Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 44

Experimental Psychology 7th Edition

Myers Test Bank


Visit to download the full and correct content document: https://testbankdeal.com/dow
nload/experimental-psychology-7th-edition-myers-test-bank/
Chapter 8

Chapter 8

Solving Problems: Controlling Extraneous Variables

Chapter Overview
Extraneous variables threaten an experiment’s internal validity when they are
allowed to confound the experiment. In well-designed experiments, only the
independent variable changes systematically across treatment conditions. This chapter
examines four kinds of extraneous variables (physical, social, personality, and context)
and the procedures used to control them.

Physical variables are aspects of the testing conditions that need to be controlled.
Elimination, constancy of conditions, and balancing are three techniques that
researchers use to control physical variables. While elimination is the most effective
strategy since it removes the extraneous variable from the experiment, it is not always
feasible. In these cases, a researcher can either use constancy of conditions or
balancing to ensure that the extraneous variable does not affect the treatment
conditions differently.

Experiments are social situations and can be confounded by uncontrolled social


variables, including demand characteristics and experimenter bias. Single-blind
experiments and cover stories can control demand characteristics, and double-blind
experiments can control both demand characteristics and experimenter bias. Since
cover stories involve deception, researchers must only use them when absolutely
necessary and the participants should be debriefed.

Personality variables, characteristics of experimenters and volunteer subjects, can


affect subject behavior in an experiment. We control these variables by standardizing
our procedures and minimizing face-to-face contact with subjects. Volunteers are more
sociable and intelligent than nonvolunteers, and these differences might decrease the
external validity of experiments that mainly recruit college student subjects.

Context variables are extraneous variables created by the experimental setting


itself. Subject selection of experiments and experimenter selection of subjects threaten
both an experiment’s internal and external validity. Experimenters should be aware that
they can show practice and fatigue effects over the course of an entire experiment and
should take precautions to control for them.

199
Chapter 8

Chapter Outline
Physical Variables
Elimination and Constancy
Balancing
Social Variables
Demand Characteristics
Experimenter Bias
Personality Variables
Experimenters
Volunteer Subjects
Context Variables
When the Subjects Select the Experiment
When the Experimenter Selects the Subjects
Some Folklore about Subjects
Summary
Key Terms
Review and Study Questions
Critical Thinking Exercise
Online Resources

Key Terms
DSBalancing Experimenter bias
Constancy of conditions Personality variables
Context variable Physical variables
Cover story Placebo effect
Demand characteristics Rosenthal effect
Double-blind experiment Single-blind experiment
Elimination Social variables

200
Chapter 8

Teaching Suggestions

Cengage Online Workshop Exercises

http://www.wadsworth.com/psychology_d/templates/student_resources/workshops/res_
methd/controls/controls_01.html
Many of the workshops referred to in the last chapter are relevant to the topics of this
chapter as well. For example, this is a workshop on various issues of “Control” in
experiments. Students should learn to answer:
Why is control an important part of experimentation?
What are inclusion and exclusion criteria?
Define the term “double-blind” and explain its usefulness
How can we control a between-subjects design? A within-subjects design?
What are order effects? How can we control for them?

http://www.wadsworth.com/psychology_d/templates/student_resources/workshops/res_
methd/confounds/confounds_01.html
The workshop on “Confounds” should be covered again here as well, as it overlaps with
content of the current chapter. Have students work through it and then ask them the
following:
What good is internal validity in an experiment?
How can you tell if you have construct validity?
What are confounds and why are they bad?
What is differential mortality?
What problems are reduced when you make your study double-blind?

Additional Web Resources

A classic article on Demand Characteristics is “The Nature and Role of Demand


Characteristics in Scientific Inquiry,” by Ralph Rosnow (2002). Check your library’s full-
text databases for it. The journal is Prevention & Treatment, Vol 5(1), Oct, 2002. ArtID:
37.

http://www.psywww.com/intropsych/ch08_animals/clever_hans.html
http://www.comnet.ca/~pballan/C2P1.htm
Above are just two of many sites that describe Clever Hans, the mathematical horse. No
discussion of demand characteristics or experimenter bias is complete without mention
of Hans.

http://www.psy.gla.ac.uk/~steve/hawth.html
This page presents a nice description of many of the topics discussed in Chapter 8:
Rosenthal effect, Hawthorne effect, Placebo effects, etc.

201
Chapter 8

Classroom/Homework Exercises

Handout 8-1: Identifying and Fixing Extraneous Variables


There are several extraneous variables in the experiment described below. Identify all
the extraneous variables you see. For each one, explain what type it is (e.g., social,
personality), whether or not it is confounding, and how you could eliminate or minimize
the problem it creates.

