Lynn1984 Nonvolition Expectancies and Hypnotic Rapport

You might also like

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 9

Journal of Abnormal Psycfaolog> Copyright 1984 by the

1984, Vol 93, No 3. 295-303 American Psychological Association, Inc

Nonvolition, Expectancies, and Hypnotic Rapport


Steven Jay Lynn, Michael R. Nash, Judith W. Rhue,
David C. Frauman, and Carol A. Sweeney
Ohio University

Prior to hypnosis, subjects were informed either that hypnotizable subjects can
resist motonc suggestions or that such control does not characterize good hypnotic
subjects. During hypnosis, susceptible and simulating subjects received counter-
suggestions involving inhibiting suggestion-related movements Susceptible subjects'
responses were found to be sensitive to prehypnotic normative information There
was a corresponding tendency for reports of involuntanness to be sensitive to the
expectancy manipulation Furthermore, subjects were able to feel deeply hypnotized
and to rate themselves as good subjects yet concomitantly experience themselves
as in control over their actions when normative information supported this attri-
bution Reports of suggestion-related sensations but not imaginative involvement
were associated with movements m response to countersuggestion. Simulators were
unable to fake susceptibles' reports of sensations and involuntanness However,
for all subjects, movements paralleled expectancies about appropriate response,
supporting the hypothesis that involuntary experiences are sensitive to the broad
expectational context and are mediated by active cognitive processes Also, rapport
with the hypnotist was found to be a factor Susceptible subjects with highly positive
rapport resolved hypnotic conflict, in part, by achieving a compromise between
meeting normative expectations and complying with the hypnotist's counterdemand

The experience of nonvolition is one of the One account (e.g., Hilgard, 1977, 1979;
hallmarks of hypnosis. Because susceptible Kihlstrom, Evans, E. Orne, & M. Orne, 1980)
subjects' reports of involuntary experiences contends that reports of nonvolition during
are often striking and seem to reflect a sub- hypnosis accurately reflected subjects' dimin-
jectively compelling feature of hypnosis, ac- ished control over behavior that is normally
counts of such experiences are close to the subject to conscious control. Hilgard (1977)
heart of major theories of hypnosis. At least captured the essence of this position by noting
three theories currently vie for empirical at- that "one of the most striking features of hyp-
tention and support. Each of these theories, nosis is the loss of control over actions nor-
either explicitly or implicitly, addresses the mally voluntary (p. 115)." He viewed these
thorny issue of whether subjects' involuntary alterations in control as dissociations that oc-
experiences are "real" or illusory. cur at the level of the executive function of
the personality that is responsible for volitional
activity. Hilgard argued that the operation of
We gratefully extend our thanks to Kevin McConkey, dissociated cognitive subsystems during hyp-
Nicholas P. Spanos, and Michael Snodgrass for their helpful nosis underlies subjects' diminished control
comments on an earlier version of this manuscript We over muscular movements relative to more
also extend our appreciation to Jothiratnam, Margret Ap- conscious, voluntary processes that mediate
pel, Michael Snodgrass, Richard Hardaway, Margaret
McKenzie, John Casey, Deborah Givens, and Jessie Lenz nonhypnotic, goal-directed experiences.
for serving as raters and to Joan Wigal for helping to Magda Arnold (1946) offered a conceptu-
prepare the manuscript alization of involuntariness that emphasizes
Portions of these results were presented at the annual
meeting of the Society for Clinical and Experimental Hyp-
imaginative processes as crucial determinants
nosis, Indianapolis, Indiana, 1982 of the experience of nonvolition. According
Michael Nash is now at North Texas State University, to the principle ofideomotor action, sustained,
Denton, Texas Carol Sweeney is now at the University of uncontradicted, and vivid imagining of a de-
North Carolina, Chapel Hill, North Carolina scribed action results in an automatic tendency
Requests for reprints should be sent to Steven Jay Lynn,
Department of Psychology, Ohio University, Athens, Ohio
to carry out the action. Conversely, the ability
45701 to resist suggestions and retain control over
295
296 LYNN, NASH, RHUE, FRAUMAN, AND SWEENEY

