026999312E20102E508610

You might also like

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 12

See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.

net/publication/51554447

Prospective memory, emotional valence and ageing

Article in Cognition and Emotion · August 2011


DOI: 10.1080/02699931.2010.508610 · Source: PubMed

CITATIONS READS
69 443

7 authors, including:

Peter Rendell Louise H Phillips


Australian Catholic University University of Aberdeen
160 PUBLICATIONS 5,738 CITATIONS 160 PUBLICATIONS 13,088 CITATIONS

SEE PROFILE SEE PROFILE

Julie Henry Mareike Altgassen


The University of Queensland Technische Universität Dresden
258 PUBLICATIONS 22,339 CITATIONS 94 PUBLICATIONS 2,811 CITATIONS

SEE PROFILE SEE PROFILE

All content following this page was uploaded by Matthias Kliegel on 23 May 2014.

The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file.


This article was downloaded by: [Université de Genève]
On: 12 August 2013, At: 06:01
Publisher: Routledge
Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954 Registered office:
Mortimer House, 37-41 Mortimer Street, London W1T 3JH, UK

Cognition & Emotion


Publication details, including instructions for authors and subscription
information:
http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/pcem20

Prospective memory, emotional valence and


ageing
a b c
Peter G. Rendell , Louise H. Phillips , Julie D. Henry , Tristan Brumby-
a a d
Rendell , Xochitl de la Piedad Garcia , Mareike Altgassen & Matthias
d
Kliegel
a
Australia Catholic University, Melbourne, Australia
b
Aberdeen University, Aberdeen, UK
c
University of New South Wales, Sydney, Australia
d
Technische Universität Dresden, Dresden, Germany
Published online: 05 Oct 2010.

To cite this article: Peter G. Rendell , Louise H. Phillips , Julie D. Henry , Tristan Brumby-Rendell , Xochitl de
la Piedad Garcia , Mareike Altgassen & Matthias Kliegel (2011) Prospective memory, emotional valence and
ageing, Cognition & Emotion, 25:5, 916-925, DOI: 10.1080/02699931.2010.508610

To link to this article: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/02699931.2010.508610

PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE

Taylor & Francis makes every effort to ensure the accuracy of all the information (the “Content”)
contained in the publications on our platform. However, Taylor & Francis, our agents, and our
licensors make no representations or warranties whatsoever as to the accuracy, completeness, or
suitability for any purpose of the Content. Any opinions and views expressed in this publication
are the opinions and views of the authors, and are not the views of or endorsed by Taylor &
Francis. The accuracy of the Content should not be relied upon and should be independently
verified with primary sources of information. Taylor and Francis shall not be liable for any
losses, actions, claims, proceedings, demands, costs, expenses, damages, and other liabilities
whatsoever or howsoever caused arising directly or indirectly in connection with, in relation to or
arising out of the use of the Content.

This article may be used for research, teaching, and private study purposes. Any substantial
or systematic reproduction, redistribution, reselling, loan, sub-licensing, systematic supply, or
distribution in any form to anyone is expressly forbidden. Terms & Conditions of access and use
can be found at http://www.tandfonline.com/page/terms-and-conditions
COGNITION AND EMOTION
2011, 25 (5), 916925

BRIEF REPORT

Prospective memory, emotional valence and ageing

Peter G. Rendell
Australia Catholic University, Melbourne, Australia

Louise H. Phillips
Aberdeen University, Aberdeen, UK

Julie D. Henry
Downloaded by [Université de Genève] at 06:01 12 August 2013

University of New South Wales, Sydney, Australia

Tristan Brumby-Rendell and Xochitl de la Piedad Garcia


Australia Catholic University, Melbourne, Australia

Mareike Altgassen and Matthias Kliegel


Technische Universita
¨ t Dresden, Dresden, Germany

Emotional factors have been found to be an important influence on memory. The current study
investigated the influence of emotional salience and age on a laboratory measure of prospective
memory (PM); Virtual Week. Thirty young and 30 old adults completed Virtual Week, in which the
emotional salience of the tasks at encoding was manipulated to be positive, negative or neutral in
content. For event-based, but not time-based tasks, positivity enhancement in both age groups was
seen, with a greater number of positive PM tasks being performed relative to neutral tasks. There was
no negativity enhancement effect. Older adults showed generally poorer levels of PM, but they also
demonstrated greater beneficial effects of positive valence compared to young. These effects of
emotion on PM accuracy do not appear to reflect the retrospective component of the task as a different
pattern of emotion effects was seen on the recall of PM content. Results indicate that older adults’
difficulties in prospective remembering can be reduced where the tasks to be remembered are positive.

Keywords: Prospective memory; Emotional saliency; Emotional valence; Positivity enhancement;


Ageing; Event- and time-based.

