Download as docx, pdf, or txt
Download as docx, pdf, or txt
You are on page 1of 270

Part IV – Tools and Approaches Français

Chapter 29 - Ergonomics
OVERVIEW
Wolfgang Laurig and Joachim Vedder

In the 3rd edition of the ILO’s Encyclopaedia, published in 1983, ergonomics was
summarized in one article that was only about four pages long. Since the
publication of the 3rd edition, there has been a major change in emphasis and in
understanding of interrelationships in safety and health: the world is no longer
easily classifiable into medicine, safety and hazard prevention. In the last decade
almost every branch in the production and service industries has expended great
effort in improving productivity and quality. This restructuring process has yielded
practical experience which clearly shows that productivity and quality are directly
related to the design of working conditions. One direct economical measure of
productivity—the costs of absenteeism through illness—is affected by working
conditions. Therefore it should be possible to increase productivity and quality and
to avoid absenteeism by paying more attention to the design of working conditions.

In sum, the simple hypothesis of modern ergonomics can be stated thus: Pain and
exhaustion cause health hazards, wasted productivity and reduced quality, which
are measures of the costs and benefits of human work.

This simple hypothesis can be contrasted to occupational medicine which generally


restricts itself to establishing the aetiology of occupational diseases. Occupational
medicine’s goal is to establish conditions under which the probability of
developing such diseases is minimized. Using ergonomic principles these
conditions can be most easily formulated in the form of demands and load
limitations. Occupational medicine can be summed up as establishing “limitations
through medico-scientific studies”. Traditional ergonomics regards its role as one
of formulating the methods where, using design and work organization, the
limitations established through occupational medicine can be put into practice.
Traditional ergonomics could then be described as developing “corrections through
scientific studies”, where “corrections” are understood to be all work design
recommendations that call for attention to be paid to load limits only in order to
prevent health hazards. It is a characteristic of such corrective recommendations
that practitioners are finally left alone with the problem of applying them—there is
no multidisciplinary team effort.

The original aim of inventing ergonomics in 1857 stands in contrast to this kind of
“ergonomics by correction”:

... a scientific approach enabling us to reap, for the benefit of ourselves and others,
the best fruits of life’s labour for the minimum effort and maximum satisfaction
(Jastrzebowski 1857).

The root of the term “ergonomics” stems from the Greek “nomos” meaning rule,
and “ergo” meaning work. One could propose that ergonomics should develop
“rules” for a more forward-looking, prospective concept of design. In contrast to
“corrective ergonomics”, the idea of prospective ergonomics is based on applying
ergonomic recommendations which simultaneously take into consideration
profitability margins (Laurig 1992).

The basic rules for the development of this approach can be deduced from practical
experience and reinforced by the results of occupational hygiene and ergonomics
research. In other words, prospective ergonomics means searching for alternatives
in work design which prevent fatigue and exhaustion on the part of the working
subject in order to promote human productivity (“... for the benefit of ourselves
and others”). This comprehensive approach of prospective ergonomics includes
workplace and equipment design as well as the design of working conditions
determined by an increasing amount of information processing and a changing
work organization. Prospective ergonomics is, therefore, an interdisciplinary
approach of researchers and practitioners from a wide range of fields united by the
same goal, and one part of a general basis for a modern understanding of
occupational safety and health (UNESCO 1992).

Based on this understanding, the Ergonomics chapter in the 4th edition of the ILO
Encyclopaedia covers the different clusters of knowledge and experiences oriented
toward worker characteristics and capabilities, and aimed at an optimum use of the
resource “human work” by making work more “ergonomic”, that is, more humane.

The choice of topics and the structure of articles in this chapter follows the
structure of typical questions in the field as practised in industry. Beginning with
the goals, principles and methods of ergonomics, the articles which follow cover
fundamental principles from basic sciences, such as physiology and psychology.
Based on this foundation, the next articles introduce major aspects of an ergonomic
design of working conditions ranging from work organization to product design.
“Designing for everyone” puts special emphasis on an ergonomic approach that is
based on the characteristics and capabilities of the worker, a concept often
overlooked in practice. The importance and diversity of ergonomics is shown in
two examples at the end of the chapter and can also be found in the fact that many
other chapters in this edition of the ILO Encyclopaedia are directly related to
ergonomics, such as Heat and Cold, Noise, Vibration, Visual Display Units , and
virtually all chapters in the sections Accident and Safety Management and
Management and Policy.

THE NATURE AND AIMS OF ERGONOMICS


William T. Singleton

Definition and Scope

Ergonomics means literally the study or measurement of work. In this context, the
term work signifies purposeful human function; it extends beyond the more
restricted concept of work as labour for monetary gain to incorporate all activities
whereby a rational human operator systematically pursues an objective. Thus it
includes sports and other leisure activities, domestic work such as child care and
home maintenance, education and training, health and social service, and either
controlling engineered systems or adapting to them, for example, as a passenger in
a vehicle.

The human operator, the focus of study, may be a skilled professional operating a
complex machine in an artificial environment, a customer who has casually
purchased a new piece of equipment for personal use, a child sitting in a classroom
or a disabled person in a wheelchair. The human being is highly adaptable but not
infinitely so. There are ranges of optimum conditions for any activity. One of the
tasks of ergonomics is to define what these ranges are and to explore the
undesirable effects which occur if the limits are transgressed—for example if a
person is expected to work in conditions of excessive heat, noise or vibration, or if
the physical or mental workload is too high or too low.

Ergonomics examines not only the passive ambient situation but also the unique
advantages of the human operator and the contributions that can be made if a work
situation is designed to permit and encourage the person to make the best use of his
or her abilities. Human abilities may be characterized not only with reference to the
generic human operator but also with respect to those more particular abilities that
are called upon in specific situations where high performance is essential. For
example, an automobile manufacturer will consider the range of physical size and
strength of the population of drivers who are expected to use a particular model to
ensure that the seats are comfortable, that the controls are readily identifiable and
within reach, that there is clear visibility to the front and the rear, and that the
internal instruments are easy to read. Ease of entry and egress will also be taken
into account. By contrast, the designer of a racing car will assume that the driver is
athletic so that ease of getting in and out, for example, is not important and, in fact,
design features as a whole as they relate to the driver may well be tailored to the
dimensions and preferences of a particular driver to ensure that he or she can
exercise his or her full potential and skill as a driver.

In all situations, activities and tasks the focus is the person or persons involved. It
is assumed that the structure, the engineering and any other technology is there to
serve the operator, not the other way round.

History and Status

About a century ago it was recognized that working hours and conditions in some
mines and factories were not tolerable in terms of safety and health, and the need
was evident to pass laws to set permissible limits in these respects. The
determination and statement of those limits can be regarded as the beginning of
ergonomics. They were, incidentally, the beginning of all the activities which now
find expression through the work of the International Labour Organization (ILO).

Research, development and application proceeded slowly until the Second World
War. This triggered greatly accelerated development of machines and
instrumentation such as vehicles, aircraft, tanks, guns and vastly improved sensing
and navigation devices. As technology advanced, greater flexibility was available
to allow adaptation to the operator, an adaptation that became the more necessary
because human performance was limiting the performance of the system. If a
powered vehicle can travel at a speed of only a few kilometres per hour there is no
need to worry about the performance of the driver, but when the vehicle’s
maximum speed is increased by a factor of ten or a hundred, then the driver has to
react more quickly and there is no time to correct mistakes to avert disaster.
Similarly, as technology is improved there is less need to worry about mechanical
or electrical failure (for instance) and attention is freed to think about the needs of
the driver.

Thus ergonomics, in the sense of adapting engineering technology to the needs of


the operator, becomes simultaneously both more necessary and more feasible as
engineering advances.

The term ergonomics came into use about 1950 when the priorities of developing
industry were taking over from the priorities of the military. The development of
research and application for the following thirty years is described in detail in
Singleton (1982). The United Nations agencies, particularly the ILO and the World
Health Organization (WHO), became active in this field in the 1960s.

