Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 12

Brain Research, 462 (1988) 211-222 211

Elsevier

BRE 13980

Persistent sensitization of dopamine neurotransmission in ventral


striatum (nucleus accumbens) produced by prior experience with
(+)-amphetamine: a microdialysis study in freely moving rats

Terry E. Robinson, Phillip A. Jurson, Julie A. Bennett and Kris M. Bentgen


Department of Psychology and NeuroscienceProgram, The Universityof Michigan, Ann Arbor, M148109 (U.S.A.)
(Accepted 3 May 1988)

Key words: Sensitization; Amphetamine; Dopamine; Nucleus accurnbens; Dialysis; Dopamine release; Locomotion;
Stereotyped behavior; Stimulant; Chronic drug effect; Amphetamine psychosis; Neural plasticity

In humans the repeated use of amphetamine (AMPH) produces a hypersensitivity to the psychotogenic effects of AMPH that per-
sists for months to years after the cessation of drug use. To explore the neurobiological basis of this phenomenon the long-term effects
of dextroamphetamine ((+)-AMPH) on brain monoamines and behavior were studied in an animal model of AMPH lbsychosis.An es-
calating dose pretreatment regimen (from 1 to 10 mg/kg over 5 weeks) was used to mimic the pattern of drug use associated with the de-
velopment of addiction and AMPH psychosis. The effect of pretreatment with AMPH on dopamine (DA) release in the ventral stria-
turn (nucleus accumbens) was determined by measuring the extracellular concentrations of DA and DA metabolites using in vivo mi-
crodialysis, both before and after an AMPH challenge. The postmortem tissue concentrations of DA, serotonin and their metabolites
were measured to determine if this treatment was neurotoxic. Escalating doses of (+)-AMPH were not neurotoxic, and 25-30 days af-
ter the cessation of drug treatment animals showed relatively normal levels of spontaneous motor activity across the day-night cycle.
However, AMPH pretreatment produced robust behavioral sensitization. Animals showed a marked hypersensitivity to the motor
stimulant effects of an AMPH challenge, even after 15-20 days of withdrawal. Most importantly, this hyperdopaminergic behavioral
syndrome was accompanied by significantly elevated DA release in the ventral striatum. In contrast, AMPH pretreatment had no ef-
fect on the ~oasalextracellular concentrations of DA. It is suggested that the sensitization produced by chronic AMPH use is due to en-
during changes in the releasability of DA, and that this may represent an example of neural plasticity common to other forms of behav-
ioral adaptation.

INTRODUCTION former A M P H addicts who have been abstinent for


months to years 35'36,46. The enduring hypersensitivity
People who abuse psychomotor stimulant drugs, to A M P H seen in former addicts suggests that
such as a m p h e t a m i n e ( A M P H ) or cocaine, frequent- chronic A M P H use has long-term effects on brain
ly develop a drug-induced paranoid psychotic condi- function.
tion characterized by delusions of persecution, hallu- A n u m b e r of animal models have been developed
cinations, affective disturbances and perseverative to study the neurobioiogical consequences of chronic
behavior 6'9'4°. A M P H psychosis usually dissipates A M P H treatment. For example, A M P H addicts fre-
upon the cessation of drug use, but there are also quently take very high doses of A M P H in 'runs' that
long-term sequelae associated with A M P H abuse. can last for 4 - 6 days 21, and some researchers have at-
For example, re-exposure to a relatively low dose of tempted to capture this pattern of use by repeatedly
A M P H will often precipitate a psychotic episode in administering very high doses of A M P H ( ~ 1 0 - 2 0

Correspondence: T.E. Robinson, The University of Michigan, Neuroscience Laboratory Bldg., 1103 E. Huron St., Ann Arbor, MI
48109, U.S.A.
212

mg/kg) 2 - 6 times a day 13'19'25'27A3'44'45. Others have and water. Each rat received 2 daily intraperitoneal
continuously maintained elevated blood levels of injections of either 0.9% saline or ( + ) - a m p h e t a m i n e
A M P H with a slow-release subcutaneous implant 12. sulfate ( A M P H ) , with 8 - 1 0 h separating the two in-
These two approaches are usually neurotoxic, pro- jections. A f t e r each injection the animals were
ducing a p e r m a n e n t depletion of d o p a m i n e ( D A ) and placed back into their home cages and left undis-
serotonin (5-HT) in selected brain regions ll'3° (cf. turbed. Injections were given on consecutive week-
ref. 48). A third approach has been to r e p e a t e d l y and days, but not on weekends, and animals p r e t r e a t e d
intermittently administer a constant low dose of with A M P H received increasing doses of A M P H ac-
A M P H ( ~ 1 - 5 mg/kg) no more than twice a day. This cording to the schedule illustrated in Table i. Follow-
latter treatment regimen is not neurotoxic, but pro- ing the last injection of A M P H or saline the animals
duces a progressive and enduring e n h a n c e m e n t (sen- were left in their home cages until used in one of the
sitization) in the m o t o r stimulant effects of A M P H 24' two experiments described below.
30,~2 and in A M P H - s t i m u l a t e d striatal D A release in
vitro 20'29"31'49. It has been argued that the p h e n o m e - Experiment 1. The effects of pretreatment with escalat-
non of behavioral sensitization provides a good ani- ing doses of A M P H on spontaneous motor activity
mal model of A M P H psychosis 3°'4°. and postmortem tissue concentrations of monoamines
Nevertheless, A M P H addicts initially take rela- and monoamine metabolites
tively low doses of A M P H , using higher doses only as Twenty-five days after the last p r e t r e a t m e n t injec-
they become tolerant to the euphorigenic and auto- tion of A M P H or saline the animals were transferred
nomic effects of A M P H 7"1°'1s'21, and none of the ap- from their home cage to a u t o m a t e d activity monitors
proaches for studying chronic A M P H treatment (41 x 24 x 18 cm), which are described in detail else-
mentioned above take this escalating pattern of where 33. The monitors detect m o v e m e n t from one
A M P H use into account. It may be important to do so end of the cage to the other (locomotion), and are not
because the neurochemical effects of high doses of sensitive to small movements of the head or body
A M P H are changed by p r e t r e a t m e n t with escalating made in one location. The animals r e m a i n e d in these
doses of A M P H 37'3s. The present experiments were monitors for 24 h/day for the next 5 days, and the
conducted, therefore, to determine the long-term ef- number of photocell b e a m interruptions each animal
fects of escalating (but non-toxic) doses of A M P H made was cumulated over 30 min intervals for 20
on: (1) regional brain concentrations of D A , 5-HT h/day. The l i g h t - d a r k cycle was the same as in the
and their m a j o r metabolites; (2) spontaneous m o t o r main colony room. A f t e r this the animals were
activity across the d a y - n i g h t cycle; and (3) in vivo placed back into their home cages.
extracellular concentrations of D A , dihydroxy- Three to four days later the animals were killed
phenylacetic acid ( D O P A C ) , homovanillic acid rapidly by decapitation. Each brain was r e m o v e d
(HVA) and 5-hydroxyindoleacetic acid ( 5 - H I A A ) in
the ventral striatum (nucleus accumbens), both be-
fore and after a challenge injection of A M P H in free- TABLE I
ly moving rats. The ventral striatum was chosen for Amphetaminepretreatmentregimen
study because it has been implicated in the m o t o r
The numbers represent mg/kg of AMPH per injection (weight
stimulant, affective, and rewarding effects of psycho- of the salt) on each pretreatment day. Animals received two
m o t o r stimulant drugs 14"5°. injections/day (see Methods).

