Blood Blood 2019 Bullying in Adolescents Who Stutter Communicative Competence and Self Esteem

You might also like

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 11

Bullying in Adolescents Who Stutter:

Communicative Competence and


Self-Esteem
Gordon W. Blood
Ingrid M. Blood
The Pennsylvania State University, University Park

A dolescence is a time of self-exploration,


experimentation, and identity development,
with many problems being episodic in nature
and not persisting into adulthood (Lerner & Galambos,
1998; Steinberg, 1999). Although the research for the past
inherently problematic and dysfunctional, some difficulties
have negative lifelong impact. Bullying, peer rejection, and
victimization during adolescence are problems that lead to
serious, negative long-term outcomes (Garbarino & deLara,
2002; Garrett, 2003; Geffner, Loring, & Young, 2001;
two decades confirms that this period of development is not Olweus, 1993; Rigby, 1997; Thompson, Arora, & Sharp,
2002). Research reports from interviews of bullies about
ABSTRACT: This study examined the perceived communi-
their “targets” suggest that they mark students who are
cative competence, self-esteem, and vulnerability to perceived as cautious, anxious, quiet, passive, withdrawn,
bullying of 53 adolescents who stutter and 53 adoles- unassertive, insecure, and unhappy (Byrne, 1994; Olweus,
cents who do not stutter. Adolescents who stutter were at 1993). Students with mild and moderate disabilities are also
a significantly higher risk of experiencing bullying at high risk for bullying (Dawkins, 1996; Sweeting & West;
behavior (43%) than were adolescents who do not stutter 2001; Wilde & Haslam, 1996).
(11%). The majority of adolescents who stutter (57%) The systematic study of stuttering during adolescence has
rated themselves as having poor communicative compe- been neglected. Few studies have examined peer relations,
tence. In contrast, only 13% of the adolescents who do communicative competence, personal development, social
not stutter rated themselves as having poor communica-
adjustments, and internalization of personal value systems
tive competence. Seventy-two percent of adolescents who
stutter scored within 1 SD from the mean on a standard-
within the context of stuttering (Blood, Blood, Tellis, &
ized measure of self-esteem, which is indicative of Gabel, 2001, 2003; Blood et al., 1998; Davis, Howell, &
positive self-esteem. Students with low self-esteem and Cooke, 2002). However, the developmental challenges and
poor confidence in their communicative competence transformations that define adolescence may be significantly
were more likely to be victimized by bullies. The influenced by stuttering. Negative stereotypes about
importance of perceived communication skills, especially students who stutter are common. Students who stutter are
in establishing and maintaining positive social interac- described as more withdrawn, insecure, introverted, tense,
tions, and their relationship to potential victimization by anxious, fearful, shy, nonassertive, and fearful of communi-
bullies is discussed. Social and communication skills cation than students who do not stutter (Lass, Ruscello,
training programs may be an important part of treatment
Pannbacker, Schmitt, & Everly-Myers, 1989; Lass,
programs for some adolescents who stutter to manage
potential bullying behavior.
Ruscello, Schmitt et al., 1992; Ruscello, Lass, Schmitt, &
Pannbacker, 1994; Silverman, 1982; Turnbaugh, Guitar, &
KEY WORDS: bullying, stuttering, adolescents, self-esteem Hoffman, 1979; Woods, 1978; Woods & Williams, 1971;
Yairi & Williams, 1970; Yeakle & Cooper, 1986). These

CONTEMPORARY ISSUES IN COMMUNICATION SCIENCE AND DISORDERS • Volume 31 • 69–79 • Spring 2004 © NSSLHA 69
1092-5171/04/3101-0069

Downloaded from: https://pubs.asha.org 212.96.37.65 on 01/19/2024, Terms of Use: https://pubs.asha.org/pubs/rights_and_permissions


negative stereotypes are remarkably similar to how bullies In contrast, peer rejection may lead to loneliness,
define their “targets,” and may place adolescents who depression, poorer academic performance, lower self-
stutter at high risk for being bullied and victimized in esteem, aggression, withdrawal, irritability, an increased
school settings. The underlying framework for this study likelihood of antisocial behavior, and high risk for victim-
includes (a) the relationship between social and communi- ization and bullying (Parker & Asher, 1987, 1993; Pope &
cation skills in adolescents, (b) the negative effects of Bierman, 1999). Peer rejection has also been identified
bullying behavior, and (c) the possible relationship between along with poor social skills and perceived low social
bullying and stuttering. status as part of the bullying cycle. Peer rejection and
victimization during adolescence can have negative lifelong
effects. Successful interventions have been reported that
were designed to improve both social and communicative
SOCIAL AND COMMUNICATION SKILLS interactions and social competencies (Smith & Ananiadou,
2003; Smith & Brain, 2000; Steinberg, 1999; Welsh,
Social relationships and belonging to peer groups play Domitrovich, Bierman, & Lang, 2003). However, children
critical roles in adolescent development (Parker & Asher, who fail to develop or display important communication
1987, 1993; Parkhurst & Asher, 1992). Peer relationships skills may be less likely to build or maintain social and
influence adolescents’ social identity, self-esteem, and interpersonal peer relations. These children may also be at
social status. A number of studies report on the influence higher risk of being bullied. Perceived poor communicative
of peer acceptance and peer rejection (Brown & Lohr, competence has been related to communication apprehen-
1987; Callaghan & Joseph, 1995; Espelage, 2002; sion and fear, isolation, loneliness, introversion, and
Espelage & Holt, 2001; Hodges, Boivin, Vitaro, & shyness (Daly, McCroskey, Ayres, Hopf, & Ayres, 1997;
Bukowski, 1999; Lerner & Galambos, 1998; Sharp, 1995, McCroskey & McCroskey, 1988; Richmond & McCroskey,
1996). Peer acceptance is linked to popularity in groups, 1998; Richmond, McCroskey, & McCroskey, 1989).
closer friendships, positive self-concepts, academic Studies have also suggested that children with communi-
success, and expanded social competencies. During this cation disabilities are more likely to be rejected by peers
time, how adolescents perceive themselves (e.g., the “in” and ignored in social interactions (Botting & Conti-
group, part of the clique, leader in a group) often be- Ramsden, 2000; Brinton & Fujiki, 1999; Fujiki, Brinton, &
comes a self-fulfilling prophecy. Social status, academic Todd, 1996; Hadley & Rice, 1991; Knox & Conti-Ramsden,
and/or athletic achievement, physical appearance, socio- 2003; Rice, Sell, & Hadley, 1991). It appears that children
economic class, and social norms all play important roles. with even mild disabilities are at risk for peer rejection and
“Fitting in” is often one of the keys for peer acceptance, lower social status in the classroom. Adolescents who are
with rigid group expectations for appearance, prosocial perceived as “different,” or who perceive themselves as
and antisocial behaviors, and communication styles and “different” due to the absence of or lack of skills in using
skills. Buhrmester (1996) suggested that peer relationships peer-sanctioned social and communication skills, may be at
mature during adolescence through increased social and high risk for victimization or bullying.
communicative activities, including initiating interactions,
self-disclosure, and intimacy in conversations and activi-
ties. Effective social and communicative skills become the
foundation for social interactions. Richmond and EFFECTS OF BULLYING BEHAVIOR
McCroskey (1998) reported that “effective communicators
exhibit assertiveness, responsiveness, and versatility. These Bullying is a subtype of aggression and an international
skills allow speakers to make requests, actively disagree, problem for school-age children (Olweus, 1993; Thompson
express their feelings, initiate, maintain, and disengage in et al., 2002). American schools are often perceived as a
conversations” (p. 85). They also allow individuals to be dangerous place by many students. Fried and Fried (1996)
more flexible in multiple settings and with different reported that more than 160,000 children missed school
conversation partners. Effective communicators show a daily due to fear of attacks and victimization by bullies. In
willingness to communicate and to engage in social a typical week, students receive an average of 230 verbal
interactions. Richmond and McCroskey reported that put-downs, or approximately 30 per day (Garrett, 2003). In
perceived communicative competence is positively related an extensive study of 15,686 students in the United States,
to high self-esteem, confident attitudes toward communi- nearly 30% of all children in schools are involved in
cation, and adept social skills. Perceptions of communica- bullying behavior (Nansel et al., 2001). Thirteen percent of
tive competence are related to individuals’ willingness and the children in their study self-identified as bullies, 11%
desire to communicate and are a critical predictor of self-identified as victims, and 6% self-identified as both
positive peer relations and acceptance. As friendships bullies and victims. The researchers defined bullying as
evolve and peer interactions intensify, supportive peer “when a teenager’s behavior is purposely meant to harm or
relationships, healthy self-concepts, positive self-esteem, disturb another child, when it occurs over time, and when
and effective communication also play protective roles there is an imbalance in power between the kids involved”
against victimization (Garbarino & deLara, 2002; Garrett, (p. 2992). Types of bullying include disparaging verbal
2003; Geffner et al., 2001; Olweus, 1993; Thompson et al., comments, shunning, spreading rumors, unwanted physical
2002). contact, beatings, stealing, and threats.

