Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 12

On Pubescent Voice Change in Males

Harry Hollien, Gainesville, Florida

Summary: This review of adolescent voice change (AVC) in males is introduced by a brief historical overview of the
area. That effort is followed by a summarization of those core studies which describe the AVC process in the normal boy.
Although no new experiments are presented, a number of related investigations are organized into a cohesive base—one
that permits development of an AVC model The model, then, can be employed to establish a reasonable description of
the process by providing information about its initiation, duration, and completion. It also assists in establishing per-
spectives about pubescent voice change and a baseline for future research.
Key Words: Adolescent voice change–AVC–Pubescence–Adolescence–Male voice change.

‘‘I now would like to add an acknowledgment to this article; one about our editor: Robert T. Sataloff, MD. He is to be commended,
of course, for his fine development of our Annual Symposia and The Voice Foundation. But, more importantly, I would like to
salute him for his superb effort in building the Journal of Voice into a publication of international stature. This important accom-
plishment is deserving of recognition. Indeed, one of the primary reasons I wrote this article (at 84 years of age) was to acknowl-
edge his leadership. Please join me in commending him.’’

INTRODUCTION adolescence, and voice change. In response, it would appear


First, it should be made clear that there are three major groups useful to establish some common ground—and ‘‘definitions’’
who legitimately study pubescent voice change. One group is seems to be a good place to start. By this means, it maybe pos-
made up of the teachers who train and develop the singers, sible to boil down the subsequent discussion to one which is
the second consists of medical and clinical specialists who pro- both reasonable and has manageable boundaries.
vide a remedial response to difficulties experienced by these As would be expected, generalized definitions abound. We all
artists. The third, and newest, cadre are the scientists (voice have read or heard of many of them. Briefly, the process in gen-
and phonetic) who are interested in—and study—the normal eral involves that developmental segment of life which occurs
process of pubescence and voice change. The essay to follow when the individual male changes his status from being a child
will focus on the work of the third group (of which the author to that of a young adult. Many transformations occur during this
is a member) in an attempt to pull the experiments on the nor- period, including somatic growth, maturation of the gonads, and
mal process of adolescent voice change (AVC) into a cohesive emergence of secondary sexual characteristics. In addition, met-
core that will permit the structuring of a predictive model. It is abolic changes occur in parallel with those cited, as do shifts in
recognized, however, that the other two groups have made psychological status and in other types of behavior.1 More to the
important contributions to the understanding of this process point, ‘‘pubescence,’’ which literally means ‘‘to grow hairy,’’ is
and a number of them will be included in this review. defined as that life stage when sexual status is changed and
Second, why just males? For one thing, the present author is maturity is reached. Adolescence, on the other hand, can be
a male and just as women are often interested in studying the be- defined as the period during which the characteristics associated
haviors of their sex; he cheerfully pleads guilty for being curious with childhood shift to those of an adult. Although technically
about what happens to members of his own cadre. Also, he still different, these two terms often are applied interchangeably.1
remembers the puzzlement he felt when, as a boy, he experienced Moreover, the process is one that is recognized as (1) being quite
singing soprano one day and, then, being only able to sing bari- complex, (2) involving not just a smooth general growth pattern
tone at what seemed to be but a day later—that is, if one could but one that exhibits spurts and (3) being marked by varying
characterize the sounds he made as ‘‘singing.’’ Additionally, the physiological and psychological changes. The problem is that,
dramatic and (sometimes, anyway) seemingly haphazard vocal although some of the correlates of adolescence have been estab-
events that occur during male adolescence often are more chal- lished, others have not. What is needed, specifically, is better
lenging than the more orderly process found in females. information about (1) the exact nature of these many changes,
(2) when they start, (3) when they are complete, and (4) the
interrelationships among them. Even more important, many of
Definitions these events are not well understood when a boy is considered
The behavioral events to be considered here can be identified in as an ‘‘individual.’’
many ways. Some of the more common names are pubescence, Covering these many processes would involve a far greater
Accepted for publication January 14, 2011. task than could be (or should be) attempted in this modest essay.
From the Institute of Advanced Study of the Communication Processes, University of
Florida, Gainesville, Florida.
And, anyway, our common focus here is on the human voice
Address correspondence and reprint requests to Dr. Harry Hollien, Institute of Advanced and its properties. Thus, the primary effort will be on the effects
Study of the Communication Processes, 63 Dauer Hall, University of Florida, Gainesville,
FL 32601. E-mail: hollien@grove.ufl.edu
of the pubescent process on a boy’s voice. It will be both on its
Journal of Voice, Vol. 26, No. 2, pp. e29-e40 overall function and on those events specific to the individual.
0892-1997/$36.00
Ó 2012 The Voice Foundation
One of the areas to be considered will be the (often) rapid low-
doi:10.1016/j.jvoice.2011.01.007 ering of speaking fundamental frequency (SFF or Fo). Even
e30 Journal of Voice, Vol. 26, No. 2, 2012

more to the point, the question will be asked if change in SFF PERSPECTIVE
should be viewed simply as one of the many mutations which A little background
occur during adolescence, or if it is in some way more funda- Almost any individual reading these words will have a reason-
mental to the process. That is, should voice change be of partic- ably good idea of where at least some of our interest in AVC
ular interest to us only because we are specialists involved in originated and how those concerns were passed on down
some aspect of laryngeal operation, or can it be employed to through the ages. For example, people engaged in training
predict or describe pubescence in some manner? For example, young singers have always had a keen interest in the pubescent
is it possible that voice change could be used to accurately sig- process. So have medical and related caregivers. Moreover,
nal the initiation and completion of adolescence? Perhaps it very young singers—both individually and those in choral
can, perhaps not. Yet, there is indication that good groups—have attracted people who simply enjoy listening to
information about voice can provide us with a better under- this type of musical performance. Indeed, enthusiasms of that
standing of a number of human behaviors. kind reach back beyond recorded history. But what hap-
Think not? Consider the patterns that can be seen in Figure 1. pened—‘‘back then’’—when children (especially boys) experi-
They demonstrate how human voice frequency (Fo) generally enced AVC? Sometimes it didn’t matter, as other singers would
varies over life—and for both men and women. Here is an emerge to replace those that were ‘‘lost.’’ In yet other cases,
instance where fundamental frequency alone can be used to women simply took over the roles of soprano and alto. But,
track, and explain to some extent, a specific human behavior. in some cultures, women were not allowed to perform, thereby
That is, these data can be used to provide one of the foundations creating a problem. So, what was the response (in that case) on
for the male-female coalescence theory of aging.2 First, note that the part of the decision-makers? Some groups pressured reli-
the SFF levels for adult men and women have been reasonably gious authorities to let women perform, some teachers worked
well established,3–10 and that the relationships also are clear- to prolong the adolescents’ ability to sing in the higher registers,
cut—and for both sexes—with respect to old age.6,11–23 The and others worked with tenors to develop their ‘‘falsetto.’’ Still
same maybe said for children.24–32 Now, observe those others simply attempted to ‘‘remove’’ the effects of pubescence.
portions of the curves where adolescence is represented. Here, That is, noting that most eunuchs retained their childlike phona-
it is found that Fo drops to some extent in females and tional range even though their bodies otherwise attained various
dramatically in males. In turn, these changes provide the levels of maturity, certain individuals, in position to do so,
contrasts, seen in later life, where SFF shift brings members of engaged in the barbaric approach of countering voice change
the two genders back toward each other. Hence, Fo provides by castrating the child singer. After all, eunuchs had been serv-
one of the relationships that define the coalescence theory. ing as ‘‘guardians of the harem,’’ as chamberlains and in certain
Can a similar relationship be established for pubescence? other political positions throughout history and in such places
as Egypt, India, Persia, China, and Greece. So this practice
developed in Europe and flourished there during the Baroque
period (especially between the years 1600 and 1750) where it
is said that as many as 70% of the operatic singers were castrati.
Fortunately, this evil practice was largely eliminated in the 19th
century partly through the efforts of enlightened lawmakers and
partly by edicts of the Catholic Church.
But, what about more modern times? Here, teachers of sing-
ing appear to have concentrated on doing the best they could
with the material at hand (with reference to singers and pro-
spective singers). Nonetheless, a number of them still struggled
to understand the pubescent process and the nature of AVC. So
did a number of physicians and therapists. However, the prob-
lems faced by all of these groups were manifold. Relevant tech-
nology was either nonexistent or very crude, at least, until after
World War I. Moreover, teachers and clinicians had neither the
time nor the training to carry out sophisticated research.
On the other hand, many of the individuals within the cited
groups were both intellectually capable and observant. Hence,
generalizations about these processes began to emerge. To
illustrate, consider some of the cadaveric work, which was
conducted early in the 20th century. It permitted a better
understanding—generally anyway—of the overall growth of
FIGURE 1. Male-female SFF means and ranges as a function of age. the body and the larynx. Nonetheless, Kahane33,34 when
Note the greater drop for males at puberty and the convergence of the reporting on his breakthrough work in the area, indicated
male-female Fo trends for the elderly. that ‘‘only fragmentary data’’ resulted from all that effort. He
Harry Hollien On Pubescent Voice Change in Males e31

