GiftedChildQuarterly 2015 RinnBishop 213 35

You might also like

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 24

See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.

net/publication/282521290

Gifted Adults

Article in Gifted Child Quarterly · October 2015


DOI: 10.1177/0016986215600795

CITATIONS READS
53 23,916

2 authors:

Anne N. Rinn James Bishop


University of North Texas University of North Texas
87 PUBLICATIONS 1,940 CITATIONS 10 PUBLICATIONS 101 CITATIONS

SEE PROFILE SEE PROFILE

All content following this page was uploaded by James Bishop on 09 March 2016.

The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file.


600795
research-article2015
GCQXXX10.1177/0016986215600795Gifted Child QuarterlyRinn and Bishop

Article

Gifted Child Quarterly

Gifted Adults: A Systematic Review and


2015, Vol. 59(4) 213­–235
© 2015 National Association for
Gifted Children
Analysis of the Literature Reprints and permissions:
sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.nav
DOI: 10.1177/0016986215600795
gcq.sagepub.com

Anne N. Rinn1 and James Bishop1

Abstract
What happens when a gifted child grows up? Despite a slew of provocative book titles regarding gifted adults in the
mainstream media, and the inclusion of the notion of giftedness among adults in the definition of giftedness proposed by
Subotnik, Olszewski-Kubilius, and Worrell, there is a limited amount of research that has examined the gifted adult. In this
systematic review of the literature, we categorized the available research literature into nine thematic areas: (a) whether or
not gifted children become gifted adults; (b) family of origin; (c) effects of early educational experiences; (d) characteristics of
gifted adults; (e) career; (f) family of procreation; (g) career and family interaction; (h) life goals, satisfaction, and well-being;
and (i) counseling. Implications for gifted adults and the field of gifted education are discussed.

Keywords
adults, giftedness, eminence, lifespan

What happens when a gifted child grows up? Educators, par- Gifted Children Grow Up (Freeman, 2010), Gifted Workers:
ents, researchers, policy makers, and counselors spend a con- Hitting the Target (Nauta & Ronner, 2013), Remarkable
siderable amount of time planning for, and attending to, the Women: Perspectives on Female Talent Development
educational, cognitive, and socioemotional needs of gifted (Arnold, Noble, & Subotnik, 1995), and Living with Intensity:
children, but for what purpose? To ensure the gifted child is Understanding the Sensitivity, Excitability, and the Emotional
adequately intellectually stimulated during childhood and Development of Gifted Children, Adolescents, and Adults
that his or her social and emotional needs are met? Do we (Daniels & Piechowski, 2008). In addition, some organiza-
also have a bigger end goal? Many in the field of gifted edu- tions serve as outlets for gifted adults: Mensa International,
cation would argue that we want to prepare gifted children to which is an international high IQ society, and the Mega
meet their potential and to be capable, productive, success- Foundation, which serves gifted adults with an IQ of 164 or
ful, happy, and even eminent (Subotnik, Olszewski-Kubilius, greater. Despite these efforts, there remains a dearth of
& Worrell, 2011; Subotnik & Rickoff, 2010) adults. But, research on gifted adults that provides us with an integrated
how do we know gifted education has met these goals? If we understanding of what giftedness means or looks like during
accept that gifted education has a larger purpose than the adulthood.
day-to-day activities, projects, and homework assignments In 2011, Subotnik et al. proposed a new definition of
that come along with a gifted program, we must also accept giftedness:
that gifted children grow up. “It’s not as though these former
children slough off their giftedness like discarded skin at the Giftedness (a) reflects the values of society; (b) is typically
age of sixteen or eighteen or twenty-one. Gifted children do manifested in actual outcomes, especially in adulthood; (c) is
grow up, and they become gifted adults” (Jacobsen, 1999a, specific to domains of endeavor; (d) is the result of the coalescing
p. 9). Yet we know very little about what happens once a of biological, pedagogical, psychological, and psychosocial
factors; and (e) is relative not just to the ordinary (e.g., a child
gifted individual graduates from high school (Rinn &
with exceptional art ability compared to peers) but to the
Plucker, 2004), and even less about what happens when a extraordinary (e.g., an artist who revolutionizes a field of art).
gifted individual “grows up.” (p. 3)
Over the past two decades, a number of provocative book
titles regarding gifted adults have appeared in mainstream
media that attempt to answer some questions about the lives 1
University of North Texas, Denton, TX, USA
of gifted adults: The Gifted Adult: A Revolutionary Guide for
Corresponding Author:
Liberating Everyday Genius (Jacobsen, 1999a), Gifted Anne N. Rinn, Department of Educational Psychology, University of
Grown Ups: The Mixed Blessings of Extraordinary Potential North Texas, 1155 Union Circle #311335, Denton, TX 76203, USA.
(Streznewski, 1999), Gifted Lives: What Happens When Email: anne.rinn@unt.edu

Downloaded from gcq.sagepub.com by guest on September 8, 2015


214 Gifted Child Quarterly 59(4)

Their definition of giftedness specifically includes adults pertaining to the characteristics, experiences, and needs of
and, in combination with the previously mentioned books in gifted adults.
mainstream media and the purported goals of gifted educa-
tion, raises the question: Who is a gifted adult?
Method
Strong studies on gifted adults are rare. The available
research is often flawed by methodological shortcomings, We collected articles in several ways. First, we searched
including imprecise and unclear definitions of giftedness, multiple online databases (Academic Search Complete,
small sample sizes, and a lack of adequate comparison Education Research Complete, ERIC, Professional
groups. Some studies are based on anecdotal experiences Development Collection, PsycARTICLES, Psychology and
rather than empirical observations (such as that by Lovecky, Behavioral Sciences Collection, PsycINFO, and SocINDEX
1986; Roeper, 1991), and many studies of gifted adults with Full Text) to identify peer-reviewed articles in psychol-
come from the same databases (e.g., Perrone et al., 2004, ogy, education, sociology, and other relevant fields using the
2006, 2007, 2010, 2011, 20121; Study of Mathematically keywords “gifted,” “high-achieving,” “eminent,” “high-abil-
Precocious Youth, see Lubinski & Benbow, 2006). Some ity,” “IQ,” and “intelligence,” each in combination with the
research on high ability adults exists outside the field of word “adult.” Dissertations, master’s theses, and articles that
gifted education, such as in the field of individual differ- were not peer-reviewed (e.g., newsletter articles, newspaper
ences, which has produced a large amount of research on articles) were excluded from the current study. Second, we
IQ differences among individuals of varying ages. However, searched back issues of the following journals for relevant
as in the case of individual differences research, in particu- articles: Gifted Child Quarterly (1957-2015), Journal for the
lar, definitional clarity becomes an issue. The term gifted is Education of the Gifted (1983-2015), Roeper Review (1978-
not typically used and studies of high ability are based 2015), Journal of Secondary Gifted Education (1996-2006),
solely on one’s IQ (see, e.g., Ashton, 2013, and Cooper, High Ability Studies (1996-2015), Journal of Advanced
2010). The field of gifted education, though, views high Academics (2006-2015), and Advanced Development (1989-
ability more broadly than just one’s IQ score (National 2014). Finally, we scanned the references section of each
Association for Gifted Children, 2010), including other included article for any relevant article we may have missed.
domains such as specific academic ability, music, creativ- Searches were conducted between June 2012 and June 2013,
ity, and leadership. Thus, inconsistencies in defining a with follow-up searches conducted in June 2014 and January
gifted adult across fields of study also complicate our abil- 2015.
ity to adequately understand the characteristics, experi- “Gifted adults,” for the purposes of this study, included
ences, and needs of gifted adults. (a) current eminent, gifted, or high-achieving adults (defined
Regardless of the shortcomings of existing research, the by each particular study) or adults who were identified as
study of gifted adults is important to the field of gifted and gifted during their youth (defined by each particular study);
talented education for a variety of reasons. First, there is a and (b) those who had surpassed the traditional age for col-
direct benefit to the current programs servicing gifted chil- lege completion (the early 20s). While college students could
dren. The outcomes and experiences of gifted adults give certainly be considered adults by legal (i.e., age of majority)
insight into the successes or failures of educational programs and biological (i.e., sexual maturity) definitions, postsecond-
for gifted children and can inform current and future practice ary education and other postsecondary endeavors (e.g., mili-
(Subotnik & Olszewski-Kubilius, 1997). Second, from a tary service) are often considered a time of “emerging
utilitarian view, because it can be said that the majority of adulthood,” or the period between approximately ages 18
people are most productive in adulthood, the same can be and 25 whereby young people develop autonomy and a sense
said for gifted people. If the end goal of gifted and talented of self before moving into early adulthood (see Arnett, 2000).
education is to produce eminence and innovative products See Table 1 for information regarding the indicators of adult
(Subotnik & Rickoff, 2010), then adulthood is the period giftedness as defined by each study included in this review.
when gifted and talented education bears its fruit (Simonton, Finally, although we did not include a specific cutoff date for
2008). Third, and perhaps most fundamentally, giftedness the inclusion of articles in this article, very few studies of
should be recognized holistically as an experience that runs gifted adults were found to exist prior to the late 1970s,
through the entire lifespan (Keating, 2009). Given that child- except for the work of Terman and his colleagues (Terman
hood comprises little more than a quarter of the average life- et al., 1925) and the beginning of the Study of Mathematically
time, the remaining three quarters of the lifetime of a gifted Precocious Youth (SMPY) in the early 1970s (see Lubinski
person should not remain unexamined. & Benbow, 2006).
The current study synthesizes and analyzes the research In this systematic review of the literature, we categorized
literature on gifted adults in order to (a) begin a conversation the included articles into thematic areas using the grounded
regarding the characteristics, experiences, and needs of theory approach (Glaser & Strauss, 1967). Initial coding was
gifted adults; (b) identify gaps in the literature regarding done individually and was then refined as a team. The the-
gifted adults; and (c) to suggest areas of future research matic areas are (a) whether or not gifted children become

Downloaded from gcq.sagepub.com by guest on September 8, 2015


Rinn and Bishop 215

Table 1. Indicators of Adult Giftedness as Designated by Each Study.

Youth Status as
Youth IQ standardized HS class Academic doctoral Adult Mensa or adult
Study score(s) test score(s) rank prize in HS student accomplishments IQ score(s) Other
Antshel et al. (2009) X
Antshel et al. (2010) X
Benbow, Lubinski, Shea, and X
Eftekhari-Sanjani (2000)
Campbell and Feng (2011) X X
Dijkstra, Barelds, Groothof, X
Ronner, and Nauta (2012)
Feldman (1984) X
Ferriman, Lubinski, and X
Benbow (2009)
Firkowska-Mankiewicz X
(2011)
Freeman and Walberg X
(1999)
Hansen and Hall (1997) X
Holahan and Velasquez X
(2011)
Kaufmann (1981) X X
Kaufmann, Harrel, Milam, X X
Woolverton, and Miller
(1986)
Kaufmann and Matthews X X
(2012)
Kitano and Perkins (1996) X
Lewis, Kitano, and Lynch X
(1992)
Lubinski, Benbow, and Ryan X
(1995)
Lubinski, Benbow, Webb, X
and Bleske-Rechek (2006)
Lubinski. Schmidt, and X
Benbow (1996)
Park, Imboden, Park, Hulse, X
and Unger (1992)
Park, Lubinski, and Benbow X
(2007)
Park, Lubinski, and Benbow X
(2008)
Park, Lubinski, and Benbow
(2012)
Perrone, Civiletto, Webb, X X
and Fitch (2004)
Perrone, Ksiazak, et al. X X
(2010)
Perrone, Perrone, Ksiazak, X X
Wright, and Jackson
(2007)
Perrone, Tschopp, Snyder, X X
Boo, and Hyatt (2010)
Perrone, Webb, Wright, X X
Jackson, and Ksiazak
(2006)

(continued)

Downloaded from gcq.sagepub.com by guest on September 8, 2015


216 Gifted Child Quarterly 59(4)

Table 1. (continued)

Youth Status as
Youth IQ standardized HS class Academic doctoral Adult Mensa or adult
Study score(s) test score(s) rank prize in HS student accomplishments IQ score(s) Other
Perrone, Wright, Ksiazak, X X
Crane, and Vannatter
(2010)
Perrone-McGovern, Boo, X X
and Vannatter (2012)
Perrone-McGovern et al. X X
(2011)
Persson (2009) X
Peterson (2012) X
Piechowski and Colangelo X X X
(1984)
Robertson, Smeets, Lubinski, X
and Benbow (2010)
Ruf (2001) X
Sears (1977) X
Sękowski and Siekańska X
(2008)
Siekańska and Sękowski X
(2006)
Simonton (2008) X
Stålnacke and Smedler X
(2011)
Standing, Aikins, Madigan, X
and Nohl (2015)
Stewart and Porath (1999) X
Subotnik and Arnold (1995) X X
Terman (1942) X
Terman (1954) X
Tomlinson-Keasey and Little X
(1990)
Wai (2013) X
Wai (2014a) X
Wai, Lubinski, and Benbow X
(2005)
Wai, Lubinski, Benbow, and X
Steiger (2010)
Walberg et al. (1981) X
Walker, Reis, and Leonard X
(1992)
Watve (2008) X
Weidenheim, Escobar, and X
Rapin (2012)
Wirthwein, Becker, Loehr, X
and Rost (2011)
Wirthwein and Rost (2011a) X
Wirthwein and Rost (2011b) X
Yewchuk and Schlosser X
(1995)

Note. Standardized Test Scores = On-level or above-level; HS = high school; Mensa = membership in Mensa. Other refers to recommendations from high
school officials, autobiographical statements, scores from a rating scale based on Gardner’s (1983) model of personal intelligence (i.e., interpersonal and
intrapersonal intelligence), participation in gifted programs as a child, or graduating from a highly selective school.