Jared and Molly are conducting an experiment on how performance is


affected by the presence of other people. They ask their participants to
solve a series of difficult math problems in one of two conditions: alone or
in a large group. All the volunteers for the Alone condition are recruited
from an Introductory Psychology class, and those for the Group condition
are recruited from a Social Psychology class. Jared tests the individuals in
the Alone condition, and Molly tests the Group condition. The two rooms
they have reserved are appropriate for the treatment sizes; Jared’s is very
small and Molly’s is an auditorium. While they are testing some of their
participants, the lights in the rooms go out for a minute or so (this happens
occasionally to both Jared and Molly). They each grade half of the
problems from each condition; but when Molly grades them, she marks
any problem wrong if the participant failed to show his or her work,
whereas Jared only marks a problem wrong if the answer was incorrect.

Identifying Extraneous Variables


Students often assume that published research is perfect, and thus fail to examine
previous studies with a critical eye. For this exercise, ask students to bring to class
published articles that they find interesting and comprehensible. Divide the class into
small groups and have each group work on one or two of the papers. Ask them to
scrutinize the Method section in order to identify possible extraneous variables. Have
them specify how these variables might have affected the data and then ask them to
indicate how they might fix the problems if they were to replicate the study.

202
Chapter 8

Key to Review and Study Questions

1. What are physical variables in an experiment?

Physical variables are aspects of the testing conditions that need to be controlled.
These include the day of the week, time of day, the testing room, and potential
distractions like noise.

2. How is elimination used as a control procedure? Give two examples of variables


that could be controlled by elimination.

Elimination controls extraneous variables by removing them from an experiment. A


researcher could eliminate noise by testing subjects in a soundproof room and
visual distraction by closing window blinds.

3. What is constancy of conditions? Give two examples of variables that could be


controlled through constancy of conditions.

Constancy of conditions prevents confounding by holding extraneous variables


constant across treatment conditions. An experimenter could control the effect of
room color by testing all subjects in the same room. She could control the effect of
instructions by reading directions from a printed script.

4. What is balancing? Give two examples of variables that could be controlled by


balancing.

Balancing prevents confounding by distributing the effects of extraneous variables


equally across treatment conditions. We could control the effect of experimenter
personality by asking each experimenter to run half the subjects in each treatment
group. We could control the effect of time of day by running half of each treatment
group in the morning and the remaining half in the evening.

5. You are doing a study at a local school. Because of the way things are scheduled,
you can have one small testing room in the morning and another much larger
testing room in the afternoon. If you have two treatment conditions (A and B), how
can you assign subjects to the testing rooms so that the type of room will not lead
to confounding in your experiment?

Room size is a potentially confounding physical variable. Since you are required to
use two rooms, the best control procedure is balancing. After you randomly assign
subjects to condition A or B, you could randomly assign half of each treatment
group to the small room and half to the large room.

203
Chapter 8

6. What are demand characteristics?

Demand characteristics are aspects of the experimental situation that demand or


elicit specific responses that subjects believe are expected of them in the
experiment. For example, Orne and Scheibe’s (1964) “sensory deprivation”
experiment employed cues like an “Emergency Tray” and white medical coats to
manipulate expectations of sensory distortion.

How do they affect our data?

Since demand characteristics can consciously or unconsciously alter subject


behavior, they can distort data and confound an experiment.

How can they be controlled?

Researchers can control demand characteristics using single-blind experiments


and cover stories. In a single-blind experiment, subjects do not know which
treatment they are receiving. A cover story is an alternative procedure where a
researcher provides a plausible but false explanation of the experimental
procedures to disguise the research hypothesis.

7. A researcher says, “I want my experiment to be a success. I’m sure my hypothesis


is correct, so I’ll just give my subjects a couple of hints here and there. You know,
maybe a wink now and then if they give a good answer. That way I’ll really be able
to show that my independent variable had an effect.”

a. How would you convince her that her plan is faulty?

I would explain that providing different demand characteristics for each


treatment condition constitutes experimenter bias that would confound the
experiment. She could not separate the effects of her cues from the changes
produced by the independent variable and therefore could not draw causal
conclusions.

b. What is a double-blind experiment? Would you recommend that she use it?
Why or why not?

In a double-blind experiment, neither the subjects nor the experimenter know


the treatment the subjects are receiving. Given the researcher’s eagerness to
confirm her experimental hypothesis, a double-blind experiment is necessary
to keep her honest by preventing her from selectively providing hints to
subjects in different conditions. If she doesn’t know their experimental
condition, she can’t systematically influence them through demand
characteristics.

204
Chapter 8

8. Dr. L. is planning a large-scale learning experiment. He would like to have 100 rats
in one treatment group and another 100 in the other group. Because he needs so
many rats, he says, “Well, I can’t test all these animals by myself. I’ll ask Dr. P. to
help me. He can run the animals in the one group while I test the animals in the
other group.”

a. Knowing what you know about confounding, is Dr. L.’s solution a good
one? What can happen if one experimenter tests all the subjects in one group
while another tests all the subjects in another group?

Dr. L.’s solution could confound the experiment with experimenter bias since
the two researchers may have different expectations about the treatment
groups’ performance and may not handle the animals in the same manner.

b. Given what you know about balancing procedures, work out a better plan for
Dr. L.

The two researchers should be randomly assigned to run 50 rats from each
treatment group to balance the effects of experimenter bias across both
conditions.