movements is associated with subjects' inter- and experience five motoric suggestions that
rupting or less than fully immersing themselves were to follow, but to resist engaging in move-
in imaginings. Because subjects ultimately re- ments. Their suggestion-related movements
tain control over their imaginings, this position were rated by observers and, in a postexper-
does not imply an actual inability to resist imental phase, subjective reports were col-
suggested behavior. lected. Consistent with several other studies
Spanos and his colleagues (e.g., Spanos, (Spanos et al., 1977: Spanos, Cobb, & Gor-
1981, 1982; Spanos, Radtke, & Bertrand, in assini, 1983), we found that high-susceptible
press; Spanos, Rivers, & Ross, 1977; Spanos, subjects when asked to counter suggested re-
Weekes, & deGroh, 1983) maintained that sponses, often failed to do so and afterward
subjects actually retain control over suggested stated that this occurred despite their best ef-
responses. Reports of nonvolition are viewed forts to counter or prevent them. In this con-
as attributions or interpretations of actions that text, movements may be thought of as a be-
are, in fact, goal directed and purposeful. havioral index of nonvolition. Hypnotized as
Hypnotizable subjects are viewed as active opposed to imagining (Lynn, Nash, Rhue,
"cognizers" who are invested in meeting the Frauman, & Stanley, 1983) and simulating
requirements of hypnotic role behavior and subjects (Lynn et al., 1982; Lynn, Nash, Rhue,
who are sensitively attuned to the broad de- Frauman, & Stanley, 1983) moved in response
mands of the testing context. Attributions of to countersuggestion and denned their sug-
involuntariness may be evoked by such factors gestion-related responses as involuntary. Con-
as preconceptions concerning hypnosis (Spa- trary to Arnold's ideomotor-action hypothesis,
nos, Cobb, & Gorassini, 1983) and the wording susceptible imagining subjects reported feeling
of test suggestions (Spanos & Gorassini, 1984). as absorbed and involved in imaginings as did
Relatedly, Sheehan and his colleagues (Dolby hypnotic subjects but resisted responding to
& Sheehan, 1977; McConkey, 1979; Sheehan, suggestions.
1971a, 1977, 1980; Sheehan & McConkey, Although the real-simulating differences
1982; Sheehan & Perry, 1976) emphasized obtained m our earlier research could be in-
transference and rapport factors and stressed terpreted as supporting a neodissociation ac-
susceptible subjects' motivated involvement count of involuntariness, otherfindings(Lynn
with the hypnotist and sensitivity to his per- et al., 1982) suggested that the responses of
ceived intent as critical factors in enhancing both real and simulating subjects may be ex-
the compelling nature of hypnotic suggestions. pectancy based. Simulating subjects, relative
Despite certain differences, Spanos's and to susceptible subjects, not only moved less
Sheehan's views seem to converge on hypnotic but tended to report that other good subjects
responding as involving active, constructive were less likely to move in response to coun-
processing of information during hypnosis, as tersuggestion. Several other studies (Sheehan,
opposed to passive, automatic responding to 1971b; Spanos, Bridgeman, Stam, Gwynn, &
test suggestions. Expectancies associated with Saad, 1983) indicated that real-simulator dif-
the hypnotic experience may serve as potent ferences may reflect between-group differences
determinants of subjective and behavioral re- in expectancies arising from divergent expe-
sponse. riences and experimental demands associated
The present study was undertaken to eval- with the task of simulation.
uate the general hypothesis that hypnotic sub- The present study departed from our pre-
jects' involuntary experiences are associated vious research in an important respect Sub-
with situation-based expectancies and active jects' expectancies regarding the experience of
cognitive processes. This hypothesis was in- nonvolition during hypnosis were manipulated
vestigated via a countersuggestion paradigm prior to their being hypnotized and receiving
that was also employed in our previous re- countersuggestions. To be more specific, prior
search on nonvolition (Lynn et al., 1982; Lynn, to hypnosis, subjects were informed either that
Nash, Rhue, Frauman, & Stanley, 1983). In other good hypnotic subjects could successfully
our earlier studies, hypnotic subjects (as op- resist suggestions and retain control over their
posed to nonhypnotic controls) were first hyp- movements or that other "good" subjects fail
notized and then instructed to vividly imagine to resist suggestions and experience loss of vol-
NONVOUTION, EXPECTANCIES, RAPPORT 297
untary control over their actions during hyp- moving together and the arm-rigidity items These items
