Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 19

The Mate Screening Motive: How Women

Use Luxury Consumption to Signal to Men

QIHUI CHEN
YAJIN WANG
NAILYA ORDABAYEVA

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jcr/article/50/2/303/6650085 by guest on 22 August 2023


Previous research has found that for men, activating a mating motive increases
luxury consumption as a way to attract a romantic partner. However, little is known
about the role of luxury consumption in women’s romantic endeavors. The present
research conceptualizes a mate screening motive, which explains how women
use luxury consumption to romantically signal to men. Six studies and two follow-
ups conducted in controlled and field settings show that the mate screening motive
boosts women’s consumption of luxury goods as a way to signal their mating
standards to men and thereby deter undesirable pursuers. The effect is dimin-
ished when mate screening is less necessary such as when external screening
tools are available (e.g., screening filters on dating websites), the quality of poten-
tial mates is high, and the focus is on selecting a desirable partner rather than
deterring undesirable pursuers. The findings have important implications for un-
derstanding how consumers use products and brands in romantic relationships
and for designing marketing strategies and communication for luxury brands, com-
mercial dating services, and dating apps. Our findings also provide insights for
consumers on how to use brands and products as effective communication devi-
ces in romantic endeavors.

Keywords: mating motive, mate screening motive, romantic relationship, luxury


consumption, conspicuous consumption

Qihui Chen (qhchen@umd.edu) is a doctoral candidate in marketing at Robert


H. Smith School of Business, University of Maryland, College Park, MD 20742,
USA. Yajin Wang (yajinwang@ceibs.edu) is a professor of marketing at China
Europe International Business School (CEIBS), Shanghai 201206 China. Nailya
Ordabayeva (nailya.ordabayeva@tuck.dartmouth.edu) is an associate professor of
T rue love is priceless, but finding love can be difficult
and costly. An average American spends $120,000
over a lifetime on dating—including romantic dinners,
business administration at Tuck School of Business, Dartmouth College, Hanover, movie dates, and romantic gifts—as well as on personal
NH 03755, USA. Please address correspondence to Qihui Chen. The work described
in this article was partially supported by Dean’s Research Grant, China Europe grooming and cosmetics (Gervis 2019). Dating-related
International Business School (CEIBS), to the second author. The authors would expenses have pushed more than one in four Americans
like to thank the editor, the associate editor, and the anonymous reviewers for their
constructive comments and guidance throughout the review process. The authors into debt (CompareCards 2020). At the same time, the U.S.
also thank Ceren Aksu, Jinhyung Kim, Andy Li, Shaaref Shah, and Mengchen dating industry has grown at an annual rate of 10.7% over
Zheng for their research assistance. The authors gratefully acknowledge the helpful
input from the research seminar participants at the University of Maryland, London the last 5 years (IBIS World 2021). These trends indicate
School of Economics and Political Science, and China Europe International that consumption is an important tool that helps individuals
Business School (CEIBS). The authors also thank Vladas Griskevicius for his in-
sightful comments on this research. Supplementary materials are included in the
form romantic relationships. Accordingly, marketers, in-
web appendix accompanying the online version of this article. cluding luxury brands, regularly appeal to consumers’ ro-
mantic pursuits in their marketing strategies and
communications (see examples in web appendix A).
Prior literature reports that activating a romantic mating
Editor: Bernd H. Schmitt motive prompts men to display luxury goods to attract po-
tential romantic partners by signaling social status
Associate Editor: Echo Wen Wan (Griskevicius et al. 2007; Janssens et al. 2011; Sundie et al.
2011). However, it argues that for women, luxury con-
Advance Access publication July 26, 2022
sumption is less useful in the relationship formation stage,
C The Author(s) 2022. Published by Oxford University Press on behalf of Journal of Consumer Research, Inc.
V
This is an Open Access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial License (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc/4.0/),
which permits non-commercial re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited. For commercial re-use,
please contact journals.permissions@oup.com  Vol. 50  2023
https://doi.org/10.1093/jcr/ucac034
303
304 JOURNAL OF CONSUMER RESEARCH

because men value non-status-related qualities such as so- the growing literature on the role of mating motives in con-
ciability in potential partners. Consistent with this notion, sumption choices (Hasford, Kidwell, and Lopez-Kidwell
prior research finds that a mating motive neither increases 2018; Van den Bergh, Dewitte, and Warlop 2008).
nor decreases women’s desire for luxury goods in the rela- Moreover, it addresses recent calls (Otterbring et al. 2020)
tionship formation stage (Griskevicius et al. 2007; Wang for more research on how consumers’ romantic goals, part-
and Griskevicius 2014). Related work on the relationship ners, and relationships influence consumption decisions
maintenance stage examining the behavior of non-single (Dahl, Sengupta, and Vohs 2009; Mende et al. 2019;
women suggests that these women display luxury to guard Simpson, Griskevicius, and Rothman 2012). Our findings
their existing romantic partner from female competitors make additional contributions to research on luxury con-
(Wang and Griskevicius 2014). Against this backdrop, an sumption, which previously focused on how luxury con-
important open question is what overlooked factors might sumption can attract customers, followers, and employers
drive women’s luxury consumption in their romantic rela- (Desmichel, Ordabayeva, and Kocher 2020; Lee, Ko, and

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jcr/article/50/2/303/6650085 by guest on 22 August 2023


tionship formation pursuits. Megehee 2015; Nelissen and Meijers 2011; Scott, Mende,
To address this question, the present research takes a and Bolton 2013). Extending that literature, we identify
closer look at women’s unique priorities in the romantic re- contexts in which luxury consumption can be used strategi-
lationship formation stage. We propose that in the romantic cally to detract an undesirable audience. Next, we outline
formation stage, individuals need to narrow down their the theoretical foundations of our research.
mating options and deter undesirable pursuers. We concep-
tualize this as a mate screening motive. There are reasons THEORETICAL FOUNDATIONS
to expect this motive to be more prevalent in women than
in men. First, a recent survey of 4,860 single U.S. adults Romantic Relationship Formation through
has revealed that whereas single men report having greater Consumption Choices
concerns about approaching and attracting potential part-
ners, single women focus on finding someone who meets Consumers use products in strategic ways to overcome
their mating standards (Pew Research Center 2020). These challenges, seize opportunities, and ensure favorable out-
survey results echo prior research in biology, anthropology, comes in romantic pursuits (Durante and Arsena 2015;
and evolutionary and social psychology arguing that select- Hasford et al. 2018; Van den Bergh et al. 2008). Prior re-
ing the right partner is more important and relevant for search examines how consumer behavior is shaped by a
women (vs. men), who assume the role of a chooser in general mating motive—a general desire to form a roman-
early romantic encounters (Buss and Schmitt 1993; tic relationship—such as when imagining a date with an at-
Fletcher et al. 2014; Saad, Eba, and Sejean 2009). We posit tractive individual or viewing pictures of potential partners
that women’s mate screening motive emerges in romantic (Griskevicius and Kenrick 2013; Griskevicius et al. 2007;
contexts where they garner a lot of attention from potential Janssens et al. 2011; Sundie et al. 2011). Previous work
partners. As such, this motive increases women’s con- suggests that a mating motive impacts the behaviors of
sumption of luxury goods as a way to signal their mating men and women differently. In particular, it leads men to
standards and deter undesirable pursuers. We document display luxury goods (Griskevicius et al. 2007; Janssens
this phenomenon across different romantic settings (e.g., et al. 2011; Otterbring et al. 2018; Sundie et al. 2011) to
online dating and singles’ events) and identify theoretically loudly signal their high social status, wealth, and ability to
grounded and practically relevant boundary conditions. provide resources for women (Buss 1989; Eastwick and
Our findings contribute to the marketing literature and Finkel 2008; Kenrick et al. 2001; Townsend and Levy
practice in meaningful ways. From a substantive stand- 1990). In contrast, women display prosocial behaviors
point, our results offer useful insights for managers of lux- (Griskevicius et al. 2007) and conformity with men’s pref-
ury brands and dating services on unique strategies that erences (Hasford et al. 2018) to signal their kindness, help-
they may utilize to market to women, who are a significant fulness, and agreeableness, which they believe men value
portion of their customer base. From a theoretical stand- (Campbell 2002; Hornsey et al. 2015). Thus, extant work
point, prior research presumes that the mating motive does presumes that women do not use luxury goods in their rela-
not impact women’s luxury consumption in the relation- tionship formation pursuits; this presumes no effect of the
ship formation stage. In contrast, we conceptualize a criti- mating motive on women’s luxury consumption
cal understudied mate screening motive in women’s (Griskevicius et al. 2007; Wang and Griskevicius 2014).
mating pursuits and show that it boosts women’s desire for In the present research, we challenge this assumption by
luxury products. Thus, our work advances the limited un- taking a closer look at distinct priorities that individuals
derstanding of how women use luxury goods for mating may have when pursuing a romantic relationship. We argue
purposes differently from men. that beyond the typical arousal and unilateral desire to find
By identifying the triggers, consequences, and bound- (any) mate, which characterizes the mating motive exam-
aries of the mate screening motive, our work also extends ined in prior research (Chen, Zheng, and Zhang 2016;
CHEN, WANG, AND ORDABAYEVA 305

Griskevicius et al. 2007; Li et al. 2012), individuals may of the mate screening motive for women’s consumption
have more specific priorities in some romantic contexts to behavior, as well as the relevant boundary conditions.
support their broader mating objective. Specifically,
enriching prior work on the broad conceptualization of Consequences of Mate Screening Motive for
consumers’ mating motive as a unilateral desire to procure Women’s Luxury Consumption
(any) mate, we posit that when individuals face excessive
mating options (e.g., when women receive a lot of roman- As argued above, in the early stages of romantic rela-
tic attention), they may prioritize narrowing down their ro- tionship formation, women often encounter situations in
mantic options and deterring undesirable pursuers. We which they receive a lot of attention from many potential
conceptualize this as a mate screening motive and propose pursuers. This may happen, for example, when there is a
that it has unique implications for consumption vis-a-vis higher proportion of single men relative to single women
the more general mating motive considered in previous in the environment due to a male-biased sex ratio (Durante

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jcr/article/50/2/303/6650085 by guest on 22 August 2023


studies (Griskevicius et al. 2007; Wang and Griskevicius et al. 2012). In such situations, women’s mate screening
2014). motive would be activated and they would need to narrow
down their romantic options (Regan 1998; Stone,
Women’s Mate Screening Motive during Shackelford, and Buss 2007). However, faced with exces-
sive mating options, women may experience choice over-
Romantic Relationship Formation load (Chernev, Böckenholt, and Goodman 2015) and find
Although selecting a suitable romantic partner who it difficult to identify the most suitable partner (D’Angelo
meets an individual’s expectations is a prevalent challenge and Toma 2017), due to the amount of time and effort that
in relationship formation (Buss and Schmitt 1993), it is carefully vetting each option would require (Chernev et al.
more relevant for women than men based on parental in- 2015). As a result, women may employ tactics to aid the
vestment theory from evolutionary psychology (Trivers screening process. For example, they may prioritize certain
1972). The main proposition of this theory is that the sex characteristics of men over others and form ranking stand-
that invests more in offspring is the chooser during rela- ards to facilitate the selection process (Li et al. 2002).
tionship formation, whereas the sex that invests less is the Faced with limited time for screening, women may focus
pursuer and has to compete more for mating opportunities. on fewer characteristics of men in order to increase screen-
Because women typically invest more time and effort in ing efficiency (Hou, Shu, and Fang 2022).
parenting (childbearing and childcare) (Buss and Schmitt We propose that as one strategy to screen out and deter
1993; Kenrick et al. 1993; Saad et al. 2009; Trivers 1972) undesirable pursuers, women may signal their high mating
across cultures (Katz-Wise, Priess, and Hyde 2010; Li, standards by engaging in luxury consumption. We inte-
Haws, and Griskevicius 2019) and throughout history grate two streams of research to support this proposition.
(Ferriman, Lubinski, and Benbow 2009), they cannot af- First, evolutionary psychology highlights the priority that
ford to mate with just anyone and are therefore prone to be women assign to the economic standing and resources of
the chooser in the mating market (Fletcher et al. 2014; potential romantic partners. Studies document that, regard-
Saad et al. 2009). less of a woman’s own income and occupation, she will
As choosers, women tend to be more passive in early ro- consider men’s financial capability to be one of the most
mantic encounters and often receive more mating opportu- important criteria when evaluating romantic prospects
nities than men (Clark, Shaver, and Abrahams 1999). This (Buss 1989; Buss and Schmitt 1993; Campbell and
presence of abundant mating opportunities requires women Fletcher 2015; Kenrick et al. 1990; Townsend 1989;
to be more efficient at assessing their mating options and Walter et al. 2020; Wiederman and Allgeier 1992).
to be more prudent and selective when starting a romantic Second, luxury research highlights two characteristics of
relationship. Evidence consistently suggests that, compared luxury products that make them well suited for signaling
to men, women invest more effort in searching for informa- women’s high standards, particularly in the financial do-
tion about potential partners (Saad et al. 2009), they are main. Namely, luxury products have a high price tag and,
more sensitive to negative information about prospective thereby, embody owners’ high income and affluence (Goor
partners (Saad and Gill 2014), and they are less interested et al. 2020; Ordabayeva and Chandon 2011; Rucker and
in further contact with potential pursuers (Fletcher et al. Galinsky 2008). Luxury products are also distant from the
2014; Perilloux, Easton, and Buss 2012). In sum, we posit masses and exclusive at multiple levels (Berger and Ward
that the mate screening motive—that is, the desire to nar- 2010; Han, Nunes, and Drèze 2010), including in selective
row down potential romantic options and deter undesirable production processes, customer service, and distribution
pursuers—is particularly relevant to women compared to strategies that limit product supply to grant access only to a
men in early stages of romantic relationship formation. A limited segment of consumers who can afford it (Dion and
pilot study (web appendix B) provided empirical support Borraz 2017; Han et al. 2010; Wang 2021). Thus, by sport-
for this proposition. Next, we delineate the consequences ing a luxury product in a mating context, a woman may
306 JOURNAL OF CONSUMER RESEARCH