In immediate postwar industry the overriding objective, shared by ergonomics, was


greater productivity. This was a feasible objective for ergonomics because so much
industrial productivity was determined directly by the physical effort of the
workers involved—speed of assembly and rate of lifting and movement
determined the extent of output. Gradually, mechanical power replaced human
muscle power. More power, however, leads to more accidents on the simple
principle that an accident is the consequence of power in the wrong place at the
wrong time. When things are happening faster, the potential for accidents is further
increased. Thus the concern of industry and the aim of ergonomics gradually
shifted from productivity to safety. This occurred in the 1960s and early 1970s.
About and after this time, much of manufacturing industry shifted from batch
production to flow and process production. The role of the operator shifted
correspondingly from direct participation to monitoring and inspection. This
resulted in a lower frequency of accidents because the operator was more remote
from the scene of action but sometimes in a greater severity of accidents because of
the speed and power inherent in the process.

When output is determined by the speed at which machines function then


productivity becomes a matter of keeping the system running: in other words,
reliability is the objective. Thus the operator becomes a monitor, a trouble-shooter
and a maintainer rather than a direct manipulator.

This historical sketch of the postwar changes in manufacturing industry might


suggest that the ergonomist has regularly dropped one set of problems and taken up
another set but this is not the case for several reasons. As explained earlier, the
concerns of ergonomics are much wider than those of manufacturing industry. In
addition to production ergonomics, there is product or design ergonomics, that is,
adapting the machine or product to the user. In the car industry, for example,
ergonomics is important not only to component manufacturing and the production
lines but also to the eventual driver, passenger and maintainer. It is now routine in
the marketing of cars and in their critical appraisal by others to review the quality
of the ergonomics, considering ride, seat comfort, handling, noise and vibration
levels, ease of use of controls, visibility inside and outside, and so on.

It was suggested above that human performance is usually optimized within a


tolerance range of a relevant variable. Much of the early ergonomics attempted to
reduce both muscle power output and the extent and variety of movement by way
of ensuring that such tolerances were not exceeded. The greatest change in the
work situation, the advent of computers, has created the opposite problem. Unless
it is well designed ergonomically, a computer workspace can induce too fixed a
posture, too little bodily movement and too much repetition of particular
combinations of joint movements.

This brief historical review is intended to indicate that, although there has been
continuous development of ergonomics, it has taken the form of adding more and
more problems rather than changing the problems. However, the corpus of
knowledge grows and becomes more reliable and valid, energy expenditure norms
are not dependent on how or why the energy is expended, postural issues are the
same in aircraft seats and in front of computer screens, much human activity now
involves using videoscreens and there are well-established principles based on a
mix of laboratory evidence and field studies.

Ergonomics and Related Disciplines

The development of a science-based application which is intermediate between the


well-established technologies of engineering and medicine inevitably overlaps into
many related disciplines. In terms of its scientific basis, much of ergonomic
knowledge derives from the human sciences: anatomy, physiology and
psychology. The physical sciences also make a contribution, for example, to
solving problems of lighting, heating, noise and vibration.

Most of the European pioneers in ergonomics were workers among the human
sciences and it is for this reason that ergonomics is well-balanced between
physiology and psychology. A physiological orientation is required as a
background to problems such as energy expenditure, posture and application of
forces, including lifting. A psychological orientation is required to study problems
such as information presentation and job satisfaction. There are of course many
problems which require a mixed human sciences approach such as stress, fatigue
and shift work.

Most of the American pioneers in this field were involved in either experimental
psychology or engineering and it is for this reason that their typical occupational
titles—human engineering and human factors—reflect a difference in emphasis
(but not in core interests) from European ergonomics. This also explains why
occupational hygiene, from its close relationship to medicine, particularly
occupational medicine, is regarded in the United States as quite different from
human factors or ergonomics. The difference in other parts of the world is less
marked. Ergonomics concentrates on the human operator in action, occupational
hygiene concentrates on the hazards to the human operator present in the ambient
environment. Thus the central interest of the occupational hygienist is toxic
hazards, which are outside the scope of the ergonomist. The occupational hygienist
is concerned about effects on health, either long-term or short-term; the ergonomist
is, of course, concerned about health but he or she is also concerned about other
consequences, such as productivity, work design and workspace design. Safety and
health are the generic issues which run through ergonomics, occupational hygiene,
occupational health and occupational medicine. It is, therefore, not surprising to
find that in a large institution of a research, design or production kind, these
subjects are often grouped together. This makes possible an approach based on a
team of experts in these separate subjects, each making a specialist contribution to
the general problem of health, not only of the workers in the institution but also of
those affected by its activities and products. By contrast, in institutions concerned
with design or provision of services, the ergonomist might be closer to the
engineers and other technologists.
It will be clear from this discussion that because ergonomics is interdisciplinary
and still quite new there is an important problem of how it should best be fitted
into an existing organization. It overlaps onto so many other fields because it is
concerned with people and people are the basic and all-pervading resource of every
organization. There are many ways in which it can be fitted in, depending on the
history and objectives of the particular organization. The main criteria are that
ergonomics objectives are understood and appreciated and that mechanisms for
implementation of recommendations are built into the organization.

Aims of Ergonomics

It will be clear already that the benefits of ergonomics can appear in many different
forms, in productivity and quality, in safety and health, in reliability, in job
satisfaction and in personal development.

The reason for this breadth of scope is that its basic aim is efficiency in purposeful
activity—efficiency in the widest sense of achieving the desired result without
wasteful input, without error and without damage to the person involved or to
others. It is not efficient to expend unnecessary energy or time because insufficient
thought has been given to the design of the work, the workspace, the working
environment and the working conditions. It is not efficient to achieve the desired
result in spite of the situation design rather than with support from it.

The aim of ergonomics is to ensure that the working situation is in harmony with
the activities of the worker. This aim is self-evidently valid but attaining it is far
from easy for a variety of reasons. The human operator is flexible and adaptable
and there is continuous learning, but there are quite large individual differences.
Some differences, such as physical size and strength, are obvious, but others, such
as cultural differences and differences in style and in level of skill, are less easy to
identify.

In view of these complexities it might seem that the solution is to provide a flexible
situation where the human operator can optimize a specifically appropriate way of
doing things. Unfortunately such an approach is sometimes impracticable because
the more efficient way is often not obvious, with the result that a worker can go on
doing something the wrong way or in the wrong conditions for years.

Thus it is necessary to adopt a systematic approach: to start from a sound theory, to


set measurable objectives and to check success against these objectives. The
various possible objectives are considered below.

Safety and health

There can be no disagreement about the desirability of safety and health objectives.
The difficulty stems from the fact that neither is directly measurable: their
achievement is assessed by their absence rather than their presence. The data in
question always pertain to departures from safety and health.

In the case of health, much of the evidence is long-term as it is based on


populations rather than individuals. It is, therefore, necessary to maintain careful
records over long periods and to adopt an epidemiological approach through which
risk factors can be identified and measured. For example, what should be the
maximum hours per day or per year required of a worker at a computer
workstation? It depends on the design of the workstation, the kind of work and the
kind of person (age, vision, abilities and so on). The effects on health can be
diverse, from wrist problems to mental apathy, so it is necessary to carry out
comprehensive studies covering quite large populations while simultaneously
keeping track of differences within the populations.

Safety is more directly measurable in a negative sense in terms of kinds and


frequencies of accidents and damage. There are problems in defining different
kinds of accidents and identifying the often multiple causal factors and there is
often a distant relationship between the kind of accident and the degree of harm,
from none to fatality.

Nevertheless, an enormous body of evidence concerning safety and health has been
accumulated over the past fifty years and consistencies have been discovered
which can be related back to theory, to laws and standards and to principles
operative in particular kinds of situations.

Productivity and efficiency

Productivity is usually defined in terms of output per unit of time, whereas


efficiency incorporates other variables, particularly the ratio of output to input.
Efficiency incorporates the cost of what is done in relation to achievement, and in
human terms this requires the consideration of the penalties to the human operator.

