Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 10

Behavioural Science Section / Original Paper

Gerontology Received: January 2, 2019


Accepted after revision: May 27, 2019
DOI: 10.1159/000501168 Published online: July 22, 2019

Exploring Cognitive Frailty: Prevalence and


Associations with Other Frailty Domains in
Older People with Different Degrees of
Cognitive Impairment
Ellen E. De Roeck a, b, d Anne van der Vorst c Sebastiaan Engelborghs b, f
G.A. Rixt Zijlstra c Eva Dierckx a, e D-SCOPE Consortium
a Developmental and Lifespan Psychology, Vrije Universiteit Brussel, Brussels, Belgium; b Laboratory of

Neurochemistry and Behavior, Institute Born-Bunge, University of Antwerp, Antwerp, Belgium; c Department of
Health Services Research, CAPHRI Care and Public Health Research Institute, Maastricht University,
Maastricht, The Netherlands; d Department of Neurology and Memory Clinic, Hospital Network Antwerp (ZNA)
Middelheim and Hoge Beuken, Antwerp, Belgium; e Alexian Psychiatric Hospital, Tienen, Belgium; f Department of
Neurology, VUB University Hospital Brussels (UZ Brussel), Brussels, Belgium, Brussels, Belgium

Keywords creased risk of frailty (n = 95); and (3) a sample of memory


Cognitive frailty · Cognitive decline · Multidimensional clinic patients who scored 0.5 on the Clinical Dementia Rat-
frailty · Mild cognitive impairment · Community-dwelling ing scale – according to the “original” definition of cognitive
frailty (n = 47). Multidimensional frailty was assessed with
the Comprehensive Frailty Assessment Instrument – Plus
Abstract and general cognitive functioning with the Montreal Cogni-
Background: Cognitive frailty has long been defined as the tive Assessment. Descriptive statistics and linear regression
co-occurrence of mild cognitive deficits and physical frailty. were used to determine the prevalence of cognitive frailty
However, recently, a new approach to cognitive frailty has and to explore the relationship between cognitive frailty and
been proposed: cognitive frailty as a distinct construct. the other types of frailty in each sample. Results: The preva-
Nonetheless, the relationship between this relatively new lence of cognitive frailty increased along with the degree of
construct of cognitive frailty and other frailty domains is un- objective cognitive impairment in the 3 samples (range
clear. Objectives: The aims of this study were to explore the 35.1–80.9%), while its co-occurrence with (one of) the other
prevalence of cognitive frailty in groups with different de- types of frailty was most frequent in the frail and community
grees of cognitive impairment, as well as to explore the as- samples. Regarding its relationship with the other domains,
sociations between frailty domains, and if this varies with cognitive frailty was positively associated with psychological
level of objective cognitive impairment. Method: Cross-sec-
tional, secondary data from 3 research projects among com-
Membership of the D-SCOPE Consortium is provided in the Acknowl-
munity-dwelling people aged ≥60 years, with different de- edgments.
grees of objective cognitive impairment, were used: (1) a Ellen E. De Roeck and Anne van der Vorst should be considered joint
randomly selected sample (n = 353); (2) a sample at an in- first author.
129.173.72.87 - 8/5/2019 5:48:22 AM

© 2019 S. Karger AG, Basel Prof. Dr. Eva Dierckx


Kellogg Health Sciences Libr.

Developmental and Lifespan Psychology, Vrije Universiteit Brussel


Faculty of Psychology and Educational Sciences, Pleinlaan 2
E-Mail karger@karger.com
BE–1050 Brussels (Belgium)
www.karger.com/ger
Downloaded by:

E-Mail eva.dierckx @ vub.be


frailty’s subdomain mood disorder symptoms in all 3 sam- ible form [16]. Recently, reversible cognitive frailty has
ples (p ≤ 0.01), while there was no significant association shown to be a short- and long-term predictor of all-cause
with environmental frailty and social loneliness. The associa- mortality and overall dementia [17].
tions between cognitive frailty and the other types of frailty In the definitions described above, cognitive frailty can
differed between the samples. Conclusion: Psychological only exist when one also suffers from physical frailty,
and cognitive frailty are strongly associated, irrespective of which makes physical frailty a prerequisite for cognitive
the objective degree of cognitive impairment. In addition, it frailty. However, from a multidimensional frailty perspec-
is shown that cognitive frailty can occur independently from tive it is assumed that each frailty domain is given equal
the other frailty domains, including physical frailty, and weight and that frailty on one domain can exist without
therefore it can be seen as a distinct concept. the presence of frailty on (one of the) other domains [6,
© 2019 S. Karger AG, Basel 10]. Therewith, is important to gain a holistic view of all
the domains older people experience difficulties in be-
cause otherwise important determinants of functioning
Introduction may be overlooked [18]. In addition, while a recent sys-
tematic literature review, on the one hand, showed that
With the global growth of the proportion of elderly physically frail older people are at an increased risk of in-
people [1] and recognition of the importance of aging in cident cognitive impairment, 68% of the people with de-
place [2–4], frailty has become an increasingly important mentia of the Alzheimer type were not physically frail
concept. Instead of focusing purely on physical aspects [19]. Thus, it seems worthwhile to assess cognitive frailty
[5], there is a growing tendency to view frailty from a mul- as a distinct construct (i.e., which can exist without the
tidimensional perspective. In these multidimensional ap- presence of physical frailty). Therefore, in 2018, a new
proaches, psychological and social factors [6, 7], cognitive measure of cognitive frailty was developed by De Roeck et
functioning [6, 8, 9], and environmental factors are taken al. [8], based on the reversible form described by Panza et
into account as well [10]. Psychological frailty can be con- al. [16]. In this measure, cognitive frailty differs from the
ceptualized as the presence of depressive symptoms, neg- other types of frailty on a conceptual level as it measures
ative affect, or sadness [11], while social frailty includes a additional aspects, containing several components of cog-
lack of social support, loneliness, or the absence of emo- nitive functioning (e.g., learning abilities and attention).
tionally rewarding social contacts [9], and environmental The main distinction between this measure and the defini-
frailty refers to poor-quality housing or deprived living tion as proposed by Kelaiditi et al. [13] and Panza et al.
environments [10]. In recent years, increasing attention [16] is that cognitive frailty is defined as a distinct concept
has especially been paid to cognitive frailty – resulting in and thus that it can occur without the presence of physical
several conceptualizations, along with different ways to frailty. In addition to a lack of research on cognitive frail-
measure it. Initially, merely memory problems were taken ty as a distinct construct and its prevalence, little is known
into account, that is, in the Groningen Frailty Indicator [9] about its relationship with other types of frailty. Most pre-
and Tilburg Frailty Indicator [6]. In 2013, an internation- vious research has examined the relationship between ob-
al consensus group defined cognitive frailty as the pres- jective cognitive impairment, as measured by instruments,
ence of physical frailty and a score of 0.5 on the Clinical rather than cognitive frailty (i.e., subjective complaints),
Dementia Rating scale (CDR; [12]), without the presence and one or more other types of frailty. For example, Gob-
of dementia [13]. It was conceptualized as a condition that bens et al. [6] found that cognitive impairment, as assessed
increases the risk of cognitive impairment [13–15] but is with the Mini Mental State Examination, was correlated
not a clinical condition itself [13]. This definition was fur- with physical and social frailty, but not with psychological
ther refined in 2015 by Panza et al. [16]. They suggested frailty. The most frequently studied relationship is that
classifying cognitive frailty into a reversible and a poten- between objective cognitive impairment and physical
tially reversible form. Reversible cognitive frailty is com- frailty [15, 20–22], which have consistently been found to
parable to subjective cognitive impairment and can be be positively associated. The combination of cognitive and
seen as a precursor of potentially reversible cognitive frail- physical dysfunction has been linked to adverse outcomes,
ty, which is associated with mild cognitive impairment such as increased risk of progression to dementia [23] or
(MCI). In other words, people with potentially reversible other neurocognitive disorders [24]. There have been no
cognitive frailty have objective cognitive impairment, studies examining the relationship between environmen-
while an objective cognitive deficit is absent in the revers- tal frailty and cognitive impairment and no studies inves-
129.173.72.87 - 8/5/2019 5:48:22 AM