signal that she is highly selective (i.e., “out of your league” abundant options (e.g., a large assortment, excessive
for the masses of suitors). That is, because consumption search) can increase consumers’ uncertainty about quality,
choices often indicate what consumers value, particularly and lower their confidence in the quality of their choices
in symbolic domains (Escalas and Bettman 2005), wom- (Chernev 2003; Diehl 2005; Malhotra 1982). Therefore,
en’s luxury consumption may signal to potential mates the we reason that if the quality of the mating pool is known to
woman’s prioritization and expectations of high financial be high, then the need to further eliminate pursuers who do
capability in a romantic partner and, hence, their availabil- not meet the woman’s standards should be attenuated.
ity only to a limited set of pursuers who are financially Accordingly, women’s need to engage in luxury consump-
sound. Formally, we hypothesize: tion as a screening tool is reduced.
H1: Activating a strong (vs. weak) mate screening motive H3: When the quality of men in the mating pool is known
increases women’s desire for luxury consumption. to be high (vs. mixed), the effect of a strong (vs. weak) mate

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jcr/article/50/2/303/6650085 by guest on 22 August 2023


screening motive on women’s desire for luxury consumption
is attenuated.
Boundary Conditions
We propose three theory-driven boundary conditions Selection Mindset. We posit that the mate screening
that illuminate the mechanism through which a mate motive assumes that women prioritize deterring or elimi-
screening motive influences women’s luxury consumption nating certain (undesirable) pursuers when faced with
in the relationship formation stage. Specifically, we exam- abundant choices. This logic of elimination embodies a re-
ine the moderating effects of the presence of an external jection mindset that emerges in certain decision contexts.
screening tool, the quality of men in the mating environ- In particular, decision-making research suggests that con-
ment, and the salience of a selection (vs. rejection) sumers tend to adopt a rejection mindset when choosing
mindset. from large choice sets (Bettman, Luce, and Payne 1998).
We propose that mating contexts featuring excessive mat-
Presence of an External Screening Tool. If the ratio- ing options may naturally induce such a mindset in women
nale for women’s luxury consumption in the relationship and that luxury consumption is their preferred option elimi-
formation stage is to screen potentially undesirable pur- nation tool. However, consumers may also be externally
suers, then when unsuitable mating options are eliminated prompted to employ a different mindset when choosing
externally, the need to further engage in screening through from large option sets. Specifically, externally inducing a
luxury consumption should be attenuated. Consumers have
selection mindset by asking consumers to identify an op-
opportunities to leverage such external screening tools in
tion that they like (vs. a rejection mindset whereby they
today’s dating marketplace. For example, a popular dating
identify options that they dislike) can create deviations in
app, Tinder, offers filters based on dating candidates’ de-
consumers’ evaluations of available options and yield dis-
mographic characteristics. Some dating services specifi-
tinct behaviors and outcomes (Ross, Meloy, and Bolton
cally screen users on their financial capability. For
2021). In the romantic context, externally prompting a se-
example, the League platform scrutinizes applicants’
lection mindset in women may lead them to exhibit behav-
LinkedIn profiles (e.g., education, occupation) and individ-
iors focused on keeping desirable options vis-a-vis a
ually screens applicants before deciding whether or not to
rejection mindset, which prompts the elimination of unde-
accept them. We propose that in the presence of such exter-
sirable partners (screening). Because luxury consumption
nal screening tools, the focal effect of the mate screening
helps screen men who do not meet women’s standards, we
motive on women’s luxury consumption will be
predict that in contexts that activate a mate screening mo-
attenuated.
tive, externally inducing a selection (vs. rejection) mindset
H2: When external screening tools are present (vs. absent), may attenuate women’s interest in luxury consumption.
the effect of a strong (vs. weak) mate screening motive on
H4: When a selection (vs. rejection) mindset is induced, the
women’s desire for luxury consumption is attenuated.
effect of a strong (vs. weak) mate screening motive on wom-
en’s desire for luxury consumption is attenuated.
Quality of Men. When faced with many mating oppor-
tunities during early romantic encounters, women need to
screen pursuers because it is unclear to them which men OVERVIEW OF STUDIES
meet their mating standards and which men do not. In other
words, women engage in the screening process to deter un- Six studies and two follow-ups tested our theory using
desirable pursuers, as they may assume that the abundant hypothetical and consequential choices in the field and in
mating pool includes candidates of mixed quality (Lenton controlled settings across different cultures (U.S. and
and Francesconi 2011). This assumption aligns with China) and populations, including actual single female cus-
decision-making studies suggesting that the presence of tomers of a real-world dating service. Study 1
CHEN, WANG, AND ORDABAYEVA 307

demonstrated that a stronger mate screening motive among online messaging platform that the agency frequently uses
actual female customers of a real-world dating service led to contact their subscribers. Women were asked to provide
to women’s stronger preference for luxury brands. Study 2 feedback about a future singles event for Chinese
tested hypothesis 1 in a controlled lab setting and showed Valentine’s Day. A total of 172 women (Mage ¼ 31.74,
the proposed effect is driven by the positive effect on lux- SD ¼ 7.88) completed the study in exchange for a small
ury consumption of a stronger mate screening motive, monetary compensation (10 RMB/about $1.40). The study
rather than the negative effect of a weaker mate screening used a two-cell (mate screening motive: strong vs. weak)
motive. Studies 3–5 explored the three theory-driven between-subjects design.
boundary conditions (hypotheses 2–4). They showed that
Motive Manipulation. Participants read a description of
the effect is attenuated when external screening tools are
an offline singles event on Chinese Valentine’s Day. To in-
present (vs. absent) (study 3), the quality of men is high
duce a strong (vs. weak) mate screening motive, we varied
(vs. mixed) (study 4), and a selection (vs. rejection) mind-

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jcr/article/50/2/303/6650085 by guest on 22 August 2023


the sex ratio (Durante et al. 2012; Griskevicius et al. 2012)
set is externally induced (study 5). The studies also com-
and the communication rules for attendees at the described
pared the role of the screening motive within a mating vis-
a-vis a non-mating context and showed that the proposed event. In the strong mate screening motive condition, par-
ticipants read that there would be two men for every
effect of screening on luxury consumption is unique to
mating environments. Finally, to reconcile our propositions woman at the event, that during the event men could send
with prior work on the link between women’s mate guard- one message to the three women in whom they were the
ing motives and luxury preferences (Wang and most interested, and that women would be obligated to re-
Griskevicius 2014), study 6 contrasted the effect on the de- spond. This indicated that each woman would likely re-
sire for luxury consumption of single women’s mate ceive attention from many men and would be obliged to
screening motive in the relationship formation stage vis-a- communicate with each pursuer, which should trigger a
vis non-single women’s mate guarding motive in the rela- stronger mate screening motive. In the weak mate screen-
tionship maintenance stage. ing motive condition, participants read that there would be
Following prior research on romantic relationships and one man for every woman at the event, that men could
consumer behavior, all studies except study 6 a priori send a message to one woman in whom they were the most
recruited heterosexual women, who have been the focus of interested, and that women could choose not to respond.
relevant theories, and thus enabled the tests of our theoreti- This indicated that, in this dating context, women would
cal predictions. In study 6, we included all women and receive less attention from men, which should trigger a
measured their sexual orientation instead of a priori screen- weaker mate screening motive.
ing them. We found that the results did not change if we Luxury Consumption Measures. Two measures cap-
excluded non-heterosexual participants from the analyses tured participants’ preference for luxury consumption.
(web appendix H). In the General Discussion, we discuss First, participants indicated their preference for luxury (vs.
the potential theoretical implications of sexual orientation non-luxury) brands to be featured in a promotion for a new
as an important direction for future research. shopping website that targeted their segment. Participants
read that the dating agency was considering hosting a
STUDY 1: PREFERENCE FOR LUXURY Valentine’s Day event sponsored by a new shopping web-
CONSUMPTION AMONG FEMALE site. Participants indicated which type of brands they
CUSTOMERS OF A DATING SERVICE would be more interested in seeing featured in the web-
site’s advertisements (1 ¼ high-quality regular brands,
Study 1 examined the effect of a mate screening motive 7 ¼ luxury designer brands).
on single women’s preferences for luxury products (hy- Participants then made an incentive-compatible choice.
pothesis 1) in a field setting. We collaborated with a dating They were told that they would be entered into a lottery for
agency in China that works with over 40,000 heterosexual a 1,000 RMB (about $150) gift card as an additional re-
singles to help them in their quest for love. The agency ward for participation. Participants indicated which type of
organizes singles events in exchange for a membership fee. brand they would like the gift card to be from if they won
In this study, we induced a strong (vs. weak) mate screen- the lottery (1 ¼ a luxury brand, such as Chanel, Dior, or
ing motive by varying the number of potential mating Louis Vuitton; vs. 0 ¼ a mid-level brand, such as
options that women faced in a dating context. Maybelline, Mo&Co., or Lily). At the end of the study, one
participant was randomly selected to receive the gift card.
Method
Participants and Design. The study was conducted 1
Results and Discussion
week before Chinese Valentine’s Day (July 7). Dating Pre-Test. A pre-test (N ¼ 150 females, Mage ¼ 23.04,
agency staff approached female subscribers through an SD ¼ 2.89) confirmed the effectiveness of the
308 JOURNAL OF CONSUMER RESEARCH

manipulation. The strong (vs. weak) mate screening motive and spending time with someone to whom they felt imme-
manipulation bolstered the mate screening motive (Mstrong diately attracted at the event. In the non-mating control
¼ 5.48, SD ¼ 0.88 vs. Mweak ¼ 4.92, SD ¼ 1.15, F(1, 148) condition, in line with extant work (Griskevicius, Cialdini,
¼ 11.03, p ¼ .001, gP2 ¼ 0.069). (See web appendix C for and Kenrick 2006; Griskevicius et al. 2007), participants
full details.) read about wanting to make new friends, going to a movie
with potential candidates, losing the tickets, and missing
Luxury Consumption. Participants expressed a signifi-
the movie. This condition sought to rule out the role of
cantly stronger preference for luxury brands in the strong
negative mood.
(vs. weak) mate screening motive condition, both for the
website promotion (Mstrong ¼ 4.47, SD ¼ 1.96 vs. Mweak ¼ Luxury Consumption Measures. Participants read that
3.72, SD ¼ 1.89, F(1, 170) ¼ 6.53, p ¼ .012, gP2 ¼ 0.037) 1 week after the described event, they would be attending a
and the incentive-compatible gift card choice (Mstrong vs. similar event for which they were buying an outfit. Two
Mweak ¼ 48.72% vs. 32.98%, v2 (1) ¼ 4.40, p ¼ .043, u ¼

