Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 7

601625

research-article2015
CDPXXX10.1177/0963721415601625Shnabel, NadlerAgency and Morality in Reconciliation

Current Directions in Psychological

The Role of Agency and Morality in Science


2015, Vol. 24(6) 477­–483
© The Author(s) 2015
Reconciliation Processes: The Perspective Reprints and permissions:
sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.nav

of the Needs-Based Model DOI: 10.1177/0963721415601625


cdps.sagepub.com

Nurit Shnabel and Arie Nadler


School of Psychological Sciences, Tel-Aviv University

Abstract
According to the needs-based model of reconciliation, transgressions threaten victims’ sense of agency and perpetrators’
moral image. Consequently, victims and perpetrators experience heightened needs for empowerment and acceptance,
respectively. Exchange interactions (e.g., expressions of apologies and forgiveness) through which victims and
perpetrators satisfy each other’s needs facilitate reconciliation. We present research that has supported the model
in both interpersonal and intergroup contexts. We then extend the model to “dual” conflicts, in which both parties
transgress against each other and compete over the victim status, and to intergroup contexts of structural inequality.
Finally, we discuss need satisfaction outside the victim-perpetrator dyad as an intriguing avenue for future research.

Keywords
reconciliation, the needs-based model, competitive victimhood, forgiveness, apology, agency, moral image, identity
restoration, interpersonal conflict, intergroup conflict

Whether it is divorcing couples fighting over their com- apology, forgiveness, and reconciliation after mass atroc-
mon property or nations clashing over territory, conflict- ity into public focus” (p. 59). The needs-based model of
ing parties almost never fight solely about tangible reconciliation (Nadler & Shnabel, 2008; Shnabel & Nadler,
resources. Rather, their conflict often revolves around 2008) was developed to explain the dynamics between
symbolic resources such as honor, recognition, and jus- victims and perpetrators within the apology-forgiveness
tice or identity-related issues like who are the “good cycle in both the private and the public spheres.
guys” and the “bad guys.” Therefore, restoring harmoni-
ous relations requires not only reaching agreed-upon for-
The Needs-Based Model: Basic Logic
mulas (e.g., financial agreements or peace treaties) for
distributing the concrete resources under dispute but and Empirical Support
also overcoming emotional barriers that block the path to Social-psychological theorizing suggests that there are
reconciliation. A major social mechanism for overcoming two fundamental dimensions, the “Big Two,” along which
such barriers is the apology-forgiveness cycle, which has people judge themselves and others at both individual
the power to dramatically transform the relations between and group levels: the agency dimension, representing
conflicting parties (Tavuchis, 1991). traits such as strength, competence, and influence, and
While the power of apology and forgiveness in inter- the moral-social dimension, representing traits such as
personal relationships may be well known, there has also morality, warmth, and trustworthiness (Abele & Wojciszke,
been a growing use of this mechanism in the public 2013). Building on this theorizing, the needs-based model
sphere. One renowned example is the Truth and suggests that transgressions threaten victims’ and per­
Reconciliation Commission in post-apartheid South petrators’ identities asymmetrically: Victims, who feel
Africa, in which victims took an active role in the prose-
cution process and perpetrators were granted amnesty if
Corresponding Author:
they disclosed the truth about their apartheid-related Nurit Shnabel, School of Psychological Sciences, Tel-Aviv University,
crimes. According to Gobodo-Madikizela (2008), who Ramat Aviv, Tel-Aviv 69978, Israel
served on the commission, it “brought the language of E-mail: shnabeln@post.tau.ac.il
478 Shnabel, Nadler