2 Gerontology De Roeck/van der Vorst/Engelborghs/


Kellogg Health Sciences Libr.

DOI: 10.1159/000501168 Zijlstra/Dierckx/D-SCOPE Consortium


Downloaded by:
tigating how stable these potential associations are across pled. In addition, snowball sampling was used. People with a known
levels of cognitive frailty. However, associations between diagnosis of dementia or severe psychiatric disorder were excluded.
However, also for this sample, no full neuropsychological examina-
cognitive frailty and the other domains may vary between tion was administered. More details regarding the study sample can
groups (e.g., the general community-dwelling older popu- be found in the article by Dury et al. [26]. Data were collected by 6
lation versus memory clinic patients). Gaining insight trained PhD students. The third sample consisted of 47 memory
into the associations between the domains in different clinic patients with a reported CDR score of 0.5, as in accordance
populations would make it possible to make interventions with previous definitions of cognitive frailty [13–15], who had vis-
ited the Memory Clinic of the Hospital Network Antwerp. The ex-
more individually tailored (i.e., by knowing on which clusion criteria were a history of neurological diseases or comorbid
frailty domains the focus should be in each population). neurological disorder and severe psychiatric illness. Data were col-
To conclude, it seems worthwhile to gain further in- lected by one trained PhD student (psychologist).
sight into the conceptualization of cognitive frailty as a Hereafter, the 3 samples will be referred to as the “community,”
distinct concept [8], as it differs from the other domains “potentially frail,” and “clinical” samples, respectively. People were
excluded from all samples if there were missing values in the CFAI-
on a conceptual level, it is important to gain a holistic view Plus or Montreal Cognitive Assessment (MoCA; for more details,
of the older person [18], and because physical frailty and see section “Measurements”; community sample n = 2; frail sample
cognitive impairment do not always occur together [19]. n = 26; clinical sample: n = 0). All 3 studies were part of a larger re-
In addition, while previous research indicates that it is im- search project called D-SCOPE, which stands for Detection, Support
portant to assess cognitive frailty in relation to the other and Care for older people: Prevention and Empowerment (www.d-
scope.be/en). The same neuropsychologists (authors E.E.D.R. and
domains, there is a dearth of research in this area, which E.D.) trained the different administrators to make sure that the data
makes it more difficult to design effective interventions. collection was carried out consistently throughout the studies. The
Multidisciplinary, tailored interventions can only be de- research in this manuscript encompasses secondary data analyses.
veloped and implemented when (1) the underlying con- All participants were recruited between December 2015 and
ceptualization of concepts such as cognitive frailty are April 2017. Research protocols were approved by the local Ethical
Committees (i.e., of the Vrije Universiteit Brussel for the frail sam-
clear; (2) the relationships between the different types of ple; ECHW_031; and of the University of Antwerp/Antwerp Uni-
frailty are understood; and (3) information about indi- versity Hospital for the community and clinical samples:
viduals’ pattern of frailty is available. The aims of this B300201525772). Written, informed consent was obtained from
study were, therefore, to explore the prevalence of cogni- all participants prior to data collection.
tive frailty in groups with different degrees of cognitive
Measurements
impairment, as well as to explore the associations between First, the following sociodemographic characteristics were as-
frailty domains, as measured with the Comprehensive sessed in all 3 samples: age, gender, education, and marital status.
Frailty Assessment Instrument – Plus (CFAI-Plus) [8]. Second, frailty was measured with the 25-item CFAI-Plus [8, 10].
The “original” CFAI has been validated against the Tilburg Frailty
Indicator [10]. Recently, De Roeck et al. [8] added 4 questions about
cognitive frailty, which were validated against the MoCA [27], re-
Materials and Methods sulting in the CFAI-Plus. This self-report questionnaire measures
cognitive (e.g., “I have trouble remembering things that happened
Study Participants recently”), environmental (e.g., “My house is in a bad condition/
Cross-sectional data from 3 different research projects were poorly kept”), physical (e.g., “I have been hampered by my state of
used. The general inclusion criteria were as follows: community- health in less demanding activities like carrying shopping bags”),
dwelling people aged 60 years and over, living in Flanders or Brus- psychological (e.g., “I feel unhappy and depressed”), and social (e.g.,
sels. The first sample was a random sample of 353 individuals; people “I know many people whom I can totally trust”) frailty. There are 2
with a known diagnosis of dementia or MCI (including, e.g., people components to both psychological and social frailty, respectively,
who took medication prescribed for people with MCI or dementia), mood disorders and emotional loneliness, and social loneliness and
severe psychiatric disorders, or analphabetism were excluded. How- social support network. Cognitive, environmental, and social frailty
ever, no full neuropsychological examination was administered to are rated on a 5-point scale (0 = completely disagree; 4 = complete-
test whether or not people had MCI or dementia. These participants ly agree), as is emotional loneliness (a subdomain of psychological
were recruited and tested by final-year undergraduate psychology frailty). Physical frailty is rated on a 3-point scale (0 = not at all; 1 =
students from the Vrije Universiteit Brussel (Brussels, Belgium). up to 3 months; 2 = >3 months) and mood disorders (a subdomain
The second sample compromised 121 older people with a high prob- of psychological frailty) on a 4-point scale (0 = not at all; 3 = consid-
ability of being frail. Different Flemish care organizations identified erably more than usual) [8, 10]. Scores for each frailty domain range
potential participants, that is, people with a high probability of being from 0 to 25, and cutoffs for high frailty are as follows: cognitive:
frail, based on risk profiles for multidimensional frailty, as described 10.94 [8]; environmental: 7.51; physical: 18.81; psychological: 11.51;
in the detailed manuscript of Dury et al. [25]. This means that people and social: 16.01 [28]. Third, the MoCA, a brief cognitive screening
with a higher age, who were not married, who had moved in the past tool designed to detect MCI or mild dementia [27], was used to as-
10 years, and/or who were born in a foreign country were oversam- sess overall cognitive functioning in all 3 samples. The MoCA ex-
129.173.72.87 - 8/5/2019 5:48:22 AM