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jcr/article/50/2/303/6650085 by guest on 22 August 2023


items captured their interest in luxury consumption. One
0.160). These findings provided support for the effect (hy- item asked participants to indicate whether they would
pothesis 1) in the context of consequential and hypothetical shop for an outfit at a mall mostly featuring luxurious de-
preferences for luxury brands in a population of single signer brands (e.g., Louis Vuitton, Burberry) or a mall
women who were actually seeking to form a romantic mostly featuring high-quality everyday fashion brands
relationship. (e.g., ASOS, Zara) (1 ¼ high-quality everyday brands,
7 ¼ luxury designer brands). The other item asked partici-
STUDY 2: COMPARISON TO A NON- pants to indicate whether their outfit for the event would
MATING CONTROL feature more items from luxury designer brands versus af-
fordable high-quality brands (1 ¼ definitely more from af-
Study 2 had two objectives. First, the study used a con- fordable high-quality everyday brands; 7 ¼ definitely more
trolled experimental setting to compare the effect of a from luxury designer brands).
strong (vs. weak) mate screening motive to a non-mating
control condition (hypothesis 1). This allowed us to test Results
whether the focal phenomenon emerges because the strong
Pre-Test. A pre-test (N ¼ 150 females, Mage ¼ 27.14,
mate screening motive increases women’s luxury con-
SD ¼ 11.07) confirmed the effectiveness of our manipula-
sumption or because the weak mate screening motive
tion. Participants had a significantly stronger mate screen-
decreases it. Second, the study tested one potential alterna-
ing motive in the strong mate screening motive condition
tive explanation: that attention from too many men
than in the other two conditions (Mstrong ¼ 5.64, SD ¼ 1.05
increases luxury consumption because it induces negative
vs. Mweak ¼ 4.93, SD ¼ 1.13, t(147) ¼ 2.81, p ¼ .006,
mood (Makkar and Yap 2018). To consider this possibility,
Cohen’s d ¼ 0.55; Mstrong ¼ 5.64, SD ¼ 1.05 vs. Mcontrol ¼
the non-mating control condition also induced negative
2.34, SD ¼ 1.63, t(147) ¼ 12.86, p < .001, Cohen’s
mood.
d ¼ 2.58). This confirmed the success of our manipulation
in triggering the mate screening motive. In addition, the
Method strong mate screening motive condition and the non-
Participants and Design. A total of 302 single hetero- mating control condition did not differ from each other in
sexual women (Mage ¼ 27.55, SD ¼ 10.45) participated in negative mood (Mstrong ¼ 3.05, SD ¼ 1.22 vs. Mcontrol ¼
the study on Prolific Academic. The study used a three-cell 2.87, SD ¼ 1.16, t(147) ¼ 0.84, p ¼ .401, Cohen’s d ¼
(mate screening motive: strong vs. weak vs. non-mating 0.17). This minimized the role of negative mood as a po-
control) between-subjects design. tential alternative explanation. (Web appendix E provides
the full details of the pre-test.)
Motive Manipulation. In line with prior marketing
studies that used scenario-based mating manipulations Luxury Consumption. An analysis of variance
(Dahl et al. 2009; Wang and Griskevicius 2014; Yang et al. (ANOVA) on preference for the luxury (vs. non-luxury)
2019), we asked participants to read and visualize a sce- mall with motive as a fixed factor (mate screening motive:
nario that induced a certain motive (web appendix E). In strong vs. weak vs. non-mating control) revealed a signifi-
the strong mate screening motive condition, participants cant effect (F(2, 299) ¼ 7.14, p ¼ .001, g2p ¼ 0.046).
read about being single, attending a singles event, and re- Women had a significantly stronger preference for the lux-
ceiving an overwhelming amount of attention from men at ury mall in the strong mate screening motive condition
the event. In the weak mate screening motive condition, (Mstrong ¼ 3.13, SD ¼ 1.91) than in the weak mate screen-
participants read a scenario from prior research ing motive condition (Mweak ¼ 2.41, SD ¼ 1.76, t(299) ¼
(Griskevicius et al. 2007; Wang and Griskevicius 2014) 2.84, p ¼ .005, Cohen’s d ¼ 0.41) or the non-mating con-
about being single, attending a singles event, and meeting trol condition (Mcontrol ¼ 2.25, SD ¼ 1.63, t(299) ¼ 3.57,
CHEN, WANG, AND ORDABAYEVA 309

p < .001, Cohen’s d ¼ 0.50). This was consistent with hy- reasons. First, friendships are less exclusive than romantic
pothesis 1. There was no difference between the weak relationships as romantic relationships are often considered
mate screening motive and the non-mating control condi- monogamous while friendships are not. That is, when
tions (t(299) ¼ 0.65, p ¼ .516, Cohen’s d ¼ 0.09). women receive a lot of attention from friends, they do not
An ANOVA on preference for luxury (vs. non-luxury) necessarily need to narrow down their friend options; in-
items in the outfit also revealed a significant effect (F(2, stead, they can make friends with as many people as they
299) ¼ 6.64, p ¼ .002, g2p ¼ 0.042). Women had a signifi- want. Second, financial capability might not be as impor-
cantly stronger preference for luxury items in the strong tant in selecting a friend as it is in selecting a date
mate screening motive condition (Mstrong ¼ 3.15, (Sprecher and Regan 2002). Thus, luxury products are less
SD ¼ 1.72) than in the weak mate screening motive condi- likely to be used as a signaling tool in non-romantic friend-
tion (Mweak ¼ 2.53, SD ¼ 1.74, t(299) ¼ 2.66, p ¼ .008, ships. Therefore, this study design would enable us to as-
Cohen’s d ¼ 0.38) or the non-mating control (Mcontrol ¼ sess the uniqueness of the focal phenomenon within

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jcr/article/50/2/303/6650085 by guest on 22 August 2023


2.36, SD ¼ 1.46, t(299) ¼ 3.45, p ¼ .001, Cohen’s d ¼ romantic contexts.
0.48). The weak mate screening motive and the control Our prediction is not only theoretically relevant, but it is
conditions did not significantly differ from each other also practically important. Indeed, tools that ease the pro-
(t(299) ¼ 0.71, p ¼ .479, Cohen’s d ¼ 0.10). cess of screening potential mating candidates (e.g., add-on
features on dating platforms that limit the audience that
Discussion can view subscribers’ profiles) are increasingly popular
and prevalent (Bruch, Feinberg, and Lee 2016; Wolfe
Study 2 included a control condition to corroborate how 2020).
the strong (vs. weak) mate screening motive drives wom-
en’s luxury consumption to facilitate their romantic pur-
Method
suits. The results confirmed that the effect emerges
because a strong mate screening motive increases women’s The study used a 2 (motive: strong mate screening mo-
luxury consumption, and not because a weak mate screen- tive vs. non-mating control)  2 (external screening tool:
ing motive reduces it. Importantly, study 2 minimized the present vs. absent) between-subjects design.
role of a salient alternative explanation—that is, negative Participants. We recruited 395 heterosexual female
mood. college students (Mage ¼ 22.11, SD ¼ 2.08) for a small pay-
Nevertheless, one might argue that another alternative ment through the Data Market application on Credamo in
explanation could be that the mate screening motive ma- China (https://www.credamo.com/#/). Credamo is a profes-
nipulation involves receiving a lot of attention from others, sional data collection platform. University students are of-
and it is the feeling of being popular in general, rather than ten recruited for studies on romantic relationships, as
receiving attention from too many romantic pursuers, that romantic motives are typically salient in this population
increases women’s desire for luxury goods. Follow-up (Hasford et al. 2018; Yang et al. 2019).
study A (web appendix D) tested this possibility using a
similar three-cell design as in study 2 and a non-mating Motive Manipulation. All participants read that the
control condition that induced the feeling of popularity. study examined how consumers manage their profiles on
The results supported the role of mate screening, rather online networking platforms. In the strong mate screening
than the general feeling of popularity, in driving the effect. motive condition, participants imagined that they were sin-
Studies 3–5 build on these findings to test three theoreti- gle and were considering using a dating platform to find a
cally and practically relevant moderators (hypotheses 2–4). romantic partner. In the non-mating control condition, par-
ticipants read that they were considering using a friendship
platform to find friends. In both conditions, participants
STUDY 3: MODERATING EFFECT OF were presented with a list of characteristics (age, height,
EXTERNAL SCREENING weight, occupation, hobbies, favorite brands; see web ap-
Study 3 examined the first theory-driven boundary con- pendix F for the scenario) that they could consider posting
dition: the presence of external screening tools. We pre- in their profile on the platform. All participants read that
dicted (hypothesis 2) that the presence (vs. absence) of the platform was very popular (with over 1.5 million users)
external screening tools would attenuate women’s need for and that they received a lot of attention from other mem-
bers on the platform (i.e., a high number of views, likes,
further screening and reduce the effect of the mate screen-
and comments on their profile). This description was used
ing motive on luxury consumption. We compared this ef-
to keep the general feeling of being popular comparable
fect of screening tools across mating and non-mating
across the mating and non-mating conditions.
(friendship) settings. Specifically, we did not expect the
presence of external screening tools to produce an attenua- External Screening Manipulation. In the external
tion effect in non-romanting friendship settings for two screening present condition, participants further read that a
310 JOURNAL OF CONSUMER RESEARCH

new platform feature enabled users to screen who could FIGURE 1


view their profile. Specifically, women could set up filters
(e.g., based on the viewers’ occupation, age, and height) to MODERATING EFFECT OF EXTERNAL SCREENING (STUDY 3)
allow only certain candidates to view their profile.
However, in the external screening absent condition, par-
ticipants read that all users could view their profile (web
appendix F).
Luxury Consumption Measures. Participants were told
that they had decided to become a member of the described
platform and were choosing what to display in their profile
on the platform. Embedded within the list of characteristics

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jcr/article/50/2/303/6650085 by guest on 22 August 2023


