Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 17

Behavioral Development Bulletin © 2017 American Psychological Association

2017, Vol. 22, No. 1, 94 –110 1942-0722/17/$12.00 http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/bdb0000041

Relational Responding: Testing, Training, and Sequencing Effects


Among Children With Autism and Typically Developing Children

Gráinne Kent and Edel Galvin Yvonne Barnes-Holmes


National University of Ireland Maynooth Ghent University

Carol Murphy Dermot Barnes-Holmes


National University of Ireland Maynooth Ghent University
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Relational Frame Theory (RFT) proposes that derived relational responding is crucial
to the development of verbal behavior. According to RFT, typically developing
children acquire the ability to derive relations through natural language interactions. In
contrast, children with autism often do not acquire these skills as readily and require
interventions to target their development. Limited research has examined the optimal
training context for establishing the core relational skills, such as the sequence in which
the relations might be optimally trained. The current research comprised 3 studies to
investigate the emergence of specific relational responding repertoires in typically
developing children and children with autism. The results demonstrate that the typically
developing children had a fluent repertoire of these relational skills, while those with
autism demonstrated significant deficits. The results shed some light on the possible
role of training sequence.

Keywords: typically developing children, autism spectrum disorder, relational frame


theory, derived relational responding, training sequence

Relational Frame Theory (RFT) centers fun- essential learning context for the complex ver-
damentally around the concept of derived rela- bal repertoires that emerge subsequently, and
tional responding and its role in all aspects of RFT points primarily to word– object interac-
language and cognition. The approach draws tions in this regard. For example, in naturalistic
mainly on the concept of arbitrarily applicable settings many exemplars of naming behavior
relational responding (AARRing, also known are directly reinforced and appear to give rise to
as relational framing; see Barnes, 1994), which subsequent emergent performances.
is offered as the basis for linguistic generativity Consider a simple example involving a child
and verbal behavior more broadly (Barnes- interacting with a ball. In establishing the word–
Holmes, McHugh, & Barnes Holmes, 2004). object relation (“ball”– ball), a parent may ask a
For RFT, early verbal exchanges provide the child “Where’s the ball?” and the child will
point to the ball, followed by parental praise.
The object–word relation with the ball would be
established similarly. The parent would hold up
This article was published Online First January 23, 2017. the ball and ask “What’s this?” to which the
Gráinne Kent and Edel Galvin, Department of Psychol- child would say “ball”, followed by praise. In
ogy, National University of Ireland Maynooth; Yvonne the context of the ball, therefore, both word–
Barnes-Holmes, Department of Experimental Clinical and
Health Psychology, Ghent University; Carol Murphy, De- object and object–word relations have been di-
partment of Psychology, National University of Ireland rectly trained. Now consider how this learning
Maynooth; Dermot Barnes-Holmes, Department of Experi- might generalize to interactions with a toy car.
mental Clinical and Health Psychology, Ghent University. In establishing the word– object relation, the
Correspondence concerning this article should be ad-
dressed to Gráinne Kent, Department of Psychology, Na-
parent may ask “Where’s the car?” and the child
tional University of Ireland, Maynooth, Ireland. E-mail: will point to the car, followed by praise. If the
GRAINNE.KENT.2012@nuim.ie parent now holds up the car and asks “What’s
94
RELATIONAL RESPONDING: TESTING, TRAINING 95

this?” the child will likely say “car”, even with- children with autism and adults with learning
out a history of reinforcement for doing so. impairments. The results showed that seven par-
According to RFT, this is a derived mutually ticipants with autism successfully demonstrated
entailed word– object coordination relation that derived manding—in the first clear demonstra-
emerges from the direct learning history of both tion of a derived or generative form of one of
word– object and object–word relations with the Skinner’s (1957) verbal operants with this pop-
ball, the trained word– object relation with the ulation.
car, and so on. In short, the behavior is novel, In spite of a growing body of supporting evi-
but based on a history of direct training with dence, many details of relational training regimes
other stimuli and relations. remain to be investigated. For example, there is
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

little empirical evidence to suggest the putative


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

role of the sequence in which the frames are


The Educational Significance of
established. A sequence along the lines of co-
Relational Responding ordination, distinction, opposition, comparison,
and finally hierarchical relations would make
Given the substantive body of evidence sup-
developmental sense. Specifically, coordination
porting the core concepts of RFT (e.g., Barnes
relations appear to emerge first because they
& Keenan, 1993; Barnes-Holmes, Barnes-
form the basis of the other relations (Luciano,
Holmes, Smeets, Cullinan, & Leader, 2004;
Gómez Becerra, & Rodríguez Valverde, 2007).
Hayes, Barnes-Holmes, & Roche, 2001), and
Distinction relations may emerge thereafter be-
growing evidence of the theory’s applied utility
cause these form the basis of opposition and
in establishing verbal behavior (e.g., Barnes-
comparison relations. For example, one must
Holmes, Barnes-Holmes, Smeets, Strand, &
discriminate that two stimuli are different in
Friman, 2004; Berens & Hayes, 2007; Dunne,
order to discriminate that they are opposite. It
Foody, Barnes-Holmes, Barnes-Holmes, &
would seem logical to assume that opposition
Murphy, 2014; O’Connor, Rafferty, Barnes-
relations emerge before comparison relations,
Holmes, & Barnes-Holmes, 2009), calls have
because opposition relations appear more simi-
been made to incorporate RFT-based protocols
lar in nature to distinction relations than com-
into traditional Early Intensive Behavioral In-
parison relations. Furthermore, opposition rela-
tervention (EIBI) programs, especially for chil-
tions would appear to be less complex than
dren with autism (e.g., Lerman et al., 2005;
comparison relations. In developing a training
Luciano et al., 2009; Moore, 2009; Rehfeldt,
sequence for relational responding, Rehfeldt
2011). RFT proposes that derived relational re-
and Barnes-Holmes (2009) suggested that the
sponding is the root of complex verbal ability
establishment of each relational frame poten-
and the basis of much generalized behavior—
tially renders the next easier to acquire, because
skills often deficient in children with autism.
all relational frames share the same properties
Thus, Barnes-Holmes, Barnes-Holmes, Roche,
of generalized operant behavior (Hayes, Fox, et
and Smeets (2001) recommended that the core
al., 2001).
relational frames should be targeted directly and
In one of the few relevant studies, Cassidy,
trained to high levels of flexibility, as part of
Roche, and Hayes (2011) used multiple exem-
remedial training of verbal behavior.
plar training to establish coordination, opposi-
tion, and comparison relations (see also
The Training Context for Establishing Cassidy, 2008). In Study 1, four typically de-
Relational Repertoires veloping children, between the ages of 8 and 12
years old, were presented with a training se-
There is some evidence to support the effi- quence that targeted coordination, opposition,
cacy of incorporating relational training into and then comparison relations. Both an experi-
early intervention for children with develop- mental and a control group were exposed to
mental disabilities (e.g., Murphy & Barnes- equivalence testing and training that comprised
Holmes, 2006, 2009; Rosales & Rehfeldt, conditional discrimination training, and tests for
2007). For example, Murphy, Barnes-Holmes, symmetry and transitivity. The experimental
and Barnes-Holmes (2005) developed proce- participants were exposed to four additional
dures for establishing generative manding in phases of multiple exemplar training for equiv-
96 KENT ET AL.

alence, coordination, opposition, and compari- The current research sought to assess and
son relations. The results indicated statistically facilitate/establish relational responding in chil-
significant improvements on the WISC, relative dren with autism. We attempted to explore the
to the control group. In Study 2, eight children relative benefits of manipulating the sequence
with educational and behavioral difficulties of testing and training of the core repertoires of
were presented with an alternate training se- relational responding, and to loosely compare
quence utilizing multiple exemplar training in- these skills between children with autism and
volving coordination, comparison, and then op- typically developing children. Specifically,
posite relations. Again, most of the participants Study 1 tested relational responding in five typ-
showed significant improvements on the WISC. ically developing children in the following se-
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

