Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 10

Acta Psychiatr Scand 2009: 119: 383–392 Copyright  2008 The Authors

All rights reserved Journal Compilation  2008 Blackwell Munksgaard


DOI: 10.1111/j.1600-0447.2008.01323.x ACTA PSYCHIATRICA
SCANDINAVICA

Pathological dissociation and


neuropsychological functioning in borderline
personality disorder
Haaland VØ, Landrø NI. Pathological dissociation and V. Ø. Haaland1,2, N. I. Landrø2
neuropsychological functioning in borderline personality disorder. 1
Department of Psychiatry, Sørlandet Hospital HF,
Kristiansand, Norway and 2Center for the Study of
Objective: Transient, stress-related severe dissociative symptoms or Human Cognition, Department of Psychology, University
paranoid ideation is one of the criteria defining the borderline of Oslo, Oslo, Norway
personality disorder (BPD). Examinations of the neuropsychological
correlates of BPD reveal various findings. The purpose of this study
was to investigate the association between dissociation and
neuropsychological functioning in patients with BPD.
Method: The performance on an extensive neuropsychological
battery of patients with BPD with (n = 10) and without (n = 20)
pathological dissociation was compared with that of healthy controls
Key words: personality disorders; dissociation;
(n = 30). cognition; cognitive science; neuropsychology
Results: Patients with pathological dissociation were found to have
reduced functioning on every neuropsychological domain when Vegard Øksendal Haaland, Sørlandet sykehus HF,
Psykiatrisk avdeling, Serviceboks 416, N-4604
compared with healthy controls. Patients without pathological
Kristiansand, Norway.
dissociation were found to have reduced executive functioning, but no E-mail: vegard.oksendal.haaland@sshf.no
other differences were found.
Conclusion: Pathological dissociation is a clinical variable that
differentiates patients with BPD with regard to cognitive functioning. Accepted for publication November 7, 2008

Significant outcomes
• This study reports deficits in executive functioning, working memory and long-term verbal memory
performance in a subgroup of patients with BPD and pathological dissociation compared with that
in patients with BPD without pathological dissociation.
• Patients with BPD who also show pathological dissociation seem to represent a more severely
disturbed subgroup of patients with BPD.
• Pathological dissociation is a clinically important and relatively easily assessable psychopathological
variable.

Limitations
• The sample is relatively small, a factor that in particular influences the size of the subgroup of
patients demonstrating pathological dissociation.
• Dissociation is investigated both as a trait and as a categorical variable. The effect of dissociative
states per se is not studied.

includes dissociative symptoms, non-delusional


Introduction
suspiciousness and quasi-psychotic symptoms.
Borderline personality disorder (BPD) is charac- Although neurocognitive impairment has been
terized by disturbed relational abilities, affective suggested to act as a moderator in the develop-
dysregulation, lack of behavior control and dis- ment of the disorder (4), only a few studies have,
turbed cognition (1–3). Disturbed cognition with a variety of results, been devoted to the

383
Haaland and Landrø

neuropsychological functioning of persons with


Aims of the study
BPD. A recent review of neuropsychological cor-
relates of BPD suggests the existence of several We sought to investigate associations between
neuropsychological deficits (5). These deficits are dissociation and neuropsychological functioning
particularly pertinent to executive functions. Still, in patients with BPD. Our focus was on correla-
more research would be needed to examine vari- tions between total dissociative experiences and
able results with regard to other neuropsycholog- neuropsychological performance, as well as on
ical functions. comparison of the neuropsychological functioning
Dissociation can be defined as a disruption in the of patients with BPD with and without patholog-
usually integrated functions of consciousness, ical dissociation with the performance of healthy
memory, identity and perception (3). The occur- controls.
rence of Ôtransient, stress-related severe dissociative
symptoms or paranoid ideationÕ is a defining
criterion of BPD that was added to the current Material and methods
version of Diagnostic and Statistical Manual
Participants with borderline personality disorder
(DSM) (3). This criterion, which occurs in
around 75% of patients with BPD, shows excellent This study was approved by the Regional Commit-
specificity (1). According to the DSM-IV, disso- tee for Medical Research Ethics, and written
ciative symptoms in patients with BPD are in informed consent was obtained after the partici-
particular associated with situations with high pants had been provided with a complete description
levels of stress (3), a relation that has been of the study. The sample consisted of 35 patients,
demonstrated experimentally (6, 7). recruited through in- and outpatient settings from
Dissociative experiences also occur in the different units of the Clinic for Mental Health –
general population (8) and measures like the Psychiatry and Dependency treatment at Sørlandet
Dissociative Experiences Scale (DES) assess dis- Hospital HF – a general hospital in southern
sociation as a continuous variable or a trait (9). Norway serving a population of about 265 000.
However, some studies indicate that dissociative We aimed for a sample of patients with BPD who
experiences are not uniformly distributed in may be considered representative of subjects in an
clinical groups the way a dimensional model of inpatient or outpatient setting. Knowing that com-
dissociation would imply. Differences in mean orbidity for axis I and axis II disorders is frequent in
dissociative scores between different diagnostic patients with BPD (15, 16), we sought to limit the
groups seem to depend on differences in the exclusion criteria so as to include patients with the
frequency of high-dissociative individuals (10). A most common comorbid disorders such as depres-
taxometric analysis of dissociative experiences sion and post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD).
revealed that two types of dissociation exists Patients had to be between 18 and 40 years of age
(11). Non-pathological dissociation occurs along and were required to fulfill the DSM-IV (3) criteria
a continuum and is considered a manifestation for BPD. Exclusion criteria were a history of head
of a dimensional construct or a dissociative trauma or epilepsy, significant neurological findings,
trait. Pathological dissociation is considered a mental retardation and ongoing substance abuse. A
manifestation of a latent class variable or a total of 47 patients were recruited for evaluation; of
taxon. these, 12 did not fulfill the criteria and five had not
Recent research has increasingly tended to completed the clinical measure for dissociative
focus on associations between cognition and experiences (DES). These were excluded from the
dissociation. Non-pathological dissociation has study, leaving the final clinical sample with a total of
in this context been approached as a fundamen- 30 patients. Twenty-six patients were diagnosed
tal information-processing mechanism (12). Few with lifetime affective disorder – 10 in remission, 16
studies have focused on cognition and dissocia- with PTSD, 11 with an anxiety disorder other than
tion in clinical populations except for dissociative PTSD, 9 with another personality disorder and 6
disorders and none of those have focused on with substance abuse. All but six patients were
pathological dissociation. In a sample of forensic taking psychotropic medications of which the most
inpatients, a negative correlation was found common were SSRIs and atypical antipsychotics.
between executive test performance and dissocia-
tive symptoms (13). In patients with BPD,
Healthy controls
dissociative symptoms have been related to
difficulties in recalling specific autobiographic Thirty-one non-clinical comparison subjects were
memories (14). recruited among non-healthcare employees at the