were fashion, beauty, and car brands that women could
choose to feature as their favorite brands. In the fashion
category, the description of their characteristics stated that
they liked affordable brands, such as Zara, as well as high-
end luxury brands, such as Gucci. In the beauty category,
the description stated that they liked affordable brands,
significant main effect of motive (F(1, 391) ¼ 7.85, p ¼
such as Maybelline, as well as high-end luxury brands,
.005, gp2 ¼ 0.020) and a non-significant main effect of ex-
such as Dior. In the car category, the description stated that
ternal screening (F(1, 391) ¼ 2.59, p ¼ .109, gp2 ¼ 0.007).
they liked everyday car brands, such as Honda, as well as
As a robustness check, we also analyzed the data using a
high-end luxury brands, such as BMW. Participants chose
Poisson regression (to accommodate the count nature of
whether to display luxury or non-luxury brands (dichoto-
the dependent variable). The results and conclusions
mous: 1 ¼ luxury, 0 ¼ non-luxury) in each of the three
remained unchanged. (Web appendix F provides the full
product categories.
details.)
Results Discussion
Pre-Test. A pre-test (N ¼ 200 females, Mage ¼ 21.71, Study 3 demonstrated the first boundary condition of the
SD ¼ 2.14) confirmed the effectiveness of the manipula- focal effect: the presence (vs. absence) of external screen-
tions. The presence (vs. absence) of external screening ing tools. When women had other ways to screen out and
tools significantly lowered the mate screening motive deter unwanted romantic pursuers, they no longer engaged
(Mpresent ¼ 5.38, SD ¼ 0.98 vs. Mabsent ¼ 6.11, SD ¼ 0.60, in luxury consumption. This further supported the screen-
F(1, 196) ¼ 15.31, p < .001, gp2 ¼ 0.072) in the strong ing function of luxury consumption in women’s romantic
mate screening motive condition. However, the presence pursuits.
of external screening tools did not impact the mate screen-
ing motive in the non-mating control condition (Mpresent ¼
4.99, SD ¼ 0.88, Mabsent ¼ 4.80, SD ¼ 1.15, F(1, 196) ¼ STUDY 4: MODERATING EFFECT OF
1.11, p ¼ .293, gp2 ¼ 0.006; motive  tools interaction: QUALITY OF MEN
F(1, 196) ¼ 12.40, p ¼ .001, gp2 ¼ 0.060). (Web appendix
Study 4 examined the second theory-driven boundary
F provides the full details.)
condition of the effect: the quality of men (hypothesis 3).
Luxury Consumption. We used the total number that We examined the role of audience quality across mating
luxury brands were chosen over non-luxury brands across versus non-mating online platform contexts to increase the
the three product categories (count: from 0 to 3) as the de- practical relevance and implications of our findings.
pendent measure. A 2  2 ANOVA revealed a significant Indeed, online dating platforms feature a variety of audien-
interaction effect (F(1, 391) ¼ 8.51, p ¼ .004, gp2 ¼ 0.021; ces ranging from highly specialized (e.g., Elite Singles and
figure 1). As predicted (hypothesis 2), in the strong mate The League, which only allow certain segments of con-
screening motive condition, women were less likely to dis- sumers to join) to less specialized and diverse
play luxury brands when external screening tools were pre- (e.g., OkCupid and Bumble, which welcome everybody).
sent (Mpresent ¼ 1.15, SD ¼ 1.04) rather than absent We predicted that in a romantic context (i.e., a dating
(Mabsent ¼ 1.78, SD ¼ 1.06, F(1, 391) ¼ 16.07, p < .001, platform), when platform viewers’ (i.e., men’s) quality
gp2 ¼ 0.039). In the non-mating control condition, the ef- (i.e., financial capability) was known to be high
fect of external screening was non-significant (Mpresent ¼ (vs. mixed), women would be less likely to engage in lux-
1.29, SD ¼ 1.06 vs. Mabsent ¼ 1.28, SD ¼ 1.21, F(1, 391) ¼ ury consumption due to their attenuated motive to screen
0.01, p ¼ .934, gp2 < 0.001). The results also revealed a pursuers (hypothesis 4). In contrast, in a non-mating
CHEN, WANG, AND ORDABAYEVA 311

context (i.e., a social friendship platform), viewers’ (i.e., SD ¼ 0.62, F(1, 188) ¼ 6.32, p ¼ .013, gp2 ¼ 0.033), but it
users’) quality should not impact luxury consumption in did not impact women’s mate screening motive in the non-
this way because financial capability is less critical when mating control condition (Mhigh ¼ 4.85, SD ¼ 1.14 vs.
choosing friends than romantic partners (Sprecher and Mmixed ¼ 4.76, SD ¼ 0.94, F(1, 188) ¼ 0.29, p ¼ .590, gp2
Regan 2002). It is possible that high (vs. mixed) quality of ¼ 0.002; motive  quality interaction: F(1, 188) ¼ 4.66, p
the audience might even increase luxury consumption in ¼ .032, gp2 ¼ 0.024). (Web appendix G provides the full
non-romantic friendship settings, as it might motivate details.)
women to fit in with the high-quality audience through
Luxury Consumption. As in study 3, we used the total
high spending (Mead et al. 2011).
number of luxury brands chosen across categories (count:
from 0 to 3) as the dependent measure. A 2  2 ANOVA
Method revealed a significant interaction effect (F(1, 396) ¼ 47.07,

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jcr/article/50/2/303/6650085 by guest on 22 August 2023


The study used a 2 (motive: strong mate screening mo- p < .001, gp2 ¼ 0.106; figure 2).
tive vs. non-mating control)  2 (audience quality: high vs. As predicted (hypothesis 4), in the high mate screening
mixed) between-subjects design. motive condition, women were less likely to display luxury
brands when audience quality was high (Mhigh ¼ 1.43,
Participants. We recruited 400 heterosexual female
SD ¼ 1.04) rather than mixed (Mmixed ¼ 1.99, SD ¼ 1.10,
university students (Mage ¼ 21.65, SD ¼ 2.63) for a small
F(1, 396) ¼ 13.66, p < .001, gp2 ¼ 0.033). Interestingly, in
payment through the Data Market application on Credamo
the non-mating control condition, the effect was reversed:
in China.
women were more likely to display luxury brands when au-
Motive Manipulation. The study used the same manip- dience quality was high (Mhigh ¼ 2.15, SD ¼ 1.04) rather
ulation of motives as in study 3. As in study 3, participants than mixed (Mmixed ¼ 1.24, SD ¼ 1.11, F(1, 396) ¼ 36.08,
read about managing their profile on an online platform p < .001, gp2 ¼ 0.083). The results yielded non-significant
(dating vs. social), and they saw a list of characteristics (in- main effects of motive (F(1, 396) ¼ 0.02, p ¼ .889, gp2 <
cluding favorite brands; web appendix F) that they could 0.001) and audience quality (F(1, 396) ¼ 2.67, p ¼ .103,
include in their profile. As in study 3, participants also read gp2 ¼ 0.007). Once again, a robustness check using a
that the platform was very popular (with over 1.5 million Poisson regression yielded similar results. (Web appendix
users) and that they received a lot of attention from G provides the full details.)
viewers.
Audience Quality Manipulation. In the high audience Discussion
quality condition, participants read that, based on their re- Study 4 provided further evidence of the deterring role
search, platform users were of high quality and met their that luxury consumption serves in the romantic context for
standards for a romantic partner (vs. friend) (i.e., had women. As predicted, when the quality of men in the mat-
steady jobs in growing industries and higher education). In ing pool was known to be high, women’s need to screen
the mixed quality condition, participants read that the plat- pursuers by engaging in luxury consumption was attenu-
form users were of mixed quality: some users satisfied ated. Interestingly, the study also revealed women’s stron-
their standards and others did not (i.e., some had steady ger preference for luxury consumption in a friendship
jobs in growing industries and higher education, whereas context featuring a high (vs. mixed) quality audience.
others had unstable jobs in declining industries and basic Although this finding is not related to our theorizing or the
education). romantic mating phenomenon that we examine, it is consis-
Luxury Consumption Measures. As in study 3, partici- tent with prior work on social norms (Mead et al. 2011).
pants were asked to choose which personal characteristics
they would like to display in their online profile. Luxury FIGURE 2
consumption was captured by women’s choice to display
luxury (vs. non-luxury) brands as their favorites in the MODERATING EFFECT OF AUDIENCE QUALITY (STUDY 4)
fashion, beauty, and car categories.

Results
Pre-Test. A pre-test (N ¼ 192 females, Mage ¼ 21.86,
SD ¼ 3.17) confirmed the effectiveness of the manipula-
tions. High (vs. mixed) audience quality lowered women’s
mate screening motive in the high mate screening motive
condition (Mhigh ¼ 5.25, SD ¼ 0.73 vs. Mmixed ¼ 5.71,
312 JOURNAL OF CONSUMER RESEARCH

Specifically, a high (vs. mixed) quality social environment instructed to narrow down the choice set by identifying the
may create a stronger expectation for individuals’ high- six products that they liked the most. In contrast, partici-
status consumption due to a desire to affiliate with and con- pants in the rejection condition were instructed to narrow
form to the elite audience, resulting in greater luxury down the set by identifying the six products that they liked
displays. This further underscores the uniqueness of our the least. Following Ross et al. (2021), to check the effec-
predictions to the mating context. Luxury consumption tiveness of the manipulation, participants indicated to what
may be used to affiliate with others in the friendship con- extent they focused on selecting the products that they
text; however, it serves the reverse function of deterring wanted versus eliminating the products that they did not
undesirable pursuers in the romantic context at the early want (1 ¼ “definitely the products I wanted to select,” 7 ¼
stages of romantic relationship formation. Study 5 reports “definitely the products I wanted to get rid of”).
an additional test of the process underlying the phenome- Brand Recognizability Manipulation. After completing
non by examining the moderating effect of activating a se-

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jcr/article/50/2/303/6650085 by guest on 22 August 2023


the mindset manipulation, participants were asked to recall
lection (vs. rejection) mindset in women during their the scenario about the singles event that they had read at
mating pursuits. the beginning of the study. They read that the same event
would be held soon and they needed to choose an outfit.
STUDY 5: MODERATING EFFECT OF A Participants read that they were choosing between clothing
SELECTION MINDSET items from luxury brands versus non-luxury brands. In the
high (vs. low) recognizability condition, they read that a re-
Study 5 had two objectives. First, it examined the third cent consumer survey had shown that the clothing brands
theory-driven boundary condition of the focal effect: acti- they were considering were widely (vs. barely) recogniz-
vation of a selection (vs. rejection) mindset. We predicted able to men. Participants also read that all options that they
(hypothesis 4) that because the mate screening motive pri- were considering were well-designed, high-quality, and
oritizes deterring or eliminating undesirable options, which tasteful and that they looked good in all of them.
embodies a rejection mindset, then activating a selection
mindset that prioritizes choosing desirable options should Luxury Consumption Measure. As the dependent mea-
attenuate the effect. Second, this study tested our key as- sure, participants indicated their preference for wearing
sumption: luxury consumption is utilized by women as a luxury versus non-luxury clothes to the event (1 ¼
signaling tool targeted at men. If luxury consumption “definitely an outfit from non-luxury brands,” 7 ¼
serves as a communication tool, as we theorize, then “definitely an outfit from luxury brands”).
women should utilize it less when luxury products are
unrecognizable by men. Results
Mindset Manipulation Check. We conducted an
Method ANOVA on the manipulation check item with mindset,
The study used a 2 (mindset: selection vs. rejection)  2 brand recognizability, and their interaction as fixed factors.
(brand recognizability: high vs. low) between-subjects There was a significant main effect of mindset (F(1, 483)
design. ¼ 358.91, p < .001, gp2 ¼ 0.426): participants in the selec-
tion (vs. rejection) condition thought less about the items
Participants. A total of 487 heterosexual single female they wanted to eliminate and more about items they wanted
U.S. participants (Mage ¼ 40.75, SD ¼ 13.27) completed to select (Mselection ¼ 1.94, SD ¼ 1.60 vs. Mrejection ¼ 5.06,
the study on Amazon Mechanical Turk (MTurk). All par- SD ¼ 2.01). This confirmed the effectiveness of the manip-
ticipants read a scenario similar to study 2 that prompted ulation. The main effect of brand recognizability and its in-
the mate screening motive. They imagined moving to a teraction with mindset were non-significant (respectively,
new city, being single, attending a singles event, and re- F(1, 483) ¼ 0.88, p ¼ .348, gp2 ¼ 0.002 and F(1, 483) ¼
ceiving a lot of attention from men. 0.50, p ¼ .479, gp2 ¼ 0.001).
Mindset Manipulation. Participants were then directed Luxury Consumption. We performed a similar
to the second task involving a time delay that would osten- ANOVA on preferences for luxury (vs. non-luxury) brands.
sibly allow their memory of the first scenario to settle. This The interaction effect was significant (F(1, 483) ¼ 4.00,
second task, which was adapted from prior research (Ross p ¼ .046, gp2 ¼ 0.008). Specifically, high (vs. low) brand
et al. 2021), sought to activate a selection or a rejection recognizability boosted women’s preferences for
mindset. Specifically, participants saw lists of 12 products luxury over non-luxury brands when a rejection mindset
in three categories (fruit, beverage, and pizza on three sep- was activated (Mhigh recognizeability ¼ 4.81, SD ¼ 2.04 vs.
arate pages), and they were asked to select six products in Mlow recognizeability ¼ 3.92, SD ¼ 1.89, F(1, 483) ¼ 13.57,
each category. Consistent with prior research (Ross et al. p < .001, gp2 ¼ 0.027). However, as predicted (hypothesis
2021), participants in the selection condition were 4) and shown in figure 3, activating a selection mindset
CHEN, WANG, AND ORDABAYEVA 313

FIGURE 3 screening motives and to reconcile this effect with findings


from prior literature.
MODERATING EFFECT OF A SELECTION MINDSET (STUDY 5)

