Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 10

685838

research-article2017
JADXXX10.1177/1087054716685838Journal of Attention DisordersÅsberg Johnels et al.

Article
Journal of Attention Disorders

A Hyperlexic-Like Reading Style Is


1­–10
© The Author(s) 2017
Reprints and permissions:
Associated With Increased Autistic sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.nav
DOI: 10.1177/1087054716685838
https://doi.org/10.1177/1087054716685838

Features in Girls With ADHD journals.sagepub.com/home/jad

Jakob Åsberg Johnels1, Christopher Gillberg1, and Svenny Kopp1

Abstract
Objective: Hyperlexic-like reading (defined as word decoding much better than comprehension) has been associated with
autism spectrum disorder (ASD). Here we study correlates of a hyperlexic-like reading style (HPL) in ADHD, a condition
known to co-occur both with reading difficulties and ASD. Method: We compared 10 girls with an ADHD diagnosis plus
HPL with 26 with ADHD minus HPL. Results: Girls with HPL scored marginally lower in reading comprehension but did
not differ from non-HPL girls in IQ, vocabulary, or in the severity of ADHD ratings. However, in addition to scoring much
better on word decoding, HPL readers also displayed higher levels of social-communication deficits on the ADOS-G and
the ADI-R. Moreover, correlation analysis in the full sample revealed an association between increasing autistic features
and word reading. Conclusion: The study underscores the heterogeneity of reading skills in ADHD, and shows the
relevance of subclinic autistic features for understanding this variability. (J. of Att. Dis. XXXX; XX(X) XX-XX)

Keywords
autism, ADHD, reading, comprehension, hyperlexia, autistic traits

Introduction The relative strength in word decoding in ASD contrasts


with reading comprehension, that is, the extraction of mean-
Research into the reading skills of school-age children with ing from written text, which is often difficult for many chil-
autism spectrum disorder (ASD) has noted great within- dren with ASD. The source of poor reading comprehension in
group variation (e.g., Åsberg, Kopp, Berg-Kelly, & Gillberg, ASD is largely unknown (Saldana & Frith, 2007), but has pre-
2010; Nation, Clarke, Wright, & Williams, 2006; White viously been suggested to reflect the coherence-making diffi-
et al., 2006), which is similar to the variability in cognitive culties, the oral language impairments, or the executive
and oral language skills seen in the population. Still, several difficulties that tend to be associated with ASD (Nation, 1999;
research studies of ASD have also noted a distinct pattern of Newman et al., 2007; Henderson, Clarke, & Snowling, 2011).
reading ability. Specifically, word reading/decoding skills Core difficulties in social-communication might also affect
have often been reported to be better developed than read- reading comprehension negatively, possibly independently of
ing comprehension (for recent reviews and empirical exam- basic language and cognitive skills. For instance, Jones et al.
ples, see Huemer & Mann, 2010; Nation, 1999; Nation (2009) described and explored “dips” in reading comprehen-
et al., 2006; Newman et al., 2007; Saldana, Carreiras, & sion attainment relative to IQ in a relatively large sample of
Frith, 2009). Interestingly, some researchers have argued adolescent males (n = 80) and females (n = 20) with ASD.
that the word reading process draws on cognitive strengths These researchers found that a discrepancy score between
in ASD; for example, a preoccupation with and close atten- reading comprehension performance and IQ was significantly
tion to details (Nation, 1999), relatively well developed related to the severity of social and communicative difficulties
phonological skills (Newman et al., 2007; Saldana et al., according to results from the Autistic Diagnostic Observation
2009) and strong visuo-spatial skills (Åsberg Johnels & Schedule–Generic (ADOS-G; Lord et al., 2000), thus “sug-
Miniscalco, 2014) that could support written word process- gesting that reading comprehension falls increasingly below
ing skills and development. Furthermore, Grigorenko, Klin,
and Volkmar (2003) highlighted that some children with
ASD display a restricted and repetitive interest for decoding 1
University of Gothenburg, Sweden
print that may fuel a (circumscribed) word reading exper-
Corresponding Author:
tise. In fact, the hypothesis of an attentional orientation Jakob Åsberg Johnels, Gillbergcentrum, Göteborgs universitet,
toward print and letters in ASD has received empirical sup- Kungsgatan 12, 411 19 Göteborg, Sweden.
port in an eye-tracking study (Nakano et al., 2010). Email: jakob.asberg@gnc.gu.se
2 Journal of Attention Disorders 