384
Dissociation and cognition in BPD

hospital, students having practicum at the hospital, Table 1. Test battery used to assess neuropsychological and intellectual perfor-
mance grouped by cognitive domain
through newspaper advertisement or among friends
and relatives of the staff of the hospital. In addition Neuropsychological domain and constituent items Variables
to the exclusion criteria that applied to the patient
Attention
group, selection criteria for the healthy controls
Digit Symbol-Coding (WAIS-III) (20) Total raw score
were no history of availing psychiatric services, no Conner Continuous Omissions; hit reaction time –
history of psychotropic medication and no history Performance Test (21) by block
which indicated psychiatric disorders. Working memory
Digit Span (WAIS-III) (20) Total raw score
Paced Auditory Serial Addition Test (22) Number of correct responses
Clinical and diagnostic assessments Letter Number Span (23) Number of correct responses
n-Back (24) Number of correct responses
Diagnoses were established using the structured in each of the 1–3 back
conditions
clinical interviews for DSM-IV SCID-I (17) and Executive functions
SCID-II (18) conducted by one of the authors Stroop Color-Word Test (25) Stroop interference score:
(V.Ø.H.). Twenty of the interviews with potentially time to read incongruent
participating patients were videotaped. The rating list minus time to read
congruent list
was repeated by an external experienced clinical Tower of London (TOL-4) (26) 0–3 points dependent of
psychologist who was unaware as to whether the number of trials to solve
patients were included in the study or not. The each problem. Total score
inter-rater agreement for the ratings of the nine was the sum of points over
all 10 problems
criteria for BPD was found to be good (average Controlled Oral Word Association (27) Semantic and phonetic fluency
CohenÕs j = 0.640). Depression was measured Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (28) Percent preservative errors
with the Hamilton Depression Rating Scale (19), Trail Making Test (29) Flexibility index [trail B (time)
minus trail A (time)]
global functioning by the Global Assessment of
Iowa Gambling Task (30) Number of choices from the
Functioning Scale (GAF) (3), whereas dissociative advantageous decks minus
symptoms were measured using the Norwegian the number of choices from
version of the DES (9) a 28-item rating scale where the disadvantageous decks
Verbal long-term memory
each item is scored from 0 to 100. Individuals with Hopkins Verbal Learning Test (32) Learning score: sum for trials
pathological dissociation can be identified using 1–3; immediate free recall;
eight items from the DES, the DES-T (11). delayed free recall,
after 20 min
Visual long-term memory
Neuropsychological assessment Rey–Osterrieth Complex Figure (29) Immediate recall; delayed
recall, after 20 min; item
Every participant underwent an extensive battery of recognition with
intellectual and neuropsychological tests, where 50% distractors
Kimura Recurring Recognition Number of correct responses
most cognitive domains were covered with several Figures Test (33)
tests to obtain stable and robust measures. Domains General cognitive ability
covered included attention, working memory, exec- Picture Arrangement (WAIS-III) (20) Total raw score
utive functions, verbal and non-verbal long-term Block Design (WAIS-III) (20) Total raw score
Picture Completion (WAIS-III) (20) Total raw score
memory, and general cognitive abilities. The neuro- Vocabulary (WAIS-III) (20) Total raw score
psychological battery was administered by a test Similarities (WAIS-III) (20) Total raw score
technician or a clinical psychologist trained in
WAIS-III, Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale 3 edition.
standardized assessment. Participants were tested
individually, and the total time required for testing
was about 4 h divided into two sessions. The tasks attention. In the Digit Symbol-Coding subtest,
were given in the same order, and testing was symbols must be matched and drawn as fast as
performed at the clinic in the same location for all possible under the corresponding digit. The CPT-II
participants. The neuropsychological test battery asks participants to monitor a random series of
with definitions of the variables used grouped by single letters, presented continuously at different
cognitive domain is presented in Table 1. rates on a monitor. The space bar has to be pressed
when a letter is presented except for the letter ÔxÕ.
Measures of attention
Measures of working memory
The Digit Symbol-Coding subtest from WAIS-III
(20) and the Conners Continuous Performance The Digit Span subtest from WAIS-III (20), a
Test II (CPT-II) (21) were used as measures of variant of the Paced Auditory Serial Addition Test