Downloaded from gcq.sagepub.com by guest on September 8, 2015


Rinn and Bishop 217

gifted adults; (b) family of origin; (c) effects of early educa- study of intellectual talent (Lubinski & Benbow, 2006). The
tional experiences; (d) characteristics of gifted adults; (e) first four cohorts were identified through talent searches,
career; (f) family of procreation; (g) career and family inter- meaning at or before age 13, each of these individuals were
action; (h) life goals, satisfaction, and well-being; and (i) identified based on their scores on subtests of the College
counseling. We discuss each of these themes in detail, but Board Scholastic Aptitude Test (SAT). The 1972 cohort
first, we discuss three major longitudinal studies of gifted- (Cohort 1) includes 2,188 participants who earned a score of
ness that contribute to much of what is covered in this review. at least 390 on the math subtest of the SAT or 370 on the
verbal subtest of the SAT. The 1976 cohort (Cohort 2)
includes 778 participants who earned a score of at least 500
Historical Research and Seminal
on the math subtest of the SAT or 430 on the verbal subtest
Longitudinal Studies of Gifted Adults of the SAT. The 1980 cohort (Cohort 3) includes 501 partici-
Several well-known longitudinal studies of giftedness that pants who earned a score of at least 700 on the math subtest
range across the lifespan or portions of the lifespan make up of the SAT or 630 on the verbal subtest of the SAT. All par-
the bulk of research on gifted adults. For an in-depth discus- ticipants were followed up at ages 18, 23, and 33. The 1987
sion of longitudinal studies of giftedness and talent across cohort (Cohort 4) includes 1,000 participants who earned a
the lifespan, we refer the reader to Subotnik and Arnold score of at least 500 on the math subtest of the SAT, 430 on
(1994) and Horowitz, Subotnik, and Matthews (2009). As the verbal subtest of the SAT, or 20 on an ACT subtest.
several longitudinal studies are referenced numerous times Cohort 5 consists of students selected from top math/sci-
throughout our review, we discuss them in detail here. ence graduate programs in 1992, and who were again fol-
lowed up 10 years later. While SMPY participants may have
advanced capabilities in other domains, the participants are
Terman’s Study of the Gifted included in the SMPY research studies because of their spe-
Longitudinal research in gifted education largely started with cific abilities in a particular domain (i.e., math, science, ver-
the Terman Study of the Gifted (Terman, 1926; Terman et al., bal), which somewhat limits the generalizability of the
1925) in 1921, whereby Terman began following the lives of findings from the SMPY research. However, research has
more than 1,500 gifted children. At selection, participants shown a correlation between SAT scores and IQ scores
were an average age of 11 years and all had an IQ of 135 or (ranging from r = .53 to .82, p < .01), suggesting that higher
greater. At the time of the last survey in 1999, 97% of the SAT scores are at least somewhat indicative of a higher IQ
remaining participants had reached the age of 80 or older. (Brodnick & Ree, 1995; Frey & Detterman, 2004).
This longitudinal study of gifted individuals, from childhood
to adulthood, largely reveals a population considered healthy “1988 Midwestern Study”
and successful (Terman & Oden, 1947), and was among the
first to refute previous claims that gifted children were sickly In the “1988 Midwestern Study,” individuals have been
and awkward. It should be noted, though, that there are participating in a longitudinal study of giftedness since they
numerous issues with Terman’s study, including the fact that were adolescents in 1988. Beginning in 1988, directors of
most of the children selected for participation were White guidance in all public and private high schools in a
and from a middle-to-upper class socioeconomic status. Midwestern state identified the two highest ranking gradu-
Much of the success of these gifted individuals could be ates from schools graduating fewer than 250 students, the
attributed to their backgrounds and not their IQs (Vialle, five highest ranking graduates from schools graduating
1994). Also, because Terman’s research started nearly a cen- more than 250 students, and all Merit Scholar finalists and
tury ago, many of the findings could be outdated. This is not semifinalists for inclusion in a longitudinal study of gifted-
to say that Terman’s study is not influential to the field of ness. Perrone and colleagues (2004, 2006, 2007, 2010,
gifted education, as it certainly has been, but to note that his 2011, 2012) have been following these gifted individuals
study was not inclusive of individuals from underserved for more than 20 years. The inclusion of both class rank and
backgrounds (e.g., those in poverty or from racial minority standardized test scores as indicators of giftedness limits
groups) and his findings may not always be relevant to con- the sample to academically high achieving students and
temporary issues in gifted education. those with specific aptitude for math and verbal abilities,
also affecting the generalizability of the findings. However,
as previously mentioned, a relationship likely exists
Study of Mathematically Precocious Youth between SAT scores and IQ scores, suggesting general
Five cohorts of individuals identified between 1972 and intelligence levels were taken into account in the design of
1997 (more than 5,000 individuals) are being tracked this study (Brodnick & Ree, 1995; Frey & Detterman,
through the SMPY, which is a planned 50-year longitudinal 2004).

Downloaded from gcq.sagepub.com by guest on September 8, 2015


218 Gifted Child Quarterly 59(4)

Do Gifted Children Become Gifted with a comparison group of 26 randomly chosen individuals
Adults? (11 women and 15 men) with an average IQ of 150, Feldman
found the 180 IQ group and the 150 IQ group did not differ
A simple answer to the question “Do gifted children become markedly in terms of educational attainment, jobs or profes-
gifted adults?” is, “Yes, they do.” If identified as gifted dur- sions chosen, marriage, having children, or life satisfaction.
ing childhood on the basis of one’s IQ score, for example, In contrast to the threshold effect, though, research from
we can assume the “gifted” label persists throughout adult- the SMPY studies suggest higher levels of cognitive ability,
hood, as one’s IQ score is fairly stable from childhood to even within the top 1% of cognitive ability, do matter (Kell,
adulthood (Deary, Whalley, Lemmon, Crawford, & Starr, Lubinski, & Benbow, 2013; Robertson, Smeets, Lubinski, &
2000; Deary, Whiteman, Starr, Whalley, & Fox, 2004; Benbow, 2010). When examining the top 1% of mathemati-
Mackintosh, 2011), and any age-related declines in IQ may cally precocious individuals involved in the first three
be delayed in individuals with higher than average IQs cohorts of SMPY, Park, Lubinski, and Benbow (2008) found
(Baxendale, 2011). If identified as gifted in the realm of cre- that mathematical ability level differences at age 13 could
ativity, leadership, or a specific academic ability during predict advanced degree attainment in science, technology,
childhood, we have no reason to believe that specific area of engineering, or mathematics (STEM), the likelihood of
giftedness would dissipate without cause as an individual authoring a peer-reviewed STEM publication, and the likeli-
ages. However, if we reframe the question as, “Do gifted hood of earning a patent 25 years later. Furthermore, also
children use their giftedness in a significant or outstanding using the same sample of the top 1% of mathematically pre-
way as adults and/or become eminent2 as adults?” then we cocious individuals involved in the first three cohorts of
can begin to answer the question based on the available SMPY, Wai, Lubinski, and Benbow (2005) examined the top
research literature. versus bottom quartiles of the top 1% and found statistically
One camp of researchers says intelligence during child- significant effect sizes favoring the top quartile with regard
hood, whether formally recognized or not, is largely a to secured doctorates, doctorates in the STEM fields, income,
requirement for eminence in adulthood, but it is not a guar- patents, and tenure track positions at top universities in the
antee (Simonton & Song, 2009; Walberg et al., 1996). Much United States about 20 years later. Even within the top 1%,
of this research is retrospective in nature, whereby the child- then, differences emerge between the “able” and the “excep-
hoods of eminent adults are examined for common themes tionally able” (p. 489). Wai (2013, 2014a) adds to the idea
and experiences (e.g., Albert, 1978; Cox, 1926; Goertzel, that higher cognitive ability levels matter by retrospectively
Goertzel, & Goertzel, 1978). For example, using a sample of assessing the educational experiences and cognitive abilities
291 eminent African Americans,3 Simonton (2008) con- of “America’s elite”4 (Wai, 2013, p. 203) and “the world’s
ducted a historiometric study to determine factors that con- rich and powerful”5 (Wai, 2014a, p. 54). By examining the
tribute to adult eminence. Even after the inclusion of a variety universities that each of these individuals attended, Wai was
of control variables (e.g., gender, year of birth) in his analy- able to estimate the cognitive ability of each individual based
ses, he found that early giftedness, in particular, was predic- on the average standardized test scores of individuals attend-
tive of adulthood eminence and creative achievement. In ing those universities (e.g., the average combined SAT math
other words, eminent adults were once gifted children. He and critical reading scores of individuals attending the
did not, however, include a control group of adults who are California Institute of Technology is 1525, making it the
not eminent, but who were identified as gifted when chil- most elite institution in the United States, according to Wai).
dren. Walberg et al. (1981) conducted a similar historiomet- Because of the correlation between SAT scores and IQ scores
ric study of 200 men born between the 14th and 20th centuries (Brodnick & Ree, 1995; Frey & Detterman, 2004), Wai con-
(e.g., Mozart, Lincoln) and found that early intelligence was cludes that the global elite and America’s elite are largely
predictive of adult eminence. Freeman and Walberg (1999) drawn from the intellectually and academically gifted, and
also conducted a historiometric study of 256 eminent African he suspects many are in the top 1% of ability.
American women who lived in the 20th century and found Similarly, using SMPY data, Park, Lubinski, and Benbow
evidence of early intellectual ability among these women, in (2007) showed that ability patterns at age 13, detected by
addition to the traits of perseverance and single-mindedness. both the sum of math and verbal SAT scores (general ability
Similar to Simonton (2008), though, Walberg et al. and level) and the math SAT score minus the verbal SAT score
Freeman and Walberg did not include control groups. (tilt, or differential ability strength), could predict creative
Nonetheless, while childhood giftedness may matter, accomplishments and the domain in which they occur 25
Feldman (1984) found somewhat of a threshold effect, which years later (e.g., humanities vs. STEM). A positive ability tilt
suggests that above a certain threshold of cognitive ability indicated a greater strength in quantitative ability (e.g.,
(an IQ of 120, or an ability level in the 90th percentile, for STEM) and a negative ability tilt indicated a greater strength
example), differences in cognitive ability no longer matter. in verbal ability (e.g., humanities). Robertson et al. (2010)
In comparing the 26 individuals (7 women and 19 men) with added to these findings to suggest vocational interests and
an IQ greater than 180 from Terman’s Study of the Gifted lifestyle preferences, in combination with ability patterns,

Downloaded from gcq.sagepub.com by guest on September 8, 2015


Rinn and Bishop 219

can predict educational and occupational domains and per- In examining which gifted children grow up to become
formance among SMPY young adolescents in the top 1% of eminent and which ones do not, we have to take into account
quantitative ability decades later. There might also be cross- more than just childhood IQ. In addition to the early identifi-
cultural similarities in these findings. Firkowska-Mankiewicz cation of ability as well as skills and interests during child-
(2011) discusses the “Warsaw Studies,” which was a longitu- hood, other childhood experiences also play a role in the
dinal study conducted with a cohort of 11 to 13 year olds in development of giftedness or eminence during adulthood. In
Warsaw, Poland, beginning in the 1970s. In 1994, there were the following sections, we explore characteristics of one’s
170 people from the High IQ group (mean IQ = 130 in child- family of origin and early educational experiences that may
hood); in 1995, 141 people remained from the High IQ or may not lead to eminence during adulthood.
group; and in 1999, 97 people remained from the High IQ
group. The results from a series of studies related to the
Warsaw children indicate that IQ score at age 13 is a good
Family of Origin
indicator of attained education, occupational status, and Research is mixed regarding whether eminence in adulthood
material well-being later in life. is associated with a harmonious home and family life during
On the other hand, researchers have found that “most childhood or with a stressful environment (Albert, 1991, per-
gifted children, even most child prodigies, do not go on to haps due to parental dysfunction or the early loss of a parent
become adult creators” (Winner, 2000, p. 165) and most [the Phaeton Effect; Iremonger, 1970]). For example, some
gifted children identified as gifted on the basis of traditional gifted children from harmonious families have exhibited
measures of intellectual ability do not become eminent adults lower than expected levels of eminence in adulthood
(Olszewski-Kubilius, 2000; Subotnik, 2009; Subotnik & (Subotnik, Kassan, Summers, & Wasser, 1993), but may be
Steiner, 1994; VanTassel-Baska, 1989). Howard (2008) says, scholastic achievers during the school years (Albert, 1978).
“There is little apparent link between being a prodigy and Eminent and creative achievers, then, can “come from fami-
becoming eminent . . . and some recognized geniuses, such lies where there were tense family relationships, unconven-
as Albert Einstein, reportedly showed no early signs of great tional parenting and socialization practices in use, and a
talent” (pp. 119-120). In a study using data from Terman’s greater likelihood of parental dysfunction or parental loss”
Study of the Gifted, Tomlinson-Keasey and Little (1990) (Olszewski-Kubilius, 2000, p. 65). For example, among a
found that parental education and intellectual determination sample of 145 Presidential Scholars6 from 1964 to 1968 (70
were predictors of the intellectual skill of gifted adults later male, 75 female), 60% reported experiencing significant trau-
in life, while sociability was a negative predictor. Terman matic events or extreme conditions before the age of 18 (e.g.,
himself (1942) found that vocational success was not neces- bullying, economic hardship, family discord, death of a par-
sarily correlated with intellectual ability within his sample, ent or guardian; Kaufmann & Matthews, 2012). Stewart and
but that the drive to achieve and one’s emotional stability Porath (1999) conducted a content analysis of written bio-
contributed more to success in one’s chosen field. graphical and autobiographical accounts of five British boys
Regardless of whether a gifted child will become an emi- who were born between 1880 and 1933. These boys were
nent adult, interests and values during childhood may be born into low socioeconomic conditions, but became eminent
somewhat stable over time for a gifted individual. In a study in adulthood. Each boy, and later man, was considered ver-
of 114 males and 48 females from Cohort 2 of the SMPY bally talented. All five boys faced significant difficulty during
studies, Lubinski, Benbow, and Ryan (1995) administered a their childhoods, including poverty, parental ill health, paren-
vocational interest inventory at age 13 and then again 15 tal death, and moving frequently.
years later, at age 28. Over the 15-year time period, one’s Furthermore, in a study of 41 female and 20 male outpa-
dominant vocational interest at age 13 was more likely than tients diagnosed with personality disorders, 38 of who were
chance to be one’s dominant or adjacent to the dominant diagnosed with borderline personality disorder, Park,
value at age 28 (κ = .18, 95% confidence interval = [.07, Imboden, Park, Hulse, and Unger (1992), using a rating
.29]). Similarly, in a study of 94 male and 109 female stu- scale based on Gardner’s (1983) model of personal intelli-
dents from Cohort 1 of the SMPY studies, Lubinski, Schmidt, gence (i.e., interpersonal and intrapersonal intelligences),
and Benbow (1996) examined participants’ values (i.e., theo- found that 74% of the borderline personality patients met
retical, economic, political, aesthetic, social, and religious) at the definition for giftedness compared with only 34% of the
age 13 and then again 20 years later at age 33. Lubinski et al. control patients. Of interest, though, 74% (17 of 23) of the
found “global, educationally, and vocationally relevant pref- borderline personality patients were also identified as hav-
erences among intellectually gifted adults are somewhat ing experienced psychological abuse in their childhood,
related to early adolescent assessments” (p. 447). Over the whereas the control group reported psychological abuse in
20-year time period, one’s dominant value at age 13 was only 13% of the cases. Of those patients reporting psycho-
more likely than chance to be one’s dominant or adjacent to logical abuse, 91% identified a dominant parent who was
the dominant value at age 33 (κ = .12, 95% confidence inter- also the primary psychological abuser, with 78% character-
val = [.04, .20]). izing the psychological abuse as intrusive over involvement,