Week Day'
MATERIALS AND METHODS
M T W Th F Sa Su
1 l 1 2 2 2 - -
Subjects and pretreatment regimen
2 3 4 4 4 4 - -
A d u l t female Holtzman rats (Holtzman, Madison, 3 4 5 5 5 5 - -
WI) were housed individually in a t e m p e r a t u r e - c o n - 4 6 7 7 7 7 -
trolled room on a normal light:dark cycle (14:10 h, 5 8 9 9 9 9 -
6 9 10 10 10 10 -
with lights on at 08.00 h), and with free access to food
213

from the cranium (30-45 s), placed into ice-cold sa- mM CI-, pH 6.0) flowed continuously through the
line for approx. 30 s, and then placed in a chilled cut- probe at approximately 1.0 pl/min. The dialysis
ting block. Coronal sections were obtained as de- probe was fixed to the skull with stainless steel screws
scribed by Heffner et al. ~7 and the following regions and dental acrylic, and the entire assembly was en-
dissected in ice-cold saline. (a) Frontal cortex. This cased in a protective housing constructed from the
consisted of the DA-rich anteromedial portion of the dental acrylic. The inlet and outlet tubing was
frontal cortex, from just anterior to the genu of the threaded through a coiled steel tether, and the inlet
corpus callosum to the frontal pole. (b) Striatum. The line connected to a single channel liquid swivel (In-
corpus of the striatum was removed with a 3 mm stech Laboratories, Plymouth Meeting, PA). The an-
diameter micropunch. (c) Nucleus accumbens. The imals were then placed into a 29 × 29 × 31 cm high
nucleus accumbens was removed with a 2 mm diame- plexiglass test chamber and left there overnight, dur-
ter micropunch. (d) Hypothalamus. The entire hypo- ing which time Ringer solution was continuously
thalamus was dissected as described by Heffner et pumped through the probes at 0.2pl/min using a Har-
al.17. Tissue from the left and right hemispheres was vard Model 22 syringe pump and 2.5 ml gas-tight
pooled, weighed, placed into tubes containing 0.05 N Hamilton 1000 series syringes.
HCIO4, with dihydroxybenzylamine added as an in- The dialysis probes and method have been de-
ternal standard, and then homogenized. Samples scribed in detail previously 34. Briefly, the dialysis
were centrifugedat 5,000 g for 45 min at 2 - 4 °C, the probe was of a concentric design and consisted of a
supernatant filtered through 0.45 ~tm syringe filters 2.0 mm length of regenerated cellulose hollow dialy-
(Gelman Sciences, LC3A) and stored frozen at sis fiber (Spectrum), which had an o.d. of 250~m and
-20 °C until assayed for dopamine (DA), dihydroxy- M.W. cut-off of 6000. The outlet tube consisted of a
phenylacetic acid (DOPAC), homovanillic acid 30 cm length of fused silica capillary tubing (Polymi-
(HVA), 5-hydroxytryptamine (5-HT) and 5-hy- cro Technologies, Phoenix, AZ) with an o.d. of 150
droxyindoleacetic acid (5-HIAA) by HPLC-EC, as um and an i.d. of 75 pm. Dialysate was collected in
described previously 32. minivials mounted on the tether approx. 25 cm above
the animal and could be quickly exchanged without
Experiment 2. The effects of pretreatment with escalat- touching the animal. Prior to implantation all probes
ing doses of A M P H on behavior and the concentra- were tested for their ability to recover known con-
tions of DA, DOPA C, HVA and 5-HIAA in ventral centrations of DA, DOPAC, HVA and 5-HIAA in
striatal extracellular fluid in vivo, both before and af- vitro, at 37 °C and a flow rate of 1.5/,tl/min 34.
ter an A M P H challenge Eighteen to 20 h after probe implantation the
Fifteen to 21 days after the last pretreatment injec- pump speed was set to 1.5 ~d/min, and after waiting at
tion of AMPH or saline matched pairs of animals least 30 rain, dialysate samples were collected over
(one AMPH and one saline-pretreated) were anes- 20 min intervals in tubes containing 10 ktl of 0.05 N
thetized with sodium pentobarbital, supplemented HC104, also containing 0.1 mM sodium bisulfite and
with methoxyflurane, and placed into a stereotaxic 0.2 mM E D T A per 25 ml of HCIO4. At least 3 base-
instrument. Using standard stereotaxic techniques, a line samples were collected and then each animal re-
microdialysis probe was lowered into the ventral ceived a challenge injection of 2.0 mg/kg of (+)-
striatum in either the left or right hemisphere (half AMPH, and an additional 10 samples collected. This
the animals received an implant on the left and half challenge dose was used because it was previously
on the right). The term ventral striatum is used ac- shown to be effective in behaviorally differentiating
cording to the terminology for brain D A systems sug- sensitized and control animals 33. Dialysate was in-
gested by BjOrklund and LindvaW, and takes into jected directly onto the HPLC column within 10 min
account the concept of a ventral striatal region intro- after collection and without further treatment, using
duced by Heimer and Wilson 16, consisting primarily assay procedures described by Robinson and Whi-
of the nucleus accumbens and tuberculum olfactori- shaw 34.
urn. During implantation a modified Ringer solution The test chambers were located in a separate
(147 mM Na +, 2.3 mM Ca 2+, 4 mM K + and 155.6 room, adjacent to the neurochemistry lab, and white
214