nosis. Hypnotic and simulating subjects then were included as motoric suggestions in the experimental
phase of the study The Harvard mean scores, standard
received a hypnotic induction and were in- deviations, and the sex compositions of the groups are as
structed to fully experience yet to resist re- follows- Hypnotic/move, Harvard M = 9.23, SD = 0 44,
sponding to the motoric suggestions that would male/female (3/10); hypnotic/resist, Harvard M = 9.47,
follow. Subjects' reports of their experiences SD = 0.52, male/female (2/13), simulating/move, Harvard
were coded with respect to mvoluntariness, M = 1 73, SD = 0 90, male/female (3/8), simulating/
sensations, conflict about responding, and resist, Harvard M = 1 80, SD = 0 79, male/female (5/5)
Experimental subjects were chosen from 205 subjects
imaginative involvement. Rapport with the screened
hypnotist was assessed to determine whether
subjects with highly positive rapport resolve
hypnotic conflict m a different manner than Treatment of Hypnotic and
do subjects who experience less positive rap- Simulating Subjects
port. Subjects were also asked to rate how In the screening phase, subjects were informed that our
deeply hypnotized they felt, how good a subject research group was attempting to compile a profile of how
they thought they were, how many suggestions Ohio University students compare with other hypnosis
subjects across the nation Only subjects motivated to take
they moved in response to, and finally, how part in other studies were asked to volunteer No subject
many suggestions other good subjects moved who volunteered for the initial screening refused to par-
in response to. ticipate For the actual experimental phase, a project co-
ordinator read subjects simulating instructions that were
This design permitted a direct test of the identical to those used in our previous research Simulators
hypothesis that susceptible subjects are re- were informed that if their pretense were detected, the
sponsive to the broad expectational context in hypnotist would tap them on the shoulder and excuse
which the experiment is conducted and that them from the room The simulators were informed that
highly susceptible subjects would be coparticipants Sim-
involuntanness is mediated by active cognitive ulators were also asked to continue their role playing until
processes and expectancies. Support for this the experimental assistant signaled them to stop The hyp-
hypothesis would be forthcoming if (a) hyp- notic subjects had no knowledge that simulating subjects
notic subjects' reports of involuntanness and were participating in the same groups '
their response to countersuggestion varied as
a function of prehypnotic normative infor- Move/Resist Instructions
mation that specified the way good subjects
Another assistant administered the instructions, which
experience hypnosis and (b) hypnotized and constituted the experimental manipulation, to hypnotic
simulating subjects responses to counter- and simulating subjects The instructions were presented
suggestion were mediated by expectancies re- in written form, but the assistant, who was blind to the
garding appropriate responding in the exper- status of the subjects and the hypotheses, answered any
imental context. If imaginative involvement questions that subjects had The manipulation is presented
below The words in parentheses highlight the cognitive
were found to parallel subjects' responses to set to move, the underlined words emphasize the cognitive
countersuggestion, support would be garnered set to resist
for Arnold's theory of involuntariness.
As part of our efforts to match Ohio University students
with others across the nation, we are interested in seeing
Method how you compare with other highly susceptible subjects
across the country What happens when a good hypnotic
Procedure subject is told to imagine suggestions vividly but is asked
to resist going along with them"? The answer seems to
To evaluate the hypothesis under consideration, a 2 X be that the good hypnotic subject is able (unable) to
2 (Group/hypnotic vs simulating X Instructions/move vs resist the suggestion while he or she experiences it.
resist) experimental design was employed Within suscep-
Good hypnotic subjects do not feel (feel) as if their
tibility levels, subjects were randomly assigned to move/
responses to suggestions "just happen" to them and that
resist treatments (hypnotic/move n - 13, hypnotic/resist
they respond automatically They experience the way
n = 15, simulating/move n = 11, simulating/resist n = 10)
they respond to suggestions as occurring within their
(with little or no) voluntary control, while they remain
Subjects deeply hypnotized Therefore, they are able (unable) to