Downloaded from gcq.sagepub.com by guest on September 8, 2015


220 Gifted Child Quarterly 59(4)

criticism, and constant devaluation. Childhood psychologi- (2011) replicated Terman’s study with a modern sample of
cal abuse may certainly be handicapping, but it appears to Group A (most successful, N = 24) and Group C (least suc-
spur some gifted individuals toward self-development. In a cessful, N = 24) American Academic Olympians.9 Campbell
study of 41 intellectually gifted adults7 between the ages of and Feng also found differences between the groups regard-
40 and 60 years, Ruf (2001) found that 56% of the partici- ing early life experiences: Individuals in Group A were
pants reported childhood psychological abuse. Participants characterized by an abundance of books and magazines in
who overcame resentment of childhood psychological abuse their childhood homes, parents who recognized their talent
were more likely to pursue self-actualization, defined as and encouraged their development in that domain, and
“living up to one’s potential” (p. 267). Ruf’s study failed to higher socioeconomic statuses. Individuals in Group C were
explicitly identify the catalyst for the emotional growth, characterized by lower socioeconomic statuses, less intel-
although a review of her results indicated that of those par- lectual stimulation during childhood, less recognition from
ticipants who were identified as self-actualizing and survi- their parents regarding the development of their talents, and
vors of childhood psychological abuse, 80% had received negative affective school experiences (i.e., insensitive
counseling. teachers and peers).
Csikszentmihalyi, Rathunde, and Whalen (1993) and Sękowski and Siekańska (2008) compared the early fam-
Therival (1999) assert that stress and trauma during child- ily experiences of 90 gifted adults10 (ranging in age from 26
hood are not necessary for adult eminence, but that a bal- to 35 years) and 90 average ability adults (ranging in age
ance of parental, familial, or other support and tension or from 26 to 35 years) who were a matched comparison group.
misfortune may result in later eminence. Standing, Aikins, They found numerous statistically significant differences
Madigan, and Nohl (2015) examined eminent American between the groups. For example, regarding siblings, 43.3%
writers, presidents of the United States, and the 100 most of the gifted adults grew up without siblings, while only
influential 20th century Americans as judged by Life maga- 8.9% of the comparison group had no siblings. Among the
zine and found that the majority experienced some form of gifted adults, 15.6% of their fathers held doctoral degrees,
early loss, but that “mentoring, innate ability, education, while only 1.1% of the fathers of the comparison group held
favorable external circumstances, or luck” contributed to doctoral degrees. Regarding their mothers, among the gifted
their success (p. 196). Similarly, in a study of 197 eminent adults, 56.7% of the mothers had a university education and
Canadian women,8 Yewchuk and Schlosser (1995) found 8.9% held a doctoral degree. Among the comparison group,
most of the women viewed their parents as encouraging and 35.6% of the mothers had a university education and 0% held
supportive during childhood and adolescence. Other sources a doctoral degree. Kaufmann (1981) reports similar findings
of support seem to matter, too. Kaufmann, Harrel, Milam, with a sample of 322 Presidential Scholars from 1964 to
Woolverton, and Miller (1986) had 255 of the 1964 to 1968 1968, which were described previously, such that 60% of the
Presidential Scholars complete a questionnaire concerning Scholars’ fathers attended college and 44% earned advanced
significant milestones in their undergraduate and graduate degrees. Of the Scholars’ mothers, 73% attended college and
school experiences. Of these, 139 participants (68 men and 15% earned advanced degrees.
71 women) identified the presence of a mentor in their lives In a study of the perceptions of multigenerational gifted-
as a significant experience, with 79% reporting it as a posi- ness among a sample of 89 gifted adults from the 1988
tive experience. The majority (66%) of mentors were identi- Midwestern Study (mean age = 36.73 years, SD = 0.62),
fied as teachers, with counselors mentioned to a lesser Perrone, Ksiazak, et al. (2010) found that 37% of partici-
degree. When questioned about the function of a mentor, pants thought neither of their parents were gifted, 34%
61% of the gifted adults surveyed considered role modeling thought both of their parents were gifted, 14% thought only
as one of the functions, with support and encouragement a their mother was gifted, 14% thought only their father was
close second at 58%. gifted, and 1% was unsure. Those participants who thought
Indeed, early experiences seem to matter later in life. In their parents were gifted suggested giftedness in the follow-
1954, Terman analyzed data from his longitudinal study of ing categories: “math and science; exceptional memory;
gifted individuals and focused specifically on the life suc- mechanical or spatial ability; interpersonal skills; language
cess of 730 men at age 30, which was measured by examin- and writing; problem solving and critical thinking; athletic
ing the extent to which a man had made use of his superior ability; leadership, business, or organizational skills; creative
intellectual ability. The 150 most successful men were or artistic abilities; and general academic abilities” (p. 614).
labeled Group A, and the 150 least successful men were The participants’ mothers were most likely to be identified as
labeled Group C. Notable differences regarding early life gifted in the areas of creative or artistic abilities, language
experiences emerged between the groups: For example, half and writing, and general academic abilities. The participants’
of the fathers of Group A men were college graduates and fathers were most likely to be identified as gifted in the areas
15% of the fathers of Group C men were college graduates. of math and science, mechanical or spatial ability, interper-
Group A men had nearly 50% more books in their homes sonal skills, creative or artistic skills, and general academic
during childhood than Group C men. Campbell and Feng abilities.

Downloaded from gcq.sagepub.com by guest on September 8, 2015


Rinn and Bishop 221

Effects of Early Educational Experiences 1986; Gagne, 1995). For example, Subotnik and Olszewski-
Kubilius (1997) note summer programs for the gifted and
Acceleration and Advanced Coursework interaction with intellectual peers during childhood and ado-
Using a sample of the top 1% of mathematically precocious lescence are essential for talent development and adult emi-
individuals involved in the first three cohorts of SMPY, Park, nence. In a study of 53 men between the ages of 30 and 40
Lubinski, and Benbow (2012) examined the effects of grade years,11 Watve (2008) examined the effects of enriched edu-
skipping on adult productivity in the STEM fields. Park et al. cational programs during childhood on academic (educa-
used exact and propensity score matching to create matched tional qualifications, acquisition of languages, literature
samples, resulting in a group of “grade skippers” (1972 published, etc.), practical (occupational status, income,
cohort n = 179; 1976 cohort n = 116; 1980 cohort n = 68) and material-possessions, etc.), and social (leadership performed,
a control group (1972 cohort n = 358; 1976 cohort n = 231; special recognitions achieved, membership of groups, etc.)
1980 cohort n = 68). Results indicate that in every cohort, a accomplishments during adulthood. Comparison was made
greater proportion of grade skippers than control participants between those men who had participated in 6 years of
earned doctoral degrees, doctoral degrees in STEM, STEM enriched educational programs during childhood and those
publications, and patents. Also, pooled comparisons of equally able men who had not participated in enriched edu-
cohorts show age advantages for the grade skippers of cational programs during childhood. Watve found that those
between 1 year to 1.5 years for doctoral degree attainment, men who participated in enriched educational programs dur-
doctoral degree attainment in STEM, and first STEM publi- ing childhood were more likely than those who had not to
cation, meaning grade skippers reach each of these outcomes have academic and social accomplishments, specifically (a)
1 year to 1.5 years earlier than the control participants. The to achieve basic educational qualifications, (b) to acquire
effects of acceleration on subsequent STEM accomplish- additional qualifications in addition to basic educational
ments (defined as citations and citation indices) continued at qualifications, (c) to become members of social groups, (d)
ages 42, 46, and 50 years, although the effects were different to conduct training programs, and (e) to deliver talks outside
for men and women as well as for those in different cohorts. of their academic area. Furthermore, among the most math-
Overall, these findings support Pressey’s time-saving theory, ematically precocious SMPY participants (the top 1 in 200 in
suggesting that appropriately used grade-based acceleration quantitative reasoning ability for their age taken from Cohort
with mathematically precocious adolescents can have lasting 1 and Cohort 2, and the top 1 in 10,000 in quantitative rea-
effects on the productivity of those pursuing careers in the soning ability for their age taken from Cohort 3 of the SMPY
STEM fields (as cited in Park et al., 2012). studies), those with more pre-collegiate STEM educational
Using a sample of 88 participants (mean age = 35.89, opportunities, termed STEM educational dose, had more
SD = 0.35) from the 1988 Midwestern Study, Perrone, Wright, STEM accomplishments (doctorate in the STEM fields,
Ksiazak, Crane, and Vannatter (2010) examined the academic STEM publications, STEM tenure, and STEM occupations)
and interpersonal experiences of participants who were 20 years later (Wai, Lubinski, Benbow, & Steiger, 2010).
enrolled in advanced classes during elementary school, sec- A discussion of the childhood experiences of gifted adults
ondary school, or college. Of the 88 participants, 72% were leads us to the characteristics, experiences, and needs of
enrolled in advanced classes during elementary or secondary gifted individuals during adulthood. In the following sec-
school. Of these, 85% indicated that their academic experi- tions, characteristics of gifted adults are discussed, followed
ences in these classes were positive and 59% indicated that by a discussion of career and family of procreation, and then
their interpersonal experiences in these classes were positive. life satisfaction (or lack thereof).
Positive experiences included college preparation, academic
challenge, and intellectual engagement, among others. Characteristics of Gifted Adults
Negative experiences, though, included segregation/isolation
from peers and social stigma from the gifted label, among Several researchers discuss anecdotal and observational
others. Of the 88 participants, only 32% were enrolled in experiences with gifted adults and describe characteristics of
advanced classes during college. Positive experiences were these gifted adults that set them apart from their average abil-
largely noted, including the rewarding challenge provided in ity peers. Lovecky (1986) outlines the following traits of
these classes, social interaction with other advanced students, gifted adults: divergent thinking, excitability, sensitivity, per-
smaller class sizes, and access to the best professors. ceptivity, and entelechy (i.e., having a goal). Roeper (1991)
explains gifted adults have the following characteristics,
among others: complex intellectual ability, childlike emo-
Enrichment tions, feelings of being fundamentally different from others,
Researchers interested in the talent development process driven by their giftedness, feelings of being overwhelmed by
have noted that deep immersion in a discipline or domain of their own creativity, introversion, need for solitude, need for
talent during childhood and adolescence is predictive of meaning, individualized methods of learning, able to see pat-
adult accomplishment and eminence (Bloom, 1985; Feldman, terns of development and growth (trends), need for truth,

Downloaded from gcq.sagepub.com by guest on September 8, 2015


222 Gifted Child Quarterly 59(4)

perfectionism, feelings of being misunderstood, difficulty in intellectual overexcitability subscale of the OEQ-II than the
understanding the behaviors of others, strong sense of humor, average-IQ group (d = .56) and also on the sensual overexcit-
difficulty with authority figures, and strong moral convic- ability subscale (d = .32). Piechowski and Colangelo (1984)
tions. Tolan (1994) also discusses similar characteristics: found gifted adolescents to score lower than gifted adults
emotional intensity, moral sensitivity and concern for justice, (N = 28, mean age 36.4)13 in intellectual overexcitability.
and social experiences that depend on finding like-minded Piechowski and Colangelo also found gifted adolescents to
others. Of the characteristics described by Lovecky (1986), score lower than gifted adults, and gifted adolescents to score
Roeper (1991), and Tolan (1994), only a few have been lower than average ability graduate students, on a measure of
empirically examined, most notably emotional sensitivity, sensual overexcitability, concluding that sensual overexcit-
intensity, and perceptions of giftedness. ability may be a characteristic of age and maturity rather than
giftedness.
Emotional Sensitivity and Intensity
Perceptions of Giftedness
Overexcitabilities are ways that an individual experiences
the world (Piechowski, 1979). The notion of overexcitabili- Stålnacke and Smedler (2011) studied the psychosocial
ties stems from Dabrowski’s (1964) Theory of Positive experiences of 302 adults (mean age = 36 years, SD = 9.6)
Disintegration, which is a theory of personality develop- who were members of the Swedish Mensa Society.14 As a
ment. For a thorough description of Dabrowski’s ideas and group, the majority of participants experienced feelings of
the Theory of Positive Disintegration, the reader is referred being “different,” but not different enough to downplay
to Dabrowski (1937, 1964, 1972) and Dabrowski, Kawczak, their giftedness or to adjust their behavior in order to be
and Piechowski (1970). The five forms of overexcitabilities accepted as adults. Many experienced difficulties in child-
may be described as follows: A psychomotor overexcitability hood, though, suggesting it takes time to come to terms with
refers to a surplus of energy and nervousness. A sensual one’s abilities and talents. In a similar study of 83 gifted
overexcitability is an enhanced level of sensory experience adults (ranging in age from 34 to 36 years) from the 1988
and is marked by the pursuit of pleasure through senses such Midwestern Study, Perrone, Perrone, Ksiazak, Wright, and
as tastes and smell. An intellectual overexcitability is associ- Jackson (2007) surveyed the participants on issues related to
ated with striving for knowledge and truth through question- self-perceptions of giftedness. Most participants saw others’
ing, discovering, and analyzing, but differs from the construct perceptions of them as gifted as a positive force (e.g.,
of intelligence. An imaginational overexcitability is charac- increasing their self-confidence, as a motivating factor,
terized by daydreaming, fantasizing, dramatization, and the increasing their self-awareness). A minority of the partici-
use of imagery and metaphors. An emotional overexcitability pants felt others’ perceptions of them as gifted had a nega-
is marked by the intensified level of interpersonal relations to tive impact on their lives (e.g., causing too much pressure,
people, things, and places, and compassionate feelings for embarrassment). Lewis, Kitano, and Lynch (1992) also
others. Within a gifted adult sample, overexcitabilities are examined the cognitive and affective experiences of 31
usually measured using the Overexcitability Questionnaire- gifted adults (mean age = 42.0 years, SD = 6.4)15 and found
Two (OEQ-II; Falk, Lind, Miller, Piechowski, & Silverman, the gifted adults rated themselves as different from their per-
1999), which is a Likert-type self-report questionnaire ception of average ability individuals in the areas of internal
designed to measure the five overexcitabilities. However, it motivation and cognitive versatility (gifted adults having
should be noted that the study of overexcitabilities and the more of both), and need for recognition (average ability
use of the OEQ-II from both theoretical (see Mendaglio, adults having more).
2012) and measurement (Van den Broeck, Hofmans,
Cooremans, & Staels, 2014; Warne, 2011) perspectives has
been controversial and findings from this questionnaire Career
should be interpreted with these controversies in mind.
Educational and Occupational Success
In a study of 96 intellectually gifted adults12 (mean IQ =
136, SD = 8.3) and 91 adults of average intelligence (mean Cognitive ability seems to be predictive of later educational
IQ = 103, SD = 9.2), all aged 30 to 31 years, both male and and occupational success (Wai, 2014b). Among a sample of
female gifted adults scored higher on the intellectual overex- 286 male and 94 female SMPY participants (mean age 33.6
citability subscale of the OEQ-II than the males and females years), which represents the ablest cohort of all SMPY par-
of average intelligence (d = 0.42; Wirthwein, Becker, Loehr, ticipants (within the top 0.01%), 51.7% of the males and
& Rost, 2011). In a related study using the same sample 54.3% of the females earned doctoral-level degrees (PhD,
(Wirthwein & Rost, 2011a), a high-IQ group (N = 123; mean MD, or JD; Lubinski, Benbow, Webb, & Bleske-Rechek,
IQ = 117, SD = 11.9) and an average-IQ group (N = 97; mean 2006). These SMPY participants earned doctoral degrees at 50
IQ = 102, SD = 12.7) was compared on overexcitability sub- times the base-rate expectation for the general population. Of
scale scores. Again, the high-IQ group scored higher on the the total sample, 59.4% of them had careers in postsecondary