noise was played continuously to mask sounds from sections were stained with cresyl violet and examined
nearby rooms. The animals were left undisturbed in to determine the location of the dialysis probe in each
this room, but were monitored from the neurochem- animal.
istry lab by closed-circuit television. The experi-
menter entered the test room periodically only to RESULTS
quickly change sample collection vials.
The animals were video-taped during one 20 min Experiment 1
baseline interval, and for 200 min following the Behavior. Fig. 1 shows the average locomotor ac-
A M P H challenge. Behavior was analyzed later by tivity counts for saline and AMPH-pretreated rats
viewing the videotapes, often at 2 - 8 x normal cumulated over 30 min intervals for 7.5 h prior to
speed. Locomotor activity was estimated by counting lights-off, the 10 h lights-off period, and then an addi-
the number of times an animal moved from one side tional 2.5 h after the lights came on. Data for the re-
of the cage to the other (crossovers), and the number maining 4 h were not included to minimize the distur-
of times an animal reared (both front feet off the bance associated with daily feeding, cage cleaning
ground), during consecutive 10 min intervals. Stereo- and data collection (printing). To reduce intrasubject
typed behavior was quantified by an observer blind variability in activity associated with the estrous
to pretreatment condition, who rated the occurrence cycle, the data were averaged over the 5 days of test-
of repetitive head and limb movements, and repeti- ing, during which time each animal would have had
tive sniffing behavior, at the half-way point of each only one complete cycle.
20 min collection interval. Head and limb movements Both groups showed a similar pattern of motor ac-
were rated as: 0 = no head movements, or head tivity across most of the light-dark cycle. Activity
movements associated with normal locomotion; 1 = levels were low during the afternoon, but rose slowly
increased frequency of otherwise normal appearing prior to lights off. There was a marked increase in lo-
head movements associated with hyperactivity; dis- comotor activity when the lights initially went off.
continuous; mild intensity; 2 = stereotyped (repeti- Activity subsided from this initial high to a low point
tive) up and down head movements and shuffling of 5 h after lights-off, but still remained higher than dur-
the forepaws; discontinuous; 3 = continuous, in ing the day. There was no difference between saline
place, repetitive head and limb movements; moder- and AMPH-pretreated animals when all 40 intervals
ate intensity and frequency; 4 = as 3, but high inten- were analyzed, or when just daytime or nocturnal ac-
sity and frequency. Sniffing was rated as: 0 = no
sniffing, or a normal pattern of sniffing associated
with breathing or normal exploration of an object or Saline- 3retrecrted
place; 1 = increased rate of sniffing associated with • AMPH- iretrealed

increased locomotion and rearing (hyperactivity); 16"

discontinuous; mild intensity; 2 = stereotyped sniff- ev


> 12"
ing (repetitive vibrissae and snout movements); dis-
continuous; not directed at a specific object or place; 0
8"
moderate intensity; 3 = continuous, in place repeti- 0
tive vibrissae and snout movements; moderate inten-
sity and frequency; 4 = intense, continuous vibrissae
and snout movements aimed at the cage floor.
At the completion of the dialysis experiment the
4-

0 J I 0 pm 8 am

animals were killed with an overdose of sodium pen- TIME


tobarbital and perfused through the heart, first with Fig. 1. The average frequency of photocell beam interruptions
0.9% saline, followed by 10% formalin in saline. The (crossovers) cumulated over 30 min intervals across the day-
brains were removed and stored in 10% formalin. night cycle in amphetamine-pretreated (closed circles) and sa-
line-pretreated (open circles) rats. The lights-off period, which
They were later frozen and horizontal sections pre- started at 10.00 h and ended at 08.00 h, is illustrated by the
pared using standard histological techniques. The thick black line on the horizontal axis.
215

T A B L E I[
Average (+ S. E.M.) regional concentrations (ng/mg wet tissue weight) of monoamines and monoamine metabolites in postmortem tissue
obtained from amphetamine (AMPH)- and saline-pretreated rats

Compound Pretreatment Striatum Accumbens Cortex Hypothalamus

DA Saline 14.23 + 0.43 9.76 + 0.34 0.022 + 0.007 0,661 _+ 0,036


AMPH 13.22 + 0.53 9.41 + 0.33 0.028 + 0.009 0.678 _+ 0.028
DOPAC Saline 1.11 + 0.06 1.21 + 0.08 0.057 + 0.007 0.616 + 0.033
AMPH 1.08 + 0.05 1.28 + 0.05 0.065 + 0.009 0.679 _+ 0,042
HVA Saline 0.978 + 0.065 0,791 + 0.063 0,073 + 0.010 -
AMPH 1.055 + 0.073 0.854 + 0.042 0,064 + 0.007 -
5-HT Saline 0.420 + 0.030 0,893 + 0.103 1.85 + 0.11 1.83 _+ 0.09
AMPH 0.436 + 0,035 1.041 + 0.133 1.88 + 0.12 1.82 _+ 0.10
5-HIAA Saline 0.614 + 0.019 0,733 + 0.069 0.729 + 0.052 0.946 _+ 0.048
AMPH 0.616 + 0.014 0,904 + 0.067 0,771 + 0.050 0.913 _+ 0.025

tivity was analyzed (2-way A N O V A s with repeated The animals typically sat still, sometimes making
measures). A M P H - p r e t r e a t e d animals, however, small head movements, grooming or walking; but
tended to be less active than saline-pretreated con- frequently they appeared to be asleep (curled immo-
trois towards the end of the dark period (2-way bile posture). They would often maintain this sleep-
A N O V A on the last 2 h of nocturnal activity; Fl,18 = ing posture while sample collection tubes were ex-
4.24, P = 0.054, main effect). This was because sa- changed.
line-pretreated rats showed a statistically significant
increase in activity from the low point during the Crossovers Rearing
50"
middle of the night (02.30-03.00 h) to the last inter-
40"
val before lights on (1-way A N O V A for repeated
Z 30"
measures, F10,90 = 2.65, P = 0.007), whereas
AMPH-pretreated rats did n o t (F10,90 --- 1.69, P =
10
0.095; Fig. 1).
0
Neurochemistry. Table II shows the average post- 4 8 12 16 20 4 8 12 16 20
I0 MIN INTERVALS
mortem tissue concentrations of D A , D O P A C ,
HVA, 5-HT and 5 - H I A A in the striatum, nucleus ac- ® Head & Limb Mvts.
Z
cumbens, frontal cortex and hypothalamus, mea-
sured 33-34 days after the last pretreatment injec-
tion. There was no difference between the saline and 0
AMPH-pretreated groups on any measure, in any
structure (t-tests). The D O P A C / D A , H V A / D A and . . . . . . . .

B I 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 8 I 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10
5-HIAA/5-HT ratios for each structure were also 20 MIN INTERVALS
analyzed, and again there was no significant effect of
Fig. 2. The effects of pretreatment with saline (open symbols)
A M P H pretreatment. or amphetamine (closed symbols) on behavior before and after
a challenge injection of 2.0 mg/kg of amphetamine given 15-21
days after the last pretreatment injection. The frequency of
Experiment 2
crossovers (locomotion from one side of the cage to the other)
Behavior. There was no difference in activity be- and rearing was cumulated over 10 min intervals, and the arrow
tween saline- and A M P H - p r e t r e a t e d animals during in the upper left panel indicates when AMPH was given. Ste-
reotypy ratings were obtained once during each 20 rain interval
baseline testing, and animals from both groups were
(see Methods). B represents the average values obtained dur-
very inactive as indicated by low levels of locomotion ing baseline and the remaining symbols the average values ob-
and rearing (Fig. 2). This was expected because test- tained after the amphetamine challenge. For the stereotypy
ratings ___ S.E.M. bars are also shown. Amphetamine-pre-
ing took place during the lights-on period, and after
treated animals differed significantly from saline-pretreated
the animals had been in the chambers for 18-20 h. animals on all measures (see text).
216