In industrial situations, productivity is relatively easy to measure: the amount


produced can be counted and the time taken to produce it is simple to record.
Productivity data are often used in before/after comparisons of working methods,
situations or conditions. It involves assumptions about equivalence of effort and
other costs because it is based on the principle that the human operator will
perform as well as is feasible in the circumstances. If the productivity is higher
then the circumstances must be better. There is much to recommend this simple
approach provided that it is used with due regard to the many possible
complicating factors which can disguise what is really happening. The best
safeguard is to try to make sure that nothing has changed between the before and
after situations except the aspects being studied.
Efficiency is a more comprehensive but always a more difficult measure. It usually
has to be specifically defined for a particular situation and in assessing the results
of any studies the definition should be checked for its relevance and validity in
terms of the conclusions being drawn. For example, is bicycling more efficient
than walking? Bicycling is much more productive in terms of the distance that can
be covered on a road in a given time, and it is more efficient in terms of energy
expenditure per unit of distance or, for indoor exercise, because the apparatus
required is cheaper and simpler. On the other hand, the purpose of the exercise
might be energy expenditure for health reasons or to climb a mountain over
difficult terrain; in these circumstances walking will be more efficient. Thus, an
efficiency measure has meaning only in a well-defined context.

Reliability and quality

As explained above, reliability rather than productivity becomes the key measure
in high technology systems (for instance, transport aircraft, oil refining and power
generation). The controllers of such systems monitor performance and make their
contribution to productivity and to safety by making tuning adjustments to ensure
that the automatic machines stay on line and function within limits. All these
systems are in their safest states either when they are quiescent or when they are
functioning steadily within the designed performance envelope. They become
more dangerous when moving or being moved between equilibrium states, for
example, when an aircraft is taking off or a process system is being shut down.
High reliability is the key characteristic not only for safety reasons but also
because unplanned shut-down or stoppage is extremely expensive. Reliability is
straightforward to measure after performance but is extremely difficult to predict
except by reference to the past performance of similar systems. When or if
something goes wrong human error is invariably a contributing cause, but it is not
necessarily an error on the part of the controller: human errors can originate at the
design stage and during setting up and maintenance. It is now accepted that such
complex high-technology systems require a considerable and continuous
ergonomics input from design to the assessment of any failures that occur.

Quality is related to reliability but is very difficult if not impossible to measure.


Traditionally, in batch and flow production systems, quality has been checked by
inspection after output, but the current established principle is to combine
production and quality maintenance. Thus each operator has parallel responsibility
as an inspector. This usually proves to be more effective, but it may mean
abandoning work incentives based simply on rate of production. In ergonomic
terms it makes sense to treat the operator as a responsible person rather than as a
kind of robot programmed for repetitive performance.

Job satisfaction and personal development


From the principle that the worker or human operator should be recognized as a
person and not a robot it follows that consideration should be given to
responsibilities, attitudes, beliefs and values. This is not easy because there are
many variables, mostly detectable but not quantifiable, and there are large
individual and cultural differences. Nevertheless a great deal of effort now goes
into the design and management of work with the aim of ensuring that the situation
is as satisfactory as is reasonably practicable from the operator’s viewpoint. Some
measurement is possible by using survey techniques and some principles are
available based on such working features as autonomy and empowerment.

Even accepting that these efforts take time and cost money, there can still be
considerable dividends from listening to the suggestions, opinions and attitudes of
the people actually doing the work. Their approach may not be the same as that of
the external work designer and not the same as the assumptions made by the work
designer or manager. These differences of view are important and can provide a
refreshing change in strategy on the part of everyone involved.

It is well established that the human being is a continuous learner or can be, given
the appropriate conditions. The key condition is to provide feedback about past and
present performance which can be used to improve future performance. Moreover,
such feedback itself acts as an incentive to performance. Thus everyone gains, the
performer and those responsible in a wider sense for the performance. It follows
that there is much to be gained from performance improvement, including self-
development. The principle that personal development should be an aspect of the
application of ergonomics requires greater designer and manager skills but, if it can
be applied successfully, can improve all the aspects of human performance
discussed above.

Successful application of ergonomics often follows from doing no more than


developing the appropriate attitude or point of view. The people involved are
inevitably the central factor in any human effort and the systematic consideration
of their advantages, limitations, needs and aspirations is inherently important.

Conclusion

Ergonomics is the systematic study of people at work with the objective of


improving the work situation, the working conditions and the tasks performed. The
emphasis is on acquiring relevant and reliable evidence on which to base
recommendation for changes in specific situations and on developing more general
theories, concepts, guidelines and procedures which will contribute to the
continually developing expertise available from ergonomics.

ANALYSIS OF ACTIVITIES, TASKS AND WORK


SYSTEMS
Véronique De Keyser

It is difficult to speak of work analysis without putting it in the perspective of


recent changes in the industrial world, because the nature of activities and the
conditions in which they are carried out have undergone considerable evolution in
recent years. The factors giving rise to these changes have been numerous, but
there are two whose impact has proved crucial. On the one hand, technological
progress with its ever-quickening pace and the upheavals brought about by
information technologies have revolutionized jobs (De Keyser 1986). On the other
hand, the uncertainty of the economic market has required more flexibility in
personnel management and work organization. If the workers have gained a wider
view of the production process that is less routine-oriented and undoubtedly more
systematic, they have at the same time lost exclusive links with an environment, a
team, a production tool. It is difficult to view these changes with serenity, but we
have to face the fact that a new industrial landscape has been created, sometimes
more enriching for those workers who can find their place in it, but also filled with
pitfalls and worries for those who are marginalized or excluded. However, one idea
is being taken up in firms and has been confirmed by pilot experiments in many
countries: it should be possible to guide changes and soften their adverse effects
with the use of relevant analyses and by using all resources for negotiation between
the different work actors. It is within this context that we must place work analyses
today—as tools allowing us to describe tasks and activities better in order to guide
interventions of different kinds, such as training, the setting up of new
organizational modes or the design of tools and work systems. We speak of
analyses, and not just one analysis, since there exist a large number of them,
depending on the theoretical and cultural contexts in which they are developed, the
particular goals they pursue, the evidence they collect, or the analyser’s concern for
either specificity or generality. In this article, we will limit ourselves to presenting
a few characteristics of work analyses, and emphasizing the importance of
collective work. Our conclusions will highlight other paths that the limits of this
text prevent us from pursuing in greater depth.

Some Characteristics of Work Analyses

The context

If the primary goal of any work analysis is to describe what the operator does, or
should do, placing it more precisely into its context has often seemed indispensable
to researchers. They mention, according to their own views, but in a broadly
similar manner, the concepts of context, situation, environment, work domain,
work world or work environment. The problem lies less in the nuances between
these terms than in the selection of variables that need to be described in order to
give them a useful meaning. Indeed, the world is vast and industry is complex, and
the characteristics that could be referred to are innumerable. Two tendencies can be
noted among authors in the field. The first one sees the description of the context
as a means of capturing the reader’s interest and providing him or her with an
adequate semantic framework. The second has a different theoretical perspective: it
attempts to embrace both context and activity, describing only those elements of
the context that are capable of influencing the behaviour of operators.
The semantic framework

Context has evocative power. It is enough, for an informed reader, to read about an
operator in a control room engaged in a continuous process to call up a picture of
work through commands and surveillance at a distance, where the tasks of
detection, diagnosis and regulation predominate. What variables need to be
described in order to create a sufficiently meaningful context? It all depends on the
reader. Nonetheless, there is a consensus in the literature on a few key variables.
The nature of the economic sector, the type of production or service, the size and
the geographical location of the site are useful.

