Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 16

Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied Copyright 1995 by the American Psychological Association, Inc.

1995, Vol. 1, No. 4,305-320 1076-898X/95/$3.00

Expertise in Aeronautical Weather-Related Decision Making:


A Cross-Sectional Analysis of General Aviation Pilots
Mark Wiggins David O'Hare
University of Newcastle University of Otago

Weather-related crashes continue to account for a significant proportion of


general aviation (GA) accidents. In the present study, the decision-making
performance and information acquisition strategies of inexperienced, interme-
diate, and experienced pilots were examined from a cognitive skill perspective.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Data were obtained from information search patterns and verbal protocols
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

during a series of 6 computer-based simulated flight scenarios. The results


revealed both quantitative and qualitative differences between the strategies
of experienced and inexperienced pilots. The implications of these results are
discussed in terms of weather-related decision-making training and the validity
of a cognitive skill model of aeronautical decision making.

Weather-related crashes are one of the common- sion making is often considered a skill that cannot
est causes of general aviation fatalities, accounting be prescribed during training. Rather it is expected
for 20.8% of fatal general aviation (GA) crashes in to develop gradually through practical experience.
the United States between 1982 and 1988. (Air- However, in developing this type of experience,
craft Owners and Pilots Association Air Safety relatively inexperienced pilots may be exposed to
Foundation, 1991). Weather-related decision- hazardous situations with which they are ill-
making (WRDM) skills can be defined as those equipped to cope. The aviation literature is replete
skills necessary to recognize and avoid meteorologi- with tales of survival against the odds, which have
cal phenomena that present a hazard to the flight. often been portrayed in hindsight as pivotal experi-
Most GA pilots are qualified to fly only in Visual ences that marked the authors' transition from raw
Meteorological Conditions (VMC), which are de- novice to seasoned professional (e.g., Gann, 1961).
fined by rules relating to cloud base and visibility. Although there is a growing recognition of the
Should such a pilot fail to conform to these need for WRDM to become an integral aspect of
requirements and enter Instrument Meteorologi- pilot training, the characteristics necessary for an
cal Conditions (IMC), the result will almost inevi- optimal training program remain unclear. Unlike
tably involve loss of control of the aircraft resulting
the training environments necessary for develop-
in a fatal crash (Bryan, Stonecipher, & Aron,
ing skills such as forced-landing procedures, it is
1955). Because of the variable nature of operations
in the aviation environment, weather-related deci- more difficult to simulate deteriorating weather
conditions and thereby provide an opportunity to
practice WRDM skills. In addition, there are no
Mark Wiggins, Department of Aviation and Technol- simple rules that inexperienced pilots can be
ogy, University of Newcastle, Newcastle, Australia; David taught, particularly in regards to judging the sepa-
O'Hare, Department of Psychology, University of Otago, ration distances and precise significance of various
Dunedin, New Zealand. meteorological phenomena. Therefore, what is
The computer used in this experiment was provided required is a detailed identification of the skills
by a science research grant from the New Zealand
necessary for effective and efficient WRDM, which
Lottery Grants Board.
Correspondence concerning this article should be can be used as the basis for the development of
addressed to David O'Hare, Department of Psychology, training systems. The aim of the present study was
University of Otago, P.O. Box 56, Dunedin, New Zea- to develop a task that could be used to capture the
land. Electronic mail may be sent via Internet to structure of expert WRDM in the laboratory.
ohare@rivendell.otago.ac.nz. Performance on this task was to be used to

305
306 WIGGINS AND O'HARE

examine the basic differences between expert and capability and decision-making performance dur-
novice weather-related decision making to deter- ing simulated cross-country flights. A comparison
mine if expertise in an open dynamic environment, between the performance of inexperienced and
such as aviation, can be characterized in similar experienced pilots indicated that although several
terms to those used to account for expertise in measures of information processing were predic-
more highly structured domains, such as chess and tive of inexperienced pilots' performance, no rela-
physics (Larkin, McDermott, Simon, & Simon, tionships were found for the experienced group.
1980), The long-term goal of this research is to Because information-processing capacity is an in-
provide a theoretical foundation for the training trinsic element of analytical decision making, Stokes
and development of expert WRDM. As noted et al. (1992) concluded that experienced pilots'
above, the general aviation accident record sug- aeronautical decision making is mediated less by
gests that there is considerable need for improve- analytical strategies and more by intuitive strate-
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

ment. gies such as the direct retrieval of relevant sche-


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

mata from long-term memory. A similar conclu-


sion is proposed by Klein (1989) and Klein and
Characteristics of the Weather-Related Klinger (1991) concerning the diagnosis of the
Decision-Making Task sources of fires by fireground commanders.
According to Woods (1988), the cognitive de- According to Ericsson and Smith (1991), "[R]e-
mands associated with a particular task can be search on expertise may be one of the most rapidly
represented along four dimensions: the extent to expanding areas within cognitive psychology and
which the process is constrained by the environ- cognitive science" (p. 1). The tasks that have been
ment; the complexity of the interrelationships extensively investigated (e.g., chess playing; phys-
between variables; the degree of risk associated ics problem solving) lack many of the key features
with the decision; and the uncertainty of the (e.g., ill-structured problems, time stress, and mul-
information available. Particular problem-solving tiple goals) that characterize real-world decision
strategies become more or less effective, depend- making (Orasanu & Connolly, 1993). The most
ing on the relative values for these dimensions. For notable attempt to capture and model the essence
example, Hammond, Hamm, Grassia, and Pearson of expertise in dynamic environments, involving
(1987) suggested that analytical strategies are best ill-structured problems, time-pressure, and signifi-
suited to tasks with a high degree of risk and cant risks has been made by Klein (1989).
uncertainty in which the problem-solving process is According to Klein (1989), problem-solving and
unconstrained by the environment and the interre- decision-making expertise in dynamic tasks is char-
lationships between variables are complex. acterized by the serial evaluation of options avail-
However, weather-related decision making can able in long-term memory. The option selected is
be conceptualized as a process that is severely dependent on an unconscious and rapid compari-
constrained by the environment. Moreover, the son between the characteristics associated with the
interrelationship between variables is particularly present situation and those embodied within pre-
complex, and there is a high degree of risk and existing memory structures. Empirical evidence for
often extreme uncertainty associated with the the serial evaluation of options among experts has
available information. Indeed, Wiggins and O'Hare been derived primarily from research involving
(1993) contended that the nature of WRDM is expert firefighters during their diagnosis of the
such that analytical strategies are likely to be less probable sources of fires (Klein, 1989). To trace
effective than intuitive strategies in eliciting appro- the decision-making process, Klein asked partici-
priate and timely decisions. pants to "think aloud" as they diagnosed the
Support for this assertion can be derived from a situation and selected the strategy that they consid-
series of analyses of aeronautical decision making ered most effective under the circumstances. Ex-
with the Micro-Computer Decision Simulator pert firefighters were found to evaluate options
(MIDIS; Barnett, Stokes, Wickens, Davis, Rosen- sequentially, discarding those that did not meet
blum, & Hyman, 1987; Stokes, Kemper, & Marsh, the needs of the current situation and retaining
1992). Initial studies were designed to investigate and modifying as necessary, those strategies that
the relationship between information-processing matched the existing situation. Moreover, these
EXPERTISE IN AERONAUTICAL DECISION MAKING 307