further suggested that most of it actually was drawn from contrasted these same voice ‘‘measures’’ to three levels of
general anatomical texts—with even Gedgowd35 and Klock36 (observed) pubic hair growth. Among other things, he was sur-
(who ‘‘described’’ adolescent laryngeal growth) neglecting to prised to note that only four voice breaks were recorded during
provide very much useful detail. more than 85 hours of oral reading by his subjects. Although
Practitioners of other types also were active during this period. some useful insights into the voice change process were gained
Clinicians were especially interested in—and studied—such here, not all of Pedrey’s measures were quantitative; hence, the
behaviors as (1) voice breaks (sudden shifts from-and-to the impact of his findings was a little diluted. Nonetheless, certain
adult register), (2) ‘‘husky’’ voice (a condition sometimes found of the somatic data aided investigators such as Tanner45 in their
in the voices of males during change), and (3) mutational falsetto contribution to the overall understanding of the area.
(ie, ‘‘the failure of a boy to change from the higher pitched voice A different approach was adopted by Curry.52 He drew on the
of preadolescence to the lower pitched voice of adolescence and ‘‘sectional’’ research design employed by earlier scholars as
adulthood’’)37. As would be expected, singing teachers also were well as on technical progress that had occurred during the
interested in these same vocal problems and made contributions 1930s. The equipment he used, the phonellegraph,53–55 was
to their understanding. However, it must be said that, although one of the very earliest devices where the fundamental
these issues are much better understood today than they were frequency of ‘‘cold running speech’’ could be measured
then, some controversy still exists relative to their actual source accurately. Curry designed and carried out a study on groups
and the best method for treating them. of six each 10-, 14-, and 18-year-old males. He reasoned that
As has been suggested, a substantial number of individuals the first group consisted of prepubescent boys, the second of
among professional singing teachers have been interested and boys ‘‘within’’ adolescence, and the last as consisting pretty
active in the area of voice change in youth and have endeavored much of young adults. Because the groups were small
to classify the various stages of adolescent voice (see below). (obtaining phonellegraphic data, while accurate, was very
They have done so especially in attempts to upgrade their teach- labor intensive), they were very carefully selected to be both
ing and to avoid damaging their pupils’ voices. Numerous sys- homogeneous and representative of their cultural group.
tems were proposed during the early part of the 20th century. Height, weight, Intelligence Quotient, and about a dozen
Some were insightful, others not very well structured—or, measures of ‘‘vocal pitch’’ were obtained and reported. The
even, potentially damaging. This general effort was expanded three groups exhibited mean SFF levels of 270, 242, and
after World War II when yet more useful information became 137 Hz, respectively. This effort made a signal contribution to
available. Some of the leaders during this period provided the study of AVC if only on the basis of the classic display
insight and developed rather sophisticated concepts and/or sys- that can be seen in Figure 2. As maybe observed there, male
tems.38–42 The cited trend appears to have somewhat peaked pubescence can be typified over time in, at least, three general
with Cooksey43 but actually it continues on into the current dimensions—ie, by increasing height and weight curves and
century.44 a falling Fo curve. The project further solidified the ‘‘sectional’’
research pattern—one that many subsequent researchers would
follow. What it did not do was answer the basic question about
CURRENT RESEARCH when AVC starts, when it ends, how long it takes, and most
Early breakthroughs importantly, what variability occurs among boys. Worse yet,
It was not until after many centuries of effort, and thousands of the labor-intensive aspect of the phonellegraph limited the
publications, that real progress began to be made in the study of extent of data collection, and it would be many years before
male pubescence and the vocal events associated with this phe- efficient fundamental frequency trackers would become avail-
nomenon.45 The rapid rise of technology, plus the emergence of able. Hence, the limitations related to small subject groups
the scientific method, had a powerful influence on research dur-
ing the period around World War II. What resulted was a marked
upgrading of the quality of research. The publications shifted
from being mostly anecdotal and subjective (there were excep-
tions, of course, for example Fournier46) to efforts that were
more objective and databased.47–50
It was during that period when Pedrey51 attempted to respond
to some of the basic problems facing investigators who were
trying to discover when voice change started and when it ended.
His approach was to observe 1014 boys (sorted into 1 year cat-
egories) between the ages of 11 and 16 years. He gathered data
on such variables as perceived voice status (ie, subjective judg-
ments of vocal characteristics as they appeared related to pre-,
neo-, and postadolescence), height, weight, density of pubic
hair, voice breaks, and so on. Some rather useful information FIGURE 2. A figure summarizing the general shifts in male height,
resulted from this effort. For example, Pedrey compared sub- weight, and SFF as a function of age during puberty. HT, height; WT,
jects’ actual age to three classes of ‘‘voice maturity’’; he then weight.
e32 Journal of Voice, Vol. 26, No. 2, 2012

