Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 41

(eBook PDF) Social Psychology: Goals

in Interaction 6th Edition


Visit to download the full and correct content document:
https://ebooksecure.com/download/ebook-pdf-social-psychology-goals-in-interaction-
6th-edition/
Contents  vii

Active and Public Commitments 205 Hormones and Sexual Desire 250
Gender and Public Conformity 206 Sociosexual Orientation 251
Revisiting: The Turnaround of Steve Hassan 208 Same-Sex Attraction 252
Chapter Summary 209 • Key Terms 210 Arousing Settings 253
Cultural Norms about Sexuality 253
Sexual Situations Look Different to Men and Women 254
7 Affiliation and Friendship 211 Cultural Practices May Trick Evolved Mechanisms 255
Establishing Family Bonds 256
The Fugitive Who Befriended the God-King 212
The Importance of Attachment 257
What Is a Friend? 213
Attachment Style 257
Goals of Affiliation and Friendship 213
Exchange/Communal Orientation 258
Getting Social Support 216 Threats Magnify Attachment 259
Bridging Theory and Application: Health Psychology Bridging Function and Dysfunction:
and Emotional Support216 Obsessive Relationships and Unrequited Love 259
Do Women Tend and Befriend While Men Jealousy and Same-Sex Competitors 260
Fight or Take Flight? 217
Relationships Change Our Personalities 261
Threats: Why Misery (Sometimes) Loves Company 219
Gaining Resources and Social Status 262
Pushing Support Away 220
Gender and Sexual Orientation 262
Bridging Function and Dysfunction:
Culture, Resources, and Polygamy 265
The Self-Perpetuating Cycle of Loneliness
Social Exchange in Committed Relationships 266
and Depression 220
When Dominance Matters 267
Attachment and Social Development 222
Breaking Up (and Staying Together) 269
Getting Information 223
Some People Are Better at Getting Along 270
Social Comparison and Liking for Similar Others 224
Some Situations Pull Couples Apart 270
Self-Disclosers and Nondisclosers 224
Interactions: It Takes Two to Tango 271
Uncertainty about Important Issues 225
Bridging Theory and Application: Studying Healthy
Similarity to Us 225
Communication to Save Marriages272
When Dissimilarity Can Save Self-Esteem 227
Revisiting: The Love Affair of “The Elephant
Gaining Status 228
and the Dove” 273
Men’s Friendships Are More Hierarchical 228
Chapter Summary 274 • Key Terms 275
Status by Association 229
Men’s Status-Seeking May Erode Social Support 230
Exchanging Material Benefits
Fundamental Patterns of Social Exchange
231
232
9 Prosocial Behavior 276
Individual Differences in Communal Orientation 233 The Strange Case of Sempo Sugihara 277
Communal and Exchange Relationships 234 The Goals of Prosocial Behavior 278
Proximity and Social Capital 234 Improving Our Basic Welfare: Gaining Genetic
Distant Friends: Television, Facebook, and the Internet 235 and Material Benefits 279
Are Exchange Relationships Different in Western Insights into the Evolution of Help 280
and Non-Western Cultures? 236
Using Behavioral Genetics to Study Helping 281
Revisiting: The Fugitive Who Befriended the God-King238
Learning to Help 282
Chapter Summary 239 • Key Terms 241 Similarity and Familiarity 283
Bridging Theory and Application: Getting Help
by Adjusting the Helper’s Sense of “We”284
8 Love and Romantic Relationships 242
Gaining Social Status and Approval 286
The Love Affair of “The Elephant and the Dove” 243 Social Responsibility: The Helping Norm 287
Defining Love and Romantic Attraction 244 Desire for Approval 290
The Defining Features of Love 244 Effects of Those around Us 290
Are There Different Varieties of Love? 245 Gender and Help 291
The Goals of Romantic Relationships 246 Managing Self-Image 293
Obtaining Sexual Gratification 247 Personal Norms and Religious Codes 294
Who’s Sexually Attractive? 247 Labeling and Self-focus 296
Gender Differences in Sexuality 248 Deciding Not to Help Friends or to Seek Their Help 297
viii  Contents

Bridging Function and Dysfunction: Failing to Seek


Needed Help298 11 Prejudice, Stereotyping,
Managing Our Emotions and Moods 299 and Discrimination 349
Managing Emotional Arousal in Emergencies:
The Unlikely Journey of Ann Atwater and C. P. Ellis 350
The Arousal/Cost–Reward Model 300
Managing Mood in Nonemergencies: Planet Prejudice 351
The Negative State Relief Model 301 Prejudice and Stereotypes 353
Does Pure Altruism Exist? 305 Discrimination353
The Empathy–Altruism Sequence 305 The Costs of Prejudice, Stereotyping, and
An Egoistic Interpretation 307 Discrimination355
Revisiting: The Case of Sempo Sugihara 308 The Goals of Prejudice, Stereotyping, and
Discrimination358
Chapter Summary 309 • Key Terms 310
Supporting and Protecting One’s Group 358
Creating and Maintaining Ingroup Advantage 359
10 Aggression311 Social Dominance Orientation 361
Intergroup Competition 362
A Wave of Senseless Violence 312
The Self-Fulfilling Spiral of Intergroup Competition 363
What Is Aggression? 313
Seeking Social Approval 364
Different Types of Aggression 314
Religiosity and Prejudice 365
Gender Differences in Aggression May Depend
Prejudice Norms Change Over Time 366
on Your Definition 314
Perceived Social Standing and Prejudice
The Goals of Aggressive Behavior 316
Expression367
Coping with Feelings of Annoyance 318
Managing Self-Image 368
The Frustration–Aggression Hypothesis 318
Personal and Social Identities 368
Feelings of Arousal and Irritability 319
Ingroup Identification 369
Unpleasant Situations 320
Authoritarianism and Prejudice 370
Annoyance Leads to Changes in Perception of Situations322
Bridging Function and Dysfunction:
Some People Create Their Own Annoying Situations 324
The Authoritarian Personality370
Gaining Material and Social Rewards 325
Failure and Self-Image Threat 371
Bridging Function and Dysfunction:
Self-Esteem and Threat 371
Gangland Violence325
Seeking Mental Efficiency 372
Social Learning Theory: Rewarding Violence 326
The Characteristics of Efficient Stereotypes 374
Who Finds Rewards in Violence? 326
Need for Structure 376
Glamorized Violence in the Media 327
Moods and Emotions 376
Using Meta-analysis to Examine the Effects
Cognitively Taxing Circumstances 377
of Violent Media 328
Overheard Ethnic Slurs 378
Violent Media Magnify Violent Inclinations 331
Reducing Prejudice, Stereotyping, and Discrimination 379
Gaining or Maintaining Social Status 332
Interventions Based on the Ignorance Hypothesis 379
Aggression and Sexual Selection 332
The Goal-Based Approach 380
Sex and Testosterone 333
When Contact Helps 382
Insults and the Culture of Honor 335
Bridging Theory and Application: Cooperation
When Status Matters 336
in the Classroom384
Protecting Oneself or Others 338
Revisiting: The Journey of Ann Atwater
Self-Defenders338
and C. P. Ellis 386
Perceived Threats 339
Chapter Summary 387 • Key Terms 388
Self-protective Aggression Can Increase Danger 340
Reducing Violence 341
Rewarding Alternatives to Aggression 341
Bridging Theory and Application: Using Cognition
12 Groups389
to Manage Angry Arousal 342 Blowing the Whistle on Hidden Group Pathologies 390
Legal Punishments 343 The Nature of Groups 392
Revisiting: Senseless Violence 345 The Mere Presence of Others and Social Facilitation 392
Chapter Summary 346 • Key Terms 348 Crowds and Deindividuation 394
Contents  ix

Groups as Dynamic Systems: The Emergence of Norms395 Bridging Theory and Application: Increasing
“Real” Groups 397 Intergroup Cooperation with the GRIT Strategy453
Why Do People Belong to Groups? 399 Revisiting: The Future 455
Getting Things Done 400 Chapter Summary 455 • Key Terms 457
Lightening the Load, Dividing the Labor 400
Bridging Function and Dysfunction: The Social
Disease of Social Loafing 401 14 Integrating Social Psychology 458
Expectations of Individual Failure and Group Success 403 Public Spectacles, Hidden Conspiracies,
Current Needs, Individualistic Societies 403 and Multiple Motives 459
When Are Groups Most Productive? 404
What Ground Have We Covered? 460
Making Accurate Decisions 407
Findings and Theories 461
The Need to Know 408
Major Theoretical Perspectives of Social Psychology 462
Uncertain Circumstances 408
The Sociocultural Perspective 463
Discussion and Decision Making 409
The Evolutionary Perspective 464
Bridging Theory and Application: Majority
The Social Learning Perspective 467
and Minority Influence in the Jury Room 412
The Social Cognitive Perspective 467
Gaining Positions of Leadership 415 Are Gender Differences in Our Genes, in Our Cultural
Who Wants to Lead? 416 Learning Experiences, or All in Our Minds? 468
When Opportunity Knocks 417 Combining the Different Perspectives 471
Who Gets to Lead? 418 Social Behavior Is Goal Oriented 471
When Are Leaders Effective? 419 Bridging Function and Dysfunction:
Revisiting: The Revealed Pathologies of the FBI, The Thin Line between Normal and Abnormal
Enron, and WorldCom 422 Social Functioning 474
Chapter Summary 424 • Key Terms 425 The Interaction between the Person and the Situation 476
Why Research Methods Matter 480
Bridging Method and Evidence: Some Conclusions
13 Social Dilemmas: Cooperation for Consumers of Social Science Information 481
versus Conflict 426 How Does Social Psychology Fit into the Network
Contrasting Future Worlds 427 of Knowledge? 482
Defining Social Dilemmas 428 Bridging Theory and Application: Social Psychology’s
Usefulness for Business, Medicine, and Law484
Bridging Function and Dysfunction: The Tragedy
of the Commons430 The Future of Social Psychology 486
Chapter Summary 487 • Key Terms 488
Interlocking Problems and Solutions 432
What Goals Underlie Global Social Dilemmas? 432
Gaining Immediate Satisfaction 433 Glossary489
Social Traps 434 References495
Egoistic versus Prosocial Orientations 435
Changing the Consequences of Short-Sighted Text Credits 545
Selfishness437 Photo Credits 548
Matching Interventions with Motives 439
Name Index 550
Defending Ourselves and Valued Others 441
Outgroup Bias and International Conflict 441 Subject Index 577
Some of Us Are More Defensive Than Others 442 Answer Key 589
Competition and Threat 445
Intercultural Misperception and International Conflict 449
The Reciprocal Dynamics of Cooperation and Conflict 450
Welcome from the Authors