Subjects were assigned to groups on the basis of their


scores on a shortened, 10-pomt version of the Harvard
1
Group Scale of Hypnotic Susceptibility, Form A The complete simulating instructions are available on
(HGSHSA, Shor & O n e , 1962), which omitted the hands request to the first author
298 LYNN, NASH, RHUE, FRAUMAN, AND SWEENEY

resist going along with suggestions despite being told to sessions prior to the actual experiment Raters recorded
do so. All of this occurs because of the high susceptible movements on a scale ranging from no movement, to some
subjects* ability to experience what we call "trance" movement, to full responsiveness3 For all possible pairs of
Perhaps you have already discovered something like this raters, interrater reliability coefficients were computed and
when you were hypnotized. averaged (r = .95, range = .91-.98); the impressively high
In this part of the study you will first be hypnotized, agreement justified the use of one rater/subject in the ex-
using another similar, widely used hypnotic induction perimental situation Half way through the experiment,
After you are hypnotized, the hypnotist will ask you to interrater reliabilities were computed in the same manner
vividly imagine, but resist following the next five sug- demonstrating stability in the agreement among raters
gestions Like other good subjects, you will probably (average r = .96, range = .93- 98)
find that you are able (unable) to do this That is, it is Following the reading of the instructions, the hypnotist
very likely that you will not (will) go along with the read five motonc suggestions (head falling, hand lowering,
suggestions when (even though) the hypnotist tells you arm rigidity, moving hands together, hand levitation) that
to resist corresponded to those administered in our earlier research.
We will schedule another optional meeting which you None of the subjects had experience with the last three
may wish to attend about two weeks from now, to let suggestions The order of suggestions was varied on a ran-
you know exactly how your responses and those of your dom basis across groups of subjects
fellow students compare with a national sample of sub- Following the experimental procedure, subjects were
jects instructed to recall the prehypnotic and countersuggestion
instructions and write them down along with an essay of
at least 100 words, describing their thoughts, feelings, and
Induction and Instructions to actions (if any) during the experimental procedure Subjects
Resist Suggestions were asked to indicate whether they responded to any of
the suggestions and what they did or experienced in relation
Subjects were seated in groups of four to eight in such to each of the described actions
a manner that they could not see the responses of other
subjects The project coordinator ensured that each of the
three hypnotists had not worked with any of the subjects Dependent Variables
previously and was blind to the subjects' status Subjects Variables derived from open-ended reports Subjects'
were informed that some people might enter the room at essays and all comments were transcribed The subjective
some time during the procedure report measures derived from their descriptions were iden-
The hypnotic procedure was identical for both simulating tical to those employed in our previous studies A blind
and hypnotic subjects Subjects received a version of the rater counted the number of sensations experienced by
Stanford Profile Scales of Hypnotic Susceptibility, Form each subject using a scheme developed by Spanos et al
2 (Weitzenhoifer & Hilgard, 1967) adapted for group ad- (1977) Naive raters coded subjects' conflict, mvoluntar-
ministration Deepening suggestions that followed the in- mess, and imaginative involvement in the suggestions
duction by hand lowering instructed subjects to imagine Three-point scales were utilized with anchors describing
that they were walking down a magnificent spiral staircase increasing conflict—no indication of conflict (1), some
Subjects received only the induction; no suggestions from conflict (2), and a great deal (3), lack of volitional control—
the scale followed2 dehberateness and conscious control/no sense of things
The subjects received the following instructions during happening by themselves (1), some control/ability to direct
hypnosis after the deepening instructions were read The actions (2), and no conscious control suggested/behavior
countersuggestion instructions were identical to those em- experienced as totally involuntary (3), and involvement
ployed m our previous research in imaginings and fantasy—no indication of suggestion-
relatedfantasy/imaginal involvement (1), imaginal/fantasy
Please listen very carefully It is very important that involvement in response to 1 or 2 suggestions (2), and
you listen very carefully For each of the following five imaginal/fantasy involvement in response to three or more
suggestions, just listen as carefully and intently as you suggestions of a vivid or elaborated nature or fantasy/
can, but be sure not to act on any of the suggestions imaginative productions in response to all suggestions (3)
So, even if I suggest that you do something, you will Interrater reliabilities were good, ranging from conflict (80)
not do it Just think and imagine along with the sug- to imaginative involvement (.90)
gestions, but do not actually take any actions or engage
in anj; of the behaviors that I suggest until I give you Questionnaire variables On a postexpenmental in-
instructions to come out of your hypnotic trance. The quiry form, subjects also responded to Likert-type items
important thing for you to remember is that you can (range 1-5) that assessed how deeply hypnotized they felt—
think and imagine along with what I suggest, but do deeply hypnotized (1), somewhat hypnotized (3), or not at
not actually do anything 1 suggest to you Q.K, now all hypnotized (5); how good a subject they thought they
we will begin with thefirstsuggestion with you listening were—excellent (1), average (3), or poor (5), how many
and imagining to the greatest extent you are cap- suggestions they moved in response to—0 to 5, and how
able of

2
After the instructions were read, raters quietly entered The complete induction is available on request to the
the room and positioned themselves about 3 feet in front first author
3
of a subject. As in our previous research, the raters were The scoring criteria for each of the suggestions are
carefully trained by rating a model's movements in group available on request to the first author
NONVOLITION, EXPECTANCIES, RAPPORT 299

Table 1
Means and Standard Deviations of Dependent Variables in Move and Resist Conditions

Move Resist

Hypnotic Simulating Hypnotic Simulating


<« = 13) (n = 11) (n = 15) (« =• 10)

Variable M SD M SD M SD M SD

Observer-rated movements 1200 3 27 7 09 '101 111 3 17 6.50 2.92


How hypnotized felt 2.38 1.04 2.18 ()98 1 80 101 2 40 126
How good a subject 185 0.80 218 ()98 193 0 96 2 20 0.92
Suggestions good subjects respond to 531 0 97 3 36 .83 2.13 176 2.20 199
Nonvohtion 4 70 125 3 45 04 3 80 132 2 40 1.26
Sensations 431 206 200 26 2 27 144 190 2.60
Imagination 3 92 175 2 64 12 300 196 3 10 179
Conflict 3 38 1.56 2 55 04 2 67 135 2.30 0 67
Rapport 2 46 133 2 27 ().90 2 27 1.10 2 70 0.95
Liking 161 0 87 182 33 1 67 1 11 2 30 0.95