strategies were successful in ameleriorating the tal to the ACT*/ACT-R models that the system
situations because they were based on a large learns by doing and that task-related experience
repertoire of prior experiences coupled with a should be a better predictor of expert strategy than
well-developed mental model of the system within any global measure of experience. Ericsson and
which they operated. Charness (1994) highlighted the importance of
In contrast, novice problem-solving perfor- "deliberate practice" sustained over periods of
mance was characterized by the evaluation of years in expert performance.
options concurrently and the subsequent integra- Process-tracing techniques are one method
tion of this information with generalized problem- through which the sequential nature of informa-
solving strategies such as means-ends analysis and tion processing can be identified and the nature of
backward chaining (Klein, 1989). The result is a decision making and problem solving examined in
cumbersome and time-consuming strategy that is detail. In previous research, this has often involved
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

often unsuited to the demands associated with an analysis of eye movements or verbal protocol
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

time-limited and uncertain environments. Ironi- data (Guindon, 1990; Lusk, 1993; Terranova,
cally, this kind of analytical procedure has often Snyder, Seamster, & Treitler, 1989). The former
been recommended as a means of improving the facilitates the identification of information-search
performance of novices (e.g., Benner, 1975). patterns, whereas the latter is designed to identify
Whereas the decision-making strategies of ex- the thought processes that are engaged during the
perts show the features of automatic processing as task. However, the use of verbal protocol data
identified by Shiffrin and Schneider (1977), the exclusively has been criticized widely as being both
decision making of novices more closely resembles obtrusive and subjective (Nisbett & Wilson, 1977).
that of controlled processing because the informa- Bainbridge (1979) provides a thorough discussion
tion pertaining to each situation must be mediated of the value and limitations of verbal protocol data
within working memory. As working memory is in the analysis of operator behavior in complex
both capacity limited and subject to transience, systems. Sanderson, Verhage, and Fuld (1989)
information acquired by novice performers is often showed how a combination of objective measures
lost and/or overwritten such that task-related and verbal protocol analysis can yield valuable
information must be reacquired on a regular basis. insights into complex process control behavior.
Hershey, Walsh, Read, and Chulef (1990) referred The present study was designed to use a combi-
to this reacquisition of task-related information as nation information search and verbal protocol
information recursion. Kirschenbaum (1992) hypoth- methodology (see Sanderson et al., 1989) to inves-
esized that the tendency to "re-look at" informa- tigate the differences in performance on a simu-
tion would differentiate expert and novice subma- lated WRDM task between pilots of different
rine officers. levels of experience at such tasks. The following
The most detailed model of cognitive skill acqui- hypotheses were derived from previous research
sition has been developed by Anderson (1982, on expertise in other domains, and the theoretical
1987,1993). In the ACT* model (and its successor, implications of the recognition-primed (Klein,
ACT-R), all cognitive behavior is controlled by 1989), ACT*/ACT-R (Anderson, 1982,1993), and
production rules. In the early stages of skill acqui- automatic-controlled (Shiffrin & Schneider, 1977)
sition, knowledge is encoded declaratively. Prac- models of decision-making and problem-solving
tice at a task results in the compilation of task- skills.
specific productions. Therefore, the experienced Hypothesis 1: If expertise lies in the develop-
performer is not limited by the characteristics of ment of task-specific, procedural knowledge, then:
working memory, as productions are run autono- (a) experts will be less able to verbalize this
mously. The features of automatic processing dis- procedural knowledge than novices who must
cussed above can also be derived from the ACT* interpret declarative knowledge directly; (b) differ-
model (Anderson, 1992). The ACT* model strongly ences in decision quality will be less apparent when
suggests that as production systems become more expertise is distinguished on the basis of total flight
highly attuned with further task-related experi- hours than on the basis of actual cross-country
ence, experts are likely to search a problem space flying experience; (c) general problem-solving abil-
more selectively and more rapidly. It is fundamen- ity will be unrelated to differences in performance
308 WIGGINS AND O'HARE

within the expert group, but will be moderately outlined the details of the proposed flight includ-
related to performance within the novice group. ing the departure point, the intended destination,
Hypothesis 2: Experts' information search pat- and the magnetic bearing and distance between
terns will be more structured and directed. Conse- the two airports. In addition, the geographical
quently, experts will (a) acquire a smaller quantity location of each decision point was described in
of information than novices; (b) spend less time terms of its distance from the point of departure
examining information; (c) be more concerned along a magnetic bearing running directly from the
with the implications to be drawn from informa- departure point to the destination.
tion gathered; (d) be less likely to articulate their
goals; (e) be less likely to return to information
already accessed (information recursion); (f) not Method
access information on the basis of the "surface"
Participants
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

representation of the problem provided by the


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

experimenter-defined information categories; (g) Forty participants were recruited, of whom 25