Prospective memory (PM) is memory for future a specific action while being involved in an
intentions, and involves remembering to perform ongoing activity. Real-world PM tasks important

Correspondence should be addressed to: Peter G. Rendell, School of Psychology, Australian Catholic University, Locked bag
4115, Fitzroy, Victoria, Australia 3065. E-mail: peter.rendell@acu.edu.au
Support for this study was provided by a Discovery Project grant from the Australian Research Council.
We acknowledge the help of Trevor Daniels in programming Virtual Week.

# 2010 Psychology Press, an imprint of the Taylor & Francis Group, an Informa business 916
http://www.psypress.com/cogemotion DOI:10.1080/02699931.2010.508610
VALENCE AND PROSPECTIVE MEMORY

for independent living include remembering to manipulation that increases the strategic demands
take medication and turn off appliances. PM tasks of a PM task increases age-related difficulties. One
consist of distinct components: forming an inten- factor influencing age-related deficits in PM is
tion, initiating the intention at the correct time, therefore the saliency of target events. Prior PM
and successfully remembering the content of the studies that have investigated specific aspects of
intention (Kliegel, McDaniel, & Einstein, 2000). target salience have supported this model, showing
Remembering the PM content is regarded as that increased salience of the target event does
the retrospective component and both forming attenuate age deficits (e.g., Cohen, Dixon, Lind-
and carrying out intentions comprise the prospec- say, & Masson, 2003).
tive component. In relation to ageing and PM, Emotional factors have been found to be an
contradictory findings have been identified. While important influence on PM. Depression is asso-
laboratory studies typically show age-related de- ciated with poor PM function (Altgassen, Kliegel,
cline, older participants generally outperform their & Martin, 2009). Further, a sad mood induction
younger counterparts in naturalistic studies. This led to reduced PM (Kliegel et al., 2005). In
Downloaded by [Université de Genève] at 06:01 12 August 2013

‘‘paradoxical’’ pattern was initially identified by addition to person-related mood effects on neutral
Rendell and Thompson (1999), and was con- PM tasks, there are other possibilities for emotion
firmed in a meta-analysis (Henry, MacLeod, cognition interactions. Specifically, there may be
Phillips, & Crawford, 2004). However it remains task-related effects of valence of the PM task itself.
unclear what specific factors of the task settings Emotionally salient information is more likely to be
are critical for determining the direction of age recalled and attended to than emotionally neutral
effects (Phillips, Henry, & Martin, 2008). information (Murphy & Isaacowitz, 2008). Con-
One possibility is that the content of the sequently, increasing the emotional salience of the
tests themselves may contribute to the paradox. PM task may be expected to improve task perfor-
Laboratory tasks typically involve embedding a mance for both young and old, as has been shown in
PM task into another ongoing task. Such tasks other cognitive domains.
consist of highly abstract material with little However, whether age interacts with the valence
emotional salience, and this might influence age of information to influence cognitive performance
differences (Rendell & Thomson, 1999). Natur- may depend on the tasks used. Some studies have
alistic PM tasks involve a task being carried out found that increasing the emotional salience of task
within the daily life of participants (Phillips et al., material reduces age deficits in cognitive domains
2008) and often contain an interpersonal compo- such as problem solving (Blanchard-Fields, Jahnke,
nent, such as telephoning experimenters at spe- & Camp, 1995), and recognition memory
cific times (Devolder, Brigham, & Pressley, (Charles, Mather, & Carstensen, 2003), consistent
1990). For older adults, the socioemotional with a generally increased emotional enhancement
salience of such activities may boost performance effect in late adulthood. Other studies suggest that
on naturalistic PM tasks; in contrast, the abstract age interacts with the valence of the emotional
nature of most laboratory-based PM tasks is material, so that older adults increase attention to
unlikely to provide such motivation. When the positive and/or decrease attention to negative
PM task is irrelevant or has little emotional information relative to other types of information
connection for the participant, task motivation (Carstensen & Mikels, 2005). However, others
may be reduced (Phillips et al., 2008). have found no evidence for valence-based effects
Emotional salience as a determinant of age with ageing (Leclerc & Kensinger, 2008). One
effects is consistent with McDaniel and Einstein’s study experimentally manipulated the emotionality
(2000) multiprocess framework. In this model, PM of a PM task and found that emotional valence was
performance involves both automatic and stra- detrimental to performance. Clark-Foos, Brewer,
tegic processes, the relative prominence of which Marsh, Meeks, and Cook (2009) found that
varies according to specific task demands. Any negatively valenced cues were detected less often

COGNITION AND EMOTION, 2011, 25 (5) 917


RENDELL ET AL.