385
Haaland and Landrø

(PASAT) (22), the Letter Number Span (LNS) (23) learning task with a 12-item list, and three succes-
and the n-back task (24) were used as measures of sive learning trials.
working memory. In the Digit Span subtest, series
of orally presented numbers must be repeated
Measures of visual long-term memory
verbatim and in reverse. In PASAT (22), 60 CD-
recorded digits are presented at a rate of 3.0 s. The Kimura Recurring Recognition Figures Test
Participants are required to add each number to (33) and the Rey Complex Figure Test (RCFT) (29)
the one that immediately precedes it, and articulate were used as measures of visual long-term memory.
the new number promptly. In the LNS (23), orally The Kimura (33) is a continuous recognition task
presented sequences of letters and numbers must be that involves recognizing 20 cards containing previ-
sorted and presented by listing numbers in ascend- ously learned drawn figures among 100 cards which
ing order and letters in alphabetical order. In the are presented one by one. The RCFT (29) was used
n-back task (24), the numbers 2, 4, 6 or 8 are along with a recognition task. It involves copying
displayed randomly on a computer screen. The and recalling a complex geometric figure. The
task is to respond on the keyboard with the scoring of the figures followed the 36-point scoring
number presented n positions back in the sequence. system described by Lezak (29).
In our study, four conditions were used, 0-, 1-, 2-
and 3-back. Participants completed three trials in
Measures of general cognitive ability
each condition, with 14 stimuli per trial.
The Vocabulary, Block Design, Picture Completion,
Picture Arrangement and the Similarities subtests
Measures of executive functions
from WAIS-III (20) were used as measures of
The Stroop Color-Word Test (25), a version of the general cognitive ability. In the subtest Picture
Tower of London (TOL-4) (26), the Controlled Oral Arrangement, cartoon-type pictures must be rear-
Word Association Test (COWA) (27), the Wiscon- ranged to make a logical story sequence. In Block
sin Card Sorting Test (WCST) (28), the Trail Design, a set of blocks has to be arranged to replicate
Making Tests (TMT) (29) and the Iowa Gambling various patterns. In Picture Completion, the most
Task (IGT) were used (30) as measures of executive important part missing in each of a set of color
functions. The Stroop Color-Word Test (25) con- pictures has to be identified. The Vocabulary subtest
sists of three white cards, each containing a 6 · 8 asks participants to define orally and visually
matrix of stimulus. The critical part of the test presented words, whereas in the Similarities subtest
involves inhibition of response to color words they are asked to explain the similarity between pairs
printed in incongruently colored inks. The TOL-4 of words. The original scoring procedure was used.
(26) is a problem-solving task involving forward
planning to match arrangements of colored balls.
Statistical analysis
The COWA (27) is a measure of phonetic and
semantic verbal fluency requiring the ability to Data analysis was completed by means of spss 16.0
generate words beginning with the letters F, A and S (SPSS Inc., Chicago, IL, USA). The DES-T (11)
and words in the categories of animals and cloths, score was computed and the clinical sample was
each in 1 min. In the WCST (28) the principles divided into a group belonging to the pathological
according to which a deck of cards must be sorted dissociation taxon and a group not belonging to
has to be discovered. In TMT-A (29), lines have to the pathological dissociation taxon. Pathological
be drawn sequentially connecting 25 encircled taxon membership was determined by calculating
numbers distributed on a sheet of paper. In TMT- Bayesian posterior taxon membership probabilities
B, the participants must alternate between numbers according to procedures described by Waller and
and letters. A computerized version of IGT was used Ross (34). Analyses of variance (anova) and chi-
(30). IGT is a decision-making task under condi- squared tests were used to compare the three
tions of limited knowledge about reward and groups regarding demographic variables whereas
penalty, involving choices from advantageous and t-tests and chi-squared tests were used to compare
disadvantageous decks of cards. the two clinical groups regarding clinical variables.
For the purpose of analysis, all neuropsycholog-
ical scores were transformed into standardized
Measures of verbal long-term memory
scores based on the mean values and standard
The Hopkins Verbal Learning Test – Revised deviations of the whole clinical sample. The stan-
(HVLT-R) (31, 32) was used as a measure of verbal dardized measures were grouped into five different
long-term memory. This is a classical word list cognitive domains and summary scores were

386
Dissociation and cognition in BPD

computed. Prorated IQ was computed using the members with pathological dissociation and 20
sum of the scaled scores of those five subtests non-members among the patients with BPD. One
multiplied with 11 ⁄ 5 as raw score. Table 1 presents healthy control participant was also identified as a
the constituent test scores in each cognitive taxon member and was excluded from further
domain. The correlations between the neuropsy- analysis. The probability for every participant in
chological domains and the DES and DES-T the remaining healthy control group was 0.04%
scores in the clinical group were computed using (the lowest possible value) indicating no scores
PearsonÕs r. The pathological taxon group and the above cut-off for any of the DES-T items for any of
non-pathological taxon group were compared with the participants. For the non-member patient
healthy controls with respect to neuropsychologi- group, the average membership probability was
cal performance by the use of a multivariate 1.9% (range 0.04–7.0%), and for the member
analysis of variance (manova) followed up with group 89% (range 72.5–100%), indicating no
anovas. Where significant effects were demon- uncertainty with respect to membership status.
strated, post hoc pairwise comparisons were con-
ducted using Bonferroni adjustments in order to
Demographic characteristics
correct for multiple comparisons. Effect sizes were
also computed for those differences. Table 2 displays the mean values and standard
deviations in demographic and clinical variables
for the three groups including results of statistical
Results comparisons. The groups were statistically similar
in terms of all demographic variables except for
Taxon membership
participantsÕ educational level. Post hoc tests
The computation of Bayesian posterior taxon (Scheffé) indicated that both the taxon member
membership probabilities resulted in 10 taxon group (P = 0.004) and the non-taxon member