70 CONTEMPORARY ISSUES IN COMMUNICATION SCIENCE AND DISORDERS • Volume 31 • 69–79 • Spring 2004

Downloaded from: https://pubs.asha.org 212.96.37.65 on 01/19/2024, Terms of Use: https://pubs.asha.org/pubs/rights_and_permissions


Although males and females are bullied equally (Banks,
1997; O’Moore, Kirkham, & Smith, 1999; Rigby, 1997, BULLYING AND STUTTERING
1999; Whitney & Smith, 1993), males are more likely to
use direct bullying (e.g., threats and physical hitting) Although some research suggests that the majority of
whereas females tend to use indirect bullying (e.g., verbal adolescents who stutter do not suffer from stigmatization and
insults and isolation). Victims of bullying were perceived report positive self-esteem, a subgroup of adolescents who
as different from peers in dress, speech, and personal stutter (15%–41%) report high communication fears, poor
hygiene. Dawkins (1996) reported that children with communication confidence, and low self-esteem (Blood et
visible disabilities were twice as likely to be bullied as al., 2001, 2003; Yovetich, Leschied, & Flicht, 2000).
children with no visible disabilities. Children with Recently, Davis et al. (2002) administered a sociometric
physical, intellectual, and sensory disabilities such as scale to assess the peer relationships of 16 children who
cerebral palsy, epilepsy, spina bifida, hearing/visual stutter and their 403 classmates. They found that the children
impairments, emotional problems, severe intellectual who stutter were rejected significantly more often than their
disabilities, hyperactivity, or cleft palate have an increased peers, were categorized as less “popular,” and were less
risk of victimization (Bond, Carlin, Thomas, Rubin, & likely to be named “leaders.” Only 1 of the 16 students was
Patton, 2001; Johnson et al., 2002; Sheard, Clegg, categorized as a “leader.” The students who stutter were
Standen, & Cromby, 2001; Sweeting & West, 2001). three times as likely to be categorized as “victims of
Similarly, children with learning disabilities, poorer bullying” than their fluent peers and, as a group, demon-
academic achievement, and special needs are also reported strated low social status among peers.
to be at higher risk of being bullied (Mishna, 2003; Unfortunately, the same negative labels that are applied to
Nabuzoka, 2003; Nabuzoka & Smith, 1993, 1999; Whitney children who stutter are also reported as descriptors of
& Smith, 1993). Recently, Knox and Conti-Ramsden victims of bullying. Children and adolescents who are targets
(2003) reported on the severity and risk of being bullied for bullies are described as unassertive, insecure, more
in 100 children with specific language impairment (SLI). sensitive, cautious, anxious, quiet, passive, withdrawn, and
They found that 36% of the children with SLI self- less likely to interact and speak with other children (Hodges
reported being at high risk for victimization of bullying & Perry, 1999; Olweus, 1990, 1993, 1997, 2003). It is
behavior as compared with 12% of the non-SLI group. possible that some adolescents who stutter may be at high
Their findings support previous studies that children with risk for bullying due to their stuttering and/or a combination
special education needs are at higher risk of being bullied. of their stuttering and low social status among peers (Davis
Hodges and Perry (1999) reported that children who are et al., 2002). The potential negative effect of stuttering on
bullied are more likely to be disliked by their peers and to specific social interactions and verbal exchanges, coupled
have fewer friends than those who are not bullied. As with the negative stereotypes held by peers and low social
stated previously, peer relationships play an important role status, may place students who stutter at risk for bullying.
in bullying behavior. Poor social skills, low social status, A number of exceptional books include sections on
and poor social relationships may be both a cause and a dealing with bullying in children and adolescents who
result in the bullying cycle. Research suggests that victims stutter (Bloom & Cooperman, 1999; Conture, 2001; Guitar,
of bullying show similar characteristics: They tend to be 1998; Gregory, 2003; Shapiro, 1999; Silverman, 2004; Van
more cautious, anxious, quiet, withdrawn, passive, unasser- Riper, 1982). Authors have also contributed articles in
tive, insecure, unhappy, and hypersensitive than children dealing with bullying and teasing behavior in individuals
who are not victims of bullying (Byrne, 1994; Garrett, who stutter (Murphy & Quesal, 2002; Ramig & Bennett,
2003; Geffner et al., 2001; Olweus, 1993). Unfortunately, 1995). However, few studies have systematically reported
these children tend to respond to their victimization with on victimization and bullying of adolescents who stutter
greater avoidance of social situations, absenteeism, emo- (Hugh-Jones & Smith, 1999; Langevin, Bortnick, Hammer,
tional problems, and insecurities. These response behaviors & Wiebe, 1998; Mooney & Smith, 1995). Langevin et al.
are the same ones that bullies report as the target behaviors (1998) developed a questionnaire for teasing/bullying for
of their victims (Garbarino & de Laura, 2002; Garrett, children who stutter. Their pilot instrument was adminis-
2003; Thompson et al., 2002). tered to 28 Canadian children between the ages of 7 to 15
Victims of bullying during childhood and adolescence years who stutter. Results revealed that 16 of the 28
show long-term psychosocial and physical health problems children were teased/bullied about their stuttering, and 81%
(Geffner et al., 2001; Olweus, 1993; Rigby, 1997, 1999, of the children reported that they were upset about the
2000; Smith et al., 2002). Students who are repeatedly teasing/bullying. The authors suggested that a larger sample
bullied report sleeplessness, lower self-esteem, poorer self- was needed to validate the scale. Another potential limita-
concepts, increased rates of absenteeism, truancy, depres- tion of their study was the wording in their questionnaire.
sion, loss of confidence, greater isolation, and poorer The questionnaire began with, “Students sometimes bully
academic performance than students who are not bullied kids at school by hurting or upsetting them in some way”
(Callaghan & Joseph, 1995; Garrett, 2003; Olweus, 1993; followed with the first question “Have you been teased/
Sharp, 1995). One purpose of this preliminary study was to bullied about your stuttering?” (p. 22). Unfortunately, the
determine if stuttering contributes to the reported “stereo- orientation provided by the definition and the very general
type of difference” that is characteristic of bullied adoles- first question may have limited value for assessing the
cents (Garrett, 2003). multiple levels of bullying and may have biased the results.