were numbered among the unintended consequences that so


TABLE 1.
often accompany research of this type. Comparisons of Northern White Boys (NW)52 Studied in
1940 and Southern Negro Boys (SN)70 Studied in 1962
An attempt to fill in a few of the blanks
Post World War II was a period of chaos in the area of voice; Age Height Weight Median
Group (mo) (in) (lb) Fo (Hz)
indeed, there were but few phonatory relationships that were
not open to dispute during that era. For example, a debate 10-y olds
existed as to how the vocal folds basically operated to initiate NW 120 54 66 270
phonation. So did another as to whether or not they were elon- SN 120 54 65 223
gated for frequency change during phonation. Both of these dis- 14-y olds
putes (and many others) had been going on for decades.48,56–62 NW 170 60 102 242
Actually, the Moore and von Leden60 work adequately SN 168 64 114 163
addressed the first of these issues, but it was not until 1960 18-y olds
that the debate about ‘‘vocal fold elongation’’ was resolved.63 NW 217 68 139 137
Perhaps most surprising (and hard to believe) was that it SN 216 69 138 124
was not until 1958 that Van Den Berg64 was able to provide Note: Fo variability ¼ NW: 1.56 tones; SN; 1.51 tones.
a cohesive defense of the myoelastic-aerodynamic theory of
phonation—one powerful enough to finally send the neurochro-
naxic theory and its followers into oblivion. tions.71,72 The first involved Southern white boys and the
The same kind of confusions existed in the domain of AVC. second Northern white boys; the groups (10-, 14-. 18-year
Of course, a number of investigators had been making contribu- olds) were selected on the basis of the same six rigorous
tions of various types,37,50,65–69 but it seemed reasonable that criteria as were the first two. Hence, direct four-way compari-
a follow up on both Pedrey’s and, especially, Curry’s work sons could be made.
would be fruitful. Accordingly, the sectional model was used The 10-year olds displayed no statistically significant varia-
to generate some information as to how the AVC process tion for any of the somatic or frequency measures. However, the
might relate to race, climate, and even nutrition. The first of three groups studied in the 1960s were more like each other
these studies contrasted blacks residing in the Southern part of (and bigger) than were Curry’s boys. Moreover, and as can be
the United States (ie, Waco, Texas) to Curry’s groups. seen in Table 2, the 1940 group of white boys exhibited SFF
Happenstance had a lot to do with both the initiation and the levels that were the highest of all groups—and the white boys
precision of this project.70 That is, the present author attended studied in 1965 and 1967 were more like the African-
Boston University with a man who later became the President Americans. The data modestly suggested that accelerated mat-
of the Paul Quinn College (an institution affiliated with the uration was occurring but about all that can be said is that all
African Methodist Episcopal church). He renewed his friend- four groups of preadolescents (ie, the 10-year olds) exhibited
ship with that individual when appointed as the faculty at Baylor generally childlike SFF.
University (also in Waco). The possibility of replicating Curry’s The 14-year olds presented a somewhat different picture.
work, but on southern blacks, came up during one of the lectures Again, no statistically significant differences were found
he gave at Paul Quinn, and the faculty at that institution among the groups but there was a slight trend for the three post-
provided the necessary expertise to make the project a success. war groups to be bigger than Curry’s. And, as can be seen from
Specifically, groups of six each 10-, 14-, and 18-years-old Table 2, the ‘‘modern’’ white boys displayed SFF more in a class
African-American males were assessed; their selection closely with the Southern blacks. However, the African-Americans still
paralleled that of the white subjects studied by Curry. That is, exhibited the lowest Fo. Even better evidence for accelerated
a rigorous selection process was used, one where subjects maturation (if not racial differences) maybe found in Table 3,
were within two months of their respective birthday and were where the boys who started and/or had completed voice change
representative (ie, close to median) of their age groups with
respect to (1) height, (2) weight, (3) reading ability, and (4)
speaking ability. Comparisons among the obtained data can be
found in Table 1, which summarizes the age, weight, height TABLE 2.
and mean Fo from both studies. While, it would appear that Median Fundamental Frequency Levels (in Hz) of Six
blacks experience voice change earlier than do whites (note Subjects in Each of the Four Studies52,70–72 of Three Age
especially their lower mean Fo at 14 years), the obtained results Groups
actually led to more questions than to answers. That is, did Study 10-y olds 14-y olds 18-y olds
a racial factor actually exist or were the differences because of
NWB-1 (1940) 270 242 137
(1) climate, (2) nutrition, and/or (3) time (over 20 years had
SNB (1962) 223 163 124
passed before the second study was initiated). SWB (1965) 248 199 128
In an effort to clarify the questions asked—rather than just NWB-2 (1967) 237 189 —*
obscure them, as this particular study may have done—the
* An 18-year-old group was not considered necessary for the fourth study.
Hollien-Malcik team carried out two additional investiga-
Harry Hollien On Pubescent Voice Change in Males e33

tive language. This phonatory performance was recorded and


TABLE 3.
Number of 14-Year-Old Boys Who Have Not Started
subsequently analyzed by FFI-8, a solid-state version of the
Pubescent Voice Change and Have Either Initiated or original FFI. The data most relevant to this review are summa-
Completed It rized in Table 4.
When the obtained relationships are analyzed, the best that
Group Not Started Initiated Completed
can be said of them is that the curves found in Figure 2 had
52
NWB-1 (1940) 5 1 0 been validated and somewhat enhanced. They did not appear
SNB (1962)70 1 3 2 to support the Luchsinger/Perello postulate that residence in
SWB (1965)71 2 2 2 warmer climates would lead to earlier voice change. Of course,
NWB-2 (1967)72 2 2 2 it can be conceded that much of their original thinking was
based on the emergence of menstruation in girls rather than
on some sort of observation of boys. Nonetheless, their general
are listed. This pattern also is in agreement with Pedrey’s51 hypothesis had been a logical one. It was just not supported by
estimates. Unfortunately, however, even when taken as a whole, the data to be found in Table 4. Indeed, AVC seemed to occur
this compilation of four data sets did not provide clear-cut earlier in those boys (ie, the Swedes) who were from a relatively
answers to the questions posed about race, nutrition, and/or cli- cold climate and then from those (Dutch, Poles) residing in tem-
mate. Moreover, they did little to upgrade the information about perate domains. The Catalan boys from Spain tended to experi-
the limits, details, and dimensions of pubescent voice change in ence these changes later in life. Oddly enough, the three data
the individual. sets showed fairly close pattern alignment if not the expected
absolute levels—and some socioeconomic and cultural effects
A larger effort may have been present. In short, this large study provided a rea-
Influenced by Perello67 (J. Perello, Personal communication, sonable validation of some of the prior (and smaller) ones but
June 1963, August 1964.), the data cited above, and the devel- little support for them with respect to either the environmental
opment of the present author’s electronic Fundamental effects on the process or the developmental trends among the
Frequency Indicator (FFI), he and his group shifted focus. boys themselves.
That is, they attempted to test the hypothesis that climate
change might be a factor that exerts an external but important
influence on AVC. However, before reviewing one of the very A SHIFT IN FOCUS
large studies that resulted (see below), a brief introduction to Longitudinal approach
the FFI apparatus would appear useful. FFI73 was a system It became apparent early on that, no matter how large and
that continuously extracted SFF from recorded speech and sophisticated the sectional research project might be, the criti-
analyzed the resulting (digital) frequency data. It did not sample cal information about AVC would be unattainable until more
Fo but rather measured the individual waves obtained from sophisticated research designs were carried out. Accordingly,
a series of first-stage, very sharp, low pass filters (ie, 75 dB a large longitudinal study of boys was proposed, funded
per octave). The subsequent stages consisted of A/D converters, (National Institutes of Health), and conducted. Although a num-
which monitored the filters and coupled only the lowest one ber of interim reports—including attempts to develop an AVC
containing energy to the output system. As maybe seen in model—were made public,76–78 an extensive review of the
Figure 3, the resulting display included a printed table of all longitudinal data was not published until 1994.79
measurements (by semitone interval) and a histogram. It also Basically, this project involved a large number of subjects
provided the geometric mean SFF in Hertz and semitone level drawn from a group of 10.5- to 11.5-year-old boys residing in
as well as the standard deviation (SD) in semitones. Other use- Wichita, Kansas. All received physical, speech, and hearing
ful measures and a series of built-in validity tests ensured accu- tests as well as other assessments. Ultimately, 65 of them met
racy. The processing of a one-minute speech sample took about all selection criteria and were included in the study. Specifi-
6 minutes, hence very large studies were possible.5,6,74 cally, they (1) had been born in the Mid-West region of the
The investigation on AVC versus climate involved the collec- United States and resided there during their childhood, (2)
tion of data on height, weight, mean Fo, and variability exhibited a general American dialect, (3) were of middle-
(SD in semitones) for 491 boys residing in four countries.74,75 class socioeconomic status, (4) exhibited test scores placing
They were equally divided (roughly) into five groups of 12-, them in average to above-average intelligence categories, (5)
13-, 14-, 15-, and 16-year olds. Of the total, 150 were evidenced average-to-good speaking ability, (6) were in gener-
Swedish, 180 Dutch or Polish, and 161 Spanish. That three- ally good health, and (7) were free from speech or hearing dis-
way sort permitted data to be obtained on subjects from cold, orders. In all, 48 of the 65 selected completed the 5-year study.
temperate, and warm climates. Information concerning age, Data were gathered on all subjects for up to 6 years (ie, the
medical and vocal history, speech and hearing defects, speaking grant period plus an extension). They were as follows:
and singing training, and so on, was obtained on each volunteer
and used in the selection process. The major factors studied 1. Physical size: Height, weight, and six body dimensions.
were physical size and SFF. The speech materials were obtained Laryngeal size measures (by means of soft tissue
by having each subject orally read a standard passage in his na- X-ray) had been proposed, but this procedure was not
e34 Journal of Voice, Vol. 26, No. 2, 2012