STUDY 6: MATE SCREENING MOTIVE


VERSUS MATE GUARDING MOTIVE
Previous research shows that women pursuing a mate
guarding motive display luxury products to protect their
existing mate from female competitors (Wang and
Griskevicius 2014). Our research departs from this prior
finding in three ways. First, we examine women in the rela-

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jcr/article/50/2/303/6650085 by guest on 22 August 2023


tionship formation stage (i.e., who are single), whereas ex-
tant research examines women who are already in a
romantic relationship and, therefore, are in the relationship
maintenance stage (i.e., are not single). Second, the mean-
attenuated this effect (Mhigh recognizeability ¼ 4.15, SD ¼ ing of luxury consumption is unique in our research. Prior
1.70 vs. Mlow recognizeability ¼ 3.94, SD ¼ 1.85, F(1, 483) ¼ work on mate guarding views luxury as a signal of the de-
0.79, p ¼ .374, gP2 ¼ 0.002). In other words, a selection
votion of women’s existing partners. In contrast, we regard
(vs. rejection) mindset significantly lowered women’s pref-
luxury as a reflection of women’s own mating standards.
erences for luxury brands when the brands were recogniz-
Third, women’s luxury consumption for screening pur-
able (Mselection ¼ 4.15, SD ¼ 1.70 vs. Mrejection ¼ 4.81, SD
poses is directed at a distinct audience—prospective mates
¼ 2.04, F(1, 483) ¼ 7.72, p ¼ .006, gp2 ¼ 0.016).
However, mindset had no effect on luxury preferences (men)—versus competitors (other women) in previous
when the brands were unrecognizable (Mselection ¼ 3.94, mate guarding research.
SD ¼ 1.85 vs. Mrejection ¼ 3.92, SD ¼ 1.89, F(1, 483) ¼ To validate our unique perspective on women’s luxury
0.003, p ¼ .953, gp2 < 0.001). These results further sup- consumption for mate screening purposes, in study 6 we
ported the role of luxury consumption in women’s screen- contrasted the distinct functions (screening vs. guarding) of
ing attempts. Specifically, luxury functions is an luxury consumption for distinct segments of female con-
elimination tool, and it is meant to be a visible signal to sumers (single vs. non-single). Specifically, in we exam-
men. ined when single versus non-single women are more
There was a marginally significant main effect of mind- interested in luxury consumption. We predicted that a
set (F(1, 483) ¼ 3.67, p ¼ .056, gp2 ¼ 0.008): women had stronger mate screening motive would increase the desire
a marginally stronger preference for luxury brands in the for luxury consumption among single women, but that a
rejection (Mrejection ¼ 4.38, SD ¼ 2.02) than in the selection stronger mate guarding motive would increase the desire
(Mselection ¼ 4.04, SD ¼ 1.78) condition. There was also a for luxury consumption among non-single women.
significant main effect of brand recognizability (F(1, 483) To activate a mate screening motive versus a mate
¼ 10.55, p ¼ .001, gp2 ¼ 0.021): women had a stronger guarding motive, we varied the prevalence of single men
preference for luxury brands in the high recognizability versus women (i.e., the sex ratio) in the mating environ-
(Mhigh recognizeability ¼ 4.49, SD ¼ 1.90) than in the low rec- ment, which is consistent with prior literature (Durante
ognizability (Mlow recognizeability ¼ 3.93, SD ¼ 1.87) et al. 2012; Griskevicius et al. 2012). A female-biased sex
condition. ratio (i.e., greater prevalence of single women vs. men) in
the environment should activate non-single women’s mate
Discussion guarding motive (Wang and Griskevicius 2014), but a
Together, studies 3–5 uncovered the boundary condi- male-based sex ratio (i.e., greater prevalence of single men
tions of the focal phenomenon and supported the proposed vs. women) should activate single women’s mate screening
process of rejecting undesirable pursuers through modera- motive. We predicted that single women would engage in
tion. The final study 6 examines how this function of deter- luxury consumption more when the sex ratio in the mating
ring undesirable pursuers that women’s luxury environment was male- (vs. female-) biased due to the sa-
consumption performs under strong mate screening lience of their mate screening motive. In contrast, we
motives differs from luxury’s role in deterring female com- expected non-single women to engage in luxury consump-
petitors under strong mate guarding motives (Wang and tion more when the sex ratio in the mating environment
Griskevicius 2014). Study 6 thus seeks to further support was female- (vs. male-) biased due to the salience of their
the uniqueness and novelty of the proposed effect of mate mate guarding motive.
314 JOURNAL OF CONSUMER RESEARCH

Method Relationship Status Measure. At the end of the experi-


The study used a 2 (measured relationship status: single ment, along with demographics, participants indicated their
vs. non-single)  3 (manipulated sex ratio: male-biased vs. current relationship status. There were five options: “not
female-biased vs. control) between-subjects design. dating anyone” and “casually dating one or more people”
were classified as single (N ¼ 279, Mage ¼ 30.32,
Participants. A total of 559 U.S. women participated
SD ¼ 6.95); “in a committed relationship,” “long-term co-
in the study on MTurk. Twenty-three participants did not
habitation but not married,” and “married” were classified
disclose their relationship status, thus leaving 536 valid
as non-single (N ¼ 257, Mage ¼ 34.51, SD ¼ 5.72).
responses (Mage ¼ 32.33, SD ¼ 6.72). Because we assessed
women’s relationship status to identify the effect of the
mating motive, we did not use additional screeners to re- Results
cruit participants. Therefore, this study included women Pre-Test. A pre-test (N ¼ 615 females, Mage ¼ 32.88,

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jcr/article/50/2/303/6650085 by guest on 22 August 2023


from both heterosexual and non-heterosexual populations; SD ¼ 6.60) verified the effectiveness of our manipulations.
the results did not change when excluding non- The male-biased condition bolstered the mate screening
heterosexual participants (see web appendix H). We report motive over the female-biased condition (Mmale-biased ¼
the results without exclusions. 4.50, SD ¼ 1.47 vs. Mfemale-biased ¼ 3.75, SD ¼ 1.77, F(1,
Motive Manipulation. We relied on the combination of 609) ¼ 9.11, p ¼ .003, Cohen’s d ¼ 0.46) and the non-
the sex ratio in the mating environment and women’s rela- mating control condition (Mcontrol ¼ 2.99, SD ¼ 1.87, F(1,
tionship status to operationalize different mating motives. 609) ¼ 42.50, p < .001, Cohen’s d ¼ 0.90) for single
Participants read a scenario where they were going on a women. The sex ratio manipulation did not impact the
cruise with their family and that they would attend a gala mate screening motive for non-single women (all ps >
dinner on the first night of the cruise. In the male-biased .200). In addition, the female-biased condition bolstered
sex ratio condition, participants read that the dinner was the mate guarding motive over the male-biased condition
open to all travelers, that there were more men than women (Mfemale-biased ¼ 4.43, SD ¼ 2.23 vs. Mmale-biased ¼ 3.68,
on the cruise, and that many of the men were single. In the SD ¼ 2.30, F(1, 609) ¼ 6.51, p ¼ .011, Cohen’s d ¼ 0.33)
female-biased sex ratio condition, the scenario was identi- and the non-mating control condition (Mcontrol ¼ 2.79,
cal, except there were more women than men, and many of SD ¼ 2.12, p < .001, Cohen’s d ¼ 0.75) for non-single
the women were single. In the control condition, partici- women. The mate guarding motive did not differ between
pants read about a dinner with their family and were not the male-biased (Mmale-biased ¼ 3.24, SD ¼ 1.78) and the
given information about the sex ratio (see web appendix H female-biased (Mfemale-biased ¼ 3.08, SD ¼ 1.92, F(1, 609)
for the scenario). All participants were asked to describe ¼ 0.65, p ¼ .422) conditions for single women. Together,
how they visualized the scenario. these results confirmed the success of the manipulation of
the mate screening motive for single women and of the
Luxury Consumption Measure. Participants were pre- mate guarding motive for non-single women. (Web appen-
sented with descriptions and pictures of six black shoe dix H provides the full details.)
options (web appendix H) in similar styles that they could
wear to the gala dinner: three luxury brands (Dior, Gucci, Luxury Consumption. A 3  2 ANOVA on the number
Christian Louboutin) and three non-luxury brands (J. of luxury brands chosen with sex ratio (male-biased vs.
Crew, Aldo, Anne Klein). Participants were asked to indi- female-biased vs. control) and relationship status (single
cate which three of the six pairs they would be the most in- vs. non-single) as fixed factors revealed a significant inter-
terested in wearing. The dependent variable was the action effect (F(2, 530) ¼ 5.26, p ¼ .005, gp2 ¼ 0.019,
number of luxury brand shoes (out of the three pairs) that figure 4). There were no significant main effects of sex ra-
the participants selected. We conducted a post-test tio (F(2, 530) ¼ 2.09, p ¼ .124, gp2 ¼ 0.008) or relation-
(N ¼ 323 females, Mage ¼ 34.31, SD ¼ 14.60) to examine ship status (F(1, 530) ¼ 0.03, p ¼ .872, gp2 < 0.001).
whether women perceived that a male audience could cor- As predicted, single women in the male-biased condition
rectly recognize the luxury (vs. non-luxury) brand shoes as (whose mate screening motive was salient) chose signifi-
more luxurious in the scenario. The results showed that cantly more luxury brands (M ¼ 1.60, SD ¼ 0.78) than sin-
women believed that men would recognize the luxury gle women in the female-biased (M ¼ 1.32, SD ¼ 0.78,
brand shoes (Dior, Gucci, Christian Louboutin) as more F(1, 530) ¼ 5.41, p ¼ .020, gp2 ¼ 0.010) and the control
luxurious than the non-luxury brand shoes (J. Crew, Aldo, conditions (M ¼ 1.28, SD ¼ 0.69, F(1, 530) ¼ 7.24, p ¼
Anne Klein) (Mluxury ¼ 5.11, SD ¼ 1.20 vs. Mnon-luxury ¼ .007, gp2 ¼ 0.013). The latter two conditions were not sig-
4.38, SD ¼ 1.15, F(1, 322) ¼ 138.89, p < .001, gp2 ¼ nificantly different from each other (F(1, 530) ¼ 0.11, p ¼
0.301). This confirmed that women inferred the stronger .743, gp2 < 0.001). This pattern was consistent with our
signaling value of luxury (vs. non-luxury) shoes to men theory. In contrast, non-single women in the female-biased
(see web appendix H for full details). condition (whose mate guarding motive was salient) chose
CHEN, WANG, AND ORDABAYEVA 315

FIGURE 4 mating standards to men and to thereby deter men who do


not meet these standards.
THE EFFECTS OF RELATIONSHIP STATUS AND SEX RATIO ON Multiple studies provided support for our theory across
WOMEN’S PREFERENCE FOR LUXURY CONSUMPTION different populations and cultures, operationalizations of
(STUDY 6)
the mate screening motive, and hypothetical preferences,
as well as consequential choices of actual single female
subscribers of a real-world dating service. Importantly, the
studies supported the proposed mechanism by showing that
the phenomenon is attenuated when external tools for
screening romantic partners are present (vs. absent), the
quality of pursuers is high (vs. mixed), and a selection (vs.
rejection) mindset is externally activated. Complementing