and ASD features has also been established in the literature


(e.g., Gillberg, 1983; Hattori et al., 2006; Kopp, Berg-Kelly,
& Gillberg, 2010). Moreover, a literature review by Uchida
et al. (2013) and a recent study by Biederman et al. (2016)
found that when compared with children with ADHD with-
out these traits, the presence of autistic traits in children
with ADHD was associated with more severe dysfunction
in a wide range of non-overlapping domains.
Much research has been done on word reading-decoding
in ADHD. The existing research has established that word
decoding/dyslexic difficulties are common among children
with ADHD. Estimates from different studies indicate that
somewhere between 25% and 40% has comorbid reading
disability (e.g., Gillberg & Gillberg 1989; cf. Germano,
Gagliano, & Curatolo, 2010, for a review). According to
Willcutt et al. (2007), correlations between continuous mea-
Figure 1. A graphic topology of different types of reading/ sures of word reading and ADHD-symptomatology usually
comprehension difficulties based on the framework of Aaron fall between .2 and .4 among school-aged children, and
(1989/2012).
Source. Figure from Åsberg Johnels (2014). there is much evidence to suggest that the correlations with
word reading tend to be stronger for inattention than for
hyperactivity-impulsivity. Less research is available for
general intellectual ability in line with increasing social and reading comprehension in ADHD, but available evidence
communication impairments” (p. 724). In a more recent study, suggests that reading comprehension appears to be a chal-
using the same sample, Ricketts, Jones, Happé, and Charman lenge (e.g., Mayes & Calhoun, 2006). Such difficulties have
(2013) showed that measures of social behavior and social been linked with executive dysfunctions within the reading
cognition predicted reading comprehension performance context (Barkley, 1997; Miller et al., 2013), but could in
even when statistically controlling for the variance explained many cases also be secondary to inaccurate and/or dysflu-
by word reading-decoding and oral language comprehension. ent word decoding. To our knowledge, no research has
The authors argue that this result suggests that text reading examined the hyperlexic-like patterns of reading in school-
comprehension and social-communicative functioning may age children with ADHD.
be functionally coupled in ASD. Specifically, failing to under- In a previous study, we have reported data on reading
stand social and communicative norms may hamper a reader’s skills in school-aged girls with ASD, ADHD, and typical
ability to make inferences and, consequently, constrain the development (Åsberg et al., 2010). In that study, the ADHD-
processing of the overall gist in connected text (cf. Wahlberg group as a whole conformed to a “mixed word reading/
& Magliano, 2004, for a similar argument). comprehension difficulty”–pattern of reading, in keeping
Although consensus is lacking in the best usage of the with previous research on male or predominantly male sam-
term,1 in previous research, the discrepant pattern of stron- ples (Mayes & Calhoun, 2006). In the current study, we
ger word reading vis-à-vis comprehension has been referred return to these data specifically to look for hyperlexic-like
to as hyperlexia (for reviews, see Grigorenko et al., 2003; discrepancies between (better) word decoding and (poorer)
Nation, 1999). This pattern of reading is clearly distinct reading comprehension among individual participants in
from dyslexia, which often is defined as poor decoding the group with ADHD. Initially, we therefore asked whether
skills despite normal language comprehension (e.g., Catts, there were cases of hyperlexic-like reading (HPL) within
Hogan, & Fey, 2003, and many others). Hyperlexia also dif- the ADHD–group. Once identified, we asked whether HPL
fers from mixed word reading/comprehension difficulty, reading in ADHD was coupled with a certain cognitive,
where both comprehension and decoding are poorly devel- psycholinguistic, or behavioral phenotype when compared
oped. A graphic topology of these types of reading/compre- with non-HPL cases of ADHD. Specifically, considering
hension difficulties is presented in Figure 1. that an overlap in expression has been suggested, we
Another, much more common, neurodevelopmental dis- hypothesized that HPL would be associated with elevated
order is ADHD. ADHD is characterized by a constellation levels of autistic symptomatology (i.e., social, communica-
of difficulties in attention and/or activity level and impul- tive, and/or restricted and repetitive behaviors). We took a
sivity, and is typically associated with impaired social and dimensional approach in analyzing this question as the ASD
academic functioning (American Psychiatric Association symptoms of all ADHD participants in the study were at
[APA], 1994). While ADHD is commonly considered a dis- “subthreshold” levels for an ASD-diagnosis. In addition, a
tinct condition relative to ASD, an overlap between ADHD number of other potentially explanatory factors that have
Åsberg Johnels et al. 3

been linked with word decoding and/or reading comprehen- disorder were met. On the basis of all available information,
sion skills in children were assessed. Specifically, perfor- the senior author S.K., an experienced child, and adolescent
mance and verbal IQ, oral vocabulary, as well as the severity psychiatrist assigned diagnoses after a thorough discussion
of core difficulties in hyperactivity-impulsivity and/or inat- with the professionals who had performed the individual
tention could be assumed to be important for both word assessments. All records and diagnoses were rechecked on
decoding and reading comprehension skill development. two further occasions: at a follow-up meeting after 3 months
Therefore, these factors were also explored in relation to and after all evaluations were finished. The diagnoses of
HPL reading in ADHD, although we had no a priori hypoth- ambiguous cases were discussed in detail with another
eses regarding these latter associations. author (C.G.), a professor in child and adolescent psychia-
try with very long-term experience of clinical and research
work in neuropsychiatry. Of the 36 cases with ADHD, 20
Method met criteria for the combined subtype, 14 for the predomi-
Participants nantly inattentive subtype, and two for the predominantly
hyperactive-impulsive subtype. A separate paper published
Three groups participated in the previously mentioned in the current journal provides detailed description of the
study (“The Girl Project”) by Åsberg et al. (2010), namely, procedure and clinical presentation of the participants
clinic girls with ADHD (n = 36), ASD (n = 20), and com- (Kopp et al., 2010).
parison girls without known disabilities (n = 54), aged The mothers of all girls provided informed consent to
between 8 to 17 years of age. The three groups were not participate. All the girls ≥16 years also provided informed
significantly different in terms of chronological age or consent personally. The Ethics Committee at the University
WISC-III performance IQ (Wechsler, 1992) (see below) . of Gothenburg approved the study.
Girls with ASD and ADHD had been referred to the child There is no consensus as to how to define or identify
neuropsychiatric clinic (CNC) in Gothenburg, Sweden, for hyperlexia (e.g., with regard to the severity of the compre-
social, mental health, and/or attentional difficulties (no hension deficit, the type and size of discrepancy, the neces-
advertisement). None of the girls was or had previously sity for a spontaneously acquired reading skill, and so on;
been on medication for difficulties relating to ADHD at the for example, Grigorenko et al., 2003; Nation, 1999), and it
time of assessment. Girls in the comparison group were is not a formal diagnosis in the DSM-IV or in any other offi-
recruited from a local pediatric outpatient clinic register in cial diagnostic manual. Because of this situation and these
the Gothenburg region (Mölnlycke), and these girls did not inconsistencies, we here prefer to use the term “hyperlexic-
have any neurodevelopmental disabilities according to out- like reading style” (HPL) rather than “hyperlexia” in our
patient registers and interviews with parents. study to signal that this is a descriptive label rather than a
All girls had a full scale IQ above 70, and diagnoses formal medical diagnosis with clearly agreed criteria. To
were based on Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental operationalize HPL, a >15 standard score discrepancy (i.e.,
Disorders (4th ed.; DSM-IV; American Psychiatric 1 SD) cutoff was applied between (stronger) word decoding
Association, 1994) criteria. All girls received a full neuro- and (weaker) reading comprehension according to test
psychiatric/neurodevelopmental examination by a multi- results collected at the clinic (see test descriptions below).
professional team at the CNC, consisting of psychiatrists, This means, in turn, that levels of word decoding and read-
psychologist, and educational specialist. All members of the ing comprehension were allowed to be quite variable in
team had relevant formal training and experience for their absolute levels. However, the procedure ensured that all
respective part of the assessments as described below. The HPL readers were much stronger in word decoding than in
assessment included interview and examination of each girl reading comprehension, which is consistent with the opera-
and interview with the parent(s) regarding the child’s early tionalization in most current research (e.g., Castles,
development and current behaviors and symptoms. The Crichton, & Prior, 2010).
examination lasted on an average 20 hr for clinic girls and According to the criterion adopted, there were individu-
10 hr for comparison girls. Moreover, all girls were observed als with HPL in all three groups in the sample described in
at school by the study educationalists, and the same educa- Åsberg et al. (2010). Specifically, in the ASD group, eight
tionalist also interviewed all the teachers about the girls’ out of 192 girls (42%) showed HPL. This was significantly
behavioral problems. The “Five to Fifteen” (FTF) question- different (p = .017; Fisher’s exact two-tailed test) from the
naire (Kadesjö et al., 2004) and the Conners’ Teacher Rating comparison group where seven out of 54 girls (13%)
Scale–Revised: Long Form (CTRS-R; Conners, Sitarenios, showed HPL.3 Interestingly, also in the ADHD-group, a siz-
Parker, & Epstein, 1998; see details below) were used to able minority were classified as displaying HPL.
gather ratings of ADHD symptoms from parents and teach- Specifically, 10 out of 36 (28%) met our criteria for HPL,
ers, respectively. Diagnoses according to DSM-IV were and this rate approached significance (p = .10) in relation to
assigned when both symptom and impairment criteria for a the comparison group. The current study report describes
4 Journal of Attention Disorders 