S How Do We Accomplish
ocial psychology is intrinsically fun. Indeed, to stu-
dents assigned a typical social psychology textbook,
the field must sometimes seem like an amazing three-
the Integration?
ring circus, where every turn of the eye reveals a dizzying For a full year before deciding to write this book, we met for
assortment of attention-demanding performances. A differ- an afternoon every week to try to develop a truly integrative
ent show unfolds in each ring—awe-inspiring acts of altru- framework for the course. We knew that we had one ironic
ism, shocking deeds of aggression, persuasive tricks from advantage: In a basic way, we disagreed with one another.
magicians’ hats, human pyramids of cooperation, and mys- Each of us had approached the task with a different one of
terious feats of self-delusion. At the center of it all stands the the major, sometimes opposing, theoretical perspectives in
course instructor, the ringmaster, calling students’ attention social psychology today—social cognition, social learning,
alternately to one then another facet of the spectacle—And and evolutionary psychology. We realized that if we could
now, ladies and gentlemen, I invite you to shift your gaze from the find an overarching framework that would bridge our
clownish antics of self-deception to the daring men and women at- ­diverse approaches, it would provide an especially broad
tempting to traverse the tightrope of romantic love, and then back foundation for integrating the course material—one that
down to the wild lion pit of aggression. ­allowed and incorporated a full range of theoretical starting
points.
The Need for an Integrative Those meetings were an exhilarating mix of good-natured
conflicts, eye-opening insights, false starts, blind alleys,
Approach to Social Psychology and gratifying breakthroughs—always accompanied by
But there’s a problem with the three-ring circus presen- the shared sense that our understanding of social psychol-
tation of social psychology. It masks a critically import- ogy was growing. The effort would have been worthwhile
ant point: Human social behaviors are woven together even if no book had come of it. At the end of that year of
in related, interconnected patterns. To present an array discussion and debate, not only did we have an invaluable
of separate, disjointed chapter topics—aggression here, mid-career learning experience under our belts, but, as well,
persuasion, p ­ rejudice, and personal relationships there, we had consensus on an integrative framework about which
there, and there—offers a sorely inadequate view of the we were all genuinely enthusiastic.
field. Hidden beneath all the dazzling aspects of human The text’s subtitle, “Goals in Interaction,” reflects the
­social behavior, there are a central set of common concepts, two key themes that we use to tie together the text material
­dimensions, and principles. We are convinced that students within and across chapter topics:
benefit greatly from discovering those underlying princi-
1. The goal-directed nature of social behavior. First, we stress
ples. After all, a primary rule of learning and memory is
that social responding is goal directed. People might not
that people grasp and retain more material, more easily,
even be able to consciously describe their goals, but when
when the various parts can be connected by organizing
they obey an authority figure, begin a new relationship, or
principles.
raise a fist against another, they do so in the service of some
As entertaining and stimulating as a circus may be, it is
goal—perhaps to gain another’s approval, verify a self-im-
not a good arena for learning. Much better, and equally en-
age, or acquire social status. In Chapter 1, we describe how
gaging, is a well-constructed work of theater, cinema, or lit-
everyday goals flow from fundamental social motives, such
erature. The field of social psychology should be presented
as establishing social ties, attracting mates, and under-
to students as a captivating and coherent chronicle, not a be-
standing ourselves and those around us. In Chapter 2, we
wildering circus. It’s an intricate chronicle to be sure, rich in
examine how goals work. In each succeeding chapter, we
twists and variations. But it is coherent nonetheless, with re-
reestablish this emphasis on goals by asking the question
curring characters, scenes, and themes linking its elements.
“What particular goals are served by aggression, confor-
Our major purpose in writing this text is to offer students
mity, prejudice (or whichever particular behaviors we con-
and instructors a cohesive framework that retains social
sider in that chapter)?”
psychology’s renowned ability to captivate student interest
but that adds the more intellectually helpful (and satisfying) 2. The interaction of the person and the situation. Second, to
feature of integration. understand fully the causes of a person’s social behavior,

x
Welcome from the Authors  xi

we need to consider how aspects of that person interact important bridge discipline, connecting different areas of
with aspects of his or her situation. How do features inside psychology (such as neuroscience, developmental, and
the individual—attitudes, traits, expectations, attributions, clinical psychology) as well as other behavioral sciences
moods, goals, stereotypes, and emotions—work together (such as anthropology, economics, political science, and
with features of the situation to influence social behavior? zoology).
Beginning with Kurt Lewin, this interactionist theme has
been prominent in our field. Unfortunately, introductory
social psychology texts have rarely engaged the full explan-
What’s New in the Sixth Edition?
atory power of interactionism. In contrast, in this book, we 1. Videos to accompany the opening mysteries. In this edi-
continuously invite readers to consider the interplay of in- tion, David Lundberg Kenrick has lent his film production
fluences inside and outside the person. talents to developing a short animated video at the begin-
ning of each chapter, in which the text authors introduce the
mysteries of social life we will try to unravel in the c­ hapter.
Bridging Perspectives: Cognition, Chapter 1 begins with the story of a formerly destitute sin-
Culture, and Evolution gle mom who, once her luck turned around, began giving
away millions and millions of her hard-earned dollars. The
For the last two decades, social psychologists have profit-
question of why some people hoard their wealth, while
ably mined the cognitive perspective for insights into how
others become generous philanthropists, connects to a fas-
humans process information about their social situations.
cinating series of studies of social psychologist Elizabeth
These insights added to a foundation of findings discovered
Dunn and her colleagues, on the psychological benefits of
within the social learning perspective. In recent years, as
giving to others. J.K. Rowling is the formerly destitute sin-
researchers have made fascinating discoveries about social
gle mom in this story, and her case also helps raise ques-
behavior in different human cultures and different animal
tions about the relative influences of social learning, culture,
species, the sociocultural and evolutionary perspectives
cognition, and biology on our social decisions. Throughout
have increasingly contributed to the mix.
the book, we introduce other mysteries of social behavior, in
The sociocultural perspective has emphasized how our
the stories of people as diverse as Martin Luther King, the
social thoughts and behaviors are encompassed within the
Dalai Lama, and Charles Manson, in each case, to introduce
larger context of the societies we live in. Cultural influences can
questions that have been addressed by scientific research in
change the answer to questions about which techniques of per-
­social psychology.
suasion will be effective, whether a person will define herself in
terms of her group memberships or her individual qualities, or 2. Original research videos. Social psychologists not only
whether that person will marry one partner or many. The study probe into some of the most fascinating mysteries of so-
of culture is fascinating because it often highlights differences, cial life, but they do so with scientific methods that are, in
and reminds us that “our way” isn’t always the only way. themselves, quite fascinating. For this edition, we also in-
But cross-cultural research has also taught us that hu- troduce researchers from around the world, who briefly de-
mans the world over have some common ways of thinking scribe one of the questions they were able to answer with
and behaving around one another. The evolutionary per- their research. For example, to accompany the first chapter’s
spective has helped us understand why there are similarities opening mystery, Liz Dunn from the University of British
not only across human cultures, but even across different Columbia describes her work on the benefits of giving
species. Initial forays into evolutionary psychology empha- money away, including some fascinating new cross-cultural
sized the darker side of human nature—“selfish genes” driv- and developmental twists on the topic. In the chapter on
ing aggression, sexuality, and the battle between the sexes. groups, Mark Van Vugt from VU Amsterdam describes his
But evolutionary analyses have revealed that our ancestors work on leadership, explaining how, contrary to stereotypes,
survived not just by selfish competition but also by posi- people are much more likely to choose female leaders over
tive behaviors: forming friendships, cooperating with other males under the right circumstances. In the chapter on social
members of their groups, and forging loving family bonds. dilemmas, Texas Christian University’s Sarah Hill describes
It has become clear that these various perspectives some fascinating research demonstrating how economic fac-
are not “alternatives” to one another. Instead, they work tors can influence White people’s tendencies to perceive a
together to enable a fuller understanding of the social “mixed-race” person as either Black or White. And for the
world. As long-term students of cognition, culture, and chapter on self-presentation, University of Queensland’s
evolutionary psychology, we have woven these threads to- Bill Von Hippel describes some research he conducted in
gether into the unique interactionist tapestry of this book. a skateboard park, demonstrating how the mere presence
In this edition, we emphasize how social psychology is an of a beautiful young woman boosted male skateboarders’
xii  Welcome from the Authors

testosterone levels, which in turn led them to literally risk begin their search with an interesting or perplexing question,
their necks doing more dangerous tricks. then examine clues, gather evidence, test hypotheses, elimi-
nate alternatives and—if things fall into place—uncover the
3. Learning Objectives. Each major section of every chapter
right answer. To mine these instructive parallels, we return
begins with a set of explicit learning objectives that serve as
often in the text to the concept of researcher-as-detective.
road-maps to focus the reader on the central concepts in up-
coming sections.
2. The Goals
4. Quick Quiz Self-Tests. Following each major section, we Next we introduce readers to the set of goals underlying
present a short series of multiple-choice questions, to give the behavior covered in the chapter, by asking “What pur-
you a chance to check your understanding of the material poses does this behavior (e.g., aggression or helping or con-
and practice for exams. formity) serve for an individual?” and “Which factors lead
5. New and expanded coverage. There are a wide range of new an individual to use this behavior to achieve those goals?”
and expanded topics covered in this edition, including many Taking each goal of the set in turn, we consider factors in the
new findings linking social psychology, culture, and neuro- person, in the situation, and in their interaction:
science. In Chapter 7, for example, we present new findings PERSON The person. Here, we present research show-
suggesting that your brain responds differently to your wins
ing which factors inside the individual trigger each partic-
versus those of your friends, unless your culture encourages
ular goal. So, which traits motivate people to seek social
you to think about yourself as part of a collective. Many other
approval through conformity? Which moods influence peo-
new findings build bridges between social psychology and
ple to think deeply in order to understand themselves and
other disciplines, such as findings showing that you r­ espond
others more accurately?
very differently to economic losses when you are in mat-
ing-motivated frame of mind (Chapter 14). In fact, almost SITUATION The situation. Here, we consider evidence of
three hundred new references have been added to the sixth situational factors that trigger each goal. How do personal
edition, the majority of which come from new research papers threats engage self-protective prejudices? How do cul-
published in 2011 or later. tural norms influence the desire to seek sexual gratification
through casual relationships? How does time pressure af-
The Structure of Each Chapter fect the inclination to think deeply before deciding what a
stranger’s personality is like?
After introducing social psychology (Chapter 1) and taking a
closer look at the person and the social situation (Chapter 2), we interaction The person–situation interaction. In this sec-
organize the remaining chapters around a common structure: tion, we present data demonstrating how personal and situa-
1. The Mystery tional factors interact. Social psychologists are used to thinking
about how people with different attitudes, expectations, and
 ach chapter begins with an account of a baffling pattern
E
traits act differently in the same situation. But interactions
of human behavior—an incident or a set of incidents that
are much richer than this: People choose their life situations,
seems beyond understanding. For example:
change situations they do not like, and are themselves rejected
• Why did the beautiful and talented artist Frida Kahlo fall from some situations and changed by others. For example,
for the much older, and much less attractive, Diego Rivera, lonely people sometimes act in needy ways that alienate oth-
and then tolerate his numerous extramarital affairs? ers. In turn, others may avoid them and stop inviting them to
• What forces could persuade a young man to sign a con- social events, further enhancing their inner feelings of social
fession saying he’d killed his own mother, when later isolation. By systematically showing students the importance
evidence suggested he could not possibly have done it? of person–situation interactions, we hope to illustrate the lim-
• How did a Black civil rights advocate and a member of itations of the usual single-factor explanations—such as put-
the Ku Klux Klan turn around and become friends with ting all the blame for aggression or blind obedience on the
one another? person or the converse error of viewing people as interchange-
able pawns on a giant interpersonal chess board.
Later, as the chapter progresses, we introduce general prin-
ciples of human behavior that, when put together properly, 3. Special Features
resolve the mystery. These mysteries are more than simple
Several of social psychology’s messages and themes are
devices for engaging readers’ interest. They are designed to
highlighted in each chapter’s special features:
convey something basic about how we approach the text ma-
terial: Our approach is heavily research based, and research Investigation. Building on our metaphor of social psychol-
is akin to good detective work. Researchers, like detectives, ogist as detective, we invite students to connect themselves
Welcome from the Authors  xiii