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jcr/article/50/2/303/6650085 by guest on 22 August 2023


the moderation approach used in the main studies, follow-
up study B (detailed in web appendix I) offers additional
evidence of the psychological process by confirming that
women believe that consuming luxury (vs. non-luxury) in
dating settings can communicate their mating standards
and deter undesirable pursuers more effectively. Our find-
ings make important contributions to marketing theory and
significantly more luxury brands (M ¼ 1.59, SD ¼ 0.90) practice.
than non-single women in the male-biased (M ¼ 1.32, SD
¼ 0.84, F(1, 530) ¼ 5.02, p ¼ .025, gp2 ¼ 0.009) or the Theoretical Contributions
control conditions (M ¼ 1.33, SD ¼ 0.78, F(1, 530) ¼
4.66, p ¼ .031, gp2 ¼ 0.009). The latter two conditions Past research on consumers’ romantic pursuits has fo-
were not significantly different from each other (F(1, 530) cused primarily on the role of a general mating motive in
¼ 0.01, p ¼ .919, gp2 < 0.001). This was consistent with consumption behavior (Chen et al. 2016; Griskevicius
prior research on the role of luxury consumption in guard- et al. 2007; Li et al. 2012). By doing so, it has overlooked
ing women’s romantic partners in the relationship mainte- more specific but important subgoals that individuals may
nance stage (Wang and Griskevicius 2014). prioritize in different romantic contexts to support their
general mating objective (Chen et al. 2016; Griskevicius
Discussion et al. 2006; Richerson, Mead, and Li 2020). The present re-
search addresses this gap by identifying the mate screening
Study 6 showed that luxury consumption could fulfill motive, which is particularly relevant to women and arises
different functions for women in the relationship formation when women encounter excessive romantic attention from
stage (single) versus the relationship maintenance stage men. We show that a stronger mate screening motive
(non-single). As a result, distinct features of the environ- boosts women’s luxury consumption, and we further delin-
ment (e.g., the abundance of single men) can prompt single eate the triggers and boundaries of this phenomenon.
women’s mate screening motive and drive their luxury Accordingly, our work adds to the limited understanding of
consumption in new ways vis-a-vis what was considered in how women use luxury, specifically, and consumption,
prior mate guarding research (Wang and Griskevicius more broadly, in romantic pursuits differently from men.
2014). Our findings pave the way for future work on additional
mating motives that may uniquely drive consumption be-
GENERAL DISCUSSION havior in different segments and romantic contexts.
Our work adds novel insights to extant theories on the
The present research identified and examined how an role of luxury products in consumers’ interpersonal rela-
important but overlooked mating motive—the mate screen- tionships. Whereas prior work has typically viewed luxury
ing motive—shapes consumption choices distinctly from as a tool to attract audiences (Desmichel et al. 2020; Lee
the more general mating motive which has been the main et al. 2015; Nelissen and Meijers 2011; Scott et al. 2013),
focus of prior research. We posited that the mate screening we show that in certain situations (romantic relationship
motive is particularly relevant for the mating pursuits of formation) and segments (single women), luxury can be an
women (vs. men) and that it has unique triggers and conse- effective tool to deter or detract undesirable audiences.
quences (vs. the general mating and the mate guarding This novel screening function of luxury adds nuance to our
motives studied previously). Importantly, we showed that a current understanding of individuals’ signaling behavior
stronger mate screening motive leads women to increase through luxury, specifically, and consumption, more
their luxury consumption as a way to signal their high generally.
316 JOURNAL OF CONSUMER RESEARCH

Finally, our findings contribute to the broader literature data suggest that women increasingly display pet owner-
on the dynamics of mate selection in social psychology ship on dating platforms (Sullivan 2019). A popular dating
and related fields (Fletcher et al. 2004; Li et al. 2002; app OkCupid reported that 84% of their female customers
Sprecher, Sullivan, and Hatfield 1994). Extant research has highlighted their current and aspired dog ownership in their
focused on the mating criteria that individuals adopt and profiles (Sullivan 2019). Could such displays help women
prioritize. In contrast, our findings establish consumption detract suitors who lack empathy and patience (typically
as an appealing tool that can help individuals communicate required for pet ownership)? Examining how women use
their mating criteria to potential partners. This is important, consumption to communicate their mating standards, par-
as communicating relationship expectations is a key to the ticularly within less visible domains (e.g., personality traits
success of romantic relationships (Thomas and Fletcher such as kindness), is another fruitful area for future
2003). However, direct verbal signals of individuals’ mat- research.
ing standards can be uncomfortable, especially when they

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jcr/article/50/2/303/6650085 by guest on 22 August 2023


Men. Future research can be extended to examine the
pertain to partners’ financial capability and social status role of the mate screening motive in men’s romantic pur-
(Sundie et al. 2020; Van Boven, Campbell, and Gilovich suits. First, investigating how men respond to women’s
2010). Our results suggest that consumption can convey screening-focused luxury displays and how this may poten-
one’s mating standards more subtly and effectively. tially impact the romantic consequences of luxury con-
sumption for women would be interesting. For example,
Opportunities for Future Research building on a prevalent consumer belief that there is a
This first examination of the novel mate screening mo- tradeoff between individuals’ financial success and inter-
tive in consumers’ romantic pursuits examines how this personal relationships and qualities (Goor, Keinan, and
motive uniquely shapes the consumption preferences of Ordabayeva 2021), men may perceive women who visibly
prioritize men’s financial capability as being open to men’s
women. Women constitute a large proportion of the luxury
deficiencies in the personal domain. This could potentially
market but have been understudied in the literature relative
hurt women’s long-term romantic outcomes. Future studies
to men. Our research opens the door to many interesting re-
could further explore such possibilities and outcomes.
search opportunities about various aspects of the mate
Second, men may also want or need to screen potential
screening motive in consumer behavior.
romantic partners. Examining which mating criteria men
Women’s Mate Screening through Non-Luxury prioritize and how they signal their criteria in these con-
Consumption. Although luxury displays can effectively texts would be intriguing. For example, some evidence
facilitate women’s attempts to screen mates based on fi- suggests that men value women’s physical appearance as
nancial criteria, how women may use consumption signals well as agreeableness and sense of humor (McGee and
to screen mates on other relevant factors is unclear. While Shevlin 2009). Men who have too many mating options
the financial capability is consistently reported as a promi- may display products that signal their high standards for
nent mating criterion for women (Walter et al. 2020), it beauty or humor to deter subpar candidates (e.g., by dis-
may not be the only factor that women prioritize. Thus, ex- playing pictures of very beautiful women in their life or
ploring the hierarchy of mating criteria that women estab- their consumption of comedic experiences).
lish and examining how this hierarchy may vary as a Role of Sexual Orientation. In the present research, we
function of individual characteristics and contexts would focus on heterosexual female consumers, as the evolution-
be interesting and thus merits attention. For example, be- ary psychology literature suggests that women are the most
yond men’s financial capability, women might value men’s likely to act as choosers in heterosexual relationships
educational background and cultural capital as much or (Kenrick et al. 1993; Trivers 1972). However, it would be
even more. In fact, education is an increasingly salient di- valuable to examine screening behaviors displayed by non-
mension of social status that requires substantial invest- heterosexual individuals to enhance our understanding of
ment and effort (Hoffower 2021). Would women display the dynamics of diverse structures and forms of romantic
their own high educational credentials (e.g., an Ivy League relationships and marriage in modern society (Masci,
T-shirt) or their involvement in niche intellectual groups Brown, and Kiley 2019). This interesting issue potentially
(e.g., 20th-century poetry club) to detract less educated or yields diverging predictions. One possibility is that tradi-
less sophisticated pursuers? Furthermore, could certain tional gender roles persist within non-heterosexual rela-
segments of women (e.g., high earners, divorcees) priori- tionships and that relationship dynamics are determined by
tize partners’ financial capability less and interpersonal which partner takes on the caregiver role and which partner
factors more such as empathy, emotional, and time avail- assumes the breadwinner role (Marecek, Finn, and Cardell
ability? If so, then would women display emotionally- and 1982). In this case, our theory would predict that the first
time-intensive consumption experiences to deter pursuers partner would be more selective in the mating market and
lacking these qualities and resources? For instance, recent would engage in a screening process on the criteria that are
CHEN, WANG, AND ORDABAYEVA 317

important to him/her. Another possibility is that both part- Given the importance and prevalence of the mate screening
ners within a non-heterosexual relationship take on gender motive for women’s romantic pursuits, managers could
roles similar to their heterosexual counterparts (Lawson create features and tools to help women screen and elimi-
et al. 2014). In this case, both partners may play the role of nate potentially undesirable suitors. For example, dating
either the chooser or the pursuer in the mating market sites could allow individuals to configure their audience
(depending on their sex) (Gotta et al. 2011), and their se- screening options and customize their screening criteria as
lectiveness may be determined by other factors such as a way to eliminate subpar candidates. As shown in study 3,
specific mate characteristics (Regan 1998). Exploring these the presence of such features can lower the need for
possibilities would be interesting in future research. women to send their own screening signals to potential
candidates; this can increase women’s engagement with
Additional Mating Subgoals. Importantly, our work
these platforms. For segments who perceive that they are
highlights the importance of examining specific mating
in high demand, it would be valuable to develop special-

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jcr/article/50/2/303/6650085 by guest on 22 August 2023


priorities that individuals may focus on during their roman-
ized platforms that a priori screen subscribers on certain
tic pursuits. Our findings indicated that, despite the limited
criteria (e.g., The League platform focuses on Ivy League
attention it has received in prior research, the mate screen-
graduates; the Luxy platform requires subscribers to have
ing motive is highly prevalent in individuals’ relationship
an annual income of $200,000 or above; and it playfully
formation pursuits and that it is associated with unique trig-
promises to offer the service of “Tinder minus the ugly and
gers and consequences. Delineating the antecedents and
poor people”).
outcomes of other mating motives that individuals may
Finally, this research offers useful insights for individu-
adopt to form or maintain romantic relationships would be
als looking for love on the role of consumption in their ro-
important in future research. For example, in the relation-
mantic pursuits and the potential outcomes. More
ship maintenance stage, individuals may commonly find
specifically, luxury can help women effectively ward off
themselves needing to repair or rekindle their relationship
certain pursuers during early romantic encounters.
with their partner. Behaviors they display in these situa-
However, preliminary findings of follow-up study C also
tions may significantly diverge from behaviors they adopt
suggest that luxury consumption can potentially backfire
in the relationship maintenance contexts examined in prior
later in the relationship as it could reduce men’s expecta-
studies (mate guarding). Future work examining these pos-
tions about the amount of effort they need to invest in cer-
sibilities would be welcomed.
tain non-financial aspects of the relationship (see web
appendix I). Hence, it may be helpful to carefully deploy
Practical Implications luxury during early romantic encounters and perhaps in
Our findings reveal how women use luxury in their ro- combination with other signals that signal women’s priori-
mantic pursuits differently from men. The results yield use- ties in non-financial domains.
ful input on how luxury marketers can market their More broadly, our findings expand the current under-
products to women (an important luxury segment) and in- standing of the diverse functions that luxury serves in ro-
corporate the screening motive in their marketing strate- mantic contexts for different groups. Our research focused
gies. In particular, luxury brands may seek to incorporate on women, who comprise over one-half of the U.S. popula-
the mate screening motive in communication targeting tion but drive 70–80% of all consumer purchasing deci-
women (e.g., appropriate imagery, messages, or choice of sions (Forbes 2019) and whose share of discretionary
celebrity endorsers) (see web appendix A for representative spending is expected to reach 75% by 2028 (Nielsen
romantically themed campaigns and product collections by 2020). We hope that our findings on the distinct role of one
luxury brands). In addition, it would be beneficial to pro- class of products (luxury) in this segment will inspire fu-
mote luxury products on platforms that reach single female ture work on the unique meanings, functions, and outcomes
segments (e.g., dating websites, specialized blogs, plat- of consumption choices across various domains in this im-
forms appealing to this demographic). These strategies portant consumer group.
may be even more effective in contexts where screening is
particularly salient or challenging such as in geographic DATA COLLECTION INFORMATION
markets with male-biased sex ratios, on dating platforms
with a disproportionate amount of attention for certain The data collection for all studies was supervised by the
users, or at dating events with attendees of unclear or var- first author. Study 1 data were collected with the help of
ied quality. the staff of a dating service agency in July 2019. Study 2
Second, our findings can help managers of dating serv- data were collected on Prolific by the first author in
ices and platforms design an especially pleasant and pro- November 2021. Study 3 data were collected on Credamo
ductive experience for subscribers—particularly women, by the first author in April 2021. Study 4 data were col-
who constitute a significant portion of their customer base. lected on Credamo by the first author in April 2021. Study
318 JOURNAL OF CONSUMER RESEARCH