Table 2. Demographic and clinical characteristics, and neuropsychological test performance of patients with borderline personality disorder with and without pathological
dissociation, and healthy controls

BPD Non-DES-T BPD DES-T Healthy controls


(n = 20) (n = 10) (n = 30) Analysis

Mean SD Mean SD Mean SD F d.f. P

Age (years) 25.5 5.6 23.7 4.8 25.9 6.4 0.52 2 0.597
Education (years) 12.9 2.6 12.0 1.6 14.8 2.1 8.18 2 0.001
MotherÕs education (years) 11.3 3.0 10.8 2.9 13.0 3.1 2.55 2 0.088
FatherÕs education (years) 12.9 4.5 12.2 3.5 13.8 3.0 0.77 2 0.471
t* d.f. P

Hamilton DRS 11.4 6.0 14.4 4.9 1.8 2.6 1.32 28 0.197
GAF 42.1 5.2 38.8 7.7 88.5 5.5 1.37 28 0.181
DES – total score 14.9 9.0 38.5 15.5 2.2 1.9 5.28 28 <0.001
DES – pathological 10.5 7.2 36.3 19.8 0.5 0.9 5.24 28 <0.001
dissociation score
n % n % n % v2 d.f. P

Gender
Male 4 20.0 0 0.0 4 13.3
Female 16 80.0 10 100.0 26 86.6 2.308 2 0.315
Handedness
Right 18 90.0 10 100.0 27 90.0
Left 2 10.0 0 0.0 3 10.0 1.550 2 0.461
Mean SD Mean SD Mean SD F d.f. P

Neuropsychological performance
Attention 0.25 0.41 )0.44 1.29 0.59 0.59 7.27 2, 51 0.002
Working memory 0.27 0.64 )0.33 0.84 0.67 0.54 10.06 2, 51 <0.001
Executive functions 0.26 0.40 )0.40 0.69 0.68 0.32 25.19 2, 51 <0.001
Verbal long-term memory 0.24 0.67 )0.46 1.19 0.44 0.55 7.83 2, 51 0.001
Visual long-term memory 0.25 0.61 )0.10 0.61 0.60 0.53 5.67 2, 51 0.006
Prorated IQ 100.9 12.3 87.2 12.9 113.7 11.4 20.90 2, 51 <0.001

DRS, Depression Rating Scale; GAF, Global Assessment of Functioning; DES, Dissociative Experiences Scale; BPD, Borderline Personality Disorder; DES-T, Dissociative
Experiences Scale pathological dissociation Taxon member.
*Comparisons between the two clinical groups.

387
Haaland and Landrø

group (P = 0.016) had a significantly lower edu- Bias-corrected (HedgesÕ ĝ) effect sizes with 95%
cational level than the healthy control group. No confidence intervals are presented in Fig. 1.
differences were found between the two clinical
groups (P = 0.543).
Neuropsychological performance of patients with BPD without
pathological dissociation
Clinical characteristics
We found a significant difference between the
Between the two clinical groups, the analysis healthy controls and patients with BPD without
showed no differences with respect to scores on pathological dissociation on measures of executive
the Hamilton Depression Rating Scale or on the functioning (P = 0.004), but not on measures of
GAF scale (Table 2). The analysis revealed signif- the other domains: attention (P = 0.388), working
icant differences between the two groups on memory (P = 0.307), verbal long-term memory
dissociative experiences both on the DES total (P = 1.000), non-verbal long-term memory
score and on the DES-T score. There were no (P = 0.927) or general cognitive functioning
differences between the two clinical groups in the (P = 0.057). Bias-corrected (HedgesÕ ĝ) effect
frequencies of affective disorders (v2 = 0.144; sizes with 95% confidence intervals are presented
d.f. = 1; P = 0.704) or PTSD (v2 = 0.268; in Fig. 1.
d.f. = 1; P = 0.605).
Differences in neuropsychological performance between the
Overall analysis of neuropsychological performance clinical groups
When comparing the two clinical groups and the Patients with BPD without pathological dissocia-
healthy control group with manova (Wilks lambda), tion showed better performance than patients with
we found an overall group difference in neuro- BPD with pathological dissociation on measures of
psychological test performance [F(12,92) = 4.36, working memory (P = 0.027), executive function-
P < 0.001]. anova revealed significant differences ing (P = 0.002), verbal long-term memory (P =
between the three groups in all neuropsychological 0.002) and general cognitive functioning
domains (Table 2). (P = 0.001). No differences were found between
the two groups on measures of attention (P = 0.097)
or non-verbal long-term memory (P = 0.092).
Neuropsychological performance of patients with BPD with
pathological dissociation
Correlations between neuropsychological performance and
Post hoc pairwise comparisons with Bonferroni
dissociation
adjustments revealed significant differences
between patients with BPD with pathological In the combined clinical samples, there were
dissociation and the healthy control group on significant negative correlations between DES
every cognitive domain: attention (P = 0.001), total score and attention (r = )0.56, P = 0.001),
working memory (P < 0.001), executive function- and between DES-T score and attention
ing (P < 0.001), verbal long-term memory (P = (r = )0.53, P = 0.003) and verbal long-term
0.002), non-verbal long-term memory (P = 0.004) memory (r = )0.41, P = 0.025). Correlations
and general cognitive functioning (P < 0.001). between the dissociation score and the other

3.5
(adjusted)

3 BPD/DES-T
BPD/NON-DES-T
2.5
2
Effect sizes, Hedges

1.5
1
0.5 Fig. 1. Effect sizes (with error bars
indicating 95% CIs) of the differences
0 between the groups of patients with
–0.5 BPD with and without pathological
Attention Working Executive Verbal LTM Visual LTM IQ dissociation and the healthy control
memory functions group on every cognitive domain.