and compares the characteristics of these 10 girls with et al., 2006), the oral vocabulary subtest from the
ADHD (but not ASD) with HPL (ADHD + HPL) with those verbal scale on WISC-III was also analyzed sepa-
26 girls with ADHD (but not ASD) without HPL (ADHD rately. This subtest is expressed in scaled scores
− HPL). around a mean of 10 (SD = 3).
3. Inattention and hyperactivity/impulsivity (ADHD-
symptomatology): Ratings of DSM-IV (APA, 1994)
Measures
symptoms of inattention (nine criteria) and hyperac-
Selection measures. Performance in reading comprehension tivity/impulsivity (nine criteria) were collected from
and word decoding was assessed individually at the clinic teachers and parents using the CTRS-R (Conners
by the same special educator. The following instruments et al., 1998) and the “Five to Fifteen” (FTF) ques-
were used: tionnaire (Kadesjö et al., 2004), respectively. The
CTRS-R items are scored as 0, 1, 2, or 3 for “not
1. The tests “Diagnostiska läs- och skrivprov” true at all,” “just a little true,” “pretty much true,” or
(“Diagnostic reading and writing tests”) were used “very much true,” respectively. The FTF items are
to assess reading comprehension for girls in Grades scored as 0, 1, or 2 for “does not apply,” “applies
2 to 6 (Grade 2 and 3: Björkquist & Järpsten, sometimes,” or “definitely applies,” respectively.
1975/1976; Grade 4-6: Järpsten & Taube, 1997). Five girls with ADHD did not have teacher ratings
The LS test (Johansson, 1992) was used for older (three ADHD + HPL and two ADHD − HPL).
girls. These tests all consist of passage and text com- Results are expressed in T-scores (M = 50, SD = 10)
prehension with comprehension questions or state- and raw scores on the Conners’ teacher ratings and
ments in a multiple-choice format. FTF, respectively. According to Conners et al.
2. Timed tests of single, out of context, word reading (1998), a T-score > 65 indicates a clinically relevant
were chosen as a measure of word decoding effi- level of difficulty.
ciency. The H4 test (Franzén, 1997) was used for 4. Autistic symptomatology was assessed in two
girls in Grades 2 to 6 (approximately 8-12 years), ways: (a) The Autism Diagnostic Observation
and the LS test (Johansson, 1992) was used for the Schedule–Generic (ADOS-G; Lord et al., 2000)
older girls. The number of correctly read words/time was used. The ADOS-G is a semi-structured assess-
unit was registered. Such efficiency measures are ment of social interaction, communication, play,
typically used in Sweden and other semi-consistent and imaginative use of materials. Due to the range
orthographies. in age and functioning of the individuals in our
study, either Module 3 or 4 was administered. The
As reported in Åsberg et al. (2010), the raw scores on difference between Modules 3 and 4 lies primarily
these tests were first converted to z scores, based on the in whether information about social-communica-
population means and SDs reported in the test manuals for tion is more appropriately acquired during play or
different ages. Specific norms for girls were used when an interview, but both modules involve conversa-
available. After this, z scores were converted into standard tion and overlap in activities. In the ADHD + HPL
scores based on the normative mean of 100 and a SD of 15, group, n = 5 had Module 3 and n = 4 had Module 4.
with a standard score floor of 50. This procedure could not ADOS data is lacking for one girl with ADHD +
be upheld throughout, as SDs were not reported in the read- HPL. In the ADHD − HPL group, n = 17 had
ing comprehension test manual for 8- and 9-year olds; only Module 3 and n = 9 had Module 4. Although
norm-referenced stanine values were reported (=the stan- Modules 3 and 4 are not completely parallel in
dard procedure for reporting test results in Swedish schools). terms of scaling, they were considered sufficiently
We therefore calculated the standard scores based on the similar to be analyzed together in the current study.
obtained stanine score for these participants to allow for Furthermore, we ensured that the distribution of
comparisons across participants, while also maximizing modules for the two groups was not significantly
data information. different (Fisher’s exact test: p = .69). (b) The
Autism Diagnostic Interview–Revised (ADI-R;
Other measures Lord, Rutter, & Le Couteur, 1994) is a standard-
1. Verbal IQ and Performance IQ from WISC-III were ized, semi-structured clinical interview with the
collected for the girls. These two measures are caregivers of the children. Higher scores indicate
expressed around a mean of 100 and a SD of 15. higher current and prior behaviors related to autism.
2. Considering the large literature placing oral vocabu- Besides a total score, specific scores are provided
lary skills at the center of the reading comprehen- for the social, communication, and repetitive/ste-
sion ability (e.g., Hulme & Snowling, 2009; Nation reotyped behaviors domains.
Åsberg Johnels et al. 5