show more confidence in their decisions. were residents in the Auckland region (North
Island), with the remainder residents in either the
Overview Otago or Canterbury regions (South Island) of
New Zealand. They ranged in age from 18 to 67
Participants were asked to assume that they years and had completed at least 2 hr as the
were pilots in command of a VFR cross-country pilot-in-command of cross-country flights. These
flight and had reached some point en route where were defined as instrument flight rules (IFR) or
a decision was required as to whether they should visual flight rules (VFR) operations other than
continue toward the destination, divert to an those in training areas, for which the distance to
alternate aerodrome, or return to the point of the nearest airport was at or beyond 20 nmi.1
departure. Participants were advised that each Overall, participants reported a median 120 hr
scenario would comprise an introductory phase, an (range 4-8,000) as the pilot-in-command of cross-
information search phase, a decision phase, and a country flights. In comparison, the median for the
confidence phase. A practice scenario was pro- total number of hours in command was 360 (range
vided to familiarize them with the structure and 12-20,800). The number of cross-country hours
format of the software. It was stressed that they accumulated over the previous 12 months ranged
should, in each case, examine only that informa- from 1 to 650 (mdn — 40). The total number of
tion that they considered necessary in formulating hours accumulated during the previous 12 months
their decisions because the time period in which to ranged from 2.6 to 800, with a median value of 130
conduct the information search would be limited. hr experience.
Participants were tested individually with six Participants were grouped into three categories
aeronautical decision scenarios, of which three defined as: (1) pilots with 2 to 100 hr of cross-
were situated in the North Island region and three country flying experience; (2) pilots with 101 to
in the South Island region of New Zealand. These 1,000 hr experience; and (3) pilots who had accu-
were presented alternately and counterbalanced mulated in excess of 1,000 hr of cross-country
within each group such that half received a North experience. Eighteen were classified into the
Island scenario initially and the other half received 2-100-hr category, with 14 in the 101-1,000-hr
a South Island scenario. Scenarios relating to both category, and 8 in the 1,000-hr category. Pilots in
regions were subsequently presented in a quasi- the first category had accumulated a mean 44.35 hr
random sequence within this counterbalanced (SD = 29.26 hr) as the pilot-in-command of
structure. This procedure was intended to control cross-country flights, and those in the second and
for any biasing of information search patterns that third categories had accumulated a mean 229
may have occurred because of the participants' (SD = 124.5) and 3,640 (SD = 2,601) hr, respec-
relative familiarity with one region and one area tively.
over the other.
Prior to each experimental scenario, partici-
pants were provided with a brief introduction that In aviation, distances are expressed in nautical miles.
EXPERTISE IN AERONAUTICAL DECISION MAKING 309

Apparatus conditions corresponding to the forward, star-


board, port, and rear views as observed from the
The experiment was conducted with a NEC cockpit. This information referred specifically to
ProSpeed 286 portable computer equipped with a the type of cloud visible, the approximate altitude
VGA card linked to a remote PCDIRECT visual of the cloud base-ceiling above ground level, and
graphics adaptor (VGA) monitor. A computer any noticeable change in these conditions, such as
programmed in Turbo Basic by technicians at the an apparent increase in density or lowering of the
University of Otago, collected personal details and cloud base. Relative to the decision point, the
information pertaining to the participant's flying distances and magnetic bearings to five alternate
experience. The program also incorporated a test airports were available, with the distance repre-
of general problem-solving ability, and one prac- sented in nautical miles and the bearings corrected
tice and six experimental aeronautical-decision for magnetic variation. Information relating to the
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

scenarios presented sequentially. outside air temperature, ground-speed, and alti-


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

tude of the aircraft at the decision point was also


Problem-Solving Ability provided in Submenu 1.
A series of aircraft performance characteristics
This was assessed with a syllogistic reasoning were available in Submenu 2, including a landing
task similar to the test of logical reasoning incorpo- performance table, cruise performance table, and
rated in the SPARTANS (Simple Portable Avia- crosswind landing specifications. For each sce-
tion Relevant Task Battery and Answer Scoring nario, information relating to one of three types of
System) test battery (Baddeley, 1968; Stokes & single-engined aircraft was provided alternately in
Raby, 1989). Participants were presented with order to minimize the possibility that the perfor-
eight syllogisms and required to respond true or mance characteristics accessed in one scenario
false to the validity of the conclusion presented. would be recalled and transferred to subsequent
Both response latency and accuracy were re- scenarios.
corded. Submenu 3 contained the meteorological infor-
mation that would normally be available to pilots
conducting cross-country flights within New Zea-
Aeronautical Scenarios
land. The weather patterns and characteristics
Each decision scenario included aeronautical were devised in conjunction with air traffic control
information normally available to pilots during authorities situated within the region relevant to
VFR cross-country flights within New Zealand. each scenario. As air traffic controllers are respon-
This information was presented on the computer sible for issuing hourly metereorological reports,
screen in a menu-based format with a single main they were expected to possess a thorough under-
menu and five submenus (see Figure 1). These standing of the local weather conditions and the
were designated as (1) the current state of the difficulties experienced by pilots. They were thus
aircraft; (2) the performance characteristics of the considered qualified to ensure that the meteorologi-
aircraft; (3) the meteorological conditions; (4) cal conditions included within scenarios were not
aerodrome specifications; and (5) the topographi- uncommon for the various regions (Ryan, 1984).
cal map. Submenu 1 incorporated a number of Submenu 4 contained aerodrome information
additional menus including cockpit views and the relating to the destination airport and each of the
distances and bearings to selected alternate air- five alternate airports. These were computer-
ports. Similarly, meteorological reports and termi- generated reproductions of the information issued
nal area forecasts were included as additional by the New Zealand Civil Aviation Authority,
menus within Submenu 3. which included an aerial photograph of the air-
Submenu 1 included information regarding the port, the type of runway surface, its serviceability,
amount of fuel and daylight remaining in which to and its dimensions.
complete the flight. The values were designed such A series of 10 computer-generated images of the
that the endurance in each scenario was less than World Aeronautical Chart (WAC) appropriate to
90 min at the decision point. In addition, Submenu each flight were available in Submenu 5 and
1 included four verbal descriptions of weather displayed in a "16 grays," Graphics Interchange
310 WIGGINS AND O'HARE

15:20:13 I I ^^T I I 1 I
i I I i i i i

1 -Current State of the Aircraft


2 -Aircraft Performance Specifications
3 -Meteorological Information
4 -Aerodrome Diagrams
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

5 -Topographic Map
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

:
- • • • •"•••: ' •' ' "• : • I : • " '. . II ' ' • • • • - . ' ' '•'• i
1 ESC for Main Menu II II Please Enter Option I I SPACE for PreviousMenu
f' " •• "

15:20:26

1 —Fuel Remaining 6 -Ground Speed


2 -Daylight Remaining 7 -Outside Air Temperature
3 —Distance to Airports 8 -Cockpit Views
4 -Bearings to Airports
5 -Present Altitude

ESC for Main Menu Please Enter Option SPACE for Previous Menu

Figure 1. (top) The main menu and (bottom) the submenu for current state of the
aircraft. Time elapsed is indicated by the horizontal bar at the top of the screen. ESC =
escape.
EXPERTISE IN AERONAUTICAL DECISION MAKING 311