In contrast, pretreatment condition had a large ef- rest of the session. Hyperactivity was accompanied
fect on the behavior produced by a challenge injec- by an increase in sniffing behavior, but saline-pre-
tion of 2.0 mg/kg of A M P H . In control animals treated animals did not develop focused stereotyped
A M P H caused marked hyperactivity, as indicated by behavior - - as indicated by maximal average stereo-
high levels of locomotion and rearing (Fig. 2). The typy scores of only about 2.5 for sniffing and 1.0-1.5
amount of locomotion and rearing increased quickly for head and limb movements (Fig. 2). A M P H - p r e -
after the A M P H challenge, reaching a peak during treated rats, however, showed a behavioral syn-
the second 10 min interval after the injection. Al- drome typical of a higher dose of A M P H 42. In this
though locomotion and rearing slowly declined over group the A M P H challenge produced an initial ele-
the remainder of the test session, motor activity re- vation in locomotion and rearing lasting for 20-30
mained higher than during baseline throughout the min, followed by a precipitous drop in locomotor ac-
tivity to very low levels during the period lasting ap-
DOPAMINE DOPAC
proximately 50-90 min after the injection (Fig. 2).
200- • 6000
During the period of reduced locomotor activity
175 ~ e t r e c
150 5000 A M P H - p r e t r e a t e d rats showed intense focused ste-
125 ~ AMPHpre~eotod 4000 reotyped behavior - - as indicated by average stereo-
typy ratings of approximately 3.5 (Fig. 2). This ste-

40oo~
3060

reotypy phase was followed by a period of 'post-ste-


reotypy hyperactivity '>, during which time the inci-
•E 25 1000
E B12345678910 12345678910 dence of locomotion again increased (Fig. 2). Thus,
'~ HVA 5-HIAA animals pretreated with escalating doses of A M P H
E 35oo

3500" 3000
and given a challenge injection of 2.0 mg/kg of
3006 2500 A M P H showed a multiphasic behavioral syndrome
2500" characteristic of highly sensitized animals > .
2000
2000"
In vivo microdialysis. The changes in behavior
1500" 1500
1000" 1000 produced by pretreatment with A M P H were accom-
C D
500 . . . . . . . . . . . 500 panied by changes in the concentration of D A in ex-
12345678910 B12345678910
tracellular fluid, but only following the A M P H chal-
INTERVAL lenge (Fig. 3A). During baseline there was no differ-
Fig. 3. The average (_+ S.E.M.) extracellular fluid concentra- ence between saline- and A M P H - p r e t r e a t e d animals
tions of dopamine (A), DOPAC (B; dihydroxyphenylacetic in the extracellular concentrations of D A in ventral
acid), HVA (C; homovanillic acid) and 5-HIAA (D; 5-by-
droxyindoleacetic acid) in ventral striatum in vivo, before (B) striatum (in saline-pretreated rats it averaged 57.6 _+
and after (20 min intervals, 1-10) a challenge injection of 2.0 11.8 (S.E.M.) fmol/min and in A M P H - p r e t r e a t e d
mg/kg of amphetamine in rats that were pretreated with am- rats 56.6 + 6.7 fmol/min; values corrected for recov-
phetamine (closed symbols) or saline (open symbols). All dia-
lysis probes were tested for their ability to recover known con- ery; Fig. 3A). The challenge injection of A M P H pro-
centrations of DA, DOPAC, HVA and 5-HIAA in vitro, and duced a significant increase in the extracellular con-
the values shown (in fmol/min) were corrected for recovery. centrations of D A in both saline- and A M P H - p r e -
The average relative recovery values were: DA - - 18.8 +
0.7%, DOPAC-- 17.2 _+0.8%; HVA-- 16.8 + 1.1%; and 5- treated animals, and this peaked during the second
HIAA - - 16.3 _+0.9%. The amphetamine challenge produced 2(1 min interval in both groups. However, the
a significantlygreater increase in extracellular DA in ampheta- A M P H - e v o k e d rise in extracellular D A was signifi-
mine-pretreated than in saline-pretreated rats. The results of a
two-way analysis of variance for repeated measures on each cantly greater in A M P H - p r e t r e a t e d animals than in
compound were as follows: DA - - effect of pretreatment, FL] 3 saline-pretreated animals (Fig. 3A). The two pre-
= 5.45; P = 0.036; effect of time, Fto.13o= 30.3, P < 0.001 ; pre- treatment groups did not differ significantly during
treatment × time interaction, Fro.L30= 3.44; P < 0.001 ; *, P <
0.05, Scheffe tests. DOPAC - - pretreatment, F - 6.43, P = the first 20 rain interval following the A M P H chal-
0.025; time, F = 33.6, P < 0.001; interaction, F < 1.0, non-sig- lenge, but extracellular D A concentrations rose to a
nificant (n.s.). H V A - pretreatment, F = 2.91, P = 0.11; time, significantly higher peak in A M P H - p r e t r e a t e d ani-
F = 21.5, P < 0.001 : interaction, F < 1.0, n.s. 5-HIAA - - pre-
treatment, F - 1.51, P = 0.24; time, F < 1.0, n.s. ; interaction, mals and this difference persisted for the next 80 rain.
F < 1.0, n.s. By the end of the test session extracellular fluid con-
217