Cognitive Frailty and the Association with Gerontology 3


Kellogg Health Sciences Libr.

Other Frailty Domains DOI: 10.1159/000501168


Downloaded by:
amines multiple domains of cognitive functioning including short- 0.035) and reported higher levels of physical frailty (mean
term memory, executive functioning, attention, and temporal and 20.5 [SD 7.6] vs. 10.8 [SD 8.7], p < 0.001) compared to
spatial orientation. Total score ranges from 0 to 30 and higher scores
indicate better cognition. To correct for educational effects, partici- participants who were included. For all other parameters
pants with ≤12 years of education received one extra point [29]. (MoCA score and psychological, social, environmental,
Lastly, in the clinical sample, impairments in 6 domains of cognitive and cognitive frailty), no significant differences between
functioning (such as memory, orientation, and personal care) were the included and excluded participants from the frail
rated using the CDR [12], although this was only taken into account sample were found (p > 0.050; findings not tabulated).
to identify people who were cognitively frail according to previous
definitions [13–15]. Table 1 shows sociodemographic characteristics and
mean MoCA and frailty scores by sample. There were sig-
Statistical Analyses nificant differences between the 3 samples with respect to
First, participants who were excluded from the frail sample were MoCA score and cognitive, environmental, physical, and
compared with participants who were included by means of an in- psychological frailty, including its subdomain mood dis-
dependent-sample t test (which was not done for the community
and frail samples due to the small amount of participants who were order symptoms. In addition, there was a trend for a dif-
excluded). Second, descriptive statistics for each sample were calcu- ference in the level of social frailty between the 3 groups
lated. Third, skewness and kurtosis of each variable were checked and a significant difference for its subdomain social lone-
in each sample to determine whether the distribution violated the liness. Post hoc tests (not tabulated) revealed differences
assumption of normality [30]. Because the clinical sample was between the community and frail samples with respect to
small-sized, the cutoff for nonnormality was set at z > 1.96 for both
skewness and kurtosis [31]. For the medium-sized frail sample, the mean MoCA score (p < 0.001) and cognitive (p = 0.006),
cutoff for nonnormality was set at z > 3.29, and absolute values environmental (p < 0.001), and psychological (p = 0.002)
of skewness and kurtosis were assessed for the community sample frailty (p < 0.001), as well as the psychological frailty sub-
(n > 300) [31]. Fourth, differences between the 3 samples were as- domains of mood disorders (p < 0.001) and emotional
sessed using one-way ANOVAs in the case of normally distributed loneliness (p = 0.031). The community and clinical sam-
variables (age, MoCA, cognitive frailty, and social frailty’s subdo-
main potential support network), Kruskal-Wallis tests in the case of ples differed with respect to mean age (p = 0.034), mean
nonnormally distributed variables (environmental and physical MoCA score (p < 0.001), cognitive frailty (p < 0.001), en-
frailty; psychological frailty [including both subdomains]; and so- vironmental frailty (p < 0.001), and psychological frailty
cial frailty [including the subdomain emotional loneliness]), and (p = 0.002), including both subdomains (mood disorders
chi-square tests for categorical variables (gender). Any overall dif- [p < 0.001] and emotional loneliness [p = 0.034]). Lastly,
ferences were analyzed pairwise using independent-sample t tests
(normally distributed variables) or Mann-Whitney U tests (non- the frail and clinical samples differed with respect to mean
normally distributed variables). In addition, co-occurrence of cog- age (p = 0.043), mean MoCA score (p = 0.009), cognitive
nitive frailty was assessed using crosstabs and chi-square tests. Fi- (p < 0.001) and environmental frailty (p < 0.001), and psy-
nally, multiple linear regression models were used to examine the chological frailty (p = 0.002), including both subdomains
relationship between cognitive frailty and the other frailty domains. (mood disorders [p < 0.001] and emotional loneliness
These analyses were conducted separately in each sample, after
checking collinearity using the variance inflation factor and toler- [p = 0.031]; post hoc comparisons are not tabulated).
ance statistics [32]. Because previous research had shown that cog-
nition has different relationships with social loneliness (social frail- Prevalence per Frailty Domain, and Co-occurrence
ty subdomain) and emotional loneliness (psychological frailty sub- with Cognitive Frailty
domain) [33], the subdomains were taken into account, rather than Table 2 shows the prevalence of frailty in each domain
the overall domains. To conclude, age and environmental and phys-
ical frailty and the subdomains of psychological and social frailty by sample. The prevalence of cognitive frailty was 35.1,
were predictors, while cognitive frailty was the dependent variable. 51.1, and 80.9% in the community, frail, and clinical sam-
Age was taken into account as it was associated with cognitive frail- ples, respectively. Environmental, physical, and social
ty (data not presented). Statistical significance was set at p ≤ 0.05 and frailty were most prevalent in the frail group (16.8, 16.8,
analyses were performed using SPSS 24 (IBM Corp., Armonk, NY, and 15.8%, respectively) and psychological frailty in the
USA).
clinical group (25.5%). Environmental and psychological
frailty were least prevalent in the community sample,
whereas physical and social frailty were least prevalent in
Results the clinical sample.
Regarding the co-occurrence, 50–100% of the respon-
Sample Characteristics dents who were frail on an environmental, physical, or
Participants who were excluded from the frail sample psychological level also reported cognitive frailty. Re-
were older (mean age 82 [SD 8.4] vs. 78 [SD 8.6], p = garding social frailty, 100% of the respondents in the frail
129.173.72.87 - 8/5/2019 5:48:22 AM

4 Gerontology De Roeck/van der Vorst/Engelborghs/


Kellogg Health Sciences Libr.

