Crisis in Sociology: The Need For Darwin

You might also like

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 4

Review

Author(s): J. Milton Yinger


Review by: J. Milton Yinger
Source: Contemporary Sociology, Vol. 29, No. 5 (Sep., 2000), pp. 756-758
Published by: American Sociological Association
Stable URL: http://www.jstor.org/stable/2655273
Accessed: 27-06-2016 09:38 UTC

Your use of the JSTOR archive indicates your acceptance of the Terms & Conditions of Use, available at
http://about.jstor.org/terms

JSTOR is a not-for-profit service that helps scholars, researchers, and students discover, use, and build upon a wide range of content in a trusted
digital archive. We use information technology and tools to increase productivity and facilitate new forms of scholarship. For more information about
JSTOR, please contact support@jstor.org.

Sage Publications, Inc., American Sociological Association are collaborating with JSTOR to digitize,
preserve and extend access to Contemporary Sociology

This content downloaded from 134.117.10.200 on Mon, 27 Jun 2016 09:38:54 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
756 theories and Epistemology

in mind he does mention us in the preface I discipline adrift.... To survive it must redis-
suspect that Latour's intended audience is in fact cover the brain, the circumstances of its evolu-
scientists themselves. He refers to a rather con- tion, and the evolution of its products." (p. 4)
fusing and paradoxical "we" throughout the I share with the authors of Crisis in Sociology
book. With a nod to Tonto and Lone Ranger, I the wish that sociologists might reduce some of
ask "What you mean 'we,' Kemosabe?" The sec- their errors and strengthen their interpretations
tion on the science wars seems to be very much by paying careful attention to evolutionary biol-
about showing the intersections among science ogy. Every human behavior has to be biological-
and its critics, or the ways in which "we" are ly possible. I would quickly add, however, that
"really" all on the same side. After all, don't very little human behavior is the direct and
"we" want better versions of reality? Don't "we" exclusive product of biological factors. Scarcely
want to know things about the world? Yet while a week passes without one's reading about a new
this is a marvelous book for explaining how sci- demonstration of how the brain works or how a
ence works, Pandora's Hope is entirely inade- newly discovered gene or the secretions of a
quate for explaining how to make sense of the gland influence behavior. In their first three
consequences of science on bodies and lives, both chapters, Lopreato and Crippen trace "the early
human and nonhuman. I want a science stud- promise" of sociology and then "argue that at
ies and a sociology that do more than offer present sociology offers a shallow and distorted
benign understandings of what scientists do or view of human nature that prevents it from
help them do it better. I do not want to leave understanding the real world and thus from the
worldly interventions to scientists alone. But the likelihood of demonstrating its utility to society"
"how tos" of enhancing democratization and (p. 43).
. . .

ClVlC partlclpatlon ln lncreaslng y compllcatec In examining the crisis, as they see it,
technoscientifically hybrid situations are not to Lopreato and Crippen give scant attention to
be found in Pandora's Hope, although Latour's the shifting environments within which human
book goes a long way in clarifying why these are behavior occurs. I believe it is correct to say that
necessary and important. most biologists are students of the environments
within which the body and the brain work,
agreeing with Charles Child (1924) that the
Crisis in Sociology: The Need for Darwin, by concept of a fixed hereditary program built into
Joseph Lopreato and Timothy Crippen. cell and tissue inadequately explains the
New Brunswick, NJ: Transaction, 1999. 329 pp. observed facts. More recently Steven Jay Gould
$34.95 cloth. ISBN: 1-56000-398-7. has emphasized that heretability does not mean
J. MILTON YINGER inevitability. Is it not the task of sociologists to
Oberlin College analyze the range or behaviors that occur within
JMY@Opocketmail.com the biological framework, as a result of variation
in social structures and social interaction?
Sociology is fortunate in having a steady supply In the early 1950s it was widely believed that
of critics from its own ranks, pointing out weak- men had reached the outer limit in the ability to
nesses and urging changes in basic theory, meth- run a mile, having, through several decades,
ods of research, or topics of study. Since the reduced the record to only a few seconds over 4
critics are in rather wide disagreement with each minutes. Then, in 1954, Roger Bannister ran a
other, however, and tend to speak in quite mile in 3 minutes 59.4 seconds. By 1964 more
severe one might even say apocalyptic terms, than 70 runners had broken the 4-minute barri-
it is not easy to agree with their messages. I have er. Imagine the chagrin of the several that broke
found it helpful to fall back on Durkheim's the- the barrier in some of the races only to come in
ory of deviance its inevitability and its value. fifth or sixth. This is a kind of metaphor for a
Most of us are content with doing sociology, field theoretical principle in which the several
adapting our theories as seems wise and enrich- levels of influence on human behavior (biologi-
ing our methods with new technologies. That is cal, psychological, cultural, and social) con-
not good enough, say Lopreato and Crippen. verge, sometimes with one, then another being
After a "glorious" beginning "sociology soon the dominant force.
went astray and, as is widely noted both within Interpretations of the "crisis in sociology" in
and without the profession, it has long been a this book and elsewhere often involve the ques-