inferior regarding their ability to influence their out- that they will not repeat the transgression (because they
comes, experience threat to their agency dimension of now recognize the victim’s value). Thus, the exchange of
identity. Perpetrators, by contrast, suffer from threat to these messages can restore victims’ and perpetrators’
their moral-social identity dimension. While this threat is mutual trust. The restoration of both trust and positive
sometimes accompanied by feelings of guilt, many times identities facilitates reconciliation. Figure 1 summarizes
perpetrators view their behavior as legitimate (Baumeister, the proposed process.
1997). Nevertheless, knowing that psychologically signifi- A series of experiments (Shnabel & Nadler, 2008) sup-
cant others view their behavior as immoral threatens per- ported the model’s hypotheses in contexts of interper-
petrators’ moral image and leads to anxiety over social sonal transgressions. One study used the creativity-test
exclusion, the sanction imposed upon those who violate paradigm, in which participants were assigned to be
their community’s moral standards. either “writers” who composed marketing slogans for
Because people are motivated to maintain their posi- products or “judges” who evaluated these slogans. In the
tive identities, these threats produce different motiva- experimental condition, judges were instructed to be
tional states. Victims, weak and humiliated, experience very strict because being “too nice” could harm their own
the need to restore their agency, whereas perpetrators chances of passing the test. Later on, participants were
experience the need to restore their moral image and informed that the judges had passed the test whereas
gain (re)acceptance to the community from which they writers had failed it because of their judges’ harsh evalu-
feel potentially excluded. If these needs remain unsatis- ations. In the control condition, judges were instructed to
fied, they impede reconciliation. For example, sometimes be lenient, and participants were later informed that the
victims behave vengefully in order to regain power judges had passed the test whereas writers had failed it
(Frijda, 1994) and perpetrators engage in moral disen- because of an external committee’s decision (this made it
gagement (e.g., minimizing the harm’s severity; Bandura, possible to control for success and failure in the absence
1999) to downplay their culpability. However, exchange of a deliberate transgression). As predicted, writers in the
interactions through which victims and perpetrators sat- experimental condition (i.e., victims) had the lowest
isfy each other’s needs for empowerment and acceptance sense of agency and the highest need for power, whereas
may open them to reconciliation. judges in the experimental condition (i.e., perpetrators)
The apology-forgiveness cycle is a primary social had the worst moral image and the highest need for
mechanism through which such an exchange can be acceptance. In a subsequent study, we again used the
accomplished. Specifically, perpetrators’ apology consti- creativity-test paradigm to assign participants to be either
tutes an admission of owing victims a moral debt, which victims or perpetrators. After being informed of the
returns control to the victims, and victims’ expressions of results of the test, participants received messages from
forgiveness and empathy toward the perpetrators’ per- their partner that expressed, depending on experimental
spective mitigate the perpetrators’ moral inferiority and condition, empowerment (i.e., acknowledgment of their
reassure them of their belongingness. Beyond using the high competence), acceptance (i.e., acknowledgment of
apology-forgiveness cycle, which is the most straightfor- their high social skills), or neither. As expected, victims’
ward example of exchange leading to need satisfaction, readiness to reconcile was highest in the empowerment
victims and perpetrators can satisfy each other’s needs in condition, whereas perpetrators’ readiness to reconcile
additional manners. For example, perpetrators may was highest in the acceptance condition.
empower their victims by expressing respect for their Another set of studies (Shnabel, Nadler, Ullrich,
achievements and abilities, or, in the case of victimized Dovidio, & Carmi, 2009) tested the model in intergroup
groups, appealing to their national pride and heritage. contexts. One study exposed Jewish and German partici-
Similarly, victims can express acceptance of their perpe- pants to speeches allegedly made by their out-group’s
trators through readiness to form friendships or engage representatives at the Memorial to the Murdered Jews of
in economic and cultural cooperation. Europe in Berlin. The speeches’ main message conveyed
A successful exchange of empowerment and accep- either acceptance (e.g., “we should accept the [Jews/
tance can promote reconciliation through two routes Germans] and remember that we are all human beings”)
(Shnabel, Nadler, & Dovidio, 2014). First, it can restore or empowerment (e.g., “the [Germans/Jews] have the
the conflicting parties’ positive identities through sym- right to be strong and proud”). As expected, Jews showed
bolically erasing the roles of “powerless victim” and greater readiness to reconcile with Germans following
“immoral perpetrator.” Additionally, messages of accep- empowering messages, whereas Germans showed
tance conveyed by the victims imply that they will not greater readiness to reconcile with Jews following accept-
retaliate against or avoid the perpetrator (two common ing messages. Thus, the dynamic between members of
responses among victims; McCullough, 2008), and mes- victimized and perpetrating groups corresponded to that
sages of empowerment conveyed by perpetrators imply between individual victims and perpetrators.
Agency and Morality in Reconciliation 479

