Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 5

CYBERPSYCHOLOGY, BEHAVIOR, AND SOCIAL NETWORKING

Volume 19, Number 11, 2016


ª Mary Ann Liebert, Inc.
DOI: 10.1089/cyber.2016.0355

Following Your ‘‘Friend’’:


Social Media and the Strength of Adolescents’
Parasocial Relationships with Media Personae

Bradley J. Bond, PhD

Abstract

Social media have permeated the lives of adolescents and may be altering the way that teens engage with their
favorite media celebrities and characters. This study surveyed 316 adolescents to examine the relationship
between social media surveillance and the strength of adolescents’ parasocial relationships (PSRs) with media
personae they follow on social media. Results indicated a significant positive relationship between exposure to
media personae on Twitter and strength of PSRs. Adolescents who had experienced social interactions with
their favorite media personae on Twitter in the form of retweets or responses to tweets had stronger PSRs than
adolescents who had no such interactions. The realism of the media personae did not moderate findings as
expected. The findings suggest that Twitter provides intimate glimpses into the personal lives of media personae
that increase audiences’ feelings of connectedness to those celebrities and characters, an important finding given
that teens are more likely to learn from media personae with whom they have PSRs.

Keywords: adolescence, parasocial relationship, social media, Twitter

Introduction personae. Individual one-sided interactions between audi-


ences and media personae are referred to as parasocial in-
S ocial media platforms have become nearly ubiquitous
among adolescent users. The average United States teen
spends at least 1 hour a day engaged with social media.1
teractions.5 Parasocial interactions work in similar ways to
real-life interactions; more frequent and intimate interactions
reduce uncertainty and increase feelings of connectedness.4
Almost 90 percent of United States teens use social media
PSRs are similar to face-to-face friendships in their de-
and 24 percent report being engaged almost constantly.2
velopment, maintenance, and function.6,7 Unlike face-to-face
Research suggests that social media can influence adoles-
friendships, however, PSRs are one-sided. On television, for
cents’ identity development, access to information, and
example, characters’ actions are observable to viewers, but
relationships.3 Social media may not only impact real-life
viewers’ actions are not observable to characters. PSRs are,
relationships, but may also alter teens’ parasocial rela-
therefore, characterized by their one-sided sentimentality.
tionships (PSRs)—unilateral, intimate relationships that
PSRs may be particularly noteworthy for adolescents.
audience members develop with media characters and celeb-
Young people are more likely to learn from media per-
rities. The purpose of this study is to explore how social media
sonae whom they perceive as trusted friends.5 In one study,
platforms like Facebook, Instagram, Twitter, and SnapChat
adolescents who reported strong PSRs with the cast of Jersey
contribute to the strength of adolescents’ PSRs with media
Shore had sexual attitudes that mirrored those of the cast
personae (i.e., characters and celebrities).
more so than adolescents who did not have PSRs with cast
members.8 If teens are more likely to learn from media
Parasocial relationships personae with whom they have PSRs, scholars must better
PSRs are mediated interpersonal relationships between understand the underlying factors that influence the creation
audiences and media personae.4 PSRs may arise from our and maintenance of PSRs. Social media are likely altering
instinctive need for social relationships; audiences may the way that adolescents experience PSRs because they
gratify their need for attachment to others not only through provide media personae with novel platforms to reveal tidbits
their real-life interactions, but also by interacting with media of personal information to young audiences.

Department of Communication Studies, University of San Diego, San Diego, California.