In a more recent study, Dunne et al. (2014) quence: coordination, distinction, comparison,
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

sought to establish various repertoires of rela- and opposition relations. Study 2 replicated
tional responding in children with autism who Study 1 with 11 children with autism and at-
showed significant deficits in these skills. The tempted to remediate the various relational re-
researchers began by testing and training the sponding deficits identified during the testing
targeted relational frame in nonarbitrary form procedure. Study 3 replicated Study 2 with a
before proceeding to testing and training the further four children with autism, except the
arbitrary form. In Study 1, all nine children with order of the opposition and comparison rela-
autism successfully acquired coordination rela- tions was altered during testing and training, in
tions, although the amount of training required order to determine any impact this might have
varied from 320 to 875 training trials. Interest- on learning outcomes.
ingly, higher scores on the VB-MAPP were
related to less training. In Study 2, four of the Study 1
same children successfully acquired opposition
relations, but again the amount of training re- Method
quired varied from 10 to 340 training trials.
Participants. Five children; three female
Again, higher VB-MAPP scores were related to
and two male with an age range from 4 years
less training. In Study 3, two of the same chil-
and 1 month to 8 years and 9 months (mean age
dren successfully acquired distinction relations, 6 years and 10 months), participated in Study 1.
but again the amount of training required varied All were typically developing and enrolled full-
with one participant passing all stages with no time in a mainstream school. Results of the
training and the other requiring 240 training Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test (PPVT) indi-
trials. Finally in Study 4, the same two children cated that one participant (P3) was categorized
successfully acquired comparison relations, but as low average in receptive verbal ability (i.e.,
again the amount of training varied from 168 to scoring 85–100), two participants (Ps 2 and 4)
600 training trials. were categorized as high average (scoring 100 –
The training sequence implemented for par- 115), and two (Ps 1 and 5) were moderately
ticipants involved establishing coordination, op- high (scoring 115–140) at baseline. According
position, distinction, and comparison relations to the Kaufman Brief Intelligence Test (K-BIT),
in that order, at least for two of the children with three participants (Ps 1, 3, and 5) were catego-
autism. The researchers and the data suggested rized as average in expressive verbal ability
that for some children, but clearly not for all, (scoring 90 –109) and two (Ps 2 and 4) were
training of the initial relations may have facili- above average (scoring 110 –119) at baseline.
tated the acquisition of relations trained subse- Setting. Each session was conducted in the
quently. Taken together, the two sets of studies same quiet classroom within each participant’s
above highlight training sequences through educational setting. Each child participated in-
which coordination, distinction, opposition, and dividually, accompanied only by the researcher.
comparison relations can be successfully estab- During all trials, the researcher was seated be-
lished/facilitated in typically developing and side the participant at a small table. Sessions
some autistic children. However, there have were conducted once a week, with each partic-
been no studies in which this has been explored ipant receiving a total of 24 sessions. The max-
more directly. imum duration of a session was 20 min.
RELATIONAL RESPONDING: TESTING, TRAINING 97

Materials. The materials employed in grammed consequences for correct or incorrect


Study 1 comprised two printed standardized responding. A correct response in each of these
psychometric measures and a printed protocol required the participant to emit the appropriate
for testing relational responding (see below). nonverbal or verbal response within 5 seconds
Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test (PPVT; of the instruction. Hence, an incorrect response
Dunn & Dunn, 1997). The PPVT was used to was one that did not correspond to the correct
assess receptive verbal ability. For instance, answer or that occurred after a delay of 5 sec-
participants were shown a page of four pictures onds. Although these were test trials, specific
(e.g., a baby, a car, a fish, and candies) and contingencies were in place for various forms of
asked “Put your finger on the picture that shows on-task behavior and these delivered either ver-
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

the baby.” In scoring the PPVT, participant raw bal praise (e.g., “Nice listening” or “You’re
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

scores are calculated and converted into age- doing really good work”). If participants failed
based standard scores. The minimum standard one of the tests for a target relational frame, it
score is 20, the maximum is 160, and the mean was intended that they would then be presented
is 100, with a standard deviation of 15. The with the same trials in a training format, during
PPVT provides descriptive categories based on which positive reinforcement in the form of
standard scores: 20 –70 ⫽ extremely low; 70 – tangibles and corrective feedback would be pro-
85 ⫽ moderately low; 85–100 ⫽ low average; vided on each trial. A range of items had been
100 –115 ⫽ high average; 115–140 ⫽ moder- identified as tangible reinforcement (e.g., access
ately high; and 140 –160 ⫽ extremely high. The to an iPad, toys, stickers). However, it is worth
current research analyzed age-based standard noting at this point that none of the five children
scores. who participated in Study 1 failed any of the
Kaufman Brief Intelligence Test (K-BIT; relational tests presented, hence explicit training
Kaufman & Kaufman, 2004). The K-BIT of the target relations was not required at any
was used to assess expressive verbal ability. For point.
example, participants were shown a picture of a Procedure. The current study comprised
bed and asked “What is this?” Three possible five stages, some with a number of phases (see
outcomes are generated by scoring the K-BIT: a below). These included administration of the
verbal composite based on the total score of the PPVT and K-BIT in Stage 1. Stages 2–5 in-
vocabulary subtest; a nonverbal composite volved the relational responding test protocol
based on the matrices subtest; and an IQ com- presented as coordination, distinction, compar-
posite (based on a summary of the two subtest ison, and opposition relations (in that order). All
composites). The minimum IQ composite score of the target relational performances were first
is 40, the maximum is 160, and the mean is 100, tested as nonarbitrary relations followed by ar-
with a standard deviation of 15. The KBIT bitrary relations, before proceeding to the next
provides the following descriptive categories relational frame.
for the range of IQ composite scores: ⬍69 ⫽ Stage 1: Baseline of standardized measures
below the lower extreme; 70 –79 ⫽ well below of verbal ability. All participants were admin-
average; 80 – 89 ⫽ below average; 90 –109 ⫽ istered the PPVT first followed by the K-BIT, as
average; and 110 –119 ⫽ above average. The measures of their baseline receptive and expres-
current research analyzed IQ composite scores. sive verbal abilities, respectively.
Relational responding test protocol. The Stage 2: Coordination relations. There
sequence of testing relational responding fol- were two phases in the testing of coordination
lowed in the current study is similar to that relations. Phase 1 targeted nonarbitrary coordi-
reported by Dunne et al. (2014). In short, this nation relations, while Phase 2 targeted arbi-
sequence targeted four relational frames, each trary relations. A series of 2 ⫻ 4 in. laminated
presented as both nonarbitrary and thereafter color cards and a similar series of picture cards
arbitrary trials in the following order: coordina- were employed. For nonarbitrary relations,
tion; distinction, comparison, and opposition re- there was a total of 28 color cards: two dupli-
lations. cates of 14 different colors (e.g., Set 1: blue and
Programmed consequences. All K-BIT, red, Set 2: yellow and green, etc.); as well as 60
PPVT, and relational responding trials were first picture cards: three duplicates of 20 different
presented as a test and there were no pro- cards that presented a picture of a common item
98 KENT ET AL.