to the concepts in the “Investigation” feature. These ques- Weaving Methods and Applications
tions encourage students to enter an investigation, either by
piecing together the concepts and findings in the book with
into the Story
what they know about themselves or other people, or by us- A glance at the table of contents shows that we have in-
ing their own powers of logical analysis to critically analyze cluded no separate applications chapters on such topics as
the evidence just covered. “Investigation” questions are de- health, business, or the law. This is not because of any lack of
signed not only to emphasize the relevance of social psychol- regard for their importance within social psychology. Quite
ogy to students’ lives but also to help students study more the reverse. Rather than giving these topics a tagged-on,
effectively. Research on learning and memory shows we stand-alone status in the book, we want to emphasize their
learn material more easily if we connect it to ourselves, think frequent connections to the mainstream topics of the field.
critically about it, and actively rehearse what we’ve just read. Consequently, we point out these links as they occur natu-
rally within the text discussion, and (when special elabo-
INVESTIGATION ration is appropriate) in the Bridging Theory and Application
features found in the chapters. In this way, we hope to con-
Consider two people you know whose cultural backgrounds differ
from yours (another country, a different social class, ethnicity, or vey to students the inherent relationship between the prin-
religion). In what ways do the norms of your different cultures lead ciples of social psychology and the behaviors of people in
you to behave differently in your interactions with each other? workplaces, schoolrooms, and other applied settings.
For similar reasons, there is no isolated chapter or ap-
Bridging Theory and Application. Here, we discuss how a pendix on methodology. Although we do expose the reader
specific experimental finding or body of findings relates to to the major methodological issues of social psychological
real-world issues—how research insights can be used to cre- research in Chapter 1, we blend the discussion of methods
ate less-prejudiced classrooms, help married couples stay with the puzzling research questions that inspire those meth-
together, or reduce violence. ods, so the student learns the details of the methods that can
answer them (for example, we introduce the idea of me-
Bridging Function and Dysfunction. Psychology students ta-analysis alongside the many studies of media influences
are fascinated by disordered behavior. In this feature, we on aggression). Additionally, the student learns to appreciate
tap that fascination to demonstrate broader principles. that one cannot be fully confident in the results of a study
We examine how normally healthy social behaviors can, without understanding how those results were obtained.
if taken too far, produce unhealthy consequences—for Last, and once again reflecting our emphasis on integra-
example, how the usually adaptive tendency to develop tion, the chapters are not grouped and divided into separate
strong bonds between lovers can underlie obsessive sections, such as social knowing, social influence, and social
relationships. relationships. Instead, the chapter topics flow in a contin-
uum from phenomena occurring primarily inside the indi-
4. Revisiting the Mystery vidual to those occurring primarily outside. However, there
The final section of each chapter returns to the opening mys- is no imperative to this ordering and, with the exception
tery to help students pull together the various research find- of the first and last chapters, instructors may sequence the
ings discussed in the chapter. For example, we return to the chapters to fit their own preferences without harm to stu-
puzzle of the boy who falsely confessed to a heinous crime dent understanding.
and the relationship between Frida Kahlo and Diego Rivera, One reason for this adaptability is that the integration
in light of research findings on persuasion and relationships we have proposed does not depend on any lock-step, build-
(and we pull together the new clues we revealed in the ing-block progress through the course material. Rather, that
chapter). In this way, we hope not only to capitalize on cu- integration comes from a pair of concepts, goals and the per-
riosity but also to tap another general principle of learning son–situation interaction, that apply generally to the topics
and memory—the principle that students recall more facts of the course. Although the goals may not be the same, the
when they are connected to vivid cases. ways that goals function—the mechanisms by which they
develop and operate—are similar in the case of aggression
5. Chapter Summary or attraction or self-presentation or any of the social behav-
The Chapter Summary feature at the end of each c­ hapter iors we consider. And, although the particular factors may
­includes a number of useful review tools for students: differ depending on the behavior under study, understand-
a chart that revisits how factors in the person, in the situ- ing how factors in the person interact with factors in the sit-
ation, and in their interaction relate to the chapter’s goals uation provides the most informed insights into the causes
(in Chapters 3–14); and a numbered summary of text con- of everyday social behaviors—whatever the behaviors,
tent organized by A-head. in whichever order they are considered. Our two central
xiv  Welcome from the Authors

concepts, then, allow an organization that we think is both MyTest Test Bank (ISBN 0133972720)
integrative and flexible. This powerful assessment-generation program helps in-
In the pages that follow, readers will find everyday so- structors easily create and print quizzes and exams. Ques-
cial behaviors depicted as something more tightly woven tions and tests can be authored online, allowing instructors
and interconnected than a three-ring circus. Beyond being ultimate flexibility and the ability to efficiently manage as-
“the greatest show on earth,” social psychology may well be sessments anytime, anywhere. For more information, go to
the greatest story—breathtaking, coherent, and, most of all, www.PearsonMyTest.com.
instructive. We hope you will agree.
PowerPoint Presentation (0133984958)
REVEL™ Each chapter’s PowerPoint presentation contains key points
REVEL™ is Pearson’s newest way of delivering our re- covered in the textbook, images from the textbook, and
spected content. Fully digital and highly engaging, REVEL questions to provoke effective classroom discussion and
offers an immersive learning experience designed for the add life to lectures. Available for download on the Instruc-
way today’s students read, think, and learn. Enlivening tor’s Resource Center at www.pearsonhighered.com.
course content with media interactives and assessments,
MyPsychLab™ (ISBN 0133972526)
REVEL empowers educators to increase engagement with
the course, and to better connect with students. Available at www.mypsychlab.com, MyPsychLab™ is an
For more information, please contact your local Pearson online homework, tutorial, and assessment program that
representative or visit www.pearsonhighered.com/revel. truly engages students in learning. It helps students bet-
ter prepare for class, quizzes, and exams—resulting in
For the Instructor better performance in the course. It provides educators
a dynamic set of tools for gauging individual and class
Download Instructor Resources at the Instructor’s
performance:
Resource Center
• Customizable. MyPsychLab is customizable. Instructors
Register or log in to the Instructor Resource Center to down-
choose what students’ course looks like. Homework,
load supplements from our online catalog. Go to http://
applications, and more can easily be turned on and off.
www.pearsonhighered.com/educator.
For technical support for any of your Pearson products, • Blackboard Single Sign-On. MyPsychLab can be used by
you and your students can contact http://247.pearsoned.com. itself or linked to any course management system. Black-
board single sign-on provides deep linking to all new
Instructor’s Resource Manual (ISBN 0133984974) MyPsychLab resources.
This rich collection of teaching materials can be used by first- • Pearson eText and Chapter Audio. Like the printed text,
time or experienced teachers to enrich class presentations. students can highlight relevant passages and add notes.
For each chapter of the text, the instructor’s manual includes The Pearson eText can be accessed through laptops,
an At-a-Glance Grid with detailed pedagogical information iPads, and tablets. Download the free Pearson eText app
linking chapter topics to other available resources, learning to use on tablets. Students can also listen to their text
objectives targeting specific goals for each chapter, a com- with the Audio eText.
prehensive chapter overview, a list of key terms, detailed • Assignment Calendar. A drag and drop assignment calen-
lecture outlines, lecture and discussion suggestions, class- dar makes assigning and completing work easy.
room learning activities, and other detailed pedagogical in-
formation. In addition, this manual includes a preface and a Acknowledgments
sample syllabus. Available for download on the Instructor’s
We would like to acknowledge the many reviewers who
Resource Center at www.pearsonhighered.com.
read this sixth edition, as well as those who examined earlier
Test Bank (ISBN 0133984982) editions. We greatly appreciate their thoughtful feedback.
Our comprehensive test bank contains over 100 challenging
items per chapter, in multiple-choice, true/false, and essay Sixth Edition Reviewers
format. Each question is correlated to a learning objective, Linda Bastone, Purchase College, SUNY
skill level, and a difficulty rating to allow customization of Kiersten Baughman, University of Oklahoma
the assessment materials to best fit your needs. Available Gordon Bear, Ramapo College of New Jersey
for download on the Instructor’s Resource Center at www. James Benjamin, University of Arkansas-Fort Smith
pearsonhighered.com. Alex Czopp, Western Washington University
Welcome from the Authors  xv