centrations of D A had fallen to slightly below base-


line levels in both groups, and at this time there was
again no difference between the groups (Fig. 3A).
A M P H pretreatment also changed the extracellu-
lar concentrations of D O P A C and HVA (Fig. 3B,C).
The extracellular concentration of D O P A C was sig-
nificantly higher in AMPH-pretreated than in saline-
pretreated animals. A two-way ANOVA for repeat-
ed measures that included all 11 sample intervals re-
suited in a significant effect of pretreatment condi-
tion (P = 0.025; Fig. 3B), but no significant interac- Fig. 4. Schematic drawing of the anterior portion of a hori-
tion (F < 1.0), suggesting the two groups differed at zontal section of rat brain adapted from Paxinos and Watson 26
all points in time (including baseline). However, a t- showing the area traversed by the dialysis probes. The hori-
zontal plane represented by this plate (Fig. 95 in Paxinos and
test on just the baseline values did not reach statis- Watson 26) is 7.6 mm ventral from bregma. Closed circles repre-
tical significance, probably because of greater varia- sent probes from amphetamine-pretreated animals and open
tion in extracellular fluid D O P A C concentrations circles probes from saline-pretreated animals. The diameter of
each circle is drawn to scale, and thus illustrates the approxi-
during baseline than following the A M P H challenge mate diameter of a dialysis probe relative to the nucleus accum-.
(see S.E.M. bars on Fig. 3B). There was also a sig- hens. Abbreviations: AcbC, nucleus accumbens, core; AcbSh,
nificant effect of time (P < 0.001) because the nucleus accumbens, shell; acp, anterior commissure, posterior;
AOP, anterior olfactory nucleus, posterior; BST, bed nucleus
AMPH challenge caused a large decline in extracel- of the stria terminalis; CPu, caudate putamen; f, fornix;
lular D O P A C concentrations, which was maximal by MnPO, median preoptic nucleus; Pir, piriform cortex; SI, sub-
the third 20 min interval after A M P H and persisted stantia innominata; SM, nucleus of the stria medullaris; Tu, ol-
factory tubercle; VDB, nucleus vertical limb diagonal band;
throughout the test session. The absence of a signifi- VP, ventral pallidum; 3V, third ventricle.
cant pretreatment × time interaction indicates that
the rate of decline in D O P A C was the same for
AMPH and saline-pretreated animals. A M P H pre- of each probe consisted of the epoxy plug, and the ac-
treatment had a similar effect on extracellular HVA tual dialysis region extended for 2.0 mm dorsally
concentrations, in that the average concentrations from the top of the plug. In the majority of animals
were higher in AMPH-pretreated animals than in sa- the region sampled extended from 8.35 mm to 6.35
line controls at all points in time; but this difference mm ventral from bregma, in the dorsal-ventral
did not reach statistical significance (P = 0.11). The plane. On average, approximately 12% of the dialy-
decline in HVA produced by the A M P H challenge sis surface was located in the olfactory tubercle and
was more gradual than for D O P A C (Fig. 3), but pro- the cell bridges of the ventral striatum and 88% in the
ceeded at the same rate in A M P H and saline-pre- nucleus accumbens. Fig. 4 shows the location of the
treated groups (the effect of time was significant, P < probes as they traversed the plane of a horizontal sec-
0.001, but the interaction term was non-significant). tion located 7.60 mm ventral to bregma. In summary,
Neither A M P H pretreatment (P = 0.24) nor the the probes sampled the ventral striatum 1'x6consisting
AMPH challenge (F < 1.0) had a significant effect on primarily of the nucleus accumbens, and to a lesser
the concentration of 5 - H I A A in ventral striatal extra- extent the olfactory tubercle and cell bridges of the
cellular fluid (Fig. 3D). ventral striatum. The dialysis fibers did not extend
Histology. The location of each dialysis probe was dorsally into the caudate-putamen.
estimated from examination of the histological sec-
tions. Neurochemical data from one rat were excluded DISCUSSION
because its probe was not in the ventral striatum. The
remaining probes all extended ventrally into the ol- To summarize the major findings: (a) pretreat-
factory tubercle; the deepest to 9.22 mm ventral from ment with escalating doses of AMPH produced a
bregma (all coordinates refer to those on horizontal large and persistent (15-20 days) hypersensitivity to
sections in Paxinos and Watson26). The first 250/~m the motor stimulant effects of an A M P H challenge;
218

(b) behavioral sensitization was accompanied by an for at least 21 days after the last pretreatment injec-
enhancement in ventral striatal DA neurotransmis- tion, and perhaps for much longer 3°.
sion in vivo; (c) AMPH pretreatment was not neuro- The behavioral hypersensitivity to an AMPH chal-
toxic, in that regional brain concentrations of DA lenge reported here was qualitatively similar to the
and 5-HT were normal in postmortem tissue; and (d) effects of a more typical constant low dose regimen,
AMPH pretreatment did not produce persistent as were the neurochemical sequelae 4'5'3°'39"41'42. In
(25-30 days) alterations in spontaneous motor activi- contrast, the effects were quite different from those
ty across the day-night cycle. The implications of produced by neurotoxic treatment regimens (ref. 30
these findings are discussed below. for review). The escalating dose regimen mimics to
some extent the pattern of drug use seen in addicts,
Behavior and therefore, the data further support the idea that
Spontaneous motor activity over the light-dark the sensitization phenomenon provides a good ani-
cycle was relatively normal in AMPH-pretreated rats mal model for studying the neurobiology of AMPH
by 25-30 days after the cessation of drug treatment. psychosis3°'4° - whether sensitization is induced by
The only difference between saline and AMPH-pre- pretreatment with constant low doses or escalating
treated animals was that the former group showed an doses of AMPH.
increase in activity towards the end of the dark Nevertheless, there are differences between con-
period and the latter group did not. This resulted in stant low dose and escalating dose regimens. We can-
slightly lower activity scores in AMPH-pretreated not directly compare the magnitude of the behavioral
rats during the last couple of hours before lights-on. syndrome reported here with the syndrome produced
In contrast, Robinson and Camp 33 reported that the by a lower constant dose regimen, because of meth-
same escalating dose regimen as used here produced odological differences in how behavior was quanti-
a significant decrease in nocturnal activity. But Rob- fied. But differences in the amount of individual vari-
inson and Camp 33 measured activity 8-12 days fol- ation suggest that treatment with escalating doses
lowing the cessation of drug treatment, and in the produces much more intense sensitization. In the
present study animals were withdrawn for 25-30 present study every animal pretreated with AMPH
days. A comparison of the two studies suggests that showed vigorous stereotyped behavior, as indicated
the hypoactivity seen following withdrawal from this by the cessation of locomotor activity between about
escalating dose regimen 33 largely dissipates in about 50 and 90 rain after the challenge injection of 2.0
one month. mg/kg (Fig. 5). In comparison, when Camp and Rob-
Despite relatively normal levels of spontaneous
motor activity there was a large effect of AMPH pre-
treatment on the behavioral response to a challenge ESCALATING DOSE
90 50-
injection of AMPH. In control animals the AMPH
challenge produced general hyperactivity. In 144
70 40

>
AMPH-pretreated animals it produced intense fo- o 50 30

cused stereotyped behavior, and a multiphasic pat- 0 30 20

tern of locomotion. Thus, in AMPH pretreated rats


0 10 10
the AMPH challenge produced a behavioral syn-
-10 • , , , , . ,
drome characteristic of a hyperdopaminergic 5 I0 15 20 5 10 15 20
state 39"41, indicating that even after 21 days of with- INTERVAL
drawal AMPH-pretreated animals were still highly Fig. 5. Left: the frequencyof crossovers in the 8 individual fe-
sensitized. The behavioral response to a challenge male rats pretreated with escalating doses of amphetamine dur-
ing the twenty 10 min intervals followinga challenge injection
with 2.0 mg/kg of AMPH seen here was very similar of 2.0 mg/kg of amphetamine. Each line represents data from
to that reported previously in animals withdrawn one rat. Right: the average frequency of crossovers for the
from escalating dose AMPH treatment for only 12 same 8 rats (closed symbols) and for saline-pretreated rats (re-
plotted from Fig. 2 for ease of comparison). Note that all am-
days33. This suggests that the hypersensitivity to the phetamine-pretreated rats showed a triphasic response pat-
motor stimulant effects of AMPH persists unabated tern.
219