9. When should a cover story be used?

A cover story should be used when there is a risk that subjects will guess the
experimental hypothesis and jeopardize an experiment’s internal validity.

When shouldn’t one be used?

Cover stories should not be used when they are unnecessary or when they
preclude informed consent by misrepresenting an experiment’s potential risks and
benefits.

Discuss the ethical problem raised by the use of cover stories.

Since cover stories are a form of deception, they should only be used when
needed to protect internal validity. Experimenters should fully debrief subjects after
their participation in the study.

10. Why is it important that an experimenter behave in the same way toward all
subjects in the experiment?

Rosenthal’s research documented the risk of experimenter bias. Failure of an


experimenter to behave the same way toward all subjects could confound an
experiment and prevent drawing causal conclusions.

205
Chapter 8

What precautions can be taken to ensure that he or she does this?

A double-blind experiment controls for experimenter bias since neither the subjects
nor the experimenter know the treatment the subjects are receiving.

11. In what ways are volunteer subjects different from nonvolunteers?

Volunteer subjects are more sociable and score higher in social desirability than
nonvolunteers. They hold more liberal social and political attitudes, tend to be less
authoritarian, and score higher on intelligence tests than nonvolunteers.

What difference does it make?

These differences could limit the external validity of social psychological and
cognitive studies that may be affected by these subject variables.

12. Find out the names of several experiments that are being conducted in your
department this term. Evaluate each name in terms of its potential to bias the
sample.

The answers will depend on study availability and advertising (class


announcements, e-mail, newspaper ads, sign-up boards, and web sign-up).

13. Think of two things an experimenter could do to safeguard against fatigue effects
during the course of the experiment.

Experimenters who run long or numerous experimental sessions risk fatigue.


Precautions against fatigue effects include short experimental sessions, breaks
between sessions, use of multiple experimenters distributed equally across
treatment conditions, and minimizing face-to-face contact with subjects.

206
Chapter 8

Test Questions

Multiple Choice

1. (F) Physical Variables


____ are aspects of the testing conditions that need to be controlled.
a. Context variables
b. Demand characteristics
* c. Physical variables
d. Social variables

2. (F) Physical Variables


Which of these could be a physical variable in an experiment?
a. level of illumination
b. temperature
c. time of day
* d. all of these

3. (A) Physical Variables


When Heather ran all the subjects in condition A on Thursday and those in
condition B on Friday, she confounded her experiment with a ____ variable.
a. context
b. personality
* c. physical
d. social

4. (A) Physical Variables


Tim prevented noise from distracting his subjects by testing them in a
soundproof room. This illustrates the control technique called
a. balancing.
b. constancy of conditions.
* c. elimination.
d. isolation.

5. (F) Physical Variables


Experimenters remove potentially confounding extraneous variables when
they employ
a. balancing.
b. constancy of conditions.
c. counterbalancing.
* d. elimination.

207
Chapter 8

6. (C) Physical Variables


Sometimes physical variables like ____ cannot be controlled through
elimination.
a. a blinking fluorescent light
b. noise from a custodian’s vacuum
c. seeing classmates walk by a window
* d. the weather

7. (F) Physical Variables


In ____, an experimenter controls physical variables by keeping all aspects of
treatment conditions as nearly similar as possible.
a. balancing
* b. constancy of conditions
c. elimination
d. isolation

8. (C) Physical Variables


Which of these should be controlled using constancy of conditions instead of
elimination?
a. a poster for a controversial speaker
* b. bright room lighting
c. interruptions by students opening the door
d. visual distraction by classmates outside the room

9. (A) Physical Variables


Michele ran all treatment conditions during the evening to control for the effect
of time of day. Which control technique did she use?
a. balancing
* b. constancy of conditions
c. elimination
d. isolation

10. (A) Physical Variables


Lindsay videotaped instructions for subjects to ensure that all subjects in each
condition receive the same information. This illustrates the control procedure
of
a. balancing.
* b. constancy of conditions.
c. counterbalancing.
d. elimination.

208
Chapter 8

11. (C) Physical Variables


Sometimes we cannot remove physical variables and it is impractical to hold
them steady. In these cases, we can control them using
* a. balancing.
b. constancy of conditions.
c. elimination.
d. isolation.

12. (F) Physical Variables


Balancing is a control procedure that
* a. distributes the effect of physical variables equally across the treatment
conditions.
b. distributes the effect of the independent variable equally across all of the
treatment conditions.
c. ensures that subjects in all conditions experience nearly identical physical
variables.
d. removes potentially confounding physical variables from the testing
situation.

13. (A) Physical Variables


Although James wanted to run all the subjects in his experiment by himself,
he realized that he couldn’t complete the experiment in time without a second
experimenter. To control for experimenter personality, he ran half the subjects
in each condition and his roommate ran the other half. This strategy is called
* a. balancing.
b. constancy of conditions.
c. elimination.
d. isolation.