Downloaded from gcq.sagepub.com by guest on September 8, 2015


Rinn and Bishop 223

education, engineering, science, medicine, or law. The Family of Procreation


SMPY follow-ups also show consistent gender differences:
Men are more likely than women to enter the fields of engi- Choice of Partner
neering and physical/inorganic sciences. Women are more
likely than men to enter the fields of law, medicine, social Previous research has indicated spousal correlations between
sciences, and life sciences (Benbow, Lubinski, Shea, & standardized IQ measures (r = .37, Bouchard & McGue,
Eftekhari-Sanjani, 2000; Robertson et al., 2010). An exami- 1981; r = .41, Jensen, 1978). These findings are not surpris-
nation of the 1964-1968 Presidential Scholars shows similar ing given the similarity–attraction hypothesis, which states
findings: Among a sample of 145 participants, 70% of them that the more similar two individuals are, the higher the
earned doctoral level degrees, 20% earned master’s degrees, attraction between them (Byrne, 1971; Byrne & Nelson,
21% had careers in education (more women than men), 19% 1965). SMPY follow-up research of the top 1% in quantita-
had careers in health care and health sciences (more men tive ability shows that the significant others of SMPY indi-
than women), and 14% had careers in legal fields (about viduals are typically well-educated. Of those SMPY
equal numbers of men and women; Kaufmann & Matthews, individuals in long-term relationships (81% in Cohort 1 at
2012). age 33 and 72% in Cohort 2 at age 33), 78% (Cohort 1) and
88% (Cohort 2) of their significant others had earned at least
a bachelor’s degree (Benbow et al., 2000).
Satisfaction in Chosen Career Among a sample of 87 gifted adults (33 male, 54 female)
Perhaps one of the greatest challenges for gifted adults from the 1988 Midwestern Study, ranging in age from 31 to
involves choosing a career path (Karnes & Oehler-Stinnett, 33 years old at the first data collection point and 36 to 38
1986). The path to a chosen career can be even more compli- years old at the second data collection point, marital satisfac-
cated when the gifted adult is a multipotentialite, meaning tion was found to be related to life satisfaction (Perrone-
that they may be capable of success across numerous areas McGovern, Boo, & Vannatter, 2012). Participants with gifted
due to multiple strengths and interests (Delisle & Squires, spouses had higher marital and life satisfaction scores than
1989). The degree to which this is an issue for the gifted is in participants with non-gifted spouses at the first data collec-
dispute; some studies have suggested that the issue may not tion point, but there were no significant differences between
be as critical as once thought (Achter, Benbow, & Lubinski, participants with gifted spouses, and those with non-gifted
1997; Emmett & Minor, 1993). spouses with regard to marital and life satisfaction at the sec-
In Poland, Siekańska and Sękowski (2006) examined the ond data collection point. In a study of 354 heterosexual
job satisfaction and temperament structure of 90 gifted members of Mensa and 558 heterosexual individuals of aver-
adults16 (ranging in age from 26 to 35 years) and 90 average age ability, Dijkstra, Barelds, Groothof, Ronner, and Nauta
ability adults (ranging in age from 26 to 35 years) who were (2012) found the gifted group thought it was more important
a matched comparison group. Results indicated the gifted to have a partner with high intelligence than did the individu-
adults were significantly more satisfied with their jobs than als in the average ability group. The individuals in the gifted
the comparison group. Factors that led to satisfaction with group also rated the following characteristics as more impor-
their jobs among the gifted group included (a) “that their job tant than did the individuals in the average ability group:
is connected with their interests,” (b) “that they can take highly educated and is similar to you. The individuals in the
advantage of their skills, predispositions and expertise,” (c) average ability group placed greater emphasis than the gifted
“that they can display their talent for creative thinking . . . group on the following characteristics: physically healthy,
and to be independent in their thinking and activity,” (d) wants children, good earning capacity, good heredity, good
“that work is a source of pleasure,” and (e) “that the atmo- housekeeper, religious, and is opposite to you.
sphere in their team is good” (p. 79). Perrone, Tschopp,
Snyder, Boo, and Hyatt (2010) found that of a sample of 87 Marital Status and Number of Children
participants from the 1988 Midwestern Study, most were sat-
isfied with their careers and had met their expectations As previously mentioned, Sękowski and Siekańska (2008)
regarding career predictions and success. Nauta and Corten compared the early family experiences of 90 gifted adults
(2002), though, note that gifted adults may have difficulty at (ranging in age from 26 to 35 years) and 90 average ability
work when the work environment is not conducive to the adults (ranging in age from 26 to 35 years) who were a
needs and characteristics of a gifted individual. Persson matched comparison group. They also examined the current
(2009), in a study of 287 members of Mensa (216 men and family environment of both groups of individuals. There were
71 women), all of who ranked in the 98th percentile or higher statistically significant differences regarding marital status
for intelligence, found that intellectually gifted adults were and number of children between the two groups. Among the
most happy in their careers when they were business owners gifted group, 47.8% of them were single and 50% were mar-
or in positions of leadership that allowed for considerable ried, compared with 28.9% single and 68.9% married in the
autonomy. comparison group. Regarding number of children, 65.6% of

Downloaded from gcq.sagepub.com by guest on September 8, 2015


224 Gifted Child Quarterly 59(4)

the gifted group and 48.9% of the comparison group had no and childbearing and childrearing (Perrone et al., 2004;
children. Among the gifted group, 23.3% had one child and Super 1980).
11.11% had two or more children. Among the comparison
group, 28.9% had one child and 22.2% had two or more chil-
Unique Experiences of Women
dren. Lubinski et al. (2006) examined the reproduction rates
of 286 male and 94 female former SMPY participants (mean Most research suggests gifted women and gifted men (and
age of 33.6 years), which represents the ablest cohort of all gifted girls and gifted boys) have different experiences
SMPY participants (within the top 0.01%). Of these, 62.2% (Leder, 2004). Historically, women have been subjected to
of males and 69.0% of females had no children. Of those with sex role stereotyping and have had a lack of opportunity dur-
children, the majority had only one child. Of all participants, ing childhood and adolescence, thus making it less likely that
the mean number of children for male participants was 0.61 women would attain eminence during adulthood (Kronborg,
and for female participants was 0.44. 2010; Lovecky, 1993). Even if identified as gifted during
While the Sękowski and Siekańska (2008) and Lubinski childhood, Kerr (1997) suggests that objective measures
et al. (2006) studies examined adults at or under the age of such as IQ scores, aptitude test scores, and grade point aver-
35, which could account for the low numbers of children, in ages have failed to predict the achievement of gifted women
general, the reproduction rates of individuals with a high IQ in adulthood. And, once in adulthood, gifted women are
are lower than those of average ability and lower ability more prone to experiencing the imposter syndrome, or the
adults (Shatz, 2008). Among the 1964-1968 Presidential doubting and discrediting of one’s abilities and achievements
Scholars, at around the age of 60 years, 81% of the Scholars (Bell, 1990). For these reasons, in examining career and fam-
were married (or in marriage-like relationships, such as ily interaction among gifted adults, some researchers specifi-
domestic partnerships and civil unions). However, most had cally focus on the unique experiences of women.
small families, such that 23% had no children, 52% had one Walker, Reis, and Leonard (1992) examined the experi-
or two children, 16% had three children, and 9% had four or ences of a sample of 544 gifted women representative of the
more children (Kaufmann & Matthews, 2012). decades from the 1910s to the 1980s who were graduates of
a highly selective school for intellectually gifted females.17
While 92% of the women were college graduates, and 73%
Career and Family Interaction of the women either did not have children or chose to com-
While there is no single path to eminence in adulthood, inter- bine career with homemaking, those women who chose not
personal relationships can influence one’s personal and pro- to work outside the home were found to be happier and less
fessional development (Mockros, 1996). For example, in a depressed than their working counterparts. The homemakers
study of the career goals of 113 gifted adults (ranging in age were also found to be less ambitious and less adventurous
from 30 to 32 years) from the 1988 Midwestern Study, than their working counterparts. In more recent research on
Perrone et al. (2004) conducted interviews regarding barriers women, Ferriman, Lubinski, and Benbow (2009) examined
to, and supports of, career goal attainment and family goal Cohort 5 of the SMPY study and found gender differences
attainment. They found the two most common career goals over time (between ages 25 and 35 of the participants). By
among this sample were to “achieve job success or promo- age 35, women placed more emphasis on flexibility in work
tion” and “updating skills and credentials” (p. 126). The larg- schedule, limiting the amount of time devoted to work, and
est barrier to these career goals was a commitment to not working on weekends than did men, whereas there were
non-work roles (e.g., family and community), and the most no gender differences in these work-related preferences at
commonly cited sources of support for achieving career age 25. Furthermore, more than 50% of the women with chil-
goals were “support from family” and “professional support” dren at age 35 rated working fewer than 50 to 60 hours per
(p. 127). Regarding family goals, the most commonly cited week as extremely important, but less than 20% of men with
goal revolved around children: either having more children children, women without children, and men without children
or wanting to stop having children. The second most com- had the same preference. Ferriman et al. (2009) also found
monly cited family goal was related to having a “good qual- that women with children had the highest levels of life satis-
ity of family life” (p. 126). The largest barriers to these faction, followed by men with children and men without
family goals were commitment to career and nonfamily roles children. Women without children reported the lowest levels
and financial concerns. The most commonly cited sources of of life satisfaction.
support for achieving family goals were “having adequate Subotnik and Arnold (1995) interviewed 11 elite female
financial resources” and “work flexibility of participants and scientists18 (ranging in age from 29 to 31 years) about issues
their spouses” (p. 127). These findings indicate gifted adults related to their career establishment, particularly as it per-
have a desire to be successful in their careers, but to also bal- tained to life satisfaction and their personal lives. They
ance that success with having a family. These goals reflect found that romantic relationships act as either brakes or sup-
typical goals of individuals in their early 30s, which is con- ports to the women’s career establishment and achievement.
sidered to be a time of striving for security in one’s career, Several issues affected the women’s careers: her own values

Downloaded from gcq.sagepub.com by guest on September 8, 2015


Rinn and Bishop 225

regarding the relative importance of family versus work, the decisions (i.e., the top five most important decisions partici-
relative priority of her or her partner’s career, job opportuni- pants had made since graduating from high school) and the
ties in her and her partner’s fields, the attitude of her partner effect those had on life satisfaction. The most common cate-
toward household and childbearing tasks, and mutual sup- gory for participants’ best life decision was related to an
port for each other’s careers. Similarly, Kitano and Perkins increase in level of commitment in romantic relationships
(1996) examined factors affecting the achievement of 16 (e.g., getting married). Regarding one’s worst life decision,
gifted women19 who come from countries other than the participants most commonly noted missed opportunity.
United States (Africa, Asia, Latin America and the Interestingly, the most common result of worst life decisions
Caribbean, Eastern and Western Europe). The women was dissatisfaction with, or termination of, a romantic rela-
ranged in age from 25 to 43 years, with a mean age of 35 tionship. Among a different subsample of 99 adults (40 men,
years. The women identified the following factors that sup- 59 women; all between the ages of 33 and 35 years) from the
ported their achievement: personal characteristics or behav- 1988 Midwestern Study, Perrone et al. (2006) found marital
iors; family members; teachers, advisors, or education in satisfaction (and existential well-being, defined as one’s
general; financial aid; peers or colleagues; faith in God; and sense of meaning and purpose in life) to be a predictor of life
luck, chance, or circumstance. Obstacles they encountered satisfaction. These findings support previous researchers
included financial limitations, being female, conflicting who have noted an association between strong romantic rela-
roles or values, personal characteristics, poor teachers or tionships and life satisfaction among the gifted. Hansen and
education, lack or advisors and role models, political cir- Hall (1997), for example, examined the impact of marriage
cumstances (e.g., civil war, totalitarian government), lack of on a sample of 167 high-ability women (mean age = 50
peer support, and racism. years)21 and found those in supportive and collaborative rela-
tionships were more likely to view themselves as having
high potential for achievement and increased motivation for
Life Goals, Satisfaction, and Well-Being success in both personal and professional goals. Sears (1977)
Whether they attain eminence or not, most research indicates examined a group of gifted men from the Terman Study of
positive longitudinal outcomes for high ability individuals the Gifted (mean age = 62 years) and found that those who
(Lubinski & Benbow, 2006). For example, individuals with a were happily married had the highest levels of life satisfac-
high IQ have been shown to be more creative, earn more tion. In a 3-year study of 242 men and women from the
money, be physically healthier, and to live longer lives (Batty, Terman Study of the Gifted (mean age = 83.8 years in 1996
Shipley, Gale, Mortensen, & Deary, 2008). Furthermore, and mean age = 86.4 years in 1999), social relationships with
research typically indicates a positive relationship between both friends and family continued to play a central role in the
level of intelligence and subjective well-being (Diener, 1984; lives of these individuals. In contrast, participants’ intellec-
Watten, Syversen, & Myhrer, 1995; Wulff, Bergman, & tual pursuits and productivity declined during this time
Sverke, 2009), defined largely as “people’s emotional period (Holahan & Velasquez, 2011).
responses, domain satisfactions, and global judgments of life The research in this section generally supports the idea
satisfaction” (Diener, Suh, Lucas, & Smith, 1999, p. 277). that gifted adults largely have positive experiences with their
In a study of 101 intellectually gifted adults20 (mean careers and their families of procreation. Most gifted adults
IQ = 136, SD = 8) and 91 adults of average intelligence included in the research presented here found satisfaction in
(mean IQ = 103, SD = 9), all aged 28 years old, Wirthwein both their careers and their personal lives. For those gifted
and Rost (2011b) examined the subjective well-being of both adults who are not satisfied with their life choices, or who
groups. They found that intellectually gifted adults were nei- have unresolved traumatic experiences from childhood, dual
ther “happy” nor “unhappy” relative to their peers of average diagnoses, relationship difficulties, or other issues, counsel-
intelligence. Small statistically significant differences were ing may be helpful.
found in two domains, though. Within the domain of leisure,
gifted adults were somewhat less satisfied than their peers of
Counseling
average intelligence (d = −.28). Furthermore, in examining
which domains of life were most relevant for general life sat- Similar to the experiences of gifted children, some clinicians
isfaction, the only domain contributing to life satisfaction indicate that gifted adults may face relatively unique issues
among gifted adults was “work.” Among adults of average compared with their chronological peers, some of which
intelligence, the domains of “self” and “friends” made sig- require professional help (see Peterson, 2009). For example,
nificant contributions to general life satisfaction. in both anecdotal and clinical reports about real clients,
As gifted individuals age, though, indicators of life satis- Grobman (2009), Hazell (1999), Prober (2008), and Willings
faction begin to include social relationships, in addition to (1985) suggest the main challenges for gifted adults who
one’s career. In a study of 57 gifted adults (ranging in age seek therapy are addressing painful school and childhood
from 36 to 39 years) from the 1988 Midwestern Study, experiences, high sensitivity, emotional intensity, existential
Perrone-McGovern et al. (2011) examined the role major life depression, perfectionism, multi-potentiality, relationship