Social Role

Victim Perpetrator

Impaired identity Agency Moral-Social


dimension: (e.g., control, power, status) (e.g., communion, moral image)

Enhanced motivation Restoration of Agency Restoration of Moral-Social


for: Dimension of Identity Dimension of Identity

Empowerment by Perpetrator Acceptance by Victim


May be achieved
(e.g., apology, acknowledgment (e.g., forgiveness, understanding
through:
of victim’s value and competence) of perpetrator’s perspective)

Restoration of Agency Restoration of Moral Image


and Trust in the Perpetrator and Trust in the Victim
Restore balance by:
(i.e., victim feels (i.e., rejection threat and
strong and respected) moral inferiority are removed)

Increased Willingness to Reconcile


Results: (positive orientation toward the other conflict party,
willingness to invest resources to restore the relations)

Fig. 1. The needs-based model of reconciliation.

Extending the Model to partner’s allocations were unfair. In terms of psychologi-


“Dual” Contexts cal needs, “duals” showed heightened needs for both
agency (similar to victims) and positive moral image
While the model’s original formulation referred to “vic- (similar to perpetrators). In terms of behavior, however,
tims” and “perpetrators” as mutually exclusive roles, in like victims, duals translated their heightened need for
many conflicts both parties serve as victims and perpetra- agency into vengefulness (e.g., denial of credit points
tors simultaneously. In one experiment that explored this from their partner), but unlike perpetrators, they failed to
duality (SimanTov-Nachlieli & Shnabel, 2014), partici- translate their heightened need for positive moral image
pants worked in dyads and had to allocate valuable into prosociality (e.g., donating to the partner). Similar
resources (i.e., credit points). Participants then received findings, pointing to the precedence of agency-related
feedback on the allocation task, which constituted the over morality-related needs in determining duals’ behav-
experimental manipulation: In the control condition, par- ior, were obtained in the intergroup contexts involving
ticipants learned that both their own and their partner’s the Israeli-Palestinian conflict (SimanTov-Nachlieli &
allocations were fair; in the victim condition, that their Shnabel, 2014) and the Liberian civil wars (Mazziotta,
partner’s allocation was unfair (i.e., she or he allocated Feuchte, Gausel, & Nadler, 2014).
the credit points in a selfish rather than a generous man- These findings are consistent with Baumeister’s (1997)
ner, which did not align with the accepted norms); in the observation that victimhood is more psychologically pro-
perpetrator condition, that their allocation was unfair; found than perpetration. Indeed, “dual” conflicting par-
and in the dual condition, that both their and their ties often develop a deep sense of victimization, but not
480 Shnabel, Nadler

