Villas-Bôas2016 Article TheEffectsOfAnalysesOfContinge

You might also like

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 11

Psychol Rec (2016) 66:599–609

DOI 10.1007/s40732-016-0195-y

ORIGINAL ARTICLE

The Effects of Analyses of Contingencies on Clinically Relevant


Behaviors and Out-of-session Changes in Functional Analytic
Psychotherapy
Alessandra Villas-Bôas 1 & Sonia Beatriz Meyer 1 & Jonathan William Kanter 2

Published online: 23 August 2016


# Association for Behavior Analysis International 2016

Abstract Functional Analytic Psychotherapy (FAP) is a behavior, but did not seem to influence rates of CRB1 and
behavior-analytic psychotherapy approach that shapes client CRB2 or out-of-session problems or improvements. There
behavior through application of behavioral principles in the was no evidence from this study that analyses made by ther-
therapeutic relationship. Two categories of in-session clinical- apists or clients are necessary in addition to the clear shaping
ly relevant behavior (CRB) are recognized: CRB1 (problem process conducted by FAP.
behaviors) and CRB2 (improvements). The therapist watches
for CRBs (Rule 1), evokes (Rule 2) and responds to CRBs Keywords Behavior Therapy . Contingency Analysis .
(Rule 3) and observes the effects of his/her own behavior on Generalization . External Measures . Experimental Design .
CRBs (Rule 4). This sequence constitutes an experiential in- Coding
teraction in FAP. FAP also includes an analytic interaction, in
which the therapist analyzes the client’s behavior, teaches the
client to do so (CRB3) and asks him/her to engage in home- For years, behavior therapy has been conducted mainly by
work, promoting generalization (Rule 5). The goal of this analyzing and discussing with the client the way the client
investigation was to verify if analytic interventions in FAP behaves and could behave in daily life (Guilhardi, 1997;
are indeed necessary for generalization of in-session to out- Meyer, 2004; Zamignani, 2007), without the direct shaping
of-session improvements. Two clients were submitted to a of behaviors, which is one of the most important learning
single-case experimental procedure, A-B-BC-B-BC, in which processes described in behavior analysis (Meyer et al. 2015).
A corresponded to behavior therapy without systematic FAP; While direct shaping processes have been demonstrated as
B corresponded to the introduction of Rules 1 to 4; and BC to effective in research settings, in working with children and
the addition of Rule 5. Improvements in and out of session in controlled settings (Catania, 1998), clinicians have had dif-
were achieved with the experimental procedure and main- ficulties employing these procedures in less controlled, adult,
tained until a follow-up session. Instances of Rule 5 in the outpatient settings (Meyer et al. 2015).
BC phases influenced the clients’ analyses of their own Functional Analytic Psychotherapy (FAP; Kohlenberg &
Tsai, 1991; Tsai et al. 2009) is a behavioral therapeutic ap-
proach that targets the client’s interpersonal problems as they
occur in the therapeutic relationship, recovering the direct
* Alessandra Villas-Bôas shaping process (Meyer et al. 2015). The basic FAP under-
villasboas.alessandra@gmail.com standing is that a client´s in-session behaviors are functionally
similar to out-of-session behaviors, and thus a client´s inter-
1
personal problematic behavior (CRB1) will occur in the ther-
Departamento de Psicologia Clínica, Instituto de Psicologia,
Universidade de São Paulo, Av. Prof. Mello Moraes, 1721,
apeutic relationship. Thus, the therapist should evoke and re-
CEP: 05508-030, Cidade Universitária, São Paulo, SP, Brazil inforce interpersonal improvements (CRB2) in the client´s
2
University of Washington, Center for the Science of Social
repertoire, promoting clinical changes first in session to be
Connection, Guthrie Hall 119a, Box 351525, Seattle, WA 98195, generalized later to out-of-session contexts. The therapist
USA works with the client’s CRB1 and CRB2 during the session,
600 Psychol Rec (2016) 66:599–609