DOI: 10.1159/000501168 Zijlstra/Dierckx/D-SCOPE Consortium


Downloaded by:
Table 1. Sociodemographic characteristics and MoCA and CFAI scores by sample

Community Frail Clinical p value


(n = 353) (n = 95) (n = 47)

Sociodemographic characteristics
Age, years, mean (SD) 77.7 (8.3) 78.2 (8.3) 75.3 (6.8) 0.129
Gender, %
Female 55.0 57.9 53.4 0.736
Cognition, mean (SD)
MoCA 25.1 (3.2) 21.6 (4.6) 19.5 (4.3) ≤0.001
Frailty, mean (SD)
CFAI-Plus
Cognitive 6.8 (5.4) 8.5 (5.31) 15.7 (7.3) ≤0.001
Environmental 2.6 (3.5) 4.6 (4.14) 3.5 (4.7) ≤0.001
Physical 9.1 (8.4) 10.8 (8.77) 4.9 (7.7) ≤0.001
Psychological 4.4 (4.5) 6.6 (5.75) 6.8 (6.9) 0.003
Mood disorders 1.7 (2.2) 2.7 (2.7) 3.9 (4.0) ≤0.001
Emotional loneliness 2.8 (2.9) 3.9 (3.8) 2.8 (3.5) 0.093
Social 9.8 (4.9) 10.1 (5.04) 8.1 (4.2) 0.053
Social loneliness 3.2 (3.3) 3.2 (3.0) 2.0 (2.4) 0.029
Potential support network 6.6 (3.0) 6.9 (3.0) 6.1 (2.3) 0.346

MoCA, high scores indicate good cognitive functioning (range 0–30); CFAI-Plus, higher scores indicate
higher (more severe) levels of frailty (ranges per domain: 0–25; ranges per subdomain [i.e., for psychological and
social frailty]: 0–12.5). Differences between the 3 samples were assessed using one-way ANOVAs in the case of
normally distributed variables and Kruskal-Wallis tests in the case of nonnormally distributed variables. Pairwise
comparisons are reported in the text.
MoCA, Montreal Cognitive Assessment; Comprehensive Frailty Assessment Instrument – Plus (CFAI-Plus).

Table 2. Prevalence of cognitive frailty and other types of frailty by and clinical sample who were socially frail also reported
sample cognitive frailty. In the community sample, only 24.4% of
the people who were socially frail reported co-occurring
Community Frail Clinical
(n = 353), (n = 95), (n = 47),
cognitive frailty.
% (n) % (n) % (n)
Relationship between Cognitive Frailty and the Other
Frailty Domains
Cognitive 35.1 (124) 51.6 (49) 80.9 (38) Table 3 shows the findings from the multiple linear
Environmental 9.6 (34) 16.8 (16) 14.9 (7)
Co-occurrence, n 17*** 11* 7 regression analyses. In the community sample, cognitive
Physical 12.7 (45) 16.8 (16) 8.5 (4) frailty was positively associated with age, physical frailty,
Co-occurrence, n 25*** 14* 3 mood disorder symptoms, and emotional loneliness and
Psychological 7.1 (25) 22.1 (21) 25.5 (12) negatively associated with potential support network. In
Co-occurrence, n 14*** 17** 11 both the frail and clinical samples, only mood disorders
Social 12.7 (45) 15.8 (15) 6.4 (3)
were significantly related to cognitive frailty.
Co-occurrence, n 11 15* 3

* p ≤ 0.05.
** p ≤ 0.01. Discussion
*** p ≤ 0.001.
Prevalence figures represent the proportion of participants This study aimed to explore the prevalence of cogni-
with above-threshold scores (see “Methods” section for thresholds).
Crosstabs and chi-square tests were conducted. Co-occurrence tive frailty as a distinct construct (i.e., without the pres-
relates to cognitive frailty. ence of physical frailty) in groups with different degrees
of cognitive impairment, as well as the associations be-
tween frailty domains in 495 community-dwelling older
129.173.72.87 - 8/5/2019 5:48:22 AM

Cognitive Frailty and the Association with Gerontology 5


Kellogg Health Sciences Libr.

Other Frailty Domains DOI: 10.1159/000501168


Downloaded by:
Table 3. Relationship between age and the various types of frailty with cognitive frailty by sample

Community (n = 353) Frail (n = 95) Clinical (n = 47)


B SE β B SE β B SE β

Age, years 0.205 0.032 0.315*** 0.043 0.058 0.067 –0.095 0.142 –0.088
Environmental 0.056 0.074 0.037 0.152 0.126 0.118 0.368 0.247 0.235
Physical 0.097 0.032 0.151** 0.069 0.062 0.114 0.080 0.131 0.084
Psychological
Mood disorders 0.353 0.136 0.143** 0.919 0.216 0.466*** 0.999 0.355 0.543**
Emotional loneliness 0.351 0.105 0.187*** 0.131 0.163 0.094 –0.277 0.461 –0.131
Social
Potential support network –0.222 0.084 –0.123** –0.241 0.180 –0.136 –0.251 0.500 –0.081
Social loneliness 0.039 0.078 0.024 –0.059 0.180 –0.034 –0.261 0.602 –0.084

* p ≤ 0.05.
** p ≤ 0.01.
*** p ≤ 0.001.
The independent variables were as follows: age, environmental frailty, physical frailty, mood disorders and emotional loneliness
(psychological frailty subdomains), and social loneliness and potential social support (social frailty subdomains); the dependent variable
was cognitive frailty. Multiple linear regression models were conducted.