than positive cues (Experiments 1a c). However, findings of age-related increases in positivity
they did not explore age differences in these enhancement effects we also tested whether
emotion effects. there is a differential enhancement of PM for
When considering age differences in PM an positive material in older adults. Finally, it was
important distinction is typically made between predicted that any emotional enhancement effects
event- and time-based cues. Event-based tasks are observed should be greater for event- relative to
carried out in response to a specific event (e.g., pass time-based PM tasks.
on good news when you next see a friend) while
time-based tasks are carried out at a specific time
(e.g., phoning your friend at 4 p.m. to pass on good METHOD
news). As time-based tasks require effortful time
Participants
monitoring they have been argued to be more
susceptible to age effects (McDaniel & Einstein, Thirty young (M21.9, SD3.28) and 30 old
2007). With respect to possible emotional enhance- adults (M75.0, SD5.72) participated. Young
Downloaded by [Université de Genève] at 06:01 12 August 2013

ment effects, however, those may be greater for adults were first-year psychology students and
event-based tasks. In terms of the multiprocess received course credit for participation. Old adults
model (McDaniel & Einstein, 2000), event-based were recruited from various community sites and
tasks have the potential to involve relatively strong retirement villages, and received an AUD$5.00
associations between the task content (e.g., good instant lottery ticket for participation. All had
news to pass on) and cue (meeting the friend). The normal mental status, as indexed by scores greater
emotional content of a task arguably can be linked than 83 (M94.8, SD3.97) on the Australian
to the cueing event, thus increasing the emotional version of the Revised Addenbrooke’s Cognitive
salience of the cue as well as the task content, and Examination (ACE-R; Mathuranath, Nestor,
this might improve PM performance. In contrast, it Berrios, Rakowicz, & Hodges, 2000) and scores
seems less likely that the target time of a time-based between 27 and 30 on the Mini Mental State
task (e.g., 4 p.m.) will become emotionally bound Examination (M29.6, SD0.87). The two
to the content of the PM task (pass on good news). groups differed in years of education, t(58)
Also, the content of the PM task is less likely to be 7.41, pB.001. However, older adults performed
connected to the critical aspect of time monitoring better than their younger counterparts on an index
in time-based tasks than the critical task of noticing of crystallised intelligence (Mill-Hill Vocabulary
the inherent environmental cue in event-based Test); t(58)2.70, p.009, and more poorly on
tasks. Therefore, there may be less opportunity a measure of fluid intelligence (Raven’s Progres-
for emotional task content to impact performance sive Matrices); t(58)8.19, pB.001, suggesting
on time-based relative to event-based PM tasks. that the sample is broadly representative of ageing
studies.
Aims
Materials and procedure
The primary aim of the present study was to directly
After giving informed consent and providing
test whether there are age differences in emotion
demographic information, participants completed
enhancement effects on PM. To conduct this
the dementia screening test (ACE-R; Mathura-
assessment, an adapted version of Virtual Week
nath et al., 2000) as well as the Mill Hill Vocabulary
was used in which emotional valence was manipu-
Test and the Raven’s Progressive Matrices. Then,
lated through the images and content of tasks
Virtual Week was administered.
presented at encoding. It was predicted that both
young and old would exhibit improved PM per-
formance when emotionally salient (as opposed to PM: Virtual Week. The present study used a
neutral) images were presented. Given previous computerised version of Virtual Week (Rendell &

918 COGNITION AND EMOTION, 2011, 25 (5)


VALENCE AND PROSPECTIVE MEMORY

Henry, 2009). Virtual Week is a laboratory content that corresponded to a specific IAPS
measure of PM designed to represent PM in daily image. For instance, the negative task ‘‘Go to the
life. It uses a computerised board game format, in dentist at 10 a.m.’’ was shown with a negatively
which participants move around the board with rated picture of a dentist; the positive task ‘‘Tell
the roll of a die. Each circuit of the board Kate that Margaret has had a baby girl’’ was
represents a waking day and as participants move accompanied by a positively rated picture of a
around the board, they pick up 10 event cards baby, and the neutral task ‘‘Pick up brother’s gym
giving choices about daily activities, and they membership’’ was accompanied by a neutral-rated
remember to carry out life-like activities (PM picture of dumbbells. Content-relevant images also
tasks). Each day of Virtual Week includes ten accompanied the presentation of the other PM
PM tasks (four regular, four irregular, and two tasks (regular and time-check) but for these tasks
time-check). The four regular PM tasks were the normatively rated neutral images were presented.
same each day and simulate the kinds of regular As in the original version of Virtual Week,
tasks that occur as one undertakes normal duties, participants were not required to physically under-
Downloaded by [Université de Genève] at 06:01 12 August 2013