(e.g., a tractor, a car, a dog, a cat, a house, etc.). as sample, and 10 presented the house. The fifth
For arbitrary relations, the same 60 picture test followed with a car or dog card as samples
cards were employed (excluding any used pre- and comparisons to ascertain whether the rela-
viously). tions could be derived on a novel picture set.
Phase 1: Nonarbitrary coordination relations. The sixth subphase comprised a generalization
There were six subphases to the assessment/ test involving multiple novel picture sets to
establishment of nonarbitrary coordination rela- ascertain generalization of the relational perfor-
tions, three involving colored cards and three mances across sets. Each test trial presented a
thereafter involving picture cards. This exten- new picture set.
sive experimental sequence was designed to Phase 2: Arbitrary coordination relations.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

include training where weak test performances Arbitrary coordination trials always involved
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

were observed and to thereafter provide ade- four identical picture cards (e.g., four pictures
quate testing of the derived relations on novel of a bus). These were laid out initially as one
stimuli that had not been involved in training. sample above and two comparisons below. The
However, as noted above, explicit training was researcher pointed to one of the comparisons
never required. and said “This one is the same.” She then
The first phase began with a 20-trial test handed the sample to the participant and in-
using only one (colored) stimulus set. The pass structed: “Match with same,” which required
criterion was always 80% correct (unless spec- the participant to place the sample on top of the
ified otherwise). All trials involved a blue or red comparison that had been designated as “same.”
colored card as the sample and two comparison The first arbitrary coordination test involved 20
stimuli that were also blue and red, one of trials, with the same set (i.e., three pictures of a
which was identical to the sample. Ten trials bus). A second test followed with a novel pic-
presented the blue sample, while 10 presented ture set (i.e., three pictures of a tree) to ascertain
the red sample. On each trial, the researcher said whether these relations could be derived on a
“Match same” and participants were required to novel picture set. The third test was a general-
place the sample on top of the correct (same) ization test with multiple novel picture stimulus
comparison (e.g., blue– blue). A correct (iden- sets to ascertain generalization of the relational
tity matching) response required a stimulus performances across sets.
match. The designated comparison and its loca- Stage 3: Distinction relations. There were
tion on the left or right were always random- four phases in Stage 3 that included testing
ized. distinction relations and combining distinction
A second nonarbitrary coordination test fol- relations with the coordination relations test
lowed with a novel color set—a yellow or green from Stage 2. Specifically, Phase 1 targeted
card as sample and two comparisons that were nonarbitrary distinction relations; Phase 2 tar-
also yellow and green. Ten trials presented the geted arbitrary distinction relations; Phase 3
yellow sample, 10 trials presented the green. targeted nonarbitrary mixed coordination and
This ascertained whether the nonarbitrary coor- distinction relations; and Phase 4 targeted arbi-
dination relations could be derived on a novel trary mixed coordination and distinction rela-
set. The third test was a generalization test tions.
involving multiple novel color sets to ascertain Phase 1: Nonarbitrary distinction relations.
generalization of the relational performances There were six subphases to the assessment of
across stimuli. A new color set was presented these relations, three involving colored cards
on each trial. and three thereafter involving picture cards.
The three subphases above (training stimulus This phase began with a 20-trial test using only
set, novel set, then multiple novel sets) were one (colored) stimulus set. All trials involved
then repeated, but picture sets were now em- two nonidentical color cards (one red, the other
ployed to ascertain whether the relations could blue) presented as comparisons and a sample
be derived on a more complex stimulus set than that was either red or blue. Each participant was
basic colors. That is, the fourth test involved a instructed to “Match different” and required to
single picture set—a tractor or a house as sam- place the sample on top of the correct (different)
ple and two comparisons that were also tractor comparison. Ten trials presented the blue sam-
and house. Again, 10 trials presented the tractor ple, 10 presented the red.
RELATIONAL RESPONDING: TESTING, TRAINING 99

A second test followed with a novel color set senting a novel color set on each trial. The
(yellow and green) to ascertain whether the fourth test presented a randomized series of 10
nonarbitrary distinction relations could be de- nonarbitrary coordination trials and 10 distinc-
rived on a novel color set. The third test was a tion trials using a picture set, followed by a fifth
generalization test with multiple novel color test with a novel picture set, and a sixth test with
stimulus sets to ascertain generalization of the a novel picture set presented on each trial.
relational performances across sets. Phase 4: Arbitrary mixed coordination and
The three subphases above (training stimulus distinction relations. Phase 4 involved a sin-
set, novel set, then multiple novel sets) were gle 12-trial test of arbitrary coordination, dis-
then repeated, but picture sets were now em- tinction, and mixed coordination/distinction re-
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

ployed to ascertain whether the relations could lations. It is important to note that each trial
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

be derived on a more complex stimulus set than contained all three of these elements. To test
basic colors. That is, the fourth test involved a arbitrary coordination relations, each participant
single picture set—a tractor or a house as sam- was presented with a sample (e.g., picture of a
ple and two comparisons that were also tractor dog) and two identical comparisons (e.g., two
and house. Again, 10 trials presented the tractor identical pictures of a dog). Pointing to one
as sample, and 10 presented the house. A fifth comparison, the researcher said “This one is the
test followed with a car or dog card as samples same.” Pointing to the other comparison, the
and comparisons to ascertain whether the rela- researcher then said “And this one is different,”
tions could be derived on a novel picture set. followed by the request to “Match same.”
The sixth subphase comprised a generalization The second element of the trial targeted ar-
test involving multiple novel picture sets to bitrary distinction relations. Using the same
ascertain generalization of the relational perfor- stimulus arrangements, the researcher pointed
mances across sets. to one comparison and said “This one is the
Phase 2: Arbitrary distinction relations.
same.” She then pointed to the other compari-
There were three arbitrary distinction tests. The
son, said “And this one is different,” and asked
first presented three identical pictures of a
“Match different.”
house, as one sample and two as comparisons.
The third element of the trial targeted mixed
On each trial, the researcher pointed to one
comparison and said “This one is different.” relations. The researcher pointed to one com-
She then handed the sample to participants and parison and said “This one is the same,” she
instructed “Match different” and participants then pointed to the other comparison and said
were required to place the sample on top of the “This one is different,” and then asked “Are
correct (“different”) comparison. A second test they the same or different?” The pass criterion
followed with a novel picture set (i.e., three was 11/12 correct responses.
pictures of a tree) to ascertain whether these Stage 4: Comparison relations. Again,
relations could be derived on a novel picture set. there were two phases in the testing of compar-
The third test was a generalization test with ison relations—Phase 1 for nonarbitrary rela-
multiple novel picture stimulus sets to ascertain tions and Phase 2 for arbitrary relations. A
generalization of the relational performances series of 2 ⫻ 4 in. laminated cards was em-
across sets. ployed for this. For nonarbitrary relations, there
Phase 3: Nonarbitrary mixed coordination was a total of six cards that comprised two
and distinction relations. Phase 3 involved an stimulus sets (i.e., three cards depicting brass
amalgamation of the nonarbitrary coordination coins; one with one coin, one with two coins,
and distinction trials from Stages 2 and 3 across and one with three coins, as well as three cards
six tests. The first 20-trial test presented a ran- depicting silver coins; again, one with one coin,
domized series of 10 nonarbitrary coordination one with two coins, and one with three coins).
trials (matching blue with blue and red with red) For experimental purposes, alphanumeric labels
and 10 distinction trials (matching blue with red were used to refer to the coin cards. Specifi-
and red with blue). The pass criterion was 80% cally, for nonarbitrary trials, the one-coin cards
correct with no more than two errors on the were always denoted as the A stimuli, the two-
same relation. The second test involved a novel coin cards as B, and the three-coin cards as C.
color set, with the third generalization test pre- For arbitrary relations, there were two sets of
100 KENT ET AL.

three identical cards (i.e., each depicting one (pointing to B)?” In the second combinatorial
brass/silver coin) denoted as A, B, and C. entailment trial, the researcher pointed to A, for
Phase 1: Nonarbitrary comparison relations. example, and asked “Is this more or less than
There were two tests (36 trials per test) of this (pointing to C)?” In short, nonarbitrary
nonarbitrary comparison relations. These trials comparison test trials involved testing each of
always involved six different trial-types de- the six trial types (each with four mutual entail-
noted for experimental purposes using alphanu- ment trials and two combinatorial entailment
meric labels as follows: A ⬍ B ⬍ C; A ⬍ C ⬎ trials) with the researcher pointing from left to
B; B ⬎ A ⬎ C; B ⬍ C⬎ A; C ⬎ A ⬍ B; C ⬎ right during the first three trials and then point-
B ⬎ A. Each trial-type contained six elements. ing from right to left for the remaining three
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Four of these were mutual entailment trials that trials for each of the six trial types. The test
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