Blood • Blood: Bullying and Stuttering 71

Downloaded from: https://pubs.asha.org 212.96.37.65 on 01/19/2024, Terms of Use: https://pubs.asha.org/pubs/rights_and_permissions


Hugh-Jones and Smith (1999) designed a 19-item risk of being bullied in a school setting?
questionnaire and surveyed adults from the British Stam- • Are there significant differences between adolescents
mering Association. Results from the 276 participants (31% who stutter and adolescents who do not stutter on a
response rate from a mailed survey) revealed that partici- measure of communicative competence critical to
pants recalled difficulties in peer relationships and bullying developing and fostering positive peer relations?
during their school experience that were directly related to
• Are there significant differences between adolescents
their stuttering. Eighty-three percent of the participants
who stutter and adolescents who do not stutter on a
reported being bullied at school, with 18% reporting being
standardized measure of self-esteem?
“bullied daily” and 41% recalling that they were “bullied
weekly.” The authors recognized that the recall technique • Are there significant positive relationships among the
(in some cases recalling events that occurred more than 25 risk of being bullied, perceived communicative
years ago), lack of a control group, low response rate, and confidence, and self-esteem in adolescents who stutter
the fact that all participants were members of a self-help and adolescents who do not stutter?
group may have confounded their results. They suggested
that children who stutter could be at higher risk for poor
peer relationships and bullying due to their stuttering and
that further research should be conducted in this area. METHOD

Participants

CURRENT STUDY Participants were 53 adolescents who stutter between the


ages of 13 and 18 years (mean age 15:2 [years;months])
The disruptive nature of stuttering during social interac- and 53 adolescents who do not stutter (mean age 14:8) who
tions, negative stereotypes held by peers, impact on peer were currently enrolled in school. All adolescents met the
relationships, overrepresentation of students with disabilities following criteria: no repetition of a grade level, no
who are bullied, and retrospective research suggesting that placement in a special classroom, absence of a history of
victimization/bullying occurs during the school years to chronic physical or psychological disabilities (diabetes,
individuals who stutter, all seem to suggest a relationship asthma, or neurological, learning, reading, or mental
between bullying and stuttering. However, few studies have disabilities), and no history of chronic truancy. Adolescents
explored the relationship between bullying and stuttering in who do not stutter were matched with adolescents who
adolescents who are currently experiencing the behavior. It stutter on grade level, ethnicity, gender, and approximate
is possible to examine the risk and frequency of bullying age. Assent and consent forms were completed before the
currently being experienced by students using methodology beginning of the study.
reported in the literature (Arora, 1994; Thompson et al., The demographic characteristics of the adolescents
2002). It is also possible that students who stutter may not included 89% (94) male, 11% (12) female, 85% (90)
perceive themselves as effective communicators and thereby Caucasian, 11% (12) African American, and 4% (4)
may reduce the actual behaviors necessary for fostering Hispanic American. Adolescents who stuttered were
positive social/peer relationships, thereby making them currently enrolled in treatment with a speech-language
more vulnerable to bullying. In response to negative pathologist. Stuttering severity was rated by the Stuttering
stereotypical reactions of peers, adolescents who stutter Severity Instrument for Children and Adults (Riley, 1994).
may have developed negative perceptions of their own The ratings revealed that 11% (6) of the participants’
communicative competence, avoidance strategies, and less stuttering was rated in the mild category, 40% (21) in the
outgoing interaction styles. In light of the fact that research moderate category, 26% (14) in the severe category, and
on bullying clearly indicates that one of the devastating 23% (12) in the very severe category.
results of being bullied is lowered self-esteem in victims, it
also seems important to clarify this relationship in adoles-
cents who stutter. Previous research suggests that adoles-
Procedures
cents who stutter may have developed protective techniques The procedures used in the present investigation parallel
to maintain their positive self-esteem (Blood et al., 2003; those reported in earlier studies (Blood et al., 1998; Blood
Yovetich et al., 2000). However, bullying may compromise et al., 2001, 2003). Briefly, demographic information,
the ability of adolescents who stutter to apply these stuttering history, onset, duration, spontaneous speech
protective techniques. The relationship between perceived samples, and socioeconomic status (SES) were obtained
communicative competence and self-esteem may be before the administration of the scales. SES was determined
important in understanding the complex interactions among using Hollingshead’s Four Factor Index (1975), which uses
bullying, stuttering, and psychosocial adjustment in some parent’s educational level and employment position. All
fluency clients. Therefore, four research questions were participants were tested individually using the standardized
posited to examine how a number of factors are associated instructions that required them to complete paper and
with the experience of bullying in adolescents who stutter: pencil tests. Although the test battery consisted of a
• Are there significant differences between adolescents number of scales and questionnaires, for the purposes of
who stutter and adolescents who do not stutter on the this study, the results of the Life in School (LIS) checklist

72 CONTEMPORARY ISSUES IN COMMUNICATION SCIENCE AND DISORDERS • Volume 31 • 69–79 • Spring 2004

Downloaded from: https://pubs.asha.org 212.96.37.65 on 01/19/2024, Terms of Use: https://pubs.asha.org/pubs/rights_and_permissions