388
Dissociation and cognition in BPD

Table 3. Correlations between performance on neuropsychological domains and for the variability of the findings. This clinical
measures of dissociation in patients with borderline personality disorder
characteristic may also plausibly account for the
Working Executive Verbal Visual Prorated fMRI findings which connect specific frontolimbic
memory functions LTM LTM IQ DES DES-T neural substrates to behavioral dyscontrol in
patients with BPD (45).
Attention 0.41* 0.61** 0.60** 0.37 0.47** )0.56** )0.53**
Working 0.75** 0.48** 0.46** 0.74** )0.35 )0.20 The two groups also differed with respect to
memory general intellectual abilities. The score of the
Executive 0.47** 0.50** 0.65** )0.38 )0.22 group with pathological dissociation was more
functions
Verbal LTM 0.53** 0.58** )0.34 )0.41*
than 1 SD lower than the group without
Visual LTM 0.61** )0.25 )0.24 pathological dissociation. This is a crucial finding
Prorated IQ )0.30 )0.25 that, along with the results regarding the other
DES 0.89** cognitive domains, allow for several hypotheses.
LTM, Long-term memory, DES, Dissociative Experiences Scale – total score; DES-T
First, one could argue that reduced cognitive
Dissociative Experiences Scale pathological dissociation score. Correlation is sig- abilities enhance the probability of pathological
nificant at the *0.05 (two tailed) and **0.01 (two tailed) levels. dissociation. Second, one could hypothesize that
there is a common factor to reduced cognitive
abilities and pathological dissociation; it could be
neuropsychological domains and between the dif-
a question of traumatic experiences that, at a
ferent domains are presented in Table 3.
certain stage, would effect neurobiological devel-
opment. Third, it is, of course, possible that the
Discussion pathological dissociative phenomena per se cause
reduced test performance, i.e. pathological disso-
The association between pathological dissociation and
ciation during test uptake conflicts with the tasks
neuropsychological performance
solved. Nonetheless, pathological dissociation and
To our knowledge, this is the first study that reduced working memory, executive functions,
explores associations between pathological disso- long-term verbal memory performance and IQ
ciation as a categorical variable and neuropsycho- seem to be co-occurring in patients with BPD.
logical performance in patients with BPD. Patients
with BPD and pathological dissociation were
Correlations between non-pathological dissociation and
found to have reduced executive functioning,
neuropsychological performance
working memory and long-term verbal memory
performance when compared with patients with When using a dimensional model of dissociation,
BPD without pathological dissociation. With we found a significant negative correlation between
regard to the neuropsychological domains of the total DES score and performance in the
attention and long-term non-verbal memory, no attention domain, but not between DES score
differences were found between the two groups. and performance on the neuropsychological
When compared with healthy controls, patients domains of working memory, executive functions,
with BPD with pathological dissociation showed long-term verbal memory, long-term non-verbal
reduced performance on every cognitive domain, memory or IQ. When treating the DES-T as a
whereas patients with BPD without pathological dimensional variable, it was found to be signifi-
dissociation showed only reduced executive func- cantly negatively correlated with attention as well
tioning. The only neuropsychological domain that as with verbal long-term memory, but not with
seems to be consistently impaired in patients with performance on the other domains. These findings
BPD, regardless of pathological dissociation, is the are crucial for two reasons:
executive one; a finding that concurs with previous First, we found different associations between
research (5). dissociation and neuropsychological performance
This study contributes to the understanding of when applying a dimensional model compared
cognitive functioning of patients with BPD. Path- with the results when applying a categorical model.
ological dissociation might be a clinical variable Dimensional dissociation was negatively related to
that differentiates patients with BPD with regard to attention and verbal memory, while categorical
cognitive functioning. These results elaborate the (pathological) dissociation was negatively related
findings from earlier examinations of executive to working memory, executive functions and verbal
functioning, working memory and long-term memory. When taking the DES total score pri-
verbal memory in BPD (35–44). Different propor- marily as a measure of non-pathological dissocia-
tions of pathological dissociative patients in differ- tion, this implies that non-pathological and
ent study samples may be one factor that accounts pathological dissociation are related to different