inson ~ pretreated rats with one injection of 2.6 mg/kg render some individuals very susceptible to sensitiza-
of A M P H every 3 days, for a total of 9 injections, and tion and others relatively resistant.
also challenged them with 2.0 mg/kg, there was a
great deal of individual variation in response to the Neurochemistry
challenge (Fig. 6). In the latter instance some ani- In Experiment 1 animals were killed 3 3 - 3 4 days
mals showed stereotyped behavior and a multiphasic after the last injection of A M P H and postmortem tis-
pattern of locomotion, but others showed only hyper- sue concentrations of monoamines were measured.
activity. Marked individual variation in sensitization There was no effect of A M P H pretreatment on D A ,
is typical of the constant low dose regimen, as dis- D O P A C , HVA, 5-HT or 5 - H I A A levels, or on me-
cussed recently by Segal and Kuczenski 41 and Camp tabolite/transmitter ratios, in the striatum, nucleus
and Robinson 4'5 (see ref. 28 for review). accumbens, frontal cortex or hypothalamus. This
The relative absence of individual variation in be- finding is important because it establishes that the es-
havioral response to a challenge seen with the esca- calating dose regimen used here is not neurotoxic, in
lating dose regimen may present some advantages in agreement with Robinson and Camp 33. However, we
studying sensitization, especially in trying to identify reported previously that basal D A metabolism is ele-
reliable neural correlates of sensitization. The esca- vated in sensitized animals after shorter periods of
lating dose regimen may also be advantageous for withdrawal (9-12 days) 5'33, and in this experiment, in
studying the complex pattern of changes that occur which animals were withdrawn for 33 days, no such
over time following withdrawal from repeated changes were evident. A major difference between
A M P H use. For example, the hypoactivity seen early our earlier studies 5~33 and the present study is the
after withdrawal from an escalating dose regimen 33 length of the withdrawal period, which suggests that
may be homologous to the dysphoric 'crash' de- sensitization-related changes in basal D A metabo-
scribed in A M P H addicts following withdrawal 7'2I. lism do not persist over a month following with-
On the other hand, the escalating dose regimen may drawal. This is not to say that D A systems are normal
obscure important individual differences in the sus- after a month of withdrawal, because AMPH-stimu-
ceptibility to sensitization 28. A constant low dose reg- lated striatal D A release in vitro is still enhanced 2°'31.
imen may be more suitable for studying factors that Perhaps measures of basal D A activity, and especial-
ly measures of D A metabolism, are relatively insen-
45 sitive indicators of sensitization-related changes in
90
neural activity. After a long period of withdrawal
70 35
laJ sensitization-related changes in neural activity may
0 5o, be apparent only following a challenge stimulus, such
0 3o 25 U as reexposure to A M P H 3° or exposure to stress 32.
iv
15 This is consistent with the behavioral phenomenon,
U lo

-10 ¸ 5 , , ' , • , because after a month of withdrawal sensitized ani-


0 6 12 18 24 0 6 12 18 24
mals appeared quite normal b e h a v i o r a l l y - until
INTERVAL
challenged (see above). Alternatively, the effects on
Fig. 6. Left: the frequency of crossovers in 8 individual female
rats that were pretreated with one injection of 2.6 mg/kg of am- D A metabolism reported previously could represent
phetamine every 3 days, for a total of 9 injections, and then 3 an exaggerated response to the stress associated with
days after the last pretreatment injection were given a chal- the decapitation procedure 33.
lenge injection of 2.0 mg/kg (data from ref. 5). Data are shown
for twenty-four 5 rain intervals after the amphetamine chal- The most important finding in the present study is
lenge, and each line represents data from l animal. The eight that behavioral sensitization was accompanied by an
animals were selected from a larger group of 15 animals to illus- enhancement in D A neurotransmission in vivo. This
trate the range of individual variation in response to the chal-
lenge. Right: the average frequency of crossovers in the total is the first report on the long-term consequences of
group of 15 animals given this pretreatment regimen (replotted repeated A M P H use in which behavioral measures
from Fig. 3F in Camp and RobinsonS). The individual variation
and measures of D A neurotransmission in the ventral
illustrated here is typical of the sensitization produced by a con-
stant low dose regimen (also see refs. 28, 41). Compare this striatum were obtained simultaneously from the
with the level of individual variation illustrated in Fig. 5, same animals, both before and'after an A M P H chal-
220

lenge. There was no difference between AMPH- and in in vitro superfusion studies, in which the effects of
saline-pretreated rats in the extracellular concentra- reuptake are minimized29'31'49;and (b) one would ex-
tion of DA during the resting state, just as there was pect that a change in the reuptake carrier would re-
no difference in behavior during the resting state. A sult in higher extracellular concentrations of D A dur-
challenge injection of AMPH elevated the extracel- ing the resting state, and there were none.
lular concentration of D A in both groups, but to a sig- The data suggest that an enduring enhancement in
nificantly greater extent in AMPH-pretreated rats D A neurotransmission, characterized by an increase
than in saline-pretreated controls. That is, the hyper- in the releasability of DA, may be responsible for the
dopaminergic behavioral syndrome seen in sensi- enduring changes in behavior produced by past expe-
tized animals following an AMPH challenge oc- rience with AMPH. It is worth noting in this regard
curred in association with a significantly elevated ex- that activation of D A systems appears to be neces-
tracellular concentration of D A in the ventral stria- sary for the development of behavioral sensitization,
tum. because concomitant treatment with a D A antago-
The sensitization-related elevation in the extracel- nist prevents sensitization to AMPH 23'47. The data
lular concentration of D A seen following the AMPH presented here, however, do not allow us to ascertain
challenge could be due to an increase in D A release, the direction of putative cause-effect relations - - i.e.
a decrease in D A reuptake, a decrease in D A metab- does the increase in D A release cause the enhanced
olism or a decrease in AMPH metabolism z2. De- behavioral response, or vice versa? It is important to
creased AMPH metabolism could result in higher also consider, therefore, that a similar elevation in
brain levels of AMPH in sensitized animals (and thus AMPH-stimulated DA release has been described in
greater DA release), but the available evidence does related studies involving the superfusion of dorsal
not support this hypothesis s'3°. In fact, brain levels of striatal tissue in vitro 29'31A9. The in vitro experiments
AMPH are reduced in rats given escalating doses of show that behavioral activation is not necessary to
meth-AMPH 3s. We can also eliminate decreased DA demonstrate a sensitization-related enhancement in
metabolism, because the extracellular concentra- DA release, thus supporting the idea that an eleva-
tions of D A metabolites tended to be elevated in sen- tion in DA release is responsible for the development
sitized animals (also see refs. 5, 33). It is more diffi- of behavioral sensitization.
cult to dissociate a change in reuptake from a change The mechanism responsible for the enhancement
in release. There was no effect of AMPH pretreat- in DA release is unknown, but the effect does not ap-
ment on the rate of decline of either D O P A C or HVA pear to be specific to AMPH-stimulated D A release.
following the AMPH challenge. If the decline in Castafieda et al. 3 recently reported that AMPH pre-
D O P A C and HVA is due to AMPH's ability to block treatment also enhances striatal DA release evoked
DA reuptake 22, resulting in less DA available for in- by high KCI or electrical field stimulation in vitro 3. It
traneuronal metabolism, these results would suggest has been hypothesized that a change in the intracellu-
there was no sensitization-related change in reup- lar distribution of DA could account for both an in-
take. However, the decline in D O P A C produced by crease in AMPH-evoked and depolarization-evoked
AMPH may not be due to AMPH's effect on reup- D A release 3, but whether such a mechanism is in-
take, but to an AMPH-induced depletion of newly- volved in the phenomenon reported here remains to
synthesized cytosolic DA 2"51, thus removing, 'the be determined. Whatever the mechanism, it is capa-
main intraneuronal substrate for MAO and thereby ble of producing very long-lasting changes in behav-
reducing D A metabolite levels '52 (p.8). Therefore, ior as a result of past experience, and therefore, may
the present data do not allow us to choose with confi- represent a type of neuronal plasticity common to
dence between release and reuptake hypotheses. other forms of behavioral adaptation (e.g. ref. 15).
Nevertheless, we favor the idea that the elevated ex-
tracellular concentrations of D A seen in sensitized ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS
animals are due to an increase in the ability of AMPH
to induce DA release, because: (a) a sensitization-re- Support from the Scottish Rite Schizophrenia Re-
lated enhancement in DA release has been reported search Program and the National Institutes of Health
221