5 data were collected on Amazon Mechanical Turk (M Desmichel, Perrine, Nailya Ordabayeva, and Bruno Kocher
Turk) by the first author in November 2021. Study 6 data (2020), “What If Diamonds Did Not Last Forever? Signaling
Status Achievement through Ephemeral versus Iconic
were collected on M Turk by the first author in November
Luxury Goods,” Organizational Behavior and Human
2019. The first author did the data analysis for all studies. Decision Processes, 158, 49–65.
All authors jointly designed the studies and discussed the Diehl, Kristin (2005), “When Two Rights Make a Wrong:
analyses and the results. The data are currently stored in a Searching Too Much in Ordered Environments,” Journal of
project directory on the Open Science Framework under Marketing Research, 42 (3), 313–22.
the management of all authors. Dion, Delphine and Stephane Borraz (2017), “Managing Status:
How Luxury Brands Shape Class Subjectivities in the
Service Encounter,” Journal of Marketing, 81 (5), 67–85.
REFERENCES Durante, Kristina M., Vladas Griskevicius, Jeffry A. Simpson,
Stephanie M. Cantu, and Joshua M. Tybur (2012), “Sex Ratio
Berger, Jonah and Morgan Ward (2010), “Subtle Signals of and Women’s Career Choice: Does a Scarcity of Men Lead

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jcr/article/50/2/303/6650085 by guest on 22 August 2023


Inconspicuous Consumption,” Journal of Consumer Women to Choose Briefcase over Baby?,” Journal of
Research, 37 (4), 555–69. Personality and Social Psychology, 103 (1), 121–34.
Bettman, James, R. Mary Frances Luce, and John W. Payne Durante, Kristina M. and Ashley Rae Arsena (2015), “Playing the
(1998), “Constructive Consumer Choice Processes,” Journal Field: The Effect of Fertility on Women’s Desire for
of Consumer Research, 25 (3), 187–217. Variety,” Journal of Consumer Research, 41 (6), 1372–91.
Bruch, Elizabeth, Fred Feinberg, and Kee Yeun Lee (2016), Eastwick, Paul W. and Eli J. Finkel (2008), “Sex Differences in
“Extracting Multistage Screening Rules from Online Dating Mate Preferences Revisited: Do People Know What They
Activity Data,” Proceedings of the National Academy of Initially Desire in a Romantic Partner?,” Journal of
Sciences of the United States of America, 113 (38), 10530–5. Personality and Social Psychology, 94 (2), 245–64.
Buss, David M. (1989), “Sex Differences in Human Mate Escalas, Jennifer Edson and James R. Bettman (2005), “Self-
Preferences: Evolutionary Hypotheses Tested in 37 Construal, Reference Groups, and Brand Meaning,” Journal
Cultures,” Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 12 (1), 1–49. of Consumer Research, 32 (3), 378–89.
Buss, D. M. and D. P. Schmitt (1993), “Sexual Strategies Theory: Ferriman, Kimberley, David Lubinski, and Camilla P. Benbow
An Evolutionary Perspective on Human Mating,” (2009), “Work Preferences, Life Values, and Personal Views
Psychological Review, 100 (2), 204–32. of Top Math/Science Graduate Students and the Profoundly
Campbell, Anne (2002), A Mind of Her Own: The Evolutionary Gifted: Developmental Changes and Gender Differences dur-
Psychology of Women, Oxford: Oxford University Press. ing Emerging Adulthood and Parenthood,” Journal of
Campbell, Lorne and Garth J. O. Fletcher (2015), “Romantic Personality and Social Psychology, 97 (3), 517–32.
Relationships, Ideal Standards, and Mate Selection,” Current Fletcher, Garth J. O., Jacqueline M. Tither, Claire O’Loughlin,
Opinion in Psychology, 1, 97–100. Myron Friesen, and Nickola Overall (2004), “Warm and
Chen, Rui, Yuhuang Zheng, and Yan Zhang (2016), “Fickle Men, Homely or Cold and Beautiful? Sex Differences in Trading
Faithful Women: Effects of Mating Cues on Men’s and off Traits in Mate Selection,” Personality & Social
Women’s Variety-Seeking Behavior in Consumption,” Psychology Bulletin, 30 (6), 659–72.
Fletcher, Garth J. O., Patrick S. G. Kerr, Norman P. Li, and
Journal of Consumer Psychology, 26 (2), 275–82.
Katherine A. Valentine (2014), “Predicting Romantic Interest
Chernev, Alexander (2003), “Product Assortment and Individual
and Decisions in the Very Early Stages of Mate Selection:
Decision Process,” Journal of Personality and Social
Standards, Accuracy, and Sex Differences,” Personality &
Psychology, 85 (1), 151–62.
Social Psychology Bulletin, 40 (4), 540–50.
Chernev, Alexander, Ulf Böckenholt, and Joseph Goodman
Forbes (2019), “20 Facts and Figures to Know When Marketing to
(2015), “Choice Overload: A Conceptual Review and Meta- Women,” https://www.forbes.com/sites/forbescontentmar-
Analysis,” Journal of Consumer Psychology, 25 (2), 333–58. keting/2019/05/13/20-facts-and-figures-to-know-when-mar-
Clark, Catherine L., Phillip R. Shaver, and Matthew F. Abrahams keting-to-women/?sh=7d32ac051297. Last Accessed March
(1999), “Strategic Behaviors in Romantic Relationship Initiation,” 2022.
Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 25 (6), 709–22. Gervis, Zoya (2019), “Here’s How Much Money the Average
CompareCards (2020), “Compare Cards Survey Finds Americans American Spends on Dating,” https://nypost.com/2019/09/
Expect to Spend an Average of $142 on Significant Other for 12/heres-how-much-money-the-average-american-spends-
Valentine’s Day,” https://www.prnewswire.com/news- on-dating/. Last Accessed March 2022.
releases/love-and-money-comparecards-survey-finds-ameri- Goor, Dafna, Nailya Ordabayeva, Anat Keinan, and Sandrine
cans-expect-to-spend-an-average-of-142-on-significant-other- Crener (2020), “The Impostor Syndrome from Luxury
for-valentines-day-300999941.html. Last Accessed March Consumption,” Journal of Consumer Research, 46 (6),
2022. 1031–51.
Dahl, Darren W., Jaideep Sengupta, and Kathleen D. Vohs (2009), Goor, Dafna, Anat Keinan, and Nailya Ordabayeva (2021),
“Sex in Advertising: Gender Differences and the Role of “Status Pivoting,” Journal of Consumer Research, 47 (6),
Relationship Commitment,” Journal of Consumer Research, 978–1002.
36 (2), 215–31. Gotta, Gabrielle, Robert-Jay Green, Esther Rothblum, Sondra
D’Angelo, Jonathan D. and Catalina L. Toma (2017), “There Are Solomon, Kimberly Balsam, and Pepper Schwartz (2011),
Plenty of Fish in the Sea: The Effects of Choice Overload and “Heterosexual, Lesbian, and Gay Male Relationships: A
Reversibility on Online Daters’ Satisfaction with Selected Comparison of Couples in 1975 and 2000,” Family Process,
Partners,” Media Psychology, 20 (1), 1–27. 50 (3), 353–76.
CHEN, WANG, AND ORDABAYEVA 319

Griskevicius, Vladas, Robert B. Cialdini, and Douglas T. Kenrick Searching for Nonlinearities in Mate Judgment,” Journal of
(2006), “Peacocks, Picasso, and Parental Investment: The Personality and Social Psychology, 80 (3), 462–71.
Effects of Romantic Motives on Creativity,” Journal of Lawson, Jamie F., Christine James, Anna-Ulla C. Jannson, Nicola
Personality and Social Psychology, 91 (1), 63–76. F. Koyama, and Russell A. Hill (2014), “A Comparison of
Griskevicius, Vladas, Joshua M. Tybur, Jill M. Sundie, Robert B. Heterosexual and Homosexual Mating Preferences in
Cialdini, Geoffrey F. Miller, and Douglas T. Kenrick (2007), Personal Advertisements,” Evolution and Human Behavior,
“Blatant Benevolence and Conspicuous Consumption: When 35 (5), 408–14.
Romantic Motives Elicit Strategic Costly Signals,” Journal Lee, Jieun, Eunju Ko, and Carol M. Megehee (2015), “Social
of Personality and Social Psychology, 93 (1), 85–102. Benefits of Brand Logos in Presentation of Self in Cross and
Griskevicius, Vladas, Joshua M. Tybur, Joshua M. Ackerman, Same Gender Influence Contexts,” Journal of Business
Andrew W. Delton, Theresa E. Robertson, and Andrew E. Research, 68 (6), 1341–9.
White (2012), “The Financial Consequences of Too Many Lenton, Alison P. and Marco Francesconi (2011), “Too Much of a
Men: Sex Ratio Effects on Saving, Borrowing, and Good Thing? Variety Is Confusing in Mate Choice,” Biology
Spending,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, Letters, 7 (4), 528–31.

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jcr/article/50/2/303/6650085 by guest on 22 August 2023


102 (1), 69–80. Li, Norman P., J. Michael Bailey, Douglas T. Kenrick, and Joan
Griskevicius, Vladas and Douglas T. Kenrick (2013), A. W. Linsenmeier (2002), “The Necessities and Luxuries of
“Fundamental Motives: How Evolutionary Needs Influence Mate Preferences: Testing the Tradeoffs,” Journal of
Consumer Behavior,” Journal of Consumer Psychology, 23 Personality and Social Psychology, 82 (6), 947–55.
(3), 372–86. Li, Yexin Jessica, Douglas T. Kenrick, Vladas Griskevicius, and
Han, Young Jee, Joseph C. Nunes, and Xavier Drèze (2010), Steven L. Neuberg (2012), “Economic Decision Biases and
“Signaling Status with Luxury Goods: The Role of Brand Fundamental Motivations: How Mating and Self-Protection
Prominence,” Journal of Marketing, 74 (4), 15–30. Alter Loss Aversion,” Journal of Personality and Social
Hasford, Jonathan, Blair Kidwell, and Virginie Lopez-Kidwell Psychology, 102 (3), 550–61.
(2018), “Happy Wife, Happy Life: Food Choices in Li, Yexin Jessica, Kelly L. Haws, and Vladas Griskevicius (2019),
Romantic Relationships,” Journal of Consumer Research, 44 “Parenting Motivation and Consumer Decision-Making,”
(6), 1238–56. Journal of Consumer Research, 45 (5), 1117–37.
Hoffower, Hillary (2021), “The Elite’s Favorite Status Symbols Malhotra, Naresh K. (1982), “Information Load and Consumer
Have Become Way More Expensive over the past 20 Years,” Decision Making,” Journal of Consumer Research, 8 (4),
https://www.businessinsider.com/how-discreet-wealth-sta- 419–30.
tus-symbols-affected-education-health-costs-2021-2. Last Makkar, Marian and Sheau-Fen Yap (2018), “Emotional
Accessed March 2022. Experiences behind the Pursuit of Inconspicuous Luxury,”
Hornsey, Matthew J., Richard Wellauer, Jason C. McIntyre, and Journal of Retailing and Consumer Services, 44, 222–34.
Fiona K. Barlow (2015), “A Critical Test of the Assumption Marecek, Jeanne, Stephen E. Finn, and Mona Cardell (1982),
That Men Prefer Conformist Women and Women Prefer “Gender Roles in the Relationships of Lesbians and Gay
Nonconformist Men,” Personality & Social Psychology Men,” Journal of Homosexuality, 8 (2), 45–9.
Bulletin, 41 (6), 755–68. Masci, David, Anna Brown, and Jocelyn Kiley (2019), “5 Facts
Hou, Juan, Tianxin Shu, and Xiaoyi Fang (2022), “The Influence about Same-Sex Marriage,” https://www.pewresearch.org/
of Time Resources on Mate Selection: The Moderating Role fact-tank/2019/06/24/same-sex-marriage/. Last Accessed
of Mating Strategies and Gender,” Personality and February 2022.
Individual Differences, 186, 111388. McGee, Elizabeth and Mark Shevlin (2009), “Effect of Humor on
IBIS World (2021), “Dating Services Industry in the U.S.,” https:// Interpersonal Attraction and Mate Selection,” The Journal of
www.ibisworld.com/industry-statistics/market-size/dating- Psychology, 143 (1), 67–77.
services-united-states/. Last Accessed February 2022. Mead, Nicole L., Roy F. Baumeister, Tyler F. Stillman, Catherine
Janssens, Kim, Mario Pandelaere, Bram Van den Bergh, Kobe D. Rawn, and Kathleen D. Vohs (2011), “Social Exclusion
Millet, Inge Lens, and Keith Roe (2011), “Can Buy Me Love: Causes People to Spend and Consume Strategically in the
Mate Attraction Goals Lead to Perceptual Readiness for Service of Affiliation,” Journal of Consumer Research, 37
Status Products,” Journal of Experimental Social (5), 902–19.
Psychology, 47 (1), 254–8. Mende, Martin, Maura L. Scott, Aaron M. Garvey, and Lisa E.
Katz-Wise, Sabra, Heather A. Priess, and Janet Hyde (2010), Bolton (2019), “The Marketing of Love: How Attachment
“Gender-Role Attitudes and Behavior across the Transition Styles Affect Romantic Consumption Journeys,” Journal of
to Parenthood,” Developmental Psychology, 46 (1), 18–28. the Academy of Marketing Science, 47 (2), 255–73.
Kenrick, D. T., E. K. Sadalla, G. Groth, and M. R. Trost (1990), Nelissen, Rob M. A. and Marijn H. C. Meijers (2011), “Social
“Evolution, Traits, and the Stages of Human Courtship: Benefits of Luxury Brands as Costly Signals of Wealth and
Qualifying the Parental Investment Model,” Journal of Status,” Evolution and Human Behavior, 32 (5), 343–55.
Personality, 58 (1), 97–116. Nielsen (2020), “Wise up to Women,” https://www.nielsen.com/
Kenrick, Douglas T., Gary E. Groth, Melanie R. Trost, and us/en/insights/article/2020/wise-up-to-women/. Last
Edward K. Sadalla (1993), “Integrating Evolutionary and Accessed February 2022.
Social Exchange Perspectives on Relationships: Effects of Ordabayeva, Nailya and Pierre Chandon (2011), “Getting Ahead
Gender, Self-Appraisal, and Involvement Level on Mate of the Joneses: When Equality Increases Conspicuous
Selection Criteria,” Journal of Personality and Social Consumption among Bottom-Tier Consumers,” Journal of
Psychology, 64 (6), 951–69. Consumer Research, 38 (1), 27–41.
Kenrick, D. T., J. M. Sundie, L. D. Nicastle, and G. O. Stone Otterbring, Tobias, Christine Ringler, Nancy J. Sirianni, and
(2001), “Can One Ever Be Too Wealthy or Too Chaste? Anders Gustafsson (2018), “The Abercrombie & Fitch
320 JOURNAL OF CONSUMER RESEARCH