of perpetration, and compete over the victim status are often stereotypically perceived as warm but incompe-
(Noor, Shnabel, Halabi, & Nadler, 2012). We theorized tent and advantaged groups as competent but cold and
that adversaries engage in such “competitive victimhood” immoral (Fiske, Cuddy, & Glick, 2007). The needs-based
because receiving acknowledgment of one’s victim status model’s logic suggests that when group inequality is per-
simultaneously satisfies the two needs heightened among ceived as illegitimate (e.g., reflecting unfair discrimina-
duals: the need for positive moral image (because the tion), the needs of advantaged- and disadvantaged-group
victim status is associated with innocence) and the need members should correspond to those of perpetrators and
for agency (because receiving acknowledgment of one’s victims.
victim status implies entitlement to various forms Studies by Siem, von Oettingen, Mummendey, and
of empowerment—e.g., reparations and third-party Nadler (2013) supported this logic. In one experiment,
support). participants were students of clinical psychology who
In studies that tested this theorizing (Shnabel, Halabi, were randomly assigned to either the high-status or the
& Noor, 2013), participants were Israeli Palestinians and low-status condition, in which they compared themselves
Jews assigned either to the common-victim-identity con- to social workers or psychiatrists, respectively. In the
dition, which highlighted that both groups experienced manipulation of (il)legitimacy, participants then learned
great suffering as a result of the conflict, or to the that intergroup status differences were either legitimate
common-perpetrator-identity condition, which high- because of different specialization requirements or ille-
lighted that both groups actively inflicted harm upon each gitimate because clinical psychologists and, depending
other. As illustrated in Figure 2, participants in both condi- on condition, social workers or psychiatrists (i.e., out-
tions reduced competitive victimhood and increased for- group members) perform similar work. As predicted, in
giveness (compared to those in a control condition),1 yet the legitimate-status-differences condition, there were no
this effect was mediated through different paths: reduced differences between high- and low-status members in
moral defensiveness (i.e., need to protect the in-group’s terms of needs for acceptance (wish to be perceived as
morality at any cost) in the common-victim-identity con- likable and moral) or agency (wish to be influential).
dition versus increased sense of agency in the common- However, in the illegitimate-status-differences condition,
perpetrator-identity condition. These findings support our high-status-group members’ need for acceptance was
theorizing about the dual motivations leading to competi- higher than that of low-status-group members, whereas
tive victimhood and carry practical implications for devel- the opposite pattern emerged for the need for agency.
oping strategies for overcoming competitive victimhood—a Consistent findings were obtained in the context of inter-
major barrier to reconciliation. racial interactions in the United States, such that African
Americans were primarily motivated to gain respect
Extending the Model to Contexts of whereas Caucasian Americans were primarily motivated
to be liked (Bergsieker, Shelton, & Richeson, 2010).
Structural Inequality Subsequent studies (Shnabel, Ullrich, Nadler, Dovidio,
Intergroup conflict manifests not only in direct violence & Aydin, 2013) found that messages from the out-group
(e.g., war) but also in structural violence (Galtung, 1969)— that reaffirmed the advantaged group’s warmth and the
namely, unequal social arrangements that privilege some disadvantaged group’s competence not only improved
groups while depriving others. This inequality translates the groups’ attitudes toward each other but also increased
into differential group stereotypes: Disadvantaged groups their readiness to collectively act for equality (through

Common-Victim- Reduced Moral


Identity Intervention Defensiveness
Reduced
Competitive Increased
Victimhood Forgiveness
Common-Perpetrator- Increased Sense
Identity Intervention of Agency

Fig. 2. The paths through which common-victim-identity and common-perpetrator-identity interventions


increased forgiveness among Israeli Palestinians and Jews. Adapted from “Overcoming Competitive Victim-
hood and Facilitating Forgiveness Through Re-categorization Into a Common Victim or Perpetrator Identity,”
by N. Shnabel, S. Halabi, and M. Noor, 2013, Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 49, p. 872. Copyright
2013 by Elsevier. Adapted with permission.
Agency and Morality in Reconciliation 481