FIGURE 3. A printout from the FFI. The geometric mean of cold running speech is provided in Hertz and semitones and distribution variability (ie,
standard deviation) in semitones. Note also the quantitative and graphic displays.

funded because of the potential that such radiation might The research team consisted of the principal investigator,
be harmful to the boys. a nurse, three research assistants, a speech pathologist (on call),
2. Voice: The two major phonatory characteristics studied and a technician. A physician provided the initial examinations
were SFF level/variability and phonational frequency and was then on call. All data were obtained in a comfortable
range (PFR). As it turns out, the mean SFF measures pro- laboratory; that is, excepting for the voice recordings, which
vided the breakout data and will be featured in this were made in a sound-treated room. Laboratory quality micro-
review. phones and tape recorders were used; they were calibrated
3. Other: Voice breaks, husky voice, and mutational falsetto before each set of trials. The SFF measures were obtained by
also were tracked and noted, as were behavioral and gen- having subjects read the ‘‘Rainbow Passage’’80 at a normal
eral health factors. rate and energy level. Oral reading was selected to ensure that
Harry Hollien On Pubescent Voice Change in Males e35

more rapidly during the pubescent period than before or after,


TABLE 4.
Summary Table of 491 European Boys Between the Ages
(2) they sometimes exhibited growth spurts, but (3) not much
of 12 and 16 Years temporal specificity was possible. Second, voice breaks, hus-
kiness of voice, and mutational falsetto also were tracked.
Group Number Height Weight Mean SD Just as with Pedrey, Curry, and others, these events appeared
by Age of Subjects (in) (lb) Fo (Hz) (st*)
to occur but rarely and, when they did, they were mitigated
Swedish either by the passage of time or by the efforts of the speech
12 29 59 90 220 4.4 pathologist.
13 33 63 107 206 4.7 Finally, it had been hypothesized that Phonational Frequency
14 28 64 108 182 5.2 Range (PFR) might be useful as a baseline indicator of AVC.
15 33 67 121 168 5.2
Indeed, a number of investigators had attempted to quantify it
16 27 70 138 128 5.0
for this purpose.46,65–67,77,78,82 But when the present data
150 were assessed, the across-study patterns did not provide clear-
cut relationships. Of course, the differences in experimental
Dutch and Polish
procedure may account for some of this variance. Nevertheless,
12 30 59 87 270 3.6
several authors have suggested that PFR maybe restricted dur-
13 36 62 97 246 3.8
14 39 64 110 219 4.3 ing puberty. Indeed, a few of them postulate that PFR may even
15 44 68 122 169 4.7 be reduced to a level which signals the presence of voice pathol-
16 31 69 131 145 5.0 ogy. Their position in this regard was neither supported by the
present data nor for that matter, by the opinions of several of the
180
practitioners specializing in vocal music.41,83 On the other
Spanish hand, the greatest PFR here was observed to occur during the
12 24 59 93 347 4.1 adolescence process—ie, for the period of most rapid change
13 39 61 110 254 4.7 in AVC. This finding may be due to the fact that some
14 34 64 119 254 5.5 subjects phonated in two ‘‘voices’’ during that period (ie,
15 25 66 129 222 6.0 partly in the oncoming adult register and partly in their
16 39 68 135 212 5.9 prepubescent voice). These data are consistent with
161 statements found in the general vocal music literature that
The European boys were from four countries and placed in 1-year cate-
during adolescence the lower Fo boundary for males drops at
gories.74 a faster rate than does the upper. The patterns in the present
* semitones case (which are based on nearly 1000 trials) were found to be
stable, and their internal consistency was as good as that
reported by others.10,84 The data in expanded PFR ranges also
is consistent with the position articulated by certain
the speech materials analyzed were both constant across sub- musicologists that a boy’s physiological range maybe
jects and over time. The Fo level and variability data were somewhat extended during adolescence.41,85 Yet, these data,
obtained by use of FFI73; the PFR data by procedures outlined although possibly interesting, permit but little specification
by Hollien et al81 As anyone who has conducted longitudinal of AVC.
research will attest, good record keeping (and data backup)
was a challenge. It was especially difficult because most of
the data were gathered, stored, and analyzed before the days A MODEL BASED ON FUNDAMENTAL FREQUENCY
of adequate computer support. Nevertheless, no important The major contribution of the longitudinal project may now be
records were lost over the period this investigation took place. addressed. That is, much of the research conducted by the pres-
It will be remembered that the focus of this review is on the ent author, plus publications by other relevant specialists (see
possibility that the details of voice change can be captured for references), has led to the postulation that the Fo change param-
individual boys and, if so, that they then will be useful to the eter may serve to signal the start, extent, and completion of
understanding of AVC or, even, in documenting it. To a great AVC. Materials from the longitudinal project now permitted
extent, the validity of that concept was supported by the longi- this hypothesis to be assessed.79 They also could be used to
tudinal investigation. Thus, SFF will be the dominant element determine if, indeed, there was some sort of a useful correlation
in the discussion to follow. On the other hand, the reader may between AVC and adolescence. As it turns out, the cited
find other of the results useful (a more complete discussion hypothesis appears to exhibit reasonable validity. That is, it
about them can be found in the Journal of the Acoustical Soci- was found that SFF plots could be used for the purpose of
ety of America article79). But first, a short review of some of defining AVC.
these ‘‘other’’ issues may provide a reasonable perspective for To accomplish this, and develop the relevant model, a set of
the AVC data. basic criteria first had to be established. They may be best
The somatic measures primarily confirmed those reported understood by reference to Figure 4 where the actual initiation,
by many other authors. It was noted that (1) the boys grew extent, and completion, of the cited process, can be seen
e36 Journal of Voice, Vol. 26, No. 2, 2012