two of which are event-based (triggered by some take the PM tasks. Instead, when the cue or time
specific information shown on an event card: to perform a task arrived, participants had to press
breakfast and dinner event cards) and two of a ‘‘Perform task’’ button. At this point, a list of
which are time-based (triggered by passing a possible tasks was presented and the participant
particular time on the board: 11 a.m. and had to choose the appropriate task. The list
9 p.m.). A clock was presented on screen, which included both tasks that were part of the game
indicated virtual time. The four irregular PM tasks and unrelated distracter tasks. A practice trial day
are different each day and simulate the kinds of was given, consistent with traditional administra-
occasional tasks that occur in everyday life; again, tions of Virtual Week, before participants were
two of these tasks are event-based and two are asked to complete the six trial days. Thus, in total,
time-based. Finally, the two time-check tasks are participants were required to complete 24 irregu-
the same each day but require the participant to lar tasks (8 positive, 8 negative and 8 neutral).
‘‘break set’’ from the board-game activity, and Each day included two tasks of one valence and
monitor real time on the stop-clock that was one of each of the other two valence categories
displayed prominently, and indicate when a spe- (presented in counterbalanced order).
cified period of time has passed: 2 min 30 s and
4 min 15 s from start of each virtual day. Virtual Week Free Recall Test. Upon completion
Unlike previous editions of Virtual Week, here of Virtual Week participants were given pen and
the valence of the irregular tasks was manipulated. paper and asked to free recall as many of the
Each irregular task was presented with a task- irregular PM tasks as possible. Participants were
relevant photo from the International Affective instructed that they did not have to remember
Picture System (IAPS; Lang, Bradley, & Cuthbert, when they had to carry out the task (e.g., the
2005). Each IAPS picture has unique standardised event or virtual time), only what they needed to
values, based on 1 to 9 rating scales of valence do. This measure was important to differentiate
(unpleasant to pleasant) and arousal (calm to excited). between failures of prospective and retrospective
Three sets of eight pictures were selected to components of the PM tasks.
constitute positive, negative and neutral image
sets with valence ratings M(SD)7.9(0.41),
2.9(0.48) and 5.2(0.51), respectively, and moderate RESULTS
arousal ratings M(SD)5.2(0.77), 4.9(0.35) and
PM tasks with no emotion manipulation
3.3(0.83), respectively. The irregular tasks were
modified from earlier versions of Virtual Week so In order to investigate age differences in PM
that they would have positive, negative or neutral performance without valence manipulations, a

COGNITION AND EMOTION, 2011, 25 (5) 919


RENDELL ET AL.

mixed 23 analysis of variance (ANOVA) was h2p .19, as well as an interaction between PM
conducted with the between-groups variable of Task and Age, F(2, 116)5.41, MSE0.04,
Age (young, old) and the within-groups variable p.006, h2p 09. Tests of simple effects indi-
of PM Task (regular event-based, regular time- cated that the younger participants were more
based, time-check), with performance accuracy on accurate than older participants on regular
these tasks (proportion of PM tasks completed) as time-based, F(1, 58)11.46, p.001, h2p .17,
the dependent variable (see Figure 1A). There was and on time-check, F(1, 58)11.83, p.001,
a main effect of PM Task, F(2, 116)85.24, h2p .17, but young and old did not differ on
MSE0.04, pB.001, h2p .60, and a main effect regular event-based performance (FB0.01).
of Age F(1, 58)13.85, MSE0.06, p.006,
Emotional valence, age and PM
Separate mixed 23 ANOVAs were conducted
on the event- and time-based irregular tasks with
Downloaded by [Université de Genève] at 06:01 12 August 2013

the between-groups variable of Age (young, old)


and the within-groups variable of Valence (posi-
tive, negative, neutral), with Performance Accu-
racy as the dependent variable (see Figure 1B and
C). On the event-based tasks, there was a main
effect of Valence, F(2, 116)28.71, MSE0.03,
pB.001, h2p .33, and a main effect of Age
F(1, 58)55.89, MSE0.07, pB.001, h2p 
.49, as well as an interaction between Valence
and Age, F(2, 116)4.51, MSE0.03, p.013,
h2p .07. Tests of simple effects indicated that the
younger participants were more accurate than
older participants on event-based tasks across
all valences, for positive, negative and neutral,
F(1, 58)19.33, 27.82, 39.40, psB.001, h2p 
.25, .32 and .40, respectively. Valence was a
simple main effect within both young and old,
F(2, 57)11.15 and 39.21, pB.001, h2p .28
and .58, respectively. For both young and old,
Bonferroni post hoc tests revealed participants
performed significantly better on positive than on
both the negative and neutral tasks.
On the time-based-tasks, Valence was not a
main effect, and did not interact with Age both
FsB1, h2p .01, but there was a main effect of
Age, F(1, 58)54.11, MSE0.10, pB.001,
h2p .48, with younger adults outperforming their
older counterparts (young: M0.58, SD0.28;
Figure 1. Mean proportion of correct responses on prospective old: M0.23, SD0.23).
memory (PM) tasks (error bars depict standard error of the mean).
To clarify the impact of emotional valence
(A) PM tasks with no emotion manipulation. (B) Irregular PM
tasks*event-based. (C) Irregular PM tasks*time-based. The age positivity and negativity enhancement/impairment
differences were significant in each of the three types of tasks indexes were calculated (see Figure 2, left side):
reported in A, B and C, except the regular event task in A. the difference between proportion correct on the