many suggestions other good subjects responded to—0 to nificant main effects for group, F(l, 45) =
5 Subjects also responded to questions that tapped their
feelings about or relationship with the hypnotist and ex-
8.52, p < .005, and instructions, F{1, 45) =
perimental assistant who administered the instructions 9.47, p < .004, were found. As Table 1 indi-
Subjects were asked to rate their rapport with the hyp- cates, susceptible subjects moved more than
notist—very good (1), neutral (3), or very poor (5) Five- did simulators. Subjects primed to move did,
point scales were also used to assess liking for the hyp- in fact, move more than did subjects informed
notist—I basically the hypnotist like (1), have
that good subjects were able to resist sugges-
neutral feelings for (3), or dislike (5)
After the subjects responded to these items, their papers tions. These findings were qualified by a sig-
were collected and the assistant instructed simulators to nificant Group X Instruction interaction, F(l,
abandon their role playing All of the subjects were then 45) = 4.56, p < .04). Post hoc analyses revealed
instructed in rating their own movement responses to the that the instructional set had an effect only
five suggestions on the same scale that was used by the
observer-raters All of the subjects were then debriefed
for hypnotic subjects. Susceptible subjects
moved as a function of the prehypnotic in-
structions; simulators evidenced few move-
Results ments in response to both move and resist
None of the subjects' data was excluded be- instructions. These differences in movements
cause of misinterpretation of the prehypnotic cannot be ascribed to differences in how hyp-
and countersuggestion instructions. The be- notized the subjects felt or how good subjects
havioral and subjective report data were sub- perceive themselves to be as hypnotic subjects.
jected to 2 X 2 (Group/hypnotized vs. sim- No significant main effects or interactions were
ulating X Instructions/move vs. resist) analyses found for these measures.4
of variance (ANOVAS). The means and standard Perceptions of good subjects' movements
deviations are presented in Table 1. Neuman- Subjects' ratings of how many suggestions good
Keuls post hoc comparisons were performed hypnotic subjects respond to were affected by
where appropriate. A preplanned comparison
(two-tailed) was also performed on hypnotized
subjects' reports of involuntary experiences in 4
the move versus resist instructional conditions. Observer and self-rated movements were highly cor-
related, hence, only observer-rated data are presented as
Analyses of movements Scores for ob- a separate ANOVA Subjects' observer-rated movements on
server-rated movements were obtained for each the 3-pomt movement dimension correlated extremely high
of the five suggestions on a 3-point movement (r = 93, p < .001) with self-ratings on the same scale.
Furthermore, these measures (observer/self) were highly
dimension. Total movement scores were com- correlated with the self-rated measure of number of sug-
puted for each subject by summing the scores gestions responded to (r= 73 and 78, respectively,
of the five suggestions (range = 5-15). Sig- p<.001)
300 LYNN, NASH, RHUE, FRAUMAN, AND SWEENEY

both the instructional set, / ^ l , 45) = 26.25, signment, F(l, 45) = 3.27, p < .08. Post hoc
p < .0001, and their hypnotic-simulating sta- tests revealed that hypnotized subjects with
tus, F{\, 45) = 4.51, p < .04. Subjects who the move preset reported more sensations than
received the instructional set that specified that
did subjects in the other groups. No significant
good subjects failed to resist the hypnotist in- main effects or interactions were observed for
dicated that good subjects move in response the measures of conflict or imagination.
to more suggestions than do subjects who re- Analyses of rapport data No significant
ceived the resist preset. Hypnotized subjects' main effects or interactions were observed for
expectancies differed from simulators' m that the measures of rapport with and liking for
the hypnotized subjects felt that good subjects the hypnotist. For purposes of additional anal-
moved m response to more suggestions than yses, subjects in each of the groups were di-
did the simulating subjects. As was the case vided into two categories: positive and neutral-
with the movement data, the above findings negative rapport with the hypnotist. Subjects
were qualified by a significant Instruction X considered to have positive rapport with the
Group interaction, F(l, 45) = 4.24, p < 05. hypnotist were required to score 1 or 2 on
Post hoc tests indicated that hypnotic subjects' both the rapport and liking for the hypnotist
expectancies were affected by the instructions; measures. Subjects considered to be in the
simulators' expectancies were not. Hypnotic neutral-negative rapport categories scored 3
subjects' expectancies were attuned to the or lower on the relevant measures. Nonpara-
normative information; however, regardless of metric analyses of the data indicated that more
the prior instructions, simulators indicated hypnotized subjects with positive rapport in
that good hypnotic subjects moved in response the preset to move condition fully resisted one
to few suggestions. This pattern of mean dif- or more of the hypnotist's suggestions (88%)
ferences corresponded exactly to the pattern than did subjects with less positive rapport
of means for subjects' actual movements. (20%). The Fisher's Exact Probability test was
Analyses of subjective reports For the significant at .031 No other significant dif-
measure of experienced nonvolition, signifi- ferences were confirmed for hypnotist rapport
cant main effects were observed for the in- in the other conditions.
struction, F( 1,45) = 7.44, p < .009, and group, However, combined across conditions, hyp-
/ U , 45) = 13.71, p < .001, factors. Subjects notized subjects expressed greater rapport
who received the move/involuntary set re- (Fisher's Exact Probability, p < .04) for the
ported greater involuntariness than did sub- hypnotist (17 expressed positive and 11 neu-
jects who received the resist/voluntary set. tral-negative rapport) than did simulators
Hypnotized subjects' reports reflected greater (7 expressed positive and 14 neutral-negative
involuntariness than did simulators. A pre- rapport).
planned comparison (two-tailed) revealed a
strong tendency (p < .06) for hypnotized sub- Discussion
jects to experience greater involuntariness
when the preset specified this as normative This study provided strong support for the
than when control over actions was empha- hypothesis that susceptible subjects' reports of
sized. The Instruction X Group interaction involuntary experiences are highly responsive
failed to reach significance. to the broad expectational context in which
The number of suggestion-related sensations the experiment is conducted. More specifically,
reported was affected by both the instructions, prehypnotic normative information had a
JFXI, 45) = 5.11, p < .03, and the subject's strong effect on subjects' ability to resist the
real-simulator status, F\\, 45) = 6.11, p < hypnotist and tended to affect subjects' report
.02. Instructions that portrayed involuntari- of suggestion-related involuntariness in line
ness as normative were associated with more with induced expectancies about appropriate
sensations than were those that emphasized responding. Furthermore, susceptible subjects'
the ability to resist suggestions. Hypnotized reports of how other good subjects behave in
subjects reported more sensations than did the experimental context were found to closely
simulators. There was a strong trend for the parallel their own actions, which provided ad-
instructions to interact with subject group as- ditional evidence that their experience of in-
NONVOIiTION, EXPECTANCIES, RAPPORT 301