(NS00844 and DA04294) is gratefully acknowl- Graduate Studies . ( U n i v e r s i t y of M i c h i g a n ) . We


e d g e d . D e v e l o p m e n t of the dialysis m e t h o d was sup- t h a n k D r . J . B . B e c k e r for h e r helpful c o m m e n t s and
p o r t e d in p a r t by a g r a n t f r o m the R a c k h a m S c h o o l o f assistance.

REFERENCES 15 Hawkins, R.D. and Kandel, E.R., Is there a cell-biological


alphabet for simple forms of learning?, Psychol. Rev., 91
1 Bj6rklund, A. and Lindvall, O., Dopamine containing sys- (1984) 375-391.
tems in the CNS. In A. Bj6rklund and T. H6kfelt (Eds.), 16 Heimer, L. and Wilson, R.D., The subcortical projections
Handbook of Chemical Neuroanatomy, Vol. 2: Classical of the allocortex: similarities in the neural associations of
Transmitters in the CNS, Part 1, Elsevier, Amsterdam, the hippocampus, the piriform cortex, and the neocortex.
1984, pp. 55-122. In M. Santini (Ed.), Golgi Centennial Symposium Proceed-
2 Butcher, S.P., Fairbrother, I.S., Kelly, J.S. and Arbuth- ings, Raven, New York, 1975, pp. 177-193.
nott, G.W., Amphetamine-induced dopamine release in 17 Heffner, T.G., Hartman, J.A. and Seiden, L.S., A rapid
the rat striatum: an in vivo microdialysis study, J. Neuro- method for the regional dissection of the rat brain, Pharma-
chem., 50 (1988) 346-355. col. Biochem. Behav., 13 (1980) 453-456.
3 Castafieda, E., Beeker, J. and Robinson, T.E., The long- 18 J6nsson, L.E., Pharmacological blockade of amphetamine
term effects of repeated amphetamine treatment in vivo on effects in amphetamine dependent subjects, Eur. J. Clin.
amphetamine, KCI and electrical stimulation evoked stria- Pharmacol., 4 (1972) 206-211.
tal dopamine release in vitro, Life Sci., 42 (1988) 19 Koda, L.Y. and Gibb, J.W., Adrenal and striatal tyrosine
2447-2456. hydroxylase activity after methamphetamine, J. Pharma-
4 Camp, D.M. and Robinson, T.E., Susceptibility to sensiti- col. Exp. Ther., 185 (1973) 42-48.
zation. I. Sex differences in the enduring effects of chronic 20 Kolta, M.G., Shreve, P., De Souza, V. and Uretsky, N.J.,
D-amphetamine treatment on locomotion, stereotyped be- Time course of the development of the enhanced behavior-
havior and brain monoamines, Behav. Brain Res., in press. al and biochemical responses to amphetamine after pre-
5 Camp, D.M. and Robinson, T.E., Susceptibility to sensiti- treatment with amphetamine, Neuropharmacology, 24
zation. II. The influence of gonadal hormones on enduring (1985) 823-829.
changes in brain monoamines and behavior produced by 21 Kramer, J.C., Fischman, V.S. and Littlefield, D.C., Am-
the repeated administration of D-amphetamine or restraint phetamine abuse, JAMA, 201 (1967)89-93.
stress, Behav. Brain Res., in press. 22 Kuczenski, R., Biochemical actions of amphetamine and
6 Connell, P.H., Amphetamine Psychosis, Chapman and other stimulants. In I. Creese (Ed.), Stimulants: Neuro-
Hill, London, 1958. chemical Behavioral and Clinical Perspectives, Raven, New
7 Davis, J.M. and Schlemmer, R.F., The amphetamine psy- York, 1983, pp. 31-61.
chosis. In J. Caldwell (Ed.), Amphetamines and Related 23 Kuczenski, R. and Leith, N.J., Chronic amphetamine: is
Stimulants, CRC Press, Boca Raton, FL, 1980, pp. dopamine a link in or mediator of the development of toler-
161-173. ance and reverse tolerance?, Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav.,
8 Demellweek, C. and Goudie, A.J., Behavioural tolerance 15 (1981) 405-413.
to amphetamine and other psychostimulants: the case for 24 Magos, L., Persistence of the effect of amphetamine on ste-
considering behavioral mechanisms, Psychopharmacology, reotyped activity in rats, Eur. J. Pharmacol., 6 (1969)
80 (1983) 287-307. 200-201.
9 Ellinwood, E.H., Amphetamine psychosis: I. Description 25 Morgan, M.E. and Gibb, J.W., Short-term and long-term
of the individuals and processes, J. Nerv. Merit. Dis., 144 effects of methamphetamine on biogenic amine metabolism
(1967) 273-283. in extra-striatal dopaminergic nuclei, Neuropharmacology,
10 Ellinwood, E.H., Amphetamine psychosis: individuals, 19 (1980) 989-995.
settings, and sequences. In E.H. Ellinwood and S. Cohen 26 Paxinos, G. and Watson, C., The Rat Brain in Stereotaxic
(Eds.), Current Concepts on Amphetamine Abuse, U.S. Coordinates, 2nd edn., Academic Press, New York, 1986.
Government Printing Office, Washington, DC, 1972, pp. 27 Ridley, R.M,, Baker, H.F., Owen, F., Cross, A.J. and
143-157. Crow, T.J., Behavioural and biochemical effects of chronic
11 Ellison, G.D. and Eison, M.S., Continuous amphetamine amphetamine treatment in the vervet monkey, Psycho-
intoxication: an animal model of the acute psychotic epi- pharmacology, 78 (1982) 245-251.
sode, Psychol. Med., 13 (1983) 751-762. 28 Robinson, T.E., Stimulant drugs and stress: factors in-
12 Ellison, G.D., Eison, M.S., Huberman, H.S. and Daniel, fluencing individual differences in the susceptibility to sen-
F., Long-term changes in dopaminergic innervation of cau- sitization. In C. Barnes and P. Kalivas (Eds.), Sensitization
date nucleus after continuous amphetamine administra- of the Nervous System, Telford Press, Caldwell, N J, in
tion, Science, 201 (1978) 276-278. press.
13 Fibiger, H.C. and McGeer, E.G,, Effects of acute and 29 Robinson, T.E. and Becker, J.B., Behavioral sensitization
chronic methamphetamine treatment on tyrosine hydroxyl- is accompanied by an enhancement in amphetamine-stimu-
ase activity in brain and adrenal medulla, Eur. J. Pharma- lated dopamine release from striatal tissue in vitro, Eur. J.
col., 16 (1971) 176-180. Pharmacol., 85 (1982) 253-254.
14 Fibiger, H.C. and Phillips, A., Reward, motivation, cogni- 30 Robinson, T.E. and Becker, J.B., Enduring changes in
tion: psychobiology of mesotelencephalic dopamine sys- brain and behavior produced by chronic amphetamine ad-
tems. In Handbook of Physiology, Vol. IV, Intrinsic Regu- ministration: a review and evaluation of animal models of
latory Systems of the Brain, American Physiological Socie- amphetamine psychosis, Brain Res. Rev., 11 (1986)
ty, Bethesda, MD, 1986, pp. 647-675.
222