watching for the client’s CRBs (Rule 1), evoking them (Rule been specifically investigated, as mentioned by Maitland and
2) and responding to them in a natural way, especially rein- Gaynor (2012). Currently, only two publications describe the
forcing CRB2s (Rule 3) and observing the effects of his/her analytic interaction in more depth (Abreu, Hübner &
own behavior on CRBs (Rule 4). This sequence of four rules Lucchese, 2012; Villas-Bôas et al. 2015) and no experimental
describes the direct shaping process occurring during the ses- work focusing on it has been published.
sion, changing the client’s interpersonal behavior directly, and There is some support for FAP’s efficacy and its proposed
thereby represents FAP’s primary mechanism of change. This mechanism of action provided by single-case descriptive and
process can be described as experiential, as it primarily in- experimental studies. Callaghan et al. (2003, 2008) and Busch
volves working within the live flow of experience between et al. (2010) analyzed in-session therapist and client behavior
the client and the therapist in session. in successful FAP cases and demonstrated that the use of
A second set of interventions in FAP can be described as FAP’s experiential elements led to in-session improvements.
analytic, wherein the therapist functionally analyzes the client´ Kanter et al. (2006) published a successful case in which FAP
s behaviors (Rule 5) and also teaches the client to engage in was added to CBT and out-of-session improvements were
functional analyses about his/her own behavior. Client func- only observed after the addition of FAP, and Busch et al.
tional analyses of their own behavior are known as CRB3 in (2009) analyzed the in-session therapist and client behaviors
FAP. Together, Rule 5 and CRB3 analyses focus on describing in this case and demonstrated that FAP’s mechanism and im-
the contingencies related to positive changes in the session, provements in CRBs also only occurred in the FAP phase.
and should be used as base to develop homework assignments Oshiro, Kanter and Meyer (2012), Geremias (2014) and
that entail putting into daily-life action what worked in session Mangabeira (2014) conducted reversal designs comparing be-
(Rule 5; Villas-Bôas, Meyer, Kanter, & Callaghan, 2015). havior therapy to FAP. These authors showed better in-session
This focus on Rule 5 and CRB3 is consistent with how tradi- results when FAP was applied. In all of these studies, detailed
tional behavior therapy has approached the analysis, in that observations of in-session therapist and client behavior were
the focus is on analyzing and discussing the client’s behavior, conducted and results indicated that the main mechanism of
rather than direct shaping. However, in FAP, this analysis and action was probably the therapist’s contingent responding to
discussion typically occurs in the context of, and is directly CRBs (Rule 3).
linked to, the direct shaping process that is described by Rules These studies collectively suggest that FAP is beneficial
1 to 4. but lacks specificity with respect to how important specific
The main experiential interaction in FAP focuses on what FAP rules and therapeutic strategies are to the successful
Skinner (1966, 1969, 1974) described as contingency shaped process. As suggested by several authors (Maitland and
behavior, while the analytic interaction focuses on rule- Gaynor, 2012; Mangabeira, Kanter & Del Prette, 2012;
governed behavior. When considering the rule-following pro- Weeks et al., 2012), this specificity of research design is
cess as a way of changing behavior in therapy, it is important important because the precision with which FAP’s behav-
to consider the different kinds of rule-following behavior de- ioral mechanism has been described allows for research to
scribed by Hayes, Zettle and Rosenfarb (1989) and Törneke identify with equal precision what the therapist should or
(2010) as pliance, tracking and augmenting. As discussed by shouldn’t do, thereby maximizing the benefits of the re-
Villas-Bôas et al. (2015), it seems important that client rule- search for clinical practice. Several recent studies, howev-
governed behavior is consistent with tracking and augmenting er, have shed some light on the relative importance of spe-
functions, which would put the client in contact with the direct cific interventions within FAP. Popovitz (2013) conducted
environmental contingencies of his/her behavior, rather than FAP, also in a reversal procedure, isolating therapist re-
pliance functions, which only put the client in contact with sponses to CRB2 and CRB3 versus responses to CRB1
therapist approval. In this sense, the analytic interaction in and demonstrated the importance of responding specifical-
FAP, because it flows directly from the experiential interac- ly to CRB2 for in-session improvements. Mangabeira
tion, seems likely to promote tracking and augmenting func- (2014) analyzed two ways to evoke and respond to CRBs
tions, and, therefore, should be helpful to therapy, at least in in session, one that emphasized the personal involvement
theory. of the therapist and one that did not, and observed no
The experiential interaction of FAP has been discussed as differences in frequencies of CRBs depending of the two
extremely important to client improvement and most major forms of therapeutic responses. Landes et al. (2013) com-
FAP publications focus on it (Kohlenberg & Tsai, 1991; Tsai pared FAP to a non-specific relationship-oriented therapy
et al. 2009). The analytic interaction, in turn, has been in which the therapist did Rule 1 and the results showed
discussed as important to the generalization of a client’s in- that Rule 1 is not crucial to out-of-session improvements.
session improvements to other contexts (for more distinctions Finally, Lizarazo et al. (2015) manipulated the use of FAP
between the experiential and analytic interactions in FAP and rules, showing that Rule 2 was not crucial to in- or out-of-
their implications, see Villas-Bôas, et al. 2015) but has not session improvements when isolated from Rule 3.
Psychol Rec (2016) 66:599–609 601

To date, no studies have isolated the analytic interventions situations and emitting responses showing the recognition
of FAP (including Rule 5 and focusing on CRB3) to verify and acceptance of different ideas, opinions and suggestions.
how necessary they are, over and above the experiential inter- Client 2, BSol^, was a divorced 32-year-old woman, work-
ventions. This is especially relevant to out-of-session im- ing during the day in accounting and completing her under-
provements, as the analytic interventions are theoretically hy- graduate degree at night. She had no children and lived with a
pothesized to facilitate generalization of in-session gains to relative. No psychiatric diagnosis was detected. Sol’s present-
out-of-session contexts. From the studies that obtained data ing problems were responses emitted as conflict avoidance
on improvements in daily life, only Lizarazo et al. (2015) strategies when others made abusive or excessive requests,
published data on Rule 5 frequency, showing that it was emit- doing what they wanted, instead of saying no to these re-
ted with low frequency, precluding any analyses of the poten- quests. She also remained passive in situations when she could
tial benefits of Rule 5 and suggesting that it may not be essen- reach out to friends and share important interpersonal topics.
tial to the generalization process. Goal behaviors included being assertive with others, saying
Thus, although theoretically coherent, there is yet no evi- Bno^ to abusive and excessive requests and also reaching out
dence that the analytic interventions of FAP are necessary to to friends and increasing the connection with them. Sol’s de-
produce outcomes in daily life or are at least helpful to max- fined CRB1s were described as passive behaviors, avoiding
imize the effects of the experiential interventions. To this end, personal contact with the therapist and lack of in-session crit-
two cases are presented, each of which employed an A-B-BC- icism of abusive people in her life. CRB2s were defined as
B-BC design. In this design, A consisted of a behavior therapy requesting what she needed, more personal and intimate inter-
baseline in which no systematic FAP was conducted; B in- actions with the therapist and making critical judgments about
cluded the systematic introduction of the experiential inter- abusive people in her life.
ventions of FAP (Rules 1 to 4); and BC was the maintenance The therapist-researcher (AVB) was a behavior-analytic
of the experiential interventions plus the addition of analytic therapist with 10 years of clinical experience, trained in FAP
interventions (Rules 1 to 5). The hypothesis was that in- by the second (SM) and third authors (JWK). She received
session frequencies of CRB3 as well as out-of-session out- individual supervision for both cases during the experiment.
comes would improve in the BC phases, showing that the
analytic interventions facilitate the generalization of in- Data Collection Instruments
session improvements to out-of-session contexts.
For in-session analyses, sessions were audio recorded and the
Functional Analytic Psychotherapy Rating Scale (FAPRS;
Callaghan & Follette, 2008) was used in an adapted version
Method to code the therapist and client talk on a turn-by-turn basis. At
least five sessions were coded for each experimental phase
Participants and most sessions were coded before the occurrence of the
next one. The analyzed client codes were CRB1, CRB2 (as
Client 1, BDora^, was a 38-year-old single woman with two defined previously) and CRB3. CRB3s were defined for this
children and part-time employment as a teacher. No psychiat- research as client descriptions of CRB1s and CRB2s, restrict-
ric diagnosis was detected. Dora’s presenting problems were ed to the in-session interaction and therapeutic relationship
defined as aggressive behavior towards others when in con- and its parallels with outside session behaviors; analyses ex-
flict, and indirect aggression. Examples of aggressive behav- clusively about external behavior were not considered CRB3
ior included screaming at others or being extremely inflexible in this code. Other verbalizations not related to FAP were
about her wishes and ideas, temporarily reducing anger but coded but not analyzed in this report.
not effectively solving conflict situations. Examples of indi- The therapist behavior was coded as Rule 1 when the ther-
rect aggression included acting aggressively with others not apist explicitly checked the occurrence of CRB, and described
involved in the conflict, such as yelling at her children after a possible parallels between out-of-session behavior and CRB.
stressful day at work, avoiding new requests from others and Rule 2 was coded when the therapist directly evoked CRBs.
reducing temporally chances of new conflict situations. Goal Rule 3 was coded when the therapist responded to the client’s
behaviors included expressing her feelings and needs asser- CRB1 hopefully not in a reinforcing way, such as changing
tively without aggression or hostility. Dora’s defined CRB1s the subject or punishing it more directly. Therapist Rule 3
were aggressiveness against the therapist, such as strongly responses to CRB2 also were coded when the therapist
complaining about a small part of something that was said in attempted to reinforce the CRB2 in an explicit way or even
a previous session without considering the context, or being in a more indirect way, such as responding with an encour-
inflexible about the therapist’s suggestions or requests. agement for the client to keep emitting the CRB2. Rule 4 was
CRB2s were being assertive with the therapist in conflict coded when the therapist explicitly checked with the client on
602 Psychol Rec (2016) 66:599–609