The production processes, the tools or machines and their level of automation
allow certain constraints and certain necessary qualifications to be guessed at. The
structure of the personnel, together with age and level of qualification and
experience are crucial data whenever the analysis concerns aspects of training or of
organizational flexibility. The organization of work established depends more on
the firm’s philosophy than on technology. Its description includes, notably, work
schedules, the degree of centralization of decisions and the types of control
exercised over the workers. Other elements may be added in different cases. They
are linked to the firm’s history and culture, its economic situation, work conditions
and any restructuring, mergers and investments. There exist at least as many
systems of classification as there are authors, and there are numerous descriptive
lists in circulation. In France, a special effort has been made to generalize simple
descriptive methods, notably allowing for the ranking of certain factors according
to whether or not they are satisfactory for the operator (RNUR 1976; Guelaud et al.
1977).
The description of relevant factors regarding the activity

The taxonomy of complex systems described by Rasmussen, Pejtersen and


Schmidts (1990) represents one of the most ambitious attempts to cover at the
same time the context and its influence on the operator. Its main idea is to
integrate, in a systematic fashion, the different elements of which it is composed
and to bring out the degrees of freedom and the constraints within which individual
strategies can be developed. Its exhaustive aim makes it difficult to manipulate, but
the use of multiple modes of representation, including graphs, to illustrate the
constraints has a heuristic value that is bound to be attractive to many readers.
Other approaches are more targeted. What the authors seek is the selection of
factors that can influence a precise activity. Hence, with an interest in the control
of processes in a changing environment, Brehmer (1990) proposes a series of
temporal characteristics of the context which affect the control and anticipation of
the operator (see figure 29.1). This author’s typology has been developed from
“micro-worlds”, computerized simulations of dynamic situations, but the author
himself, along with many others since, used it for the continuous-process industry
(Van Daele 1992). For certain activities, the influence of the environment is well
known, and the selection of factors is not too difficult. Thus, if we are interested in
heart rate in the work environment, we often limit ourselves to describing the air
temperatures, the physical constraints of the task or the age and training of the
subject—even though we know that by doing so we perhaps leave out relevant
elements. For others, the choice is more difficult. Studies on human error, for
example, show that the factors capable of producing them are numerous (Reason
1989). Sometimes, when theoretical knowledge is insufficient, only statistical
processing, combining context and activity analysis, allows us to bring out the
relevant contextual factors (Fadier 1990).

Figure 29.1 The criteria and sub-criteria of the taxonomy of micro-worlds proposed by
Brehmer (1990)

The Task or the Activity?

The task

The task is defined by its objectives, its constraints and the means it requires for
achievement. A function within the firm is generally characterized by a set of
tasks. The realized task differs from the prescribed task scheduled by the firm for a
large number of reasons: the strategies of operators vary within and among
individuals, the environment fluctuates and random events require responses that
are often outside the prescribed framework. Finally, the task is not always
scheduled with correct knowledge of its conditions of execution, hence the need
for adaptations in real time. But even if the task is updated during the activity,
sometimes to the point of being transformed, it still remains the central reference.

Questionnaires, inventories and taxonomies of tasks are numerous, especially in


the English-language literature—the reader will find excellent reviews in
Fleishman and Quaintance (1984) and in Greuter and Algera (1989). Certain of
these instruments are merely lists of elements—for example, the action verbs to
illustrate tasks—that are checked off according to the function studied. Others have
adopted a hierarchical principle, characterizing a task as interlocking elements,
ordered from the global to the particular. These methods are standardized and can
be applied to a large number of functions; they are simple to use, and the analytical
stage is much shortened. But where it is a question of defining specific work, they
are too static and too general to be useful.

Next, there are those instruments requiring more skill on the part of the researcher;
since the elements of analysis are not predefined, it is up to the researcher to
characterize them. The already outdated critical incident technique of Flanagan
(1954), where the observer describes a function by reference to its difficulties and
identifies the incidents which the individual will have to face, belongs to this
group.

It is also the path adopted by cognitive task analysis (Roth and Woods 1988). This
technique aims to bring to light the cognitive requirements of a job. One way to do
this is to break the job down into goals, constraints and means. Figure 29.2 shows
how the task of an anaesthetist, characterized first by a very global goal of patient
survival, can be broken down into a series of sub-goals, which can themselves be
classified as actions and means to be employed. More than 100 hours of
observation in the operating theatre and subsequent interviews with anaesthetists
were necessary to obtain this synoptic “photograph” of the requirements of the
function. This technique, although quite laborious, is nevertheless useful in
ergonomics in determining whether all the goals of a task are provided with the
means of attaining them. It also allows for an understanding of the complexity of a
task (its particular difficulties and conflicting goals, for example) and facilitates the
interpretation of certain human errors. But it suffers, as do other methods, from the
absence of a descriptive language (Grant and Mayes 1991). Moreover, it does not
permit hypotheses to be formulated as to the nature of the cognitive processes
brought into play to attain the goals in question.

Figure 29.2 Cognitive analysis of the task: general anaesthesia


Other approaches have analysed the cognitive processes associated with given
tasks by drawing up hypotheses as to the information processing necessary to
accomplish them. A frequently employed cognitive model of this kind is
Rasmussen’s (1986), which provides, according to the nature of the task and its
familiarity for the subject, three possible levels of activity—based either on skill-
based habits and reflexes, on acquired rule-based procedures or on knowledge-
based procedures. But other models or theories that reached the height of their
popularity during the 1970s remain in use. Hence, the theory of optimal control,
which considers man as a controller of discrepancies between assigned and
observed goals, is sometimes still applied to cognitive processes. And modelling
by means of networks of interconnected tasks and flow charts continues to inspire
the authors of cognitive task analysis; figure 29.3 provides a simplified description
of the behavioural sequences in an energy-control task, constructing a hypothesis
about certain mental operations. All these attempts reflect the concern of
researchers to bring together in the same description not only elements of the
context, but also the task itself and the cognitive processes that underlie it—and to
reflect the dynamic character of work as well.

Figure 29.3 Simplified description of the determinants of a behaviour sequence in an


energy control taks: case of unacceptable consumption of energy
Since the arrival of the scientific organization of work, the concept of the
prescribed task has been adversely criticized because it has been viewed as
involving the imposition on workers of tasks that are not only designed without
consulting their needs, but are often accompanied by a specific performance time, a
restriction not welcomed by many workers. Even if the imposition aspect has
become rather more flexible today and even if the workers contribute more often to
the design of tasks, an assigned time for tasks remains necessary for schedule
planning and remains an essential component of work organization. The
quantification of time should not always be perceived in a negative manner. It
constitutes a valuable indicator of workload. A simple but common method of
measuring the time pressure exerted on a worker consists of determining the
quotient of the time necessary for the execution of a task divided by the available
time. The closer this quotient is to unity, the greater the pressure (Wickens 1992).
Moreover, quantification can be used in flexible but appropriate personnel
management. Let us take the case of nurses where the technique of predictive
analysis of tasks has been generalized, for example, in the Canadian regulation
Planning of Required Nursing (PRN 80) (Kepenne 1984) or one of its European
variants. Thanks to such task lists, accompanied by their mean time of execution,
one can, each morning, taking into account the number of patients and their
medical conditions, establish a care schedule and a distribution of personnel. Far
from being a constraint, PRN 80 has, in a number of hospitals, demonstrated that a
shortage of nursing personnel exists, since the technique allows a difference to be
established (see figure 29.4) between the desired and the observed, that is, between
the number of staff necessary and the number available, and even between the
tasks planned and the tasks carried out. The times calculated are only averages, and
the fluctuations in the situation do not always make them applicable, but this
negative aspect is minimized by a flexible organization that accepts adjustments
and allows the personnel to participate in effecting those adjustments.