people aged ≥60 with different levels of objective cogni- explain the lower prevalence rates. Indeed, when labeling
tive impairment. The rationale behind our study design only the participants with co-occurring physical and cog-
was based on previous studies that showed that (1) from nitive frailty as cognitively frail in our study, our preva-
a multidimensional perspective on frailty, frailty can ex- lence rates are more in line with the prevalence rates from
ist on one domain, without the presence of frailty on oth- research (i.e., ranging from 7% in the community sample
er domains [6, 10]; (2) it is important to gain a holistic to 14.7% in the frail sample). Regarding the relationship
view of the domains older people experience difficulties with the other frailty domains, it was observed that the
in because otherwise important determinants of func- co-occurrence of cognitive frailty with (one of) the other
tioning may be overlooked [18]; and (3) physical frailty types of frailty was most frequent in the frail and commu-
and cognitive impairment do not always occur together nity samples. When assessing the specific associations be-
[19]. tween cognitive frailty and the other types of frailty in
Regarding the prevalence, it is shown that cognitive each group, 3 patterns were observed consistently. First,
frailty as an independent domain was most prevalent in social loneliness (e.g., not having enough people to rely
the clinical sample (80.9%), followed by the frail sample on, an aspect of social frailty) was not related to cognitive
(51.6%) and finally the community sample (35.1%). This frailty in any of the samples. This might seem unexpected,
is consistent with previous research indicating that objec- as Holmén et al. [33] found that social loneliness was neg-
tive cognitive impairment and subjective cognitive com- atively related to cognitive impairment. Nonetheless, be-
plaints often co-occur [34]. These prevalence figures are cause the people in our clinical sample were aware of their
also consistent with previous research on community impairment and it was at an early stage, it seems likely
samples. For example, Fritsch et al. [35] reported that that they were receiving social support from people
27.1% of participants living at home reported subjective around them.
memory complaints, while Mewton et al. [36] reported a Second, environmental frailty was not associated with
prevalence of 33.5% in a similar sample. Nonetheless, in cognitive frailty in all 3 samples. Nonetheless, while only
a recent literature review, lower prevalence rates of cogni- 50% of the community-dwelling sample who was envi-
tive frailty were reported, namely, 1.0–12.1% in commu- ronmentally frail also reported cognitive frailty, this in-
nity-dwelling samples, which increased up to 39.7% in creased to 68.8% in the frail and even 100% in the clinical
clinical settings [37]. However, they only included studies sample. Therefore, it seems plausible that due to the low
in which physical functioning was taken into account in sample size of the clinical group, there was not sufficient
the definition of cognitive frailty as well – which might statistical power to identify a significant association. It is
129.173.72.87 - 8/5/2019 5:48:22 AM

6 Gerontology De Roeck/van der Vorst/Engelborghs/


Kellogg Health Sciences Libr.

DOI: 10.1159/000501168 Zijlstra/Dierckx/D-SCOPE Consortium


Downloaded by:
known that adaptations are sometimes needed to enable are important for mental stimulation and maintenance or
aging in place in people with cognitive impairment [38]. enhancement of cognitive reserve [42]. On this basis, we
Therefore, we hypothesized that respondents in the clin- might have expected to find the same negative association
ical group had a relatively high awareness of their illness in all 3 samples, but it should be remembered that we
and consequently might have been aware that they would measured subjective cognitive functioning (cognitive
need to make adjustments to their home if their cognitive frailty) rather than objective cognitive impairment, and
impairment was to worsen. there is a difference between one’s potential social sup-
Third, the mood disorders variable (psychological port network and one’s actual social network.
frailty subdomain), which captures minor depressive Furthermore, physical frailty was only associated with
symptoms (e.g., feeling unhappy and depressed) [10], was cognitive frailty in the community sample. Because up
positively associated with cognitive frailty in all 3 sam- till now the conceptualizations of cognitive frailty always
ples. Although it is well known that depression is associ- included the presence of physical frailty [13, 16–17], and
ated with objective cognitive deficits [39], there is less ev- only few instruments measure cognitive frailty with a
idence on its relationship with subjective cognitive com- self-reported assessment (i.e., 7 out of 92; [43]), it is hard
plaints. Nonetheless, Zlatar et al. [40] found that subjective to compare our findings with previous studies. Nonethe-
cognitive complaints were associated with depression, less, numerous studies have examined the association be-
even after adjusting for objective cognitive impairment. tween physical frailty and objective cognitive impair-
It is also possible that the combination of subjective cog- ment. An overview by Canevelli et al. [14] concluded that
nitive complaints and depressive symptoms might be a cognitive impairment and physical frailty were strongly
precursor of dementia [40]. Our findings and those of Seo linked, but the vast majority of studies on which this re-
et al. [41] seem related, as mood disorders and cognitive view was based have assessed community-dwelling older
frailty were associated in all samples but co-occurred people, that is, a sample analogous to our community
more frequently at higher levels of cognitive impairment sample, the only sample in which we observed an asso-
(93.3% of those in the clinical sample who had mood dis- ciation between physical and cognitive frailty. In addi-
orders also reported cognitive frailty). It is therefore im- tion, physical frailty increases with age [25], and our clin-
portant to assess the cognitive frailty level of people who ical sample had the lowest mean age of the 3 samples,
report mood disorders and to assess mood disorders in which might explain why physical frailty was not (yet)
people who report cognitive frailty. that prevalent and was not associated with cognitive
Differences between samples were also observed. First, frailty.
emotional loneliness (e.g., missing having people around), Lastly, age was strongly associated with cognitive frail-
which is a component of psychological frailty, was strong- ty, but only in the community sample; yet on the basis of
ly associated with cognitive frailty in the community sam- previous research [44], we had expected to find this as-
ple, but not in the frail or clinical samples. Previous re- sociation in all 3 groups. Possible explanation is that par-
search by Holmén et al. [33] found that emotional loneli- ticipants in the frail sample were selected on the basis of
ness decreased with cognitive functioning, which might a broad range of risk variables, not just age [25], and the
explain why we only detected an association between participants in the clinical sample were relatively young,
emotional loneliness and cognitive frailty in participants as mentioned before.
with relatively high levels of objective cognitive function- Regarding the conceptualization of cognitive frailty as
ing. Therefore, our findings indicate that the same rela- a distinct construct, only 10 of the older people in the
tionship might hold for cognitive frailty (i.e., emotional clinical sample (21.3%) would be regarded cognitively
loneliness decreases in people with lower levels of cogni- frail based on the definition by Kelaiditi et al. [13], that is,
tive frailty), although we were not able to determine caus- being physical frail and scoring 0.5 on the CDR. Nonethe-
al relationships. less, these 10 people were all part of the respondents from
Analysis of the potential social support network com- the clinical group who were regarded cognitively frail ac-
ponent of social frailty suggested that it was negatively cording to the CFAI-Plus [8] (80.9%; findings not tabu-
associated with higher cognitive frailty, but only in the lated). In addition, physical frailty was only associated
community sample. Previous studies have found that with cognitive frailty in the community sample. There-
people who lack social ties are at an increased risk of ob- fore, it can be argued that cognitive frailty indeed can be
jective cognitive decline relative to their counterparts seen as a distinct construct – which should be further in-
with more extensive social networks, as social networks vestigated.
129.173.72.87 - 8/5/2019 5:48:22 AM

Cognitive Frailty and the Association with Gerontology 7


Kellogg Health Sciences Libr.