920 COGNITION AND EMOTION, 2011, 25 (5)


VALENCE AND PROSPECTIVE MEMORY

Post Virtual Week: Recall of the content of


irregular PM tasks
As with PM accuracy, the recall of PM con-
tent was analysed with separate mixed Age
(young, old)Valence (positive, negative, neu-
tral) ANOVAs on the event- and time-based
tasks. On the event-based tasks, there was a main
effect of Age, F(1, 58)67.60, MSE0.07,
pB.001, h2p .54, but Valence was not a main
effect, F(2, 116)2.33, MSE0.04, p.102,
h2p .04, and there was an interaction between
Age and Valence, F(2, 116)3.43, MSE0.04,
p.036, h2p .06. Tests of simple effects indi-
Downloaded by [Université de Genève] at 06:01 12 August 2013

cated that the younger participants recalled more


tasks than older participants across all valences,
for positive (young: M0.60, SD0.23; old:
M0.38, SD0.21), negative (young: M
0.73, SD0.21; old: M0.35, SD0.18) and
neutral (young: M0.76, SD0.27; old: M
0.37 SD0.23), F(1, 58)15.77, 58.33, 35.33,
psB.001, h2p .21, .50 and .38, respectively.
Valence was a simple main effect for young,
Figure 2. Mean emotional enhancement/impairment index on the
event- and time-based irregular prospective memory (PM) tasks F(2, 57)5.60, p.006, h2p .16, but not for
for PM accuracy and PM task content recall. (A) Event-based. old (FB1). Bonferroni post hoc tests revealed
(B) Time-based. Positivity indexproportion correct on positive that young participants performed significantly
minus neutral PM tasks. Negativity indexproportion correct on worse on positive than on both negative and
negative minus neutral PM tasks. *Enhancement/impairment
index significantly different to zero; horizontal bar significant
neutral.
age difference; error bars depict standard error of the mean. On the time-based tasks, Valence was not a
main effect, F(2, 116)2.05, MSE0.05,
p.133, h2p .03, and did not interact with
positive minus the neutral tasks, and the negative Age F(2, 116)2.49, MSE0.05, p.087,
minus the neutral tasks. This is consistent with h2p .04, but there was a main effect of Age,
Murphy and Isaacowitz (2008), who advocated F(1, 58)80.46, MSE0.6, pB.001, h2p .58,
comparing positively or negatively valenced mate- with young adults outperforming their older
rial with neutral material. On the event-based counterparts (young: M0.55, SD0.25; old:
tasks, the positivity enhancement was greater than M0.22, SD0.25).
zero for both young and old participants: t(29) As with PM accuracy, positivity and negativity
2.97, 6.24, ps5.006, respectively, but the nega- enhancement/impairment indexes were calculated
tivity index did not differ from zero for either (see Figure 2, right side): the difference between
young or old, t(29)1.16, 0.84, ps].257. task content accurately recalled on the positive
Further analysis confirmed the positivity enhance- minus the neutral tasks, and the negative minus the
ment on the event-based tasks was larger for the neutral tasks, respectively. For the event-based
old compared to the young participants, t(58) tasks, there was positivity impairment: for young
3.12, p.003. On the time-based tasks, positivity adults: t(29)2.73, p.011, representing a
and negativity indexes did not differ from zero for decrease in the performance on recalling the
either young or old, ts(29)51.22, ps].231. content of PM tasks in positive compared to

COGNITION AND EMOTION, 2011, 25 (5) 921


RENDELL ET AL.

neutral conditions; in contrast the positivity index event-based tasks (Rendell & Henry, 2009). As
did not differ from zero for the old adults, t(29) predicted, only for event-based PM performance
0.20, p.845. Further analysis confirmed the was an effect of valence observed. Both younger
positivity impairment was different for the younger and older adults showed positivity enhancement,
compared to the older participants, t(58)2.32, successfully performing more positive than neutral
p.024. The negativity index on event-based tasks, and this positivity enhancement was con-
tasks did not differ from zero for either young or siderably larger for older than for younger adults.
old, t(29)50.47, ps].693. On the time-based There was no negativity enhancement or impair-
tasks, the positivity enhancement was greater than ment on PM performance. Thus, in the current
zero for old adults: t(29)2.73, p.011, but did study negative emotional valence incurred no PM
not differ from zero for young adults, t(29)0.35, costs or benefits, while positive emotional valence
p.730. Further analysis confirmed the positivity confers PM benefits that are larger for older
enhancement was different for the younger com- adults. These findings contrast with the report
pared to the older participants, t(58)2.33, p
Downloaded by [Université de Genève] at 06:01 12 August 2013