656
SOCIAL MEDIA AND TEENS’ PARASOCIAL RELATIONSHIPS 657

Social media are rare,11 media personae can mention their followers to in-
When media personae post to social media, they provide crease feelings of connectedness, give back to loyal fans, and
audiences with intimate, behind-the-scene peeks into their manage their image.9 If parasocial interactions lead to PSRs, a
everyday lives and taste preferences. The ability of media repost or response that briefly transforms the parasocial into
personae to carefully craft an image and create a sense of social will likely have an even stronger influence on adoles-
closeness and familiarity between themselves and their fol- cents’ perceptions of their PSRs with media personae.
lowers on social media is referred to as performative inti-
H3: Participants who have engaged in computer-mediated
macy.9 For celebrities, performative intimacy could increase interactions with their favorite media personae will have
branding and marketing opportunities.10 For audiences, stronger PSRs than participants who have not.
performative intimacy likely strengthens PSRs.
Let us assume that a teenage girl in 1986 experienced a PSR Method
with pop musician Cyndi Lauper. In line with the conceptu-
alization of PSRs, the teenage girl would have built her emo- Sample
tional connection to Lauper through parasocial interactions. Vice principals from six randomly selected southern Cali-
However, in 1986 the media landscape would have limited the fornia middle and high schools were invited to recruit their
teen’s parasocial interactions to watching Lauper on television, students to participate in the study. Vice principals reviewed
reading about her in print, and listening to her music. Though the questionnaire before providing written consent for partic-
the teen may have learned intimate details about Lauper’s ipation. A university institutional review board approved re-
personal life from television appearances or magazine inter- placing parents’ consent with school administrators’ consent
views, such appearances would have been relatively infre- because the study presented minimal risk to participants. School
quent. Fast-forward 30 years. Today’s teenage girl has a PSR administrators who provided consent were instructed to send
with Katy Perry. The teen watches Perry on television, reads recruitment email messages to their students. The recruitment
about her in print, and listens to her music. She, however, also email included a hyperlink to an assent form. Once participants
has access to Perry’s posts on social media. These posts can provided assent, they were directed to an online survey. The
provide quick glimpses into the everyday occurrences of adolescents in the sample (N = 316) ranged in age from 13 to 17
Perry and, in turn, create or reinforce a sense of privilege years (M age = 15.64, SD = 1.19). More females (74 percent)
to Perry’s personal backstage.9,11 In just 1 week, Perry posted participated in the study than males (26 percent). The sample
to Twitter twelve times, disclosing her support for a presi- was composed primarily of participants identifying as White (73
dential candidate, love of tacos, wardrobe difficulties at a percent), mixed race (9 percent), Latino/Hispanic (8 percent),
music festival, and excitement for cherry trees to blossom. and Asian (7 percent). Few participants identified as Black (1
Perry’s taste preferences in politics, food, clothing, and botany percent) or another racial identity (2 percent).
were self-disclosed in a matter of days on social media. When
adolescents follow their favorite media personae on social
media, they increase the frequency and intimacy of their Measures
parasocial interactions, reduce uncertainty about their PSRs, The first item on the online questionnaire asked participants
and likely strengthen PSRs. to report their favorite media persona, a common practice in
the data collection procedures of studies examining PSRs
H1: A positive relationship will exist between PSR strength based on the assumption that individuals will most likely
and exposure to favorite media personae on social media.
have PSRs with media personae that they consider to be their
favorite.15 Participants were instructed to name anyone they
A key element to PSRs is realism. For individuals to de-
would consider their favorite regardless of the reality status,
velop PSRs, they need to be able to make person judgments
nature, or role of the persona. However, participants were
much the same way individuals do when developing real-life
asked to list only individual names rather than groups (e.g.,
relationships.12,13 Users must perceive social media posts by
the entire cast of a television show or a musical group). Two
media personae as authentic for the information garnered
coders blind to the purpose of the study then coded each of
from social media to strengthen feelings of connectedness.13
the favorite media personae to determine whether they were
Giles12 argued that individuals are more likely to develop
fictional characters or nonfictional celebrities using infor-
strong PSRs with nonfictional celebrities than with fictional
mation garnered through independent internet searches of
characters given the importance of realism. Realism may
the media personae (j = 1.00). Following the favorite media
then moderate the predicted relationship between PSR
persona item, participants responded to items measuring
strength and social media.
PSR strength, exposure to favorite media personae in tradi-
H2: The predicted relationship between PSR strength and tional and social media, and demographics. Age and race
exposure to favorite media personae on social media will be were presented as open-ended questions; gender was mea-
stronger for participants who reported PSRs with nonfic- sured using a single close-ended item. Following the favorite
tional personae than for participants who reported PSRs persona item, all other measures were presented to partici-
with fictional personae.
pants in random order to control for potential order effects.
Though PSRs are characterized by their one-sidedness,
social media permit two-way interactions between media PSR strength. The 15-item parasocial interaction scale
personae and their followers. Media personae can repost and quantifies the strength of one-way emotional connections that
respond to their followers in unique and novel ways not af- television audiences experience with television characters.16
forded by more traditional media.14 Though such interactions The measure was altered for the purpose of this study to
658 BOND