Other Frailty Domains DOI: 10.1159/000501168


Downloaded by:
Strengths and Limitations different frailty domains. For clinical practice, the CFAI-
This study has several strengths. First, we analyzed 3 plus, including cognitive frailty as a distinct construct,
different samples with varying levels of objective cogni- seems very useful as a first screening instrument to iden-
tive impairment, including a random sample of people tify people in need of care. Focusing merely on people
living in their own homes. We thus were able to explore with both physical and cognitive frailty may cause pre-
whether the relationship between cognitive frailty and vention opportunities to be lost. For example, our results
other types of frailty varied with the level of cognitive suggest that both researchers and clinicians should pay
functioning. Second, we assessed several aspects of frailty, special attention to the relationship between cognitive
whereas most previous studies have only looked at 1 or 2 and psychological frailty. Longitudinal research projects
frailty domains (usually physical and social frailty). could study whether the presence of both cognitive and
Some limitations of the study should also be acknowl- psychological frailty, and specific mood disorders, can in-
edged. First, full neuropsychological assessments of the deed be used as a marker for risk of dementia, as suggest-
community and frail samples were not administered. Our ed by Seo et al. [41]. When this is the case, this sample
aim, however, was to get an overall indication of level of might particularly benefit from early, multimodal pre-
cognitive functioning in each sample and the MoCA is vention strategies integrating interventions focusing on
appropriate for this purpose. Nonetheless, because of cognitive and psychological functioning. In addition,
this, we cannot state with certainty that none of the par- longitudinal research is needed to evaluate the temporal
ticipants in the community and frail samples had MCI or course of interactions between the various frailty do-
dementia – as we merely excluded people with a known mains, for example, to determine whether the pattern of
diagnosis or who, for example, were unable to provide associations between frailty domains is different in the
adequate answers. If, unexpectedly, people with an objec- general population of older people living in the commu-
tive cognitive impairment were included in one of these nity from what it is in more vulnerable populations such
samples, cognitive frailty prevalence estimates could be as our frail (i.e., based on the risk profiles as defined by
biased. Second, there was a relatively high proportion of Dury et al. [25]) and clinical sample. This knowledge
potential participants who were excluded from the frail could be used to develop clinical guidelines for detection
sample due to missing data. It is likely that these were the of frailty and follow-up of older people deemed at risk of
most frail, especially because they were older and report- adverse outcomes. Moreover, it seems worthwhile to in-
ed higher (more severe) levels of physical frailty. Third, vestigate the predictive value of cognitive frailty as a dis-
due to our study design, selection bias may have influ- tinct construct [8], compared to cognitive frailty as de-
enced the results, especially in the case of the clinical sam- fined by Kelaiditi et al. [13] or Panza et al. [16]. For ex-
ple. It seems reasonable to assume that clinical partici- ample, reversible cognitive frailty [16] has been shown to
pants generally had a high awareness of their illness (they be a short- and long-term predictor of all-cause mortality
had visited a memory clinic because of their symptoms), and overall dementia [17]. Therefore, it is also important
so it is understandable that levels of cognitive frailty were to investigate the predictive value of cognitive frailty as a
higher in this sample. Fourth, there could have been over- distinct construct. Although there seems to be a common
lap regarding the cognitive performance (i.e., based on ground (i.e., all of the respondents who were cognitively
the MoCA) between the different samples. While pooling frail according to Kelaiditi et al. [13] [21.3%, not tabu-
all the data and redefining the groups based on their ac- lated] were also frail according to the CFAI-Plus [8]),
tual cognitive performance would have dissolved this, the there also seem to be differences (i.e., 80.9% of the clinical
authors compared the initial samples from a clinical per- sample was cognitively frail according to the CFAI-Plus
spective – as it is important to know on which frailty do- [8]).
mains the focus should be in different settings within the From a clinical perspective, it appears that it would be
“general” population. Lastly, the community sample was sensible to assess a broad spectrum of frailty domains in
by far the largest (n = 353; frail n = 95, clinical n = 47), so the general population of older people living at home but
the analyses of this sample had greater statistical power. to focus on mood disorders and cognitive frailty in more
vulnerable populations (e.g., people with an increased risk
Implications for Future Research and Clinical Practice of frailty, which can be determined by the risk profiles by
In today’s aging society, with the number of frail older Dury et al. [25], or people visiting a memory clinic). The
people increasing, there is a need for longitudinal re- CFAI-Plus, including cognitive frailty as a distinct con-
search on the time course of relationships between the struct, seems very useful as a first screening instrument to
129.173.72.87 - 8/5/2019 5:48:22 AM

8 Gerontology De Roeck/van der Vorst/Engelborghs/


Kellogg Health Sciences Libr.

DOI: 10.1159/000501168 Zijlstra/Dierckx/D-SCOPE Consortium


Downloaded by:
identify people in need of care. By means of the CFAI- Ellen E. De Roeck, Nico De Witte, Eva Dierckx, Daan Duppen,
Plus, it is not only possible to assess ones level of frailty on Sarah Dury, Sebastiaan Engelborghs, Bram Fret, Sylvia Hoens,
Lieve Hoeyberghs, Tinie Kardol, Gertrudis I.J.M. Kempen, Debo-
5 different domains, which gives a more holistic view [18], rah Lambotte, Birgitte Schoenmakers, Jos M.G.A. Schols, An-Sofie
but also possible to assess associations between domains. Smetcoren, Lise Switsers, Michaël Van Der Elst, Anne van der
Moreover, the same amount of attention (equal weight) is Vorst, Dominique Verté, and G.A. Rixt Zijlstra.
paid to each frailty domain. In this way, clinicians can eas- The authors would like to thank all participating older people.
ily see on which domains they should focus. For example,
focusing merely on people with both physical and cogni-
tive frailty may cause prevention opportunities to be lost. Statement of Ethics
For instance, in this study, cognitive frailty was often ac- Research protocols were approved by the local ethical commit-
companied by mood disorder symptoms in the commu- tee of the Vrije Universiteit Brussel (ECHW_031) and the ethical
nity sample – which thus seem important domains to in- committee of the University of Antwerp and Antwerp University
tegrate while developing early interventions as well. Hospital (B300201525772). Written, informed consent was ob-
tained from all participants prior to data collection.