Figure 29.4 Discrepancies between the numbers of personnel present and required on
the basis of PRN80
The activity, the evidence and the performance

An activity is defined as the set of behaviours and resources used by the operator
so that work occurs—that is to say, the transformation or production of goods or
the rendering of a service. This activity can be understood through observation in
different ways. Faverge (1972) has described four forms of analysis. The first is an
analysis in terms of gestures and postures, where the observer locates, within the
visible activity of the operator, classes of behaviour that are recognizable and
repeated during work. These activities are often coupled with a precise response:
for example, the heart rate, which allows us to assess the physical load associated
with each activity. The second form of analysis is in terms of information uptake.
What is discovered, through direct observation—or with the aid of cameras or
recorders of eye movements—is the set of signals picked up by the operator in the
information field surrounding him or her. This analysis is particularly useful in
cognitive ergonomics in trying to better understand the information processing
carried out by the operator. The third type of analysis is in terms of regulation. The
idea is to identify the adjustments of activity carried out by the operator in order to
deal with either fluctuations in the environment or changes in his own condition.
There we find the direct intervention of context within the analysis. One of the
most frequently cited research projects in this area is that of Sperandio (1972). This
author studied the activity of air traffic controllers and identified important strategy
changes during an increase in air traffic. He interpreted them as an attempt to
simplify the activity by aiming to maintain an acceptable load level, while at the
same time continuing to meet the requirements of the task. The fourth is an
analysis in terms of thought processes. This type of analysis has been widely used
in the ergonomics of highly automated posts. Indeed, the design of computerized
aids, and notably intelligent aids for the operator, requires a thorough
understanding of the way in which the operator reasons in order to solve certain
problems. The reasoning involved in scheduling, anticipation and diagnosis has
been the subject of analyses, an example of which can be found in figure 29.5 .
However, the evidence of mental activity can only be inferred. Apart from certain
observable aspects of behaviour, such as eye movements and problem-solving
time, most of these analyses resort to verbal response. Particular emphasis has been
placed, in recent years, on the knowledge necessary to accomplish certain
activities, with researchers trying not to postulate them at the outset but to make
them apparent through the analysis itself.

Figure 29.5 Analysis of mental activity. Strategies in the control of processes with long
response times: need for computerized support in diagnosis
Such efforts have brought to light the fact that almost identical performances can
be obtained with very different levels of knowledge, as long as operators are aware
of their limits and apply strategies adapted to their capabilities. Hence, in our study
of the start-up of a thermoelectric plant (De Keyser and Housiaux 1989), the start-
ups were carried out by both engineers and operators. The theoretical and
procedural knowledge that these two groups possessed, which had been elicited by
means of interviews and questionnaires, were very different. The operators in
particular sometimes had an erroneous understanding of the variables in the
functional links of the process. In spite of this, the performances of the two groups
were very close. But the operators took into account more variables in order to
verify the control of the start-up and undertook more frequent verifications. Such
results were also obtained by Amalberti (1991), who mentioned the existence of
metaknowledges allowing experts to manage their own resources.

What evidence of activity is appropriate to elicit? Its nature, as we have seen,


depends closely on the form of analysis planned. Its form varies according to the
degree of methodological care exercised by the observer. Provoked evidence is
distinguished from spontaneous evidence, and concomitant from subsequent
evidence. Generally speaking, when the nature of the work allows, concomitant
and spontaneous evidence are to be preferred. They are free of various drawbacks
such as unreliability of memory, observer interference, the effect of rationalizing
reconstruction on the part of the subject, and so forth. To illustrate these
distinctions, we will take the example of verbalizations. Spontaneous
verbalizations are verbal exchanges, or monologues expressed spontaneously
without being requested by the observer; provoked verbalizations are those made at
the specific request of the observer, such as the request made to the subject to
“think aloud”, which is well known in the cognitive literature. Both types can be
done in real time, during work, and are thus concomitant.

They can also be subsequent, as in interviews, or subjects’ verbalizations when


they view videotapes of their work. As for the validity of the verbalizations, the
reader should not ignore the doubt raised in this regard by the controversy between
Nisbett and De Camp Wilson (1977) and White (1988) and the precautions
suggested by numerous authors aware of their importance in the study of mental
activity in view of the methodological difficulties encountered (Ericson and Simon
1984; Savoyant and Leplat 1983; Caverni 1988; Bainbridge 1986).

The organization of this evidence, its processing and its formalization require
descriptive languages and sometimes analyses which go beyond field observation.
Those mental activities which are inferred from the evidence, for example, remain
hypothetical. Today they are often described using languages derived from
artificial intelligence, making use of representations in terms of schemes,
production rules and connecting networks. Moreover, the use of computerized
simulations—of micro-worlds—to pinpoint certain mental activities has become
widespread, even though the validity of the results obtained from such
computerized simulations, in view of the complexity of the industrial world, is
subject to debate. Finally, we must mention the cognitive modellings of certain
mental activities extracted from the field. Among the best known are the diagnosis
of the operator of a nuclear power plant, carried out in ISPRA (Decortis and
Cacciabue 1990), and the planning of the combat pilot perfected in Centre d’études
et de recherches de médecine aérospatiale (CERMA) (Amalberti et al. 1989).

Measurement of the discrepancies between the performance of these models and


that of real, living operators is a fruitful field in activity analysis. Performance is
the outcome of the activity, the final response given by the subject to the
requirements of the task. It is expressed at the level of production: productivity,
quality, error, incident, accident—and even, at a more global level, absenteeism or
turnover. But it must also be identified at the individual level: the subjective
expression of satisfaction, stress, fatigue or workload, and many physiological
responses are also performance indicators. Only the entire set of data permits
interpretation of the activity—that is to say, judging whether or not it furthers the
desired goals, while remaining within human limits. There exists a set of norms
which, up to a certain point, guide the observer. But these norms are not situated—
they do not take into account the context, its fluctuations and the condition of the
worker. This is why in design ergonomics, even when rules, norms and models
exist, designers are advised to test the product using prototypes as early as possible
and to evaluate the users’ activity and performance.

Individual or Collective Work?

While in the vast majority of cases, work is a collective act, most work analyses
focus on tasks or individual activities. Nonetheless the fact is that technological
evolution, just like work organization, today emphasizes distributed work, whether
it be between workers and machines or simply within a group. What paths have
been explored by authors so as to take this distribution into account (Rasmussen,
Pejtersen and Schmidts 1990)? They focus on three aspects: structure, the nature of
exchanges and structural lability.

Structure

Whether we view structure as elements of the analysis of people, or of services, or


even of different branches of a firm working in a network, the description of the
links that unite them remains a problem. We are very familiar with the organigrams
within firms that indicate the structure of authority and whose various forms reflect
the organizational philosophy of the firm—very hierarchically organized for a
Taylor-like structure, or flattened like a rake, even matrix-like, for a more flexible
structure. Other descriptions of distributed activities are possible: an example is
given in figure 29.6 . More recently, the need for firms to represent their
information exchanges at a global level has led to a rethinking of information
systems. Thanks to certain descriptive languages—for example, design schemas, or
entity-relations-attribute matrixes—the structure of relations at the collective level
can today be described in a very abstract manner and can serve as a springboard for
the creation of computerized management systems.

Figure 29.6 Integrated life cycle design


The nature of exchanges

Simply having a description of the links uniting the entities says little about the
content itself of the exchanges; of course the nature of the relation can be specified
—movement from place to place, information transfers, hierarchical dependence,
and so on—but this is often quite inadequate. The analysis of communications
within teams has become a favoured means of capturing the very nature of
collective work, encompassing subjects mentioned, creation of a common language
in a team, modification of communications when circumstances are critical, and so
forth (Tardieu, Nanci and Pascot 1985; Rolland 1986; Navarro 1990; Van Daele
1992; Lacoste 1983; Moray, Sanderson and Vincente 1989). Knowledge of these
interactions is particularly useful for the creation of computer tools, notably
decision-making aids for understanding errors. The different stages and the
methodological difficulties linked to the use of this evidence have been well
described by Falzon (1991).
Structural lability

It is the work on activities rather than on tasks that has opened up the field of
structural lability—that is to say, of the constant reconfigurations of collective
work under the influence of contextual factors. Studies such as those of Rogalski
(1991), who over a long period analysed the collective activities dealing with forest
fires in France, and Bourdon and Weill Fassina (1994), who studied the
organizational structure set up to deal with railway accidents, are both very
informative. They clearly show how the context moulds the structure of exchanges,
the number and type of actors involved, the nature of the communications and the
number of parameters essential to the work. The more this context fluctuates, the
further the fixed descriptions of the task are removed from reality. Knowledge of
this lability, and a better understanding of the phenomena that take place within it,
are essential in planning for the unpredictable and in order to provide better
training for those involved in collective work in a crisis.