targeted the relations between two stimuli (i.e., sequence was then repeated with a novel picture
A–B; B–C; C–B; and B–A). The remaining two set of silver coins.
trials targeted combinatorial entailment in Phase 2: Arbitrary comparison relations.
which relations among the three A, B, and C There were two tests (24 trials per test) of
stimuli were assessed (i.e., A–C, C–A). This arbitrary comparison relations. Testing arbitrary
generated a total of 36 test trials; 24 mutual comparison relations involved three identical
entailment and 12 combinatorial entailment. cards of brass coins. The test of arbitrary com-
Nonarbitrary comparison trials involved the parison trials comprised four trial types as fol-
presentation of three nonidentical cards of brass lows: A ⬍ B ⬍ C; A ⬎ B ⬎ C; C ⬍ B ⬍ A, and
coins (i.e., A–B–C). On the first mutual entail- C ⬎ B ⬎ A. Again there were six trials per trial
ment trial, the participant was instructed, for type; four mutual entailment trials, and two
example, that A was less than B and B was less combinatorial entailment trials. This generated
than C (i.e., A ⬍ B ⬍ C). Pointing to B, the a total of 24 test trials; 16 mutually entailed
researcher then asked “Is this more or less than relations and eight combinatorially entailed re-
this (pointing to A)?” Responding “More”, for lations. Across all arbitrary trials, the stimuli
example, was recorded as correct. The second targeted were randomized to ensure the partic-
mutual entailment trial involved the researcher ipants were not responding based on spatial
pointing to C and asking “Is this more or less position.
than this (pointing to B)?” In the mutual entail- On the first mutual entailment trial, the par-
ment trials, we included the broader question, ticipant was instructed, for example, that A was
for example “Is this (B) more or less than this less than B and B was less than C (i.e., A ⬍ B ⬍
(A)?” rather than simply asking “Is this (A) less C). Pointing to both A and B, the researcher
than this (B)?”, which would have involved then asked “Which of these is more?” The sec-
repeating part of the initial instruction “A is less ond mutual entailment trial involved the re-
than B and B is less than C.” Hence, when B searcher pointing to both B and C, for example,
was pointed to, the target mutually entailed re- and asking “Which of these is more?” In the
lation is actually B ⬎ A (rather than A ⬍ B), first combinatorial entailment trial, the re-
but the derivations among the three stimuli are searcher pointed to both A and C and asked
ultimately the same. This broader question cir- “Which of these is more?” On the third mutual
cumvented repetitions and ruled out other pos- entailment trial, the researcher pointed to both
sible sources of control. A and B, and asked “Which of these is less?”
In the following combinatorial entailment The fourth mutual entailment trial involved the
trial, the researcher pointed to C, for example, researcher pointing to both B and C, for exam-
and asked “Is this more or less than this (point- ple, and asking “Which of these is less?” In the
ing to A)?” On the third mutual entailment trial, second combinatorial entailment trial, the re-
the participant was then instructed that C was searcher pointed to both A and C and asked
more than B and B was more than A (as the “Which of these is less?” Participants who
researcher pointed to C first). Then, pointing to passed the first test were retested on a novel
B, the researcher asked “Is this more or less picture set of silver coins. It is important to note
than this (pointing to C)?” The fourth mutual that the spatial locations of the stimuli were
entailment trial involved the researcher pointing always fixed in a manner that was consistent
to A and asking “Is this more or less than this with the trial type. For example, if the trial-type
RELATIONAL RESPONDING: TESTING, TRAINING 101

was A ⬍ B ⬍ C, A was on the left, B in the mutual entailment trial involved the researcher
center, and C on the right. However, to ensure pointing to C and asking “Is this big or small?”
that correct responding was not influenced by The combinatorial entailment trial involved the
stimulus location, no reference was made to researcher pointing to C and asking “Is this the
location. The research simply pointed to the two opposite of this (pointing to A)?” “No” was a
target stimuli and asked, for which is more or correct response. This three-step procedure was
less. repeated for each of the four trial-types. The full
Stage 5: Opposition relations. There were tests sequence was then repeated with a novel
two phases in the testing of opposition rela- picture set of identical brass coin pictures.
tions—Phase 1 for nonarbitrary relations and Phase 2: Arbitrary opposition relations.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Phase 2 for arbitrary relations. A series of 2 ⫻ There were two tests (12 trials per test) of
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

4 in. laminated cards were employed. For non- arbitrary opposition relations. These trials in-
arbitrary relations, there was a total of eight volved the presentation of three identical cards
different cards that comprised two stimulus sets of big footballs (A, B, and C). Testing involved
(i.e., four cards depicted footballs; two with a four trial-types denoted as follows: A opposite
small football and two with a big football, while B opposite C left to right; A opposite B opposite
four cards depicted brass coins; two with one C right to left; B opposite A opposite C left to
coin and two with three coins). For experimen- right; and B opposite A opposite C right to left.
tal purposes, alphanumeric labels were used to Again, each trial-type contained three elements,
refer to the cards. Specifically, for nonarbitrary two mutual entailment trials and one combina-
trials, the two identical cards were always de- torial entailment trial. This generated a total of
noted as A and C, while the nonidentical card 12 test trials; eight mutual entailment and four
was always denoted as B. For arbitrary rela- combinatorial entailment trials. Across all arbi-
tions, there were two sets of three identical trary trials, the designated stimuli were random-
cards (i.e., all depicted a big football or three ized to ensure that the participants were not
brass coins) denoted as A, B, and C. responding based on spatial position.
Phase 1: Nonarbitrary opposition relations. The first mutual entailment trial involved the
There were two tests (12 trials per test) of participant being instructed, for example, that A
nonarbitrary opposition relations. Testing these was big and then being asked to imagine that it
relations involved four trial types denoted as was opposite to B, and that B was opposite to C
follows: A opposite B opposite C (with the (i.e., A opposite B opposite C). Pointing to B,
researcher pointing from left to right); A oppo- the researcher then asked “Is this big or small?”
site B opposite C (right to left); C opposite B The second mutual entailment trial involved the
opposite A (left to right); and C opposite B researcher pointing to C and asking “Is this big
opposite A (right to left). Again, each trial type or small?” In the combinatorial entailment trial,
contained three elements, two mutual entail- the researcher pointed to C and asked “Is this
ment trials and one combinatorial entailment the opposite of this (pointing to A)?” The full
trial. This generated a total of 12 test trials; test sequence was then repeated with a novel
eight mutual entailment and four combinatorial picture set of identical brass coins.
entailment trials, with an accuracy criterion of
80%. Results and Discussion
Nonarbitrary opposition relations involved
two nonidentical cards (e.g., one with a small The primary aim of Study 1 was to examine
football and the other with a big football; de- the emergence of the target patterns of relational
noted as A and B, respectively) presented as responding for the five typically developing
comparisons, with a third sample C that was children. All participants passed all stages of
always identical to comparison A. On the first testing on the first exposure, see Table 1, sug-
mutual entailment trial, the participant was in- gesting that these skills were already in each
structed, for example, that A was big and that it participant’s repertoire. These performances
was opposite to B, and that B was opposite to C provide support for RFT’s suggestion that re-
(i.e., A opposite B opposite C). Pointing to B, sponding in accordance with arbitrary coordina-
the researcher then asked “Is this big or small?” tion, distinction, opposition, and comparison re-
“Small” was recorded as correct. The second lations is established in typically developing
102 KENT ET AL.

