau, Y. E., Gross-Tsur, V., Auerbach, J. G., Van der Meere, J., & Stroop, J. R. (1935). Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions.
Shalev, R. S. (1999). Attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder and Journal of Experimental Psychology, 18(6), 643–662. https://doi.org/
developmental right-hemisphere syndrome: Congruence and incon- 10.1037/h0054651
gruence of cognitive and behavioral aspects of attention. Journal of Su, Y., Yang, L., Stein, M. A., Cao, Q., & Wang, Y. (2016). Osmotic
Child Neurology, 14(5), 299–303. https://doi.org/10.1177/ release oral system methylphenidate versus atomoxetine for the
088307389901400506 treatment of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in Chinese
MacLeod, C. M. (1992). The Stroop task: The “gold standard” of atten- youth: 8-week comparative efficacy and 1-year follow-up. Journal of
tional measures. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 121(1), Child and Adolescent Psychopharmacology, 26(4), 362–371. https://
12–14. https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-3445.121.1.12 doi.org/10.1089/cap.2015.0031
Maziade, M., Rouleau, N., Lee, B., Rogers, A., Davis, L., & Dickson, R. Tasdelen, B. I., Karakaya, E., & Oztop, D. B. (2015). Effects of
(2009). Atomoxetine and neuropsychological function in children atomoxetine and osmotic release oral system-methylphenidate on
executive functions in patients with combined type attention-deficit/
with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: Results of a pilot study.
hyperactivity disorder. Journal of Child and Adolescent
Journal of Child and Adolescent Psychopharmacology, 19(6),
Psychopharmacology, 25(6), 494–500. https://doi.org/10.1089/cap.
709–718. https://doi.org/10.1089/cap.2008.0166
2014.0155
Newcorn, J. H., Kratochvil, C. J., Allen, A. J., Casat, C. D., Ruff, D. D.,
Wehmeier, P. M., Schacht, A., Wolff, C., Otto, W. R., Dittmann,
Moore, R. J., & Michelson, D., Atomoxetine/Methylphenidate R. W., & Banaschewski, T. (2011). Neuropsychological outcomes
Comparative Study Group (2008). Atomoxetine and osmotically across the day in children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity dis-
released methylphenidate for the treatment of attention deficit order treated with atomoxetine: Results from a placebo-controlled
hyperactivity disorder: Acute comparison and differential response. study using a computer-based continuous performance test com-
The American Journal of Psychiatry, 165(6), 721–730. https://doi.org/ bined with an infra-red motion-tracking device. Journal of Child
10.1176/appi.ajp.2007.05091676 and Adolescent Psychopharmacology, 21(5), 433–444. https://doi.org/
Ni, H. C., Shang, C. Y., Gau, S. S. F., Lin, Y. J., Huang, H. C., & Yang, 10.1089/cap.2010.0142
L. K. (2013). A head-to-head randomized clinical trial of methyl- Weintraub, S., & Mesulam, M. M. (1985). Verbal and Nonverbal
phenidate and atomoxetine treatment for executive function in Cancellation Test. FA Davis.
adults with attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. The International Willcutt, E. G., Doyle, A. E., Nigg, J. T., Faraone, S. V., & Pennington,
Journal of Neuropsychopharmacology, 16(9), 1959–1973. https://doi. B. F. (2005). Validity of the executive function theory of attention-
org/10.1017/S1461145713000357 deficit/hyperactivity disorder: a meta-analytic review. Biological
O’Driscoll, G. A., Depatie, L., Holahan, A. L. V., Savion-Lemieux, T., Psychiatry, 57(11), 1336–1346. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.
Barr, R. G., Jolicoeur, C., & Douglas, V. I. (2005). Executive func- 2005.02.006
tions and methylphenidate response in subtypes of attention-deficit/ Yang, L., Cao, Q., Shuai, L., Li, H., Chan, R. C., & Wang, Y. (2012).
hyperactivity disorder. Biological Psychiatry, 57(11), 1452–1460. Comparative study of OROS-MPH and atomoxetine on executive
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2005.02.029 function improvement in ADHD: A randomized controlled trial.
Prasad, S., Harpin, V., Poole, L., Zeitlin, H., Jamdar, S., Puvanendran, The International Journal of Neuropsychopharmacology, 15(1), 15–26.
K., & Group, S. S., on behalf of the SUNBEAM Study Group (2007). https://doi.org/10.1017/S1461145711001490
A multi-centre, randomised, open-label study of atomoxetine com- Yildiz, O., Sismanlar, S. G., Memik, N. C., Karakaya, I., & Agaoglu, B.
(2011). Atomoxetine and methylphenidate treatment in children
pared with standard current therapy in UK children and adolescents
with ADHD: The efficacy, tolerability and effects on executive func-
with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). Current
tions. Child Psychiatry and Human Development, 42(3), 257–269.
Medical Research and Opinion, 23(2), 379–394. https://doi.org/10.
https://doi.org/10.1007/s10578-010-0212-3
1185/030079906X167309 Yilmaz, A., Gokcen, C., Fettahoglu, E. C., & Ozatalay, E. (2013). The
Shang, C. Y., & Gau, S. S. F. (2012). Improving visual memory, atten- effect of methylphenidate on executive functions in children with
tion, and school function with atomoxetine in boys with attention- attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. Klinik Psikofarmakoloji
deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Journal of Child and Adolescent B€ulteni-Bulletin of Clinical Psychopharmacology, 23(2), 162–170.
Psychopharmacology, 22(5), 353–363. https://doi.org/10.1089/cap. https://doi.org/10.5455/bcp.20121130090251
2011.0149 Zheng, Y., Liang, J. M., Gao, H. Y., Yang, Z. W., Jia, F. J., Liang, Y. Z.,
Soysal, S. (2007). Dikkat eksiklig i hiperaktivite bozuklug u alttiplerinde & Zhuo, J. M. (2015). An open-label, self-control, prospective study
dikkat y€ onetici işlevler ve u €st-biliş performansının oluşturdugu on cognitive function, academic performance, and tolerability of
€r€
ilişkiler o unt€us€
un€un incelenmesi [Doktora Tezi [Doctoral disserta- osmotic-release oral system methylphenidate in children with atten-

tion]]. Hacettepe Universitesi, Sosyal Bilimler Enstit€ us€u, Ankara, tion-deficit hyperactivity disorder. Chinese Medical Journal, 128(22),
2007 (Thesis in Turkish). 2988.

You might also like