Conclusions

The various phases of the work analysis that have been described are an iterative
part of any human factors design cycle (see figure 29.6). In this design of any
technical object, whether a tool, a workstation or a factory, in which human factors
are a consideration, certain information is needed in time. In general, the beginning
of the design cycle is characterized by a need for data involving environmental
constraints, the types of jobs that are to be carried out, and the various
characteristics of the users. This initial information allows the objects
specifications to be drawn up so as to take into account work requirements. But
this is, in some sense, only a coarse model compared to the real work situation.
This explains why models and prototypes are necessary that, from their inception,
allow not the jobs themselves, but the activities of the future users to be evaluated.
Consequently, while the design of the images on a monitor in a control room can
be based on a thorough cognitive analysis of the job to be done, only a data-based
analysis of the activity will allow an accurate determination of whether the
prototype will actually be of use in the actual work situation (Van Daele 1988).
Once the finished technical object is put into operation, greater emphasis is put on
the performance of the users and on dysfunctional situations, such as accidents or
human error. The gathering of this type of information allows the final corrections
to be made that will increase the reliability and usability of the completed object.
Both the nuclear industry and the aeronautics industry serve as example:
operational feedback involves reporting every incident that occurs. In this way, the
design loop comes full circle.

ERGONOMICS AND STANDARDIZATION


Friedhelm Nachreiner
Origins

Standardization in the field of ergonomics has a relatively short history. It started


in the beginning of the 1970s when the first committees were founded on the
national level (e.g., in Germany within the standardization institute DIN), and it
continued on an international level after the foundation of the ISO (International
Organization for Standardization) TC (Technical Committee) 159 “Ergonomics”,
in 1975. In the meantime ergonomics standardization takes place on regional levels
as well, for example, on the European level within the CEN (Commission
européenne de normalisation), which established its TC 122 “Ergonomics” in
1987. The existence of the latter committee underscores the fact that one of the
important reasons for establishing committees for the standardization of
ergonomics knowledge and principles can be found in legal (and quasi-legal)
regulations, especially with respect to safety and health, which require the
application of ergonomics principles and findings in the design of products and
work systems. National laws requiring the application of well-established
ergonomics findings were the reason for the establishment of the German
ergonomics committee in 1970, and European Directives, especially the Machinery
Directive (relating to safety standards), were responsible for establishing an
ergonomics committee on the European level. Since legal regulations usually are
not, cannot and should not be very specific, the task of specifying which
ergonomics principles and findings should be applied was given to or taken up by
ergonomics standardization committees. Especially on the European level, it can
be recognized that ergonomics standardization can contribute to the task of
providing for broad and comparable conditions of machinery safety, thus removing
barriers to the free trade of machinery within the continent itself.

Perspectives

Ergonomics standardization thus started with a strong protective, although


preventive, perspective, with ergonomics standards being developed with the aim
of protecting workers against adverse effects at different levels of health
protection. Ergonomics standards were thus prepared with the following intentions
in view:

· to ensure that assigned tasks do not exceed the limits of the performance
capacities of the worker

· to prevent injury or any detrimental effects to the health of the worker whether
permanent or transient, either in the short or in the long run, even if the tasks in
question can be performed, if only for a short time, without negative effects

· to provide that tasks and working conditions will not lead to impairments, even
if recuperation is possible with time.
International standardization, which was not so closely coupled to legislation, on
the other hand, always also tried to open a perspective in the direction of producing
standards which would go beyond the prevention of and protection against adverse
effects (e.g., by specifying minimal/maximal values) and instead proactively
provide for optimal working conditions to promote the well-being and personal
development of the worker, as well as the effectiveness, efficiency, reliability and
productivity of the work system.

This is a point where it becomes evident that ergonomics, and especially


ergonomics standardization, has very distinct social and political dimensions.
Whereas the protective approach with respect to safety and health is generally
accepted and agreed upon among the parties involved (employers, unions,
administration and ergonomics experts) for all levels of standardization, the
proactive approach is not equally accepted by all parties in the same way. This
might be due to the fact that, especially where legislation requires the application
of ergonomics principles (and thus either explicitly or implicitly the application of
ergonomics standards), some parties feel that such standards might limit their
freedom of action or negotiation. Since international standards are less compelling
(transferring them into the body of national standards is at the discretion of the
national standardization committees) the proactive approach has been developed
furthest at the international level of ergonomics standardization.

The fact that certain regulations would indeed restrict the discretion of those to
whom they applied served to discourage standardization in certain areas, for
example in connection with the European Directives under Article 118a of the
Single European Act, relating to safety and health in the use and operation of
machinery at the workplace, and in the design of work systems and workplace
design. On the other hand, under the Directives issued under Article 100a, relating
to safety and health in the design of machinery with regard to the free trade of this
machinery within the European Union (EU), European ergonomics standardization
is mandated by the European Commission.

From an ergonomics point of view, however, it is difficult to understand why


ergonomics in the design of machinery should be different from that in the use and
operation of machinery within a work system. It is thus to be hoped that the
distinction will be given up in the future, since it seems to be more detrimental
than beneficial to the development of a consistent body of ergonomics standards.

Types of Ergonomics Standards

The first international ergonomics standard to have been developed (based on a


German DIN national standard) is ISO 6385, “Ergonomic principles in the design
of work systems”, published in 1981. It is the basic standard of the ergonomics
standards series and set the stage for the standards which followed by defining the
basic concepts and stating the general principles of the ergonomic design of work
systems, including tasks, tools, machinery, workstations, work space, work
environment and work organization. This international standard, which is now
undergoing revision, is a guideline standard, and as such provides guidelines to be
followed. It does not, however, provide technical or physical specifications which
have to be met. These can be found in a different type of standards, that is,
specification standards, for example, those on anthropometry or thermal
conditions. Both types of standards fulfil different functions. While guideline
standards intend to show their users “what to do and how to do it” and indicate
those principles that must or should be observed, for example, with respect to
mental workload, specification standards provide users with detailed information
about safety distances or measurement procedures, for example, that have to be
met and where compliance with these prescriptions can be tested by specified
procedures. This is not always possible with guideline standards, although despite
their relative lack of specificity it can usually be demonstrated when and where
guidelines have been violated. A subset of specification standards are “database”
standards, which provide the user with relevant ergonomics data, for example,
body dimensions.

CEN standards are classified as A-, B- and C-type standards, depending on their
scope and field of application. A-type standards are general, basic standards which
apply to all kinds of applications, B-type standards are specific for an area of
application (which means that most of the ergonomics standards within the CEN
will be of this type), and C-type standards are specific for a certain kind of
machinery, for example, hand-held drilling machines.

Standardization Committees

Ergonomics standards, like other standards, are produced in the appropriate


technical committees (TCs), their subcommittees (SCs) or working groups (WGs).
For the ISO this is TC 159, for CEN it is TC 122, and on the national level, the
respective national committees. Besides the ergonomics committees, ergonomics is
also dealt with in TCs working on machine safety (e.g., CEN TC 114 and ISO TC
199) with which liaison and close cooperation is maintained. Liaisons are also
established with other committees for which ergonomics might be of relevance.
Responsibility for ergonomics standards, however, is reserved to the ergonomics
committees themselves.

A number of other organizations are engaged in the production of ergonomics


standards, such as the IEC (International Electrotechnical Commission);
CENELEC, or the respective national committees in the electrotechnical field;
CCITT (Comité consultative international des organisations téléphoniques et
télégraphiques) or ETSI (European Telecommunication Standards Institute) in the
field of telecommunications; ECMA (European Computer Manufacturers
Association) in the field of computer systems; and CAMAC (Computer Assisted
Measurement and Control Association) in the field of new technologies in
manufacturing, to name only a few. With some of these the ergonomics
committees do have liaisons in order to avoid duplication of work or inconsistent
specifications; with some organizations (e.g., the IEC) even joint technical
committees are established for cooperation in areas of mutual interest. With other
committees, however, there is no coordination or cooperation at all. The main
purpose of these committees is to produce (ergonomics) standards that are specific
to their field of activity. Since the number of such organizations at the different
levels is rather large, it becomes quite complicated (if not impossible) to carry out
a complete overview of ergonomics standardization. The present review will
therefore be restricted to ergonomics standardization in the international and
European ergonomics committees.