the impact of previous responses to her behavior. Rule 5 was Table 1 Examples of coded FAP rules employed with each client
coded when the therapist requested or reinforced the clients’ Dora
analyses of in-session interactions or analyzed the therapeutic Rule 1
interaction herself (related to CRBs), including requesting and T: So, I will ask you something. If you think I am being invasive or
discussing parallels to outside problems and improvements tendentious, let me know. The way you told me this now was similar to
(consistent with how CRB3 was coded, analyses only about the way you would talk to your friend if she were being invasive or
tendentious?
external behavior isolated from the therapeutic relationship
Rule 2
were not included in this code). Examples of codes for each (Client had an aggressive tone of voice and talked about something that
rule with each client can be seen in Table 1 (translated from happened during the week which irritated her)
Portuguese). T: And how irritated are you with me right now?
The primary coder was the therapist-researcher. Although Rule 3
this introduced possible bias, the therapist’s detailed knowl- (Therapist requested Dora to complete some paperwork).
C: Let’s leave this for later. (CRB1)
edge of the case conceptualization was seen as helpful to the T: Sorry, but I need you to complete this before we start the session.
functional CRB identification, as proposed by Oshiro, Kanter C: I think it will be important for me to see your research after you finish
and Meyer (2012). Inter-rater agreement coding was conduct- it. No, I don’t think so, I’m sure it will be important. (CRB2)
ed by three experienced FAP therapist/researchers who were T: I really respect the clarity with which you are telling me this, and your
wish to see it. And, sure, I will show you, I have no objections to this.
not independent from the therapist. They used the therapist’s C: Now, I don’t have an answer for everything, but then I ask you… are
case conceptualization and the therapist’s provided codes you going to talk with them? Are you going to discuss this with them?
when reading the transcribed sessions to check the therapist’s (CRB2)
codes and provide gold-standard (Bbest available^) ratings T: You are asking me to talk to them, right? (Rule 1)
C: Yes, I’m asking you. (CRB2)
(Versi, 1992). A strength of the gold-standard procedure is T: So, I will. And you can ask me how it was next week and I will tell you.
that the agreement evaluators had available to them as much Rule 4
information as possible to produce the most valid ratings. The T: And did you leave here feeling better? I was wondering how you were
weakness is that because they had the primary coder’s ratings after our interaction.
as part of their information, their ratings were not independent Rule 5
and may be biased in favor of high agreement. To produce T: Can you understand what was good for you in the last session, what
made you feel so good?
agreement ratings, each evaluator coded three sessions, each
C: I believe it was the possibility to talk to you about my uncomfortable
from a different phase of the research, with some overlap feelings without arguing. (CRB3)
between the sessions coded. Kappa was calculated separately T: It is good to hear this answer. Because I believe this was the biggest
for client and therapist codes and all checked sessions provid- gain from last week.
T: And how does this work? Why is it that you can talk to me and not to
ed a kappa agreement higher than 0.73 for therapist codes and
others?
higher than 0.85 for client codes. These results indicate high C: Well, because I believe you know me a little. So, I don’t know, if I say
agreement between evaluators and the primary coder with BI don’t want to talk about this^ I believe you understand a little bit and
respect to both therapist codes and client codes. respect me. My friend does know me and, even so, she frequently
insists. And keeps questioning. And sometimes I say Bstop it! Do
Out-of-session behavior was monitored by clients using
whatever you want!^ (CRB3)
self-report diary card tracking, as per Kanter et al. (2006), T: And do you think you talk openly with her as you talk to me?
Landes et al. (2013) and Lizarazo et al. (2015) with some T: Yes, and you are doing a lot in session, right? You are asking me things,
modifications. The client and the therapist collaboratively de- you are delineating the limits with me, as you did several times today.
(…) And I believe that somehow you are learning how to do it with
termined the specific daily life problems (DLPs) to monitor
others, too. You are letting me know the limits and I have tried to
over the course of the initial sessions of the study based on a respect them as much as I can. And you told me you also did it with
FAP case conceptualization. Once the DLPs were defined, the your friend.
therapist trained the client to track the frequency of the DLPs Sol
and clients were asked to record DLPs as close to the time of Rule 1
occurrence as possible, at least on a daily basis. The therapist T: And is it difficult to tell me this?
did not look at the diary cards until the end of the experiment. Rule 2
Because the therapist was also the primary researcher who T: What do you think about what she said? Was it really an apology or
not…? (Therapist is trying to evoke some more critical judgment about
knew the experimental hypotheses, this precaution was imple- abusive people.)
mented to avoid any possible influence of the therapist- T: Yes, and I’m going to travel next week. So, actually this is our last
researcher on the DLP data. Dora had a friend collect addi- session before my trip. And I will be back only next month. How do
tional data about her out-of-session behavior, the same DLPs you feel about this, that for one month we are not going to see each
other?
that the client coded. Sol demonstrated repeated difficulties in
Rule 3
recording behavior accurately; her data is therefore not (Therapist talked about how she feels about the client)
reported.
Psychol Rec (2016) 66:599–609 603