Age of onset
Some of the relationships provided by the model should be use-
ful. The first factor to consider is the age of AVC onset
(Figure 5). Forty-five of the 48 subjects who completed this pro-
ject were found to have initiated AVC by its end. The remaining
three had not done so by 15.5 years of age. Of those 45 who did,
the very first to initiate the process reached ‘‘S’’ at the age of 11
years and 7 months (ie, at 139 months). The mean age of onset
for all subjects was approximately 161 months (or about 13.4
years) with a standard deviation of 10.4 months. The mean
would have been a bit higher if the three subjects cited above
could have been included in the distribution. In any event, the
number of initiations peaked during the half year immediately
following the mean and only 6% of the subjects had not initiated
AVC by 15.5 years of age (ie, by the end of data collection).
These data led to the postulate that, while occasionally boys
may start AVC as early as 10.5 years of age or as late as 16.5
years, the majority will initiate the process sometime between
the ages of 12.5 and 14.5 years. Further, it was noted that these
data lacked a well-defined bell-shaped curve (Figure 5) even
though this might have been evident if a very large population
of boys could have been studied. To sum, the AVC initiations
FIGURE 4. Graphic display of SFF for a subject experiencing AVC. were spread out over 3.5 years and exhibited a variable pattern.
The onset can be identified by S and completion by E. AVC duration Indeed, there were only four periods when as many as four boys
for this male was about 22 months. started the process.
Most of the relationships established by this analysis are con-
illustrated by the data-plot of a representative subject. Although sistent with those reported by investigators using a sectional
this particular example is for ‘‘N ¼ 1,’’ it should be stressed that, approach even though theirs are not extensive enough to sup-
even though the slopes exhibited for the other boys varied in port a model. For example, the mean age of AVC onset, found
steepness (see below), the basic pattern seen in Figure 4 held to be approximately 13.5 years, compared favorably with met-
for all of the subjects. rics reported (or predicted) by most authors37,66,70–72,74,83 with
To be specific, AVC onset (or ‘‘S’’ for start) can be identified the only marked exception being Curry.52 Also predictable was
as the SFF peak, which was preceded by a generally flat—albeit the generally observed range of onset (10–16 years). The wide
varying—pattern for a period of at least four months. This pat- variations found in this regard suggest that there is no specific
tern then had to be succeeded by a decreasing SFF with a slope age—or narrow range of ages—at which AVC onset can be
at a rate of approximately a semitone per month for a number of judged normal or even typical.
months (usually 6 months or more). This SFF pattern could
be—and was—established as the ‘‘onset’’ criterion in the
AVC model, and as stated, it was met by all subjects who initi-
ated that process.
It was also possible to identify the termination of AVC as fol-
lows. End or ‘‘E’’ was defined as that point occurring at the low-
est measured fundamental frequency immediately preceding
a period of general frequency stabilization.
As a result, the AVC process was found to reflect a minimum
downward shift of 8–12 semitones. Modest reversals were
expected (but none were found in excess of about a semitone),
and these minor inversions were identified in about a third of the
subjects. The most common reasons for them appeared to be
colds or vocal abuse (cheering, shouting, or singing), which
occurred before the first of two runs (during the Fo decreasing
phase) but not for the subsequent one.
In short, it became possible to define AVC as that period
occurring between the frequencies identified at points ‘‘S’’ FIGURE 5. Age of onset of AVC in months and years as reported in
and ‘‘E.’’ That is, the initiation, duration, and completion of a longitudinal investigation.79 The first of the boys initiated onset at
AVC could be established for the subjects in the experiment, about 11.5 years; three additional boys had not initiated it until some-
and the defining vector was shift in SFF. time after 15.5 years.
Harry Hollien On Pubescent Voice Change in Males e37

On the other hand, the data from this investigation do not sup-
port opinions expressed by some practitioners that deviation
from a restricted range of AVC onset times suggests the pres-
ence of a voice disorder. That is, efforts to establish vocal health
or normalcy on the basis of the presumed appropriateness of
perceived pitch86 maybe more difficult to establish than had
been imagined previously. It also appears that suggestions
that onset of male adolescent voice should occur between the
ages of 13 and 14 years69,87 must be reassessed. It probably is
more useful to postulate that most boys will initiate AVC
somewhere between the ages of 12 and 15 years but that even
the minority who start earlier or later may also have normal
voices.

AVC duration
FIGURE 6. AVC duration in months as reported in a longitudinal
Data on the completion of AVC (coupled to the patterns found
study.79 The data are for the 35 subjects who had fully completed
for its onset) provide information not only on just when the pro-
the process and the eight subjects where reliable estimates could be
cess ends but also about its duration. Even though, the data on
made. The data for the one subject who took 37 months are included
AVC completion are not quite as dramatic as those for onset,
but he is not shown in the figure.
they still support the model. In any case, while a few of the sub-
jects had not completely finished the shift by project’s end, it
can be estimated that adult SFF level, for most American reported opinions. That is, the mean AVC duration of 18 months
boys anyway, can be expected within the 5-year time frame reported here contrasts with the traditional viewpoint that it oc-
between the ages of 13 and 18 years. A range as extensive as curs in males during a short period of time and any change last-
this one was not expected and, hence, this maybe one of the sev- ing more than a year maybe pathological in nature.
eral relationships, which have previously led to confusions To sum, the SFF material provided by the longitudinal inves-
about adolescence and AVC. It also may constitute an area tigation79 answers some of the basic questions about AVC in
where the model requires further testing. normal boys. Most notably, a model is provided for assessing
As stated, the longer than predicted time that the average sub- that nature of voice change in the individual; it provides critical
ject took to complete AVC was a little unexpected. As maybe details about both its maximum range (10.5–16.5 years) and
seen from Figure 6, the AVC durations, as exhibited by those most common range (12.5–14.5 years). It also provides specifi-
boys who had fully completed the process (plus estimates for cation about duration where the normal period can take over
those for which they could be reliably identified), ranged 3 years and exhibits a mean of 18 months. Those temporal
from 8 to 37 months; the mean duration of AVC was approxi- values are greater than those commonly expected, yet do not
mately 18 months. These results are not at all consistent with signal the presence of voice disorders. In short, it should be pos-
the commonly held position that this process takes but 3–6 sible to use the AVC model to better understand, and quantify
months to complete.37,50,65 Moreover, the experience of one pubescence. Indeed, it should provide a testable hypothesis as
particular subject underscores the argument that completion well as a base for future postulations and research.
of AVC can take a very long period of time. It was possible
to track the subject even though he had not completed AVC External support for the model
after the end of data collection. It took him over 3 years Although endocrinological assays were not included among the
(ie, 37 months) to stabilize at 110 Hz even though his voice protocols for the project reviewed above, it was possible to com-
was clinically normal during the entire period. pare the AVC trends found there with certain data on hormonal
First, these rather long durations are a little difficult to resolve. shifts gathered by others.88–91 Of the number reviewed, it was
Of course, some of the prior opinions about this relationship Knorr et al,92 who best provided information about shifts in tes-
could have been based on small samples, incomplete data, or tosterone level related to adolescence. In turn, their data fur-
misinterpretations. Second, and on the other hand, it may be pos- nished external evidence useful in validating the AVC model.
sible that the process was not noticed by the relevant profes- That is, their data could be adapted for direct comparison with
sionals except during the period of maximum change. Rapid the AVC trends from the 1994 study.79 Specifically, the cumula-
shifts are bound to attract attention. Third, the shift from one reg- tive testosterone function found in Figure 7 was adapted from
ister to another is quite prominent in males (with reference to the Knorr et al92 data on 22 subjects. The estimates were made
both SFF and quality) and may signal change. Fourth, the by extending each subject’s curve (with reference to change in
existence of ‘‘voice breaks,’’ or huskiness, observed in a few testosterone) to bisect the baseline level. These intersects could
males also may have contributed to the cited opinions. While then be employed to predict age of pubescent onset. Although
voice breaks, etc. probably do not last long, they undoubtedly the range for the testosterone data is somewhat greater than
are quite noticeable when they do occur. Anyway, the data ob- that for AVC, the two curves are similar. Further, it should be
tained from this research are at odds with many of the previously noted that the slope of the AVC curve probably would more
e38 Journal of Voice, Vol. 26, No. 2, 2012