Format (GIF). In each case, the first image por- Verbal Protocols
trayed a 120 run x 45 nm quadrant. It depicted the
Following the practice decision-making sce-
decision point and the proposed track between the nario, participants were given a description of
departure and destination aerodromes. This was "Duncker's candle problem" as described by
displayed at 50% of the original size and provided Anderson (1980). The problem consists of finding
a broader perspective for those participants unfa- a way to attach a candle to a door. The following
miliar with the region. The remaining images objects are available on a table: the candle, some
portrayed 32 x 20 nm quadrants of and about the matches, and a box of tacks. Participants were
decision point. They were to 192% of the original instructed to think aloud as they worked on the
size and oriented toward true north. problem: to say everything they were thinking from
Access to each of the 50 screens of information the start of the problem until they came up with an
(see Table 1) could be accomplished by depressing answer (Ericsson & Simon, 1984). If they stopped
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

the appropriate numeric key on the computer verbalizing for more than 3 s, the experimenter
keyboard. Pressing the space bar returned the user prompted them to continue. On completion, par-
to the screen accessed previously; the escape key ticipants were told that they would be required to
returned the user to the main menu. For those think aloud during each of the weather-related
screens where information was displayed as text, decision-making scenarios.
prompts were provided at the base of each informa-
tion screen. Graphics screens did not include Results
prompts. Therefore, a card detailing the functions
of the computer keys was positioned permanently Information Acquisition
to the side of the computer. A time line, displayed
in the top right-hand corner of both the informa- To differentiate between the types of informa-
tion and menu screens, indicated the proportion of tion accessed, we divided the 50 computer screens
time remaining in which to complete the informa- of aeronautical information available in each sce-
nario into eight distinct categories. These were
tion search.
based on the submenus in the computer program
For each of the six decision scenarios, the
and the subject of the information screens con-
computer recorded both the information screens
tained therein. Table 1 provides a list of these
accessed by participants, and the duration in categories, in addition to the number of informa-
seconds between the selection and subsequent exit tion screens, and the percentage of the total
from a particular screen. Following completion of number of screens that they represented. Encod-
each scenario, participants were presented with a ing the information screens according to these
forced choice that involved either continuing to- categories allowed comparisons to be made be-
ward the proposed destination or returning to the tween both the type of information accessed by
point of departure. participants and the extent to which the sequence
with which their accessed information followed
these experimenter-defined categories.
Table 1 For each aeronautical decision scenario, informa-
Information Categories and the Number of tion screens were coded in sequence with the
Information Screens Contained In Each Scenario Hemi-Semi-Automated Protocol Analyzer
(SHAPA) computer software package for protocol
Information Information screen analysis (Sanderson, James, & Seidler, 1989). This
category Number Percentage program assists in establishing patterns and se-
Current state 5 10 quences in both verbal and nonverbal protocol
Distance to airports 5 10 data with the predicate-argument notation for
Bearings to airports 5 10 encoding. According to Sanderson et al. (1989),
Cockpit views 4 8 predicates are general terms that represent the
Performance 3 6 particular activity of interest, whereas arguments
Meteorological information 12 24 are specific to a situation and are used to qualify
Aerodrome diagrams 6 12
Topographic map
predicates. SHAPA provides for a maximum of 20
10 20
predicates, each containing at least one argument.
312 WIGGINS AND O'HARE

In addition to the frequency with which predi- flying experience, with novices making the most
cates and arguments occur, the SHAPA computer recursions and experts the fewest.
program also produces transition matrices, detail-
ing the sequence in which predicates occur. First,
second, and third order transition matrices are Search Patterns
provided, corresponding to the frequency of predi-
cate* given predicate A (first order), the frequency As outlined previously, the sequence in which
of x given A then B (second order), and the participants accessed the information screens was
frequency of A: given A then B then C (third order; recorded for each scenario. These screens were
Sanderson et a!., 1989). These facilitate the identi- categorized according to the information category
fication and quantification of interrelationships from which they originated, and the sequences
that may exist between successive predicates gener- were examined with transition matrices. Transi-
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

ated during the performance of a task. tion matrices are used primarily for the identifica-
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

A 3 x 2 x 3 mixed factorial analysis of variance tion and tabulation of recurring patterns of behav-
(ANOVA) with repeated measures on the second ior during protocol analysis (Sanderson et al.,
and third variables was conducted for each of the 1989). In the present study, the behavior of particu-
measured variables (number of information screens lar interest related to the frequency with which
successive information screens were accessed from
accessed; time spent examining the information
the same information category (predicate). This
screens; number of information recursions). The
provided an indication of the extent to which
aim was to establish whether differences existed
information was being accessed according to the
between cross-country flying experience (three
experimenter-defined categories shown in Table 1
levels), the relative familiarity of the scenario
or in some other sequence. Tabulations of pairs of
characteristics (two levels), and responses across predicates were referred to as first-order transition
trials (three levels). matrices, and sequences involving triples or qua-
Analysis of the results arising for the number of druples were labeled second- and third-order tran-
information screens accessed during scenarios re- sition matrices respectively. Transition matrices
vealed a significant main effect for cross-country enabled the tabulation of frequencies and thus the
flying experience, F(2, 37) = 3.51, p < .05. Post calculation of the proportion of the total number
hoc tests with the Tukey (B) statistic indicated that of sequences that originated from the same infor-
the difference lay between the inexperienced and mation category.
experienced groups, with novices accessing the A 3 x 2 x 3 repeated measures multivariate
greatest number of information screens, and ex- analysis of variance (MANOVA) was conducted to
perts the least. examine the relationship between cross-country
The ANOVA of the time spent examining the flying experience (three levels), relative familiarity
information screens revealed a significant main with the characteristics associated with scenarios
effect for cross-country flying experience, F(2,37) = (two levels), trials (three levels), and the three-
4.55, p < .05. Post hoc tests indicated that the element vector of measured variables comprising
inexperienced group took significantly longer than the proportions derived from the first-order, sec-
the experienced group to examine the information ond-order, and third-order transition matrices.
screens. The results disclosed a statistically significant main
The ANOVA for the number of information effect for cross-countryflyingexperience, F(2,37) =
recursions revealed a significant main effect for 3.46, p < .01, although there were no main effects
cross-country flying experience, F(2, 37) = 7.88, or interactions relating to familiarity or trials. In
p < .01, with no main effects or interactions order to identify the variable or variables within
involving familiarity or trials. Post hoc tests dis- which these differences lie, we conducted univari-
closed significant differences between the experi- ate tests between the cross-country groups for each
enced group and both the intermediate and inexpe- of the three measured variables. Statistically signifi-
rienced groups. The differences indicate that overall cant differences were evident between groups for
the number of information recursions made during the proportions derived from each of the first-
scenarios decreased as a function of cross-country order, F(2, 37) = 6.23,p < .01, second-order, F(2,
EXPERTISE IN AERONAUTICAL DECISION MAKING 313