Table 1
Total Number of Correct Responses out of Total Number of Test Trials (in Brackets) by Each Participant
Across Nonarbitrary (NA) and Arbitrary (A) Trials for Each Relational Frame in Study 1
Mixed
coordination and
Verbal scores Coordination Distinction distinction Comparison Opposition
P PPVT K-BIT NA (120) A (60) NA (120) A (60) NA (120) A (12) NA (72) A (48) NA (24) A (12)
1 120 106 120 60 120 60 120 12 72 48 24 12
2 105 115 120 60 120 60 120 12 72 48 24 12
3 85 90 120 60 120 60 120 12 72 48 24 12
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

4 112 112 120 60 120 60 120 12 72 48 24 12


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

5 116 101 120 60 120 60 120 12 72 48 24 12


Note. PPVT ⫽ Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test; K-BIT ⫽ Kaufman Brief Intelligence Test.

children between the ages of 4 and 8 years old followed by a test. If participants passed this
(Luciano et al., 2009). test, they proceeded to the next stage of testing.
These results also raise the question of how If they did not pass, they returned to training
children with lower levels of verbal ability until they passed this set of test trials. Across all
would perform on the same tests? Study 2 ad- training and testing, the same materials were
dressed this question by involving children with employed.
impairments in verbal ability relative to the
sample in Study 1. Results and Discussion

The primary aim of Study 2 was to examine


Study 2
competencies on the target relational repertoires
Method in participants with verbal abilities lower than
typically developing counterparts. The results
Participants, setting, and materials. A to- showed considerable variation across partici-
tal of 11 children, all males, participated in pants in competencies on the target relations
Study 2. Their ages ranged between 4 years 2 and the various levels of training required. Ta-
months and 13 years 6 months (mean age 8 ble 2 presents the results of each participant’s
years and 10 months). All had been indepen- performances on both nonarbitrary and arbitrary
dently diagnosed with autism and attended full- trials of the four types of relations targeted.
time at a special needs school. All aspects of the Please note that the second figure presented for
setting and materials were identical to Study 1. any test is the number of training trials required
Procedure. All aspects of the experimental before passing a second exposure to the test.
sequence were identical to Study 1, except that Participant 1. P1 scored as moderately low
all participants were provided with explicit (81) on the PPVT and average (101) on the
training on any relational tests which they failed K-BIT. He demonstrated both types of coordi-
to pass. This training was conducted on each nation relations and distinction relations imme-
relational frame prior to testing the next frame. diately, as well as passing the mixed tests. He
Across all relations, training trials were identi- failed for the first time on nonarbitrary compar-
cal to test trials, except that corrective feedback ison relations (53/72), with errors on both mu-
was delivered contingently upon responding. tual and combinatorial entailment. He required
That is, correct responding was followed by 72 training trials, all on mutual entailment, to
reinforcement, while incorrect responding was pass the full test. He subsequently passed the
not reinforced. Where a participant was not arbitrary comparison test without training. He
successful in learning from this contingency, also failed the nonarbitrary opposition test,
additional prompting was used according to the again with errors on both mutual and combina-
principle of least to most guidance. Training torial entailment (12/24). He required 24 train-
blocks consisted of the same number of trials as ing trials to pass, and then passed the arbitrary
test blocks. Each block of training trials was opposition test without training. Overall, this
RELATIONAL RESPONDING: TESTING, TRAINING 103

Table 2
Total Number of Correct Responses out of Total Number of Test Trials (in Brackets) and Total Number of
Training Trials by Each Participant Across Nonarbitrary (NA) and Arbitrary (A) Trials for Each
Relational Frame
Mixed
coordination and
Verbal scores Coordination Distinction distinction Comparison Opposition
P PPVT K-BIT NA (120) A (60) NA (120) A (60) NA (120) A (12) NA (72) A (48) NA (24) A (12)
1 81 101 120 60 120 60 120 12 53 48 12 12
72 24
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

2 89 83 120 60 120 60 120 0 43 30 11 12


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

24 216 12 24
3 88 96 120 60 120 60 120 0 41 48 14 0
12 288 96 48
4 88 86 100 60 60 60 120 0 72 48 12 12
80 60 12 12 72
5 40 20 100 80 23
40 60 640ⴱⴱ
6 40 20 120 80 22
780 1040ⴱⴱ

7 20 120 70 0
520 160ⴱⴱ
8 40 20 120 18
880ⴱⴱ

9 20 120 27
260ⴱⴱ

10 20 120 46
380ⴱⴱ

11 20 120 82 60 50 10ⴱⴱ
240 840
Note. PPVT ⫽ Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test; K-BIT ⫽ Kaufman Brief Intelligence Test. Table presents participants’
performances in relational testing as the figures in the top line per participant, with number of training trials needed to meet
criterion presented below these.

Indicates scores were indeterminate. ⴱⴱ Indicates criteria not met.

participant required minimal training (96 trials all, P2 required more training (276 trials in
in total) only on nonarbitrary comparison and total) than P1, on the arbitrary mixed, nonarbi-
opposition relations which seemed to facilitate trary comparison, arbitrary comparison, and
responding on arbitrary trials thereafter. nonarbitrary opposition relations.
Participant 2. P2 scored as low average Participant 3. P3 scored as low average
(89) on the PPVT and below average (83) on the (88) on the PPVT and average (96) on the
K-BIT. He demonstrated both types of coordi- K-BIT. He passed both types of coordination
nation relations and distinction relations imme- and distinction relations, as well as the nonar-
diately. He also passed the nonarbitrary mixed bitrary mixed test. He failed the arbitrary mixed
tests, but scored 0 on the arbitrary mixed test, test with 0 correct responses, but required only
requiring 24 training trials to pass. He failed the 12 training trials to pass. He failed the nonarbi-
nonarbitrary comparison test (43/72) and re- trary comparison test (41/72) and passed only
quired 216 training trials to pass. He also failed after 288 training trials, although he then passed
the arbitrary comparison test (30/48), but re- the arbitrary comparison test immediately. He
quired only 12 training trials to pass. He failed failed the nonarbitrary opposition test (14/24)
the nonarbitrary opposition test (11/24) with and passed after 96 training trials. He also pro-
errors on combinatorial entailment and needed duced 0 correct responses on the arbitrary op-
24 training trials to pass. He then passed the position test and required 48 training trials to
arbitrary opposition test without training. Over- pass. Overall, P3 required considerable training
104 KENT ET AL.

(444 trials in total) on mixed relations, nonar- difficulties during its delivery. Hence, he pro-
bitrary (288 trials in total) and arbitrary com- ceeded immediately to the nonarbitrary distinc-
parison, and nonarbitrary and arbitrary opposi- tion test (i.e., no test of arbitrary coordination
tion relations. relations was conducted). He failed this test
Participant 4. P4 scored as low average (80/120) and required 780 training trials to pass
(88) on the PPVT and below average (86) on the (400 using color stimuli and 380 using picture
K-BIT. He immediately failed the nonarbitrary stimuli). Given this level of required training, he
coordination test (100/120) and required 80 proceeded immediately to the nonarbitrary
training trials to pass (i.e., 40 on first color set, mixed test (i.e., there was no arbitrary distinc-
and 40 again after the second color set was tion test) but failed (22/120). Participation was
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

introduced). He passed the arbitrary coordina- terminated after 1,040 training trials and little
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

tion test immediately, but failed the nonarbi- improvement. Overall, P6 required very exten-
trary distinction test (60/120), and required 60 sive training (1,820 trials in total) on nonarbi-
training trials to pass. He passed the arbitrary trary coordination, nonarbitrary distinction,
distinction test immediately, as well as the non- and these nonarbitrary relations mixed. How-
arbitrary mixed test. However, he then scored 0 ever, there was not adequate flexibility on these
correct responses on the arbitrary mixed test, relations to pass the mixed test nor to proceed to
which he subsequently passed after only 12 testing these relations in arbitrary form or be-
training trials. He passed both types of compar- yond.
ison test immediately, but failed the nonarbi- Participant 7. P7’s score was indetermin-
trary opposition test (12/24), and required 72 able on the PPVT due to an extremely low level
training trials to pass. He then passed the arbi- of responding and he also scored below the
trary opposition test. Overall, P4 required mod- lower extreme (20) on the K-BIT. He passed the
est training (224 trials in total) on nonarbitrary nonarbitrary coordination test immediately, but
coordination, nonarbitrary distinction, arbitrary found the trials aversive. Hence, he proceeded
mixed, and nonarbitrary and arbitrary opposi- directly to the nonarbitrary distinction test (no
tion relations. arbitrary coordination test was conducted), but
Participant 5. P5 scored as extremely low failed (70/120), and eventually passed after 520
(40) on the PPVT and below the lower extreme training trials (260 using color stimuli and 260
(20) on the K-BIT. He failed the nonarbitrary using picture stimuli). Given the level of train-
coordination test (100/120), but passed all tests ing required, he proceeded immediately to the
after 40 training trials on the first color set. nonarbitrary mixed test (no test of arbitrary
However, the delivery of these trials was aver- distinction was conducted), but produced 0 cor-
sive to the participant, hence he proceeded im- rect responses. Participation was terminated af-
mediately to the nonarbitrary distinction test ter 160 training trials and little improvement.
(i.e., no test of arbitrary coordination was con- Overall, P7 required considerable training (560
ducted), but failed (80/120), and required 60 trials in total) on nonarbitrary coordination,
training trials to pass. Again, given difficulties nonarbitrary distinction, and the mixed test of
encountered in training, P5 was then exposed to these relations. However, there was not ade-
the nonarbitrary mixed test and produced a poor quate flexibility on these relations to pass the
performance (23/120). Participation was termi- mixed test nor to proceed to testing these rela-
nated after 640 training trials and little improve- tions in arbitrary form or beyond.
ment. Overall, P5 required considerable training Participant 8. P8 scored as extremely low
(740 trials in total) on nonarbitrary coordina- (40) on the PPVT and below the lower extreme
tion, nonarbitrary distinction, and nonarbitrary (20) on the K-BIT. He passed the nonarbitrary
mixed relations, but could not acquire adequate coordination test, but found it aversive, hence
flexibility on nonarbitrary coordination and dis- proceeding directly to the nonarbitrary distinc-
tinction relations to pass the mixed test or pro- tion test, which he failed (18/120). Participation
ceed beyond this point. was terminated after 880 training trials using
Participant 6. P6 scored as extremely low color stimuli only and little improvement. Over-
(40) on the PPVT and below the lower extreme all, P8 passed only nonarbitrary coordination
(20) on the K-BIT. He passed the nonarbitrary relations, but could not complete training on
coordination test immediately but encountered nonarbitrary distinction relations, in spite of ex-
RELATIONAL RESPONDING: TESTING, TRAINING 105