(Arora, 1994), the Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale (RSES, (schools with less than 100 students enrolled to more than
Rosenberg, 1965), and the Self-Perceived Communication 1,000 students enrolled). The LIS has been used with
Competence (SPCC) scale (McCroskey & McCroskey, children in regular classrooms, with special needs, with
1988) will be reported. learning disabilities, and with SLI (Arora, 1994, 1996;
Arora & Thompson, 1987; Johnson et al., 2002; Knox &
Instruments Conti-Ramsden, 2003; Smith, 1999).
RSES. Self-esteem was evaluated using the RSES
LIS. The LIS checklist (Arora, 1994) is a published scale (Rosenberg, 1965). As mentioned in previous research
that is used as an indirect, precise measure of bullying. (Blood et al., 2003), this scale is one of the most widely
According to Arora, because the LIS never asks the used scales for measuring self-esteem in the world
question “Are you bullied?” many of the problems with (Hagborg, 1996; Rosenberg, 1986). Scores on the RSES are
other measures of bullying behavior are eliminated. She the average rating of the five positively worded (e.g., I feel
reports that using the word “bullying” may elicit an I have a number of good qualities) and 5 negatively
emotional response from participants and consequently less worded (e.g., I feel I do not have much to be proud of)
accurate data due to shame, embarrassment, and/or concern items. Higher scores indicate poorer self-esteem. Extensive
with identification as a victim of bullies. The LIS provides research has demonstrated the scale’s reliability and
more precise information about what is “actually happen- validity. This scale has a reliability coefficient of .92.
ing” to the student within a specific time interval, the past Criterion validity of the scale has been demonstrated as the
week. This eliminates the recall issue inherent in retrospec- scale correlates significantly with other measures of self-
tive studies. The data obtained from students is based on esteem (Rosenberg, 1979). Researchers have demonstrated
specific observable actions in the past week. significant positive relationships between self-esteem and
The LIS consists of 39 statements that a student might other measures of self-concept and self-regard for school-
experience during the past week in school. Each statement is aged children and adolescents (Hagborg, 1996).
preceded by “During this week, another child in school…,” SPCC. The SPCC scale (McCroskey & McCroskey, 1988)
and followed with one of the 39 statements. The student is is a 12-item self-evaluation measure of an individual’s
instructed to check one of the three columns labeled “no,” perception of his or her ability to communicate effectively.
“once,” or “more than once.” Half of the statements are Participants are required to rate their competence on a scale
pleasant or neutral, such as “During this week, another child from 0 to 100 in four settings (public, meeting, group, and
in school…told me a joke; smiled at me; said something dyad) and among three communication receivers (strangers,
nice to me; talked about TV with me; asked me a stupid acquaintance, and friend). The overall SPCC score ranges
question.” The remaining statements are classified as from 0 (completely incompetent) to 100 (completely
unpleasant occurrences that a student might experience, such competent). According to the authors, higher SPCC total
as “During this week, another child in school…said they scores (>87) indicate higher self-perceived communicative
would tell on me; told me a lie; shouted at me; tried to get competence; lower scores (<59) suggest low self-perceived
me in trouble, was unkind about something I did.” Six of communicative competence. Subscores also range from 0 to
the remaining negative items were labeled as bullying by 100 for the four settings (public, meeting, group, and dyad)
70% of secondary school students and teachers in samples and among three communication receivers (strangers,
from two different countries (Arora, 1994, 1996; Arora & acquaintance, and friend). Specified subscores ranges are
Thompson, 1987; Smith, 1999). All of the items that were also included. The SPCC has good reported reliability (0.92)
selected by students and teachers included direct bullying and good predictive and construct validity on a sample of
and were related to physical aggression. The six items 2,444 individuals (Daly et al., 1997; McCroskey, 1977, 1982,
include “During this week, another child in school… 1) tried 1997; McCroskey & McCroskey, 1988; Richmond &
to kick me; 2) threatened to hurt me; 3) demanded money McCroskey, 1998; Richmond et al., 1989).
from me; 4) tried to hurt me; 5) tried to break something of Researchers have found negative correlations between
mine; and 6) tried to hit me.” Interestingly, indirect forms of communication apprehension, neuroticism, and introversion
bullying (name calling, frightening someone, laughing at and the SPCC and positive correlations between self-esteem
someone) were categorized by less than 30% of the sample and sociability in adults and adolescents and the SPCC
as bullying and therefore are not included as part of the (Chesebro et al., 1992; Richmond et al., 1989; Rosenfeld,
Bullying Index. Grant, & McCroskey, 1995; Zakahi & Duran, 1984, 1985).
The checklist can provide a Bullying Index, which Arora
states provides “a comprehensive picture of the student’s Data Analyses
life in school and a means of identifying individuals who
are likely to be victims of bullying” (p. 156). Instructions In addition to descriptive data analyses, separate t tests and
for computing the Bullying Index are provided, as are the analyses of variance (ANOVAs) were computed between
criteria for children identified as most likely to be at risk the group mean total scores of the LIS (question #1), RSES
of being bullied. Studies show that more than 66% of the (question #2), and SPCC (question #3) to determine if
children who select the “more than once” category later significant differences existed between the two groups of
admit to actually being bullied (Thompson et al., 2002). participants. The subscores of the SPCC were also ana-
Data have been collected on children in schools varying in lyzed. In order to determine if significant relationships
geographic location (rural, suburban, and urban) and size existed among the measures, Pearson product moment

Blood • Blood: Bullying and Stuttering 73

Downloaded from: https://pubs.asha.org 212.96.37.65 on 01/19/2024, Terms of Use: https://pubs.asha.org/pubs/rights_and_permissions


correlations were computed for adolescents who stutter and Table 2. Means and standard deviations for adolescents who
adolescents who do not stutter (question #4). stutter and adolescents who do not stutter on the Self-
perceived Communication Competence scale total scores and
subscores.

RESULTS Adolescents Adolescents who


who stutter do not stutter
Performance on Bullying Measures
Scale M SD M SD
As shown in Table 1, 43% (23) of the adolescents who
stutter and 11% (6) of the adolescents who do not stutter
Total score* 65.7 14.3 79.9 12.8
checked at least one of the six bullying items in the “more Public* 55.4 8.1 70.8 5.9
than once” column, providing a measure for risk of being Meeting 68.1 8.5 71.3 7.9
bullied on the LIS during the past week. The severity of the Group discussion* 55.1 9.1 81.4 7.3
risk of being bullied was calculated by counting the number Interpersonal
conversations* 54.7 6.6 74.1 8.2
of items that individuals checked in the “more than once”
Stranger* 55.5 5.9 71.3 7.7
column. The severity was rated from 2 to 6. Table 1 also Acquaintance 82.1 6.1 80.1 8.4
presents the number and percentage of participants who were Friend 83.5 9.3 84.9 7.5
classified in each severity rating. It can be seen that there
were no students who do not stutter who checked more than *Indicates significant differences at the p < .001 level of confi-
three items. In contrast, 13 (24%) students who stuttered dence.
were not only at risk of being bullied, but with varying
degrees of severity of victimization. Only 3 (6%) students level of communication than were adolescents who stutter
who do not stutter reported varying degrees of victimization. (group M of 77.4; SD = 13.5). Results of the t test
The mean Bullying Index is also presented. Adolescents revealed that the group means were significantly different,
who stutter had a group mean Bullying Index of 9.3 (SD = t(103) = 8.9, p < .001. Significant differences (p < .001)
3.1); adolescents who do not stutter had a group mean were also found between the two groups on the public
Bullying Index of 6.6 (SD = 2.7). The higher the number, speaking, group discussion, interpersonal conversations,
the greater the risk of being bullied. Results of the t test and stranger subscores of the SPCC. The group discussion
revealed that the group means were significantly different, subscore data revealed poor communication competence
t(103) = 6.14, p < .001. Adolescents who stutter were at for 24 (45%) participants who stutter and 7 (13%)
higher risk of being bullied than adolescents who do not participants who do not stutter. The interpersonal conver-
stutter. sations subscore data revealed poor self-perceived commu-
nicative competence for 28 (53%) participants who stutter
Performance on the and 8 (15%) participants who do not stutter. The stranger
subscore data revealed poor self-perceived communication
Communicative Competence Scale
competence for 27 (51%) participants who stutter and 11
Table 2 shows that adolescents who do not stutter (group M (21%) participants who do not stutter. Similarly, the
of 66.2; SD = 10.4) were more confident in their perceived public speaking subscore data revealed poor communica-
tive competence for 28 (53%) participants who stutter and
Table 1. Number and percentage of participants who stutter 8 (21%) participants who do not stutter. The data revealed
and who do not stutter on the Life in School checklist. that participants who stutter had higher scores (poorer
performance) on four of the seven subscales of the SPCC.
Significant differences between groups at the p < .001
Participants Participants level of confidence were found for group discussions,
who stutter who do not stutter interpersonal conversations, stranger, and public speaking
subscores; no significant differences were found for
Risk between group means for the meeting, acquaintance, and
checked one or more friend subscores of the SPCC.
of six items 23 (43%) 6 (11%)