Table 1 (continued) Villas-Bôas et al. (2015). In other words, analyses about out-
C: I believe I can’t accept this… or act this way. (CRB1) of-session behaviors could happen, but analyses about the
T: Right now, I just talked about how much I admire you, right? How did therapeutic relationship and parallels between this and out-
you feel listening about my admiration?
of-session behaviors were directly avoided.
C: Yes. A big issue of mine is this difficulty to accept wrong things. And I
know I’m getting better. And the fact that I would have our session In phase BC (full FAP condition), the whole experiential
today, made me much calmer, so, I thought a lot about how grateful I interaction of FAP was maintained and analyses about the
am to you. A lot… (CRB2) therapeutic relationship and its parallels with out-of-session
T: Yes, I see all this difference… and to be able to be part of this is a
behavior were added. In addition, the therapist encouraged
pleasure to me, it is very important, and it is really good to see you
feeling that you are getting better. And to see you talking, expressing and evoked the client to engage in these kinds of analyses
this is even more special! Because I know this is difficult for you! And (CRB3) and reinforced the occurrences of these analyses.
to see that you are able to talk this in such a beautiful way… wow! Providing homework was directly avoided, as the analytic
C: Yeah… and I was thinking, wondering, what a big difference would be
interaction studied here was the functional analysis of the
to me to tell you this (…).(CRB2)
T: It is very special for me that you are talking about it. I believe it is therapeutic relationship and not the direct request to try to
important for you to tell me this, and to hear you expressing yourself apply the new behavior in other contexts.
this way. To see you interacting so at ease is very special for me. Thank Phases B and BC each had eight sessions for Dora and
you for letting me know that you are feeling this way. (…)
seven for Sol. The end of the phase could be postponed in
C: I know I will miss the sessions, but I can write to you, as you told me.
(CRB2) case of big increases or decreases of CRBs or in cases of some
T: I really want to keep in touch if this suits you, but I don’t want you critical moments in the client’s life, as happened at the end of
feeling that you need to write to me. You can use this help, I am willing phase BC1 for Dora. As mentioned before, at least five ses-
to give it to you. I will be far away, but I’m close, you know?
sions of each phase were coded.
Rule 4 Around 6 months after the end of the procedure, a follow-
(Therapist made some self-disclosure in response to the client’s risk to get
emotionally closer to the therapist) up session was conducted, consistent with phase A. That way,
T: I want to ask you how it was to hear about my own battles. Does it it was possible to verify the maintenance of the in-session
make any difference? gains of therapy without systematic FAP interventions.
Rule 5 During these 6 months, therapy was still being conducted,
T: Yes, I’m very happy with this. That there is no script. This certainty that although without following systematic procedures.
there is no disappointment here in session. And maybe with your
mother and your boyfriend this process would be much more difficult.
As I said, I believe that the more you try not to worry about them, the
more they will complain. It is probably going to be difficult at first.
And I am wondering with whom it would be easier to do something Results
similar to what you do with me, not to worry, to be yourself.
T: And what do you think helped you to talk? I mean, what is missing in
your daily life to help you to express yourself there the way you do Implementation of FAP in B and BC Phases
here? What do you have here that you don’t have there?
Table 2 presents specific FAPRS codes as a percentage of total
therapist codes, summarized across each experimental phase.
Procedure Figure 1 presents these percentages, session-by-session, for
the key codes of Rule 3 and Rule 5 only. Because the percent-
During an initial period of therapy, a therapeutic relationship age of Rule 3 is considerably higher than the percentage of
was established, a case conceptualization was developed and Rule 5, each of them has a different scale, so that the right
CRBs and DLPs were defined. This period was longer for scale represents Rule 3 and the left one represents Rule 5.
Dora due to some difficulties in defining behaviorally the Figure 1 shows that the percentage of Rule 3 was higher
problem behaviors; it lasted 24 sessions, while for Sol it lasted overall in all phases after phase A, especially for Dora. For
10 sessions. After this period, the experimental period was her, the percentage of Rule 3 did not reach 30 % in any base-
initiated. line session, and was higher than 30 % throughout phases B
A single-case experimental design was used in this research and BC (except for session 6 in phase BC2). For Sol, phase A
that can be described as A-B1-BC1-B2-BC2. During phase A occurrences of Rule 3 were more frequent than with Dora
(baseline condition), five sessions of behavior therapy were already, but it was lower than 45 % in most session of this
conducted, without systematic FAP interventions. CRBs were first phase and higher than that in most sessions of phases B
not directly evoked or responded to, although they could oc- and BC. Regarding Rule 5, there were almost no occurrences
cur naturally. of it in phases A and B (except for session 2 of phase A for
In phase B (experiential interactions condition), FAP inter- Dora — 0.4 % — and session 3 of phase B1 for both clients —
ventions were employed in a consistent way, with the therapist around 1 %). Moreover, during phases BC it is possible to see
engaging in FAP Rules 1 to 4. Rule 5 was not presented as per a higher frequency of Rule 5 in most of the sessions.
604 Psychol Rec (2016) 66:599–609