voluntariness is associated with expectancies ability in subject responding. The use of the
regarding appropriate response. The finding word trance in the instructions might have
that subjects' suggestion-related sensations engendered demands that conflicted with at-
tended to conform to their movements suggests tributions of control over movements. The
that susceptible subjects may actively create prehypnotic instructions were ambiguous in
experiences to accord with their perceptions the sense that the demands for responding in
of the requirements of the hypnotic situation. accord with the instructions were qualified by
It is interesting that subjects' experience of statements such as "you will probably find
involuntanness is not necessarily associated that you are able (to resist)" and "it is very
with reports of being hypnotized. When hyp- likely that you will not go along with the sug-
nosis is defined as involving voluntary control gestions " Because subjects were not explicitly
over actions, subjects report that they are both informed that they should necessarily fully re-
good hypnotic subjects and deeply hypnotized sist all of the suggestions, it is not surprising
and that they retain control over their actions. that some of the subjects did not fully resist
Combined, thesefindingsprovide substan- the hypnotist. This hypothesis is supported by
tial support for the theoretical formulations Spanos, Cobb, and Gorassini (1983), who
of Spanos and others who have emphasized found that susceptible subjects almost always
the goal-directed, active nature of hypnotic resisted suggestions when doing so was clearly
cognition and responsiveness (e.g., Sheehan, defined as consistent with a self-presentation
1977, 1980; Sheehan & McConkey, 1982; as deeply hypnotized.
Spanos, 1981, 1982). Our findings are com- One of the most interesting findings gen-
patible with those secured by Sheehan and erated by the present study is that simulators'
Dolby (1975), who found that hypnotic sub- behavior differed dramatically from that of
jects may be more sensitive to expectancy than hypnotized subjects, although both groups of
to recency effects. Our results may also be subjects behaved in conformance with their
interpreted as congruent with a self-presen- expectancies rather than counterexpectation-
tational account of hypnosis (Coe & Sarbin, ally. Susceptible subjects' behavior was finely
1977;Sarbin, 1982, 1983; Sarbin & Coe, 1977) tuned to the expectancy manipation. Simu-
that considers subjects' responsiveness to ex- lators given the set to move not only moved
perimental demands from a dramaturgical less than did their hypnotized counterparts,
perspective. Our results offer no support for but they also indicated that other good subjects
Arnold's viewpoint in that imaginative in- moved less.
volvement was not associated with the expe- The discrepant behavior and reports of sim-
rience of involuntanness. Studies by Zaman- ulators may be associated, in part, with the
sky (1977) and Spanos and his associates special demands of the simulating condition.
(Spanos, Cobb, & Gorassim, 1983) also dem- Simulators' behavior largely coincided with the
onstrate that susceptible subjects can be ac- hypnotist's formal message to resist even when
tively involved in imaginings, yet counter sug- the prior instructions emphasized involun-
gestions when such behavior is deemed ap- tanness. Simulators, highly motivated to avoid
propriate in the experimental situation. detection by the hypnotist, may accord nor-
Although hypnotized and simulating sub- mative information presented by someone
jects were strongly affected by the prehypnotic other than the hypnotist little pnority, espe-
normative information, both hypnotized and cially when it appears to conflict with the hyp-
simulating subjects did not always move in the notist's explicit communication. Simulators
move condition and moved to some degree in may resolve the ambiguity inherent m the
the resist condition. Moving despite being in- complex expenmental situation by responding
structed that good subjects resist responding to the literal instructions of the hypnotist to
could be interpreted as consistent with neo- "play it safe" in the experimental context
dissociation theory. However, it is quite likely (Sheehan, 1971b, 1973). The cost of moving
that the complexity of the procedure, conflict- clearly differs across expenmental conditions
ing demands inherent in the methodology, and and may, in itself, account for the real-sim-
the lack of specificity of the prehypnotic in- ulating differences This interpretation is rein-
structions accounted for much of the vari- forced by the spontaneous comments of five
302 LYNN, NASH, RHUE, FRAUMAN, AND SWEENEY