Table 1. Descriptives (Means and SDs) in Study Groups For the Selection, Background, and Clinical Measures.

Group

Measure ADHD + HPL ADHD − HPL


Selection and background measures
Word decodinga* 109.2 (23.6) 76.5 (16.0)
Reading comprehensiona 75.8 (21.4) 89.5 (18.6)
Chronological ageb 13.1 (3.1) 12.9 (2.1)
Clinical measures
Performance IQa 93.6 (15.3) 99.8. (15.5)
Verbal IQa 93.1 (16.4) 93.4 (11.9)
Vocabularyc 9.7 (2.7) 9.0 (2.5)
Inattention, teacher ratingd 72.4 (16.1) 73.9 (13.3)
Hyperactivity-impulsivity, teacher ratingd 62.1 (17.3) 69.0 (18.6)
Inattention, parent ratingb 14.5 (3.0) 13.1 (4.6)
Hyperactivity-impulsivity, parent ratingb 9.1 (4.2) 8.0 (5.4)
ADOS-G, totalb* 5.2 (4.4) 2.3 (2.3)
ADI-R, communicationb* 4.9 (3.4) 2.8 (3.0)
ADI-R, socialb 4.5 (4.6) 3.7 (3.4)
ADI-R, repetitive behaviorsb 1.9 (1.6) .96 (1.2)
ADI-R, totalb 12.2 (9.4) 8.5 (6.5)

Note. ADOS-G = Autistic Diagnostic Observation Schedule–Generic; ADI-R = The Autism Diagnostic Interview–Revised.
a
Standard scores (based on the normative M = 100, SD = 15).
b
Raw score.
c
Scaled scores (based on the normative M = 10, SD = 3).
d
T-scores (based on the normative M = 50, SD = 10).
*Significant group difference according to Mann–Whitney U test.

Statistical Analyses As expected, the ADHD + HPL group scored statistically


better than the ADHD − HPL group in word decoding, U =
Mann–Whitney U tests were used to compare the ADHD + 34.0, p = .001. This contrasted with the results in reading
HPL and ADHD − HPL groups on the measures of interest. comprehension where there was a non-significant advan-
Nonparametric statistics was preferred (a) due to small and tage for the ADHD − HPL group, U = 79.0, p = .07.
unequally sized samples, (b) due to several cases of non- The two groups did not differ in chronological age, U =
normality according to histogram inspection, and (c) to 115.5, p = .61.
reduce the impact of any outliers in the data set. In addition,
non-parametric correlation analyses (Spearman’s rho) were
conducted to explore bivariate associations. The signifi- IQ and Vocabulary Scores
cance level was set to p < .05 (two-tailed). The groups did not differ on performance IQ (U = 94.0, p =
.20) or on verbal IQ (U = 124.5, p = .85). Furthermore, they did
Results not differ on oral vocabulary (U = 121.5, p = 76), with both
groups approaching the normative value of 10 on this subtest.
Selection and Background Measures
Descriptives (means and SDs) are presented in Table 1 for
Inattentive and Hyperactive-Impulsive Symptoms
selection, background, and clinical measures. Girls with
ADHD + HPL had a mean score on the word decoding mea- The groups were equally challenged in terms of ADHD-
sure in the high average range, but scored strikingly poorly symptomatology according teacher and parent ratings of
in reading comprehension. As can be seen in Table 1, the inattention and hyperactivity-impulsivity, ps range between
average discrepancy between word reading and reading .24 to .78. Note that both groups scored high on the stan-
comprehension in the HPL group was more than 30 stan- dardized teacher ratings, with the Inattentiveness scale
dard points (i.e., 2 SDs). The ADHD − HPL group scored averaging more than 2 SDs above the normative mean of 50
poorly in word decoding (approximately 1.5 SDs below the on the T scale.
normative mean) and also low (albeit somewhat higher) in We also checked the prevalence of ADHD subtypes in
reading comprehension. the two groups. In the ADHD + HPL group, five had the
6 Journal of Attention Disorders 