37) = 8.72, p < .01, and third-order, F(2, 37) = ences between scenarios or between the sequence
11.82, p < .01, transition matrices. with which the scenarios were presented. How-
Because all three measured variables showed ever, statistical analysis revealed differences be-
significant main effects for cross-country flying tween levels of cross-country flying experience.
experience, it was considered appropriate to deter- Pilots in the intermediate and experienced groups
mine whether any similarities existed between the were more likely to choose to continue toward
responses. Post hoc tests (Tukey B) for each of the their destinations than pilots categorized as inexpe-
first-, second-, and third-order transition matrices rienced who were more likely to elect to return to
revealed a significant difference between the expe- their take-off points, x2(2,N = 40) = 9.02, p < .02.
rienced group and the intermediate and inexperi-
enced groups. These results indicate that of the
Response Latency
total number of information screens accessed by
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

each pilot, the proportion of successive screens In addition to recording the particular informa-
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

from the same information category was lower for tion screen selected, the computer program also
those in the experienced group than for those recorded the response latency for each selection.
pilots in either the intermediate or inexperienced Across participants, the mean response latency for
groups. As Figure 2 reveals, the proportions de- the forced choice between returning to the point of
creased as a function of cross-country category for take-off or continuing en route to the destination
all three transition matrices. was 18.9 s (SD = 17.97). The raw data were
transformed logarithmically prior to statistical
Decision Making analysis to reduce any disparity between the covari-
ance matrices of groups. These data were sub-
Following the information acquisition phase of jected to a 3 x 2 x 3 mixed factorial ANOVA to
each scenario, participants were asked to select determine whether differences existed between
from pairs of alternative strategies, those which cross-country experience (three levels), familiarity
they would most prefer to undertake given the (two levels), and trials (three levels), for the
particular circumstances. One of these pairs in- response latency recorded for the forced choice
volved a forced choice between either returning to between continuing toward the destination or
the departure point or continuing en route to the returning to the take-off point.
destination. Overall, participants chose to return The results disclosed a significant main effect for
to their point of departure in 43% of cases, cross-country flying experience, F(2, 37) = 3.29,
whereas 57% of selections involved continuing the p < .01. An inspection of the data for the three
flight en route to the destination. Chi-square tests cross-country groups indicated that the mean re-
did not reveal any statistically significant differ- sponse latency for the intermediate group ex-
0.2

• First-Order TM
c 0.15 nSecond-Order TM
o

L.
OThird-Order TM
a
CL
a 0.1

03
0)
0.05

Novices Intermediate Experts


Cross-country group
Figure 2. Mean proportion of the sequences of information screens accessed from the
same category for first-order, second-order, and third-order transition matrices (TM).
314 WIGGINS AND O'HARE

ceeded that for the inexperienced group, which in Table 2


turn exceeded that recorded for the experienced Multiple Regression Models for Each Cross-Country
group. Group Between the Accuracy and Response Latency
Associated With the Nonsense Syllogisms Task and
the Number of Information
Confidence
Recursions During Scenarios
The final aspect of the decision selection phase Problem solving (p)
Multiple
involved participants choosing from a 7-point Likert Group R Accuracy SD Latency SD
scale, the number that best represented their level
of confidence in the efficacy of their selections. Inexperienced .584* -.486* 1.35 .475* 7.59
Intermediate .215 .234 0.63 .149 7.86
Overall, participants expressed a confidence level .447 .402 1.19 -.157 9.75
Experienced
of 2.8 (SD = 1.4) where a score of 1 indicated
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

absolute confidence in the decision. A 3 x 2 x 3 Note. One pilot from the experienced group was excluded
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

from this analysis as an outlier.


mixed factorial ANOVA was used to determine *p < .05.
whether differences in confidence existed between
cross-country flying experience (three levels), famil-
iarity (two levels), and trials (three levels). The Indeed, the results indicated that this type of
results of this analysis revealed a statistically signifi- problem-solving ability was unrelated to the perfor-
cant main effect for cross-country flying experi- mance of intermediate or expert pilots in the
ence, F(2,37) = 5A5,p < .01, although there were decision scenarios. However, it was predictive of
no main effects or interactions involving familiarity performance by the inexperienced pilots and thus
or trials. provides support for Hypothesis Ic.
Post hoc tests with Tukey's B statistic indicated
that the inexperienced group was significantly less Analysis Determined on the Basis of Total
confident than either the intermediate or the Flight Hours
experienced group. Inspection of the respective
category means showed that the level of decision- All the previous analyses were repeated with
making confidence was greatest for the experi- pilots classified into three categories according to
enced group. This is consistent with Shanteau's total flying experience rather than cross-country
(1988) characterization of expert performance. experience. The same criteria (hours of experi-
ence) were used as that applied for the cross-
country categories. No main effects or interactions
Problem-Solving Ability and were found for the number of information screens
Information Acquisition accessed or for time spent examining information
Overall, accuracy of responses in the syllogistic screens. A significant main effect of experience
reasoning task was high (mean correct = 7.05; group on number of information recursions was
SD = 1.26) with a mean response latency of 25.12 s found, F(2, 37) = 3.49, p < .05. The same analysis
(SD = 65). There was a nonsignificant correlation on the basis of cross-country categorization was
between accuracy and latency, r(40) = - .19, p > significant (p < .01). Overall, these results sup-
.05. Both accuracy and latency were entered into port Hypothesis Ib that differences in perfor-
multiple regression equations with the mean num- mance will be more evident as a function of
ber of information recursions (log transformed) as task-specific experience than of flying experience
the criterion variable. As noted in the introduc- in general.
tion, information recursions measure the amount
of reacquisition of task related information that Verbal Protocol Analysis
takes place. Separate equations were calculated
for each experience level (see Table 2). Because of the very large quantity of verbal
Inspection of the means and standard deviations protocol data, 3 participants were randomly se-
for each of the three groups of pilots indicated that lected from each group for detailed analysis. The
valid comparisons could be made regarding their mean total flight hours of the three groups were
performance during the syllogistic reasoning task. experts = 7,483; intermediate = 200; and novices =
EXPERTISE IN AERONAUTICAL DECISION MAKING 315

55. The audiotapes were transcribed and entered percentage of each group's total verbalizations
into the SHAPA system (Sanderson et al., 1989). that were encoded into each category.
The verbalizations were divided into segments It can be seen that the expert pilots made fewer
containing one predicate. The predicates were explicit goal or detection references than other
derived from Rasmussen's (1982) analysis of hu- pilots, but rather more diagnostic statements. Nov-
man error that had previously been applied to the ice pilots were mostly concerned with detecting
coding of aircraft accident reports (O'Hare, Wig- appropriate information and with explicitly articu-
gins, Batt, & Morrison, 1994). The six predicates lating their goals. The very small number of partici-
were detection (identifying information); diagno- pants in each group means that conventional
sis (discerning the meaning of the information); statistical tests lack sufficient power to detect true
goal (selecting a goal); strategy (identifying a differences. Despite this, a test of the absolute
means to achieve the goal); procedure (specifying number of goal statements produced by the three
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

tasks needed to carry out the strategy); and action groups approached statistical significance, F(2,6) =
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