Table 2 Percentage (of total therapist and client codes, respectively) of ding at the follow-up session, without any evident or
each rule and each CRB per experimental phase, for Dora and Sol (F =
systematic differences between phases B and BC.
follow-up)
Figure 3 presents the Rule 5 and CRB3 percentages for
Dora Sol all sessions. It is possible to see that there is an increase in
the clients’ analyses about therapeutic relationship at the
A B1 BC1 B2 BC2 F A B1 BC1 B2 BC2 F
moments when the therapist invested more in in-session
Rule 1 0 0.2 0 0.1 0.1 0 0 0.8 0.5 0.5 1.1 0 analyses, in the BC phases. In Dora’s case, it is possible
Rule 2 1.8 2 2.7 1.6 1.6 2 5.2 9.3 5.9 7.6 7.5 2.6 to observe small increases in CRB3 during these phases,
Rule 3 19 40 43 48 42 32 39 52 44 55 45 44 with higher frequency than most phase B sessions, where
Rule 4 0.1 0.2 0.1 0 0.1 0 0 0.1 0.3 0.2 1.5 0 no analyses were emitted in most sessions. In Sol´s case,
Rule 5 0.1 0.2 1.6 0 2.7 0 0 0.1 4.8 0 3 1.1 there is a bigger increase in CRB3s in the BC phases than
CRB1 17 18 14 15 11 4 31 16 5.6 11 12 2.1 observed for Dora, when a higher percentage of Rule 5 was
CRB2 13 26 34 35 35 31 15 37 42 44 35 42 presented.
CRB3 0.2 0.2 0.4 0 1.9 0 0 0.3 2.1 0 2.2 0.5
Frequency of Out-of-Session Behavior

Figure 4 shows DLP data from diary cards recorded by


Frequency of In-Session CRBs Dora and the external observer. It is possible to see an
increase in both behaviors recorded by Dora when FAP
Table 2 also presents specific FAPRS codes for client was implemented (phase B1), but this may be due to in-
behavior (CRB1, CRB2, and CRB3) as a percentage creased awareness about these behaviors after she started
of total client codes, summarized across each experi- to record them. The external observer’s records, showing
mental phase, and Fig. 2 presents CRB1s and CRB2s that both behaviors became stable from A to B1 phase, also
at the session-by-session level. support this. It is also possible to see that both problem
It is possible to see that for both clients CRB1 was behaviors recorded by Dora and the observer decreased
predominant over CRB2 during the baseline sessions. during the FAP phases. There is a reduction of all behav-
When FAP was introduced, at phase B1, the total iors from phase B1 to phase BC1, except for indirect ag-
amount of CRB2s increased. For Dora, at session 4 gression recorded by the external observer, but there is no
of phase B1, CRB2 became more frequent than evidence of increases in these behaviors in phase B2 or
CRB1, while for Sol CRB2s become more prominent decreases in phase BC2. Thus, there is no evidence that
at the first B1 phase session. CRB2 remained higher there are systematic differences between the B and BC
for both clients for the rest of the experiment, inclu phases.

Fig. 1 Percentage Rule 3 (scale Dora


8 80
Rule 5 percentage

Rule 3 percentage
on the right) and Rule 5 (scale on 7 70
6 60
the left) codes for all sessions 5 50
rated for Dora and Sol 4 40
(F = follow-up) 3 30
2 20
1 10
0 0
1 2 3 4 5 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8
A B1 BC1 B2 BC2 F
Experimental phases
Rule 5 Rule 3

Sol
12 100
Rule 5 percentage

Rule 3 percentage

10 80
8
60
6
40
4
2 20
0 0
1 2 3 4 5 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
A B1 BC1 B2 BC2 F
Experimental phases
Rule 5 Rule 3
Psychol Rec (2016) 66:599–609 605

Fig. 2 Percentage of CRB1 and Dora


50
CRB2 codes through 40

Percentage
experimental sessions, for Dora 30
and Sol (F = follow-up)
20
10
0
1 2 3 4 5 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8
A B1 BC1 B2 BC2 F
Experimental phases
CRB1 CRB2

Sol
70
60

Percentage
50
40
30
20
10
0
1 2 3 4 5 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
A B1 BC1 B2 BC2 F
Experimental phases
CRB1 CRB2

Clinical Impressions of Improvement Sol: Both the therapist and Sol described that she be-
came more assertive with others and better at not com-
plying with abusive and excessive requests. Sol reported
that this effectively resulted in a decrease in the requests
Dora: Both the therapist and Dora reported strongly made by others. The client also started to reach out for
that her life improved during the procedure, describ- friends, inviting them sometimes to go out and
ing that Dora’s aggressive behaviors decreased and accepting more invitations. Sol did not appear to under-
assertive behaviors increased. Consistent with the di- stand the whole functional class seen by the therapist,
ary cards, the therapist did not believe that Dora’s recording only specific topographies (like saying Bno"
improvements behaviors were related to changes in to abusive requests) and not others which changed more
the experimental phase. rapidly (like doing things for herself instead of offering

Fig. 3 Percentage of Rule 5 and Dora


10
CRB3 codes, throughout 8
Percentage

experimental sessions, for Dora 6


and Sol (F = follow-up)
4
2
0
1 2 3 4 5 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8
A B1 BC1 B2 BC2 F
Experimental phases

CRB3 Rule 5

Sol
12
10
Percentage

8
6
4
2
0
1 2 3 4 5 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
A B1 BC1 B2 BC2 F
Experimental phases
CRB3 Rule 5
606 Psychol Rec (2016) 66:599–609

Fig. 4 Weekly average 1.6 3.5

Weekly average occurrence


occurrence of responses recorded 1.4 3
by Dora per experimental phase 1.2 2.5
(continuous line — scale on left); 1

Score
2
and observations of the external 0.8
observer (dashed line — scale on 0.6
1.5
right) 0.4 1

0.2 0.5
0 0
A B1 BC1 B2 BC2
Experimental phases
Dora Agression Dora Indirect agression
External Observer Agression External Observer Indirect agression