tensive training, nor could he proceed beyond evidence that this facilitated responding on ar-
this point. bitrary distinction relations, yet he could not
Participant 9. P9’s score was indetermin- proceed beyond arbitrary distinction relations,
able on the PPVT due to an extremely low level in spite of extensive training.
of responding and was below the lower extreme Study 2 involved 11 children with autism.
(20) on the K-BIT. However, he passed the These individuals demonstrated different com-
nonarbitrary coordination test with some diffi- petencies in the various patterns of relational
culties on delivery of trials and thus proceeded responding, and in some cases, but not others,
directly to the nonarbitrary distinction test, these were remediated through explicit training.
which he failed with a very weak performance Four of the 11 participants (Ps 1, 2, 3, and 4)
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

(27/120). Participation was terminated after 260 completed the full test sequence, one (P11)
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

training trials using color stimuli only and little reached arbitrary distinction relations, three (Ps
improvement. Overall, P9 required modest 5, 6, and 7) reached nonarbitrary distinction,
training (260 trials in total) on nonarbitrary co- and three (Ps 8, 9, and 10) reached reached
ordination and nonarbitrary distinction rela- nonarbitrary coordination. There was some sup-
tions. port for the suggestion that higher scores on the
Participant 10. Participant 10’s score was standardized measures was related to less train-
indeterminable on the PPVT due to an ex- ing. Overall, the results provide some evidence
tremely low level of responding and was below for the use of an RFT-based intervention pro-
the lower extreme (20) on the K-BIT. He passed gram, especially the utility of targeting nonar-
the nonarbitrary coordination test on first expo- bitrary relations before arbitrary relations, to
sure of both color and picture stimuli. However, support the development of relational respond-
he failed the nonarbitrary distinction test (46/ ing skills in children with autism.
120). Participation was terminated after 380 One important issue raised by the findings
training trials using color stimuli only and little from Study 2 concerns the potential impact of
improvement. Overall, P10 required modest the sequence of the testing and training. In other
training (380 trials in total) on nonarbitrary co- words, if the order various relations was re-
ordination and nonarbitrary distinction rela- versed, would similar patterns of responding be
tions, but could not complete training on the observed? This issue was addressed in Study 3.
latter, hence he could not proceed to the remain-
ing relations.
Study 3
Participant 11. Participant 11’s score was
indeterminable on the PPVT due to an ex-
Method
tremely low level of responding and was below
the lower extreme (20) on the K-BIT. He passed Participants, setting, and materials. Four
the nonarbitrary coordination test immediately, experimentally naïve children, all males, partic-
but difficulties therein suggested the utility of ipated in Study 3. All were aged between 3
proceeding directly to nonarbitrary distinction years 4 months and 4 years 2 months (mean age
relations. He failed this test (82/120), but even- 4 years). All had been independently diagnosed
tually passed after 240 training trials (mostly on with autism and attended full time at a special
the color stimuli). He was exposed directly to needs school.
the nonarbitrary mixed test, but performed Procedure. All aspects of the experimental
poorly (50/120) and required 840 training trials sequence were identical to Study 2 with the
to pass. Given that he had now passed the exception of the sequence of testing and training
nonarbitrary mixed test, he was exposed to the being rearranged, such that opposition relations
arbitrary distinction test, and passed without were now targeted before comparison relations.
training. He also passed the arbitrary mixed test
without combinatorial entailment trials, but Results
however, failed once these trials were tested
(10/12) and the participant was asked “are these The primary aim of Study 3 was to examine
same/different?” Overall, P11 required exten- the impact of a specific testing and training
sive training (1,060 trials in total), mostly on the sequence on the relational responding perfor-
nonarbitrary mixed relations. There was some mances of four participants with autism. The
106 KENT ET AL.

results showed considerable variation across tests immediately, but failed the nonarbitrary
participants in competencies on the target rela- distinction test (101/120), which he then passed
tions and the levels of training required. Table 3 after only 20 training trials. He passed the arbi-
presents the results of each participant’s perfor- trary distinction and nonarbitrary mixed tests
mances on nonarbitrary and arbitrary trials in immediately, but produced 0 on the arbitrary
each of the target relations. mixed test. However, he required only 12 train-
Participant 1. P1 scored as high average ing trials to pass. He then passed all further tests
(104) on the PPVT and average (97) on the without training. Overall, P3 required very little
K-BIT. She immediately passed both types of training (44 trials in total) on nonarbitrary dis-
coordination and distinction test, as well as the tinction and arbitrary mixed relations before
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

nonarbitrary mixed test, but surprisingly pro- completing the full tests protocol.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

duced 0 on the arbitrary mixed test, although Participant 4. P4 scored as moderately low
only 12 training trials were required. She also (80) on the PPVT and well below average (73)
failed the nonarbitrary opposition test (12/24), on the K-BIT. He immediately passed all tests
but again passed after 12 training trials. She prior to nonarbitrary opposition relations (12/
then passed arbitrary opposition, and both types 24), but passed this after only 12 training trials.
of comparison relations immediately. Overall, He then passed all further tests without training.
P1 required very little training (24 trials in total) Overall, P4 required very little training (12 tri-
on the arbitrary mixed and nonarbitrary oppo- als in total) on nonarbitrary opposition relations
sition relations, but was able to complete the to complete the full tests protocol.
full test protocol.
Participant 2. P2 scored as low average General Discussion
(99) on the PPVT and average (98) on the
K-BIT. He immediately passed all tests prior to The current research comprised three studies
the nonarbitrary opposition test (12/24), which that sought to explore the baseline and estab-
he passed after only 12 training trials. He then lishment of key repertoires of nonarbitrary and
passed all subsequent tests without further train- arbitrary relational responding in typically de-
ing. Overall, P2 required very little training (12 veloping children and children with autism. We
trials in total) only on nonarbitrary opposition also explored the potential relationship between
relations to complete the full test protocol. participants’ expressive and receptive language
Participant 3. P3 scored as high average on standardized measures and their perfor-
(103) on the PPVT and average (90) on the mances on the relational test protocol. In com-
K-BIT. He passed both types of coordination paring Studies 2 and 3, we were also interested