(implementation). Two independent raters coded 4.13, p < .07, as did the absolute number of
samples of two transcripts. The cross-coder reliabil- detection statements, F(2, 6) = 3.38,p < .10. The
ity was assessed by calculating values of Cohen's difference in absolute number of diagnosis state-
kappa. These were 0.73 and 0.79 for the two ments between the three groups was nonsignifi-
samples. These indicate excellent levels of agree- cant, F(2, 6) = 0.33, p > .7. However, the ratio of
ment (Fleiss, 1981). diagnostic to detection statements was less than
A simple frequency analysis of the six predicates one for all participants except for 2 of the 3 expert
showed that although the first three (detection, pilots. Again, this difference approached signifi-
diagnosis, and goal) were used frequently, in- cance,^, 6) = 3.23,p < .11.
stances of strategy, procedure, and action were As noted previously, SHAPA provides a number
rarely recorded. Therefore, for the present analy- of techniques for exploring the sequential depen-
sis, these last three categories were collapsed into dencies in data in greater depth. The first-order
one group referred to as tactics. The novice group transition matrices show that the most common
produced more verbalizations (n = 1,155) than transition for experts was from one diagnostic
either the intermediate (« = 700) or expert groups statement to another (148 examples—more than
(n = 732), although these differences were not 60% greater than the next most common transi-
statistically significant. tion), whereas this pattern was less frequent for
The distribution of the predicates across the novices (64 examples). The most common pattern
three groups (expert, intermediate, and novice) is for novices was from a goal statement to a detec-
shown in Figure 3. The numbers represent the tion statement (224 examples), which was much

50
* • Experts
tn ^Intermediates
£ 40
a; DNovices j

to 30

20
C
o
•H
10
o
Q.
O

Detection Diagnosis Goal Tactics


Category
Figure 3. Proportions of total predicate encodings in each category obtained from the
concurrent verbal protocol data.
316 WIGGINS AND O'HARE

less frequent for experts (86 examples). This differ- lem. Collectively, the results provide considerable
ence approached statistical significance, F(2, 6) = empirical support for the theoretical distinctions
3.78,p < .08. These differences are consistent with proposed between the information acquisition strat-
the working backward and working forward strate- egies of experienced and inexperienced pilots.
gies adopted by novices and experts respectively in These results occurred independently of either
solving physics problems (Larkin et al., 1980). the particular characteristics of the scenarios (trial)
The second-order transition matrices showed or the extent to which pilots were familiar with the
that experts were most likely to transition to a third particular area. Thus it can be concluded that the
diagnosis statement, whereas novices were most differences evident between experienced and inex-
likely to transition to a detection statement. Inspec- perienced pilots, both qualitatively and quantita-
tion of the frequency of predicate cycles supplied tively, were relatively consistent and not the prod-
by SHAPA confirms these observations. Further uct of idiosyncracies associated with any particular
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

detail of these analyses of the verbal protocols scenario.


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

obtained in the present study can be found in Because we were unable to differentiate signifi-
O'Hare, Wiggins, and Jones (1995). cantly between the performance of intermediate
and inexperienced pilots in the experimental sce-
narios, we believe this suggests that either the
Discussion transition between stages was continuous instead
Strategies of Information Acquisition of stepwise or that the classification procedure
used did not distinguish effectively between pilots
During the acquisition of aeronautical informa- at the two stages. This is a matter for further
tion, experienced pilots accessed significantly fewer research.
information screens, made fewer information recur-
sions, and spent relatively less time examining the
information screens than inexperienced pilots. Verbal Protocol Analysis
These results are consistent with Hypotheses 2a, e,
and b, respectively, and suggest that experienced Combined with differences in information acqui-
pilots required relatively less task-related informa- sition strategies, the distinction between automatic
tion with which to formulate a decision that they and controlled processing also predicts differences
considered appropriate under the circumstances. between experienced and inexperienced pilots in
This is consistent with research in other domains, terms of the extent to which information is verbal-
such as financial analysis (Bouman, 1980) and ized (Shanteau, 1988). Thus, in Hypothesis la, we
auditing (Bedard, 1989), and suggests that experi- predicted that experts would be less able to verbal-
enced decision makers use information search ize procedural knowledge than the novices who
patterns that are more structured and efficient interpret declarative information directly. Al-
than those of their inexperienced counterparts. though the novice pilots produced more verbaliza-
Further support for this assertion can be derived tions than both the intermediate and expert groups,
from the results arising from the analysis of transi- these differences were not statistically significant.
tion matrices. Of the total number of sequences of However, the hypotheses that experts would be
information screens accessed by pilots, a signifi- less likely to articulate their goals (Hypothesis 2d)
cantly greater proportion was accessed from the and would be more concerned with implications or
same submenu by inexperienced pilots than by diagnosis (Hypothesis 2c) were confirmed.
experienced pilots. Therefore, those pilots who Although they are based on a small sample,
were experienced in formulating weather-related these findings are strikingly consistent with those
decisions were less likely to access information of Larkin et al. (1980) who noted that when
according to the experimenter-defined categories, attempting to solve elementary physics problems
supporting Hypothesis 2f. This is consistent with novices "seem to require goals and sub-goals to
research in other domains that suggests that ex- direct their search" (p. 1338). Larkin et al. hypoth-
perts operate on a different representation of the esized that the management of these goals takes
problem than novices, who are more likely to up a substantial proportion of working memory
operate with the surface representation of a prob- capacity, thus accounting in part for the relatively
EXPERTISE IN AERONAUTICAL DECISION MAKING 317

slow and effortful problem-solving performance of Problem-Solving Skills and Pilot Performance
novices.
As an individual develops experience in a particu-
lar domain, Klein (1989) and Klein and Klinger
(1991) suggested that task-specific experiences are
Aeronautical Decisions acquired. These experiences through further prac-
tice can be applied successfully to a variety of
Because of the nature of the terrain, coupled problems and circumstances. This makes redun-
with the daylight and fuel limitations, the decision dant any reliance on general procedures for prob-
to return to the point of departure would, in most lem resolution. Consequently, the performance of
of scenarios used, be ill-advised. A chi-square
individuals at later stages of skill acquisition would
analysis of the decisions indicated that pilots in the
be expected to occur independent of any profi-
intermediate and experienced groups were signifi-
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

ciency associated with the application of problem-


cantly more likely to continue toward their destina-
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