unnecessary help for others). Even so, the clinical im- change, for Dora). In addition, the data presented from the
pressions were that the client’s out-of-session behaviors BC1 and BC2 phases did not provide evidence of a consistent
improved during the experiment, but with no evidence effect of Rule 5 or CRB3 on client behavior. In that sense,
of differences between the B and BC phases. there is no evidence from this data that the analytic interaction
maximized the gains produced by the experiential interaction.
Although the current findings do not find evidence for the
impact of the analytic interaction on outcomes, it is important
Discussion to note that this design did not evaluate the role of analytic
interactions in a therapy focused on teaching the client to
The results suggest that the experiment was conducted as analyze their behavior and the environmental contingencies
planned for each experimental phase, with higher rates of as the primary focus. Rather, this design explored if the ana-
Rule 3 after the A phase, and rates of Rule 5 increasing and lytic interaction of FAP complements and adds to the experi-
decreasing in the appropriate phases for both clients. Results ential interaction of FAP, as per theoretical writings on FAP
indicate that the introduction of FAP had a positive impact on and the role of Rule 5 as part of the larger intervention. The
CRBs, which was maintained at the follow-up session, and the role of such analyses in improvements in FAP therapy process
introduction of FAP had a positive impact on out-of-session is still unsubstantiated, at least with respect to how the analysis
behavior (for Dora), corresponding to observed clinical im- was implemented in this research and in the way the improve-
provements. The addition of analytic techniques increased the ments were recorded.
frequency of CRB3s in the BC phases. However, the change As per the current findings, it is possible to consider that
in frequency of therapist Rule 5 behavior and client CRB3 when FAP is consistently and systematically implemented,
behavior did not seem to have a differential impact on analyses about this process are secondary and add little to
CRB1s or CRB2s or on out-of-session behaviors (per client the experiential interaction. As suggested by others
self-report and diary cards). (Abreu-Rodrigues & Sanabio-Heck, 2004; Matos, 2001;
The changes in the percentages of CRB1 and CRB2 Meyer, 2005), when there are clear contingencies present
(Fig. 2) after the A phase, combined with the high rates of during the experiential interaction, the supplementation by
Rule 3 (much higher than any other FAP rule percentage) in rules may not make a big impact on the behavior. Simple
these phases, suggest the importance of Rule 3 and the expe- schedules of reinforcement, like continuous reinforcement
riential interaction, rather than Rule 5 and the analytic inter- schedules, present a higher discriminability level than in-
action, to the in-session changes. Rule 3 is seen as the primary termittent and mixed schedules, showing that there are dis-
mechanism of in-session behavior change in FAP, both in criminative properties in reinforcement schedules that fa-
terms of the theoretical literature (Follette & Bonow, 2009; cilitate contingency discrimination (Cerutti, 1991; Abreu-
Follette, Naugle & Calaghan, 1996; Kohlenberg & Tsai, Rodrigues and Sanabio-Heck, 2004; Meyer, 2005). This is
1991; Tsai et al. 2009, 2012) and the existing empirical evi- consistent with the hypothesis of Follette, Naugle and
dence (Callaghan et al. 2003; Busch et al. 2009, 2010; Callaghan (1996) about this process in FAP, who argued
Geremias, 2014; Kanter et al. 2006; Lizarazo et al. 2015; that very high frequencies of contingent responses to the
Mangabeira, 2014; Meurer, 2011; Oshiro et al. 2012; client’s behavior in therapy increase the chance of learning
Popovitz, 2013; Xavier et al. 2012). The present results add more effective responses than when the feedback happens
more evidence suggesting the power of the experiential inter- at random intervals (very intermittent schedules of rein-
action in FAP, with Rule 3 at the center of this interaction, to forcement). These higher frequency schedules may be
produce in-session clinical change (and out-of-session more likely at the beginning of therapy.
Psychol Rec (2016) 66:599–609 607

In this research, CRB2s were reinforced with high frequen- riment, it is not possible to generalize these results to the larger
cy and CRB1s were systematically punished or not reinforced population; only principles can be derived.
(extinction). In-session contingencies were probably clear to One may argue that Rule 5 should have occurred at a higher
the clients and easily discriminable even when not explicitly frequency; thus, the analytic interaction was not adequately
described. As the shaping process was constantly conducted tested in this research. It is difficult to determine what an ideal
in session and the environmental contingencies were clear, percentage of Rule 5 would be in a FAP session. Only two
perhaps the need for supplemental control by rules was empirical studies on FAP have described the amount of Rule 5
minimal. in FAP sessions (Geremias, 2014; Lizarazo et al. 2015). Both
Considering the small amount of data collected in this re- reported a percentage or frequency of Rule 5 similar to or
search and the limitations of the methodology, it may be that lower than those presented in this investigation. Thus, there
the experiential interaction in FAP is enough to produce is no evidence in the literature that FAP sessions have been or
changes in some cases, and the analytic interaction may be should be conducted with a higher amount of Rule 5 than was
only a needed supplementation in some cases, but not neces- done in this study. It also was expected that Rules 1 to 4 would
sary for the expected changes. But more data are needed to be similar in the B and BC phases, as the experiential interac-
confirm these hypotheses. tion of FAP should not suffer from the addition of the analytic
Another issue is that functional analysis of the client´s in- interaction; thus, it would be difficult to produce rates of Rule
session improvements can be helpful in creating clear and 5 much higher than they were. These results indicate that FAP
objective homework assignments, which is another element was conducted in a natural way in the BC phases, exceeding in
of Rule 5 (Tsai et al, 2009); but this was not included in the some sessions the percentage presented in other studies.
current research. The current research measured the effects of However, it may be the case that, if FAP can be conducted
explicit functional analyses of behavior in session, seen as a appropriately and with higher rates of Rule 5 emissions, these
central mechanism of change within behavior analysis and analyses may show a greater impact on client behavior.
consistent with how Rule 5 was originally defined in FAP The low frequency of DLPs could make it difficult to see an
(which did not include homework; Kohlenberg & Tsai, impact of Rule 5 and CRB3 interventions. The diary cards
1991), versus homework which is common in behavior ther- included only DLPs and did not include daily life improve-
apy and cognitive-behavioral approaches. Perhaps, therefore, ments, because Dora had difficulties discriminating her im-
the addition of concrete homework as part of Rule 5 would provements when they were only starting to happen. The re-
have resulted in observable improvements during the analytic cording of those improvements would help to clarify the re-
phases. sults found in this research. The fact that Sol did not provide
One last issue to be addressed is that, although the therapist reliable diary card data is also a shortcoming of this study.
did not explicitly generate or teach rules about in-session im- Unlike studies by Kanter et al. (2006), Landes et al. (2013)
provements and their out-of-session parallels during the B and Lizarazo et al. (2015), during this study, the therapist did
phases, the clients still could have abstracted rules from the not influence the diary card reporting during the experimental
in-session experiences without an explicit focus on doing so. procedure, decreasing the influence that the therapist can have
However, if this was the case and if these abstracted rules had on the data, but losing the chance to correct possible inaccu-
an impact on behavior, it possibly would be expected that racies that may have happened in Sol’s or even Dora’s report.
client would emit more CRB2 in the B2 phase, compared to The client had also completed the diary cards for a long peri-
the B1 phase. Even so, it is possible that the clients had ab- od, without any pauses, contrary to what was recommended
stracted these rules in both B phases as a rationale was pro- by Bohm and Gimenes (2008).
vided at the beginning of therapy, suggesting that in session Other limitations of the current study were that the therapist
improvements would be generated, presumably in the service was the primary rater, and also how inter-rater agreement was
of out-of-session improvements. Regardless, whether or not calculated. Although very high levels of agreement were
this process occurred within the client does not speak to the reached, the coders were not blind to the codes provided by
issue of whether it is required to specifically and explicitly the therapist, such that they only checked them instead of
engage in Rule 5 behavior. coding it independently. This introduces the possibility that
the coding was biased by the therapist-researcher coder and
Research Limitations this bias was not sufficiently checked by the reliability proce-
dure, obscuring the true nature of the data and reflecting ther-
Both participants of this study were similar in terms of gender, apist biases instead. The fact that the findings run counter to
presenting problems, cultural context and education levels. It the unique hypotheses of this study (i.e., that there was no
is possible that analytic interventions would function dif evidence for the impact of the analytic interventions of FAP
ferently with clients with different difficulties or contexts. on CRBs) but are consistent with the previous research (i.e.,
Also, it needs to be considered that as a single-case expe that there is evidence for the impact of the experiential
608 Psychol Rec (2016) 66:599–609