Severity Performance on the Self-Esteem Scale


checked one item 10 (19%) 3 (5%)
checked two items 4 (7%) 2 (4%) The results of the RSES revealed a range of scores from
checked three items 2 (4%) 1 (2%) 10 to 32, with a group mean score for adolescents who
checked four items 2 (4%) 0
checked five items 2 (4%) 0 stutter of 17.1 (SD = 4.5) and a group mean score for
checked six items 3 (5%) 0 adolescents who do not stutter of 15.9 (SD = 4.3). Results
of a t test revealed no significant differences between the
Bullying Index group means (p = 0.21). Adolescents who stutter reported
Group M 9.3 6.6
similar levels of self-esteem as adolescents who do not
SD 3.1 2.7
stutter.

74 CONTEMPORARY ISSUES IN COMMUNICATION SCIENCE AND DISORDERS • Volume 31 • 69–79 • Spring 2004

Downloaded from: https://pubs.asha.org 212.96.37.65 on 01/19/2024, Terms of Use: https://pubs.asha.org/pubs/rights_and_permissions


Figure 1 displays the group mean responses for all Correlations Among Measures
participants. The higher scores are indicative of lower
self-esteem. The group mean and standard deviation for the Correlations between the Bullying Index (# of items
“at risk for bullying” group of 23 adolescents who stutter checked from 0 to 6) and the RSES total score and SPCC
was 20.7 (SD = 5.8) and for the 6 adolescents who do not total score were calculated. A significant, positive correla-
stutter was 19.1 (SD = 6.4). The group mean and standard tion was found between risk of bullying and self-esteem (r
deviation for the “not at risk for bullying” group of 30 = +0.69; p < .001). As the risk of bullying increased
adolescents who stutter was 15.1 (SD = 3.9) and for the 47 (higher scores), the likelihood of poorer self-esteem also
adolescents who do not stutter was 15.3 (SD = 3.5). increased (higher scores). A significant, negative correlation
Results of a one-way ANOVA revealed significant differ- was found between the risk of bullying and communicative
ences among the four groups (adolescents who stutter at competence (r = –0.51; p < .001). As the risk of bullying
risk for bullying; adolescents who do not stutter at risk for increased (higher scores), all participants displayed poorer
bullying; adolescents who stutter and are not at risk for communicative competence (lower scores). A low, negative,
bullying; adolescents who do not stutter and are not at risk significant correlation was also found between self-esteem
for bullying), F( 3,102) = 25.4, p < .001. Tukey Kramer and communicative competence (r = –0.36; p < .001). Low
HSD post hoc test revealed that adolescents who stutter who self-esteem (higher scores) was associated with poor
were at risk for bullying had significantly lower self-esteem perceived communicative competence (low scores).
(higher scores) than adolescents who were not at risk for
bullying. Similarly, adolescents who do not stutter who were
at risk for bullying had significantly lower self-esteem
DISCUSSION
(higher scores) than adolescents who were not at risk for
bullying. Participants who were at risk for being bullied had In this study, 43% of the adolescents who stutter and 11%
lower self-esteem (higher scores), regardless of fluency. of the adolescents who do not stutter reported experiencing
It is possible that the number of years in current treat- bullying within the past week. The rates reported for
ment programs (Guitar, 1998; Shapiro, 1999; Silverman, adolescents who do not stutter are similar to those reported
2004; Van Riper, 1982) that may have focused on self- in other studies, suggesting that 10%–15% of all children
esteem, general well-being, and self-concept skills could in schools are victims of bullying (Garbarino & deLara,
have a confounding effect on the data. Therefore, we 2002; Garrett, 2003; Nansel et al., 2001; Thompson et al.,
determined if any relationship existed between the number 2002). Similarly, the significantly higher percentage
of years in treatment and self-esteem scores. No significant reported by participants who stutter parallels studies with
correlations were found between number of years in children with other mild and moderate disabilities who are
treatment and self-esteem scores (r = +0.09; p = 0.41). reported to be victims of bullying (Bond et al., 2001;

Figure 1. Group mean scores on the self-esteem scale for adolescents who are at risk and who are
not at risk for being bullied.
Mean Self-Esteem Scores

Adolescents Who Stutter Adolescents Who Do Not Stutter


At Risk for Bullying Not At Risk for Bullying Group Mean

Blood • Blood: Bullying and Stuttering 75

Downloaded from: https://pubs.asha.org 212.96.37.65 on 01/19/2024, Terms of Use: https://pubs.asha.org/pubs/rights_and_permissions