Structure of Standardization Committees

Ergonomics standardization committees are quite similar to one another in


structure. Usually one TC within a standardization organization is responsible for
ergonomics. This committee (e.g., ISO TC 159) mainly has to do with decisions
about what should be standardized (e.g., work items) and how to organize and
coordinate the standardization within the committee, but usually no standards are
prepared at this level. Below the TC level are other committees. For example, the
ISO has subcommittees (SCs), which are responsible for a defined field of
standardization: SC 1 for general ergonomic guiding principles, SC 3 for
anthropometry and biomechanics, SC 4 for human-system interaction and SC 5 for
the physical work environment. CEN TC 122 has working groups (WGs) below
the TC level which are so constituted as to deal with specified fields within
ergonomics standardization. SCs within ISO TC 159 operate as steering
committees for their field of responsibility and do the first voting, but usually they
do not also prepare standards. This is done in their WGs, which are composed of
experts nominated by their national committees, whereas SC and TC meetings are
attended by national delegations representing national points of view. Within the
CEN, duties are not sharply distinguished at the WG level; WGs operate both as
steering and production committees, although a good deal of work is accomplished
in ad hoc groups, which are composed of members of the WG (nominated by their
national committees) and established to prepare the drafts for a standard. WGs
within an ISO SC are established to do the practical standardization work, that is,
prepare drafts, work on comments, identify needs for standardization, and prepare
proposals to the SC and TC, which will then take the appropriate decisions or
actions.

Preparation of Ergonomics Standards

The preparation of ergonomics standards has changed quite markedly within the
last years in view of the stronger emphasis now being placed on European and
other international developments. In the beginning, national standards, which had
been prepared by experts from one country in their national committee and agreed
upon by the interested parties among the general public of that country in a
specified voting procedure, were transferred as input to the responsible SC and
WG of ISO TC 159, after a formal vote had been taken at the TC level that such an
international standard should be prepared. The working group, composed of
ergonomics experts (and experts from politically interested parties) from all
participating member bodies (i.e., the national standardization organizations) of TC
159 who were willing to cooperate in this work project, would then work on any
inputs and prepare a working draft (WD). After this draft proposal is agreed upon
in the WG, it becomes a committee draft (CD), which is distributed to the member
bodies of the SC for approval and comments. If the draft receives substantial
support from the SC member bodies (i.e., if at least two-thirds vote in favour) and
after comments by the national committees have been incorporated by the WG in
the improved version, a Draft International Standard (DIS) is submitted for voting
to all members of TC 159. If substantial support, at this step from the member
bodies of the TC, is achieved (and perhaps after incorporating editorial changes),
this version will then be published as an International Standard (IS) by the ISO.
Voting of the member bodies at the TC and SC level is based on voting at the
national level, and comments can be supplied through the member bodies by
experts or interested parties in each country. The procedure is roughly equivalent
in CEN TC 122, with the exception that there are no SCs below the TC level and
that voting takes part with weighted votes (according to the size of the country)
whereas within the ISO the rule is one country, one vote. If a draft fails at any step,
and unless the WG decides that an agreeable revision cannot be achieved, it has to
be revised and then has to pass through the voting procedure again.

International standards are then transferred into national standards if the national
committees vote accordingly. By contrast, European Standards (ENs) have to be
transferred into national standards by the CEN members and conflicting national
standards have to be withdrawn. That means that harmonized ENs will be effective
in all CEN countries (and, due to their influence on trade, will be relevant to
manufacturers in all other countries who intend to sell goods to a customer in a
CEN country).

ISO-CEN Cooperation

In order to avoid conflicting standards and duplication of work and to allow non-
CEN members to take part in developments in the CEN, a cooperative agreement
between the ISO and the CEN has been achieved (the so-called Vienna Agreement)
which regulates the formalities and provides for a so-called parallel voting
procedure, which allows the same drafts to be voted upon in the CEN and the ISO
in parallel, if the responsible committees agree to do so. Among the ergonomics
committees the tendency is quite clear: avoid duplication of work (manpower and
financial resources are too limited), avoid conflicting specifications, and try to
achieve a consistent body of ergonomics standards based on a division of labour.
Whereas CEN TC 122 is bound by the decisions of the EU administration and gets
mandated work items to stipulate the specifications of European directives, ISO
TC 159 is free to standardize whatever it thinks necessary or appropriate in the
field of ergonomics. This has led to shifts in the emphasis of both committees, with
the CEN concentrating on machinery and safety-related topics and the ISO
concentrating on areas where broader market interests than Europe are concerned
(e.g., work with VDUs and control-room design for process and related industries);
on areas where the operation of machinery is concerned, as in work system design;
and on such areas as work environment and work organization as well. The
intention, however, is to transfer work results from the CEN to the ISO, and vice
versa, in order to build up a body of consistent ergonomics standards which in fact
are effective all over the world.

The formal procedure of producing standards is still the same today. But since the
emphasis has shifted more and more to the international or the European level,
more and more activities are being transferred to these committees. Drafts are now
usually worked out directly in these committees and are no longer based on
existing national standards. After the decision has been made that a standard
should be developed, work directly starts at one of these supranational levels,
based on whatever input there may be available, sometimes starting from zero.
This changes the role of the national ergonomics committees quite dramatically.
While heretofore they formally developed their own national standards according
to their national rules, they now have the task of observing and influencing
standardization on the supranational levels—via the experts who work out the
standards or via comments made at the different steps of voting (within the CEN, a
national standardization project will be halted if a comparable project is being
simultaneously worked on at the CEN level). This makes the task still more
complicated, since this influence can only be exerted indirectly and since the
preparation of ergonomics standards is not just a matter of pure science but a
matter of bargaining, consensus and agreement (not least due to the political
implications which the standard might have). This, of course, is one of the reasons
why the process of producing an international or European ergonomics standard
usually takes several years and why ergonomics standards cannot reflect the latest
state of the art in ergonomics. International ergonomics standards thus have to be
examined every five years, and, if necessary, undergo revision.

Fields of Ergonomics Standardization

International ergonomics standardization started with guidelines on the general


principles of ergonomics in the design of work systems; they were laid down in
ISO 6385, which is now under revision in order to incorporate new developments.
The CEN has produced a similar basic standard (EN 614, Part 1, 1994)—this is
oriented more to machinery and safety—and is preparing a standard with
guidelines on task design as a second part of this basic standard. The CEN thus
emphasizes the importance of operator tasks in the design of machinery or work
systems, for which appropriate tools or machinery have to be designed.
Another area where concepts and guidelines have been laid down in standards is
the field of mental workload. ISO 10075, Part 1, defines terms and concepts (e.g.,
fatigue, monotony, reduced vigilance), and Part 2 (at the stage of a DIS in the latter
half of the 1990s) provides guidelines for the design of work systems with respect
to mental workload in order to avoid impairments.

SC 3 of ISO TC 159 and WG 1 of CEN TC 122 produce standards on


anthropometry and biomechanics, covering, among other topics, methods of
anthropometric measurements, body dimensions, safety distances and access
dimensions, the evaluation of working postures and the design of workplaces in
relation to machinery, recommended limits of physical strength and problems of
manual handling.

SC 4 of ISO 159 shows how technological and social changes affect ergonomics
standardization and the programme of such a subcommittee. SC 4 started as
“Signals and Controls” by standardizing principles for displaying information and
designing control actuators, with one of its work items being the visual display unit
(VDU), used for office tasks. It soon became apparent, however, that standardizing
the ergonomics of VDUs would not be sufficient, and that standardization
“around” this workstation—in the sense of a work system—was required, covering
areas such as hardware (e.g., the VDU itself, including displays, keyboards, non-
keyboard input devices, workstations), work environment (e.g., lighting), work
organization (e.g., task requirements), and software (e.g., dialogue principles,
menu and direct manipulation dialogues). This led to a multipart standard (ISO
9241) covering “ergonomic requirements for office work with VDUs” with at the
moment 17 parts, 3 of which have reached the status of an IS already. This
standard will be transferred to the CEN (as EN 29241) which will specify
requirements for the VDU directive (90/270 EEC) of the EU—although this is a
directive under article 118a of the Single European Act. This series of standards
provides guidelines as well as specifications, depending on the subject of the given
part of the standard, and introduces a new concept of standardization, the user
performance approach, which might help to solve some of the problems in
ergonomics standardization. It is described more fully in the chapter Visual
Display Units.