Table 3
Total Number of Correct Responses out of Total Number of Test Trials (in Brackets) and Total Number of
Training Trials by Each Participant Across Nonarbitrary (NA) and Arbitrary (A) Trials for Each
Relational Frame
Mixed
coordination and
Verbal scores Coordination Distinction distinction Opposition Comparison
P PPVT K-BIT NA (120) A (60) NA (120) A (60) NA (120) A (12) NA (24) A (12) NA (72) A (48)
1 104 97 120 60 120 60 120 0 12 12 72 48
12 12
2 99 98 120 60 120 60 120 12 12 12 72 48
12
3 103 90 120 60 101 60 120 0 12 12 72 48
20 12 12
4 80 73 120 60 120 60 120 12 12 12 72 48
Note. PPVT ⫽ Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test; K-BIT ⫽ Kaufman Brief Intelligence Test. Table presents participants’
performances in relational testing as the figures in the top line per participant, with number of training trials needed to meet
criterion presented below these.
RELATIONAL RESPONDING: TESTING, TRAINING 107

in the potentially different outcomes that may sequence alternated the order in which the op-
be associated with altering the sequence in position and comparison tests were presented.
which the relations were tested and/or trained. Relational Frame Theory would suggest that
Study 1 involved five typically developing typically developing children acquire relational
children, aged between 4 and 8 years old, with responding through natural language interac-
expressive language skills from average to tions with caregivers from a young age and that
above average, and receptive language skills this development parallels the emergence of
from low average to moderately high, and tested language (Luciano et al., 2009). The current
the relational repertoires in the order of (nonar- research provides evidence to this effect, with
bitrary and arbitrary) coordination, distinction, typically developing children proceeding
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

comparison, and opposition. Given their ages through the test sequence with no training re-
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

and levels of linguistic competence, it is per- quirements, while participants with autism, par-
haps not surprising that all five children dem- ticularly those with lower receptive and expres-
onstrated competency on all relations, although sive language competencies, demonstrated
it was a little unexpected that none required deficits in many of the relations targeted. This
training at any point. provides support for the suggestion that re-
A very different set of outcomes emerged in sponding in accordance with arbitrary coordina-
Study 2 which presented the same test sequence tion, distinction, opposition, and comparison re-
to 11 children with autism, aged between 4 and lations is established in typically developing
13 years old, with expressive language skills children between the ages of four and eight
from below the lower extreme to average and years old (Luciano et al., 2009). The data, es-
receptive language skills from indeterminable pecially from Study 2, also show the challenges
to average. Only four participants (Ps 1, 2, 3, involved in developing interventions to estab-
and 4) completed the full test sequence, after lish these generative behaviors when they are
deficient. The utilization of interventions based
96 – 444 training trials. The remaining seven fell
on RFT to establish generative behaviors may
considerably short of completing the full test
be of tremendous benefit in EIBI programs (Lu-
protocol. One participant (P11) reached arbi-
ciano et al.).
trary distinction relations, but only after 1,080
Dunne et al. (2014) questioned the optimal
training trials, mostly on the nonarbitrary mixed sequence of training relational responding in
relations. Three participants (Ps 5, 6, and 7) children with autism. Rehfeldt and Barnes-
reached nonarbitrary distinction relations, but Holmes (2009) suggested that comparison rela-
required between 560 and 1,820 training trials tions may be better targeted after opposition
to do so. And three participants (Ps 8, 9, and 10) relations. Given the study designs and the con-
reached only nonarbitrary coordination rela- siderable variations in the sample, it was diffi-
tions, even after 260 – 880 training trials. cult to draw any firm conclusions about the
The outcomes in Study 3 more closely resem- possible role of the testing sequence. However,
bled Study 1 than Study 2, even though all four to aid in this possible comparison, we specifi-
participants had a diagnosis of autism, and some cally selected the data from participants in
clearly presented with limitations in receptive Study 2 with higher verbal scores (i.e., ⬎80 on
and expressive language, relatively speaking. the PPVT and ⬎80 on the KBIT) to generate a
This sample ranged in age from 3 to 4 years old profile of similar participants across studies.
and scored between moderately low and high Analysis of this data indicated that the differ-
average on receptive language skills, and well ences in performances across the studies re-
below average to average on expressive lan- mained. That is, presenting opposition relations
guage skills. All four completed the full test prior to comparison relations may have been
protocol after 44 training trials or less. P1 re- more beneficial in the acquisition of compar-
quired training on arbitrary mixed and nonarbi- ison relations. Evidence to this effect is ex-
trary opposition relations; P2 also on nonarbi- tracted through comparing the extent of train-
trary opposition relations; P3 on nonarbitrary ing required across participants in Study 2
distinction and arbitrary mixed relations; and P4 and Study 3.
on nonarbitrary opposition relations. What also The positive outcomes of the current studies
distinguished Studies 2 and 3 is that the test in implementing a relational responding train-
108 KENT ET AL.

ing sequence are consistent with previous re- stimuli to test arbitrary relations for all frames.
search with regard to coordination relations This created the possibility, for example, that
(Dunne et al., 2014; O’Connor et al., 2009), participants selected a comparison stimulus be-
distinction relations (Dunne et al., 2014), oppo- cause it was the last stimulus identified by the
sition relations (Dunne et al.), and comparison researcher, rather than responding to the verbal
relations (Dunne et al.), also in children with instruction. This methodological weakness is
autism. The findings from the current research hard to circumvent in arbitrary relational train-
also support previous interventions that include ing protocols, and simply highlights the chal-
multiple exemplar training, explicit feedback, lenges faced by behavioral practitioners in es-
and targeting nonarbitrary trials prior to arbi- tablishing highly generative repertoires. It also
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

trary trials (Vitale, Barnes-Holmes, Barnes- highlights the importance of using multiple ex-
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Holmes, & Campbell, 2008). Indeed, the data emplars and generalization tests.
reported in the current paper support previous A similar methodological weakness sur-
research which has found that training in non- rounds the procedure used for testing the trans-
arbitrary relations facilitates responding to arbi- formation of function in opposition, where it
trary relations in children with deficits in this could be argued that the big/small relation may
regard (Barnes-Holmes, Barnes-Holmes, be a form of “same” and “different” relations as
Smeets, Cullinen, & Leader, 2004; Barnes Hol- taught in the arbitrary coordination/distinction
mes, Barnes Holmes, Smeets, Strand, & Fri- procedures. To examine this possibility, we
man, 2004; Gorham, Barnes-Holmes, Barnes- carefully scrutinized the performances of the
Holmes, & Berens, 2009). It may be said that eight participants who reached arbitrary oppo-
the current findings provide some support for sition relations. Of these, six did not require any
the suggestion that in some, but not all, cases training on arbitrary opposition, and the two
the establishment of each relational frame may who did had not received training on arbitrary
provide a basis for the establishment of the next coordination or distinction relations. This does
relational frame, due to the existence of com- not preclude the possibility that competencies
mon features (Hayes et al., 2001). on the earlier relations accounted for apparent
The findings of the current studies provide competence on opposition relations. Only more
support for the very likely relationship between detailed research can decipher what is control-
verbal ability, as assessed on standardized mea- ling behavior in a given training context.
sures, and repertoires of relational responding. Furthermore, the current research did not em-
This relationship has been previously supported ploy baseline relation tests across the entire
by a number of RFT studies (Devany, Hayes, & training sequence. The current authors were op-
Nelson, 1986; Dunne et al., 2014; Lipkens, erating on the assumption of a developmental
Hayes, & Hayes, 1993; Luciano et al., 2007). trend in the acquisition of relational skills there-
The current studies provide support for this fore it was assumed that if a participant required
relationship with evidence that participants with training on one relational frame they would not
higher verbal abilities produced superior perfor- have the skills to respond to subsequent rela-
mances on the relational tasks. This is particu- tional frames. This assumption made by the
larly apparent when the performance of Partic- authors may have limited the potential to draw
ipants 5–11 in Study 2, who had the lowest solid conclusions on the rate of acquisition of
verbal scores, are compared to the performances relational skills among participants.
of other participants in the current studies. Par- Numerous other interpretations of the vari-
ticipants 5–11 in Study 2 demonstrated signifi- ables controlling responding are always possi-
cant deficits in relational responding that im- ble in detailed training protocols of arbitrary
peded their ability to proceed through the relations. While these open up a range of pos-
testing and training sequence, while all other sible explanations, they more importantly high-
participants progressed with little difficulty. light the challenges faced by researchers and
The current study had a number of limitations practitioners in establishing complex generative
which restrict conclusions that may be drawn, repertoires. The current data show that individ-
and which cause us uncertainty as to whether uals, who may initially appear as homogenous
spurious sources of influence may have affected can vary considerably in these critical verbal
the outcomes. For example, we used identical skills and that for some individuals, these verbal
RELATIONAL RESPONDING: TESTING, TRAINING 109