interactions of FAP) mitigates, but does not eliminate, con- Bohm, C. H., & Gimenes, L. S. (2008). Automonitoramento como técnica
terapêutica e de avaliação comportamental. Revista Psicologia, 1(1), 88–
cerns about possible bias due to unblinded coders significant-
100.
ly. It is strongly suggested that blinded coders be employed in Busch, A. M., Kanter, J. W., Callaghan, G. M., Baruch, D. E., Weeks, C.
future research. E., & Berlin, K. S. (2009). A micro-process analysis of Functional
Analytic Psychotherapy’s mechanism of chance. Behavior Therapy,
40, 280–290.
Busch, A. M., Callaghan, G. M., Kanter, J. W., Baruch, D. E., & Weeks, C. E.
Conclusion (2010). The Functional Analytic Psychotherapy Rating Scale: A replica-
tion and extension. Journal of Contemporary Psychotherapy, 40, 11–19.
In summary, the current study found that across two success- doi:10.1007/s10879-009-9122-8.
Callaghan, G. M., & Follete, W. C. (2008). FAPRS MANUAL: Manual for
ful FAP cases, the introduction of experiential interventions the Functional Analytic Psychotherapy Rating Scale. The Behavior
produced observable improvements in CRBs and correspond- Analyst Today, 9(1), 57–97.
ing out-of-session behavior (in one case). The introduction of Callaghan, G. M., Summers, C. J., & Weidman, M. (2003). The Treatment of
analytic interventions produced observable increases in the Histrionic and Narcissistic Personality Disorder Behaviors: A Single-
Subject Demonstration of Clinical Improvement Using Functional
frequencies of the clients’ CRB3s, suggesting that clients pro- Analytic Psychotherapy. Journal of Contemporary Psychotherapy,
duce more analyses about the therapeutic relationship when 33(4), 321–339.
the therapist evokes this behavior and engages in it as well. Callaghan, G. M., Follette, W. C., Ruckstuhl, L. E., Jr., & Linnerooth, P. J.
There was no evidence, however, that analytic interventions in N. (2008). The Functional Analytic Psychotherapy Rating Scale
(FAPRS): A Behavioral Psychotherapy Coding System. The
FAP made an impact on problems or improvements in or out Behavior Analyst Today, 9(1), 98–116.
of session. Possibly, when contingencies are clear, the addition Catania, A. C. (1998). Learning. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.
of analytic interventions is not important to outside improve- Cerrutti, D. T. (1991). Discriminative versus reinforcing properties of
ments. Overall, this research presents more evidence that FAP schedules as determinants of schedule insensivity in humans. The
Psychological Record, 41, 51–67.
has a positive impact and Rule 3 matters. The current conclu- Follette, W. C., & Bonow, J. T. (2009). The Challenge of Understanding
sion is that Rule 5 does not have an additional effect on clin- Process in Clinical Behavior Analysis: The Case of Functional
ical improvements, but this conclusion is tentative. Future Analytic Psychotherapy. The Behavior Analyst, 32, 135–148.
research may address current limitations by implementing Follette, W. C., Naugle, A. E., & Callaghan, G. M. (1996). A Radical
Behavioral Understanding of the Therapeutic Relationship in
the analytic interventions at higher rates, addressing home- Effecting Change. Behavior Therapy, 27, 623–641.
work requests based on these analyses, by improving the qual- Geremias, M. C. G. (2014). Manejo de esquivas emocionais na
ity of out-of-session data collection, and by obtaining more Psicoterapia Analítica Funcional: delineamento experimental de
independent observations of CRBs and FAP rules. caso único (Dissertação de Mestrado). São Paulo: Instituto de
Psicologia, Universidade de São Paulo.
Guilhardi, H. J. (1997). Com que contingências o terapeuta trabalha em sua
atuação clínica. In R. A. Banaco (Ed.), Sobre Comportamento e
Acknowledgments Research was funded by São Paulo Research
Cognição (Vol. 1, pp. 322–337). São Paulo: ARBytes.
Foundation (FAPESP), grant #2010/19816-2.
Hayes, S. C., Zettle, R. D., & Rosenfarb, R. (1989). Rule-following. In S.
C. Hayes (Ed.), Rule-governed behavior: Cognition, contingencies,
and instructional control (pp. 191–220). NY: Plenum Press.
Compliance with Ethical Standards The paper has not been pre-
Kanter, J. W., Landes, S. J., Busch, A. M., Rusch, L. C., Brown, K. R.,
viously published and has not been and will not be submitted elsewhere.
Baruch, D. E., & Holman, G. (2006). The effect of contingent rein-
Research was funded by the São Paulo Research Foundation
forcement on target variables in outpatient psychotherapy for de-
(FAPESP), grant #2010/19816-2.The authors declare that they have no
pression: A successful and unsuccessful case using functional ana-
conflicts of interest.
lytic psychotherapy. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 39, 463–
All procedures performed in studies involving human participants
467.
were in accordance with the ethical standards of the institutional and/or
Kohlenberg, R. J., & Tsai, M. (1991). Functional analytic psychotherapy:
national research committee and with the 1964 Helsinki Declaration and
A guide for creating intense and curative therapeutic relationships.
its later amendments or comparable ethical standards.
NY: Plenum.
Informed consent was obtained from all individual participants inclu-
Landes, S. J., Kanter, J. W., Weeks, C. E., & Busch, A. M. (2013). The
ded in the study.
impact of the active components of functional analytic psychother-
apy on idiographic target behaviors. Journal of Contextual
Behavioral Science, 2, 49–57.
References Lizarazo, N. E., Muñoz-Martínez, A. M., Santos, M. M., & Kanter, J. K.
(2015). A Within Subjects Evaluation of the Effects of Functional
Analytic Psychotherapy on In-Session and Out-of-Session Client
Abreu, P. A., Hübner, M. M. C., & Lucchese, F. (2012). The Role of Shaping Behavior. The Psychological Record, 65(3), 463–474.
the Client’s Interpretations in Functional Analytic Psychotherapy, 28, Maitland, D. W. M., & Gaynor, S. T. (2012). Promoting Efficacy
151–157 Research on Functional Analytic Psychotherapy. International
Abreu-Rodrigues, J., & Sanabio-Heck, E. T. (2004). Instruções e auto- Journal of Behavioral Consultation and Therapy, 7(2–3), 63–71.
instruções: contribuições da pesquisa básica. In C. N. de Abreu & H. J. Mangabeira, V. (2014). Efeitos da sinalização de intervenções na psicoterapia
Guilhardi (Eds.), Terapia comportamental e cognitivo-comportamental analítica funcional (Tese de Doutorado). São Paulo: Instituto de
(Vol. 1, pp. 152–168). São Paulo: Roca. Psicologia, Universidade de São Paulo.
Psychol Rec (2016) 66:599–609 609