can be used to signal the onset of voice change. On the other


hand, it would appear just as likely that these conditions would
result from other types of problems or occur after mutation
(ie, at varying times after the onset of AVC). These authors
also draw from earlier work94–96 in their attempts to code
mutation, its onset, and possible acceleration. Nonetheless,
although their contributions are important, they appear to
revolve around maintaining the vocal health of adolescent
performers. Note that Fuchs et al93 end their report by recom-
mending that young singers receive batteries of medical exam-
inations every 3 months during the period of possible mutation.
On the other hand, the research reported in the present review
involves the study of many hundreds of normal boys experienc-
ing AVC, and it can be used to suggest that voice disorders in
general are not common during that period of life. That is, it
FIGURE 7. A contrast of two methods by which the initiation of may be possible that the high vocal usage and stress, associated
puberty is identified; both are shown as cumulative frequency curves. with performing, results in greater rates of vocal pathology
The first one, which was derived from measures of voice frequency, is among the boys seen by the cited authors than among popula-
AVC. The other one tracks testosterone levels as shifts from their base- tions of nonsingers. Perhaps, the data provided by the longitu-
line. The curve at the center top was extrapolated by adding the theo- dinal AVC research, and the model associated with it, could
retical voice data for the three boys who had not initiated AVC onset by supply reasonable normative values for use by medical profes-
the end of that study. sionals in their work with, and research on, young singers.
The other general thrust involves research on the proper
closely parallel that for the testosterone shift if the three subjects training of singers and some of the behavioral changes that con-
who had not initiated onset could have been included in the mix. stitute a challenge to voice teachers. Again, it might be helpful
In short, it can be argued that the testosterone data are consistent for investigators to use the normative data now available as
with those from the AVC study and, hence, this convergence a model upon which to base their classifications of pre-, neo-,
tends to validate both. It appears, then, that the cohesiveness and postadolescence. Some authors, of course, have already
of the AVC model (onset, duration, termination) permit it to attempted to do so but have used older systems for this purpose.
be used as a predictor of pubescent voice change—and, perhaps For example, Harries et al97 tried to develop classifications
even for the pubescent process in general. based on their work and by coupling Cooksey’s43,44 six-stage
structure with Tanner’s91 five-stage pubertal maturity index.
OTHER ISSUES To be specific, they organized their research by means of the
Research on AVC continues even though it still tends to be scat- combined Tanner-Cooksey systems in an attempt to model
tered among a number of issues. As in the past, many of the the adolescent process. They were successful to some extent
studies tend to cluster in two areas (1) voice disorders— in doing so. That is, they were able to fit a portion of their sing-
especially those found in singers—associated with the pre- ing and speaking Fo data into the somewhat different structures
sumed vocal instability, which may accompany pubescence proposed by the other two investigators. Although they were not
and (2) the upgrading of the teaching of adolescent singers able to resolve all the necessary dimensions, their results
both on an individual basis and for choral performances. appeared to permit them to better describe the pubescent pro-
Just as in the past, the medical practitioners and therapists cess as it related to vocal dynamics.
interested in the first of these problems tend to focus on pathol- Finally, one line of inquiry that has seen a resurgence of
ogy. As would be expected, they are somewhat less interested in interest recently is that involving gaps in the PFR that can be
the singers per se than with the possible presence of voice dis- found in the voices of some adolescents.82,98,99 A similar
orders, and what to do about them; a legitimate approach, of phenomenon—ie, gaps between registers—has previously
course. However, they also appear to suggest that although been reported in a large study on normal PFR.81 Although the
baseline data about the dimensions of the pubescent process two issues do not appear to result from parallel vocal operations
(and/or voice change itself) would be useful, pathology is or conditions, they do raise concerns for the vocal pedagogic
paramount—especially if it interferes with the development effort. In the first instance, it is important for teachers to be alert
and/or quality of the subject’s singing voice. Good examples to the problem in adolescent singers. In such case, they maybe
maybe found in several recent articles. In one of them,93 the able to help male performers, who are well within the AVC pro-
research effort appeared to be focused on predicting AVC onset cess (and have not left it), to cope with both the child’s singing
to establish when to look for, and treat, the voice disorders that register and that of the adult. It would seem here that the teacher
accompany such mutation. Here, the voices of young singers and pupil could choose to focus on one ‘‘register’’ or the other.
were examined using a variety of (mostly medical) techniques. In any case, the problem is one that teachers have to deal with
Perhaps detection of greater hoarseness, noise components, although it is not a permanent condition. Finally, the other
jitter, shimmer, and the like in the voices of young male singers issue—gaps between registers in the adult (ie, between the
Harry Hollien On Pubescent Voice Change in Males e39