14. (C) Physical Variables


When an extraneous variable systematically changes across treatments,
a. the extraneous variable has the same value in the control and
experimental conditions.
* b. the extraneous variable has one value in the control condition and a
different value in the experimental condition.
c. the independent variable has no effect on the dependent variable.
d. this presents a lower risk of confounding than when it changes randomly.

209
Chapter 8

15. (C) Physical Variables


By randomly assigning subjects to treatment conditions, many extraneous
variables are automatically ____ across conditions.
* a. balanced
b. eliminated
c. isolated
d. kept constant

16. (A) Physical Variables


Chase is designing an experiment that requires intense concentration and is
concerned about confounding by outside noise. If he can use any of the
control techniques discussed in this chapter, his first choice should be
a. balancing.
b. constancy of conditions.
* c. elimination.
d. isolation.

17. (F) Social Variables


Which of these are social variables?
a. demand characteristics
b. experimenter biases
c. response sets
* d. demand characteristics and experimenter bias

18. (F) Social Variables


The demand characteristics of an experiment may result in a subject
a. following his/her own interpretation of the experimenter’s directions.
b. trying to confirm the experimental hypothesis.
c. trying to guess the experimental hypothesis.
* d. all of these

19. (A) Social Variables


Anna participates in a social psychological experiment in which she observes
pairs of lights that are briefly turned on and off in a completely darkened
room. Although the distance between each pair of lights in the Hex paradigm
is always identical, the experimenter asks, “Is the first pair of lights farther
apart or closer together than the second pair?” This question, which implies
that Anna should perceive different spacing, illustrates the concept of a
* a. demand characteristic.
b. physical variable.
c. placebo effect.
d. Rosenthal effect.

210
Chapter 8

20. (F) Social Variables


Experimenters generally want subjects to be as naïve as possible concerning
the experimental hypothesis to reduce confounding by
a. context variables.
* b. demand characteristics.
c. experimenter bias.
d. response sets.

21. (C) Social Variables


In Orne and Scheibe’s (1964) sensory deprivation experiment, the
“Emergency Tray” and alarm button in the experimental condition were
* a. demand characteristics.
b. extraneous variables.
c. latent cues.
d. placebos.

22. (F) Social Variables


In single-blind experiments,
a. experimenters are unaware of the treatments given to subjects.
b. experimenters encourage subjects to guess the experimental hypothesis.
* c. subjects do not know which treatment they are receiving.
d. subjects do not receive feedback on their performance.

23. (F) Social Variables


Single-blind experiments only control for
a. context variables.
* b. demand characteristics.
c. experimenter bias.
d. response set.

24. (A) Social Variables


Russ suffers from social anxiety and participated in a 6-month study of a drug
developed to treat this disorder. While the physician who provided his
medication knew that Russ was in the experimental group, he did not disclose
this information to Russ until the study was completed. Russ participated in
a(n)
a. double-blind experiment.
b. ex post facto study.
c. quasi-experiment.
* d. single-blind experiment.

211
Chapter 8

25. (C) Social Variables


Placebo effects in drug trials are due to
* a. demand characteristics.
b. experimenter bias.
c. maturation threat.
d. response set.

26. (F) Social Variables


Which of these techniques to control demand characteristics involves
deception?
* a. cover story
b. double-blind experiment
c. quasi-experiment
d. single-blind experiment

27. (C) Social Variables


Hannover and Kuhnan (2002) informed their subjects were that they would be
participating in a study of eye-witness identification and that they would be
part of a “police line-up.” This constituted a ____ to disguise the experimental
hypothesis.
* a. cover story
b. double-blind experiment
c. quasi-experiment
d. single-blind experiment

28. (C) Social Variables


Researchers should not routinely employ cover stories because they
a. are a departure from fully-informed consent.
b. can confound a study with demand characteristics.
c. involve deception.
* d. are a departure from fully-informed consent and involve deception.

29. (F) Social Variables


____ is a form of confounding due to an experimenter providing implicit or
explicit cues to subjects to respond in a particular way.
* a. Experimenter bias
b. History threat
c. Response set
d. Response style

212
Chapter 8

30. (A) Social Variables


Tammy’s experiment compared the effectiveness of videotape and textbook
presentation of a history lesson. Unexpectedly, her demeanor changed with
each treatment condition. She appeared worried during the videotape
condition because the audiovisual equipment periodically broke down. In
contrast, she seemed relaxed in the textbook condition during which subjects
read from a book. Which problem does this illustrate?
a. confounding by a context variable
* b. experimenter bias
c. history threat
d. selection threat

31. (A) Social Variables


Graduate students who were told their rats were either “maze-bright” or
“stupid” observed dramatic differences in maze performance. The “maze-
bright” rats quickly learned to run the maze while the “stupid” rats did poorly.
What do these results illustrate?
a. experimenter bias
b. selection threat
c. the Rosenthal effect
* d. experimenter bias and the Rosenthal effect

32. (F) Social Variables


In animal studies, experimenters should not handle animals in treatment
conditions differently than those in control conditions since this would be a
form of
a. demand characteristic.
* b. experimenter bias.
c. history threat.
d. the Rosenthal effect.