simulators (move set condition) who remarked motivated to be highly responsive to the hyp-
that they were unsure about how to behave notist. This group of researchers also argued
but decided not to move because the hypnotist that hypnotized subjects may be more invested
repeatedly emphasized resisting and they in the hypnotic relationship and the events of
feared that movement would jeopardize their hypnosis than are simulating subjects (e.g.,
role play. Dolby & Sheehan, 1977; Sheehan, 1980). This
Furthermore, simulating instructions ac- hypothesis is supported by the finding that
tively discourage cognitive activity compatible more susceptible subjects than simulating
with suggested effects and may, instead, evoke subjects expressed highly positive rapport with
anxiety and apprehension. Hence, it is not the hypnotist. Indeed, the task of simulation,
surprising that simulators' reports of involun- which fosters perceptions of a virtual adver-
tariness, sensations, and ratings of appropriate sarial relationship with the hypnotist, may ac-
responding are not comparable to the reports tively discourage the development of positive
of nonsimulating subjects who may fully in- feelings for the hypnotist and involvement in
volve themselves in suggestion and move in the hypnotic relationship. Our results extend
accord with their action tendencies when Sheehan's prior research on countering to the
movement is denned as appropriate. Given domain of involuntariness during hypnosis.
the complexities inherent in the counterde- The present research suggests that rapport may
mand paradigm, it is essential that future interact with situation-based expectancies and
studies include careful postexpenmental in- a number of other factors mentioned here to
quiries to examine subjects' interpretations of determine the hypnotized subject's experience
the experimental demands as well as other fac- of nonvolition.
tors that may be related to nonvolition (e.g.,
imagination, wording of test suggestions, in- References
terpretations of suggestions, susceptibility
Arnold, M B (1946) On the mechanism of suggestion
level). and hypnosis Journal of Abnormal and Social Psy-
The present study is the first to demonstrate chology, 41, 107-128.
that rapport may play a role m subjects' ex- Coe, W C, & Sarbm, T R (1977) Hypnosis from the
standpoint of a contextualist Annals of the New York
perience of involuntanness. In the condition Academy of Sciences, 296, 2-13
in which susceptible subjects were preset to Dolby, R M., & Sheehan, P W (1977) Cognitive pro-
move yet the hypnotist instructed them not cessing and expectancy behavior in hypnosis. Journal
to, subjects with highly positive rapport were of Abnormal Psychology, 86, 334-345
more likely to respond to the hypnotist by Frauman, D , Lynn, S J , Hardaway, R, & Molteni, A.
(in press) Subliminal symbiotic activation, hypnotic
fully resisting at least one suggestion than were rapport, and susceptibility Journal of Abnormal Psy-
subjects with less positive rapport. Thus, sus- chology
ceptible subjects with highly positive rapport Hilgard, E R (1977) Divided consciousness Multiple
resolved hypnotic conflict by achieving a com- controls in human thought and action New York Wiley
promise between meeting normative expec- Hilgard, E R (1979) Divided consciousness m hypnosis
The implications of the hidden observer In E Fromm
tations and complying with the hypnotist's & R E Shor (Eds.), Hypnosis Developments in research
counterdemand. Susceptible subjects with less and new perspectives (2nd ed, pp 45-76). New York
positive rapport respond primarily in accor- Aldine
dance with the normative expectations. The Kihlstrom, J F, Evans, F J , Orne, E C , & Orne, M T
question of whether comparable results would (1980). Attempting to breach hypnotic amnesia. Journal
of Abnormal Psychology, 89, 603-616
be obtained with more subtle, behavioral Lynn, S J , Nash, M. R., Rhue, J W, Carlson, V, Sweeney,
measures of rapport such as hypnotic dream- C , Frauman, D., & Givens, D. (1982, August) Real
ing (Sheehan & Dolby, 1975) and self-disclo- vs simulators Experiential and behavioral differences
sure tasks (Frauman, Lynn, Hardaway, & in response to conflicting suggestions during hypnosis
Molteni, in press; McConkey & Sheehan, Paper presented at the Ninth International Congress of
Hypnosis and Psychosomatic Medicine, Glasgow, Scot-
1976) might be addressed in future research land.
endeavors. Lynn, S. J., Nash, M R., Rhue, J W, & Frauman,
D C (1983, August) The counlersuggestwn paradigm
These findings are supportive of the view, and the experience of nonvolition Paper presented at
propounded by Sheehan and his colleagues, the meeting of the American Psychological Association,
that some susceptible subjects may be specially Anaheim, CA
NONVOLITION, EXPECTANCIES, RAPPORT 303