Michele Delucia, Gateway Community College Southern Illinois University, Edwardsville; Joshua Duntley,
Heidi English, College of the Siskiyous Florida Institute of Technology; Nancy F. Dye, Humboldt
Stacy Fambro, Wesleyan University State University; Kelli England, Virginia Polytechnic Insti-
Carey Fitzgerald, Oakland University tute; Victoria Esses, University of Western Ontario; Robert
Omri Gillath, University of Kansas Fern, Mesa Community College; Joseph R. Ferrari, DePaul
Sheldon Helms, Ohlone College University; Phillip Finney, Southeast Missouri State Uni-
Alishia Huntoon, Oregon Institute of Technology versity; Barry Friedman, Lebanon Valley College; Robert W.
Jill Lorenzi, Virginia Tech Fuhrman, University of Texas—San Antonio; Grace G ­ alliano,
Julie Luker, The College of St. Scholastica Kennesaw State College; Stella Garcia, University of Texas—
Katy Neidhardt, Cuesta Community College San Antonio; Bill Garris, Cumberland College; Bryan
Karyn Plumm, University of North Dakota Gibson, Central Michigan University; Noah Goldstein,
Joanna Schug, College of William & Mary ­A rizona State University; Sheldon W. Helms, Ohlone
Dylan Selterman, University of Maryland, College Park ­College; Marti Hope Gonzales, University of Minnesota;
Heather Terrell, University of North Dakota Kenneth J. Good, Minnesota State University, Mankato;
Anre Venter, University of Notre Dame Mark Hartlaub, Texas A & M University at Corpus Christi;
Edward Witt, Michigan State University John Harvey, University of Iowa; Cindy Hazan, C ­ ornell
University; Edward Hirt, Indiana University; David
Previous Edition Reviewers and Survey ­H ouston, University of Memphis; Lisa Hollis-Sawyer,
Respondents Northeastern Illinois University; Robert Hymes, Univer-
Jeffrey M. Adams, High Point University; William Adler, sity of Michigan—Dearborn; Blair Johnson, University of
Colin County Community College; David W. Alfano, Com- Connecticut; Heide Island, University of Central ­Arkansas;
munity College of Rhode Island; Scott T. Allison, University Craig A. Johnson, Hofstra University; Mike Jordan, F ­ rancis
of Richmond; Michael L. Atkinson, University of Western Marion University; Pat Kalata, Burlington County ­College;
Ontario; Anita P. Barbee, University of Louisville; Jennifer Martin Kaplan, Northern Illinois University; Rich Keefe,
Barber, University of Michigan; John Bargh, Yale University; Scottsdale Community College; Suzanne Keiffer, Uni-
Bruce Bartholow, University of North Carolina, Chapel Hill; versity of Houston; Bruce Kelly, Lindenwood Univer-
Deborah Belle, Boston University; Roy Baumeister, Case sity; Cyndi Kernahan, University of Wisconsin; Michael
Western Reserve University; Gordon Bear, Ramapo College; Kitchens, Lebanon Valley College; Linda Kline, C ­ alifornia
Susan E. Beers, Sweet Briar College; Frank Bernieri, Univer- State University–Chico; Roger J. Kreuz, The University
sity of Toledo; John Bickford, University of Massachusetts, of Memphis; Joachim Krueger, Brown University; Robin
Amherst; Victor L. Bissonnette, Southeastern Louisiana ­K owalski, Clemson University; Catherine T. Kwantes,
University; Melinda Blackman, California State University– ­E astern Michigan University; Sherri Lantinga, Dordt
Fullerton; Galen Bodenhausen, Northwestern University; College; Mark Leary, Wake Forest University; Michael R.
Martin Bolt, Calvin College; Nyla Branscombe, University Leippe, St. Louis University; Richard Leo, University of
of Kansas; Martin Bourgeois, University of Wyoming; Fred ­California at Irvine; Heather Lench, Texas A&M Univer-
B. Bryant, Loyola University Chicago; Jeff Bryson, San Diego sity; Angela Lipsitz, Northern Kentucky University; Chris
State University; Cheri Budzynski, Heidelberg College; Brad ­Loersch, Ohio State University; Keith Maddox, Tufts Uni-
J. Bushman, Iowa State University; Delia Cioffi, Dartmouth versity; Paul Magro, Saint Joseph’s College; Heike I. M.
College; Lisa N. Coates-Shrider, McMurry University; Diana Mahler, California State University at San Marcos; Robyn
Cordova, Yale University; Traci Craig, University of Idaho; Mallett, University of Virginia; Alan Marks, Morehouse
Robert Cramer, California State University, San Bernadino; College; David Marx, San Diego State University; Rene
Christian Crandall, University of Kansas; Michael Crow, Martin, University of Iowa; Pam McAuslan, University of
Southern Methodist University; Cynthia Crown, Xavier Michigan–Dearborn; Larry Messé, Michigan State Univer-
University; Virginia Cylke, Sweet Briar College; Deborah sity; Sarah A. Meyers, Simpson College; Rowland Miller,
Davis, University of Nevada-Reno; Joseph Davis, San Diego Sam Houston State University; Jeffrey Scott Mio, California
State University; Mark H. Davis, Eckerd College; Carl Denti, State Polytechnic University, Pomona; Paul A. Mongeau,
Dutchess Community College; Bella DePaulo, University Miami University; Paul Nail, Southwestern Oklahoma
of California-Santa Barbara; Virginia DeRoma, The Citadel; State University; Roderick Neal, Bluefield State College;
Patricia Devine, University of Wisconsin, Madison; Joan Joseph S. Neuschab, Roger Williams University; Cindy
DiGiovanni, Western New England College; Kenneth I. Nordstrom, Southern Illinois University, Edwardsville;
Dion, University of Toronto; William Dragon, Cornell Carol K. Oyster, University of Wisconsin at La Crosse;
­College; Steve Duck, University of Iowa; Michael G. Dudley, Ernest Park, Michigan State University; Miles L. Patterson,
xvi  Welcome from the Authors

University of Missouri—St. Louis; Lou Penner, Univer- Way-Schramm, Yavapi College; Ann Weber, University of
sity of South Florida; Alison Pfent, Ohio State University; North Carolina—Asheville; Anne Weiher, University of
­Lawrence Pervin, Rutgers University; Jacqueline Pope-Tar- Colorado; James Whyte, Grandview College; David Wilder,
rence, Western Kentucky University; Felicia Pratto, Univer- Rutgers University; Kevin Woller, Rogers State University;
sity of Connecticut; Diane Quinn, University of Connecticut; Wilhelmina Wosinska, Arizona State University; Marga-
Brandon Randolph-Seng, Texas Tech University; Cynthia K. ret Zimmerman, Virginia Wesleyan College; and Anthony
S. Reed, Tarrant County College; Pamela Regan, California Zoccolillo, DeVry College of Technology.
State University-Los Angeles; Robert Reeves, Augusta State Our home in the psychology department at Arizona
University; Pamela Regan, California State University, Los State University is intellectually stimulating and interper-
Angeles; John W. Reich, Arizona State University; Harry sonally collegial, for which we have always been grateful.
Reis, University of Rochester; Nancy Rhodes, Texas A & We wish to thank, in particular, our colleagues and s­ tudents
M University; Robert Ridge, Brigham Young University; who commented on early drafts of this book: Terrilee Asher,
Robert Riedel, Lynn University; James Roney, University Dan Barrett, Adam Cohen, Linda Demaine, Nancy Eisenberg,
of California-Santa Barbara; Martin Rosenman, Morehouse Rosanna Guadagno, Sara Gutierres, Carol Luce, Greg Neidert,
College; Alexander Rothman, University of Minnesota; Dan John Reich, Kelton Rhoads, Ed Sadalla, Brad Sagarin, Delia
Sachau, Minnesota State University, Mankato; Brad Sagarin, Saenz, Melanie Trost, and Wilhemina Wosinska. The last four
Northern Illinois State University; Catherine Salmon, editions also profited greatly from the help of Andy Delton,
Redlands University (CA); Eric Sambolec, Michigan State Andreana Kenrick (no relation), Jean Luce, Andy Menzel,
University; Mark Schaller, University of British Columbia; Megan Ringel, Kristopher Smith, and Josh Tybur. Ariana
Connie Schick, Bloomsburg University, Pennsylvania; P. Ehuan and Kelly Morford provided invaluable assistance
Wesley Schultz, California State University, San Marcos; Bill in preparing the animations for the new video series for the
Scott, Oklahoma State University; Chris Segrin, University 6th edition. Finally, Carol Luce and Jean Luce provided edi-
of Kansas; Charles F. Seidez, Mansfield University; Todd K. torial assistance, advice, and social support to the first author.
Shackelford, Florida Atlantic University; James Shepperd, We would especially like to thank the students in our
University of Florida; Robert Short, Arizona State Univer- social psychology classes for providing invaluable insights
sity; Laura S. Sidorowicz, Nassau Community College; Jeff from the perspective of the readers that most matter—
Simpson, Texas A & M University; Jessi Smith, Ohio State undergraduate students.
University; Jana Spain, High Point University; Kari Terzino, In writing this book, we have searched for interesting
Iowa State University; Dianne Tice, Case Western Reserve real-world events and stories to help illustrate the concepts
University; David M. Tom, Columbus State Community of social psychology. Several people were able to help us go
College; Timothy P. Tomczak, Genesee Community College; beyond what was already available in published books and
David Trafimow, New Mexico State University; ­Stephen articles, and we greatly appreciate their efforts: Dr. Avrum
Trotter, Tennessee State University; Robin Vallacher, ­Florida Bluming, Lenell Geter, Steven Hassan, Bradley Henry,
Atlantic University; Paul van Lange, Free University Cindy Jackson, Darlene and Bob Krueger, Eric Saul, and
(Amsterdam); James Clay Vaughn, Western Michigan Uni- Rabbi Marvin Tokayer.
versity; Anre Venter, University of Notre Dame; Terri Vescio, Turning a set of ideas into a textbook is a long, complex
Pennsylvania State University; Cheri Vetter, Glendale Com- task, and Jim Anker was there at the beginning to offer great
munity College; T. Joel Wade, Bucknell University; Karly advice. Much thanks.
About the Authors
For over ten years, Douglas Kenrick, Steven Neuberg, and Steven L. Neuberg is Foundation
Robert Cialdini met weekly over enchiladas, shwarma, or Professor of Psychology at Arizona
pasta to design experiments and debate the big issues in so- State University. He received his un-
cial psychology. Over time, they came to realize that they dergraduate degree from Cornell
agreed on several important things and that these ideas University and his graduate degrees
could form the foundation of an integrative and exciting from Carnegie-Mellon University. He
social psychology textbook. The authors each have years spent a postdoctoral year at the Uni-
of experience teaching social psychology to undergraduate versity of Waterloo in Canada and
and graduate students, in environments ranging from small has since taught at ASU. Neuberg’s
private colleges to large public universities. They have pub- research has been published in outlets such as Advances in
lished research in the field’s most prestigious journals on a Experimental Social Psychology, Journal of Personality and Social
wide range of topics, including social cognition, self-presen- Psychology, Psychological Science, Handbook of Social Psychol-
tation, persuasion and social influence, friendship and ro- ogy, and Perspectives on Psychological Science, and has been
mance, helping, aggression, and prejudice and stereotyping. supported by the National Institute of Mental Health and
Each is independently recognized for integrative research the National Science Foundation. He has received a half
that, when combined, inspires the two major themes of the dozen teaching honors, including his college’s Outstanding
book. This textbook brings together their many teaching Teaching Award and the ASU Honors College Outstanding
and research interests. Honors Disciplinary Faculty Award. He has served on fed-
eral grant review panels and as associate editor of the Jour-
Douglas T. Kenrick is a professor at nal of Experimental Social Psychology and teaches a graduate
Arizona State University. He received course on teaching social psychology.
his B.A. from Dowling College and
his Ph.D. from Arizona State Uni- Robert B. Cialdini is Regents’ Profes-
versity. He taught at Montana State sor Emeritus at Arizona State Univer-
University for four years before re- sity, where he has also been named
turning to ASU. His research has Graduate Distinguished Professor.
been published in a number of presti- He received his undergraduate de-
gious outlets, including Psychological gree from the University of Wiscon-
Review, Behavioral and Brain Sciences, American Psychologist, sin and his graduate degrees from the
Handbook of Social Psychology, Journal of Personality and Social University of North Carolina. He is
Psychology, Current Directions in Psychological Science, Per- a past president of the Society of Per-
spectives on Psychological Science, and Personality and Social sonality and Social Psychology and has received the Society’s
Psychology Review. He is author of the 2011 book: Sex, Mur- award for Distinguished Scientific Contributions. His research
der, and the Meaning of Life: A psychologist investigates how evo- has appeared in numerous publications, including Handbook
lution, cognition, and complexity are revolutionizing our view of of Social Psychology, Advances in Experimental Social Psychol-
human nature, and in 2013, with Vlad Griskevicius, he wrote ogy, and Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. His book,
The Rational Animal: How evolution made us smarter than we Influence: Science and Practice, has sold over 2 million copies
think. He has taught a graduate course on teaching psy- and has appeared in 28 languages.
chology, and he thoroughly enjoys teaching undergraduate
sections of social psychology, for which he has won several
teaching awards.