This content downloaded from 134.117.10.200 on Mon, 27 Jun 2016 09:38:54 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
theories and Epistemology 757

tion, at least by implication: What is sociology? uncomfortable with their emphasis on the failH
There is no Supreme Court to answer the quesH ures of destratification efforts, the authors note
tion, but probably most of us answer it by the that "we have deliberately stressed the prehistorH
way we do our work. Although Lopreato and ical period of our hunting and fathering past" (p.
Crippen offer no formal definition, they might 241). They discuss briefly a few changes that
be comfortable with something like this: may have modified the rigidity of stratification
Sociology is the scientific study of the influence systems. One would welcome a fuller discussion
of social structures, cultures, and social interacH of the influence of the domestication of plants
tion on human behavior, the powerful and limH and animals, bringing a more stable food supply,
iting influence of biological imperatives and the invention of writing, the appearance of culH
needs as developed in the process of evoluH tural norms emphasizing more open and egaliH
tion having been taken fully into account. In tarian systems, and other social and cultural
their eagerness to bring biological forces into developments that have created quite different
sociological study, they tend to exaggerate, or so environments within which the nature of stratiH
it seems to me, the rigidity of the genetic influH fication systems is worked out.
ence. They also exaggerate the alleged failure of A chapter on "The Clannish Brain" examH
sociologists to recognize the importance of bioH ines the extent to which ethnic group boundH
logical forces. "Sociology is still addicted to the aries are most strongly marked by kinship by
increasingly implausible assumption that human an inherited sense of the division between "us"
behavior is solely the result of socialization" (p. and "them." Lopreato and Crippen cite some of
21). All sociologists are thus addicted? Some? A the numerous sociological uses of"ethnic group"
few? that emphasize the family aspects: Weber's "staH
In addition to a more nuance use of the place tus groups," Sumner's "ethnocentrism" concept,
of biological forces in the definition of sociology, Pareto's picture of the family with all of its
I missed reference to another aspect of the quesH appendages.
tion: Does sociology incorporate an activist The terms used to describe and analyze
dimension the Auguste Comte of Systeme de human groups, however, cannot readily draw
politique positive as well as the Comte of Cours de sharp lines of distinction. We are not separating
philosophie positive? In its origins and in its curH lions and tigers. "Ethnic group" is now a concept
rent activities, sociology is deeply involved in widely used to refer to populations that share
"social problems." I believe that many socioloH one or more of several characteristics lanH
gists will agree with the current president of the guage, national background, historical memoH
ASA, Joe Feagin, saying (1999) that "the ries, religion, and a label from the "others." In
strength of sociology has long resided in its intelH the United States we often refer to "Hispanics"
lectual diversity." Which includes a strong interH as an ethnic group. Some Hispanics are primariH
est in "urgent moral and practical concerns." ly of European background and speak Spanish as
An interesting chapter is devoted to "fundaH their first language. Others are partly Native
mentals of social stratification." The authors American or African American, speak a variety
start with a valuable discussion of the strengths of languages, and bring to their place as
and weaknesses, as they see them, of the views of Hispanics a wide variety of historical memories.
Marx, Weber, and the team of Kingsley Davis Thus we have a rather sharp distinction
and Wilbert Moore. They shift then to a review drawn by those who see ethnicity in "primorH
of"Dominance Orders among Primates," drawH dial" terms and others, looking at current situaH
ing from this evolutionary background the view tions, in "instrumental" terms, to use the
that stratification systems are much more deeply distinction drawn by Pierre van den Berghe.
rooted in our inheritance, which most socioloH One can understand why Lopreato and Crippen,
gists "obsessed" with interest in complex sociH given their goal of raising the evolutionary conH
eties, as they put it overlook almost entirely. sciousness of sociologists, state that their aim "is
"Sexual selection, the competition for to argue the hypothesis that ethnicity, ethnoH
mates," Lopreato and Crippen affirm, "has centrism, and ethnic conflict hark back to preH
played the crucial role in the evolution of domiH historic, perhaps even prehuman times" (p.
nance orders" (p. 229). The persisting power of 248).
these inherited influences explains "the failure We cannot stop with that hypothesis howevH
of destratification efforts." Perhaps feeling a bit er. The term ethnic is now used so widely to refer