389
Haaland and Landrø

cognitive functions. When using the DES-T results dissociation per se that results in poorer outcome,
dimensionally, we found approximately the same but the cognitive functioning of the patients
results as when we used the DES, indicating that involved in those studies. Such a hypothesis,
research based on DES-T dimensionally as a however, is not entirely congruent with the results
measure of pathological dissociation may produce presented in a recent study by Braakmann et al.
different results compared with that of studies (59) in which no negative prognostic value was
treating pathological dissociation as a latent class found of dissociative symptoms on the effect of
variable. inpatient Dialectic Behavior Therapy. The authorsÕ
Second, these findings allow for interesting hypothesis is that this is because of the specific
comparisons with studies investigating the rela- focus on dissociative symptoms facilitated by an
tionship between dissociation and cognition in inpatient setting. Further studies on the treatment
non-clinical samples. High-dissociative persons of BPD should investigate the prognostic value of
have been found to have an advantage on different neuropsychological variables as well as psycho-
cognitive tasks in some studies (12, 46–48), a pathological measures.
disadvantage on different cognitive tasks in other
studies (49–52), whereas some studies report no
Limitations
differences between high and low dissociative
participants in tests of executive functions and The two groups of patients with BPD were
working memory (53, 54). Although not signifi- homogenous with respect to demographic and
cant, all the correlations in our sample were clinical variables, including depressive symptoms
negative, indicating a common direction. The and GAF values, except for degree of dissociative
reason for this may be that we used domain results symptoms. One could argue that patients in the
of the neuropsychological performance rather than pathological dissociation group also had a disso-
results on single tests or single variables. Another ciative disorder; however, as SCID-D was not used
explanation may be that in a clinical sample like in this study, we cannot give answer to this
ours, with high proportions of persons with patho- question. These findings may therefore be
logical dissociation, the possible advantageous explained by comorbidity alone, but another and
effect of non-pathological dissociation would be in our view more plausible explanation would be
overshadowed. Our results differ, however, from a that patients with BPD who also show pathological
recent study which suggests that patients with dissociation represent a more severe subgroup of
dissociative disorders present elevated frontal patients with BPD. Such an interpretation is
function and enhanced working memory perfor- supported by studies showing enhanced working
mance (55). A possible, but perhaps not plausible, memory performance in patients with dissociative
explanation for this discrepancy in results could be disorders (55).
that dissociation is associated with reduced frontal Most patients in our sample were taking psy-
functions in patients with BPD, but with elevated chotropic medication when the tests were con-
frontal function in patients with dissociative dis- ducted, and no steps were taken to reduce what
orders. Another possibility is that a diagnosis of influences medication may have had. For SSRIs,
dissociative disorders not necessarily implies mem- some positive effects have been found on neuro-
bership in the pathological taxon as measured with psychological performance in healthy subjects (60).
the DES-T. Atypical antipsychotics have been found to have
some negative effects on attention, but no effect on
executive functions in healthy subjects (61). Con-
Dissociation and treatment outcome
sidering that patients in both groups used medica-
Dissociative symptoms have been associated with tion and that SSRIs and atypical neuroleptics were
poorer outcome following psychotherapeutic treat- the most frequently used medication in both
ment in patients with panic disorder, agoraphobia samples, it is unlikely that medication influences
and obsessive–compulsive disorder, as well as in would have caused the reduced neuropsychological
patients with affective, anxiety or somatoform performance found in this study.
disorders (56–58). Although these studies did not Given the high comorbidity of DSM-IV axis I
include patients with BPD, our study may give a and II disorders in BPD (15, 16), comorbidity was
hypothesis for a neuropsychological-based expla- not a criterion for exclusion except for severe
nation to the findings. We found both dimensional ongoing substance abuse. PTSD and lifetime
and categorical measures of dissociation to be affective disorders were the most frequent comor-
associated with dysfunctions in fundamental bid disorders. When comparing the two groups
cognitive abilities. It may therefore not be the with and without pathological dissociation, we