Downloaded from gcq.sagepub.com by guest on September 8, 2015


226 Gifted Child Quarterly 59(4)

difficulties, and suicide ideation. Rocamora (1992) goes fur- example of a twice exceptional adult, Weidenheim, Escobar,
ther, suggesting that there are different sets of issues for the and Rapin (2012) discussed the neuropathology of a 63-year-
recognized and unrecognized gifted. While the recognized old gifted man with Asperger syndrome on his death. He was
gifted experience inner and external pressures as well as the described as having superior intellect, with a particular talent
need for public recognition, the unrecognized gifted struggle for mathematics (he earned a doctorate in mathematics), and
to come to terms with the label and with the pressure to be fulfilled the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental
normal. The counseling relationship offers both the recog- Disorders Text Revision (DSM IV-TR) criteria for Asperger
nized and unrecognized gifted adult, perhaps for the first syndrome in an intellectually gifted individual (see American
time, an opportunity to be understood and valued (Jacobsen, Psychiatric Association, 2000). Although this case study is
1999b). not generalizable, it provides some evidence that twice
Although there is little research regarding the misdiagno- exceptional individuals can overcome obstacles and have
sis of the gifted (see Hartnett, Nelson, & Rinn, 2004), Webb successful careers.
et al. (2006) suggested that more than 25% of gifted children
may be misdiagnosed. This could be even greater for gifted
Discussion
adults, for while gifted children have their counseling needs
provided by school counselors and child psychologists, The purpose of the current study was to explore the research
gifted adults, often unrecognized, may seek out the counsel- literature on gifted adults in order to (a) begin a conversation
ing services of private practitioners, the majority of who are regarding the characteristics, experiences, and needs of
unlikely to be have any training on the subject of giftedness gifted adults; (b) identify gaps in the literature regarding
or may be pathology-focused (Bourdeau & Thomas, 2003). gifted adults; and (c) to suggest areas of future research per-
According to Alvarado (1989), a counselor’s failure to rec- taining to the characteristics, experiences, and needs of gifted
ognize giftedness, a lack of understanding of the issues adults. We categorized the research into themes: (a) whether
related to being gifted, and evaluating gifted adults using or not gifted children become gifted adults; (b) family of ori-
normal populations as a baseline, complicates the problems gin; (c) effects of early educational experiences; (d) charac-
of the gifted. These factors increase the risk of misdiagnosis teristics of gifted adults; (e) career; (f) family of procreation;
as well as decrease the likelihood that gifted adults will (g) career and family interaction; (h) life goals, satisfaction,
receive the counseling that they need. For example, the par- and well-being; and (i) counseling. A review of the coverage
ticipant22 in Peterson’s (2012) 15-year longitudinal case across thematic sections reveals two noteworthy findings.
study reported the continued efforts of various psychological First, the majority of research on gifted adults discusses the
professionals to pathologize her emotional intensities, well childhood experiences of those gifted adults, be it a discus-
into adulthood, with one even suggesting a diagnosis of sion of whether or not early identification of giftedness is
schizophrenia. predictive of adult accomplishments/eminence, examination
Some gifted adults legitimately have another exceptional- of one’s family of origin, or childhood educational experi-
ity, such as attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). ences. Fewer studies examine the current lives of gifted
Very little research exists in the area of twice exceptional adults, namely the characteristics of gifted adults, their
adults, but some researchers and practitioners have offered careers, their families of procreation, the interaction between
guidelines for facilitating the development of twice excep- career and family, and their life goals, satisfaction, and well-
tional adults. For example, Hayes (1994) discusses the tran- being. Very few studies examine twice exceptional adults
sition of the gifted/learning disabled adolescent to adulthood. and the counseling needs/experiences of gifted adults. In
Kutner (1999) discusses ADHD among gifted adults and order to better understand the characteristics, needs, and
suggests that “for some gifted AD/HD adults, without diag- experiences of gifted adults, more research should be con-
nosis and treatment, high intelligence may be a liability” (p. ducted on the current lives and experiences of gifted adults,
89), as gifted adults with ADHD are “decidedly different rather than retrospective examinations of their childhoods.
from gifted adults who have the ability to monitor their own Second, of the 60 studies included in Table 1, 25 of them
conscious thoughts and control their attention” (p. 96). defined adult giftedness on the basis of childhood or adoles-
Among a sample of 53 high IQ adults (full scale IQ > 120) cent standardized test scores (either on-level or above-level).
who did not have ADHD and 64 high IQ adults who met the The majority of these were from the SMPY studies and the
diagnostic criteria for ADHD, the twice exceptional adults 1988 Midwestern Study. Furthermore, 11 of the studies used
reported lower quality of life, had poorer familial and occu- childhood IQ scores as an indicator of adult giftedness, 10
pational functioning, had more functional impairments, and used high school class rank, and 3 used one’s status as an aca-
had more co-morbid psychiatric disorders (Antshel et al., demic prize winner during high school.23 Only 6 studies used
2009). In addition, the gifted adults with ADHD performed adult IQ scores and 10 studies used adult accomplishments as
worse than gifted adults without ADHD on tests of executive indicators of adult giftedness. These indicators of adult gifted-
functioning (Antshel et al., 2010). Not all twice exceptional ness lead us to the conclusion that most researchers in the field
adults will experience such difficulties, though. In another are defining adult giftedness based on childhood cognitive and

Downloaded from gcq.sagepub.com by guest on September 8, 2015


Rinn and Bishop 227

academic ability rather than performance during adulthood. families or experience traumatic events at an early age
The research included in this article supports the idea that, (Kaufmann & Matthews, 2012; Olszewski-Kubilius, 2000). It
while childhood giftedness appears to be an important predic- is unclear whether dysfunction and trauma during childhood is
tor of later eminence (Simonton & Song, 2009; Walberg et al., the exception or the rule, though, as other researchers suggest
1996), eminence is not required for the label of adult gifted- family or parental support during childhood and adolescence
ness. While not every gifted child achieves eminence in adult- are important precursors to later adult achievement (Campbell
hood, an individual’s giftedness does not “disappear” after & Feng, 2011; Terman, 1954). The available research on family
high school or college (Rinn & Plucker, 2004). of origin, though, suggests gifted adults come from smaller
In the definition of giftedness proposed by Subotnik et al. families (no siblings or one sibling; Sękowski & Siekańska,
(2011), giftedness “is typically manifested in actual out- 2008), have educated parents (Kaufmann, 1981; Sękowski &
comes, especially in adulthood” (p. 3). Yet what about the Siekańska, 2008; Terman, 1954), have at least one gifted parent
gifted adult whose outcomes are not so obvious? If a gifted (Perrone, Ksiazak, et al., 2010), had an abundance of books and
man or woman chooses to stay home and raise his or her chil- magazines in their childhood homes (Campbell & Feng, 2011),
dren rather than have a career outside the home, does he or and had a high socioeconomic status as a child (Campbell &
she cease being gifted? Or, if a gifted man or woman chooses Feng, 2011). In most of the studies examined here, childhood
a career in a field that is not preceded by graduate education dysfunction and trauma were simply not examined. Research
or advanced training, do they also renounce their giftedness? on adverse childhood experiences with average ability popula-
Is there a “time limit” by which an adult must achieve emi- tions typically shows negative adult outcomes, such as health
nence before the possibility of doing so ceases to exist? problems and a tendency to engage in risky behavior (e.g.,
Subotnik et al., when speaking of outcomes, are actually talk- Felitti et al., 1998), suggesting more research is needed to
ing about eminence. There are a number of factors, though, examine the role of giftedness in predicting eminence among
outside of giftedness or talent that can determine eminence, those individuals who have experienced trauma during
such as one’s family of origin and one’s early educational childhood.
experiences, as seen in the research included in this article. A number of researchers have suggested that early educa-
The experience of transitioning from gifted child to gifted tional advantage is one of the most important factors that
adult does not necessarily depend on the adult producing in a contribute to later adult achievement (Bloom, 1985; Feldman,
manner acceptable to his or her society. Future research 1986; Gagne, 1995; Oden, 1968; Terman, 1925). Acceleration
should examine the career decision-making processes of and enrichment, during childhood and adolescence, seem to
gifted adults in order to understand why some gifted adults contribute to productivity, achievement, and excellence dur-
choose or aim for a path of eminence and why some do not, ing adulthood (Firkowska-Mankiewicz, 2011; Park et al.,
and/or for whom pursuing eminence is an option. 2012; Perrone, Wright, et al., 2010; Wai et al., 2010; Watve,
Giftedness can be seen as an inherent capacity for excep- 2008). These findings shed a positive light on gifted educa-
tional performance within a particular talent domain, not tion in the United States, such that components of gifted edu-
necessarily in the product of that talent. Not all gifted adults cation during the elementary and secondary grades appear
seek to achieve eminence with their gifts, nor do they always beneficial for future endeavors during adulthood. Consistent
have the opportunity to do so. When eminence is made the with recommendations from the National Association for
requirement of classification of “gifted adult,” when results Gifted Children, we maintain that
or products become necessary criteria, inequity arises, and
we ignore the role of free will in determining how those gifts to educate all of our children and allow them to compete in a
are applied. Consequently, we suggest adult giftedness and global economy in all fields of human endeavor, the nation must
eminence should be examined as independent constructs in provide an environment in which gifted and talented students,
future research. While Subotnik et al. (2011) suggest gifted- along with all our children, can reach their full potential. . . . To
ness is developmental, such that an individual moves from do otherwise is to shortchange a significant segment of the K-12
potential to achievement to eminence, we argue adult gifted- population that deserves an appropriate education and can make
ness, as in childhood, is still based on potential. Achievement important contributions to our society. (National Association for
and eminence are wonderful and desirable outcomes, but Gifted Children, 2013, p. 1)
they should not necessarily be a requirement for adult gifted-
ness. Eminence, too, should be defined in future research. Research on gifted adults’ choice of romantic partners sup-
Most of the research examined here views eminence more ports the similarity–attraction hypothesis, suggesting gifted
along the lines of high achievement than significantly affect- adults are romantically attracted to other gifted or high ability
ing one’s field. Subotnik et al. call for a distinction between adults (Benbow et al., 2000; Byrne, 1971; Byrne & Nelson,
high achievement and eminence, but current research does 1965; Dijkstra et al., 2012). Furthermore, those gifted adults
not seem to make that distinction. in romantic relationships with other gifted or high ability
Regarding the family backgrounds of gifted adults, some adults are likely to be more satisfied with their relationships,
researchers argue that eminent adults come from dysfunctional at least during the early 30s (Perrone-McGovern et al., 2012).

Downloaded from gcq.sagepub.com by guest on September 8, 2015


228 Gifted Child Quarterly 59(4)

The available literature regarding romantic relationships, contributors to life satisfaction include romantic relationships
however, is limited in terms of representing the entire popula- (Perrone et al., 2006; Perrone-McGovern et al., 2011) and
tion of gifted adults. Dijkstra et al. (2012), for example, draw parenting (Ferriman et al., 2009). Strong romantic relation-
conclusions about the desired characteristics of romantic part- ships are even viewed as helpful in attaining professional
ners from a sampling of Mensa members, whose motivations goals during early and middle adulthood (Hansen & Hall,
for seeking membership in the high IQ group may affect the 1997). By late adulthood (60s and on), romantic relationships
study results and may not be representative of all gifted adults. and social relationships with friends and family contribute the
Also, it is important to note that the available research only most to life satisfaction, while intellectual and career pursuits
examines heterosexual couples. Research on the romantic provide less life satisfaction over time (Holahan & Velasquez,
relationships of gifted gay, lesbian, bisexual, transgendered, 2011; Sears, 1977).
or questioning (GLBTQ) adults has not been conducted at For gifted adults, counseling can play a considerable role
this point in time, suggesting an obvious need for future in their emotional growth. Gifted adults seek counseling for
research in this area. a variety of reasons, including addressing painful school
In the early 30s, gifted adults have a 50% chance or so of experiences, high sensitivity, emotional intensity, existential
being married, but the majority of gifted adults at this time depression, perfectionism, multi-potentiality, relationship
either have no children or only one child (Lubinski et al., difficulties, suicidal ideation, and career counseling (Delisle
2006; Sękowski & Siekańska, 2008). By their 60s, gifted & Squires, 1989; Karnes & Oehler-Stinnett, 1986; Prober,
adults are likely to be married, but still have small families 2008; Willings, 1985). Additionally, some gifted adults expe-
(either no children or one to two children; Kaufmann & rienced childhood psychological abuse that still presents
Matthews, 2012). Research repeatedly shows a negative cor- psychological problems for them, with a number of those
relation between IQ and fertility rates, both in the United affected seeking counseling (Ruf, 2001). The literature sug-
States and European countries (Lynn, 1996; Lynn & Van gests, though, that counselors do not appear to be prepared to
Court, 2004; Rodgers, Cleveland, van den Oord, & Rowe, address the needs of the gifted adult (Alvarado, 1989). One
2000) and between nations using national IQ scores and shortcoming of the literature addressing counselor compe-
national fertility rates (Shatz, 2008). It is not surprising, then, tency is that the evidence has been, thus far, largely anec-
that gifted adults are not having children at the same rates as dotal, and more research needs to be conducted to validate
average and lower ability adults. that general perception.
For those gifted adults who do decide to have children,
they often must combine this desire with a desire to be suc- Limitations and Directions for Future
cessful in their careers, which may also explain the small
family sizes of gifted adults. These goals reflect typical goals
Research
of individuals in their early 30s, which is considered to be a The current study has unveiled a number of limitations with
time of striving for security in one’s career, and childbearing regard to the study of gifted adults. As discussed earlier in
and childrearing (Perrone et al., 2004; Super 1980). The this article, some research on gifted adults is flawed because
gifted adults across several studies included in this article are of imprecise and unclear definitions of giftedness, small
a highly educated group, with more than half of them earning sample sizes, lack of adequate comparison groups, numerous
doctorate degrees (Kaufmann & Matthews, 2012; Lubinski studies from the same databases, and anecdotal descriptions
et al., 2006), suggesting some overlap in the timing of one’s rather than empirical observations. All of these issues arose
graduate education and one’s childbearing/childrearing years. in the current review. In addition, the quantitative research in
By age 35, gifted women with children or those planning for this review is correlational in nature, thus limiting our ability
children begin to place more emphasis on flexibility in work to make causal inferences. Furthermore, we see numerous
schedule than do men (Ferriman et al., 2009). It appears that words used to describe adult giftedness, including eminence,
women, in particular, still have to compromise some aspect of creative production, and achievement. Because of the multi-
having a career and having a family. In academia, for exam- ple definitions of adult giftedness, it is very possible that we
ple, the research productivity of women professors declines missed important research studies, or even fields of research,
after the birth of a child (Hunter & Leahey, 2010). in collecting articles for this study. As with defining gifted-
Research typically indicates that high ability adults have ness in children and adolescents, defining giftedness in
positive outcomes later in life (Batty et al., 2008; Lubinski & adulthood is complicated. Future research should consider
Benbow, 2006) and are largely happy and satisfied with their the current study in defining adult giftedness, be it eminent
lives (Diener, 1984; Watten et al., 1995; Wulff et al., 2009). production during adulthood or a cognitive ability and psy-
During early adulthood, a gifted adult’s career contributes chosocial experience left over from childhood.
most heavily to life satisfaction (Wirthwein & Rost, 2011b) Future research should also consider replicating and
and most gifted adults across the studies examined here are extending previous research on gifted adults. Some of the
satisfied with their careers (Perrone, Tschopp, et al., 2010; topics included in the current analysis are not generalizable
Siekańska & Sękowski, 2006), but as gifted adults age, because they are drawn from isolated research studies and/or