demonstrations, petitions, etc.). The latter finding high- conflicting out-group have been shown to effectively pro-
lights the critical role of identity-restoration processes in mote reconciliation. For instance, messages from Jordanian
promoting structural equality. Regarding disadvantaged representatives increased Jews’ readiness to reconcile with
groups, research on collective action has shown that Palestinians (Harth & Shnabel, 2015).
although they tend to perceive inequality as unfair, they Identity restoration outside the victim-perpetrator
often fail to actively challenge the status quo because dyad can also be accomplished through self-affirmation
they feel they lack collective efficacy (Mummendey, processes. Supporting this possibility, Woodyatt and
Kessler, Klink, & Mielke, 1999). Optimistically, however, Wenzel (2014) found that affirmation of the moral values
restoring disadvantaged-group members’ sense of agency breached by the transgression increased perpetrators’
through competence-reaffirming messages from the genuine self-forgiveness, which in turn facilitated inter-
advantaged group prevented this passive acceptance of personal reconciliation (see Barlow et al., 2015, for posi-
inequality and increased disadvantaged-group members’ tive effects of moral self-affirmation on perpetrating
readiness to act for change. Practical interventions to pro- groups’ conciliatory tendencies). Also, SimanTov-
mote positive intergroup relations, which often focus on Nachlieli, Shnabel, Aydin, and Ullrich (2015)2 found that
the communion dimension (e.g., fostering cross-group the affirmation of dual conflicting parties’ agency
friendship), should therefore also address power-related increased their mutual conciliatory tendencies. Future
issues and directly challenge the stereotypical percep- research should further explore the conditions under
tion, which perpetuates inequality, of disadvantaged which third parties or self-affirmation interventions can
groups as incompetent. contribute to, or possibly hamper (e.g., by leading to
As for advantaged-group members, our findings sup- moral licensing effects), reconciliation.
port Iyer and Leach’s (2010) criticism that the traditional In conclusion, unlike the study of conflict resolution
view within social-psychological theorizing fails to recog- (in the sense of addressing concrete conflict-related
nize the importance of morality in intergroup relations. issues), the scientific study of reconciliation, both within
This failure leads to a limited conceptualization of social and outside psychology (e.g., de Waal, 2000), is relatively
change as driven mainly by the action of disadvantaged young and requires much additional research. We hope
groups, because advantaged groups are assumed to be that research on the needs-based model will ultimately
primarily motivated to maintain their privilege. Opposite contribute not only to increasing the theoretical under-
to this traditional view, and consistent with Iyer and standing of reconciliation but also to developing practical
Leach’s argument regarding the critical role of morality, interventions to promote it.
we found that once their threatened moral image was
reassured, advantaged-group members were ready to Recommended Reading
relinquish their in-group’s privilege and exhibit solidarity- Abele, A. E., & Wojciszke, B. (Eds.). (2013). The Big Two
based collective action. in social judgment and behavior [Special issue]. Social
Psychology, 44(2). A special issue about the two funda-
mental content dimensions that underlie judgment of social
Future Directions and Conclusion targets, which integrates highly diverse lines of research in
social psychology and explains the rationale for subsum-
While the needs-based model has identified the type of
ing components that had been shown to be distinct (e.g.,
messages that adversaries can exchange in order to pro- competence vs. dominance; morality vs. sociability) under
mote reconciliation, in practice, adversaries often avoid two overarching categories.
conveying conciliatory messages to each other because Nadler, A., & Shnabel, N. (in press). Intergroup reconcilia-
they fear these positive gestures might not be reciprocated. tion: Instrumental and socio-emotional processes and the
Therefore, a critical direction for future research is to needs-based model. European Review of Social Psychology.
examine the potential for identity restoration occurring An article that provides a comprehensive review of the
outside the victim-perpetrator dyad to facilitate reconcilia- research conducted within the needs-based model’s frame-
tion. One source of such external identity restoration is work, including in-depth discussions of issues that could
third parties. Admittedly, in contexts of interpersonal trans- not be addressed in this short article, such as the seeming
discrepancy between the needs-based model’s logic and
gressions, empowering and accepting messages from third
empirical findings pointing to the ineffectiveness of group
parties have been found to be relatively ineffective
apologies.
(Shnabel et al., 2014). However, within intergroup con- Tavuchis, N. (1991). (See References). A seminal analysis of the
texts, in which conciliatory messages are typically expected sociology of apology and forgiveness drawing from litera-
to be conveyed via group representatives rather than ture in sociology, philosophy, linguistics, philology, psy-
directly by the victims and perpetrators (Simon & chology, anthropology, law, and religious studies.
Klandermans, 2001), empowering and accepting messages Wohl, M. J. A., Hornsey, M. J., & Philpot, C. R. (2011). A criti-
by third parties who share a common identity with the cal review of official public apologies: Aims, pitfalls, and
482 Shnabel, Nadler