390
Dissociation and cognition in BPD

found no differences in frequency of those comor- personality disorder. Acta Psychiatr Scand 2008;117:139–
bid disorders. Average GAF scores indicated that 147.
8. Ross CA, Joshi S, Currie R. Dissociative experiences in the
patients were moderately affected. Our sample may general population. Am J Psychiatry 1990;147:1547–1552.
therefore be considered representative of patients 9. Bernstein EM, Putnam FW. Development, reliability, and
in general clinical settings, and inferences may be validity of a dissociation scale. J Nerv Ment Dis 1986;
drawn from this sample to these patients, but not 174:727–735.
necessarily to the patient group with BPD and no 10. Putnam FW, Carlson EB, Ross CA et al. Patterns of dis-
sociation in clinical and nonclinical samples. J Nerv Ment
comorbid disorders without psychotropic medica- Dis 1996;184:673–679.
tion. Future studies should take the effect of 11. Waller NG, Putnam FW, Carlson EB. Types of dissociation
comorbidity and medications systematically into and dissociative types: a taxometric analysis of dissociative
consideration. experiences. Psychol Methods 1996;1:300–321.
Studies similar to ours, in which neuropsycho- 12. de Ruiter MB, Phaf RH, Elzinga BM, van Dyck R. Disso-
ciative style and individual differences in verbal working
logical measures and its relations to psychopatho- memory span. Conscious Cogn 2004;13:821–828.
logical phenomenon are investigated, may expand 13. Cima M, Merckelbach H, Klein B, Shellbach-Matties R,
our understanding of the behavioral characteristics Kremer K. Frontal lobe dysfunctions, dissociation, and
of various psychiatric disorders. Further studies trauma self-reports in forensic psychiatric patients. J Nerv
will have to be undertaken to specify the connec- Ment Dis 2001;189:188–190.
14. Jones B, Heard H, Startup M, Swales M, Williams JMG,
tions that exist between pathological dissociation Jones RSP. Autobiographical memory and dissociation in
in patients with BPD, various aspects of neuro- borderline personality disorder. Psychol Med 1999;29:
psychological functioning and psychosocial func- 1397–1404.
tioning and other outcome variables. 15. Zanarini MC, Frankenburg FR, Dubo ED et al. Axis I
comorbidity of borderline personality disorder. Am J
Psychiatry 1998;155:1733–1739.
Acknowledgements 16. Becker DF, Grilo CM, Edell WS, McGlashan TH. Com-
orbidity of borderline personality disorder with other
We thank study participants, colleagues at Sørlandet Hospital personality disorders in hospitalized adolescents and
HF for assistance in recruiting the participants, Torunn adults. Am J Psychiatry 2000;157:2011–2016.
Haaland PhD for valuable language consultation, and research 17. First MB, Spitzer RL, Gibbon M, Williams JBW. Structured
nurse Gro Steensohn for assisting us with the data collection. Clinical Interview for DSM-IV-TR Axis I Disorders, Re-
The study was financially supported by Sørlandet Hospital HF search Version, Patient Edition. (SCID-I ⁄ P). New York:
and the Southern Norway Regional Health Authority. The Biometrics Research, New York State Psychiatric Institute,
content of this manuscript and the manuscript itself have never 1997.
been published either electronically or in print. 18. First MB, Spitzer RL, Gibbon M, Williams JBW. Structured
Clinical Interview for DSM-IV Personality Disorders,
(SCID-II). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Press,
Declaration of interest Inc., 1997.
None. 19. Hamilton M. Development of a rating scale for primary
depressive illness. Br J Soc Clin Psychol 1967;6:278–296.
20. Wechsler D, Nyman H, Nordvik H. Wais-III: Wechsler
References Adult Intelligence Scale: manual (Norwegian edition), 3rd
edn. Stockholm: Psykologiförlaget, 2003.
1. Skodol AE, Gunderson JG, Pfohl B, Widiger TA, Livesley 21. Conners CK. ConnersÕ Continuous Performance test
WJ, Siever LJ. The borderline diagnosis I: psychopathol- (CPTII). Technical guide and software manual. North
ogy, comorbidity, and personality structure. Biol Psychi- Tonawanda, NY: Multi Health Systems Inc., 2002.
atry 2002;51:936–950. 22. Gronwall DM. Paced auditory serial-addition task: a
2. Lieb K, Zanarini MC, Schmahl C, Linehan MM, Bohus M. measure of recovery from concussion. Percept Mot Skills
Borderline personality disorder. Lancet 2004;364:453–461. 1977;44:367–373.
3. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and statisti- 23. Gold JM, Carpenter C, Randolph C, Goldberg TE, Wein-
cal manual of mental disorders: DSM-IV-TR, 4th, text berger DR. Auditory working memory and Wisconsin
revision edn. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Card Sorting Test performance in schizophrenia. Arch
Association, 2000. Gen Psychiatry 1997;54:159–165.
4. Judd PH. Neurocognitive impairment as a moderator in the 24. Callicott JH, Ramsey NF, Tallent K et al. Functional
development of borderline personality disorder. Dev Psy- magnetic resonance imaging brain mapping in psychiatry:
chopathol 2005;17:1173–1196. methodological issues illustrated in a study of working
5. LeGris J, van Reekum R. The neuropsychological correlates memory in schizophrenia. Neuropsychopharmacology
of borderline personality disorder and suicidal behaviour. 1998;18:186–196.
Can J Psychiatry 2006;51:131–142. 25. Lund-Johansen M, Hugdahl K, Wester K. Cognitive func-
6. Stiglmayr CE, Shapiro DA, Stieglitz RD, Limberger MF, tion in patients with ParkinsonÕs disease undergoing ste-
Bohus M. Experience of aversive tension and dissociation reotaxic thalamotomy. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry
in female patients with borderline personality disorder – a 1996;60:564–571.
controlled study. J Psychiatr Res 2001;35:111–118. 26. Shum D, Short L, Tunstall J et al. Performance of children
7. Stiglmayr CE, Ebner-Priemer UW, Bretz J et al. Dissocia- with traumatic brain injury on a 4-disk version of the
tive symptoms are positively related to stress in borderline