Downloaded from gcq.sagepub.com by guest on September 8, 2015


Rinn and Bishop 229

convenience samples (e.g., research on twice exceptional adulthood, control group comparisons, and the replication
adults, individuals who voluntarily joined Mensa). and extension of previous studies. Research on eminence and
Replication and extension would provide a stronger sense of giftedness during adulthood may help researchers and educa-
the characteristics of the gifted adult as well as the experi- tors to better understand the experiences of gifted children
ences and needs of the gifted adult. and to give insight into those children who may become emi-
The current research on gifted adults is also limited in that nent adults, but also to help gifted adults better understand
it largely focuses on intellectual or academic giftedness. The themselves and to reach their own potential.
experiences of individuals from one domain-specific group,
such as the SMPY, may not be generalizable to gifted adults Declaration of Conflicting Interests
in other domains. Adults who are gifted in the areas of cre- The author(s) declared no potential conflicts of interest with respect
ativity, leadership, the arts, or physical domains were not to the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article.
included in the research we examined.
Some research on gifted adults (e.g., Issacs, 1961; Wilson, Funding
Sakata, & Frumkin, 1968), and gifted women in particular, is The author(s) received no financial support for the research, author-
outdated. For example, Holahan (1984-1985) examined life ship, and/or publication of this article.
goals at age 30 and perceptions of goal attainment and life
satisfaction at age 70 among a subsample of individuals par- Notes
ticipating in the Terman Study of the Gifted. She found men 1. Full citations appear when discussed in detail.
were predominately oriented toward occupational pursuits 2. As noted in the Method section, we use both the terms gifted
while women were predominately oriented toward home and and eminent throughout this article, depending on the study
family life. As we have seen from more current research referenced.
(Subotnik & Arnold, 1995; Walker et al., 1992), women are 3. In this historiometric analysis, whereby biographical informa-
more likely to have careers outside the home than they did tion about eminent adults was subjected to measurement and
prior to the women’s liberation movement of the 1960s and quantitative analysis, giftedness was assessed by raters blind
1970s. Future research should examine modern issues related to the identity of the individuals being evaluated. The Creative
Achievement Scale (Ludwig, 1992) was used to provide esti-
to women and men combining career and family including
mates of early giftedness based on adult eminence.
maternity and paternity leave, childcare arrangements, and 4. “Five groups of America’s elite (total N = 2,254) were exam-
the role of step-families and blended families, among other ined: Fortune 500 CEOs, federal judges, billionaires, Senators,
issues. and members of the House of Representatives” (Wai, 2013,
Related to research on adult women, Maitra (2012) sug- p. 203).
gests there is a need to discuss the importance of motherhood 5. “To investigate who becomes a member of the global elite,
when conducting research on gifted women. “Mothering” is three groups were examined: the world’s billionaires (N =
often cited in gifted women’s talent development as a barrier 1426), most powerful people (N = 231) according to Forbes
to one’s career, with the balance between career and mother- magazine, and World Economic Forum (Davos) attendees
hood being particularly difficult for women in certain careers. (N = 2624)” (Wai, 2014a, p. 54).
For example, in a study of 15 artists and 4 professionals, all 6. The 1964 to 1968 cohort of Presidential Scholars was chosen
from a pool of 14,000 candidates who scored in the top 0.5%
of whom are mothers, the artists experienced more conflict
on the 1963 to 1967 National Merit Scholarship Qualifying
between their work and motherhood than did the profes- Test (a measure of verbal and mathematical abilities). Each
sional women (Foley, 1996). Yet women who are mothers year, 121 Presidential Scholars were selected from this pool
and also have careers in the STEM fields also report having based on scores on other standardized tests, recommendations
difficulty balancing family and career, as well as facing dis- from high school officials, and autobiographical statements.
crimination, negative perceptions, and unequal amounts of 7. Giftedness was self-reported and required at least one 99th
pressure (Monosson, 2008). Prober (1999) suggests gifted percentile subscale score on a nationally recognized standard-
women undertake mothering with the same intensity that ized test of ability taken during childhood.
they bring to the rest of their lives, and that researchers and 8. The women were identified as eminent on the basis of their
others would benefit from understanding how motherhood inclusion in the Who’s Who of Canadian Women. These
affects the careers of gifted women and how being a gifted “women were recognized for their exceptional achievement
in all facets of public life: politics, sports, the arts, education,
woman affects one’s ability to parent.
literature, journalism, the professions and so on” (Yewchuk &
Schlosser, 1995, p. 79).
Conclusion 9. The Olympians examined in this study were 190 United States
Mathematics, Physics, and Chemistry Olympians from cohorts
Gifted adults are a relatively unexamined group in the spanning the 1970s to the 2000s. The intellectual ability of the
research literature. Comprehensive studies of the characteris- Olympians was determined by constructing a composite of
tics, experiences, and needs of gifted adults are warranted, but standardized test scores (SAT, PSAT, and GRE), grade point
with a particular focus on clearly defining giftedness during averages, and high school ranking at graduation.

Downloaded from gcq.sagepub.com by guest on September 8, 2015


230 Gifted Child Quarterly 59(4)

10. A “gifted adult” in this study was one who was a prizewinner References
of school subject contests at the nationwide (Poland) level dur-
ing secondary school. Achter, J. A., Benbow, C. P., & Lubinski, D. (1997). Rethinking
11. The men had been identified as gifted during the school years multipotentiality among the intellectually gifted: A critical
“on the basis of standardized intelligence tests scores and . . . review and recommendations. Gifted Child Quarterly, 41,
on the Raven’s Standard Progressive Matrices test” (Watve, 5-15. doi:10.1177/001698629704100102
2008, p. 255). Albert, R. S. (1978). Observations and suggestions regarding gift-
12. Giftedness was operationalized according to Spearman’s g edness, familial influence, and the achievement of eminence.
(1927), based on three intelligence test scores obtained during Gifted Child Quarterly, 22, 201-211.
both childhood and adolescence. Participants were drawn from Albert, R. S. (1991). People, processes, and developmental paths
the Marburg Giftedness Project (Rost, 2009). to eminence: A developmental-interactional model. In R.
13. The adults were identified as gifted either because they were M. Milgram (Ed.), Counseling gifted and talented children:
members of Mensa or because they qualified for similar status A guide for teachers, counselors, and parents (pp. 75-93).
on the basis of high GRE, SAT, or IQ scores, former place- Norwood, NJ: Ablex.
ment in gifted classes, or known and recognized scholarly Alvarado, N. (1989). Adjustment of gifted adults. Advanced
achievement. Development, 1, 77-86.
14. To be a member of the Swedish Mensa Society, one must have American Psychiatric Association. (2000). Diagnostic and statisti-
an IQ of 130 or more. cal manual of mental disorders (4th ed., text rev.). Washington,
15. Participants in this study were identified as gifted on the basis DC: Author.
of their status as doctoral students. Antshel, K. M., Faraone, S. V., Maglione, K., Doyle, A., Fried, R.,
Seidman, L., & Biederman, J. (2009). Is adult attention defi-
16. A “gifted adult” in this study was one who was a prizewinner
cit hyperactivity disorder a valid diagnosis in the presence of
of school subject contests at the nationwide (Poland) level dur-
high IQ? Psychological Medicine, 39, 1325-1335. doi:10.1017/
ing secondary school.
S0033291708004959
17. Criteria for admission and indicators of giftedness were not
Antshel, K. M., Faraone, S. V., Maglione, K., Doyle, A., Fried, R.,
included in this article.
Seidman, L., & Biederman, J. (2010). Executive functioning
18. Women were selected from the Westinghouse Science Talent
in high-IQ adults with ADHD. Psychological Medicine, 40,
Search 1983 cohort (N = 146; Subotnik, Duschl, & Selmon,
1909-1918. doi:10.1017/S0033291709992273
1993) and the Illinois Valedictorian Project 1981 cohort (n =
Arnett, J. J. (2000). Emerging adulthood: A theory of develop-
46 women; Arnold, 1993, 1994). The Westinghouse Science
ment from the late teens through the twenties. American
Talent Search is a nationwide annual contest for high school
Psychologist, 55, 469-480. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.55.5.469
seniors gifted in science or mathematics. Students compete Arnold, K. D. (1993). Academically talented women in the
by submitting a technical paper describing an experiment or 1980s: The Illinois valedictorian project. In K.D. Hulbert &
proof. The Illinois Valedictorian Project is a longitudinal study D.T. Schuster (Eds.), Women’s lives through time: Educated
exploring the postsecondary educational and career achieve- American women of the twentieth century (pp. 393-414). San
ments of valedictorians and salutatorians since their 1981 high Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass.
school graduation. Arnold, K. D. (1994). The Illinois Valedictorian Project: Early adult
19. Participants were the recipients of 1-year scholarship for grad- careers of academically talented male high school students.
uate or post-graduate study or research in the United States In R. F. Subotnik & K. D. Arnold (Eds.), Beyond Terman:
awarded to women who demonstrate outstanding academic Contemporary longitudinal studies of giftedness and talent (pp.
ability and who are not citizens or permanent residents of the 24-51). Norwood, NJ: Ablex.
United States. Arnold, K. D., Noble, K. D., & Subotnik, R. F. (Eds.). (1995).
20. Intelligence was assessed in both childhood and adolescence. Remarkable women: Perspectives on female talent develop-
Giftedness was operationalized according to Spearman’s g ment. New York, NY: Hampton Press.
(1927), based on three intelligence test scores. Participants Ashton, M. C. (2013). Individual differences and personality (2nd
were drawn from the Marburg Giftedness Project (Rost, ed.). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
2009). Batty, G. D., Shipley, M. J., Gale, C. R., Mortensen, L. H., &
21. These women “were not formally identified as gifted, how- Deary, I. J. (2008). Does IQ predict total and cardiovascular
ever, they scored in the top 10% of the population on mea- disease mortality as strongly as other risk factors? Comparison
sures of aptitude and achievement to gain entry into the of effect estimates using the Vietnam Experience Study. Heart,
University of Michigan. Nearly all women met and exceeded 94, 1541-1544. doi:10.1136/hrt.2008.149567
the University’s entrance requirements, and 80% of the alum- Baxendale, S. (2011). IQ and ability across the adult life span.
nae were honor and scholarship award winners” (Hansen & Applied Neuropsychology, 18, 164-167. doi:10.1080/0908428
Hall, 1997, p. 171). 2.2011.595442
22. Clear information on the identification of giftedness was not Bell, L. A. (1990). The gifted woman as imposter. Advanced
provided. Peterson (2012) says the participant “was identified Development, 2, 55-64.
as gifted early in school” and “participated in a program for Benbow, C. P., Lubinski, D., Shea, D. L., & Eftekhari-Sanjani, H.
gifted children” (p. 297). (2000). Sex differences in mathematical reasoning ability at
23. There is overlap here, as some studies used multiple indicators age 13: Their status 20 years later. Psychological Science, 11,
of adult giftedness. 474-480. doi:10.1111/1467-9280.00291

Downloaded from gcq.sagepub.com by guest on September 8, 2015


Rinn and Bishop 231

Bloom, B. (Ed.). (1985). Developing talent in young people. New Diener, E. (1984). Subjective well-being. Psychological Bulletin,
York, NY: Ballantine. 95, 542-575. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.95.3.542
Bouchard, T. J., Jr., & McGue, M. (1981). Familial studies of intel- Diener, E., Suh, E. M., Lucas, R. E., & Smith, H. L. (1999). Subjective
ligence: A review. Science, 212, 1055-1059. doi:10.1126/sci- well-being: Three decades of progress. Psychological Bulletin,
ence.7195071 125, 276-302. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.125.2.276
Bourdeau, B., & Thomas, V. (2003). Counseling gifted clients and Dijkstra, P., Barelds, D. P. H., Groothof, H. A. K., Ronner, S., &
their families: Comparing clients’ and counselors’ perspec- Nauta, A. P. (2012). Partner preferences of the intellectually
tives. Journal of Secondary Gifted Education, 14, 114-126. gifted. Marriage & Family Review, 48, 96-108. doi:10.1080/0
doi:10.4219/jsge-2006-415 1494929.2011.628779
Brodnick, R. J., & Ree, M. J. (1995). A structural model of aca- Emmett, J. D., & Minor, C. W. (1993). Career decision-making fac-
demic performance, socioeconomic status, and Spearman’s g. tors in gifted young adults. Career Development Quarterly, 41,
Educational and Psychological Measurement, 55, 583-594. 350-366. doi:10.1002/j.2161-0045.1993.tb00409.x
doi:10.1177/0013164495055004006 Falk, R. F., Lind, S., Miller, N. B., Piechowski, M. M., &
Byrne, D. (1971). The attraction paradigm. New York, NY: Silverman, L. K. (1999). The Overexcitability Questionnaire-
Academic Press. Two (OEQII): Manual, scoring system, and questionnaire.
Byrne, D., & Nelson, D. (1965). Attraction as a linear function of Denver, CO: Institute for the Study of Advanced Development.
proportion of positive reinforcements. Journal of Personality Feldman, D. H. (1984). A follow-up of subjects scoring above
and Social Psychology, 1, 659-663. doi:10.1037/h0022073 180 IQ in Terman’s “Genetic Studies of Genius.” Exceptional
Campbell, J. R., & Feng, A. X. (2011). Comparing adult pro- Children, 50, 518-523. Retrieved from http://ecx.sagepub.com/
ductivity of American mathematics, chemistry, and physics content/50/6/518.short
Olympians with Terman’s longitudinal study. Roeper Review, Feldman, D. H. (1986). Nature’s gambit: Child prodigies and the
33, 18-25. doi:10.1080/02783193.2011.530203 development of human potential. New York, NY: Basic Books.
Cooper, C. (2010). Individual differences and personality (3rd ed.). Felitti, V. J., Anda, R. F., Nordenberg, D., Williamson, D.
London, England: Hodder Education. F., Spitz, A. M., Edwards, V., & . . . Marks, J. S. (1998).
Cox, C. M. (1926). Genetic studies of genius: The early men- Relationship of childhood abuse and household dysfunction
tal traits of three hundred geniuses (Vol. 2). Stanford, CA: to many of the leading causes of death in adults: The adverse
Stanford University Press. childhood experiences (ACE) study. American Journal of
Csikszentmihalyi, M., Rathunde, K., & Whalen, S. (1993). Talented Preventive Medicine, 14, 245-258. doi:10.1016/S0749-
teenagers: The roots of success and failure. Cambridge, 3797(98)00017-8
England: Cambridge University Press. Ferriman, K., Lubinski, D., & Benbow, C. P. (2009). Work pref-
Dabrowski, K. (1937). Psychological basis of self-mutilation. erences, life values, and personal views of top math/science
Genetic Psychology Monographs, 19, 1-104. graduate students and the profoundly gifted: Developmental
Dabrowski, K. (1964). Positive disintegration. Boston, MA: Little, changes and gender differences during emerging adulthood
Brown. and parenthood. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
Dabrowski, K. (1972). Psychoneurosis is not an illness: Neuroses 97, 517-532. doi:10.1037/a0016030
and psychoneuroses from the perspective of positive disinte- Firkowska-Mankiewicz, A. (2011). Adult careers: Does childhood
gration. London, England: Gryf. IQ predict later life outcome? Journal of Policy and Practice
Dabrowski, K., Kawczak, A., & Piechowski, M. M. (1970). Mental in Intellectual Disabilities, 8, 1-9. doi:10.1111/j.1741-
growth through positive disintegration. London, England: 1130.2011.00281.x
Gryf. Foley, P. W. (1996). Artists who mother. Advanced Development,
Daniels, S., & Piechowski, M. M. (Eds.). (2008). Living with 7, 1-18.
intensity: Understanding the sensitivity, excitability, and emo- Freeman, J. (2010). Gifted lives: What happens when gifted chil-
tional development of gifted children, adolescents, and adults. dren grow up? New York, NY: Routledge.
Scottsdale, AZ: Great Potential Press. Freeman, K. A., & Walberg, H. J. (1999). Childhood traits and
Deary, I. J., Whalley, L. J., Lemmon, H., Crawford, J. R., & Starr, conditions of eminent African American women. Journal
J. M. (2000). The stability of individual differences in men- for the Education of the Gifted, 22, 402-419. doi:10.1177/
tal ability from childhood to old age: Follow-up of the 1932 016235329902200405
Scottish Mental Survey. Intelligence, 28, 49-55. doi:10.1016/ Frey, M. C., & Detterman, D. K. (2004). Scholastic assessment or
S0160-2896(99)00031-8 g? The relationship between the Scholastic Assessment Test
Deary, I. J., Whiteman, M. C., Starr, J. M., Whalley, L. J., & Fox, and General Cognitive Ability. Psychological Science, 15,
H. C. (2004). The impact of childhood intelligence on later life: 373-378. doi:10.1111/j.0956-7976.2004.00687.x
Following up the Scottish Mental Surveys of 1932 and 1947. Gagne, F. (1995). Hidden meanings of the “talent devel-
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 86, 130-147. opment” concept. Educational Forum, 59, 350-362.
doi:10.1037/0022-3514.86.1.130 doi:10.1080/00131729509335067
Delisle, J., & Squires, S. (1989). Career development for gifted Gardner, H. (1983). Frames of mind: The theory of multiple intel-
and talented youth: Division on Career Development (DCD) ligences. New York, NY: Basic Books.
and The Association for the Gifted (TAG) position state- Glaser, B. G., & Strauss, A. L. (1967). The discovery of grounded
ment. Journal for the Education of the Gifted, 12, 65-70. theory: Strategies for qualitative research. Chicago, IL: Aldine
doi:10.1177/088572888901200202 Transaction.