skills are highly deficient and arduous to estab- imental Analysis of Behavior, 46, 243–257. http://
lish. The aim of the current research was to dx.doi.org/10.1901/jeab.1986.46-243
highlight these deficits and shed some light on Dunn, L. M., & Dunn, L. M. (1997). Peabody Picture
how they might be established. However, given Vocabulary Test, Third Edition. Circle Pines, MN:
American Guidance Service.
the current failure to train a number of the
Dunne, S., Foody, M., Barnes-Holmes, Y., Barnes-
children with autism, much more research is Holmes, D., & Murphy, C. (2014). Facilitating
needed to indicate how these critical repertoires repertoires of coordination, opposition distinction,
can be established. and comparison in young children with autism.
Behavioral Development Bulletin, 19, 37– 47.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/h0100576
References
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Gorham, M., Barnes-Holmes, Y., Barnes-Holmes,


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Barnes, D. (1994). Stimulus equivalence and rela- D., & Berens, N. (2009). Derived comparative and
tional frame theory. The Psychological Record, 44, transitive relations in young children with and
91–124. without autism. The Psychological Record, 59,
Barnes, D., & Keenan, M. (1993). A transfer of 221–246.
functions through derived arbitrary and nonarbi- Hayes, S. C., Barnes-Holmes, D., & Roche, B. (Eds.).
trary stimulus relations. Journal of the Experimen- (2001). Relational frame theory: A post- Skinne-
tal Analysis of Behavior, 59, 61– 81. http://dx.doi rian account of human language and cognition.
.org/10.1901/jeab.1993.59-61 New York, NY: Plenum Press.
Barnes-Holmes, D., Barnes-Holmes, Y., Smeets, Hayes, S. C., Fox, E., Gifford, E., Wilson, K.,
P. M., Cullinan, V., & Leader, G. (2004). Rela- Barnes-Holmes, D., & Healy, O. (2001). Derived
tional frame theory and stimulus equivalence: relational responding as learned behaviour. In S. C.
Conceptual and procedural issues. International Hayes, D. Barnes-Holmes, & B. Roche (Eds.),
Journal of Psychology & Psychological Therapy, Relational frame theory: A post-Skinnerian ac-
4, 181–214. count of human language and cognition (pp. 21–
Barnes-Holmes, Y., Barnes-Holmes, D., Roche, B., 50). New York, NY: Plenum Press.
& Smeets, P. M. (2001). Exemplar training and a Kaufman, A. S., & Kaufman, N. L. (2004). Kaufman
derived transformation of function in accordance Brief Intelligence Test, Second Edition. Blooming-
with symmetry. The Psychological Record, 51, ton, MN: Pearson, Inc.
287–308. Lerman, D. C., Parten, M., Addison, L. R., Vorndran,
Barnes-Holmes, Y., Barnes-Holmes, D., Smeets, C. M., Volkert, V. M., & Kodak, T. (2005). A
P. M., Strand, P., & Friman, P. (2004). Establish- methodology for assessing the functions of emerg-
ing relational responding in accordance with more- ing speech in children with developmental disabil-
than and less-than as generalized operant behavior ities. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 38,
in young children. International Journal of Psy- 303–316. http://dx.doi.org/10.1901/jaba.2005
chology & Psychological Therapy, 4, 531–558. .106-04
Barnes-Holmes, Y., McHugh, L., & Barnes-Holmes, Lipkens, G., Hayes, S. C., & Hayes, L. J. (1993).
D. (2004). Perspective taking and theory of mind: Longitudinal study of derived stimulus relations in
A relational frame account. The Behavior Analyst an infant. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of
Today, 5, 15–25. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/ Behavior, 56, 201–239.
h0100133 Luciano, C., Gómez Becerra, I., & Rodríguez
Berens, N. M., & Hayes, S. C. (2007). Arbitrarily Valverde, M. (2007). The role of multiple-
applicable comparative relations: Experimental ev- exemplar training and naming in establishing de-
idence for a relational operant. Journal of Applied rived equivalence in an infant. Journal of the Ex-
Behavior Analysis, 40, 45–71. http://dx.doi.org/10 perimental Analysis of Behavior, 87, 349 –365.
.1901/jaba.2007.7-06 http://dx.doi.org/10.1901/jeab.2007.08-06
Cassidy, S. (2008). Relational frame theory and hu- Luciano, C., Valdivia-Salas, S., Berens, N. M., Ro-
man intelligence: A conceptual and empirical driguez, M., Manas, I., & Ruiz, F. (2009). Acquir-
analysis (Unpublished doctoral thesis). National ing the earliest relational operants: Co-ordination,
University of Ireland, Maynooth. distinction, opposition, comparison, and hierarchy.
Cassidy, S., Roche, B., & Hayes, S. C. (2011). A In R. A. Rehfeldt & Y. Barnes-Holmes (Eds.),
relational frame training intervention to raise In- Derived relational responding: Applications for
telligence Quotients: A pilot study. The Psycho- learners with autism and other developmental dis-
logical Record, 61, 173–198. abilities (pp. 149 –170). Oakland, CA: New Har-
Devany, J. M., Hayes, S. C., & Nelson, R. O. (1986). binger Publications.
Equivalence class formation in language-able and Moore, J. (2009). Some thoughts on the relation
language-disabled children. Journal of the Exper- between derived relational responding and verbal
110 KENT ET AL.

behavior. European Journal of Behavior Analysis, ysis, 1992–2009. Journal of Applied Behavior
10, 31– 47. Analysis, 44, 109 –119. http://dx.doi.org/10.1901/
Murphy, C., & Barnes-Holmes, D. (2006). Derived jaba.2011.44-109
more/less relational mands with four children di- Rehfeldt, R. A., & Barnes-Holmes, Y. (2009). De-
agnosed with autism: Synthesizing Skinner’s Ver- rived relational responding: Applications for
bal Behavior with relational frame theory II. Jour- learners with autism and other developmental dis-
nal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 38, 445– 462. abilities. Oakland, CA: New Harbinger Publica-
Murphy, C., & Barnes-Holmes, D. (2009). Derived tions.
more-less relational mands in children diagnosed Rosales, R., & Rehfeldt, R. A. (2007). Contriving
with autism. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, transitive conditioned establishing operations to
42, 253–268. http://dx.doi.org/10.1901/jaba.2009 establish derived manding skills in adults with
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

.42-253 severe developmental disabilities. Journal of Ap-


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Murphy, C., Barnes-Holmes, D., & Barnes-Holmes, plied Behavior Analysis, 40, 105–121. http://dx.doi
Y. (2005). Derived manding in children with au- .org/10.1901/jaba.2007.117-05
tism: Synthesizing Skinner’s verbal behavior with Skinner, B. F. (1957). Verbal behavior. Acton, MA:
relational frame theory. Journal of Applied Behav- Copley Publishing Group.
ior Analysis, 38, 445– 462. Vitale, A., Barnes-Holmes, Y., Barnes-Holmes, D.,
O’Connor, J., Rafferty, A., Barnes-Holmes, D., & & Campbell, C. (2008). Facilitating responding In
Barnes-Homes, Y. (2009). The role of verbal be- accordance with the relational frame of compari-
haviour, stimulus nameability, and familiarity on son: Systematic empirical analyses. The Psycho-
the equivalence performances of autistic and nor- logical Record, 58, 365–390.
mally developing children. The Psychological Re-
cord, 59, 53–74.
Rehfeldt, R. A. (2011). Toward a technology of de- Received February 18, 2016
rived stimulus relations: An analysis of articles Revision received September 19, 2016
published in the journal of applied behavior anal- Accepted October 12, 2016 䡲

You might also like