Effect: The Impact of Physical Dominance on Male matches-heres-the-verdict-18544033. Last Accessed March
Customers’ Status-Signaling Consumption,” Journal of 2022.
Marketing Research, 55 (1), 69–79. Sundie, Jill M., Douglas T. Kenrick, Vladas Griskevicius, Joshua
Otterbring, Tobias, Jill Sundie, Yexin J. Li, and Sarah Hill (2020), M. Tybur, Kathleen D. Vohs, and Daniel J. Beal (2011),
“Evolutionary Psychological Consumer Research: Bold, “Peacocks, Porsches, and Thorstein Veblen: Conspicuous
Bright, but Better with Behavior,” Journal of Business Consumption as a Sexual Signaling System,” Journal of
Research, 120, 473–84. Personality and Social Psychology, 100 (4), 664–80.
Perilloux, Carin, Judith A. Easton, and David M. Buss (2012), Sundie, Jill M., Mario Pandelaere, Inge Lens, and Luk Warlop
“The Misperception of Sexual Interest,” Psychological (2020), “Setting the Bar: The Influence of Women’s
Science, 23 (2), 146–51. Conspicuous Display on Men’s Affiliative Behavior,”
Pew Research Center (2020), “Nearly Half of U.S. Adults Say Journal of Business Research, 120, 569–85.
Dating Has Gotten Harder for Most People in the Last 10 Thomas, Geoff and Garth J. O. Fletcher (2003), “Mind-Reading
Years,” https://www.pewresearch.org/social-trends/2020/08/ Accuracy in Intimate Relationships: Assessing the Roles of
20/nearly-half-of-u-s-adults-say-dating-has-gotten-harder- the Relationship, the Target, and the Judge,” Journal of

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jcr/article/50/2/303/6650085 by guest on 22 August 2023


for-most-people-in-the-last-10-years/. Last Accessed Personality and Social Psychology, 85 (6), 1079–94.
February 2022. Townsend, John Marshall (1989), “Mate Selection Criteria: A
Regan, Pamela C. (1998), “What If You Can’t Get What You Pilot Study,” Ethology and Sociobiology, 10 (4), 241–53.
Want? Willingness to Compromise Ideal Mate Selection Townsend, J. M. and G. D. Levy (1990), “Effects of Potential
Standards as a Function of Sex, Mate Value, and Partners’ Physical Attractiveness and Socioeconomic Status
Relationship Context,” Personality and Social Psychology on Sexuality and Partner Selection,” Archives of Sexual
Bulletin, 24 (12), 1294–303. Behavior, 19 (2), 149–64.
Richerson, Rob, James A. Mead, and Wenjing Li (2020), Trivers, Robert (1972), “Parental Investment and Sexual
“Evolutionary Motives and Food Behavior Modeling in Selection,” in Sexual Selection and the Descent of Man:
Romantic Relationships,” Journal of Business Research, 120, 1871–1971, ed. Bernard Campbell, Chicago: Aldine, 136–79.
509–19. Van Boven, Leaf, Margaret C. Campbell, and Thomas Gilovich
Ross, Gretchen R., Margaret G. Meloy, and Lisa E. Bolton (2021), (2010), “Stigmatizing Materialism: On Stereotypes and
“Disorder and Downsizing,” Journal of Consumer Research, Impressions of Materialistic and Experiential Pursuits,”
47 (6), 959–77. Personality & Social Psychology Bulletin, 36 (4), 551–63.
Rucker, Derek D. and Adam D. Galinsky (2008), “Desire to Van den Bergh, Bram, Siegfried Dewitte, and Luk Warlop (2008),
Acquire: Powerlessness and Compensatory Consumption,” “Bikinis Instigate Generalized Impatience in Intertemporal
Journal of Consumer Research, 35 (2), 257–67. Choice,” Journal of Consumer Research, 35 (1), 85–97.
Saad, Gad, Aliza Eba, and Richard Sejean (2009), “Sex Walter, Kathryn V., Daniel Conroy-Beam, David M. Buss, Kelly
Differences When Searching for a Mate: A Process-Tracing Asao, Agnieszka Sorokowska, Piotr Sorokowski, Toivo
Approach,” Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 22 (2), Aavik, Grace Akello, Mohammad Madallh Alhabahba,
171–90. Charlotte Alm, Naumana Amjad, Afifa Anjum, Chiemezie S.
Saad, Gad and Tripat Gill (2014), “The Framing Effect When Atama, Derya Atamtürk Duyar, Richard Ayebare, Carlota
Evaluating Prospective Mates: An Adaptationist Batres, Mons Bendixen, Aicha Bensafia, Boris Bizumic,
Perspective,” Evolution and Human Behavior, 35 (3), Mahmoud Boussena, Marina Butovskaya, Seda Can,
184–92. Katarzyna Cantarero, Antonin Carrier, Hakan Cetinkaya,
Scott, Maura L., Martin Mende, and Lisa E. Bolton (2013), Ilona Croy, Rosa Marıa Cueto, Marcin Czub, Daria Dronova,
“Judging the Book by Its Cover? How Consumers Decide Seda Dural, Izzet Duyar, Berna Ertugrul, Agustın Espinosa,
Conspicuous Consumption Cues in Buyer-Seller Ignacio Estevan, Carla Sofia Esteves, Luxi Fang, Tomasz
Relationship,” Journal of Marketing Research, 50 (3), Frackowiak, Jorge Contreras Gardu~no, Karina Ugalde
334–47. Gonzalez, Farida Guemaz, Petra Gyuris, Maria Halamova,
Simpson, Jeffry A., Vladas Griskevicius, and Alexander J. Iskra Herak, Marina Horvat, Ivana Hromatko, Chin-Ming
Rothman (2012), “Consumer Decisions in Relationships,” Hui, Jas Laile Jaafar, Feng Jiang, Konstantinos Kafetsios,
Journal of Consumer Psychology, 22 (3), 304–14. Tina Kavcic, Leif Edward Ottesen Kennair, Nicolas Kervyn,
Sprecher, Susan, Quintin Sullivan, and Elaine Hatfield (1994), Truong Thi Khanh Ha, Imran Ahmed Khilji, Nils C. Köbis,
“Mate Selection Preferences: Gender Differences Examined Hoang Moc Lan, Andras Lang, Georgina R. Lennard, Ernesto
in a National Sample,” Journal of Personality and Social Leon, Torun Lindholm, Trinh Thi Linh, Giulia Lopez,
Psychology, 66 (6), 1074–80. Nguyen Van Luot, Alvaro Mailhos, Zoi Manesi, Rocio
Sprecher, Susan and Pamela C. Regan (2002), “Liking Some Martinez, Sarah L. McKerchar, Norbert Mesko, Girishwar
Things (in Some People) More than Others: Partner Misra, Conal Monaghan, Emanuel C. Mora, Alba Moya-
Preferences in Romantic Relationships and Friendships,” Garofano, Bojan Musil, Jean Carlos Natividade, Agnieszka
Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 19 (4), Niemczyk, George Nizharadze, Elisabeth Oberzaucher, Anna
463–81. Oleszkiewicz, Mohd Sofian Omar-Fauzee, Ike E. Onyishi,
Stone, Emily A., Todd K. Shackelford, and David M. Buss (2007), €
Baris Ozener, Ariela Francesca Pagani, Vilmante
“Sex Ratio and Mate Preferences: A Cross-Cultural Pakalniskiene, Miriam Parise, Farid Pazhoohi, Annette
Investigation,” European Journal of Social Psychology, 37 Pisanski, Katarzyna Pisanski, Edna Ponciano, Camelia Popa,
(2), 288–96. Pavol Prokop, Muhammad Rizwan, Mario Sainz, Svjetlana
Sullivan, Cornne (2019), “Here’s the Verdict on Whether Dog Salkicevic, Ruta Sargautyte, Ivan Sarmany-Schuller, Susanne
Photos Get You More Dating App Matches,” https://www. Schmehl, Shivantika Sharad, Razi Sultan Siddiqui, Franco
elitedaily.com/p/do-dog-photos-get-you-more-dating-app- Simonetti, Stanislava Yordanova Stoyanova, Meri Tadinac,
CHEN, WANG, AND ORDABAYEVA 321

Marco Antonio Correa Varella, Christin-Melanie Vauclair, Wiederman, Michael W. and Elizabeth Rice Allgeier (1992),
Luis Diego Vega, Dwi Ajeng Widarini, Gyesook Yoo, Marta “Gender Differences in Mate Selection Criteria:
Zat’kova, and Maja Zupancic (2020), “Sex Differences in Sociobiological or Socioeconomic Explanation?,” Ethology
Mate Preferences across 45 Countries: A Large-Scale and Sociobiology, 13 (2), 115–24.
Replication,” Psychological Science, 31 (4), 408–23. Wolfe, Rachel (2020), “The New Way Online Daters Can Weed
Wang, Yajin (2021), “A Conceptual Framework of Contemporary out the Duds,” https://www.wsj.com/articles/the-new-way-
Luxury Consumption,” International Journal of Research in online-daters-can-weed-out-the-duds-11583420700. Last
Marketing, 39 (3), 788–803. Accessed March 2022.
Wang, Yajin and Vladas Griskevicius (2014), “Conspicuous Yang, Xiaojing, Huifang Mao, Lei Jia, and Melissa G. Bublitz
Consumption, Relationships, and Rivals: Women’s Luxury (2019), “A Sweet Romance: Divergent Effects of Romantic
Products as Signals to Other Women,” Journal of Consumer Stimuli on the Consumption of Sweets,” Journal of
Research, 40 (5), 834–54. Consumer Research, 45 (6), 1213–29.

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jcr/article/50/2/303/6650085 by guest on 22 August 2023

You might also like