Conclusion
Disclosure Statement
In older people living at home, cognitive frailty increas- The authors have no conflicts of interest to declare.
es with level of cognitive impairment and is often accom-
panied by one or more other types of frailty, especially
physical and social frailty. It is important to include cogni- Funding Sources
tive frailty in multidimensional clinical assessments of
frailty. Robust associations were found between cognitive This work was supported by the Flemish government Agency
for Innovation by Science and Technology (VLAIO), embedded in
and psychological frailty, in particular, the mood disor- the Strategic Basic Research under Grant number IWT-140027-
ders subdomain. Physical and social frailty, and more spe- SBO; the University of Antwerp Research Fund; the Institute
cifically the (lack of a) potential social support network, Born-Bunge (www.bornbunge.be); and the Flanders Impulse Pro-
seem to be particularly associated with cognitive frailty in gram on Networks for Dementia Research (VIND). The funders
the general population of older people living at home. had no role in the design of the study, data collection, analysis, and
interpretation of the data, or in writing the manuscript and deci-
Moreover, cognitive frailty can be seen as an independent sion to publish the manuscript.
domain, as it can occur separately from physical frailty.

Author Contributions
Acknowledgments
E.E.D.R. and A.V.: conception and design of the study, data col-
The D-SCOPE Consortium is an international research group lection, analysis, and interpretation of the data, drafting the article,
and is composed of researchers from University of Antwerp, Vrije and final approval of this version to be submitted. S.E., G.A.R.Z.,
Universiteit Brussel, University College Ghent, Catholic Univer- and E.D.: conception and design of the study, interpretation of the
sity of Leuven (Belgium), and Maastricht University (the Nether- data, revising the article, and final approval of this version to be
lands): Peter Paul De Deyn, Liesbeth De Donder, Jan De Lepeleire, submitted. D-SCOPE: conception and design of the study.

References
1 European Commission [Internet]. The 2015 4 Wiles JL, Leibing A, Guberman N, Reeve J, Al- Frailty Indicator: psychometric properties. J
Aging Report. 2014. http://ec.europa.eu/ len RE. The meaning of “aging in place” to older Am Med Dir Assoc. 2010 Jun;11(5):344–55.
economy_finance/publications/european_ people. Gerontologist. 2012 Jun;52(3):357–66. 7 Rockwood K, Mitnitski A. Frailty in relation
economy/2014/pdf/ee8n.pdf. 5 Fried LP, Tangen CM, Walston J, Newman to the accumulation of deficits. J Gerontol A
2 Scharlach A. Creating aging-friendly commu- AB, Hirsch C, Gottdiener J, et al.; Cardiovas- Biol Sci Med Sci. 2007 Jul;62(7):722–7.
nities in the United States. Ageing Int. 2012; cular Health Study Collaborative Research 8 De Roeck EE, Dury S, De Witte N, De Donder
37(1):25–38. Group. Frailty in older adults: evidence for a L, Bjerke M, De Deyn PP, et al. CFAI-Plus:
3 Walker A, Maltby T. Active ageing: a strategic phenotype. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci. adding cognitive frailty as a new domain to
policy solution to demographic ageing in the 2001 Mar;56(3):M146–56. the comprehensive frailty assessment instru-
European Union. Int J Soc Welf. 2012; 6 Gobbens RJ, van Assen MA, Luijkx KG, Wi- ment. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2018 Jul;33(7):
21:S117–30. jnen-Sponselee MT, Schols JM. The Tilburg 941–7.
129.173.72.87 - 8/5/2019 5:48:22 AM

Cognitive Frailty and the Association with Gerontology 9


Kellogg Health Sciences Libr.