account for media personae not on television and to allow the Table 1. Summary of Regression Analysis
questionnaire to electronically pipe the names of the partic- Predicting Parasocial Relationship Strength
ipants’ favorite media personae into each item. For example,
the original item, ‘‘I look forward to watching this character B SE b
when his/her show is on’’ was altered to read, ‘‘I look forward Sex 0.32 0.07 0.26***
to watching, listening to, or reading about [name of media Age 0.03 0.03 0.07
persona].’’ Other examples include, ‘‘[Name of media per- Race 0.09 0.07 0.07
sona] would fit in well with my group of friends’’ and ‘‘[Name Realism -0.02 0.07 -0.01
of media persona] makes me feel comfortable, like I am with a Traditional Media 0.01 0.02 0.02
friend.’’ Responses to the items were measured on a five-point Facebook 0.02 0.02 0.06
scale from 1 (strongly disagree) to 5 (strongly agree). Mean Instagram -0.01 0.02 -0.05
scores were calculated for the 15 items (a = 0.86) and ranged Twitter 0.05 0.02 0.22**
SnapChat 0.06 0.05 0.07
from 2.20 to 5.00 (M = 4.10, SD = 0.57).
Note. Sex (male/female), race (White/racial minority), and
Exposure to favorite media personae. Participants re- realism (fictional/non-fictional) were dichotomously coded. Inter-
ported how often they were exposed to their favorite media action terms were nonsignificant and thus removed from analysis to
preserve power.
persona ‘‘on television, in film, in magazines, or on the radio’’ F (9, 289) = 5.68, p < 0.001. R2 = 0.15. **p < 0.01, ***p < 0.001.
(i.e., traditional screen media) as one item on a scale from 1
(never) to 7 (at least once/day) (M = 4.05, SD = 1.95). The
same anchor points were then used to measure how often was conducted using age, sex, race, media personae realism,
participants checked the posts of their favorite media persona traditional media exposure, exposure to the four social media
on Facebook (M = 2.76, SD = 2.29), Instagram (M = 3.37, platforms, and interaction terms as predictor variables and
SD = 2.56), SnapChat (M = 1.95, SD = 1.88), and Twitter strength of PSR as the criterion variable. All variables were
(M = 2.68, SD = 2.42). These four social media platforms were mean-centered before analysis to prevent issues related to
included on the questionnaire because current national survey multicollinearity. The resulting model was significant, F (9,
data suggest that they are the most popular among adoles- 289) = 5.68, p < 0.001. Participant sex was related to PSR
cents.2 Participants were also asked how many times their strength (b = 0.26, p < 0.001) such that females had stronger
favorite media persona had ever reposted or replied to any of PSRs than males. Twitter (b = 0.22, p < 0.01) significantly
their posts on Facebook, Instagram, SnapChat, or Twitter. contributed to PSR strength. Facebook (b = 0.06), Instagram
(b = -0.05), SnapChat (b = 0.07), traditional media (b = 0.02),
Results and realism (b = -0.01) failed to contribute to PSR strength
(Table 1). Hypothesis 1 was partially supported; exposure to
Descriptive analyses
favorite media personae on Twitter was significantly related
Favorite media personae. Participants reported 201 dif- to PSR strength. Interaction terms were not significant con-
ferent media personae as favorites. The most frequently cited tributors to PSR strength. Thus, realism of the media personae
favorites were Jennifer Lawrence (11 percent), Taylor Swift failed to moderate the relationship between social media and
(3 percent), Harry Styles (2 percent), Sheldon Cooper (2 PSR strength. Hypothesis 2 was not supported.
percent), and Miley Cyrus (2 percent). Only 28 percent of The third hypothesis predicted that participants who had
participants reported favorite media personae that were among engaged in computer-mediated interactions with favorite
the top 10 most cited, indicating considerable variance among media personae would have stronger PSRs than participants
adolescents’ favorite media personae. Media personae were who had not engaged in such interactions. Only 3 percent of
more likely nonfictional celebrities (60 percent) than fictional the sample (n = 9) reported engaging in computer-mediated
characters (40 percent). interactions with their favorite media persona. All of these
interactions came in the form of a retweet or response tweet
Media use. Most adolescents (70 percent) reported fol- on Twitter. No participant reported more than one such in-
lowing their favorite media persona on at least one social media teraction. Participants who had interacted with their favorite
platform, and half (50 percent) reported following their favor- media personae on Twitter had stronger PSRs (M = 4.50,
ite on at least two different social media platforms. A SD = 0.40) than participants who had no such interactions
smaller percent (15 percent) noted that they use all four (M = 4.08, SD = 0.56), t (312) = 2.23, p < 0.05. Hypothesis 3
social media platforms included in the study to follow their was supported.
favorite media persona. Instagram was the most popular for
following media personae (55 percent), followed by Face- Discussion
book (46 percent), Twitter (36 percent), and SnapChat (26
percent). The purpose of this study was to examine how exposure to
media personae on social media was related to PSR strength
among adolescents. A positive relationship was found be-
Hypotheses and research questions
tween surveilling favorite media personae on Twitter and
The first hypothesis predicted a relationship between PSR strength of PSRs that was not significant for other social
strength and frequency of exposure to favorite media personae media platforms. Though more adolescents reported fol-
via social media. The second hypothesis predicted that this lowing favorite media personae on Instagram and Facebook
relationship would be moderated by the realism of the media than Twitter, the relationship between Twitter and PSRs may
personae. To test these hypotheses, a linear regression analysis be explained by the nature and function of the social media
SOCIAL MEDIA AND TEENS’ PARASOCIAL RELATIONSHIPS 659