combined subtype of ADHD and five the primarily inatten- 2010), the ADHD-group appeared to resemble a mixed
tive subtype. In the ADHD − HPL group, 15 had the com- word reading/comprehension difficulty group as a whole,
bined subtype of ADHD, nine the primarily inattentive but in the current study, it was clear that a substantial minor-
subtype, and two the primarily hyperactive-impulsive ity among children with ADHD sharply diverge from this
subtype. pattern. This finding, once again, highlights that the “aver-
aging artefact” is a constant threat in research on childhood
neurodevelopmental disabilities, suggesting that the mean
Autistic Symptomatology
score does not necessarily well represent the performance
On the ADOS, readers with ADHD + HPL had a signifi- of any single participant (Brock, 2011). Yet, we also want to
cantly higher total social-communication score, thus indi- remind the reader that the current study exclusively included
cating greater autistic symptomatology in this group, U = Swedish-speaking girls. Whether the results can be general-
54.0, p = .016. Also on the ADI-R communication deficit ized to boys and to children from other countries and lan-
score did the ADHD + HPL group score significantly higher guage background could be topics for future research.
than the ADHD − HPL group, U = 71.5, p = .037. No sig- Second, strong word decoding in the context of poorer
nificant difference (p > .05) was found between groups on comprehension was associated with an increased presence
the ADI total, “repetitive,” or “social” scores, though of certain autistic symptoms. The ADHD + HPL group
numerically the trend pointed in the same direction on these scored in the high average range according to the norm-
measures. Mean scores on the ADOS-G and the ADI-R all referenced word decoding measure. This result is particu-
fell below clinical cutoffs for ASD in both reading sub- larly striking in view of the fact that the two groups did not
groups, providing further support that we explore the asso- differ in ratings of ADHD-characteristics, which were very
ciations between HPL and autistic features in cases with high in both groups. The fact that a complementary analysis
ADHD and subthreshold levels for ASD. also revealed a positive correlation between increasing
autistic features ADOS-G and word decoding skills is all
the more remarkable. However, this association was not
Complementary Correlation Analyses in the Full
equally clear on the ADI-scores, suggesting that the result
Sample should be interpreted with caution. Still, these finding high-
Given the pattern of performance displayed by the ADHD + lights that researchers with an interest in the connection
HPL group, we wanted to explore the associations between between word reading difficulties/dyslexia and ADHD may
autistic features (measured with the ADOS-G and ADI-R) and want to take account of the moderating role of autistic
word reading-decoding and reading comprehension in greater symptomatology in their studies.
detail. For this purpose, correlation analyses was performed Although striking, the finding that co-occurring autism
between these variables in the full sample with ADHD (N = features in a clinical sample are coupled with good word
36). Results showed that ADOS scores were positively associ- decoding is not unique to this study. A longitudinal study by
ated with word decoding (rho = .357, p = .035) suggesting St. Clair, Durkin, Conti-Ramsden, and Pickles (2010) fol-
that, in this sample, increasing autistic features were associ- lowed children with specific language impairment (SLI)
ated with better word reading capacity. However, the correla- with or without elevated levels of autistic features, and in
tions between word decoding and the different ADI-scores fact found stronger word decoding skills in the comorbid
were not significant (all p > .09), nor were the correlations group. While the study by St. Clair et al. differed in a num-
with reading comprehension (all p > .16). ber of ways from the current one,4 it is still of interest in the
current context, as it pointed in the same direction.
Moreover, when researchers and clinicians in the field of
Discussion neurodevelopmental disorders examine comorbidity, it is
A hyperlexic-like style of reading (HPL), defined as word typically framed in terms of “extra burdens” (Biederman
decoding substantially better than reading comprehension, et al., 2016). The current research and that by St. Clair et al.
was found in an important minority of female school-age both demonstrate that when it comes to the isolated skill of
children with a diagnosis of ADHD. The subgroup that pre- word decoding, then the comorbidity of autism features is
sented with HPL was furthermore found to have elevated not necessarily an “extra burden.”
levels of autistic features according to the social and com- Third, the other aspect of the HPL phenotype is severe
munication score of the ADOS-G and the communication comprehension difficulties, and this was evident in the
difficulties score of the ADI-R. These findings are, we ADHD + HPL group in the current study as well. Given a
think, of interest for several reasons. strong word reading score (ss109), the question arises why
First, it proved to be worthwhile to look for individual the HPL-group experienced poor reading comprehension
differences and reading patterns within the ADHD-group. (ss76) in the first place. While acknowledging the associa-
In our first literacy study on this sample (Åsberg et al., tional nature of the current study, we suspect that certain
Åsberg Johnels et al. 7