33. (A) Social Variables


A swimming instructor, who was told his athletes had outstanding motor
coordination and the potential to become excellent swimmers, coached them
so well that they became excellent swimmers even though their motor skills
were really about average. These results illustrate
a. experimenter bias.
b. selection threat.
c. the placebo effect.
* d. the Rosenthal effect.

213
Chapter 8

34. (C) Social Variables


Experimenters sometimes make errors in recording data that tend to favor the
experimental hypothesis. This is an example of
a. demand characteristics.
* b. experimenter bias.
c. the Rosenthal effect.
d. demand characteristics and experimenter bias.

35. (F) Social Variables


Rosenthal proposed all of these explanations of how “special” teacher
expectations affected student performance on IQ tests except
a. teachers gave “bloomers” more feedback.
b. teachers gave “bloomers” more chances to ask questions.
* c. teachers rewarded “bloomers” with increased privileges.
d. teachers taught “bloomers” more challenging material.

36. (F) Social Variables


In a double-blind experiment,
a. control and experimental subjects are told cover stories.
b. experimenters do not know which treatment the subjects are receiving.
c. subjects do not know which treatment they are receiving.
* d. neither the subjects nor the experimenter know which treatment the
subjects are receiving.

37. (A) Social Variables


Researchers implanted electrical devices into patients diagnosed with
congestive heart failure. They randomly assigned patients to either an
experimental condition (device turned on) or control condition (device turned
off) for 6 months. Neither the subjects nor the supervising physicians knew
their treatment condition until the study ended. Which design did these
researchers use?
* a. double-blind experiment
b. ex post facto study
c. pretest/posttest
d. single-blind experiment

38. (C) Social Variables


What does a double-blind experiment control?
a. demand characteristics
b. experimenter bias
c. confounding by context variables
* d. demand characteristics and experimenter bias

214
Chapter 8

39. (F) Social Variables


Wasson and colleagues’ (1984) study of outpatient health care in elderly men
kept both the patients and health-care providers from knowing
a. the medication administered to the patient.
b. the patient’s admitting diagnosis.
c. the patient’s prognosis.
* d. whether the patient was assigned to only one health-care provider.

40. (C) Social Variables


A double-blind experiment is not possible in studies like the hypothetical
investigation of cartoons and children’s art described in this chapter. Instead,
an experimenter can take precautions like keeping the person who scores
their drawings “blind” to their treatment condition, and standardizing testing
and scoring procedures. These steps help control confounding due to
a. context variables.
* b. experimenter bias.
c. Rosenthal effects.
d. subject selection threat.

41. (F) Personality Variables


Rosenthal reported that when an experimenter acts in a friendly manner,
subjects sometimes
a. achieve higher scores on adjustment tests.
b. achieve higher scores on intelligence tests.
c. learn better.
* d. all of these

42. (F) Personality Variables


All of these describe experimenters who score high on the social desirability
scale except
a. they are more enthusiastic.
b. they are more friendly.
* c. they obtain more “I don’t know” answers from respondents.
d. they smile more at their subjects.

43. (F) Personality Variables


____ interviewers often obtain the best data and the most usable responses.
a. Aggressive
b. Authoritarian
c. Hostile
* d. Likeable

215
Chapter 8

44. (F) Personality Variables


Experimenters who have a ____ demeanor obtain the most natural subject
behavior.
a. distant
b. hostile
* c. pleasant
d. warm

45. (A) Personality Variables


Zack is excited about how experimenter personality can positively influence
subject performance. He says, “This is so cool! I’m going to smile at all my
experimental subjects and they’re going to perform like Pavlov’s dog for me.”
What issue has Zack overlooked?
a. external validity
* b. internal validity
c. selection threat
d. selection-interaction threat

46. (A) Personality Variables


Pat’s relationship with his girlfriend during the 3 weeks he ran subjects
resembled a very scary roller coaster ride. Their frequent fights and making
up affected his performance in the lab. Some days he was angry and
withdrawn while on others he was happy and outgoing. His inconsistent
demeanor in both treatment conditions probably
a. increased statistical power.
b. made it harder to detect the effect of the IV.
c. made subject responses more variable.
* d. made it harder to detect the effect of the IV and made subject responses
more variable.

47. (A) Personality Variables


Keesha’s study involved deception. She was supposed to truthfully inform
control subjects that they might not have time to solve a difficult puzzle and lie
to experimental subjects that previous subjects completed the same puzzle
with time to spare. Sadly, she has no talent for lying. While she was relaxed in
the control condition, she was visibly anxious in the experimental condition.
This systematic change in her emotional state
* a. confounded her experiment.
b. made it easier to detect the effect of the IV.
c. made subject responses in each condition more variable.
d. made it easier to detect the effect of the IV and made subject responses
in each condition more variable.

216
Chapter 8

48. (F) Personality Variables


When you are the only experimenter, the best way to control the effect of your
personality on subject performance is to
a. adjust experimental procedures to each subject’s personality.
b. adjust your demeanor to each subject’s personality.
* c. minimize face-to-face contact.
d. adjust experimental procedures and your demeanor to each subject’s
personality.