xvii
This page intentionally left blank
Social Psychology
Chapter 1

Introduction to
Social Psychology
Video

Outline
The Mysteries of Social Life 3 Basic Principles of Social Behavior 13 Social Psychology’s Bridges
Social Behavior Is Goal Oriented 14 with Other Areas of Knowledge 29
What Is Social Psychology? 4
The Interaction between the Person Social Psychology and Other Areas of
Describing and Explaining Social
and the Situation 16 Psychology 29
Behavior 4
Social Psychology and Other
Social Psychology Is an Interdisciplinary How Psychologists Study Social
Disciplines 31
Bridge 5 Behavior 17
Descriptive Methods 18 Revisiting the Mysteries
Major Theoretical Perspectives
Correlation and Causation 22 of Social Life 32
of Social Psychology 6
The Sociocultural Perspective 6 Experimental Methods 23 Chapter Summary 33
The Evolutionary Perspective 7 Why Social Psychologists Combine
Different Methods 25
The Social Learning Perspective 9
Ethical Issues in Social Psychological
The Social Cognitive Perspective 10
Research 27
Combining Perspectives 12

2
Chapter 1  •   The Mysteries of Social Life   3

Learning Objectives
LO 1.1 Define social psychology and explain why it relies on scientific description
and theory.
LO 1.2 Explain why social psychology is considered a bridge discipline.
LO 1.3 Summarize the four major theoretical perspectives of social psychology.
LO 1.4 Discuss how the four major perspectives work together to explain human
social behavior.
LO 1.5 Describe the five fundamental motives behind goal-oriented social behavior.
LO 1.6 Explain what is meant by the person, the situation, and person–situation
interactions.
LO 1.7 List the strengths and weaknesses of each of the different descriptive
methods (e.g., naturalistic observation, case study) and experimental
methods, and explain why researchers find value in combining them.
LO 1.8 Explain why it is difficult to infer causality from correlation.
LO 1.9 Discuss some of the ethical risks that social psychologists face.
LO 1.10 Discuss the links between social psychology and other disciplines of
psychology.
LO 1.11 Explain why an understanding of social psychology is valuable to
disciplines outside of psychology.

The Mysteries of Social Life


A few years after graduating from college, things were not going well for Joyce R. As
she describes it:
I had failed on an epic scale. An exceptionally short-lived marriage had imploded, and I
was jobless, a lone parent, and as poor as it is possible to be in modern Britain, without
being homeless. The fears that my parents had had for me, and that I had had for myself,
had both come to pass, and by every usual standard, I was the biggest failure I knew.

In the face of all this personal and economic failure, many people might have
stopped trying. But Joyce didn’t passively accept her fate. Besides struggling to put
bread on the table for her young daughter, she worked long hours into the night, using
her knowledge of classic literature, to write a children’s novel.
Writing a novel is not a very practical formula for economic success. There are
approximately 493,000 books published in English every year, and many more that are
written but never find a publisher. In fact, Joyce’s novel seemed to be just another one
of her life’s failures: It was rejected by 12 publishers.
But an editor at the thirteenth publishing house accepted the book and offered
her a £1,500 advance as well as some practical advice: He gently informed her that
she was not likely to make any money writing children’s books and suggested that
she ­instead get a day job (Blais, 2005). But Joyce’s book defied the unfavorable odds
and did quite well in the bookstores. Joyce, rather than taking a day job, wrote a series
of follow-up books, which also sold handsomely. Indeed, in a few short years the for-
merly ­poverty-stricken single mom was listed on Fortune magazine’s list of billionaires.
You may know Joyce as J.K. Rowling, author of the Harry Potter series.
It might not have been surprising if Ms. Rowling, having experienced poverty,
had hoarded her hard-earned cash. Many people who start making a lot of money are
suddenly shocked at how many dollars they pay in taxes and begin to seek tax shelters,
or to consider migrating to a place with lower taxes. But not J.K. Rowling. Not only
did she proudly pay her taxes, she began giving large portions of the rest of her money
4  Chapter 1  •   Introduction to Social Psychology

away. In just one single contribution, she once wrote a check for $15,000,000. And there
were many, many more such checks. Indeed, she was giving away so much that she
was removed from Fortune magazine’s list of billionaires.
After her great financial success, Ms. Rowling was invited to give a speech to the
graduates of Harvard University, amongst whom were many future millionaires and
world leaders. She implored them to use their intelligence, capacity for hard work, and
education to work not just for themselves, but to improve the plight of the thousands
and millions of powerless people suffering throughout the world.
J.K. Rowling’s story raises a number of interesting mysteries. One view of human
­nature foundational to many of the social sciences is that our minds are designed to be
selfish—to make decisions that serve our own self-interest. If so, why are some people,
like J.K. Rowling, so generous with their money and so concerned about the welfare
of others?
In this book we will explore not only broad questions about human nature, but
also everyday mysteries about love and hatred, generosity and aggression, and hero-
ism and betrayal. Why do we react generously and lovingly toward some of the peo-
ple we meet (and in some situations), but defensively or aggressively toward others?
What are the roots of romance versus parental love? What causes some marriages,
like J.K. Rowling’s, to implode after a few months, and others to flourish for a life-
time? How can we get our coworkers to cooperate with us? Why do some people
make better leaders? How are our reactions to other people affected by our cultural
background, by our early experiences, by our sex, and by neurochemical events in our
brains?
Most of us try to solve mysteries like these in our minds, by devouring news
­stories and books and chatting with friends about our feelings and opinions. Social
psychologists go a step further in their detective work; they apply the systematic meth-
ods of scientific inquiry.

What Is Social Psychology?


LO 1.1 Define social psychology and explain why it relies on scientific description
and theory.
LO 1.2 Explain why social psychology is considered a bridge discipline.

Social psychology is the scientific study of how people’s thoughts, feelings, and be-
haviors are influenced by other people. What does it mean, though, to say that social
psychology is “scientific”?

Describing and Explaining Social Behavior


We can divide the tasks of a scientific social psychology into two general categories:
­description and explanation. As a first step toward a scientific account of any phenom-
enon (bird migrations, earthquakes, or intertribal warfare), we need an objective and
reliable description. Part of what scientists do is to develop reliable and valid methods
to help them avoid careless or biased descriptions.
Careful description is a first step, but it is not, in itself, enough to satisfy scientific
curiosity. Social psychologists also seek to explain why people influence one another
in the ways they do. A good scientific explanation can connect many thousands of
Social psychology The scientific study
unconnected observations into an interconnected, coherent, and meaningful pattern.
of how people’s thoughts, feelings, and The philosopher Jules Henri Poincaré compared scientific facts to the stones used to
behaviors are influenced by other people. build a house, but he also observed that without a theory those facts are merely a pile
of stones, rather than a well-formed house. Scientific explanations that connect and
Theory Scientific explanation that
connects and organizes existing
organize existing observations are called theories.
observations and suggests fruitful In addition to organizing what we already know, scientific theories give us hints
paths for future research. about where to look next. What causes some people, like J.K. Rowling, to be especially
Chapter 1  •   What Is Social Psychology?   5

likely to extend help, and others to be more selfish? Without a good theory, we would
not know where to start searching for an answer. Maybe an inclination to help ­others
is caused by the arrangement of the planets under which altruists are born or by
something in the water they drank as children. Social psychological theories are more
likely to suggest searching elsewhere for the causes of social behavior—in a per-
son’s interpretation of his or her immediate social environment, in his or her family
background, in the broader culture, or in general predispositions humans share with
baboons and other social animals. And, as we’ll see, social psychologists have devel-
oped some intriguing research methods designed to sort out those different sources
of influence.
Finally, scientific theories can help us make predictions about future events and
control previously unmanageable phenomena. Scientific theories have led to the elec-
tric light bulb, the personal computer, the space shuttle, and the control of diseases
such as smallpox. As we will see, social psychological theories have provided useful
information about the roots of prejudice, kindness, and love; about why people join
rioting mobs or religious cults; and about a host of other puzzling phenomena.