features directly associated with the autism might be caus- Speaking against this hypothesis is, however, the fact that
ally linked with text-level comprehension difficulties. several other populations have been shown to sometimes
Cognitively oriented research has focused on different comprehend text poorly without obvious signs of either
psycholinguistic levels as potential sources to text compre- autism or word decoding difficulties, for example, typically
hension difficulties experienced by children with fluent developing children who read in a second language (e.g.,
word reading-decoding: from word semantics (vocabulary) Burgoyne, Kelly née Hutchinson, Whiteley, & Spooner,
to meta-cognition and self-regulation within the reading 2009). Taken together, our study show that HPL reading
context (for reviews, see Cain & Oakhill, 2007; Hulme & appears to be associated with subclinical social-communi-
Snowling, 2009). In their research on children with ASD- cative difficulties among students with ADHD as the pri-
diagnosis, Nation et al. (2006) and Norbury and Nation mary disorder, and we have tentatively ruled out some
(2011) have stressed the importance of impaired vocabulary possible explanatory variables or confounders (e.g., oral
and other core oral language skills when explaining reading vocabulary skills or general IQ) in this association. Rather,
comprehension difficulties in this group of children. In the our study strengthens the notion of a connection between
current study, the groups did not differ in oral vocabulary. autistic features (especially communicative or social-com-
Furthermore, both groups had age-adequate oral vocabulary municative difficulties) and HPL reading, which in this
skills relative to norm data (mean scaled scores of 10 and 9 study was expanded to a novel population (ADHD) and at a
for ADHD + HPL and ADHD − HPL, respectively). Thus, it subclinical level of autistic traits.
seems difficult to explain the reading comprehension diffi- A final question pertains to the label used to describe
culties displayed by the ADHD + HPL group with reference the study groups in our study. Given the definitional
to their vocabulary knowledge. Of course, this does not inconsistencies surrounding the hyperlexia term, we pre-
mean that oral language comprehension skills in the HPL- ferred to use the term “a hyperlexic-like reading style” to
group were “normal.” Rather, research on reading in chil- describe the patterns of reading displayed by the ADHD +
dren with clinical diagnoses of ASD suggest that poor HPL group. Still, it could be questioned whether “hyper-
text-level comprehension might reflect underlying social- lexic” is the best word to use at all. Grigorenko and col-
communicative difficulties that by definition are integral to leagues (2003) suggested that the hyperlexia-concept
ASD, possibly in the form of impaired pragmatic inferenc- should be reserved for a type of reading behavior seen in
ing skills (e.g., Ricketts et al., 2013) and/or difficulties in some children with ASD-diagnosis, and they also sug-
identifying author intentions and tracking mental states of gested other aspects than a word decoding vis-à-vis read-
characters in the texts (Hulme & Snowling, 2009). The text ing comprehension discrepancy in the definition (e.g., a
comprehension impairment could also be related to linguis- precocious and spontaneously acquired reading ability). In
tically based cognitive control difficulties. A novel perspec- contrast, Snowling and Frith (1986) argued that “hyper-
tive in this context was offered by Henderson et al. (2011). lexia is not a syndrome-specific phenomenon” (p. 410).
These researchers used a semantic priming paradigm origi- This discussion relates broadly to the unsettled issue of
nally developed in cognitive psychology to show that indi- whether hyperlexia best should be conceptualized a super-
viduals with ASD appear to have intact access to semantic ability (in word decoding relative to a low overall func-
information early in the time course of text processing, but tioning of the child) or as a disability (in reading
that impairments are present in the later—top-down con- comprehension relative to age-expectations and word
trolled—aspects of the meaning-making process. Future decoding; Grigorenko et al., 2003). Notwithstanding
research could examine if such difficulties underlie text which “label” to use to best describe the current group’s
comprehension difficulties in children with ADHD with or pattern of ability, we believe that there is a quite complex
without autistic traits, and whether these measures also dif- causal-developmental story behind the set of findings
ferentiate within a population of children with ADHD reported in the current study, and the mismatch between
depending on HPL status. strong word decoding and weak comprehension is of both
A completely different possible explanation for the link theoretical and clinical/practical importance. We also
between autistic features and weak reading comprehension believe that a lot could be learned about reading and com-
was preliminarily suggested by St. Clair et al. (2010) in prehension as well as the cognitive phenotypes of ADHD
their SLI study, namely, that the direction of the relation and autism, more generally, by shedding further light on
could be reversed. Indeed, these authors speculated that the nature of these associations. In terms of practical clini-
“being able to adequately decode words without the corre- cal consequences, the study underscores the heterogeneity
sponding level of understanding may . . . create a sense of of reading skills in ADHD. The results also speak against
isolation in these individuals, which may lead to social dif- models of neurodevelopmental examinations that build on
ficulties and other features of autistic symptomatology” (St. sharp demarcations between categorically defined condi-
Clair et al., 2010, p. 127). This is an intriguing proposal and tions; instead, they show the value of also using a dimen-
may account for the findings also in the present study. sional approach to parse this heterogeneity, where
8 Journal of Attention Disorders 