modal and falsetto registers)—may present a problem when the 18. McGlone RE, Hollien H. Vocal pitch characteristics of aged women.
teacher attempts to increase the students’ singing range. J Speech Hear Res. 1963;6:164–170.
19. Morgan E, Ratsatter M. Variability of voice fundamental frequency in
elderly female speakers. Percept Mot Skills. 1986;63:215–218.
A SUMMARY 20. Mysak E. Pitch and duration characteristics of older males. J Speech Hear
Although there are many issues to consider by those of us who Res. 1959;2:46–54.
21. Ptacek PH, Sander EK, Maroney W, Jackson C. Phonatory and related
are interested in the voices of adolescents—and many hundreds
changes with advanced age. J Speech Hear Res. 1966;9:353–360.
more for those dealing with pubescence in general—our spe- 22. Ramig LA, Ringel RL. Effects of physiological aging on selected acoustic
cialty area is currently experiencing good closure on some of characteristics of voice. J Speech Hear Res. 1983;26:22–30.
its enigmas. Treatment of many voice pathologies has coa- 23. Scukanec G, Petrosino L, Ratsatter M. Fundamental frequency variability
lesced and become much more sophisticated. The education in elderly women during production of stressed and unstressed words. Per-
cept Mot Skills. 1992;74:1091–1095.
of young singers exhibits progress at much the same level.
24. Bennett S. A 3-year longitudinal study of school-aged children’s fundamen-
The work in this area by Experimental Phoneticians (including tal frequencies. J Speech Hear Res. 1983;26:137–142.
Voice Scientists) also has resulted in good progress. Perhaps the 25. Cornut G, Riow-Bourret V, Lonis M. Contribution a l’etude de la voix parles
concepts that were detailed throughout this essay, and culmi- et chantee de l’enfant normal de 5 a 9 ans. [Study of the contribution of the
nated in a model for AVC, can serve as one basis for the next talking and singing voices of normal children 5 to 9 years old]. Folia
Phoniatr. 1971;23:381–389.
upgrade in at least some of the relevant areas. Certainly, the
26. Eguchi S, Hirsch IJ. Development of speech sounds in children. Acta Oto-
baseline data provided about the key aspects of male AVC laryngol. 1969;257(suppl):1–59.
should aid in the planning and conduct of research to be carried 27. Fairbanks G, Wiley J, Lassman F. An acoustical study of voice pitch in
out by investigators in all three of the areas cited. It certainly seven- and eight-year-old boys. Child Dev. 1949;20:63–69.
should provide for better cohesion in future investigations. 28. Hasek CS, Singh S, Murry T. Acoustic attributes of preadolescent voices.
J Acoust Soc Am. 1980;68:1262–1265.
29. Murry T, Murry J. Infant Communication: Cry and Early Speech. Houston,
REFERENCES TX: College-Hill; 1980.
1. Marshall WA, Tanner JM. Puberty In: Faulkner F, Tanner JM, eds. Human 30. Murry T, Amundson P, Hollien H. Acoustical characteristics of infant cries:
Growth, Vol 2. New York, NY: Plenum Press; 1986:171–209. fundamental frequency. J Child Lang. 1977;4:321–328.
2. Hollien H. The normal aging voice In: Huntley RA, Helfer KS, eds. Com- 31. Reich AR, Mason JA, Frederichson RR, Schlauch RS. Factors influencing
munication in Later Life. Boston, MA: Butterworth-Heinemann; 1995: fundamental frequency range estimates in children. J Speech Hear Res.
23–40. chap 2. 1989;59:429–438.
3. Britto AI, Doyle PC. A comparison of habitual and derived optimal voice 32. Van Oordt HWA, Drost HA. Development of the frequency range of the
fundamental frequency values in normal young adult speakers. J Speech voice in children. Folia Phoniatr (Basel). 1963;15:289–298.
Hear Disord. 1990;55:476–484. 33. Kahane JC. A morphological study of the human prepubertal and pubertal
4. Fitch J, Holbrook A. Modal vocal fundamental frequency for young adults. larynx. Am J Anat. 1978;151:11–20.
Arch Otolaryngol. 1970;92:379–382. 34. Kahane JC. Growth of the human prepubertal and pubertal larynx. J Speech
5. Hollien H, Jackson B. Normative data on the speaking fundamental fre- Hear Res. 1982;25:446–455.
quency characteristics of young adult males. J Phonetics. 1973;1: 35. Gedgowd VA. Anatomical Peculiarities of the Respiratory Organs in Chil-
117–120. dren [unpublished doctoral dissertation]. Polevny S, trans. St. Petersburg,
6. Hollien H, Shipp T. Speaking fundamental frequency and chronological Russia: University of St. Petersburg; 1900.
age in males. J Speech Hear Res. 1972;15:155–159. 36. Klock LE. The Growth and Development of the Human Larynx from Birth to
7. Hollien H, Hollien PA, DeJong G. Effects of three parameters on speaking Adolescence [master’s thesis]. Seattle, WA: University of Washington; 1968.
fundamental frequency. J Acoust Soc Am. 1997;102:2984–2992. 37. Aronson AE. Clinical Voice Disorders. 3rd ed. New York, NY: Thieme Inc;
8. Krook M. Speaking fundamental frequency characteristics of normal 1990.
Swedish subjects. Folia Phoniatr (Basel). 1988;40:82–90. 38. Cooper I. The boy’s changing voice. Educat Music Mag Sep/Oct 1955.
9. Kuenzel HJ. How well does average speaking fundamental frequency cor- 39. McKenzie D. Training the Boy’s Changing Voice. New Brunswick, NJ:
relate with height and weight? Phonetica. 1989;46:117–125. Rutgers University Press; 1956.
10. Reich AR, Frederisckson RR, Mason JA, Schlauch RS. Methodological 40. Naidr J, Zobril M, Sevcik K. Pubertal voice changes in boys over a period of
variables affecting phonological frequency range in adults. J Speech five years. Folia Phoniatr. 1965;17:1–18.
Hear Res. 1990;55:124–131. 41. Swanson FJ. The proper care and feeding of changing voices. Music
11. Benjamin B. Frequency variability in the aged voice. J Gerontol. 1981;36: Educat. J. 1961;49:63–66.
722–726. 42. Swanson FJ. Male Singing Voices Ages 8–18. Cedar Rapids, IA: Lawrence
12. Brown WS Jr, Morris R, Hollien H, Howell E. Speaking fundamental fre- Press; 1977.
quency characteristics as a function of age and professional singing. J Voice. 43. Cooksey JM. The development of a contemporary theory for the training
1991;5:310–315. and cultivation of the junior high school male changing voice. Choral J
13. de Pinto O, Hollien H. Speaking fundamental frequency characteristics of 1977. October:part I, 5–14; November:part II, 5–16; December:part III,
Australian women: then and now. J Phonetics. 1982;10:367–375. 5–15; and January 1978:part IV, 5–18.
14. Decoster W, Debruyne F. The aging voice: changes in fundamental fre- 44. Cooksey JM. Voice transformation in male adolescents In: Thurman L,
quency, waveform stability and spectrum. Acta Otorhinolayrngol Belg. Welch G, eds. Bodymind and Voice: Foundations of Voice Education.
1997;51:105–112. Iowa City, IA: The Voice Care Network; 2000:731–733.
15. Harnesberger JD, Shrivastav R, Brown WS, Rothman HB, Hollien H. 45. Tanner JM. Growth at Adolescence. Springfield, IL: Charles C. Thomas;
Speaking rate and fundamental frequency as speech cues to perceived 1955.
age. J Voice. 2008;22:58–69. 46. Fournier E. Voix eunochoide, First International Congress Lauryn, Milan;
16. Horii Y, Ryan W. Fundamental frequency characteristics and perceived age 1882:19.
of adult male speakers. Folia Phoniatr (Basel). 1981;33:227–233. 47. Jerome EK. Change of voice in male adolescents. Q J Speech. 1937;23:
17. Linville S, Fisher H. Acoustic characteristics of women’s voices with 648–653.
advancing age. J Gerontol. 1985;40:324–330. 48. Negus VE. The Mechanism of the Larynx. St. Louis, MO: Mosby; 1929.
e40 Journal of Voice, Vol. 26, No. 2, 2012