Johnson et al., 2002; Knox & Conti-Ramsden 2003; Mishna, speaking, group discussions, interpersonal conversations,
2003; Nabuzoka, 2003; Nabuzoka & Smith, 1993, 1999; and when talking with strangers. If bullies tend to select
Whitney & Smith, 1993). According to Garrett (2003), targets who show poor communication skills, avoidance
teasing and bullying top the list of school-age children’s strategies, and unassertive behaviors, then treatment
problems. For adolescents who stutter, social integration may strategies and programs that work on attitudes and feelings
cause problems due to the daily variability in their communi- appear to be warranted as a technique to build general
cation interactions. This research suggests that adolescents communication and social skills. For some adolescents who
who stutter are at greater risk of being bullied and presents stutter, changing motor speech behaviors may not result in
another issue for consideration when planning the optimal accompanying attitudinal and cognitive changes. Programs
learning environment in schools for adolescents who stutter. that reinforce assertive skills, positive communication
These data provide empirical support for programs that models, acceptance of stuttering, and ways of dealing with
address teasing and bullying issues in students who stutter stuttering may actually assist in dealing with potential co-
(Blood, 1995; Bloodstein, 1995; Bloom & Cooperman, 1999; occurring issues like bullying (Blood, 1995; Bloodstein,
Conture, 2001; Gregory, 2003; Guitar, 1998; Manning, 2001; 1995; Bloom & Cooperman, 1999; Conture, 2001; Gregory,
Murphy & Quesal, 2002; Ramig & Bennett, 1995; Shapiro, 2003; Guitar, 1998; Manning, 2001; Murphy & Quesal,
1999; Sheehan, 1970; Silverman, 2004; Van Riper, 1982). 2002; Ramig & Bennett, 1995; Shapiro, 1999; Sheehan,
Although this study provides evidence that students who 1970; Silverman, 2004; Van Riper, 1982).
stutter were faced with direct bullying during the past week Similar to earlier research, students with fluency disor-
before testing, we did not obtain data on the perpetrators ders reported less confidence in their communication skills
who did the bullying, other students’ responses, teachers’ in public speaking, group discussions, interpersonal
perceptions, and measures of indirect bullying. Future studies conversations, and with strangers than did students without
are being conducted that will provide this additional disorders. The school environment promotes active learning,
information. small group interactions, and public speaking. Fears or
The relationship between self-esteem and bullying is not concerns about communicative participation in classroom or
surprising. The literature is replete with studies document- school activities may compromise opportunities for social
ing the negative effects of bullying on self-esteem. How- interactions and skill building by adolescents who stutter. If
ever, this study reports a higher percentage of participants these skills are not developed and practiced, students who
with low self-esteem than earlier studies (Blood et al., stutter may be perceived as more introverted, quiet,
2003; Yovetich et al., 2000). In students who stutter, the submissive, antisocial, and less outgoing. These perceptions
increased likelihood of being bullied may lead to negative by peers could lead to negative stereotypes and could result
self-esteem. Of course, the inverse relationship may also be in increased risk of being bullied.
valid, suggesting that low self-esteem makes students who Low confidence in communicative competence was also
stutter more vulnerable to bullying. Bullies target specific linked to low self-esteem. Avoidance strategies and a lack
students. Adolescents who stutter may be perceived “differ- of confidence in communication skills could be a direct
ent” because of their communication skills. However, some result of dealing with the negative consequences of
students who are perceived as different may develop specific stuttering. It may be that students who stutter elect not to
attributes that serve as compensatory strategies and assist in participate in social interactions because of lack of experi-
developing and/or maintaining positive self-esteem and ence, fear of failure, prior negative reactions, and increased
healthy peer relations. Hodges et al. (1999) suggested that communicative pressure and stress. This could result in
having a close personal friend can actually serve to buffer increased isolation, negative stereotyping by peers, and
the negative effects of victimization and bullying. The poor self-esteem. Fortunately, the majority of students who
majority of adolescents who stutter (57%) did not report stutter demonstrated positive self-esteem. As suggested in
being bullied in the past week. It is interesting to speculate other research, many students who stutter appear to have
that these students have discovered successful ways to buffer developed protective techniques to maintain positive self-
the negative effects of bullying while maintaining their esteem even while receiving negative reactions from peers,
positive self-esteem. Although this study provides the teachers, and the public.
foundation for information about adolescent development in Our findings also support the earlier work of Hugh-Jones
the context of stuttering, there is a need for longitudinal and Smith (1999), Langevin et al. (1998), and Mooney and
studies examining the influences of social and communica- Smith (1995), who reported that bullying and/or teasing
tive relationships within the school and family environments. was a problem for individuals who stutter. The finding that
In view of the research that social and peer relationships the risk of being bullied in adolescents who stutter is high
rely heavily on communicative competence and skills, when compared to their fluent peers should be of consider-
adolescents who stutter appear to have developed successful able concern. Speech-language pathologists need to be
strategies for dealing with a lack of communicative aware of this information as they often serve as the
confidence. The data show that a relationship exists strongest advocates for students who stutter in the school
between communicative competence and the risk of being setting. Concern about the potential for being bullied and
bullied. The results also suggest that significantly more the need for school personnel and parents to take proactive
adolescents who stutter (57%) reported poorer self- roles in monitoring and/or reducing bullying behaviors
perceived communicative competence than did adolescents appear warranted. These findings lay the foundation for
who do not stutter (13%), especially during public future research in this area.

76 CONTEMPORARY ISSUES IN COMMUNICATION SCIENCE AND DISORDERS • Volume 31 • 69–79 • Spring 2004

Downloaded from: https://pubs.asha.org 212.96.37.65 on 01/19/2024, Terms of Use: https://pubs.asha.org/pubs/rights_and_permissions


Chesebro, J. W., McCroskey, J. C., Atwater, D. F., Behrenfuss,
REFERENCES R. M., Cawelti, G., Gaudino, J. L., et al. (1992). Communica-
tion apprehension and self-perceived communication competence
Arora, C. M. J. (1994). Is there any point to trying to reduce of at-risk students. Communication Education, 41, 345–360.
bullying in secondary schools? Educational Psychology in Conture, E. G. (2001). Stuttering (3rd ed.). Needham Heights,
Practice, 10(3), 155–162. MA: Allyn & Bacon.
Arora, C. M. J. (1996). Defining bullying. School Psychology Daly, J. A., McCroskey, J. C., Ayres, J., Hopf, T., & Ayres, D.
International, 17, 317–329. M. (1997). Avoiding communication: Shyness, reticence, and
Arora, C. M. J., & Thompson, D. A. (1987). Defining bullying communication apprehension (2nd ed.). Cresskill, NJ: Hampton
for a secondary school. Educational and Child Psychology, 4(3), Press.
110–120. Davis, S., Howell, P., & Cooke, F. (2002). Sociodynamic
Banks, R. (1997). Bullying in schools (Report No. EDO-PS-97- relationships between children who stutter and their non-
17). Champaign, IL: ERIC Clearinghouse on Elementary and stuttering classmates. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychia-
Early Childhood Education. (ERIC Document Reproduction try, 43(7), 939–947.
Service No. ED407154) Dawkins, J. (1996). Bullying, physical disability, and the
Blood, G. (1995). POWER2. Relapse management with adolescents paediatric patient. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology,
who stutter. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 38, 603–612.
26, 169–179. Espelage, D. L. (2002). Bullying in early adolescence: The role of
Blood, G. W., Blood, I. M., Tellis, G. M., & Gabel, R. M. the peer group (Report No. EDO-PS-02-16). Champaign, IL:
(2001). Communication apprehension and self-perceived ERIC Clearinghouse on Elementary and Early Childhood
communication competence in adolescents who stutter. Journal Education. (ERIC Document Reproduction Service No.
of Fluency Disorders, 26, 161–178. ED471912)

Blood, G. W., Blood, I. M., Tellis, G. M., & Gabel, R. M. Espelage, D. L., & Holt, M. K. (2001). Bullying and victimiza-
(2003). A preliminary study of self-esteem, stigma, and tion during early adolescence: Peer influences and psychosocial
disclosure in adolescents who stutter. Journal of Fluency correlates. In R. Geffner, M. Loring, & C. Young (Eds.),
Disorders, 28, 143–159. Bullying behavior: Current issues, research, and interventions
(pp. 123–142). New York: Haworth Press.
Blood, G., Blood, I., Tellis, G., Gabel, R., Mapp, C., Wertz, H.,
Fried S., & Fried, P. (1996). Bullies and victims: Helping your
et al. (1998). Coping with stuttering during adolescence. In C.
child survive the schoolyard battlefield. New York: M. Evans.
Healey & H. Peters (Eds.), Proceedings of the 2nd World
Congress on Fluency Disorders (pp. 319–324). Nijmegen, The Fujiki, M., Brinton, B., & Todd, C. (1996). Social skills of
Netherlands: University Press. children with specific language impairment. Language, Speech,
and Hearing Services in Schools, 27, 195–202.
Bloodstein, O. (1995). A handbook on stuttering (5th ed.). San
Diego, CA: Singular. Garbarino, J., & deLaura, E. (2002). And words can hurt
forever. New York: The Free Press.
Bloom, C., & Cooperman, D. K. (1999). Synergistic stuttering
therapy: A holistic approach. Boston: Butterworth Heinemann. Garrett, A. (2003). Bullying in American schools: Causes,
preventions, interventions. Jefferson, NC: McFarland & Company.
Bond, L., Carlin, J., Thomas, L., Rubin, K., & Patton, G.
(2001). Does bullying cause emotional problems? A prospective Geffner, R., Loring, M., & Young, C. (Eds.). (2001). Bullying
study of young teenagers. British Medical Journal, 323, behavior: Current issues, research, and interventions. New York:
480–484. Haworth Press.