solving procedures in general. Thus it was


tions than to divert. In contrast, pilots in the
inexperienced group chose with greater frequency predicted in Hypothesis Ic that general problem-
to return to the point of take-off. At the ab initio solving ability would be unrelated to the perfor-
stage of training, pilots are generally taught to mance of experts but would be moderately related
conduct a 180° turn if the conditions en route to the performance of novices.
prevent a landing at the destination proposed. This Multiple regression analyses were conducted for
recommendation is based on the assumption that each of the three cross-country groups between the
the meterological conditions are likely to be favor- number of information recursions made during
able in the direction from which the aircraft has scenarios and both the accuracy and response
traveled recently. Thus in an experimental situa- latency associated with the responses to the non-
tion, it is possible that inexperienced pilots would sense syllogisms. A significant relationship was
rapidly adopt the normative option of returning to found between the number of information recur-
the point of departure (a 180° turn), rather than sions made by novice pilots and both the accuracy
actively formulating a decision on the basis of the and response latency of their responses to the
information available. syllogisms task. No relationships were found for
The analysis of the decision response latency intermediate or expert pilots.
data disclosed a significant main effect between
cross-country groups. Further inspection of the
data revealed that the response latency for the Measures of Experience
decision phase was greater among inexperienced
pilots than among expert pilots. This suggests that According to the principles embodied within the
inexperienced pilots did not quickly select the cognitive expertise framework, the compilation of
more obvious option (to discontinue rather than task-related experiences requires that the indi-
press on) but may have had greater difficulty vidual participate actively in the performance of a
formulating an appropriate decision strategy dur- particular task. This is an essential feature of
ing the limited time available. Experienced pilots production system models of cognitive skill acquisi-
required the least time to develop a strategy that tion (Anderson, 1983; 1989; 1993). Thus in the
they considered effective. aeronautical domain, it might be expected that
The final aspect of the decision phase involved flying experience, other than that associated with
the participants rating on a 7-point Likert scale weather-related decision making, would not lead
their confidence in the appropriateness of their to the development of productions with which to
selections. The results revealed a significant main exercise skilled weather-related decisions. There-
effect for cross-country flying experience, with the fore, we predicted in Hypothesis Ib that total
inexperienced pilots showing the least confidence flying experience would be less effective than
in their decisions and the experts the most. This is cross-country flying experience as a predictor of
broadly consistent with the differences predicted differences between the weather-related decision-
in Hypothesis 2g, although the differences between making performance of inexperienced pilots and
expert and intermediate pilots was not significant. either intermediate or experienced ones.
318 WIGGINS AND O'HARE

The results for the number of information Discussion


screens accessed, the frequency of information
recursions, and the time spent examining the The present cross-sectional analysis of the
information screens did not reveal any significant weather-related decision-making performance of
effects either between- or within-subjects for those pilots with differing levels of cross-country flying
experience has provided evidence that expert-
groups classified according to total flying experi-
novice differences can be captured in a laboratory
ence. In contrast, the analyses made on the basis of task. In particular, a qualitative distinction was
pilot cross-country flying experience revealed sig- observed between both the information search and
nificant main effects for the three measured vari- problem-solving strategies used by inexperienced
ables. Consequently, the performance between and experienced pilots. This was interpreted as
groups was delineated more effectively by the qualified support for the notion that through
cross-country classification than by the total flying
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

task-specific experience, individuals develop proce-


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

experience classification. Combined, these results dures that can be generalized and applied subse-
provide empirical support for Hypothesis Ib: Dif- quently to a variety of situations.
ferences between experts and novices will be less Quantitative differences were also observed be-
apparent when a global measure of experience, tween the information search strategies of inexpe-
such as total flying hours, rather than a proximal rienced and experienced pilots, with the former
measure, such as cross-country flying experience, is accessing a greater number of information screens;
used. making a greater number of information recur-
The distinction between the cross-country and sions; and spending more time examining the
total experience classifications also provides sup- information screens than the latter. Inexperienced
port for the assumption that the acquisition of a pilots also exhibited a greater response latency in
cognitive skill is dependent on the participation of selecting from a forced choice: whether to con-
tinue to the destination or return to the point of
an individual repeatedly in the performance of
take-off.
that skill (Ericsson & Charness, 1994). Thus, the
The main aim of cross-sectional analysis is to
development of weather-related decision-making determine whether differences exist between cer-
skills is unlikely to occur if the pilot has not directly tain groups defined a priori. The main disadva-
experienced situations in which the application of tange associated with this discriminatory approach
such skills are necessary. Moreover, it is not is that transitory phases are often overlooked.
possible to instill such skills solely through text- From a training perspective, this information is
books or even through demonstrations. The evi- extremely important if effective training programs
dence suggests that these resources need to be are to be developed. Trainers and educators must
combined with practical tasks in order to facilitate not only be able to identify the quantity and quality
the transition from the initial, declarative stage to of the information required to effect the transition,
the advanced, procedural stages of skill acquisition but they must also be able to identify the character-
(Anderson, 1987). istics associated with the transition. Thus future
In an interesting study of U.S. Air Force Class A research should undertake either a longitudinal
fighter mishaps, McKinney (1993) found that the approach or a cluster analysis across participants
decisions made by highly experienced flight leads in order to determine at which point or points
in response to an unexpected mechanical malfunc- along the continuum the significant transition from
tion were generally worse than those made by novice to expert occur.
less experienced flight leads. Experience was di-
chotomized into > 500 hr on type and less than References
500 hr on type. Although flight leads are selected Anderson, J. R. (1980). Cognitive psychology and its
as pilots of exceptional all-round ability, they do implications. San Francisco: Freeman.
not routinely engage in training in this particular Anderson, J. R. (1982). Acquisition of cognitive skill.
task. As in the present study, overall experience Psychological Review, 89, 369-406.
was not synonymous with expertise in a particular Anderson, J. R. (1983). The architecture of cognition.
task. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
EXPERTISE IN AERONAUTICAL DECISION MAKING 319