Other Frailty Domains DOI: 10.1159/000501168


Downloaded by:
9 Steverink N, Slaets JPJ, Schuurmans H, van 21 Jacobs JM, Cohen A, Ein-Mor E, Maaravi Y, 34 Snitz BE, Small BJ, Wang T, Chang CC,
Lis M: Measuring frailty: Development and Stessman J. Frailty, cognitive impairment and Hughes TF, Ganguli M. Do subjective mem-
testing of the Groningen Frailty Indicator mortality among the oldest old. J Nutr Health ory complaints ad or follow objective cogni-
(GFI). Gerontologist. 2001; 41(special issue Aging. 2011 Aug;15(8):678–82. tive change? A five–year population study of
1):236–7. 22 Panza F, Seripa D, Solfrizzi V, Tortelli R, Gre- temporal influence. J Int Neuropsychol Soc.
10 De Witte N, Gobbens R, De Donder L, Dury co A, Pilotto A, et al. Targeting cognitive frail- 2015 Oct;21(9):732–42.
S, Buffel T, Schols J, et al. The comprehensive ty: clinical and neurobiological roadmap for a 35 Fritsch T, McClendon MJ, Wallendal MS,
frailty assessment instrument: development, single complex phenotype. J Alzheimers Dis. Hyde TF, Larsen JD. Prevalence and cognitive
validity and reliability. Geriatr Nurs. 2013 Jul- 2015;47(4):793–813. bases of subjective memory complaints in
Aug;34(4):274–81. 23 Montero-Odasso MM, Barnes B, Speechley older adults: evidence from a community
11 Ernsth Bravell M, Westerlind B, Midlöv P, M, Muir Hunter SW, Doherty TJ, Duque G, sample. J Neurodegener Dis. 2014; 2014:
Östgren CJ, Borgquist L, Lannering C, et al. et al. Disentangling Cognitive-Frailty: Results 176843.
How to assess frailty and the need for care? From the Gait and Brain Study. J Gerontol A 36 Mewton L, Sachdev P, Anderson T, Sunder-
Report from the Study of Health and Drugs in Biol Sci Med Sci. 2016 Nov;71(11):1476–82. land M, Andrews G. Demographic, clinical,
the Elderly (SHADES) in community dwell- 24 Feng L, Nyunt MS, Gao Q, Feng L, Lee TS, and lifestyle correlates of subjective memory
ings in Sweden. Arch Gerontol Geriatr. 2011 Tsoi T, et al. Physical frailty, cognitive impair- complaints in the Australian population. Am
Jul-Aug;53(1):40–5. ment, and the risk of neurocognitive disorder J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2014 Nov; 22(11): 1222–
12 Morris JC. The Clinical Dementia Rating in the Singapore Longitudinal Ageing Studies. 32.
(CDR): current version and scoring rules. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci. 2017 Mar; 37 Sugimoto T, Sakurai T, Ono R, Kimura A, Saji
Neurology. 1993 Nov;43(11):2412–4. 72(3):369–75. N, Niida S, et al. Epidemiological and clinical
13 Kelaiditi E, Cesari M, Canevelli M, van Kan 25 Dury S, De Roeck E, Duppen D, Fret B, Ho- significance of cognitive frailty: a mini review.
GA, Ousset PJ, Gillette-Guyonnet S, et al.; eyberghs L, Lambotte D, et al. Identifying Ageing Res Rev. 2018 Jul;44:1–7.
IANA/IAGG. Cognitive frailty: rational and frailty risk profiles of home-dwelling older 38 Van Hoof J, Kort HS. Supportive living envi-
definition from an (I.A.N.A./I.A.G.G.) inter- people: focus on sociodemographic and so- ronments: a first concept of a dwelling de-
national consensus group. J Nutr Health Ag- cioeconomic characteristics. Aging Ment signed for older adults with dementia. De-
ing. 2013 Sep;17(9):726–34. Health. 2017 Oct;21(10):1031–9. mentia. 2009;8(2):293–316.
14 Canevelli M, Cesari M, van Kan GA. Frailty 26 Dury S, Dierckx E, van der Vorst A, Van der 39 Rock PL, Roiser JP, Riedel WJ, Blackwell AD.
and cognitive decline: how do they relate? Elst M, Fret B, Duppen D, et al. Detecting Cognitive impairment in depression: a sys-
Curr Opin Clin Nutr Metab Care. 2015 Jan; frail, older adults and identifying their tematic review and meta-analysis. Psychol
18(1):43–50. strengths: results of a mixed-methods study. Med. 2014 Jul;44(10):2029–40.
15 Fitten LJ. Thinking about cognitive frailty. J BMC Public Health. 2018 Jan;18(1):191. 40 Zlatar ZZ, Muniz M, Galasko D, Salmon DP.
Prev Alzheimers Dis. 2015;2(1):7–10. 27 Nasreddine ZS, Phillips NA, Bédirian V, Char- Subjective cognitive decline correlates with
16 Panza F, Solfrizzi V, Barulli MR, Santamato bonneau S, Whitehead V, Collin I, et al. The depression symptoms and not with concur-
A, Seripa D, Pilotto A, et al. Cognitive frailty: Montreal Cognitive Assessment, MoCA: a rent objective cognition in a clinic-based sam-
a systematic review of epidemiological and brief screening tool for mild cognitive impair- ple of older adults. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci
neurobiological evidence of an age related ment. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2005 Apr;53(4):695–9. Soc Sci. 2018 Sep;73(7):1198–202.
clinical condition. Rejuvenation Res. 2015 28 De Witte N, Hoeyberghs L, Verté E, De 41 Seo EH, Kim H, Choi KY, Lee KH, Choo IH.
Oct;18(5):389–412. Donder L, Dierckx E, Verté D, et al. The Com- Association of subjective memory complaint
17 Solfrizzi V, Scafato E, Seripa D, Lozupone M, prehensive Frailty Assessment Instrument and depressive symptoms with objective cog-
Imbimbo BP, D’Amato A, et al.; Italian Lon- enables to Detect Multidimensional Frailty in nitive functions in prodromal Alzheimer’s dis-
gitudinal Study on Aging Working Group. Community-Dwelling Older People. Healthy ease including pre-mild cognitive impairment.
Reversible Cognitive Frailty, Dementia, and Aging Res. 2018;7(13). J Affect Disord. 2017 Aug;217(217):24–8.
All-Cause Mortality. J Am Med Dir Assoc. 29 Julayanont P, Hemrungrojn S, Tangwongchai 42 Fratiglioni L, Wang HX, Ericsson K, Maytan M,
2017 Jan;18(1):89.e1–8. S. The effect of education and literacy on per- Winblad B. Influence of social network on oc-
18 Clarfield AM, Bergman H, Kane R. Fragmen- formance on the Montreal Cognitive Assess- currence of dementia: a community-based lon-
tation of care for frail older people—an inter- ment among cognitively normal elderly. Al- gitudinal study. Lancet. 2000 Apr; 355(9212):
national problem. Experience from three zheimers Dement. 2013;9(4):P793. 1315–9.
countries: Israel, Canada, and the United 30 Doane PD, Seward LE. Measuring skewness: 43 Vella Azzopardi R, Beyer I, Vermeiren S,
States. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2001 Dec; 49(12): A forgotten statistic? J Stat Educ. 2013;19(2): Petrovic M, Van Den Noortgate N, Bautmans
1714–21. 1–18. I, et al.; Gerontopole Brussels Study group. In-
19 Borges MK, Canevelli M, Cesari M, Apraha- 31 Kim HY. Statistical notes for clinical research- creasing use of cognitive measures in the op-
mian I. Frailty as a predictor of cognitive dis- ers: assessing normal distribution (2) using erational definition of frailty-A systematic re-
orders: a systematic review and meta-analysis. skewness and kurtosis. Restor Dent Endod. view. Ageing Res Rev. 2018 May;43:10–6.
Front Med (Lausanne). 2019 Feb;6:26. 2013 Feb;38(1):52–4. 44 Montejo P, Montenegro M, Fernández MA,
20 Fougère B, Daumas M, Lilamand M, Sourdet 32 Field A. Discovering statistics using IBM Maestu F. Subjective memory complaints in
S, Delrieu J, Vellas B, et al. Association be- SPSS statistics. London: SAGE; 2013. the elderly: prevalence and influence of tem-
tween frailty and cognitive impairment: 33 Holmén K, Ericsson K, Winblad B. Social and poral orientation, depression and quality of
cross-sectional data from Toulouse frailty day emotional loneliness among non-demented life in a population-based study in the city of
hospital. J Am Med Dir Assoc. 2017 Nov; and demented elderly people. Arch Gerontol Madrid. Aging Ment Health. 2011 Jan; 15(1):
18(11):990.e1–5. Geriatr. 2000 Dec;31(3):177–92. 85–96.
129.173.72.87 - 8/5/2019 5:48:22 AM

10 Gerontology De Roeck/van der Vorst/Engelborghs/


Kellogg Health Sciences Libr.

DOI: 10.1159/000501168 Zijlstra/Dierckx/D-SCOPE Consortium


Downloaded by:

You might also like