391
Haaland and Landrø

Tower of London and the Porteus Maze. Brain Cogn 45. Silbersweig D, Clarkin JF, Goldstein M et al. Failure of
2000;44:59–62. frontolimbic inhibitory function in the context of negative
27. Spreen O, Strauss E. A compendium of neuropsychological emotion in borderline personality disorder. Am J Psychi-
tests: administration, norms, and commentary, 2nd edn. atry 2007;164:1832–1841.
New York: Oxford University Press, 1998. 46. de Ruiter MB, Phaf RH, Veltman DJ, Kok A, van Dyck R.
28. Heaton RK. Wisconsin card sorting test manual. Odessa, Attention as a characteristic of nonclinical dissociation: an
FL: Psychological Assessment Resources, Inc., 1981. event-related potential study. NeuroImage 2003;19:376–
29. Lezak MD. Neuropsychological assessment, 3rd edn. New 390.
York: Oxford University Press, 1995. 47. Veltman DJ, de Ruiter MB, Rombouts SA et al. Neuro-
30. Bechara A, Damasio H, Damasio AR, Lee GP. Different physiological correlates of increased verbal working
contributions of the human amygdala and ventromedial memory in high-dissociative participants: a functional
prefrontal cortex to decision-making. J Neurosci 1999;19: MRI study. Psychol Med 2005;35:175–185.
5473–5481. 48. DePrince AP, Freyd JJ. Dissociative tendencies, attention,
31. Brandt J. The Hopkins Verbal Learning Test: development and memory. Psychol Sci 1999;10:449–452.
of a new memory test with six equivalent forms. Clin 49. Wright DB, Livingston-Raper D. Memory distortion and
Neuropsychol 1991;5:125–142. dissociation: exploring the relationship in a non-clinical
32. Benedict RHB, Schretlen D, Groninger L, Brandt J. Hop- sample. J Trauma Dissociation 2002;3:97–109.
kins Verbal Learning Test – Revised: normative data and 50. Kindt M, van den Hout M. Dissociation and memory
analysis of inter-form and test-retest reliability. Clin Neu- fragmentation: experimental effects on meta-memory but
ropsychol 1998;12:43–55. not on actual memory performance. Behav Res Ther
33. Kimura D. Right temporal-lobe damage. Perception of 2003;41:167–178.
unfamiliar stimuli after damage. Arch Neurol 1963;8:264– 51. Freyd JJ, Martorello SR, Alvarado JS, Hayes AE, Christ-
271. man JC. Cognitive environments and dissociative tenden-
34. Waller NG, Ross CA. The prevalence and biometric cies: performance on the standard Stroop task for high
structure of pathological dissociation in the general versus low dissociators. Appl Cogn Psychol 1998;12:S91–
population: taxometric and behavior genetic findings. S103.
J Abnorm Psychol 1997;106:499–510. 52. Giesbrecht T, Merckelbach H, Geraerts E, Smeets E. Dis-
35. Lenzenweger MF, Clarkin JF, Fertuck EA, Kernberg OF. sociation in undergraduate students: disruptions in execu-
Executive neurocognitive functioning and neurobehavioral tive functioning. J Nerv Ment Dis 2004;192:567–569.
systems indicators in borderline personality disorder: a 53. Bruce AS, Ray WJ, Bruce JM, Arnett PA, Carlson RA. The
preliminary study. J Personal Disord 2004;18:421–438. relationship between executive functioning and dissocia-
36. van Reekum R, Conway CA, Gansler D, White R, Bachman tion. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol 2007;29:626–633.
DL. Neurobehavioral study of borderline personality dis- 54. Wright DB, Osborne JE. Dissociation, cognitive failures,
order. J Psychiatry Neurosci 1993;18:121–129. and working memory. Am J Psychol 2005;118:103–113.
37. OÕLeary KM, Brouwers P, Gardner DL, Cowdry RW. 55. Elzinga BM, Ardon AM, Heijnis MK, De Ruiter MB, van
Neuropsychological testing of patients with borderline Dyck R, Veltman DJ. Neural correlates of enhanced
personality disorder. Am J Psychiatry 1991;148:106–111. working-memory performance in dissociative disorder: a
38. Swirsky-Sacchetti T, Gorton G, Samuel S, Sobel R, Genetta- functional MRI study. Psychol Med 2007;37:235–245.
Wadley A, Burleigh B. Neuropsychological function in 56. Michelson L, June K, Vives A, Testa S, Marchione N. The
borderline personality disorder. J Clin Psychol 1993;49: role of trauma and dissociation in cognitive-behavioral
385–396. psychotherapy outcome and maintenance for panic disor-
39. Kunert HJ, Druecke HW, Sass H, Herpertz SC. Frontal lobe der with agoraphobia. Behav Res Ther 1998;36:1011–1050.
dysfunctions in borderline personality disorder? Neuro- 57. Rufer M, Held D, Cremer J et al. Dissociation as a pre-
psychological findings. J Personal Disord 2003;17:497–509. dictor of cognitive behavior therapy outcome in patients
40. Sprock J, Rader TJ, Kendall JP, Yoder CY. Neuropsycho- with obsessive–compulsive disorder. Psychother Psycho-
logical functioning in patients with borderline personality som 2006;75:40–46.
disorder. J Clin Psychol 2000;56:1587–1600. 58. Spitzer C, Barnow S, Freyberger HJ, Grabe HJ. Dissociation
41. Bazanis E, Rogers RD, Dowson JH et al. Neurocognitive predicts symptom-related treatment outcome in short-term
deficits in decision-making and planning of patients with inpatient psychotherapy. Aust N Z J Psychiatry 2007;
DSM-III-R borderline personality disorder. Psychol Med 41:682–687.
2002;32:1395–1405. 59. Braakmann D, Ludewig S, Milde J et al. Dissoziative
42. Beblo T, Saavedra AS, Mensebach C et al. Deficits in visual Symptome im Verlauf der Behandlung der Borderline-
functions and neuropsychological inconsistency in Bor- Persönlichkeitsstörung [Dissociative symptoms during
derline Personality Disorder. Psychiatry Res 2006;145:127– treatment of borderline personality disorder]. Psychother
135. Psychol Med 2007;57:154–160.
43. Haaland VØ, Landrø NI. Decision making as measured 60. Dumont GJH, de Visser SJ, Cohen AF, van Gerven JMA.
with the Iowa Gambling Task in patients with borderline Biomarkers for the effects of selective serotonin reuptake
personality disorder. J Int Neuropsychol Soc 2007;13:699– inhibitors (SSRIs) in healthy subjects. Br J Clin Pharmacol
703. 2005;59:495–510.
44. Stevens A, Burkhardt M, Hautzinger M, Schwarz J, Unckel 61. de Visser SJ, van der Post J, Pieters MSM, Cohen AF, van
C. Borderline personality disorder: impaired visual per- Gerven JMA. Biomarkers for the effects of antipsychotic
ception and working memory. Psychiatry Res 2004;125: drugs in healthy volunteers. Br J Clin Pharmacol 2001;
257–267. 51:119–132.

392

You might also like