Anderson, J. R. (1987). Skill acquisition: Compilation of during software system design. International Journal of
weak-method problem solutions. Psychological Re- Man-Machine Studies, 33, 279-304.
view, 94, 192-210. Hammond, K. R., Hamm, R. M., Grassia, J., & Pearson,
Anderson, J. R. (1992). Automaticity and the ACT* T. (1987). Direct comparison of the efficacy of intui-
theory. American Journal of 'Psychology, 105, 165-180. tive and analytical cognition in expert judgment. IEEE
Anderson, J. R. (1993). Rules of the mind. Hillsdale, NJ: Transactions on Systems, Man, and Cybernetics, 17,
Erlbaum. 753-770.
Aircraft Owners and Pilots Association Air Safety Foun- Hershey, D. A., Walsh, D. A., Read, S. J., & Chulef,
dation (1991). General aviation accident analysis book A. S. (1990). The effects of expertise on financial
for the years 1982-1988. Frederick, MD: AOPA Air problem solving: Evidence for goal directed, problem
Safety Foundation. solving scripts. Organizational Behavior and Human
Baddeley, A. D. (1968). A three-minute reasoning test Decision Processes, 46, 77-101.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

based on grammatical transformation. Psychonomic Kirschenbaum, S. S. (1992). Influence of experience on


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Science, 10, 341-342. information-gathering strategies. Journal of Applied


Bainbridge, L. (1979). Verbal reports as evidence of the Psychology, 77, 343-352.
process operator's knowledge. International Journal of Klein, G. A. (1989). Recognition-primed decisions
Man-Machine Studies, 11, 411-436. (RPD). In W. B. Rouse (Ed.), Advances in man-
Barnett, B., Stokes, A., Wickens, C. D., Davis, T., machine systems (Vol. 5, pp. 47-92). Greenwich, CT:
Rosenblum, R., & Hyman, F. (1987). A componential JAI.
analysis of pilot decision-making (pp. 842-846). In Klein, G., & Klinger, D. (1991). Naturalistic decision-
Proceedings of the Human Factors Society 31st Annual making. CSERIAC Gateway, 2, 1-4.
Meeting. Santa Monica, CA: Human Factors Society. Larkin, J., McDermott, J., Simon, D. P., & Simon, H. A.
Beck, G. A. (1987). The problem of visual flight rules flight (1980). Expert and novice performance in solving
into instrument meteorological conditions: A protocol physics problems. Science, 208, 1335-1342.
analysis of general aviation pilots' subjective weather Lusk, C. M. (1993). Assessing components of judgments
forecasting procedure. Unpublished master's thesis. in an operational setting. In O. Svenson & A. J. Maule
Columbus: Ohio State University. (Eds.), Time pressure in human judgment and decision-
Bedard, J. (1989). Expertise in auditing: Myth or reality? making (pp. 309-321). New York: Plenum Press.
Accounting, Organisations and Society, 14, 113-131. McKinney, E. H. (1993). Flight leads and crisis decision
Benner, L. (1975). D. E. C. I. D. E. in the hazardous making. Aviation, Space, and Environmental Medicine,
materials emergencies. Fire Journal, 69, 13-18. 64, 359-362.
Bouman, M. J. (1980). Application of information pro- Nisbett, R. E., & Wilson, T. D. (1977). Telling more
cessing and decision-making research. In G. R. Ung- than we can know: Verbal reports on mental pro-
son & D. N. Braunstein (Eds.), Decision making: An cesses. Psychological Review, 84, 231-259.
interdisciplinary inquiry. Boston, MA: Kent O'Hare, D., Wiggins, M., & Jones, G. (1995). A verbal
Bryan, L. A., Stonecipher, J. W., & Aron, K. (1955). protocol analysis of expert aeronautical decision making.
AOPA's 180° rating. Washington, DC: AOPA Founda- Unpublished manuscript.
tion & Aircraft Owners and Pilots Association. O'Hare, D., Wiggins, M., Batt, R., & Morrison, D.
Ericsson, K. A., & Charness, N. (1994). Expert perfor- (1994). Cognitive failure analysis for aircraft accident
mance: Its structure and acquisition. American Psy- investigation. Ergonomics, 37, 1855-1869.
chologist, 49, 725-747. Orasanu, J., & Connolly, T. (1993). The reinvention of
Ericsson, K. A., & Simon, H. A (1984). Protocol analysis: decision making. In G. A. Klein, J. Orasanu, R.
Verbal reports as data. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Calderwood, & C. E. Zsambok (Eds.), Decision mak-
Ericsson, K. A., & Smith, J. (1991). Toward a general ing in action: Models and methods. Norwood, NJ:
theory of expertise. Cambridge, England: Cambridge Ablex.
University Press. Rasmussen, J. (1982). Human errors: A taxonomy for
Fleiss, J. L. (1981). Statistical methods for rates and describing human malfunction in industrial installa-
proportions. New York: Wiley. tions. Journal of Occupational Accidents, 4, 311-333.
Gann, E. K. (1961). Fate is the hunter. London: Hodder Ryan, A. P. (1984). Flying weather: Manual of New
& Stoughton. Zealand aviation climatology. Wellington, New Zea-
Guindon, R. (1990). Knowledge exploited by experts land: New Zealand Meteorological Service.
320 WIGGINS AND O'HARE

Sanderson, P. M., James, J. M., & Seidler, K. S. (1989). Terranova, M., Snyder, C. E., Seamster, T. L., &
SHAPA: An interactive software environment for Treitler, I. E. (1989). Cognitive task analysis: Tech-
protocol analysis. Ergonomics, 32, 1271-1302. niques applied to airborne weapons training. In Pro-
Shanteau, J. (1988). Psychological characteristics and ceedings of the Human Factors Society 33rd Annual
strategies of expert decision-makers. Acta Psychologia, Meeting (pp. 1358-1362). Santa Monica, CA: Human
68, 203-215. Factors Society.
Shiffrin, R. M., & Schneider, W. (1977). Controlled and Wiggins, M. W., & O'Hare, D. (1993). A skills-based
automatic human information processing: II. Percep- approach to training aeronautical decision-making. In
tual learning, automatic attending, and general theory. R. A. Telfer (Ed.), Aviation instruction and training
Psychological Review, 84, 127-190. (pp. 430-476). Aldershot, England: Ashgate.
Stokes, A. F., Kemper, K. L., & Marsh, R. (1992). Woods, D. D. (1988). Coping with complexity: The
Time-stressed flight decision making: A study of expert psychology of human behaviour in complex systems.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

and novice aviators (Technical Report ARL-93-1/ In L. P. Goodstein, H. B. Andersen, & S. E. Olsen
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

INEL-93-1). Urbana-Champaign, IL: Aviation Re- (Eds.), Tasks, errors and mental models (pp. 128-148).
search Laboratory, University of Illinois. London: Taylor & Francis.
Stokes, A. F., & Raby, M. (1989). Stress and cognitive
performance in trainee pilots. In Proceedings of the Received June 27,1994
Human Factors Society 33rd Annual Meeting (pp. Revision received June 22,1995 m
883-887). Santa Monica, CA: Human Factors Society. Accepted June 28,1995

gsaaeaar _ «saaa

ZSSSgSS&m

aaaar-sgf

You might also like