49. (F) Personality Variables


Volunteer subjects may differ from nonvolunteers in that volunteers may be
a. less authoritarian.
b. more politically and socially liberal.
c. more intelligent.
* d. all of these

50. (C) Personality Variables


McNemar’s (1946) statement that “The existing science of human behavior is
largely the science of the behavior of sophomores” challenged the ____ of
academic research.
a. demand characteristics
b. experimenter bias
* c. external validity
d. internal validity

51. (F) Context Variables


All of these are context variables except
a. experiment selection by subjects.
b. how experimenters assign subjects to conditions.
c. how experimenters recruit subjects.
* d. how experimenters score data.

52. (C) Context Variables


When subjects sign up for studies that are scheduled at a convenient time or
have an exciting title, this can
* a. bias the sample.
b. confound the experiment.
c. reduce internal validity.
d. result in selection threat.

217
Chapter 8

53. (A) Context Variables


Cat was concerned that she might have difficulty obtaining subjects because
it was late in the semester. She brainstormed with her teammates and then
said, “Most of the experiments advertised this semester have totally boring
titles like ‘The Attribution Experiment.’ If we select an exciting title like ‘The
Extreme Sports Study,’ we’ll recruit hundreds of subjects.” The problem with
Cat’s solution is that it could
a. confound her experiment.
* b. reduce external validity.
c. reduce internal validity.
d. result in selection threat.

54. (A) Context Variables


Select the title that best follows the authors’ suggestions for obtaining an
unbiased sample.
a. “The Extreme Body Art Study”
b. “The Heavy Metal Music Experiment”
c. “The Sexual Practices Survey”
* d. “The Social Attribution Experiment”

55. (A) Context Variables


Timothy is a shy undergraduate who was pressured by his roommate to help
run a field experiment where he had to ask mall shoppers a large or small
favor. Since he found most subjects intimidating, he only approached
grandmothers and stray children in the large favor condition, and impatient
shoppers in the small favor condition. Timothy’s behavior
* a. confounded the experiment.
b. illustrated the Rosenthal effect.
c. introduced a demand characteristic.
d. was ethically inappropriate.

218
Chapter 8

Short Answer/Essay

1. Suppose you are trying to conduct your experiment, but the room has an
annoying noise. Describe the three strategies for controlling physical variables
that you should use to fix the problem.

2. Which of the three strategies for controlling physical variables is the best?
Second best? Why isn’t the best of the three strategies used all the time?

3. What are physical variables? How can they be controlled? Give an example.

4. What does it mean to “balance” an extraneous variable? Is it a good idea to


balance them? Why or why not?

5. What are social variables? Describe the two principal types of social variables.

6. What are demand characteristics? Identify a demand characteristic in the room


you are sitting in.

7. Participants in experiments often try to guess the hypothesis. Why is this a


problem?

8. State two ways you can control for demand characteristics.

9. Give an example of the placebo effect. What can be done about this problem?

10. What is meant by a cover story in research? What is its purpose? Why not
always use one?

11. What is experimenter bias? Give an example. What can be done about
experimenter bias?

12. What is the Rosenthal effect? How might this threaten an experiment? Explain.

13. What are people blind to in a “blind” experiment? Who is blind in a double-blind
experiment?

14. Give an example in which a personality variable endangers a study. What can be
done to reduce the effects of personality variables?

15. Which is more likely to affect the results of an experiment: the personality of the
researcher or the personalities of the participants? Justify your answer.

16. What are context variables? How are they different from other types of
extraneous variables?

219
Chapter 8

17. Why is it a problem when subjects select the experiments in which they
participate?

18. Give an example of a problem created by a researcher selecting his or her own
participants.

19. Name and describe four categories of extraneous variables.

20. Give an example of an experiment in which there is a social variable that is not
confounding and another example in which there is a physical variable that is
confounding. You may combine the two problems into one experiment if you
wish.

220
Another random document with
no related content on Scribd:
The Project Gutenberg eBook of The boy
mechanic, book 3
This ebook is for the use of anyone anywhere in the United
States and most other parts of the world at no cost and with
almost no restrictions whatsoever. You may copy it, give it away
or re-use it under the terms of the Project Gutenberg License
included with this ebook or online at www.gutenberg.org. If you
are not located in the United States, you will have to check the
laws of the country where you are located before using this
eBook.

Title: The boy mechanic, book 3


800 things for boys to do

Editor: H. H. Windsor

Release date: October 12, 2023 [eBook #71856]

Language: English

Original publication: Chicago: Popular Mechanics Co, 1919

Credits: Charlene Taylor, Harry Lamé and the Online Distributed


Proofreading Team at https://www.pgdp.net (This file
was produced from images generously made available
by The Internet Archive/American Libraries.)