Lynn, S. J., Nash, M. R., Rhue, J W., Frauman, D C , Shor, R E., & Orne, E C (1962) The Harvard Group
& Stanley, S (1983). Hypnosis and the experience of Scale of Hypnotic Susceptibility (Form A) Palo Alto,
nonvolition International Journal of Clinical and Ex- CA. Consulting Psychologists Press
perimental Hypnosis, 31, 293-308 Spanos, N P (1981) Hypnotic responding Automatic
McConkey, K M (1979) Conflict in hypnosis Reality dissociation or situation-relevant cognizing9 In E Khnger
versus suggestion In G D Burrows, D R Collison, & (Ed), Imagery Concepts, results and applications (Vol.
L Dennerstein (Eds), Hypnosis, 1979 (pp 102-107) 2, pp 105-131) New York Plenum Press
Amsterdam. Elsevier/North Holland Biomedical Press. Spanos, N P (1982) Hypnotic behavior. A cognitive, social
McConkey, K., & Sheehan, P W (1976) Contrasting in- psychological perspective Research Communications in
terpersonal orientations m hypnosis- Collaborative versus Psychology, Psychiatry, and Behavior, 7, 199-213
contractual modes of response Journal of Abnormal Spanos, N P , Bndgeman, M , Stam, H J , Gwynn, M ,
Psychology, 85, 390-397 &Saad,C L (1983) When seeing is not believing The
Sarbm, T R (1982) Hypnosis- The dramaturgical per- effects of contextual variables on the reports of hypnotic
spective In V L. Allen & K E Scheibe (Eds), The hallucinators Imagination, Cognition and Personality,
social context of conduct Psychological writings of 2. 195-209
Theodore Sarbin (pp 41-57) New York Praeger
Spanos, N P , Cobb, P C , & Gorassim, D R (1983)
Sarbm, T R (1983) A contextual analysis of nonvolition
Failing to resist hypnotic lest suggestions A strategy for
m hypnosis Paper presented at the meeting of the
self-presenting as deeply hypnotized Unpublished
American Psychological Association, Anaheim, CA
manuscript, Carleton University
Sarbin, T R , & Coe, W C (1977) Hypnosis, A social
psychological analysis of influence communication New Spanos, N P . & Gorassini, D R (1984). Structure of
York Holt, Rinehard & Winston hypnotic test suggestions and attributions of responding
Sheehan, P W (1971a) Countering preconceptions about involuntarily Journal of Personality and Social Psy-
hypnosis An objective index of involvement with the chology, 46. 688-696
hypnotist Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 78, 299- Spanos, N P , Radtke, H L , & Bertrand, L (in press)
322 The successful breaching of hypnotic amnesia in high
Sheehan, P W (1971b) Task structure as a limiting con- susceptible subjects Journal of Personality and Social
dition of the occurrence of the treatment effects of sim- Psychology
ulation instructions in application of the real-simulating Spanos,N P,Rivers,S M,&Ross,S (1977) Experienced
model of hypnosis International Journal of Clinical mvoluntariness and response to hypnotic suggestions
and Experimental Hypnosis, 19, 260-276 In W E. Edmonston, Jr (Ed.), Conceptual and inves-
Sheehan, P W (1973) Analysis of the heterogeneity of tigative approaches to hypnosis and hypnotic phenom-
"faking" and "simulating" performance in the hypnotic ena Annals of the New York Academy of Science, 296,
setting International Journal of Clinical and Experi- 208-221
mental Hypnosis, 21, 213-225 Spanos, N P,Weekes,J R., & deGroh, M (1983) Self-
Sheehan, P. W (1977) Incongruity in trance behavior A presenting as hypnotized by "involuntarily" countering
defining property of hypnosis7 Annals of the New York suggested requests Unpublished manuscript, Carleton
Academy of Sciences, 296, 194-207. University
Sheehan, P W (1980) Factors influencing rapport in hyp- Weitzenhoffer, A M , & Hilgard, E R (1967) Revised
nosis Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 89, 263-281 Stanford Profile Scales of Hypnotic Susceptibility (Form
Sheehan, P W , & Dolby, R M (1975) Hypnosis and the 2) Palo Alto, CA Consulting Psychologists Press
influence of most recently perceived events Journal of Zamansky, H S (1977) Suggestion and countersuggestion
Abnormal Psychology, 84, 331-345 in hypnotic behavior Journal of Abnormal Psychology,
Sheehan, P W., & McConkey, K M (1982) Hypnosis 86, 346-351
and experience The exploration of phenomena and pro-
cesses Hillsdale, NJ Erlbaum
Sheehan, P W, & Perry, C W (1976) Methodologies of
hypnosis A critical appraisal ofcontemporary paradigms Received October 28, 1983
of hypnosis Hillsdale, NJ Erlbaum Revision received March 19, 1984

You might also like