Botting, N., & Conti-Ramsden, G. (2000). Social and behavioral Gregory, H. H. (2003). Stuttering therapy: Rationale and
difficulties in children with language impairment. Child procedures. Boston: Pearson Education.
Language, Teaching and Therapy, 16, 105–120. Guitar, B. (1998). Stuttering: An integrated approach to its nature
and treatment. Baltimore: Williams & Wilkins.
Brinton, B., & Fujiki, M. (1999). Social interactional behaviours
of children with specific language impairment. Topics in Hadley, P., & Rice, M. (1991). Conversational responsiveness of
Language Disorders, 19, 49–69. speech- and language-impaired preschoolers. Journal of Speech
and Hearing Research, 34, 1308–1317.
Brown, B. B., & Lohr, M. J. (1987). Peer group affiliation and
adolescent self-esteem: An integration of ego-identity and Hagborg, W. J. (1996). Scores of middle-school-age students on
symbolic interaction theories. Journal of Personality and Social the Rosenberg self-esteem scale. Psychological Reports, 78(3),
Psychology, 52, 47–55. 1071–1074.
Buhrmester, D. (1996). Need fulfillment, interpersonal compe- Hodges, E., Boivin, M., Vitaro, F., & Bukowski, W. (1999). The
tence, and the developmental contexts of early adolescent power of friendship: Protection against an escalating cycle of
friendship. In W. Bukowski, A. Newcomb, & W. Hartup (Eds.), peer victimization. Developmental Psychology, 35, 94–101.
The company they keep (pp. 158–185). New York: Cambridge Hodges, E., & Perry, D. G. (1999). Personal and interpersonal
University Press. antecedents and consequences of victimisation by peers. Journal
Byrne, B. (1994). Bullies and victims in a school setting with of Personality and Social Psychology, 78, 677–785.
reference to some Dublin schools. Irish Journal of Psychology, Hollingshead, A. B. (1975). Four Factor Index of Social Status.
15, 574–586. Unpublished manuscript, Yale University, New Haven, CT.
Callaghan, S., & Joseph, S. (1995). Self-concept and peer Hugh-Jones, S., & Smith, P. (1999). Self-reports of short- and
victimization among school children. Personality and Individual long-term effects of bullying on children who stammer. British
Differences, 18(1), 161–163. Journal of Educational Psychology, 69, 141–158.

Blood • Blood: Bullying and Stuttering 77

Downloaded from: https://pubs.asha.org 212.96.37.65 on 01/19/2024, Terms of Use: https://pubs.asha.org/pubs/rights_and_permissions


Johnson, H. R., Thompson, M. J., Wilkinson, S., Walsh, L., U.S. youth: Prevalence and association with psychosocial
Balding, J., & Wright, V. (2002). Vulnerability to bullying: adjustment. Journal of the American Medical Association, 285,
Teacher-reported conduct and emotional problems, hyperactivity, 2992–2100.
peer relationship difficulties, and prosocial behaviour in primary Olweus, D. (1990). Bullying among school children. In K.
school children. Educational Psychology, 22(5), 553–556. Hurrelmann & F. Losel (Eds.), Health hazards in adolescence
Knox, E., & Conti-Ramsden, G. (2003). Bullying risks of 11- (pp. 88–113). Berlin: DeGruyter.
year-old children with specific language impairment (SLI): Does Olweus, D. (1993). Bullying at school: What we know and what
school placement matter? International Journal of Language and we can do. Cambridge, MA: Blackwell.
Communication Disorders, 38(1), 1–12.
Olweus, D. (1997). Bully/victim problems in school: Facts and
Langevin, M., Bortnick, K., Hammer, T., & Wiebe, E. (1998). intervention. European Journal of Psychology of Education, 12,
Teasing/bullying experienced by children who stutter: Toward 495–510.
development of a questionnaire. Contemporary Issues in
Olweus, D. (2003). A profile of bullying at school. Educational
Communication Science and Disorders, 25, 12–24.
Leadership, 60(6), 12–17.
Lass, N. J., Ruscello, D. M., Pannbacker, M. D., Schmitt, J. F.,
& Everly-Myers, D. S. (1989). Speech-language pathologists’ O’Moore, A. M., Kirkham, C., & Smith, M. (1999). Bullying
perceptions of child and adult female and male stutterers. behaviour in Irish schools: A nationwide study. Irish Journal of
Journal of Fluency Disorders, 14(2), 127–134. Psychology, 18, 141–169.

Lass, N. J., Ruscello, D. M., Schmitt, J. F., Pannbacker, M. D., Parker, J. G., & Asher, S. R. (1987). Peer acceptance and later
Orlando, M. B., Dean, K. A., et al. (1992). Teachers’ percep- personal adjustment: Are low accepted children “at risk”?
tions of stutterers. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Psychological Bulletin, 102, 357–389.
Schools, 23(1), 78–81. Parker, J. G., & Asher, S. R. (1993). Friendship and friendship
Lerner, R. M., & Galambos, N. L. (1998). Adolescent develop- quality in middle childhood: Links with peer group acceptance
ment: Challenges and opportunities for research, programs, and and feelings of loneliness and social dissatisfaction. Develop-
policies. Annual Review of Psychology, 49, 413–446. mental Psychology, 29, 611–621.

Manning, W. H. (2001). Clinical decision making in the diagnosis Parkhurst, J. T., & Asher, S. R. (1992). Peer rejection in middle
and treatment of stuttering disorders (2nd ed.). Vancouver, school: Subgroup differences in behavior, loneliness, and
Canada: Singular Thompson Learning. interpersonal concerns. Developmental Psychology, 28, 231–241.