Mangabeira, V., Kanter, J. W., & Del Prette, G. (2012). Functional Analytic Skinner, B. F. (1969). Contingencies of Reinforcement: A Theoretical
Psychotherapy (FAP): A review of publications from 1990 to 2010. Analysis. NY: Meredith Corporation.
International Journal of Behavioral Consultation and Therapy, 7(2–3), Skinner, B. F. (1974). About behaviorism. NY: Vintage books.
78–89. Törneke, N. (2010). Learning RFT: An Introduction to Relational Frame
Matos, M. A. (2001). Comportamento governado por regras. Revista Theory and Its Clinical Application. Oakland, CA: Context Press.
Brasileira de Terapia Comportamental e Cognitiva, 3(2), 51–66. Tsai, M., Kohlenberg, R. J., Kanter, J. W., Kohlenberg, B., Follette, W. C.,
Meurer. P. H. (2011). Efeito da apresentação de feedback no comportamento & Callaghan, G. M. (2009). A guide to Functional Analytic
do terapeuta de evocar e responder aos comportamentos clinicamente Psychotherapy: Awareness, courage, love and behaviorism. New
relevantes (Dissertação de Mestrado). Curitiba: Universidade Federal do York: Spring.
Paraná. Tsai, M., Kohlenberg, R. J., Kanter, J. W., Holman, G. I., & Loudon, M. P.
Meyer, S. B. (2004). Processos comportamentais na psicoterapia. In A. C. (2012). Functional Analytic Psychotherapy. Cornwall: TJ International
Cruvinel, A. L. F. Dias, & E. N. de Cillo. (Orgs.). Ciência do Ltd.
Comportamento: conhecer e Avançar (Vol. 4, pp. 151–157). Santo
Versi, E. (1992). BGold standard^ is an appropriate term. British Medical
André: ESETec.
Journal, 305(6846), 187.
Meyer, S. B. (2005). Regras e auto-regras no laboratório e na clínica. In J.
Abreu-Rodrigues & M. R. Ribeiro (Eds.), Análise do Comportamento: Villas-Bôas, A., Meyer, S., Kanter, J., & Callaghan, G. (2015). The use of
pesquisa, teoria e aplicação (pp. 211–227). Porto Alegre: ARTMED. Analytic Interventions in Functional Analytic Psychotherapy. Behavior
Meyer, S. B., Villas-Bôas, A., Franceschini, A. C. T., Oshiro, C. K. B., Analysis: Research and Practice, 15(1), 1–19. doi:10.1037/h0101065.
Kameyama, M., Rossi, P. R., & Mangabeira, V. (2015). Terapia Weeks, C. E., Kanter, J. W., Bonow, J. D., Landes, S. J., & Busch, A. M.
Analítico-Comportamental: relato de casos & de análises. São Paulo: (2012). Translating the Theoretical Into Practical: A Logical
Paradigma Centro de Ciências e Tecnologia do Comportamento. Framework of Functional Analytic Psychotherapy Interactions for
Oshiro, C. K. B., Kanter, J. W., & Meyer, S. B. (2012). A Single-Case Research, Training and Clinical Purposes. Behavior Modification,
Experimental Demonstration of Functional Analytic Psychotherapy 36(1), 87–119. doi:10.1177/0145445511422830.
with Two Clients with Severe Interpersonal Problems. International Xavier, R. N., Kanter, J. W., & Meyer, S. B. (2012). Transitional
Journal of Behavioral Consultation and Therapy, 7(2–3), 111–116. Probability Analysis of Two Child Behavior Analytic Therapy
Popovitz, J. M. B. (2013). A resposta contingente do terapeuta aos Cases. International Journal of Behavioral Consultation and
comportamentos clinicamente relevantes: especificação e Therapy, 7(2–3), 182–188.
avaliação (Dissertação de Mestrado). Curitiba: Universidade Zamignani, D. R. (2007). O desenvolvimento de um sistema multidimen-
Federal do Paraná. sional para a categorização e análise de comportamentos na
Skinner, B. F. (1966). An operant analysis of problem solving. In B. interação terapeuta-cliente (Tese de Doutorado). São Paulo:
Kleinmuntz (Ed.), Problem solving: Research, method and theory (pp. Universidade de São Paulo.
133–171). New York: John Wiley & Sons.

You might also like