symptoms at subthreshold levels for diagnosis (e.g., autis- Åsberg, J., Kopp, S., Berg-Kelly, K., & Gillberg, C. (2010). Reading
tic features) provide important information in terms of comprehension, word decoding and spelling in girls with
outcome in another domain (e.g., profiles of reading dif- autism spectrum disorders or AD/HD: Performance and pre-
ficulties). In terms of implications for educational support, dictors. International Journal of Language & Communication
Disorders, 45, 61-71.
the results show that individuals with ADHD often need
Åsberg Johnels, J., & Miniscalco, C. (2014). Excellent word-read-
support for reading-related difficulties, but that such sup-
ing ability in the context of an autism spectrum disorder: A
port must be tailored according to the individual’s profile case study of a Swedish-speaking 7-year-old boy. Journal of
of difficulties: Whereas readers with ADHD − HPL likely Cognitive Education and Psychology, 13, 88-102.
need (compensation and/or training) to ameliorate poor Barkley, R. A. (1997). ADHD and the nature of self-control. New
word reading alongside reading comprehension difficul- York, NY: Guilford.
ties, readers with ADHD + HPL need to be supported in Biederman, J., Fitzgerald, M., Faraone, S. V., Fried, R.,
“reading for meaning,” rather than just decoding the Woodworth, K. Y., Saunders, A., & Joshi, G. (2016). Are
words, which otherwise might be their spontaneous autistic traits in youth meaningful? A replication study in non-
response in a reading situation. Research describing effec- referred siblings of youth with and without attention-deficit/
tive reading comprehension interventions for students hyperactivity disorder. Scandinavian Journal of Child and
Adolescent Psychiatry and Psychology, 1, 77-87.
with an ASD-diagnosis might be informative in this regard
Björkquist, L.-M., & Järpsten, B. (1976). Manual till diagnostiska
(e.g., Randi, Newman, & Grigorenko, 2010).
läs- och skrivprov för årskurs 2/3 [“Manual for diagnostic
tests of reading and writing for grades 2/3”]. Stockholm,
Declaration of Conflicting Interests
Sweden: Psykologiförlaget. (In Swedish) (Original work pub-
The author(s) declared no potential conflicts of interest with lished 1975)
respect to the research, authorship, and/or publication of this Brock, J. (2011). Commentary: Complementary approaches to the
article. developmental cognitive neuroscience of autism—Reflections
on Pelphrey et al. (2011). Journal of Child Psychology and
Funding Psychiatry, 52, 645-646.
The author(s) received no financial support for the research, Burgoyne, K., Kelly née Hutchinson, J. M., Whiteley, H. E., &
authorship, and/or publication of this article. Spooner, A. (2009). The comprehension skills of children
learning English as an additional language. British Journal of
Notes Educational Psychology, 79, 735-747.
Cain, K., & Oakhill, J. (2007). Reading comprehension difficul-
1. See the “Method” section for a further discussion of the defi- ties: Correlates, causes and consequences. In K. Cain & J.
nition used in the current study. Oakhill (Eds.), Children’s comprehension problems in oral
2. Word decoding results was missing for one girl with autism and written language: A cognitive perspective (pp. 41-75).
spectrum disorder (ASD), due to selective mutism. New York, NY: Guilford Press.
3. To refer to children (without other known disabilities) who Castles, A., Crichton, A., & Prior, M. (2010). Developmental
present with poor reading comprehension in the face of stron- dissociations between lexical reading and comprehen-
ger word reading capacity, the term “poor comprehenders” sion: Evidence from two cases of hyperlexia. Cortex, 46,
is commonly used. To our knowledge, there is no published 1238-1247.
research on Swedish poor comprehenders, but Hulme and Catts, H. W., Hogan, T. P., & Fey, M. E. (2003). Subgrouping
Snowling (2009) reported that the incidence of poor compre- poor readers on the basis of individual differences in read-
henders is about 10% in English schools. ing-related abilities. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 36,
4. For example, it focused on another clinical group, and the 151-164.
discrepancy between word decoding and reading comprehen- Conners, C. K., Sitarenios, G., Parker, J. D., & Epstein, J. N.
sion was lower in their “comorbid” group: standard scores of (1998). Revision and restandardization of the Conners
88 versus 79, respectively. Teacher Rating Scale (CTRS-R): Factor structure, reliability,
and criterion validity. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology,
References 26, 279-291.
Aaron, P. G. (2012). Dyslexia and hyperlexia: Diagnosis and man- Franzén, L. (1997). Diagnostisering av elever med läs- och
agement of developmental reading disabilities (Vol. 1). New skrivssvårigheter [“Diagnosing reading and writing difficul-
York, NY: Springer Science & Business Media. (Original ties in pupils”]. Solna, Sweden: Ekelunds förlag. (In Swedish)
work published 1989) Germano, E., Gagliano, A., & Curatolo, P. (2010). Comorbidity
American Psychiatric Association. (1994). Diagnostic and statis- of ADHD and dyslexia. Developmental Neuropsychology, 35,
tical manual of mental disorders (4th ed.). Washington, DC: 475-493.
Author. Gillberg, C. (1983). Perceptual, motor and attentional deficits in
Åsberg Johnels, J. (2014). Reading and text comprehension skills Swedish primary school children. Some child psychiatric aspects.
in school-age children with autism spectrum disorders. In Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 24, 377-403.
VB Patel, VR Preedy, & CR Martin (Eds.), Comprehensive Gillberg, I. C., & Gillberg, C. (1989). Children with preschool
guide to autism (pp. 1031-1042). New York, NY: Springer. minor neurodevelopmental disorders. IV: Behaviour and
Åsberg Johnels et al. 9