a staircase model of effectiveness. Social Issues and Policy Gobodo-Madikizela, P. (2008). Transforming trauma in the after-
Review, 5, 70–100. A social-psychological analysis of public math of gross human rights abuses: Making public spaces
apologies, the pros and cons of using them, and strategies intimate through the South African truth and reconciliation
that may bolster their effectiveness. commission. In A. Nadler, T. Malloy, & J. D. Fisher (Eds.),
Social psychology of inter-group reconciliation (pp. 57–76).
Declaration of Conflicting Interests New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
Harth, N. S., & Shnabel, N. (2015). Third-party interven-
The authors declared that they had no conflicts of interest with
tion in intergroup reconciliation: The role of neutral-
respect to their authorship or the publication of this article.
ity and common identity with the other conflict-party.
Group Processes & Intergroup Relations, 18, 676–695.
Funding doi:10.1177/1368430215583151
This work was supported by the European Union Seventh Iyer, A., & Leach, C. W. (2010). Helping disadvantaged out-
Framework Programme (FP7/2007-2013) under Grant Agree­ groups challenge unjust inequality: The role of group-
ment Number 2934602 (PCIG09-GA-2011-293602), awarded to based emotions. In S. Stürmer & M. Snyder (Eds.), New
N. Shnabel. directions in the psychology of helping: Group-level per-
spectives on motivations, consequences, and interventions
Notes (pp. 337–353). Oxford, England: Blackwell.
Leach, C. W., Ellemers, N., & Barreto, M. (2007). Group virtue:
1. This study did not directly compare the common-perpetrator-
The importance of morality (vs. competence and socia-
identity and common-victim-identity conditions (i.e., it did not
bility) in the positive evaluation of in-groups. Journal of
test the prediction derived from later findings by SimanTov-
Personality and Social Psychology, 93, 234–249.
Nachlieli & Shnabel, 2014, that the first strategy should yield
Mazziotta, A., Feuchte, F., Gausel, N., & Nadler, A. (2014). Does
more conciliatory tendencies than the latter).
remembering past ingroup perpetration promote postwar
2. This research also demonstrated that in non-conflictual con-
cross-group contact? Insights from a field experiment in
texts, group members prioritize their morality-related needs (in
Liberia. European Journal of Social Psychology, 44, 43–52.
line with Leach, Ellemers, & Barreto’s, 2007, findings regarding
McCullough, M. (2008). Beyond revenge: The evolution of the
the primacy of morality among group members), whereas in
forgiveness instinct. San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass.
contexts of intergroup conflict, agency-related needs become
Mummendey, A., Kessler, T., Klink, A., & Mielke, R. (1999).
prioritized.
Strategies to cope with negative social identity: Predictions
by social identity theory and relative deprivation theory.
References Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 76, 229–245.
Abele, A. E., & Wojciszke, B. (2013). The Big Two in social Nadler, A., & Shnabel, N. (2008). Instrumental and socio-
judgment and behavior [Editorial]. Social Psychology, 44, emotional paths to intergroup reconciliation: And the
61–62. need based model of socio-emotional reconciliation. In
Bandura, A. (1999). Moral disengagement in the perpetration A. Nadler, T. Malloy, & J. D. Fisher (Eds.), Social psychology
of inhumanities. Personality and Social Psychology Review, of intergroup reconciliation (pp. 37–56). New York, NY:
3, 193–209. Oxford University Press.
Barlow, F. K., Thai, M., Wohl, M. J. A., White, S., Wright, M.-A., Noor, M., Shnabel, N., Halabi, S., & Nadler, A. (2012). When
& Hornsey, M. J. (2015). Perpetrator groups can enhance suffering begets suffering: The psychology of competitive
their moral self-image by accepting their own intergroup victimhood between adversarial groups in violent conflicts.
apologies. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 60, Personality and Social Psychology Review, 16, 351–374.
39–50. Shnabel, N., Halabi, S., & Noor, M. (2013). Overcoming com-
Baumeister, R. F. (1997). Evil: Inside human violence and cru- petitive victimhood and facilitating forgiveness through re-
elty. New York, NY: Henry Holt. categorization into a common victim or perpetrator identity.
Bergsieker, H. B., Shelton, J. N., & Richeson, J. A. (2010). To be Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 49, 867–877.
liked versus respected: Divergent goals in interracial inter- Shnabel, N., & Nadler, A. (2008). A needs-based model of rec-
actions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 99, onciliation: Satisfying the differential emotional needs of
248–264. victim and perpetrator as a key to promoting reconciliation.
de Waal, F. B. M. (2000). Primates: A natural heritage of conflict Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 94, 116–132.
resolution. Science, 289, 586–600. Shnabel, N., Nadler, A., & Dovidio, J. F. (2014). Beyond need
Fiske, S. T., Cuddy, A. J. C., & Glick, P. (2007). Universal dimen- satisfaction: Empowering and accepting messages from
sions of social cognition: Warmth, then competence. Trends third parties ineffectively restore trust and consequent
in Cognitive Sciences, 11, 77–83. reconciliation. European Journal of Social Psychology, 44,
Frijda, N. H. (1994). Varieties of affect: Emotions and episodes, 126–140.
moods, and sentiments. In P. Ekman & R. J. Davidson Shnabel, N., Nadler, A., Ullrich, J., Dovidio, J. F., & Carmi,
(Eds.), The nature of emotion: Fundamental questions D. (2009). Promoting reconciliation through the satisfac-
(pp. 59–67). New York, NY: Oxford University Press. tion of the emotional needs of victimized and perpetrating
Galtung, J. (1969). Violence, peace, and peace research. Journal group members: The needs-based model of reconciliation.
of Peace Research, 6, 167–191. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 35, 1021–1030.
Agency and Morality in Reconciliation 483