157-198. tration: behavioral characteristics and neurochemical cor-


31 Robinson, T.E., Becker, J.B. and Presty, S.K., Long-term relates, J. Pharmacol. Exp. Ther., 242 (1987) 917-926.
facilitation of amphetamine-induced rotational behavior 42 Segal, D.S. and Mandell, A.J., Long-term administration
and striatal dopamine release produced by a single expo- of amphetamine: progressive augmentation of motor activi-
sure to amphetamine: sex differences, Brain Research, 253 ty and stereotypy, Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav., 2 (1974)
(1982) 231-241. 249-255.
32 Robinson, T.E., Becket, J.B., Young, E.A., Akil, H. and 43 Seiden, L.S., Fischman, M.W. and Schuster, C.R., Long-
Castafieda, E., The effects of footshock stress on regional term methamphetamine induced changes in brain catechol-
brain dopamine metabolism and pituitary/3-endorphin re- amines in tolerant rhesus monkeys, Drug Alcohol Depend.,
lease in rats previously sensitized to amphetamine, Neuro- 1 (1975/76) 215-219.
pharmacology, 26 (1987) 679-691. 44 Trulson, M.E. and Crisp, T., Behavioral effects of seroton-
33 Robinson, T.E. and Camp, D.M., Long-lasting effects of ergic and dopaminergic drugs in cats following chronic am-
escalating doses of o-amphetamine on brain monoamines, phetamine administration, Eur. J. Pharmacol., 99 (1984)
amphetamine-induced stereotyped behavior and sponta- 313-324.
neous nocturnal locomotion, Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav., 45 Trulson, M.E. and Jacobs, B.L., Long-term amphetamine
26 (1987) 821-827. treatment decreases brain serotonin metabolism: implica-
34 Robinson, T.E. and Whishaw, I.Q., Normalization of ex- tions for theories of schizophrenia, Science, 205 (1979)
tracellular dopamine in striatum following recovery from a 1295-1297.
partial unilateral 6-OHDA lesion of the substantia nigra: a 46 Utena, H., On relapse-liability: schizophrenia, ampheta-
microdialysis study in freely moving rats, Brain Research, mine psychosis and animal model. In H. Mitsuda and T, Fu-
450 (1988) 209- 224. kuda (Eds.), Biological Mechanisms of Schizophrenia and
35 Sato, M., Acute exacerbation of methamphetamine psy- Schizophrenia-Like Psychoses, Igaku Shoin, Tokyo, 1974,
chosis and lasting dopaminergic supersensitivity - - a clini- p. 285.
cal survey, Psychopharmacol. Bull., 22 (1986) 751-756. 47 Vezina, P. and Stewart, J., The effects of D-1 and D-2
36 Sato, M., Chen, C.-C., Akiyama, K. and Otsuki, S., Acute dopamine receptor antagonists on the development of sen-
exacerbation of paranoid psychotic state after long-term sitization to the locomotor effects of morphine and amphet-
abstinence in patients with previous methamphetamine amine, Soc. Neurosci. Abstr., 13 (1987) 450,
psychosis, Biol. Psychiat., 18 (1983) 429-440. 48 Vogel, H.W., Miller, J., Waxman, H. and Gottheil, E.,
37 Schmidt, C.J., Sonsalla, P.K., Hanson, G.R., Peat, M.A. Biochemical and behavioral changes in rats during and af-
and Gibb, J.W., Methamphetamine-induced depression of ter chronic D-amphetamine exposure, Drug Alcohol De-
monoamine synthesis in the rat: development of tolerance, pend., 15 (1985) 245-253.
J. Neurochem., 44 (1985) 852-855. 49 Wilcox, R.A., Robinson, T.E. and Becket, J.B., Enduring
38 Schmidt, C.J., Gehlert, D.R., Peat, M,A., Sonsalla, P.K., enhancement in amphetamine-stimulated striatal dopa-
Hanson, G.R.. Wamsley, J.K. and Gibb, J.W., Studies on mine release in vitro produced by prior exposure to am-
the mechanism of tolerance to methamphetamine, Brain phetamine or stress in vivo, Eur. ]. Pharmacol., 124 (1986)
Research. 343 (1985) 305-313. 375-376.
39 Segal, D.S., Behavioral and neurochemical correlates of 50 Wise, R.A. and Bozarth, M.A., A psychomotor stimulant
repeated D-amphetamine administration, Adv. Biochem. theory of addiction, Psychol. Rev., 94 (1987) 469-492.
Psychopharmacol., 13 (1975) 247-262. 51 Zetterstr6m, T., Sharp, T. and Ungerstedt, U., Effect of
40 Segal, D.S., Geyer, M.A. and Schuckit, M.A., Stimulant- neuroleptic drugs on striatal dopamine release and metabo-
induced psychosis: an evaluation of animal models. In lism in the awake rat studied by intracerebral dialysis, Eur.
M.B.H. Youdim, W. Lovenberg, D.F. Sharman and J.R. J. Pharmacol., 106 (1984) 27-37.
Lagnado (Eds.), Essays in Neurochemistry and Neurophar- 52 Zetterstr6m, T., Sharp, T. and Ungerstedt, U., Further
macology, Vol. 5, Wiley, London, 1981, pp. 95-129. evaluation of the mechanism by which amphetamine re-
41 Segal, D.S. and Kuczenski, R., Individual differences in re- duces striatal dopamine metabolism: a brain dialysis study,
sponsiveness to single and repeated amphetamine adminis- Eur. J. Pharmacol., 132 (1986) 1-9.

You might also like