This content downloaded from 134.117.10.200 on Mon, 27 Jun 2016 09:38:54 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
758 theories and Epistemology

to large and heterogeneous groups that our Communicative Action, language is the pivot and
efforts to study the causes of their "unity" and turn of Habermas's theory out of Marxism, for it
the effects of their labels will be seriously wide of takes over where the earlier idea of praxis left
the mark if we have only a "prehistoric" concept. off. Where the later Marx substitutes labor for
We need more than a distinction between praxis, the earlier Marx is moved by the idea of
primordial and instrumental. Won't someone activity, where in principle, to read the story
reading this review invent a scale of ethnicity, backward, speech-act also counts as activity.
designating the several measures that determine Enter Habermas.
a group's placement along the range from "pure- Maeve Cooke gathers together papers that
ly ethnic" (perhaps a tribe in the headwaters of inform this research project, which itself grew to
the Amazon River or in the mountains of New metatheoretical dimensions as Habermas shifted
Guinea) to "barely ethnic," a convenient label away from rethinking critical theory to recon-
for a population with a few shared characteristics ceiving social science. In answer to the question
and facing similar problems? What difference would such an approach make,
for example, to the conduct of conversational
References
analysis? Habermas can only reply as expected,
Child, Charles M., 1924. Physiological Foundations of
indirectly. Abstraction, like theory in general,
Behavior. New York: Henry Holt.
Feagin, Joe. 1999. Chronicle of Higher Education. Oct. might sensitize us to matters that are not imma-
15. nent to analysis. In this regard, Habermas's
enthusiasm for formal pragmatics is entirely con-
sistent with the older project of critical theory.
On the Pragmatics of Communication, byJurgen So what's new? For sociologists who read
Habermas. Edited by Maeve Cooke. Structural Transformation of the Public Sphere or
Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, 1998. 454 pp. Legitimation Crisis, the most tangible essays here
$35.00 cloth. ISBN: 0-262-08265-9. are probably those that tangle with locals, like
Charles Taylor or Richard Rorty. Habermas is a
PETER BEILHARZ
stubborn interlocutor, though it is also fascinat-
Harvard University
ing to watch him think, show his wealth of intel-
beilharz(@wjh. harvard. ed u
lectual culture, revise his views in response to
criticism. Certainly the Teutonic inflection is
Why read this book, if you aren't already in the
clear, but who else among our leading thinkers
circle? Jurgen Habermas is brilliant, always has
do we see using formulations such as "my mis-
been, and well received if not canonical in
take . . ." or "as a sociologist I ought to have
American sociological theory. Judging by good
known . . ."-and even if these asides are
bookshop yardage, Habermas, together with
Bourdieu, counts. But this book would probably tongue in cheek, they at least show some sense

not be the place to start. It is dense and often of conversational detachment from our own
difficult. As Maeve Cooke aptly explains, occupational language games. There is a gen-
Habermas's formal pragmatics serves as the the- erosity of spirit in these conversations with
oretical underpinning of his theory of commu- Habermas, even if the density of the subject
nicative action, those two big books that matter overshadows it, and even as that density
somehow belong alongside Parsons's Structure of is broken by the incidental hilarity of
Social Action. Here the content is quite specifi- Habermas's propositional examples ("9. Please
cally linguistic, oriented to ongoing philosophi- bring me a glass of water. 9.1. No. You can't
cal discussion of truth, rationality, action, treat me like one of your employees," etc.).
meaning. Of course, utterance or exchange These essays by Habermas are worth reading,
counts mainly as example. Habermas's interest perhaps especially by those whose interests so
in the life world is systemic rather than experi- indicate. Some larger, contextual issues occur to
ential, so those more interested, say, in nonver- me, as I am reading them in Cambridge, visiting
bal action or power may find themselves for a year from Melbourne. Let me close on a
disappointed by the levels of abstraction broader note, to do with culture and reception.
involved. None of this is without irony, or para- The reception of thinkers such as Habermas in
dox, for language is at once the most opaque and the United States, to my mind, is part of a larg-
yet the most accessible of Habermas's interests. er phenomenon that looks from the outside like
Whatever happens to language after Theory of a mirror effect. At least since Tocqueville,

This content downloaded from 134.117.10.200 on Mon, 27 Jun 2016 09:38:54 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms

You might also like