school achievement at age 13. Developmental Medicine & Nakano, T., Tanaka, K., Endo, Y., Yamane, Y., Yamamoto, T.,
Child Neurology, 31, 3-13. Nakano, Y., . . . Kitazawa, S. (2010). Atypical gaze patterns in
Grigorenko, E. L., Klin, A., & Volkmar, F. (2003). Annotation: children and adults with autism spectrum disorders dissociated
Hyperlexia: Disability or superability? Journal of Child from developmental changes in gaze behaviour. Proceedings
Psychology and Psychiatry, 44, 1079-1091. of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological Sciences,
Hattori, J., Ogino, T., Abiru, K., Nakano, K., Oka, M., & Ohtsuka, 277, 2935-2943. doi:10.1098/rspb20100587
Y. (2006). Are pervasive developmental disorders and atten- Nation, K. (1999). Reading skills in hyperlexia: A developmental
tion-deficit/hyperactivity disorder distinct disorders. Brain & perspective. Psychological Bulletin, 125, 338-355.
Development, 28, 371-374. Nation, K., Clarke, P., Wright, B. J., & Williams, C. (2006).
Henderson, L. M., Clarke, P. J., & Snowling, M. J. (2011). Patterns of reading ability in children with autism spectrum
Accessing and selecting word meaning in autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders,
disorder. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 52, 36, 911-919.
964-973. Newman, T. M., Macomber, D., Naples, A. J., Babitz, T.,
Huemer, S. V., & Mann, V. (2010). A comprehensive profile Volkmar, F., & Grigorenko, E. L. (2007). Hyperlexia in chil-
of decoding and comprehension in autism spectrum disor- dren with autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and
ders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 40, Developmental Disorders, 37, 760-774.
485-493. Norbury, C., & Nation, K. (2011). Understanding variability in
Hulme, C., & Snowling, M. J. (2009). Developmental disorders of reading comprehension in adolescents with autism spectrum
language and cognition. Oxford, UK: Blackwell. disorders: Interactions with language status and decoding
Järpsten, B., & Taube, K. (1997). Diagnostiska läs och skrivprov skill. Scientific Studies of Reading, 15, 191-210.
– Manual för klasserna 4-6 [Diagnostic reading and writ- Randi, J., Newman, T., & Grigorenko, E. L. (2010). Teaching chil-
ing test—Manual for grades 4-6]. Stockholm, Sweden: dren with autism to read for meaning: Challenges and possi-
Psykologiförlaget. (In Swedish) bilities. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 40,
Johansson, M.-G. (1992). Klassdiagnoser i läsning och skrivning för 890-902.
högstadiet och gymnasiet [Tests in reading and writing for upper Ricketts, J., Jones, C. R., Happé, F., & Charman, T. (2013).
secondary school]. Stockholm, Sweden: Psykologiförlaget. (In
Reading comprehension in autism spectrum disorders: The
Swedish)
role of oral language and social functioning. Journal of
Jones, C. R. G., Happé, F., Golden, H., Marsden, A. J. S., Tregay,
Autism and Developmental Disorders, 43, 807-816.
J., Simonoff, E., . . . Charman, T. (2009). Reading and arith-
St. Clair, M. C., Durkin, K., Conti-Ramsden, G., & Pickles, A.
metic in adolescents with autism spectrum disorders: Peaks
(2010). Growth of reading skills in children with a history
and dips in attainment. Neuropsychology, 23, 718-728.
of specific language impairment: The role of autistic symp-
Kadesjö, B., Janols, L. O., Korkman, M., Mickelsson, K., Strand,
G., & Trillingsgaard, A. (2004). The FTF (Five to Fifteen): tomatology and language-related abilities. British Journal of
The development of a parent questionnaire for the assessment Developmental Psychology, 28, 109-131.
of ADHD and comorbid conditions. European Child and Saldana, D., Carreiras, M., & Frith, U. (2009). Orthographic
Adolescent Psychiatry, 13, 3-13. and phonological pathways to word reading in hyperlexic
Kopp, S., Berg-Kelly, K., & Gillberg, C. (2010). Girls with social readers with autism spectrum disorders. Developmental
and/or attention deficits: A descriptive study of 100 clinic Neuropsychology, 34, 240-253.
attenders. Journal of Attention Disorders, 14, 167-181. Saldana, D., & Frith, U. (2007). Do readers with autism make
Lord, C., Risi, S., Lambrecht, L., Cook, E. H., Jr., Leventhal, B. L., bridging inferences from world knowledge? Journal of
DiLavore, P. C., . . . Rutter, M. (2000). The autism diagnostic Experimental Child Psychology, 96, 310-319.
observation schedule-generic: A standard measure of social Snowling, M., & Frith, U. (1986). Comprehension in “hyper-
and communicative deficits associated with the spectrum of lexic” readers. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology,
autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 30, 42, 392-415.
205-223. Uchida, M., Faraone, S. V., Joshi, G., Spencer, A., Kenworthy,
Lord, C., Rutter, M., & Le Couteur, A. (1994). Autism Diagnostic T., Woodworth, K. Y., & Biederman, J. (2013). How preva-
Interview-Revised: A revised version of a diagnostic interview lent are autistic traits among children with attention-deficit/
for caregivers of individuals with possible pervasive devel- hyperactivity disorder? A qualitative review of the literature.
opmental disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Scandinavian Journal of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry
Disorders, 24, 659-685. and Psychology, 1, 33-40.
Mayes, S. D., & Calhoun, S. L. (2006). Frequency of reading, Wahlberg, T., & Magliano, J. P. (2004). The ability of high func-
math, and writing disabilities in children with clinical disor- tion individuals with autism to comprehend written discourse.
ders. Learning and Individual Differences, 16, 145-157. Discourse Processes, 38, 119-144.
Miller, A. C., Keenan, J. M., Betjemann, R. S., Willcutt, E. G., Wechsler, D. (1992). Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children (3rd
Pennington, B. F., & Olson, R. K. (2013). Reading compre- ed.). Stockholm, Sweden: Psykologiförlaget. (In Swedish)
hension in children with ADHD: Cognitive underpinnings of White, S., Frith, U., Milne, E., Rosen, S., Swettenham, J., &
the centrality deficit. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, Ramus, F. (2006). A double dissociation between senso-
41, 473-483. rimotor impairments and reading disability: A comparison of
10 Journal of Attention Disorders 

autistic and dyslexic children. Cognitive Neuropsychology, Christopher Gillberg is a professor of Child and Adolescent
23, 748-761. Psychiatry at the University of Gothenburg, Sweden, since the
Willcutt, E., Betjemann, R., Wadsworth, S., Samuelsson, S., mid-1980s. He heads the Gillberg Neuropsychiatry Centre. He is
Corley, R., & DeFries, J. C. (2007). Preschool twin study of the
also a Chief Physician at the Sahlgrenska University Hospital and
relation between attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder and
one of the world’s most experienced, clinically active, child- and
prereading skills. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary
Journal, 20, 103-125. adolescent psychiatrists, with over 40 years of extensive clinical
work in treatment of patients and families with complex psychiat-
ric/neurodevelopmental problems.
Author Biographies
Svenny Kopp, MD, PhD, is a specialist in child and adolescent
Jakob Åsberg Johnels, PhD, is an associate professor at the
University of Gothenburg, including at the Gillberg psychiatry, a senior physician and a researcher at the Gillberg
Neuropsychiatry Centre and the Section for Speech and language Neuropsychiatry Centre. Svenny Kopp’s research is focused on
pathology. He also works clinically with academic skills assess- females with neuropsychiatric difficulties. From 1999 to 2001,
ments at the Child Neuropsychiatric Clinic (CNC) at Queen Kopp led the “Girl Project”, and her current research involves a
Silvia Children’s Hospital, Sahlgrenska University Hospital. follow-up study of these girls (now young women).

You might also like