platform. Twitter has emerged as a legitimate channel of This study is not without limitations. The cross-sectional
celebrity voice. Tweets from celebrities and politicians are nature of the data does not allow causal claims. Teens with
often cited as official statements by news media, a privilege strong PSRs could subsequently follow their favorite media
not often afforded to other social media.17 The improvisatory personae on Twitter. The relationship is likely mutually causal,
quality of tweets or the inability to edit posted tweets may but longitudinal and experimental research would provide
contribute to their perceived legitimacy. Not only are tweets additional insight into causation. A larger, more diversified
deemed authentic, but they are also often intimate. A qual- sample would be beneficial given the lack of males and the
itative analysis of 12 media personae Twitter accounts sug- small number of participants who had experienced computer-
gested that celebrities disclose personal information on mediated communication with their favorite media personae.
Twitter and that such disclosure is meaningful and poten- Additionally, the content of tweets from media personae was
tially influential for those who consume the messages.11 The not examined here. A content analysis of celebrity tweets that
legitimacy and intimacy of tweets may increase the impor- teens are exposed to would also add to our understanding of
tance of parasocial interactions on Twitter compared to other how performative intimacy takes shape and its relationship to
social media and, in turn, influence the strength of PSRs PSRs. Another area for future research lies in the social media
between teen audiences and their favorite media personae. literacy of adolescents. For example, assistants often author
Frequency may also explain the contribution of Twitter to celebrity Twitter accounts rather than the celebrities them-
PSR strength. Media personae may post more frequently on selves, a practice known as ghost-tweeting.13 Ghost-tweeting
Twitter than other social media. The casual observation of could alter the perceived authenticity and, in turn, influence
Katy Perry’s Twitter account noted earlier found that Perry PSRs.13 Research has also suggested that reliance on social
had tweeted 12 times in 1 week. In the same week, she had media to follow media personae may be related to lower
posted to Instagram just once. Perry’s decision to tweet psychological well-being.19 Future research should continue
considerably more than to post on Instagram may be because to examine how adolescents interpret and understand para-
Twitter followers are more than twice as likely than other social interactions on social media and the effects of PSRs.
social media users to repost content on Twitter and other In sum, the findings presented here suggest that Twitter
social media.18 Reposts from everyday people are the life- provides adolescents intimate glimpses into the backstage,
blood of building and maintaining fan loyalty on social personal lives of media personae. Exposure to the backstage
media.10 If media personae are posting more frequently on increases teen audiences’ feelings of connectedness to the
Twitter, teen audiences may engage in more parasocial in- characters and celebrities they follow and strengthens their
teractions on Twitter and, thus, strengthen PSRs. PSRs. Given that research suggests teens are more likely to
The relationship between Twitter surveillance and PSR learn from media personae with whom they have PSRs, so-
strength was not moderated by the realism of the media cial media may be altering the function of media personae in
personae as hypothesized. Twitter may give life to fictional creating, reinforcing, or changing adolescents’ beliefs, atti-
characters in ways that traditional media never could. Vol- tudes, and behaviors.
demort, the Harry Potter villain, has never been interviewed
on The Tonight Show or featured in GQ. Voldemort, how- Acknowledgments
ever, is active on Twitter and is followed by over 2 million
people. The account has no affiliation with those who own The author wishes to thank Kat Pfost and Hayley West for
rights to Voldemort’s likeness. Though followers are aware their assistance with data collection and analysis.
that a fictional character cannot tweet, Voldemort’s tweets
serve as parasocial interaction between him and his followers, Author Disclosure Statement
allowing fans to suspend reality in novel ways beyond the
books and films and strengthen their PSRs with the villain. No competing financial interests exist.
Adolescents who had experienced computer-mediated
communication with media personae on Twitter via a retweet References
or response to a tweet had stronger PSRs than those who had
not experienced such communication. Such a finding rein- 1. Rideout V. (2015) The common sense census: media use by
forces the argument that a one-time social interaction can tweens and teens. San Francisco, CA: Common Sense
intensify an otherwise PSR.9,11 Retweets or response tweets Media.
strengthen emotional connections, and emotional connec- 2. Lenhart A. (2015) Teens, social media, and technology
tions characterize PSRs. Computer-mediated communica- overview 2015. Washington, DC: Pew Research Center.
3. Moreno M, Kota R. (2014) Social media. In Strasburger
tion with media personae may even have a greater impact on
VC, Wilson BJ, Jordan AB, eds. Children, adolescents, and
perceived intimacy than more traditional public appearances
the media, 3rd ed. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage, pp. 435–456.
where audiences might engage in face-to-face interactions 4. Horton D, Wohl RR. Mass communication and para-social
with their favorite celebrities like autograph signings. Au- interaction: observations on intimacy at a distance. Psy-
tograph signings are scheduled and highly structured; social chiatry 1956; 19:215–229.
media posts are perceived as spontaneous and genuine.9 Such 5. Calvert SL, Richards MN. (2014) Children’s parasocial
a finding could have implications for media personae man- relationships. In Jordan AB, Romer D, eds. Media and the
aging performative intimacy as a means of branding and well-being of children and adolescents. New York: Oxford,
marketing. Social media platforms like Twitter may be safe pp. 187–200.
spaces from which to connect with fans and increase PSR 6. Rubin AM, Perse EM. Audience activity and soap opera in-
strength, all while avoiding the risks associated with real-life volvement: a uses and effects investigation. Human Com-
interactions with fans.11 munication Research 1987; 14:246–268.
660 BOND