Shnabel, N., Ullrich, J., Nadler, A., Dovidio, J. F., & Aydin, A. L. SimanTov-Nachlieli, I., Shnabel, N., Aydin, A. L., & Ullrich,
(2013). Warm or competent? Improving intergroup relations J. (2015). Agents of morality: Affirming conflicting groups’
by addressing threatened identities of advantaged and dis- agency promotes pro-sociality towards the conflicting out-
advantaged groups. European Journal of Social Psychology, group. Manuscript submitted for publication.
43, 482–492. Simon, B., & Klandermans, B. (2001). Towards a social psy-
Siem, B., von Oettingen, M., Mummendey, A., & Nadler, A. chological analysis of politicized collective identity: Con-
(2013). When status differences are illegitimate, groups’ ceptualization, antecedents, and consequences. American
needs diverge: Testing the needs-based model of reconcili- Psychologist, 56, 319–331.
ation in contexts of status inequality. European Journal of Tavuchis, N. (1991). Mea Culpa: A sociology of apology and rec-
Social Psychology, 43, 137–148. onciliation. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.
SimanTov-Nachlieli, I., & Shnabel, N. (2014). Feeling both vic- Woodyatt, L., & Wenzel, M. (2014). A needs-based perspective
tim and perpetrator: Investigating duality within the needs- on self-forgiveness: Addressing threat to moral identity as a
based model. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, means of encouraging interpersonal and intrapersonal restora-
40, 301–314. tion. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 50, 125–135.

You might also like