49. Ramsey GV. Sexual growth in Negro and White boys. Hum Biol. 1950;22: 75. Hollien, H. Progress report on adolescent voice change, IASCP Report,
146–149. 0071, 1971:1–30
50. Weiss DA. The pubertal change of the human voice. Folia Phoniatr (Basel). 76. Hollien, H. Data on adolescent voice change, CSL Report 26: 68 to NIH,
1950;2:126–159. 1968.
51. Pedrey CP. A study of voice change in boys between the ages of eleven and 77. Hollien H. Adolescence and voice change. In: Transcripts of the Sympo-
sixteen. Speech Monogr. 1945;12:30–36. sium. Care of Professional Voice, Vol 2. New York, NY: The Voice Founda-
52. Curry ET. The pitch characteristics of the adolescent male voice. Speech tion; 1978:36–43.
Monogr. 1940;7:48–62. 78. Doherty ET, Hollien H. An indicator of the onset of puberty in males In:
53. Metfessel M. Techniques for the objective study of vocal art. Psychol Hollien H, Hollien PA, eds. Current Issues in the Phonetic Sciences.
Monogr. 1927;36:1–40. Amsterdam, The Netherlands: John Benjamins; 1978:945–953.
54. Simon CT. The variability of consecutive wave lengths in vocal instrumen- 79. Hollien H, Green R, Massey K. Longitudinal research on adolescent voice
tal sounds. Psychol Monogr. 1927;36:41–83. change in males. J Acoust Soc Am. 1994;96:2646–2654.
55. Cowan M. Pitch and intensity characteristics of stage speech. Arch Speech 80. Fairbanks G. Voice and Articulation Drillbook. New York, NY: Harper and
1936;Suppl:1–92. Row; 1960.
56. Behnnke E. The Mechanism of the Human Voice. 14th ed. London, UK: 81. Hollien H, Dew D, Phillips P. Phonational frequency range of adults.
J. Curwen; 1900. J Speech Hear Res. 1971;14:755–760.
57. Farnsworth DW. High speed motion pictures of the human vocal cords. Bell 82. Willis E, Kenny D. Relationship between weight, speaking fundamental
Lab Rec (and film). 1940;18:203–208. frequency, and appearance of phonational gaps in the adolescent male
58. Kenyon EL. Relation of oral articulative movements of speech and of changing voice. J Voice. 2008;22:431–471.
extrinsic laryngeal musculature in general to function of vocal cord. Arch 83. Cooksey JM. The male adolescent changing voice: some new perspectives.
Otolaryngol. 1927;5:481–501. Proceedings of Research Symposium on the Male Adolescent Voice. Vol 1.
59. Moore P. Vocal fold movement during vocalization. Speech Monogr. 1937; Buffalo, NY: SUNY; 1984:4–59
4:44–55. 84. Gelfer MP. Stability of phonational frequency range. J Commun Dis. 1989;
60. Moore P, von Leden H. Larynx and Voice: Function of the Normal 22:181–192.
Larynx. (motion picture). Chicago, IL: Gould Found., Laryngeal Res. 85. Nadoleczny M. Lehbrunch der Sprache und Stimmheilkunde. [Textbook of
Lab; 1956. languages and speech pathology]. Leipzig, Germany: Vogel; 1926.
61. Russell GO. Speech and Voice. New York, NY: Macmillan; 1931. 86. Boone DR, McFarlane SC. The Voice and Voice Therapy. 4th ed. Engle-
62. Sokolowsky RR. Effect of the extrinsic laryngeal muscles on voice produc- wood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall; 1988.
tion. Arch Otolaryngol. 1943;38:355–364. 87. Wilson DK. Voice Problems in Children. Baltimore, MD: Williams and
63. Hollien H, Moore GP. Measurements of the vocal fold during changes in Wilkins; 1979.
pitch. J Speech Hear Res. 1960;3:157–165. 88. Preece MA. Prepubertal and pubertal endocrinology In: Falkner F,
64. Van Den Berg J. Myoelastic-Aerodynamic Theory. J Speech Hear Res. Tanner JM, eds. Human Growth, Vol 2. New York, NY: Plenum Press;
1958;1:228–244. 1986:211–224.
65. Luchsinger R, Arnold GE. Voice-Speech-Language Clinical Communicol- 89. Sizonenko PC. Endocrinology in preadolescent and adolescents. Am J Dis
ogy: Its Physiology and Pathology. Belmont, CA: Wadsworth Publishing Child. 1978;132:704–712.
Company, Inc; 1965. 90. Wierman ME, Crowley WF Jr. Neuroendocrine control at the onset of
66. Pederson MF, Kitsing P, Krabbe S, Heramb S. The change of voice during puberty In: Falkner F, Tanner JM, eds. Human Growth, Vol 2. New York,
puberty in 11 to 16 years old choir singers measured with electroglotto- NY: Plenum Press; 1986:225–241.
graphic fundamental frequency analysis and compared to other phenomena 91. Tanner J. Growth and endocrinology of the adolescent In: Gardener L, ed.
of puberty. Acta Otolaryngol. 1982;386:189–192. Endocrine and Genetic Diseases of Childhood and Adolescence. Philadel-
67. Perello J. El cambir de vox en la pubertad y su patologia. [The camber of phia, PA: Saunders Co; 1975:14–64.
voice at puberty and its pathology]. Rev Iber Endocrinol. 1956;3:57–66. 92. Knorr D, Bidlingmaier O, Bufenandt HG, Ehrt-Wehle R. Plasma testoster-
68. Schilling R. Uber stimmwechselgeschichten. [About vocal change stories]. one in male puberty. Acta Endocrinol. 1974;75:181–194.
Folia Phoniatr. 1947;1:70–96. 93. Fuchs M, Froehlich M, Hentschel B, Stuermer W, Kruse E, Knauft D. Pre-
69. Tosi O, Postan D, Bianculli C. Longitudinal study of children’s voices at dicting mutational change in the speaking voice of boys. J Voice. 2007;21:
puberty. Proceedings of the XVI International Congress of Logopedics 169–178.
and Phoniatrics. Interlaken, Switzerland; 1974:486–490. 94. Frank F. Mutation in consideration and acceleration. Heilp€adagogik. 1969;
70. Hollien H, Malcik E. Adolescent voice change in southern Negro males. 12:18–20.
Speech Monogr. 1962;29:53–58. 95. Schilling A, Karthaus K. Acceleration and mutation. HNO. 1961;9:271–275.
71. Hollien H, Malcik E, Hollien B. Adolescent voice change in southern white 96. Doll J, Frank F, Sparber M. New evidence of vocal development in 12- to
males. Speech Monogr. 1965;32:87–90. 16-year-old boys. Sprache Stimme Geh€or. 1986;10:143–146.
72. Hollien H, Malcik E. Evaluation of cross-sectional studies of adolescent 97. Harries M, Walker J, Hawkins S, Williams D, Hughes I. Changes in the
voice change in males. Speech Monogr. 1967;34:80–84. male voice at puberty. Arch Dis Child. 1997;77:445–447.
73. Hollien H. Acoustics of Crime. New York, NY: Plenum; 1990. 98. Swanson F. Music Teaching in the Junior High and Middle School. Engle-
74. Hollien H, Hollien, PA. A cross-cultural study of adolescent voice change wood Cliff, NJ: Prentice Hall, Inc; 1973.
in European males. In: Proceedings of 7th International Congress Phonetic 99. Herman S. Building a Pyramid of Musicianship. San Diego, CA: Curtis
Sciences. The Hague, The Netherlands: Mouton and Co; 1972:332–357 Music Press; 1988. 95–97.

You might also like