that emotional valence may be detrimental to


.024. The negativity indexes on time-based tasks performance of young adults (Clark-Foos et al.,
did not differ from zero for either young or old, 2009, Experiement 2). However, the current
t(29)0.84, 1.94, ps].062. study findings concur with the Clark-Foos et al.
Experiment 1 finding of better PM for positive
than negative cues in a younger adult sample. As
DISCUSSION
they did not include a neutral condition it was not
possible to tell whether their results reflected a
These data corroborate previous laboratory find-
positivity enhancement or negativity impairment.
ings, in showing that old adults exhibit poorer
Our evidence suggests that positivity enhance-
PM than their younger counterparts as indexed by
overall performance on Virtual Week (Rendell & ment is more likely.
Craik, 2000). Specifically, when not manipulating Given the evidence for age differences in the
emotional valence (in regular tasks), age differ- effects of emotion on recall and recognition
ences were substantial on time-based and elimi- memory, and in particular the reports of positivity
nated on event-based tasks. In contrast to early enhancement effects for older adults (Murphy &
versions of Virtual Week, the new computer Isaacowitz, 2008) it is important to consider the
version is sensitive to the event- versus time- role of the retrospective component in the Virtual
based distinction in PM tasks.1 Our main predic- Week PM tasks. The first thing to note is that the
tion was that manipulating emotional valence in very different pattern of AgeValence interac-
the set of irregular PM tasks would moderate tions on prospective and retrospective components
these effects of age on PM performance in the of the task means that it is unlikely that the
same overall paradigm. Consistent with previous pattern of AgeValence effects seen on the PM
Virtual Week studies, in irregular tasks, age measure can be solely attributed to AgeValence
effects were observed in both event- and time- effects on remembering PM task content. Speci-
based tasks, possibly due to more attentional fically, older adults’ large advantage for perform-
resources required for irregular, non-routine, ing positive event-based PM tasks was not
event-based tasks compared to regular, repetitive, accompanied by an age-related advantage for

1
In the original study (Rendell & Craik, 2000), age differences were substantial on time-check but minimal on both regular
event- and time-based. In both studies a stop clock was used for time-check but the time-based tasks used times marked on squares
in the earlier study and a clock in the current study. Hence, in earlier studies the distinction between event- and time-based tasks
may have been blurred with times on board functioning as event cues.

922 COGNITION AND EMOTION, 2011, 25 (5)


VALENCE AND PROSPECTIVE MEMORY

recalling positive over neutral information for the Given the pattern on event-based tasks that
event-based tasks.2 younger people showed impaired recall of positive
Looking overall at the positivity enhancements compared to neutral information, but enhanced
seen for older adults the following pattern was enactment of positive PM tasks compared to
found. Older adults remembered to carry out neutral, this raises interesting possibilities about
more positive than neutral event-based tasks, but the effects of emotion on the different stages of
did not subsequently remember more information encoding and retrieval of prospective remember-
about positive than neutral tasks. In contrast, they ing. Future studies could try to tease apart more
did not show positivity enhancement in remem- carefully the role of emotion in influencing each
bering to carry out time-based tasks, but did stage of PM. This issue is also highlighted when
subsequently remember more information about comparing the design of the current emotion
positive than neutral time-based task content. manipulation with that reported by Clark-Foos
How can this pattern be explained? We suggest et al. (2009). Consistent with most prior literature
that a likely explanation lies in the consideration on emotional enhancement effects in memory, in
Downloaded by [Université de Genève] at 06:01 12 August 2013