Downloaded from gcq.sagepub.com by guest on September 8, 2015


232 Gifted Child Quarterly 59(4)

Goertzel, M. G., Goertzel, V., & Goertzel, T. G. (1978). Three Kaufmann, F. A., Harrel, G., Milam, C. P., Woolverton, N., &
hundred eminent personalities: A psychosocial analysis of the Miller, J. (1986). The nature, role, and influence of men-
famous. San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass. tors in the lives of gifted adults. Journal of Counseling &
Grobman, J. (2009). A psychodynamic psychotherapy approach to Development, 64, 576-578. doi:10.1002/j.1556-6676.1986.
the emotional problems of exceptionally and profoundly gifted tb01208.x
adolescents and adults: A psychiatrist’s experience. Journal for Kaufmann, F. A., & Matthews, D. J. (2012). On becoming them-
the Education of the Gifted, 33, 106-125. Retrieved from http:// selves: The 1964-1968 Presidential Scholars 40 years later.
files.eric.ed.gov/fulltext/EJ856178.pdf Roeper Review, 34, 83-93. doi:10.1080/02783193.2012.660682
Hansen, J. B., & Hall, E. G. (1997). Gifted women and mar- Keating, D. P. (2009). Developmental science and giftedness:
riage. Gifted Child Quarterly, 41, 169-180. doi:10.1177/ An integrated life-span framework. In F. D. Horowitz, R. F.
001698629704100406 Subotnik, & D. Matthews (Eds.), The development of gifted-
Hartnett, D. N., Nelson, J. M., & Rinn, A. N. (2004). Gifted or ness and talent across the lifespan (pp. 189-208). Washington,
ADHD? The possibilities of misdiagnosis. Roeper Review, 26, DC: American Psychological Association.
73-76. doi:10.1080/02783190409554245 Kell, H. J., Lubinski, D., & Benbow, C. P. (2013). Who rises to
Hayes, M. L. (1994). Gifted/learning disabled adolescent to adult. the top? Early indicators. Psychological Science, 24, 648-659.
Journal of Secondary Gifted Education, 5, 75-82. doi:10.1177/0956797612457784
Hazell, C. G. (1999). The experience of emptiness and the use of Kerr, B. A. (1997). Developing talents in girls and young women.
Dabrowski’s theory in counseling gifted clients: Clinical case In N. Colangelo & G. A. Davis (Eds.), Handbook of gifted edu-
examples. Advanced Development, 8, 31-46. cation (2nd ed., pp. 483-497). Needham Heights, MA: Allyn
Holahan, C. K. (1984-1985). The relationship between life goals at & Bacon.
thirty and perceptions of goal attainment and life satisfaction Kitano, M. K., & Perkins, C. O. (1996). International gifted women:
at seventy for gifted men and women. International Journal of Developing a critical human resource. Roeper Review, 19,
Aging and Human Development, 20, 21-31. 34-40. doi:10.1080/02783199609553781
Holahan, C. K., & Velasquez, K. S. (2011). Perceived strategies Kronborg, L. (2010). What contributes to talent development in
and activities for successful later aging. International Journal eminent women? Gifted and Talented International, 25(2), 11-
of Aging and Human Development, 72, 343-359. doi:10.2190/ 27. Retrieved from http://www.world-gifted.org/Publications/
AG.72.4.d GnTI-Journal
Horowitz, F. D., Subotnik, R. F., & Matthews, D. J. (Eds.). (2009). Kutner, D. R. (1999). Blurred brilliance: What AD/HD looks like in
The development of giftedness and talent across the life span. gifted adults. Advanced Development, 8, 87-96.
Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. Leder, G. (2004). Gender differences among gifted students:
Howard, R. W. (2008). Linking extreme precocity and adult emi- Contemporary views. High Ability Studies, 15, 103-108.
nence: A study of eight prodigies at international chess. High doi:10.1080/1359813042000225366
Ability Studies, 19, 117-130. doi:10.1080/13598130802503991 Lewis, R. B., Kitano, M. K., & Lynch, E. W. (1992). Psychological
Hunter, L. A., & Leahey, E. (2010). Parenting and research produc- intensities in gifted adults. Roeper Review, 15, 25-31.
tivity: New evidence and methods. Social Studies of Science, doi:10.1080/02783199209553452
40, 433-451. doi:10.1177/0306312709358472 Lovecky, D. V. (1986). Can you hear the flowers singing? Issues
Iremonger, L. (1970). The fiery chariot: A study of British prime for gifted adults. Journal of Counseling and Development, 64,
ministers and the search for love. London, England: Secker & 572-575. doi:10.1002/j.1556-6676.1986.tb01207.x
Warburg. Lovecky, D. V. (1993). Creative connections: Perspectives on
Issacs, A. F. (1961). Sources of motivation in cre- female giftedness. Advanced Development, 5, 117-129.
atively gifted adults. Gifted Child Quarterly, 5, 63-64. Lubinski, D., & Benbow, C. P. (2006). Study of mathematically
doi:10.1177/001698626100500208 precocious youth after 35 years: Uncovering antecedents for
Jacobsen, M. (1999a). The gifted adult: A revolutionary guide the development of math-science expertise. Perspectives
for liberating everyday genius. New York, NY: Ballantine on Psychological Science, 1, 316-345. doi:10.1111/j.1745-
Books. 6916.2006.00019.x
Jacobsen, M. (1999b). Arousing the sleeping giant: Giftedness Lubinski, D., Benbow, C. P., & Ryan, J. (1995). Stability of voca-
in adult psychotherapy. Roeper Review, 22, 36-41. tional interests among the intellectually gifted from adolescent
doi:10.1080/02783199909553995 to adulthood: A 15-year longitudinal study. Journal of Applied
Jensen, A. R. (1978). Genetic and behavioral effects of nonran- Psychology, 80, 196-200. doi:10.1037/0021-9010.80.1.196
dom mating. In R. T. Osborne, C. E. Noble, & N. Weyl (Eds.), Lubinski, D., Benbow, C. P., Webb, R. M., & Bleske-Rechek, A.
Human variation: The biopsychology of age, race, and sex (2006). Tracking exceptional human capital over two decades.
(pp. 51-105). New York, NY: Academic Press. Psychological Science, 17, 194-199. doi:10.1111/j.1467-
Karnes, F. A., & Oehler-Stinnett, J. J. (1986). Life events as stress- 9280.2006.01685.x
ors with gifted adolescents. Psychology in the Schools, 23, Lubinski, D., Schmidt, D. B., & Benbow, C. P. (1996). A 20-year
406-414. doi:10.1002/1520-6807(198610)23:4<406::AID- stability analysis of the Study of Values for intellectually gifted
PITS2310230416>3.0.CO;2-N individuals from adolescence to adulthood. Journal of Applied
Kaufmann, F. A. (1981). The 1964-1968 Presidential Scholars: A Psychology, 81, 443-451. doi:10.1037/0021-9010.81.4.443
follow-up study. Exceptional Children, 48, 164-169. Retrieved Ludwig, A. M. (1992). The Creative Achievement Scale. Creativity
from http://www.davidsongifted.org/db/Articles_id_10078.aspx Research Journal, 5, 109-119. doi:10.1080/10400419209534427

Downloaded from gcq.sagepub.com by guest on September 8, 2015


Rinn and Bishop 233

Lynn, R. (1996). Dysgenics: Genetic deterioration in modern popu- Journal of Personality Disorders, 6, 226-240. doi:10.1521/
lations. Westport, CT: Praeger. pedi.1992.6.3.226
Lynn, R., & Van Court, M. (2004). New evidence of dysgenic fer- Perrone, K. M., Civiletto, C. L., Webb, L. K., & Fitch, J. C. (2004).
tility for intelligence in the United States. Intelligence, 32, 193- Perceived barriers to and supports of attainment of career
201. doi:10.1016/j.intell.2003.09.002 and family goals among academically talented individuals.
Mackintosh, N. J. (2011). IQ and human intelligence (2nd ed.). International Journal of Stress Management, 11, 114-131.
New York, NY: Oxford University Press. doi:10.1037/1072-5245.11.2.114
Maitra, K. (2012). Gifted mothers as social capital: A view from a Perrone, K. M., Ksiazak, T. M., Wright, S. L., Vannatter, A.,
disadvantaged perspective. Gifted Education International, 28, Crane, A. L., & Tanney, A. (2010). Multigenerational gift-
185-191. doi:10.1177/0261429411435014 edness: Perceptions of giftedness across three genera-
Mendaglio, S. (2012). Overexcitabilities and giftedness research: tions. Journal for the Education of the Gifted, 33, 606-627.
A call for a paradigm shift. Journal for the Education of the doi:10.1177/016235321003300407
Gifted, 35, 207-219. doi:10.1177/0162353212451704 Perrone, K. M., Perrone, P. A., Ksiazak, T. M., Wright, S. L., &
Mockros, C. A. (1996). The social context of extraordinary indi- Jackson, Z. V. (2007). Self-perception of gifts and talents
viduals. Advanced Development, 7, 19-38. among adults in a longitudinal study of academically tal-
Monosson, E. (Ed.). (2008). Motherhood, the elephant in the ented high-school graduates. Roeper Review, 29, 259-264.
laboratory: Women scientists speak out. Ithaca, NY: Cornell doi:10.1080/02783190709554420
University Press. Perrone, K. M., Tschopp, M. K., Snyder, E. R., Boo, J. N., & Hyatt,
National Association for Gifted Children. (2010). Position state- C. (2010). A longitudinal examination of career expectations
ment: Redefining giftedness for a new century: Shifting the and outcomes of academically talented students 10 and 20 years
paradigm. Washington, DC: Author. Retrieved from http:// post-high school graduation. Journal of Career Development,
www.nagc.org.442elmp01.blackmesh.com/sites/default/files/ 36, 291-309. doi:10.1177/0894845309359347
Position%20Statement/Redefining%20Giftedness%20for%20 Perrone, K. M., Webb, L. K., Wright, S. L., Jackson, Z. V., &
a%20New%20Century.pdf Ksiazak, T. M. (2006). Relationship of spirituality to work and
National Association for Gifted Children. (2013). Position statement: family roles and life satisfaction among gifted adults. Journal
Mandated services for gifted and talented students. Washington, of Mental Health Counseling, 28, 253-268. doi:10.17744/
DC: Author. Retrieved from http://www.nagc.org/sites/default/ mehc.28.3.81a1dlhwelblce0v
files/Position%20Statement/Mandated%20Services%20for%20 Perrone, K. M., Wright, S. L., Ksiazak, T. M., Crane, A. L., &
Gifted%20and%20Talented%20Students.pdf Vannatter, A. (2010). Looking back on lessons learned: Gifted
Nauta, N., & Corten, F. (2002). Gifted adults in work. (K. Jamdagni, adults reflect on their experiences in advanced classes. Roeper
Trans.). Tijdschrift voor Bedrijfs- en Verzekeringsgeneeskunde Review, 32, 127-139. doi:10.1080/02783191003587918
[Journal for Occupational and Insurance Physicians], 10, Perrone-McGovern, K. M., Boo, J. N., & Vannatter, A. (2012).
332-335. Retrieved from http://sengifted.org/archives/articles/ Marital and life satisfaction among gifted adults. Roeper
gifted-adults-in-work Review, 34, 46-52. doi:10.1080/02783193.2012.627552
Nauta, N., & Ronner, S. (2013). Gifted workers: Hitting the target. Perrone-McGovern, K. M., Ksiazak, T. M., Wright, S. L., Vannatter,
Utrecht, Netherlands: Shaker Media. A., Hyatt, C. C., Shepler, D., & Perrone, P. A. (2011). Major
Oden, M. H. (1968). The fulfillment of promise: Forty-year fol- life decisions of gifted adults in relation to overall life satis-
low-up of the Terman gifted group. Genetic Psychology faction. Journal for the Education of the Gifted, 34, 817-838.
Monographs, 77, 3-93. doi:10.1177/0162353211425101
Olszewski-Kubilius, P. (2000). The transition from childhood Persson, R. S. (2009). Intellectually gifted individuals’ career
giftedness to adult creative productiveness: Psychological choices and work satisfaction: A descriptive study. Gifted and
characteristics and social supports. Roeper Review, 23, 65-71. Talented International, 24, 11-24. Retrieved from http://www.
doi:10.1080/02783190009554068 diva-portal.org/smash/get/diva2:281434/FULLTEXT01.pdf
Park, G., Lubinski, D., & Benbow, C. P. (2007). Contrasting Peterson, J. S. (2009). Myth 17: Gifted and talented individuals
intellectual patterns predict creativity in the arts and sci- do not have unique social and emotional needs. Gifted Child
ences: Tracking intellectually precocious youth over 25 years. Quarterly, 53, 280-282. doi:10.1177/0016986209346946
Psychological Science, 18, 948-952. doi:10.1111/j.1467- Peterson, J. S. (2012). The asset-burden paradox of giftedness: A
9280.2007.02007.x 15-year phenomenological, longitudinal case study. Roeper
Park, G., Lubinski, D., & Benbow, C. P. (2008). Ability differ- Review, 34, 244-260. doi:10.1080/02783193.2012.715336
ences among people who have commensurate degrees matter Piechowski, M. M. (1979). Developmental potential. In N.
for scientific creativity. Psychological Science, 19, 957-961. Colangelo & R. T. Zaffrann (Eds.), New voices in counseling
doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02182.x the gifted (pp. 25-58). Dubuque, IA: Kendall/Hunt.
Park, G., Lubinski, D., & Benbow, C. P. (2012). When less is more: Piechowski, M. M., & Colangelo, N. (1984). Developmental
Effects of grade skipping on adult STEM productivity among potential of the gifted. Gifted Child Quarterly, 28, 80-88.
mathematically precocious adolescents. Journal of Educational doi:10.1177/001698628402800207
Psychology, 105, 176-198. doi:10.1037/a0029481 Prober, P. (1999). Gifted women and motherhood: A workshop
Park, L. C., Imboden, J. B., Park, T. J., Hulse, S. H., & Unger, H. model. Advanced Development, 8, 77-86.
T. (1992). Giftedness and psychological abuse in borderline Prober, P. (2008). Counseling gifted adults: A case study. Annals of
personality disorder: Their relevance to genesis and treatment. the American Psychotherapy Association, 11, 10-15.