McCroskey, J. C. (1977). Oral communication apprehension: A Pope, A. W., & Bierman, K. L. (1999). Predicting adolescent peer
summary of recent theory and research. Human Communication problems and antisocial activities: The relative roles of
Research, 4, 78–96. aggression and dysregulation. Developmental Psychology, 35,
335–346.
McCroskey, J. C. (1982). An introduction to rhetorical communi-
cation (4th ed.). Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall. Ramig, P., & Bennett, E. (1995). Working with 7- to 12-year-old
children who stutter: Ideas for intervention in the public schools.
McCroskey, J. C. (1997). Willingness to communicate, communi- Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 26,
cation apprehension, and self-perceived communication compe- 138–150.
tence: Conceptualizations and perspectives. In J. A. Daly, J. C.
McCroskey, J. Ayres, T. Hopf, & D. M. Ayres (Eds.), Avoiding Rice, M., Sell, M., & Hadley, P. (1991). Social interactions of
communication: Shyness, reticence and communication apprehen- speech- and language-impaired children. Journal of Speech and
sion (2nd ed., pp. 13–38). Cresskill, NJ: Hampton Press. Hearing Research, 34, 1299–1307.
McCroskey, J. C., & McCroskey, L. L. (1988). Self-report as an Richmond, V. P., & McCroskey, J. C. (1998). Communication
approach to measuring communication competence. Communica- apprehension, avoidance, and effectiveness (5th ed.). Needham
tion Research Reports, 5, 108–113. Heights, MA: Allyn & Bacon.
Mishna, F. (2003). Learning disabilities and bullying: Double Richmond, V. P., McCroskey, J. C., & McCroskey, L. L. (1989).
jeopardy. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 36(4), 336–347. An investigation of self-perceived communication competence
and personality orientations. Communication Research Reports,
Mooney, S., & Smith, P. K. (1995). Bullying and the child who
6, 28–36.
stammers. British Journal of Special Education, 22, 24–27.
Rigby, K. (1997). Bullying in schools: And what to do about it.
Murphy, W. P., & Quesal, R. W. (2002). Strategies for addressing
London: Jessica Kingsley.
bullying with the school-age child who stutters. Seminars in
Speech and Language, 23(3), 205–212. Rigby, K. (1999). Peer victimization at school and the health of
secondary school students. British Journal of Educational
Nabuzoka, D. (2003). Teacher ratings and peer nominations of
Psychology, 69, 95–104.
bullying and other behaviour of children with and without
learning difficulties. Educational Psychology, 23(3), 307–315. Rigby, K. (2000). Effects of peer victimization in schools and
perceived social support on adolescent well-being. Journal of
Nabuzoka, D., & Smith, P. K. (1993). Sociometric status and
Adolescence, 23, 57–68.
social behaviour of children with and without learning difficul-
ties. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 24, Riley, G. (1994). Stuttering Severity Instrument for Children and
1435–1448. Adults (3rd ed.). Austin, TX: Pro-Ed.
Nabuzoka, D., & Smith, P. K. (1999). Distinguishing serious Rosenberg, M. (1965). Society and the adolescent self-image.
playful fighting by children with learning disabilities and Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.
nondisabled children. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychia- Rosenberg, M. (1979). Conceiving the self. New York: Basic
try and Allied Disciplines, 40(6), 883–890. Books.
Nansel, T. J., Overpect, M., Pilla, R. S., Ruan, W. J., Simons- Rosenberg, M. (1986). Conceiving the self (2nd ed.). Malabar, FL:
Morton, B., & Scheidt, P. (2001). Bullying behaviors among Krieger.

78 CONTEMPORARY ISSUES IN COMMUNICATION SCIENCE AND DISORDERS • Volume 31 • 69–79 • Spring 2004

Downloaded from: https://pubs.asha.org 212.96.37.65 on 01/19/2024, Terms of Use: https://pubs.asha.org/pubs/rights_and_permissions


Rosenfeld, L. B., Grant, C. H., III, & McCroskey, J. (1995). Thompson, D., Arora, T., & Sharp, S. (2002). Bullying: Effective
Communication apprehension and self-perceived communication strategies for long-term improvement. New York: Routledge
competence of academically gifted students. Communication Falmer.
Education, 44, 79–86. Turnbaugh, K. R., Guitar, B. E., & Hoffman, P. R. (1979).
Ruscello, D. M., Lass, N. J., Schmitt, J. F., & Pannbacker, M. Speech clinicians’ attribution of personality traits as a function
D. (1994). Special educators’ perceptions of stutterers. Journal of stuttering severity. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders,
of Fluency Disorders, 19(2), 125–132. 22, 37–45.
Shapiro, D. A. (1999). Stuttering intervention. Austin, TX: Pro-Ed. Van Riper, C. (1982). The treatment of stuttering. Englewood
Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.
Sharp, S. (1995). How much does bullying hurt? The effects of
bullying on the personal well-being and educational progress of Welsh, J., Domitrovich, C., Bierman, K., & Lang, J. (2003).
secondary aged students. Educational and Child Psychology, 12, Promoting safe schools and healthy students in rural Pennsylva-
81–88. nia. Psychology in the Schools, 40(5), 457–472.
Sharp, S. (1996). Self-esteem, response style and victimization. Whitney, I., & Smith, P. (1993). A survey of the nature and
School Psychology International, 17, 347–357. extent of bully/victim problems in junior/middle and secondary
schools. Educational Research, 35, 3–25.
Sheard, C., Clegg, J., Standen, P., & Cromby, J. (2001).
Bullying and people with severe intellectual disability. Journal Wilde, M., & Haslam, C. (1996). Living with epilepsy: A
of Intellectual Disability Research, 45(5), 407–415. qualitative study investigating the experiences of young people
attending outpatients clinics in Leicester. Seizure, 5, 63–72.
Sheehan, J. (1970). Personality approaches. In J. G. Sheehan
Woods, C. L. (1978). Teachers’ predictions of the social position
(Ed.), Stuttering: Research and therapy (pp. 58–169). New York:
and speaking competence of stuttering students. Language,
Harper & Row.
Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 6, 177–182.
Silverman, E. M. (1982). Speech-language clinicians’ and
Woods, C. L., & Williams, D. E. (1971). Speech clinicians’
university students’ impressions of women and girls who stutter.
conceptions of boys and men who stutter. Journal of Speech and
Journal of Fluency Disorders, 7, 469–478.
Hearing Disorders, 36, 225–234.
Silverman, F. H. (2004). Stuttering and other fluency disorders
Yairi, E., & Williams, D. E. (1970). Speech clinicians’ stereotypes
(3rd ed.). Long Grove, IL: Waveland Press.
of elementary-school boys who stutter. Journal of Communica-
Smith, P. (1999). Comparison of bullying definitions within and tion Disorders, 3, 161–170.
across cultures. In S. Sharp (Ed.), Bullying behaviour in schools Yeakle, M. K., & Cooper, E. B. (1986). Teacher perceptions of
(pp. 37–38). London: NFER-Nelson. stuttering. Journal of Fluency Disorders, 11, 345–359.
Smith, P., & Ananiadou, K. (2003). The nature of school bullying Yovetich, W. S., Leschied, A. W., & Flicht, J. (2000). Self-esteem
and the effectiveness of school-based interventions. Journal of of school-age children who stutter. Journal of Fluency Disor-
Applied Psychoanalytic Studies, 5(2), 189–209. ders, 25, 143–153.
Smith, P., & Brain, P. (2000). Bullying in schools: Lessons from Zakahi, W. R., & Duran, R. L. (1984). Attraction, communicative
two decades of research. Aggressive-Behavior, 26(1), 1–9. competence, and communication satisfaction. Communication
Smith, P., Cowie, H., Olafsson, R., Liefooghe, A., Almeida, Research Reports, 1, 54–57.
A., Araki, H., et al. (2002). Definitions of bullying: A Zakahi, W. R., & Duran, R. L. (1985). Loneliness, communica-
comparison of terms used and age and gender differences in tive competence, and communication apprehension: Extension
a fourteen-country international comparison. Child Development, and replication. Communication Quarterly, 33, 50–60.
73(4), 1119–1133.
Steinberg, L. (1999). Adolescence (5th ed.). Boston: McGraw-Hill. Contact author: Gordon W. Blood, Department of Communica-
Sweeting, H., & West, P. (2001). Being different: Correlates of tion Sciences and Disorders, 110 Moore Building, The Pennsylva-
the experience of teasing and bullying at age 11. Research nia State University, University Park, PA 16802. E-mail:
Papers in Education, 16(3), 225–246. f2x@psu.edu

Blood • Blood: Bullying and Stuttering 79

Downloaded from: https://pubs.asha.org 212.96.37.65 on 01/19/2024, Terms of Use: https://pubs.asha.org/pubs/rights_and_permissions

You might also like