of the critical difference between carrying out the present study the emotional content of the
event- and time-based tasks involving notic- task was highlighted by displaying emotionally
ing the event cues versus monitoring time. As valenced images when the intention was formed,
hypothesised on the basis of the multiprocess at encoding of the PM task. This may explain the
framework, older adults may have profited from contrast of this study with the findings in Clark-
the elevated salience of event-based PM cues that Foos et al.’s (2009, Experiment 2) study, where
possibly resulted from binding together informa- emotion was not highlighted at encoding, and
tion about the positive task contents and the only revealed at the time of retrieval. Further
accompanying cue events in the event-based research is therefore needed to clarify the relative
condition. This would enhance PM cue detection importance of emphasising the emotional salience
and thus PM accuracy in the event-based tasks. of PM tasks at encoding versus retrieval phases.
For the time-based tasks, older adults may have The current study adds to our understanding of
actively encoded and rehearsed the positive task age difference in emotion enhancement effects.
information, enhancing subsequent recall memory Age-related positivity enhancements have not
for the task. However, as this information was consistently been identified in other cognitive
difficult to bind with the time-based task’s ‘‘cue’’ domains, including retrospective memory and
(i.e., an arbitrary time point), this did not enhance attention (Murphy & Isaacowitz, 2008). Indeed,
time-based PM performance. The positivity im- the nature of age effects in emotion processing
pairment for remembering content of event-based has been found to vary as a function of mea-
tasks among younger people was not expected. surement type (Murphy & Isaacowitz, 2008),
However, it may link to at least one previous methodological design (Isaacowitz, Wadlinger,
finding that younger adults have poorer retro- Goren, & Wilson, 2006), encoding instructions
spective memory for positive than neutral IAPS (Kensinger, Gutchess, & Schacter, 2007), the
pictures reported in the meta-analysis by Murphy arousal level of the emotional word (Kensinger,
and Issaacowitz (2008). Perhaps under some 2008), as well as available cognitive resources
conditions younger people do not expend much (Petrican, Moscovitch, & Schimmack, 2008).
effort in encoding positive information*this These data appear to add to growing evidence
would fit with evidence that younger people spend that an age-related positivity enhancement may
less time fixating on positive than neutral IAPS only manifest under particular circumstances, and,
pictures (Wadlinger & Isaacowitz, 2006, Study 1). specifically, in the context of PM may be

2
The significant main effect for age on PM accuracy remained after covarying out recall of the content in an ANCOVA.

COGNITION AND EMOTION, 2011, 25 (5) 923


RENDELL ET AL.

restricted to event-based assessments of this and whether performance is measured in terms of


construct. prospective remembering (success in carrying out
The impact of emotional valence on event- (and the task) or retrospective remembering (recalling
not time-based) PM may reflect a number of the content of the task). Further, these data
factors. First, there is greater scope for emotional indicate that positivity enhancement of PM*at
valence to be manipulated in relation to events least as indexed by event-based tasks*is greater
rather than times. Second, for event-based tasks for old relative to young. Given the importance of
there is the possibility for positively valenced PM PM in everyday life, this suggests that future
task content to facilitate the initiation of PM task, interventions to improve PM in cognitively
whereas the retrieval of time-based tasks is depen- impaired populations could focus on emotional
dent on self-initiated monitoring of time, which is manipulations. Further research is needed using
presumably not linked to the content of the PM. controlled manipulations that test the degree to
The findings of larger positivity enhancement which emotional salience interacts with other
for old relative to young adults on the event-based properties of the PM task, such as personal
Downloaded by [Université de Genève] at 06:01 12 August 2013

PM tasks also potentially inform the ‘‘agePM relevance and interpersonal demands.
paradox’’. In the introduction it was suggested
that the content of the tests themselves may Manuscript received 8 October 2009
contribute to the paradoxical findings, with most Revised manuscript received 10 June 2010
Manuscript accepted 5 July 2010
laboratory tasks having minimal emotional sal-
First published online 14 September 2010
ience compared to naturalistic tasks that often
involve interpersonal component. It was suggested
older adults’ performance in naturalistic setting
could reflect the increased task salience of PM REFERENCES
tasks in this setting (McDaniel & Einstein, 2007).
This explanation is supported by this study Altgassen, M., Kliegel, M., & Martin, M. (2009).
showing that boosting emotionality of PM tasks Event-based prospective memory in depression: The
can enhance older adults’ on PM. However, a impact of cue focality. Cognition and Emotion, 23,
limitation of the current paradigm is that emo- 10411055.
tional salience was manipulated using tasks nor- Blanchard-Fields, F., Jahnke, H. C., & Camp, C.
(1995). Age differences in problem-solving style:
matively judged as being positive or negative in
The role of emotional salience. Psychology and Aging,
content. Thus, personal emotional salience was
10, 173180.
not assessed and this remains an important Carstensen, L. L., & Mikels, J. A. (2005). At the
consideration for future research. Additional intersection of emotion and cognition: Aging and
limitations were that young and old adults had the positivity effect. Current Directions in Psycho-
different forms of modest compensation (2% logical Science, 14, 117121.
course credit bonus versus $5.00 instant lottery Charles, S. T., Mather, M., & Carstensen, L. L.
ticket), which might have contributed to age (2003). Aging and emotional memory: The forget-
differences in task motivation. table nature of negative images for older adults.
In conclusion, the data reported in this study Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 132,
suggest that, as has been shown in other cognitive 310324.
Clark-Foos, A., Brewer, G. A., Marsh, R. L., Meeks,
domains, emotional salience is relevant to under-
J. T., & Cook, G. I. (2009). The valence of event-
standing PM function, and this is true for both
based prospective memory cues or the context in
younger and older cohorts. However, the magni- which they occur affects their detection. American
tude and direction of this effect appears to vary as Journal of Psychology, 122, 8997.
a function of important influences: both age and a Cohen, A.-L., Dixon, R. A., Lindsay, D. S., & Masson,
number of PM task parameters*specifically, M. E. J. (2003). The effect of perceptual dist-
whether the PM cue is time- or event-based, inctiveness on the prospective and retrospective