7. Rubin RB, McHugh MP. Development of parasocial in- sonality, interpersonal need, and television motive predict
teraction relationships. Journal of Broadcasting and Elec- their relationships with media characters? Psychology of
tronic Media 1987; 31:279–292. Popular Media Culture 2012; 1:185–200.
8. Bond BJ, Drogos KL. Sex on the shore: wishful identifi- 16. Rosaen SF, Dibble JF. Investigating the relationships
cation and parasocial relationships as mediators in the re- among child’s age, parasocial interactions, and the social
lationship between Jersey Shore exposure and emerging realism of favorite television characters. Communication
adults’ sexual attitudes and behaviors. Media Psychology Research Reports 2008; 25:145–154.
2014; 17:102–126. 17. Muntean N, Petersen AH. Celebrity Twitter: strategies of
9. Marwick A, Boyd D. To see and be seen: celebrity practice intrusion and disclosure in the age of technoculture. M/C
on Twitter. Convergence 2011; 17:139–158. Journal 2009;12: http://journal.media-culture.org.au/index
10. Lueck JA. Friend-zone with benefits: the parasocial ad- .php/mcjournal/article/view/194 Last accessed August 25,
vertising of Kim Kardashian. Journal of Marketing Com- 2016.
munications 2015; 21:91–109. 18. Zarrella D. (2010) The social media marketing book. Se-
11. Stever GS, Lawson K. Twitter as a way for celebrities to bastopol, CA: O’Reilly.
communicate with fans: implications for the study of para- 19. Baek YM, Bae Y, Jang H. Social and parasocial relationships
social interaction. North American Journal of Psychology on social network sites and their differential relationships with
2013; 15:339–354. users’ psychological well-being. Cyberpsychology, Behavior,
12. Giles DC. Parasocial interaction: a review of literature and a and Social Networking 2013; 16:512–517.
model for future research. Media Psychology 2002; 4:279–305.
13. Cohen EL, Tyler WJ. Examining perceived distance and
personal authenticity as mediators of the effects of ghost- Address correspondence to:
tweeting on parasocial interaction. Cyberpsychology, Be- Dr. Bradley J. Bond
havior, and Social Networking 2016; 19:342–346. Department of Communication Studies
14. Tsiotsou RH. The role of social and parasocial relationships University of San Diego
on social networking sites loyalty. Computers in Human 5998 Alcala Park
Behavior 2015; 48:401–414. San Diego, CA 92110
15. Tsay M, Bodine BM. Exploring parasocial interaction in
college students as a multidimensional construct: do per- E-mail: bond@sandiego.edu

You might also like