*** START OF THE PROJECT GUTENBERG EBOOK THE BOY


MECHANIC, BOOK 3 ***
Please see the Transcriber’s Notes
at the end of this text.
New original cover art included with
this eBook is granted to the public
domain.
THE BOY MECHANIC
BOOK 3
See Page 86
The
Boy Mechanic
BOOK 3

800 THINGS FOR BOYS TO

DO

HOW TO CONSTRUCT
ELECTRIC LOCOMOTIVE MODEL AND TRACK SYSTEM, BOYS’
MOTOR
CAR, PARCEL DELIVERY BICYCLE, AERIAL CABLEWAY, MINIA-
TURE TANK, SAILING CANOE, HOUSEBOAT, SUBMARINE
CAMERA, DIVING TOWER, HAMMOCKS, KITCHEN
FOR HIKERS, ICE YACHT

AND

HUNDREDS OF OTHER THINGS WHICH DELIGHT


EVERY BOY
WITH 802 ILLUSTRATIONS

COPYRIGHTED, 1919, BY H. H. WINDSOR

CHICAGO
POPULAR MECHANICS CO.
PUBLISHERS
Fig. 1 Fig. 2
SECTIONAL SIDE VIEW FRONT VIEW
Fig. 3 Fig. 4
PLAN BRAKE DETAIL
Fig. 5
DETAIL OF SUPPORT C
DETAIL OF STEERING GEAR
The General Arrangement of the Parts DETAIL OF SUPPORT D
is Shown in the Assembly Views, Figs. REAR-AXLE BRACKET E
1, 2, and 3. The Brake Detail, Fig. 4, Fig. 6
should be Considered with Fig. 9,
Shown Separately. The Detail
Construction of the Frame and Body
can be Readily Understood by
Referring to the Assembly Views in
Connection with Fig. 7
Fig. 7
DETAIL OF FRAME AND BODY
A Boys’ Motor Car
HOMEMADE
by P.P. Avery

E ven though the home-built “bearcat” roadster, or other favorite


model, does not compare in every detail with the luxurious
manufactured cars, it has an individuality that puts it in a class by
itself. The amateur mechanic, or the ambitious boy, who is fairly
skilled with tools, can build at least the main parts for his own small
car, of the simple, practical design shown in the sketch and detailed
in the working drawings. If necessary, he can call more skilled
mechanics to his aid. A motorcycle engine, or other small gasoline
motor, is used for the power plant. The control mechanism of the
engine and the electrical connections are similar to those of a
motorcycle. They are installed to be controlled handily from the
driver’s seat. The car is built without springs, but these may be
included, if desired, or the necessary comfort provided—in part at
least—by a cushioned seat. Strong bicycle wheels are used, the 1¹⁄₂
by 28-in. size being suitable. The hood may be of wood, or of sheet
metal, built over a frame of strap iron. The top of the hood can be
lifted off, and the entire hood can also be removed, when repairs are
to be made. The tool box on the rear of the frame can be replaced by
a larger compartment, or rack, for transporting loads, or an extra
seat for a passenger.
To Simplify This Small but Serviceable Motor Car for Construction by the
Young Mechanic, Only the Essential Parts are Considered. Other Useful
and Ornamental Features may be Added as the Skill and Means of the
Builder Make Possible

The construction may be begun with the chassis and the running
gear. Fit the wheels with ⁵⁄₈-in. axles, as shown in the assembly
views, Figs. 1, 2, and 3, and detailed in Fig. 4. Fit the ends of the
axles to the hubs of the wheels, providing the threaded ends with
lock nuts. Make the wooden supports for the frame, as detailed in
Fig. 6. The axles are fastened into half-round grooves, cut in the
bottoms of the supports, and secured by iron straps, as shown in
Fig. 4, at A. Make the sidepieces for the main frame 2¹⁄₂ by 3¹⁄₄ in.
thick, and 9 ft. 4 in. long, as detailed in Fig. 7. Mortise the supports
through the sidepieces, and bore the holes for the bolt fastenings
and braces. Glue the mortise-and-tenon joints before the bolts are
finally secured. Provide the bolts with washers, and lock the nuts
with additional jam nuts where needed. Keep the woodwork clean,
and apply a coat of linseed oil, so that dirt and grease cannot
penetrate readily.
Finish only the supporting structure of the chassis in the
preliminary woodwork. Set the front-axle and steering-rigging
supports C and D, and adjust the spacers F between them. Bore the
hole for the kingbolt, as detailed in Fig. 6, and fit the bevel gears and
the fifth wheel G, of ¹⁄₄-in. steel, into place, as shown in Fig. 5. The
gear H is bolted to the axle support. The pinion J is set on the end of
a short ³⁄₄-in. shaft. The latter passes through the support D, and is
fitted with washers and jam nuts, solidly, yet with sufficient play. A
bracket, K, of ¹⁄₄ by 1³⁄₄-in. strap iron, braces the shaft, as shown in
Fig. 3. The end of this short shaft is joined to one section of the
universal coupling, as shown, and, like the other half of the coupling,
is pinned with a ³⁄₁₆-in. riveted pin. The pinion is also pinned, and the
lower end of the kingbolt provided with a washer and nut, guarded by
a cotter pin. Suitable gears can be procured from old machinery. A
satisfactory set was obtained from an old differential of a well-known
small car.
Fig. 8
Detail of the Motor Support: The Engine is Mounted on Reinforced Angle
Irons, and Secured by Clamps and a Supporting Band under the Crank Case