Downloaded from gcq.sagepub.com by guest on September 8, 2015


234 Gifted Child Quarterly 59(4)

Rinn, A. N., & Plucker, J. A. (2004). We recruit them, but then Stewart, R. E., & Porath, M. (1999). From childhood rags to
what? The educational and psychological experiences of aca- adult riches: A case study. High Ability Studies, 10, 197-211.
demically talented undergraduates. Gifted Child Quarterly, 48, doi:10.1080/1359813990100206
54-67. doi:10.1177/001698620404800106 Streznewski, M. K. (1999). Gifted grownups: The mixed blessings
Robertson, K. F., Smeets, S., Lubinski, D., & Benbow, C. P. (2010). of extraordinary potential. New York, NY: Wiley.
Beyond the threshold hypothesis: Even among the gifted and top Subotnik, R. F. (2009). Developmental transitions in giftedness and
math/science graduate students, cognitive abilities, vocational talent: Adolescence into adulthood. In F. D. Horowitz, R. F.
interests, and lifestyle preferences matter for career choice, per- Subotnik, & D. Matthews (Eds.), The development of gifted-
formance, and persistence. Current Directions in Psychological ness and talent across the lifespan (pp. 155-170). Washington,
Science, 19, 346-351. doi:10.1177/0963721410391442 DC: American Psychological Association.
Rocamora, M. (1992). Counseling issues with recognized and Subotnik, R. F., & Arnold, K. D. (Eds.). (1994). Beyond Terman:
unrecognized creatively gifted adults. Advanced Development Contemporary longitudinal studies of giftedness and talent.
Journal, 4, 75-89. Retrieved from http://talentdevelop.com/ Norwood, NJ: Ablex.
articles/Page147.html Subotnik, R. F., & Arnold, K. D. (1995). Passing through the gates:
Rodgers, J. L., Cleveland, H., van den Oord, E., & Rowe, D. C. Career establishment of talented women scientists. Roeper
(2000). Resolving the debate over birth order, family size, Review, 18, 55-61. doi:10.1080/02783199509553698
and intelligence. American Psychologist, 55, 599-612. Subotnik, R. F., Duschl, R. A., & Selmon, E. H. (1993). Retention and
doi:10.1037/0003-066X.55.6.599 attrition of science talent: A longitudinal study of Westinghouse
Roeper, A. (1991). Gifted adults: Their characteristics and emo- Science Talent Search winners. International Journal of
tions. Advanced Development, 3, 85-98. Science Education, 15, 61-72. doi:10.1080/0950069930150105
Rost, D. H. (2009). Hochbegabte und hochleistende Jugendliche. Subotnik, R. F., Kassan, L., Summers, E., & Wasser, A. (1993).
Befunde aus dem Marburger Hochbegabtenprojekt [Gifted Genius revisited: High IQ children grown up. Norwood, NJ:
and high achieving adolescents: Findings from the Marburg Ablex.
Giftedness Project] (2nd ed.). Münster, Germany: Waxmann. Subotnik, R. F., & Olszewski-Kubilius, P. (1997). Restructuring
Ruf, D. L. (2001). Environmental, familial, and personal factors special programs to reflect the distinctions between children’s
that affect the self-actualization of highly gifted adults: Case and adults’ experiences with giftedness. Peabody Journal of
studies. In N. Colangelo, & S. Assouline (Eds.), Talent devel- Education, 72, 101-116. doi:10.1080/0161956X.1997.9681868
opment IV: Proceedings from the 1998 Henry B. and Jocelyn Subotnik, R. F., Olszewski-Kubilius, P., & Worrell, F. C.
Wallace National Research Symposium on Talent Development (2011). Rethinking giftedness and gifted education: A pro-
(pp. 429-433). Scottsdale, AZ: Great Potential Press. posed direction forward based on psychological science.
Sears, R. R. (1977). Sources of life satisfaction of the Terman gifted Psychological Science in the Public Interest, 12, 3-54.
men. American Psychologist, 32, 119-128. doi:10.1037/0003- doi:10.1177/1529100611418056
066X.32.2.119 Subotnik, R. F., & Rickoff, R. (2010). Should eminence based on
Sękowski, A., & Siekańska, M. (2008). National academic award outstanding innovation be the goal of gifted education and talent
winners over time: Their family situation, education and development? Implications for policy and research. Learning
interpersonal relations. High Ability Studies, 19, 155-171. and Individual Differences, 20, 358-364. doi:10.1016/j.lin-
doi:10.1080/13598130802504270 dif.2009.12.005
Shatz, S. M. (2008). IQ and fertility: A cross-national study. Subotnik, R. F., & Steiner, C. L. (1994). Adult manifestations of
Intelligence, 36, 109-111. doi:10.1016/j.intell.2007.03.002 adolescent talent in science: A longitudinal study of 1983
Siekańska, M., & Sękowski, A. (2006). Job satisfaction and tem- Westinghouse Science Talent Search winners. In R. F. Subotnik
perament structure of gifted people. High Ability Studies, 17, & K. D. Arnold (Eds.), Beyond Terman: Contemporary longi-
75-85. doi:10.1080/13598130600947127 tudinal studies of giftedness and talent (pp. 52-76). Norwood,
Simonton, D. K. (2008). Childhood giftedness and adult- NJ: Ablex.
hood genius: A historiometric analysis of 291 eminent Super, D. E. (1980). A life-span, life-space approach to career
African Americans. Gifted Child Quarterly, 52, 243-255. development. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 16, 282-298.
doi:10.1177/0016986208319294 doi:10.1016/0001-8791(80)90056-1
Simonton, D. K., & Song, A. V. (2009). Eminence, IQ, physical and Terman, L. M. (Ed.). (1925-1926). Genetic studies of genius:
mental health, and achievement domain: Cox’s 282 geniuses Mental and physical traits of a thousand gifted children (1st
revisited. Psychological Science, 20, 429-434. doi:10.1111/ and 2nd eds., Vol. 1). Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.
j.1467-9280.2009.02313.x Terman, L. M. (1942). The vocational successes of intellectually
Spearman, C. (1927). The abilities of man: Their nature and mea- gifted individuals. Occupations: The Vocational Guidance
surement. New York, NY: Macmillan. Journal, 20, 493-498. doi:10.1002/j.2164-5892.1942.
Stålnacke, J., & Smedler, A. (2011). Psychosocial experiences and tb01216.x
adjustment among adult Swedes with superior general mental Terman, L. M. (1954). Scientists and nonscientists in a group of 800
ability. Journal for the Education of the Gifted, 34, 900-918. gifted men. Psychological Monographs: General and Applied,
doi:10.1177/0162353211424988 68, 1-44. doi:10.1037/h0093672
Standing, L. G., Aikins, S., Madigan, B., & Nohl, W. (2015). Terman, L. M., & Oden, M. H. (1947). Genetic Studies of Genius:
Exceptional achievement and early parental loss: The Phaeton The gifted child grows up: Twenty-five years’ follow-up of a
Effect in American writers, presidents, and eminent individu- superior group (Vol. 4). Stanford, CA: Stanford University
als. Journal of Psychohistory, 42, 188-199. Press.

Downloaded from gcq.sagepub.com by guest on September 8, 2015


Rinn and Bishop 235

Therival, W. A. (1999). Why are eccentrics not eminently cre- Watve, S. V. (2008). Intellectually gifted adults: Life accom-
ative? Creativity Research Journal, 12, 47-55. doi:10.1207/ plishments. Bulgarian Journal of Science and Education
s15326934crj1201_6 Policy, 2, 251-266. Retrieved from http://bjsep.org/getfile.
Tolan, S. (1994). Discovering the gifted ex-child. Roeper Review, php?id=22
17, 134-138. doi:10.1080/02783199409553642 Webb, J. T., Goerss, J., Amend, E. R., Webb, N. E., Beljan, P.,
Tomlinson-Keasey, C., & Little, T. D. (1990). Predicting educa- & Olenchak, F. R. (2006). Diagnosis or misdiagnosis.
tional attainment, occupational achievement, intellectual skill, Understanding Our Gifted, 18, 15-17.
and personal adjustment among gifted men and women. Journal Weidenheim, K. M., Escobar, A., & Rapin, I. (2012). Brief report:
of Educational Psychology, 82, 442-455. doi:10.1037/0022- Life history and neuropathology of a gifted man with Asperger
0663.82.3.442 syndrome. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders,
Van den Broeck, W., Hofmans, J., Cooremans, S., & Staels, 42, 460-467. doi:10.1007/s10803-011-1259-0
E. (2014). Factorial validity and measurement invariance Willings, D. (1985). The specific needs of adults who are gifted.
across intelligence levels and gender of the Overexcitabilities Roeper Review, 8, 35-38. doi:10.1080/02783198509552925
Questionnaire-II (OEQ-II). Psychological Assessment, 26, Wilson, M. E., Jr., Sakata, R., & Frumkin, R. M. (1968). Attitudes
55-68. doi:10.1037/a0034475 of some gifted adults, future rehabilitation counselors, and
VanTassel-Baska, J. L. (1989). Characteristics of the developmen- rehabilitation professors toward disabilities. Psychological
tal path of eminent and gifted adults. In J. L. VanTassel-Baska Reports, 22, 1303-1304. doi:10.2466/pr0.1968.22.3c.1303
& P. Olszewski-Kubilius (Eds.), Patterns of influence on gifted Winner, E. (2000). The origins and ends of giftedness. American
learners: The home, the self, and the school (pp. 146-162). Psychologist, 55, 159-169. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.55.1.159
New York, NY: Teachers College Press. Wirthwein, L., Becker, C. V., Loehr, E., & Rost, D. H. (2011).
Vialle, W. (1994). “Termanal” science? The work of Lewis Overexcitabilities in gifted and non-gifted adults: Does sex
Terman revisited. Roeper Review, 17, 32-38. doi:10.1080/ matter? High Ability Studies, 22, 145-153. doi:10.1080/1359
02783199409553614 8139.2011.622944
Wai, J. (2013). Investigating America’s elite: Cognitive ability, educa- Wirthwein, L., & Rost, D. H. (2011a). Focusing on overexcit-
tion, and sex differences. Intelligence, 41, 203-211. doi:10.1016/ abilities: Studies with intellectually gifted and academically
j.intell.2013.03.005 talented adults. Personality and Individual Differences, 51,
Wai, J. (2014a). Investigating the world’s rich and powerful: 337-342. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2011.03.041
Education, cognitive ability, and sex differences. Intelligence, Wirthwein, L., & Rost, D. H. (2011b). Giftedness and subjective
46, 54-72. doi:10.1016/j.intell.2014.05.002 well-being: A study with adults. Learning and Individual
Wai, J. (2014b). Experts are born, then made: Combining pro- Differences, 21, 182-186. doi:10.1016/j.lindif.2011.01.001
spective and retrospective longitudinal data shows that cog- Wulff, C., Bergman, L. R., & Sverke, M. (2009). General mental
nitive ability matters. Intelligence, 45, 74-80. doi:10.1016/j. ability and satisfaction with school and work: A longitudinal
intell.2013.08.009 study from ages 13 to 48. Journal of Applied Developmental
Wai, J., Lubinski, D., & Benbow, C. P. (2005). Creativity and Psychology, 30, 398-408. doi:10.1016/j.appdev.2008.12.015
occupational accomplishments among intellectually preco- Yewchuk, C. R., & Schlosser, G. A. (1995). Characteristics of
cious youths: An age 13 to age 33 longitudinal study. Journal the parents of eminent Canadian women. Roeper Review, 18,
of Educational Psychology, 97, 484-492. doi:10.1037/0022- 78-83. doi:10.1080/02783199509553702
0663.97.3.484
Wai, J., Lubinski, D., Benbow, C. P., & Steiger, J. H. (2010). Author Biographies
Accomplishment in science, technology, engineering, and
Anne N. Rinn is an associate professor of educational psychology
mathematics (STEM) and its relation to STEM educational
at the University of North Texas, where she also serves as assis-
dose: A 25-year longitudinal study. Journal of Educational
tant department chair and coordinator of the Doctoral Program in
Psychology, 102, 860-871. doi:10.1037/a0019454
Gifted and Talented Education. She received her undergraduate
Walberg, H. J., Tsai, S., Weinstein, T., Gabriel, C. L., Rasher, S. P.,
degree in psychology from the University of Houston and her doc-
Rosecrans, T., . . . Vukosavich, P. (1981). Childhood traits and
torate in educational psychology from Indiana University. Her
environmental conditions of highly eminent adults. Gifted Child
expertise is in the area of the social and emotional development of
Quarterly, 25, 103-107. doi:10.1177/001698628102500303
gifted individuals. She currently serves on the Executive
Walberg, H. J., Zhang, G., Cummings, C., Fillipelli, L. A., Freeman,
Committee of the AERA Research on Giftedness, Creativity, and
K. A., Haller, E. P., . . . Zeiser, S. (1996). Childhood traits and
Talent SIG and as Program Chair of the NAGC Research and
experiences of eminent women. Creativity Research Journal,
Evaluation Network.
9, 97-102. doi:10.1207/s15326934crj0901_9
Walker, B. A., Reis, S. M., & Leonard, J. S. (1992). A develop- James Bishop is a doctoral candidate in educational psychology
mental investigation of the lives of gifted women. Gifted Child with a concentration in gifted and talented education at the
Quarterly, 36, 201-206. doi:10.1177/001698629203600406 University of North Texas. Prior to beginning his doctoral pro-
Warne, R. T. (2011). A reliability generalization of the gram, he received his bachelor’s degree in creative writing from
Overexcitability Questionnaire-Two. Journal of Advanced the University of North Texas and master’s degrees in professional
Academics, 22, 671-692. doi:10.1177/1932202X11424881 development and professional counseling from Amberton
Watten, R. G., Syversen, J. L., & Myhrer, T. (1995). Quality of University. He is a licensed professional counselor intern at
life, intelligence and mood. Social Indicators Research, 36, Fundamental Foundations Counseling Center in Frisco, Texas,
287-299. doi:10.1007/BF01078818 where he specializes in working with gifted children and adults.

Downloaded from gcq.sagepub.com by guest on September 8, 2015

View publication stats

You might also like