924 COGNITION AND EMOTION, 2011, 25 (5)


VALENCE AND PROSPECTIVE MEMORY

components of prospective memory in young disease and frontotemporal dementia. American


and old adults. Canadian Journal of Experimental Academy of Neurology, 55, 16131620.
Psychology, 57, 274289. McDaniel, M. A., & Einstein, G. O. (2000). Strategic
Devolder, P. A., Brigham, M. C., & Pressley, M. and automatic processes in prospective mem-
(1990). Memory performance awareness in younger ory retrieval: A multiprocess framework. Applied
and older adults. Psychology and Aging, 5, 291303. Cognitive Psychology. Special Issue: New Perspectives
Henry, J. D., MacLeod, M. S., Phillips, L. H., & in Prospective Memory, 14, S127S144.
Crawford, J. R. (2004). A meta-analytic review of McDaniel, M. A., & Einstein, G. O. (2007). Prospec-
prospective memory and aging. Psychology and Aging, tive memory: An overview and synthesis of an emerging
19, 2739. field. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Isaacowitz, D. M., Wadlinger, H. A., Goren, D., & Murphy, N. A., & Isaacowitz, D. M. (2008). Prefer-
Wilson, H. R. (2006). Is there an age-related ences for emotional information in older and
positivity effect in visual attention? A comparison younger adults: A meta-analysis of memory and
of two methodologies. Emotion, 6, 511516. attention tasks. Psychology and Aging, 23, 263286.
Kensinger, E. A. (2008). Age differences in memory for Petrican, R., Moscovitch, M., & Schimmack, U.
Downloaded by [Université de Genève] at 06:01 12 August 2013

arousing and nonarousing emotional words. Journal (2008). Cognitive resources, valence, and memory
of Gerontology: Psychological Sciences, 63, P13P18. retrieval of emotional events in older adults. Psycho-
Kensinger, E. A., Gutchess, A. H., & Schacter, D. L. logy and Aging, 23, 585594.
(2007). Effects of aging and encoding instructions Phillips, L. H., Henry, J. D., & Martin, M. (2008).
on emotion-induced memory trade-offs. Psychology Adult aging and prospective memory: The impor-
and Aging, 22, 781795. tance of ecological validity. In M. Kliegel, M. A.
Kliegel, M., Jäger, T., Phillips, L. H., Federspiel, E., McDaniel, & G. O. Einstein (Eds.), Prospective
Imfeld, A., Keller, M., et al. (2005). Effects of sad memory: Cognitive, neuroscience, developmental, and
mood on time-based prospective memory. Cognition applied perspectives (pp. 161186). Mahwah, NJ:
and Emotion, 19, 11991213. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.
Kliegel, M., McDaniel, M. A., & Einstein, G. O. Rendell, P. G., & Craik, F. I. M. (2000). Virtual and
(2000). Plan formation, retention, and execution in Actual Week: Age-related differences in prospective
prospective memory: A new approach and age-related memory. Applied Cognitive Psychology. Special Issue:
effects. Memory and Cognition, 28, 10411049. New Perspectives in Prospective Memory, 14, S43
Lang, P. J., Bradley, M. M., & Cuthbert, B. N. (2005). S62.
International Affective Picture System: Instruction Rendell, P. G., & Henry, J. D. (2009). A review of
manual and affective ratings (Vol. Tech. Rep. No. Virtual Week for prospective memory assessment:
A-5). Gainesville: University of Florida, Center for Clinical implications. Brain Impairment, 10, 1422.
Research in Psychophysiology. Rendell, P. G., & Thomson, D. M. (1999). Aging and
Leclerc, C. M., & Kensinger, E. A. (2008). Effects prospective memory: Differences between naturalis-
of age on detection of emotional information. tic and laboratory tasks. Journal of Gerontology:
Psychology and Aging, 23, 209215. Psychological Sciences, 54B, P256P269.
Mathuranath, P. S., Nestor, P. J., Berrios, G. E., Wadlinger, H. A., & Isaacowitz, D. M. (2006). Positive
Rakowicz, W., & Hodges, J. R. (2000). A brief mood broadens visual attention to positive stimuli.
cognitive test battery to differentiate Alzheimer’s Motivation and Emotion, 30, 89101.

COGNITION AND EMOTION, 2011, 25 (5) 925

View publication stats

You might also like