Social Psychology Is an Interdisciplinary Bridge


Psychologists aren’t the only ones pondering the mysteries of human social behavior.
Anthropologists puzzle over why people in some societies have social customs that
would seem radically inappropriate in others (in Chapter 8, we will talk about societ-
ies in which one woman marries multiple men, for example). Evolutionary biologists
search for common patterns linking human social behavior with the behaviors of chim-
panzees, hyenas, and indigo buntings (in Chapter 10, we will see that the hormone
testosterone is similarly linked to aggression, and to sex roles, across a wide range of
species). Political scientists and historians search for the determinants of warfare and
intergroup conflicts, of the sort we will explore in Chapters 11 and 13. And economists
search for the roots of people’s decisions about whether to contribute to their group’s
welfare, or hoard their resources to themselves, topics we will investigate in Chapters
9 and 13.
How do the perspectives of all these disciplines fit together into a bigger picture?
How does what you are learning in your biology class link up with what you’re learn-
ing in your anthropology class? How do the factoids of history connect with recent
discoveries in neuroscience? What are the links between geography, economics, and
marriage patterns? It turns out all these things are profoundly connected, and in ways
that affect not only the course of your personal life but also the course of world affairs
and major social problems. Evolutionary biology, neurochemistry, history, culture, and
geography, all have important implications for how people socially interact with one
another; those social interactions, in turn, affect which moral and religious sentiments
are enforced as laws, how children are educated, and even how medical doctors treat
their patients.
Because all of these influences converge to influence social behavior, social psy-
chologists consider social behavior at many different levels of analysis. For example, a
recent series of studies of societies around the world found that cultural differences in
friendliness and sociability are linked to geographic variations in disease prevalence—
where there is more disease, people have traits that lead them to avoid contact with
others (Murray et al., 2011; Schaller & Park, 2011). Other studies we’ll discuss have
examined how our relationships with other people can be affected by historical factors,
hormone levels, phase of the menstrual cycle, and brain activity, and how all these
influences can, in turn, affect our physical and mental health, as well as our economic
behavior and political beliefs (e.g., Apicella et al., 2008; Cantú et al., 2014; Gelfand et al.,
2011; Little et al., 2008; Uskul, Kitayama, & Nisbett, 2008; Varnum et al., 2014). Thus,
social psychology is in many ways the ultimate bridge discipline. Throughout this text,
we will encounter many such interdisciplinary bridges, often considering findings that
reflect culture, evolutionary biology, neuroscience, and that connect with applied disci-
plines from business to law to medicine.
6  Chapter 1  •   Introduction to Social Psychology

Quick Quiz
1 Social psychology is the scientific study of:
a. How people’s reactions to others develop over the life cycle.
b. How people’s thoughts, feelings, and behaviors are influenced by other people.
c. How societal forces contribute to the development of mental illness.
d. How the brain influences the development of social reactions.
2 Which of the following best describes scientific theories?
a. Theories are based on hypothetical conjecture as opposed to established evidence.
b. Theories explain the cause of specific behaviors.
c. Theories are a collection of facts.
d. Theories are scientific explanations that connect and organize existing observations.
3 To say that social psychology is the ultimate bridge discipline means that the field:
a. Connects laboratory findings with clinical applications.
b. Bridges careful description with theoretical explanation.
c. Links sociology and psychology.
d. Connects multiple perspectives on social behavior, from biology, anthropology, economics,
and other disciplines.

Major Theoretical Perspectives


of Social Psychology
LO 1.3 Summarize the four major theoretical perspectives of social psychology.
LO 1.4 Discuss how the four major perspectives work together to explain human
social behavior.

Social psychological theories have been influenced by intellectual developments rang-


ing from the discovery of DNA to the emergence of artificial intelligence. Four major
perspectives (or families of theories) have dominated the field: sociocultural, evolu-
tionary, social learning, and social cognitive.

The Sociocultural Perspective


The year 1908 saw the publication of the first two major textbooks titled Social
Psychology. One of these was written by sociologist Edward Alsworth Ross. Ross ar-
gued that the wellsprings of social behavior reside not in the individual but in the so-
cial group. He argued that people were carried along on “social currents,” such as “the
spread of a lynching spirit through a crowd . . . [or] an epidemic of religious emotion”
(Ross, 1908, 1–2). Ross analyzed incidents such as the Dutch tulip bulb craze of 1634, in
which people sold their houses and lands to buy flower roots that cost more than their
weight in gold, but that instantly became worthless when the craze stopped. To explain
these crazes, Ross looked at the group as a whole rather than at the psyche of the indi-
vidual group member. He viewed crazes and fads as products of “mob mind . . . that
irrational unanimity of interest, feeling, opinion, or deed in a body of communicating
individuals, which results from suggestion and imitation” (Ross, 1908, 65).
Like Ross, other sociologically based theorists emphasized larger social group-
ings, from neighborhood gangs to ethnic groups and political parties (e.g., Sumner,
1906). That emphasis continues in the modern sociocultural perspective—the view
that a person’s prejudices, preferences, and political persuasions are affected by fac-
Sociocultural perspective The tors that work at the level of the group, factors such as nationality, social class, and
theoretical viewpoint that searches current historical trends (Gelfand et al., 2014; Heine, 2010). For example, compared
for the causes of social behavior in to her working-class Irish grandmother, a modern-day Manhattan executive probably
influences from larger social groups. has different attitudes about premarital sex and women’s roles in business (Roberts &
Social norm A rule or expectation for Helson, 1997). Sociocultural theorists focus on the central importance of social norms,
appropriate social behavior. or rules about appropriate behavior, such as Don’t eat with your hands, Don’t wear shorts
Another random document with
no related content on Scribd:
ship, engined with suitable machinery. Built in London, and installed
with engines by Robert Napier (by the courtesy of whose kinsman,
Mr. James Napier, the illustration is here given), the British Queen
was considered a wonder in her day, and even exceeded the
dimensions of the famous Great Western, costing as much as
£60,000 to build. As will be seen, she is neither brig- nor ship-rigged,
but is a barque. In spite of the hideous old stern of those times and
the old-fashioned square ports, and the medieval custom of stowing
one of her anchors abreast of the fore-mast—a practice which
survived until well into the nineteenth century—her appearance
shows that she was an advance on what had gone before. She had
about seven beams to her length, and her bow gives evidence that
the old Dutch influence was at last being forsaken: it is, in fact, the
transition stage before the clippers modified it still more. The same
long space which we noted in an earlier ship, extending between the
fore- and main-mast to afford room for the engines, will here be
recognised, and the paddle-wheels, unlike those of the early river
craft, are placed about amidships. In designing her with about 40
feet greater length than the Great Western had possessed, the aim
was no doubt to attain not merely sufficient space for passengers,
cargo, engines and ample fuel, but also to be able to wrestle with the
long Atlantic waves, whose average length has been computed at
about 200 feet. Seventy years ago this British Queen was designed
to be 275 feet over all; to-day, the Lusitania is 760 feet thus
measured, and it is this appreciation of the value of length which has
a good deal to do with the evolution of the modern liner from being a
moderate-sized vessel to one of enormous proportions. In her first
voyage from Portsmouth to New York, the British Queen kept up an
average speed for one day of over ten knots, whereas the Great
Western had on her maiden voyage outward-bound averaged about
two knots less. Leaving Portsmouth on April 2nd, 1839, the British
Queen arrived in New York on April 16th, or three days quicker than
the first Royal William had done the journey in the opposite direction
under sail and steam. The British Queen consumed about 613 tons
of coal on the way.
Thus we have seen the steamship arrive at a stage very far from
being merely experimental. We have watched her gradually grow
from her infancy, when she was good only as a tug or river craft, until
now she has shown in the enthusiasm of her youth that she can
stride across the Atlantic. It will be our duty in the following chapter
to indicate how she came to be treated with entire confidence, and to
take her part in the regular routine of the world’s work.
CHAPTER IV
THE INAUGURATION OF THE LINER

It was not to be thought that the achievements which we


chronicled at the end of the preceding chapter would remain without
their immediate results. If such small vessels as the Sirius, propelled
by steam, could cross the Atlantic and return safe and sound; if still
more easily the Great Western had been able to perform the feat
and to show a substantial return on the capital laid out, surely there
was an assured future for steamship enterprise. “What man has
done, man can do,” is an old proverb, the application of which has
led to the founding of those mighty, excellently equipped fleets which
have transformed the trackless, desolate North Atlantic into a busy
thoroughfare, along whose fixed routes every day of the year are
carried thousands of passengers and tons of merchandise from one
continent to the other. Although nowadays there is scarcely a corner
of the world to which a regular line of steamships does not run, yet it
is the North Atlantic that has always been the scene of the greatest
enterprise in steamship development. We could find plenty of
reasons for this if we cared to inquire into the matter. It was not until
the advent of the transatlantic steamship that all the possibilities of
the Tudor voyages and discoveries began to be appreciated fully. A
continent, like a single country, flourishes not merely by its produce
of wealth, but by its exchange thereof. So long as it is separated by
thousands of miles, every fathom of which is fraught with danger and
has to be traversed by sailing ships whose arrival may be weeks or
months late, which may, in fact, never arrive at all, a tight restriction
is kept on the exchange of wealth; stagnation ensues, people travel
as little as possible, and remain ignorant in their own narrow
provincialism. Whereas, to-day, they take every possible advantage
of travel, of voyaging the world over, not merely to exchange wealth
but to exchange ideas, to add to their knowledge, to wipe out their
provincialism.
For this we must thank the coming of the liner.
It was that memorable year of 1838 that set all this going.
Impressed by the obvious advantages which the steamship now
showed for speed and reliability, the Lords Commissioners of the
Admiralty, to whose care was then entrusted the arrangement of
postal contracts, saw that those ancient “coffin brigs” were doomed.
Their lordships forthwith issued circulars inviting tenders for the
carrying of the American mails by steamers. It happened that one of
these circulars fell into the hands of Samuel Cunard, a prominent
merchant of Halifax, Nova Scotia. He had been anything but
disconnected with shipping, for he was the owner of a number of
sailing ships trading between Boston, Newfoundland and Bermuda,
and was agent at Halifax for the East India Company, who in their
time owned some of the very finest sailing fleets that ever put to sea.
And this Samuel Cunard had been one of the shareholders of that
first Royal William which crossed in 1833 from Pictou, Nova Scotia,
to the Isle of Wight. A man of energy and enterprise, he had already
realised that a line of steamers connecting the two continents ought
to become something real, and he had sufficient foresight to see that
this was an opportunity which does not occur many times in a
generation.
Having made up his mind, after reading this circular, the next
thing was to find the money. In Halifax it was not possible to raise the
required capital, so he crossed forthwith to London. But London is
not always ahead of the provinces, and the wealthy merchants
declined to show their financial interest in the scheme. Therefore,
armed with a letter of introduction from the secretary of the East
India Company, Mr. Cunard travelled north to Glasgow, to Mr. Robert
Napier, whose name we have already mentioned as a great Clyde
shipbuilder and engineer. Napier promised to give him all the
assistance possible, and introduced him to Mr. George Burns, and
the latter, in turn, to Mr. David MacIver. Both had an expert
knowledge of the shipping business, and to a Scotch shrewdness
united wide experience and ability to look ahead. As a result, within a
few days the necessary capital of £270,000 had been subscribed,
and an offer was made to the Admiralty for the conveyance of Her
Majesty’s mails once a fortnight between Liverpool and Halifax and
Boston. But the owners of the Great Western, with a ship all ready
for the work, were not going to let so fine a chance slip by without an
effort. They, too, competed for the privilege, though eventually the
organisation with which Cunard was connected was considered to
have made the more favourable tender. This was accepted by the
Government, and a contract for seven years was signed. The three
enterprisers went to their posts—Cunard to London, Burns to
Glasgow, and MacIver to Liverpool, but before matters had taken a
final shape the Government required that the service was to be
carried on by four ships instead of three, that fixed dates of sailings
should be adhered to, and in consideration of all this a subsidy was
eventually granted to the steamship owners of the sum of £81,000
per year. The corporation which we now know as the Cunard
Company was then called the British and North American Royal Mail
Steam Packet Company, and they proceeded to get in hand the
building of those first four steamers of which the Mauretania and
Lusitania to-day are the lineal descendants. These four, then, were
respectively the Britannia, the Acadia, the Caledonia, and the
Columbia. They were all built of wood, all propelled by paddle-
wheels, specially adapted for the transport of troops and stores in
the event of war, with an indicated horse-power of 740,
accommodation for 115 cabin passengers, a cargo capacity of 225
tons, while their dimensions and tonnage differed but slightly the one
ship from the other. Their speed averaged 8½ knots per hour on a
coal consumption of thirty-eight tons a day, the engines in each case
being not unnaturally made by that Robert Napier who had by his
introduction done so much to bring the formation of this company to
a practical conclusion. These vessels were built on the Clyde by four
different builders in the year 1840, but the Britannia was the first that
was ready for service, her measurements being 207 feet long, 34
feet 4 inches wide, and 22 feet 6 inches deep, with a tonnage of
1,154.
Before we go on to outline the marvellous growth which has
been seen under the Cunard Company’s flag, whose history is
practically a history of the Atlantic liner, varied here and there by the
happenings which other rival companies have brought about, it is
both curious and amusing to append the following letter, which has
only quite recently been made public, and which will surprise many
of those who here read it. It is evidence of the remarkable speed at
which events may happen, and men’s minds adapt themselves to
newer conditions. Although Samuel Cunard was part owner of the
first Royal William in 1833, and already three years earlier had
thought over the idea of starting a line of Atlantic steamers, yet it will
be seen that towards the end of 1829 he was not favourably inclined
to the project. Having in mind all that the Cunard Company has done
towards the inauguration of the liner, her continuous improvements,
her safety and her efficiency, it is instructive to read the reply which
was sent at this time to Messrs. Ross and Primrose, of Pictou, Nova
Scotia, who had written to Cunard and Company in regard to
steamship establishment:—