Before fitting the steering column into place, make the dashboard,
of ⁷⁄₈-in. oak, as shown in the assembly view, and in detail in Fig. 7. It
is 19¹⁄₂ in. high and 2 ft. 4 in. wide, and set on the frame and braced
to it with 4 by 4 by 1¹⁄₂-in. angle irons, ¹⁄₄ in. thick. Fit a ⁷⁄₈-in. strip of
wood around the edge of the dashboard, on the front side, as a rest
for the hood, as shown in Figs. 1 and 7, at L. A brass edging protects
the dashboard, and gives a neat appearance. Lay out carefully the
angle for the steering column, which is of ⁷⁄₈-in. shafting, so as to be
convenient for the driver. Mark the point at which it is to pass through
the dashboard, and reinforce the hole with an oak block, or an angle
flange, of iron or brass, such as is used on railings, or boat fittings. A
collar at the flange counteracts the downward pressure on the
steering post. The 12-in. steering wheel is set on the column by a
riveted pin.
The fitting of the engine may next be undertaken. The exact
position and method of setting the engine on the frame will depend
on the size and type. It should be placed as near the center as
possible, to give proper balance. The drawings show a common air-
cooled motor of the one-cylinder type. It is supported, as shown in
Figs. 1 and 3 and detailed in Fig. 8. Two iron strips, B, riveted to 1¹⁄₂
by 1¹⁄₂-in. angle irons, extend across the main frame, and support
the engine by means of bolts and steel clamps, designed to suit the
engine. Cross strips of iron steady the engine, and the clamps are
bolted to the crank case. The center clamp is a band that passes
under the crank case.
The engine is set so that the crankshaft extends across the main
frame. Other methods may be devised for special motors, and the
power transmission changed correspondingly. One end of the
crankshaft is extended beyond the right side of the frame, as shown
in Fig. 3. This extension is connected to the shaft by means of an
ordinary setscrew collar coupling. A block M, Figs. 3 and 7, is bolted
to the frame, and a section of heavy brass pipe fitted as a bearing.
The ignition and oiling systems, carburetor, and other details of the
engine control and allied mechanism, are the same as those used on
the motorcycle engine originally, fitted up as required. The oil tank is
made of a strong can, mounted on the dashboard, as shown in Figs.
1 and 2. It is connected with the crank case by copper tubing. A cut-
out switch for the ignition system is mounted on the dashboard. The
controls used for the engine of the motorcycle can be extended with
light iron rods, and the control handles mounted on the dashboard or
in other convenient position. The throttle can be mounted on the
steering column by fitting an iron pipe around the post and mounting
this pipe in the angle flange at the dashboard. A foot accelerator may
also be used, suitable mountings and pedal connections being
installed at the floor.
In setting the gasoline tank, make only as much of the body
woodwork as is necessary to support it, as shown in Figs. 1, 3, and
7. The tank may be made of a can, properly fitted, and heavy
enough, as determined by comparison with gasoline tanks in
commercial cars. The feed is through a copper tube, as shown in
Fig. 1. A small venthole, to guard against a vacuum in the tank,
should be made in the cap. The muffler from a motorcycle is used,
fitted with a longer pipe, and suspended from the side of the frame.
The transmission of the power from the motor shaft to the right
rear wheel is accomplished by means of a leather motorcycle belt,
made by fitting leather washers close together over a bicycle chain,
oiling the washers with neat’s-foot oil. A grooved iron pulley is fitted
on the end of the motor shaft, and a grooved pulley rim on the rear
wheel, as shown in Figs. 1 and 3, and detailed in Fig. 4. The motor is
started by means of a crank, and the belt drawn up gradually, by the
action of a clutch lever and its idler, detailed in Fig. 9. The clutch
lever is forged, as shown, and fitted with a ratchet lever, N, and
ratchet quadrant, O. The idler holds the belt to the tension desired,
giving considerable flexibility of speed.
The brake is shown in Figs. 1 and 3, and detailed in Figs. 4 and 9.
The fittings on the rear wheel and axle are made of wood, and
bolted, with a tension spring, as shown. The brake drum is supported
on iron bands, riveted to the wheel, and to the pulley rim. The brake
arm is connected to the brake wheel by a flexible wire. When the
pedal is forced down, the wire is wound on the brake wheel, thus
permitting of adjustment. The pedal is of iron and fixed on its shaft
with a setscrew. An iron pipe is used as a casing for the central
shaft, the shaft carrying the clutch lever, and the pipe carrying the
brake pedal and the brake wheel. The quadrant O is mounted on a
block, fastened to the main frame. The central shaft is carried in
wooden blocks, with iron caps. A catch of strap iron can be fitted on
the floor, to engage the pedal, and lock the brake when desired.

You might also like