“Dear Sirs,—We have received your letter of the 22nd


inst. We are entirely unacquainted with the cost of a
steamboat, and would not like to embark in a business of
which we are quite ignorant. Must, therefore, decline taking
any part in the one you propose getting up.—We remain,
yours, etc.
S. Cunard and Company.
“Halifax, October 28th, 1829.”

The above letter is now in the possession of Mr. John M. Ross,


of Pictou.
But to return to the first sailing of the new company: the Britannia
started the mail service in no conventional manner. Not merely was
she to throw time-honoured custom to the winds by carrying the
mails by the help of steam, but she dealt another blow to sailor-
conservatism by setting forth on her maiden voyage on a Friday,
which also happened to be the fourth of July, a day commemorative
of another kind of Independence. Of course, the old-fashioned
prophesied that so flagrant a disregard for superstition would spell
disaster; but somehow the Britannia managed to arrive quite safely
at Boston, on July 18th, 1840, after a voyage of just eight hours
beyond a fortnight, though she had touched at Halifax after eleven
days, four hours. The citizens of Boston celebrated the event with
banqueting and wild enthusiasm as the forging—shall we not say?—
of the first of those stronger links which were to bind the two
countries more closely and more securely together. Four years later,
one bitter February, when this same Britannia was hemmed in,
icebound in Boston harbour, the same enthusiasts liberated her by
cutting a canal seven miles long and a hundred feet wide through the
ice, and this entirely at their own expense.
Facing page 102 will be seen an illustration of a model of this
Britannia. Old paintings show her rigged as a barque, with a couple
of ship’s boats in davits on either side, and another hung over the
stern in a manner that will be familiar to those readers who have
seen the American sailing schooners, and some of the Norwegian
craft. The space for boilers and engines still causes that long gap
between the fore- and main-mast that we mentioned earlier. The
square stern, the old-fashioned bows, and her lines generally, show
that this first Atlantic liner was hardly a thing of beauty, if even she is
to be remembered for ever as the first of a new series. Her paddle-
wheels were 28 feet in diameter, and had 21 floats, which measured
8 feet by 2.8 feet. The mean draught of this little ship was 16.8 feet.
Her engines were of the side-lever type, of course, the making of
which Napier understood so well. Steam was generated in four
boilers with twelve furnaces, and there were two cylinders. As we
have already dealt with the working of these engines we need do
little more than ask the reader to turn to the next page, where he will
find a sectional model of an engine very similar to that which was
installed in these first four Cunard liners. The non-technical reader
will find this some considerable help in following our previous
references to engines of this type, and the section of the cylinder at
the extreme left-hand of the picture will be found illustrative of the
working of the piston inside the cylinder. As we are writing the story
of the steamship, and not a history of engineering, we need not
digress from our historical continuity, and we can now pass on to two
other steamers built in 1841, for the Royal Mail Company. In the
illustration facing this page will be seen the Teviot and Clyde
respectively, the former being of 1,793 tons, the latter of 1,371 tons.
We have already spoken of the founding of the General Steam
Navigation Company, and shall speak presently of the Peninsular
and Oriental Company. Following the precedent set by the Cunard
Company, the Royal Mail Line, on March 20th, 1840, entered into an
agreement with the British Government by which the Royal Mail
Steam Packet Company were “to provide, maintain, and keep
seaworthy, and in complete repair and readiness, for the purpose of
conveying all Her Majesty’s mails, a sufficient number (not less than
fourteen) of good, substantial, and efficient steam vessels, of such
construction and strength as to be fit and able to carry guns of the
largest calibre now used on board of Her Majesty’s steam vessels of
war, each of such vessels to be always supplied with first-rate
appropriate steam engines of not less than 400 collective horse-
power, and also a sufficient number—not less than four—of good,
substantial, and efficient sailing vessels, of at least 100 tons burthen
each.” Previous to this agreement, the Government had conveyed
the mails to the West India Islands in gun-brigs, and in those days
we must not forget that the seas were not the free highways that
they are now.
THE “TEVIOT” AND “CLYDE” (1841).
From a Painting in the Victoria and Albert Museum.

SIDE-LEVER ENGINE.
From the Model in the Victoria and Albert Museum.
The contract was for ten years, and to take effect from
December 1st, 1841. The fourteen ships were all named after British
rivers, and many readers will be aware that this custom of the
company has continued ever since, although in some cases the
names of foreign rivers have also been thus employed. Some of
these vessels were built at Northfleet on the Thames, others
(including the Teviot and Clyde) were built at Greenock, others at
Dumbarton, Leith, and Cowes. The Lords of the Admiralty stipulated
that the vessels should be built under their supervision, and a naval
officer was put in charge of the mails on each steamer, and carried
out a sort of supervision of the ship’s affairs, a boat’s crew being
always at his service when the mails were being taken aboard or
disembarked. The illustration facing page 112 shows the launch of
the Forth at Leith in 1841. This picture, which is taken from a
contemporary painting, is worthy of perusal, as showing the close
resemblance between the mercantile marine and naval architecture
of the period. Strength rather than slim beauty, massiveness rather
than fineness, formed the keynote both in the steam and sailing
ships of that time. In the same year had already been launched the
Thames from Northfleet, and in the following year that vessel
inaugurated this new service, setting forth, like the older packets,
from Falmouth. The voyage from there to the West Indies took about
eighteen days, but exceptional runs were done in seventeen days.
This new steamship departure was an undoubted success, and
the Admiralty admitted that even the Government, with all its naval
resources, could not have succeeded so well as this private
company in getting together and ready for sea in so short a time so
many large and well-equipped new steamers. Financially this meant
a very large outlay, and there was not much less than a million of
money expended on this first fleet. It should be stated, however, that
the Government subsidised the concern by a grant of £240,000 per
annum. Presently Falmouth gave way to Southampton as the
headquarters of the Royal Mail fleet. To-day there are so many big
liners calling at the Hampshire port, and there is at all times of the
day so continuous a procession of all kinds of large steamships, that
it is difficult to realise that in those days this was comparatively a
small port.
It was only natural that, as soon as ever the West Indian service
should have proved itself successful, a branch should be extended
to the South American Continent. In 1846, therefore, the company
organised a means of transit by mules and canoes across the
Isthmus of Panama, which were in 1855 superseded by the Panama
Railroad. Although we are departing from our historical sequence in
the development of the steamship, it is convenient here to sketch
very rapidly the progress of the Royal Mail Line farther still, for the
evolution of a steamship company is not necessarily that of the
steamship. A small company may be famous for having one or two
ships that are always the last word in modern ship-building and
marine engineering; a large company may possess a considerable
aggregate of tonnage, but its ships may be behind the lead of others
in improvements. For the moment we are considering the enterprise
which enabled the early steamships to penetrate to distant, over-sea
territories where the Elizabethan sailors had gone in their slow-going
ships scarcely three centuries before.

LAUNCH OF THE “FORTH” (1841).


By Permission of the Royal Mail Steam Packet Co.
THE “WILLIAM FAWCETT” AND H.M.S. “QUEEN” (1829).
From the Painting by Frank Murray in the possession of the Peninsular & Oriental
Steam Navigation Co.

In 1851 the Royal Mail Line service to South America began,


and about 1869 those steamers which had stopped short at Brazil,
and served the Argentine by transfer, continued their voyage to
Buenos Ayres. In the course of time it was only to be expected that
the heavy subsidy should be reduced. It dwindled down to £85,000 a
year, and was finally allowed to vanish altogether as recently as
June, 1905. Since then the Royal Mail Company has extended its
West Indian service to New York via Jamaica. During the Crimean
War some of the vessels of this line did good service as transports,
and even more recently still during the South African War. It was on
one of the vessels of this line that, during the American Civil War, an
incident occurred which was of international importance. The ship
which was brought so prominently into notice was the Trent, that had
been launched at Northfleet. Some readers will doubtless remember
that Messrs. Slidell and Mason were forcibly taken from this vessel
by a Federal man-of-war, and that Lord Palmerston, by his action in
the matter, set forth that valuable doctrine, still recognised, that an
individual on board a British ship is as safe from foreign interference
as if he were on British soil.
It was in 1840, also, that the Pacific Steam Navigation Company
was granted its charter, and its history is, so to speak, a complement
B
of that of the Royal Mail Company. After the latter had extended its
service to the Isthmus of Panama, and established a means of
transit across to the western coast, it was evident that the Pacific
littoral was ready for the steamship, and this the Pacific Steam
Navigation Company now supplied. In the olden days the sailing ship
had been the only means of doing this, but that meant braving the
terrors of Cape Horn, as many of the surviving sailing ships do to this
day. But the enterprise of the Royal Mail Line on the one side of the
narrow neck separating North from South America, and the co-
operation of the Pacific Steam Navigation Company on the other,
together with the intervening land-journey, brought the inhabitants of
the Southern Pacific much nearer to Europe. The Panama Canal,
which is promised for opening in 1915, was thus foreshadowed.
Sending round its two steamers, the Chile and Peru, to the west
coast, the Pacific Company opened up a new sphere of commerce,
and these two steamships were the very first steam-propelled craft
that ever passed through the Straits of Magellan.

B
The Royal Mail Co. has now absorbed the
Pacific Steam Navigation Co.

The foundation of the Peninsular Company dates back as far as


1837. Even a year or two before then its ships had commenced
running to the Peninsula, but at the time mentioned a regular service
of mail packets from London to Lisbon and Gibraltar was instituted.
Here again we find the existence of a contract between the Admiralty
and a steamship company for the carrying of the mails, but it was not
until 1840 that the line was extended to Malta and Alexandria, and
was incorporated by Royal Charter under the now well-known title of
the Peninsular and Oriental Steam Navigation Company, with a view
to carrying on operations in the Far East. The lower illustration facing
page 112 shows the first steamship owned by the Peninsular
Company, a little paddle vessel of only 209 tons. This was the
William Fawcett, which was built in the year 1829. She measured 74
feet long, only 16 feet wide, developed 60 horse-power, and was
engaged in the trade between England, Lisbon, and Gibraltar. But
the first steamer which the newly incorporated company dispatched
to India, via the Cape of Good Hope, was the Hindostan, a vessel of
1,800 tons, and 500 horse-power. She began her voyage from
England in September, 1842, and her departure was a memorable
event when we consider all that was destined to follow therefrom,
and how certainly it meant the ending of the careers of those fine
East India sailing ships which had been brought to such a high state
of perfection ere steam had appeared on the sea. The Hindostan
was a three-masted vessel with a long bowsprit, “steeved” at a big
angle, setting yards on her fore-mast for fore-sail, topsail and
t’gallant, while her main and mizen were fore-and-aft rigged. She is
interesting as having not one but two funnels, the first being placed
very far forward, just abaft the fore-mast, whilst the other was
immediately in front of the main-mast. The distance between the two
funnels was great, for the purpose already indicated. The Hindostan
was followed by other steamers, and in 1844 the P. and O. Company
undertook a mail service between England and Alexandria, and so
from Suez to Ceylon, Calcutta, and China.
Of course, as yet, there was no Suez Canal, so that, in a manner
similar to that across the Isthmus of Panama, an overland route had
to be instituted for passengers, cargo, and mails across the Isthmus
of Suez. The P. and O. Company had, then, to land their passengers
at Alexandria, and just as canoes and mules had to be employed in
America, so boats and camels were requisitioned in Africa. But it
was a complicated journey, for this “overland” route was mostly an
over-water route. By means of the Mahmoudieh Canal the
passengers and goods were sent from Alexandria to the Nile,
whence they proceeded by steamer to Cairo. From there they
travelled through the desert to Suez. Three thousand camels had to
be employed for transporting a single steamer’s loading; every
package had to be subjected to three separate transfers, and the
inconvenience was indeed considerable. But for nearly twenty years
this system continued.
Steam communication was inaugurated by the company with
Australia in 1852, by means of a branch line from Singapore, and
two years later the service between Suez and Bombay was
absorbed by the P. and O. Company. This had been retained hitherto
by the East India Company in order to keep alive their navy. In 1869,
came the opening of the Suez Canal, and it was essentially the
steamship and not the sailing ship which brought this about,
although the Suez Railway preceded the canal by ten years. It is not
generally known, perhaps, that a continuous waterway had already
existed long years before. In the times of the early Egyptians there
had been a canal which connected the Nile with the Red Sea, so that
ships could circumnavigate Africa and, returning by the
Mediterranean, could come out through the Nile into the Red Sea
again. But the Suez Canal had not been demanded so long as the
steamship remained undeveloped, and even for some time after the
traffic to Australia and New Zealand was principally carried on in
those handsome clipper-ships which were representative of the
finest examples of the sailing ship. It is only by means of the
steamship that it is possible to bring across so many thousands of
miles the great quantities of frozen meat and other perishable foods
which now reach this country, and the Suez Canal certainly assisted
to make this possible. Not merely did the steamship indirectly bring
about the Canal, but the latter increased the steamship’s sphere of
usefulness.
About the time when the Suez Canal was opened the practical
adoption of the compound engine was taking place in the mercantile
marine. This idea had been introduced about 1856 by Messrs.
Randolph Elder and Company, and had been installed in the ships of
the Pacific Steam Navigation Company. In explanation of this system
we may say at once that its great advantage lay in the fact that it
reduced the coal consumption to just about half of what it had been
hitherto in the most economical engines. The principle is based on
the fact that steam possesses elastic properties which can be put to
excellent use. Put simply, the compound engine allows the steam to
enter one cylinder at high pressure, and, after it has moved the
piston, escapes into one (or more) cylinders of larger size, where it
does its work by direct expansion, and so much more work is done
at little expense. The expression “triple expansion,” which frequently
confronts the reader interesting himself in steamships, simply means
that the steam is expanded one stage further. Quadruple expansion
is the same idea pushed still another stage. When about twenty
years ago the triple expansion system was brought in, the steam
pressures were increased from 125 lb. to 160 lb. per square inch,
and so the coal consumption was reduced also. But the triple
expansion had been preceded by the compound and the low
pressure engine, just as it was followed by the quadruple.
The opening of the Suez Canal was not devoid of side issues,
for it took away that monopoly which the P. and O. had enjoyed,
since the world’s steamships now poured in and began to go
eastward and back again. There was difficulty with the Post Office,
who refused to allow the Canal route for the conveyance of mails, on
the ground that it was not so suitable as the Egyptian Railway, and it
was not until 1888, when the charge for carrying the mails had been
reduced by nearly £100,000 a year, that the accelerated mails sent
via Brindisi were transferred to the Canal route, although the heavy
mails had already been carried by it. But the P. and O. were unlucky
in another way. The Mooltan, their first ship to be installed with the
compound engine, in 1860, had proved such a success that several
other steamers of the line were thus fitted, but the result was
disappointing. Although it was quite clear that this type of engine
made for economy, yet it was found unreliable, and in some cases
had to be replaced by less complex machinery.
We have now been able to see steamship lines established and
sending their fleets regularly with passengers, cargoes, and mails to
the uttermost ends of the earth, and we have been able to look
ahead a little so that we shall be free to concentrate our attention
very shortly on that centre of steamship activity the North Atlantic.
Between 1840 and 1860 the Cunard Company had practically a
monopoly of the Atlantic trade. For a time the American clippers
hung on, but as they had ousted the old brigs, even the fastest
sailing vessels were replaced by the steamship. From 1850 to 1858
there was, indeed, some opposition from a steamship company
called the Collins Line, which had been subsidised by the American
Government. This competition was very keen, for both lines were
compelled to put forth the best steamers they could, but in the end
the Collins Line withdrew from the contest.

DESIGNS FOR SCREW PROPELLERS PRIOR TO 1850.


From the Drawing in the Victoria and Albert Museum.

But there was now another force coming in, which was to entirely
alter the character of the liner. Let us trace the evolution of the screw
propeller, which has completely banished the old-fashioned paddle-
wheel from its place in the ocean-going ship, and is rapidly having
the same effect in cross-Channel steamers. We saw that away back
in 1804 John Stevens had crossed the Hudson in a little ship that
was driven along by a screw propeller, but it was not until the year
1836 that the screw was re-introduced. In this year John Ericsson, a
Swedish engineer, obtained a patent for his invention which
consisted of two drums, on whose exteriors were seven helical
blades, the interior of each drum having the three blades which
formed the radii of the circle. Both these drums worked on one axis,
and were placed behind the rudder, and not in front of it as is the
modern propeller. If the reader will turn to the plate facing page 118,
he will see this at the beginning of the second line to the left. The
drums were made to work in opposite directions, the object being to
avoid loss due to the rotary motion already remaining in the water
discharged by a single screw.
Ericsson applied this invention to the Francis B. Ogden, which
was built in 1837. She was 45 feet long, and was driven by a two-
cylinder steam engine with a boiler pressure of 50 lb. The result of
the experiment showed that she could tow a vessel of 630 tons
burthen at 4½ knots against the tide. The following year a larger
vessel, the Robert F. Stockton, was built by Laird Brothers, and
attained a speed of thirteen knots on the Thames, with the tide in her
favour. Afterwards she crossed the Atlantic, but under canvas, and
was turned into a tug as the New Jersey, for work in New York
waters. The illustration facing page 120, which has been lent by
Messrs. Cammell, Laird and Company, Limited, of Birkenhead,
shows her rigged as a topsail schooner under sail and steam. Her
measurements were 63.4 feet long, 10 feet beam, 7 feet deep, with a
register of 33 tons, and engines of 30 horsepower. Although she was
the first screw steamer to cross the Atlantic, yet her voyage is
interesting rather as a fairly daring trip of a small sailing ship than as
proving the reliability of the screw propeller.
But at the same time that Ericsson was working at his idea,
Francis Smith, an Englishman, who was afterwards knighted, was
also engaged at the same problem, though his method of solution
was of a different nature, as will be seen by a reference to the last
illustration on the first line of the plate facing page 118. His patent
was granted in the same year as Ericsson’s, and was tried with
success the year after on the Paddington Canal. Smith was a farmer
at Hendon, and had already experimented with a model driven by
clockwork on a farm pond, just as Fulton had carried out his early
experiments with a clockwork model in a tank. The next step was to
repeat the experiment on a six-ton boat which was driven by a steam
engine, the propeller being, like those of the modern aeroplanes, of
wood. It was while thus experimenting that an interesting accident
happened, for about one-half of the screw thus shown in the
illustration was broken off, and to everyone’s surprise the boat
instantly began to leap forward at a quicker speed. Later the boat
was fitted with a screw having one turn instead of two, and made of
metal instead of wood, and in this small craft Smith cruised as far as
Folkestone. Her speed was 5½ knots.

THE “ROBERT F. STOCKTON” (1838).


Photograph supplied by Messrs. Cammell, Laird & Co., Limited, Birkenhead.
THE “ARCHIMEDES” (1839).
From a Contemporary Print.

From these satisfactory results made by the six-tonner Francis


Smith, sufficient interest was aroused to form a syndicate to test the
proposition commercially, and to purchase Smith’s patents. The
result was that the Archimedes, of 240 tons, was launched from
Limehouse in November, 1838, and fitted with Smith’s screw. It must
be recollected that the same old obstinacy was still very much alive
that had hindered other inventions connected with the ship, and it
was not until the Archimedes had toured round Great Britain, and
steamed across the Bay of Biscay and back without mishap, that
people began to believe in this new method of propulsion. To-day
everyone knows how entirely dominated by the screw the steamship
now is, and that the paddle-wheel belongs almost exclusively to the
excursion passenger steamer.
Of course, Smith’s propeller was very different in expression
from the shape in use to-day, but the last word as to the ideal shape
and size of the screw has even yet to be said. It would be interesting
to detail all the attempts which have been made by different
inventors to deal with the screw, but their name is legion, and our

You might also like