The user performance approach is based on the idea that the aim of standardization
is to prevent impairment and to provide for optimal working conditions for the
operator, but not to establish technical specification per se. Specification is thus
regarded only as a means to the end of unimpaired, optimal user performance. The
important thing is to achieve this unimpaired performance of the operator,
regardless of whether a certain physical specification is met. This requires that the
unimpaired operator performance which has to be achieved, for example, reading
performance on a VDU, must be specified in the first place, and second, that
technical specifications be developed which will enable the desired performance to
be achieved, based on the available evidence. The manufacturer is then free to
follow these technical specifications, which will ensure that the product complies
with the ergonomics requirements. Or he may demonstrate, by comparison with a
product that is known to fulfil the requirements (either by compliance with the
technical specifications of the standard or by proven performance), that with the
new product the performance requirements are equally or better fulfilled than with
the reference product, with or without compliance to the technical specifications of
the standard. A test procedure which has to be followed for demonstrating
conformance with the user performance requirements of the standard is specified in
the standard.

This approach helps to overcome two problems. Standards, by virtue of their


specifications, which are based on the state of the art (and technology) at the time
of preparation of the standard, can restrict new developments. Specifications that
are based on a certain technology (e.g., cathode-ray tubes) may be inappropriate
for other technologies. Independently of technology, however, the user of a display
device (for instance) should be able to read and understand the information
displayed effectively and efficiently without any impairments, irrespective of
whatever technique may be used. Performance in this case must, however, not be
restricted to the pure output (as measured in terms of speed or accuracy) but must
include considerations of comfort and effort as well.

The second problem that can be dealt with by this approach is the problem of
interactions between conditions. Physical specification usually is unidimensional,
leaving other conditions out of consideration. In the case of interactive effects,
however, this can be misleading or even wrong. By specifying performance
requirements, on the other hand, and leaving the means to achieve these to the
manufacturer, any solution that satisfies these performance requirements will be
acceptable. Treating specification as a means to an end thus represents a genuine
ergonomic perspective.

Another standard with a work system approach is under preparation in SC 4, which


relates to the design of control rooms, for instance, for process industries or power
stations. A multipart standard (ISO 11064) is expected to be prepared as a result,
with the different parts dealing with such aspects of control-room design as layout,
operator workstation design, and the design of displays and input devices for
process control. Because these work items and the approach taken clearly exceed
problems of the design of “displays and controls”, SC 4 has been renamed
“Human-System Interaction”.

Environmental problems, especially those relating to thermal conditions and


communication in noisy environments, are dealt with in SC 5, where standards
have been or are being prepared on measurement methods, methods for the
estimation of heat stress, conditions of thermal comfort, metabolic heat production,
and on auditory and visual danger signals, speech interference level and the
assessment of speech communication.
CEN TC 122 covers roughly the same fields of ergonomics standardization,
although with a different emphasis and a different structure of its working groups.
It is intended, however, that by a division of labour between the ergonomics
committees, and mutual acceptance of work results, a general and usable set of
ergonomics standards will be developed.

CHECKLISTS
Pranab Kumar Nag

Work systems encompass such macro level organizational variables as the


personnel subsystem, the technological subsystem and the external environment.
The analysis of work systems is, therefore, essentially an effort to understand the
allocation of functions between the worker and the technical outfit and the division
of labour between people in a sociotechnical environment. Such an analysis can
assist in making informed decisions to enhance systems safety, efficiency in work,
technological development and the mental and physical well-being of workers.

Researchers examine work systems according to divergent approaches


(mechanistic, biological, perceptual/motor, motivational) with corresponding
individual and organizational outcomes (Campion and Thayer 1985). The selection
of methods in work systems analysis is dictated by the specific approaches taken
and the particular objective in view, the organizational context, the job and human
characteristics, and the technological complexity of the system under study (Drury
1987). Checklists and questionnaires are the common means of assembling
databases for organizational planners in prioritizing action plans in areas of
personnel selection and placement, performance appraisal, safety and health
management, worker-machine design and work design or redesign. Inventory
methods of checklists, for example the Position Analysis Questionnaire, or PAQ
(McCormick 1979), the Job Components Inventory (Banks and Miller 1984), the
Job Diagnostic Survey (Hackman and Oldham 1975), and the Multi-method Job
Design Questionnaire (Campion 1988) are the more popular instruments, and are
directed to a variety of objectives.

The PAQ has six major divisions, comprising 189 behavioural items required for
the assessment of job performance and seven supplementary items related to
monetary compensation:

· information input (where and how does one get information on the jobs to
perform) (35 items)

· mental process (information processing and decision-making in performing the


job) (14 items)

· work output (physical work done, tools and devices used) (50 items)
· interpersonal relationships (36 items)

· work situation and job context (physical/social contexts) (18 items)

· other job characteristics (work schedules, job demands) (36 items).

The Job Components Inventory Mark II contains seven sections. The introductory
section deals with the details of the organization, job descriptions and biographical
details of the job holder. Other sections are as follows:

· tools and equipment—uses of over 200 tools and equipment (26 items)

· physical and perceptual requirements—strength, coordination, selective


attention (23 items)

· mathematical requirements—uses of numbers, trigonometry, practical


applications, e.g., work with plans and drawings (127 items)

· communication requirements—the preparation of letters, use of coding


systems, interviewing people (19 items)

· decision-making and responsibility—decisions about methods, order of work,


standards and related issues (10 items)

· job conditions and perceived job characteristics.

The profile methods have common elements, that is, (1) a comprehensive set of job
factors used to select the range of work, (2) a rating scale that permits the
evaluation of job demands, and (3) the weighing of job characteristics based on
organizational structure and sociotechnical requirements. Les profils des postes,
another task profile instrument, developed in the Renault Organization (RNUR
1976), contains a table of entries of variables representing working conditions, and
provides respondents with a five-point scale on which they can select the value of a
variable that ranges from very satisfactory to very poor by way of registering
standardized responses. The variables cover (1) the design of the workstation, (2)
the physical environment, (3) the physical load factors, (4) nervous tension, (5) job
autonomy, (6) relations, (7) repetitiveness and (8) contents of work.

The AET (Ergonomic Job Analysis) (Rohmert and Landau 1985), was developed
based on the stress-strain concept. Each of the 216 items of the AET are coded: one
code defines the stressors, indicating whether a work element does or does not
qualify as a stressor; other codes define the degree of stress associated with a job;
and yet others describe the duration and frequency of stress during the work shift.

The AET consists of three parts:


· Part A. The Man-at-Work system (143 items) includes the work objects, tools
and equipment, and work environment constituting the physical, organizational,
social and economic conditions of work.

· Part B. The Task analysis (31 items) classified according to both the different
kinds of work object, such as material and abstract objects, and worker-related
tasks.

· Part C. The Work Demand analysis (42 items) comprises the elements of
perception, decision and response/activity. (The AET supplement, H-AET, covers
body postures and movements in industrial assembling activities).

Broadly speaking, the checklists adopt one of two approaches, (1) the job-oriented
approach (e.g., the AET, Les profils des postes) and (2) the worker-oriented
approach (e.g., the PAQ). The task inventories and profiles offer subtle comparison
of complex tasks and occupational profiling of jobs and determine the aspects of
work which are considered a priori as inevitable factors in improving working
conditions. The emphasis of the PAQ is on classifying job families or clusters
(Fleishman and Quaintence 1984; Mossholder and Arvey 1984; Carter and
Biersner 1987), inferring job component validity and job stress (Jeanneret 1980;
Shaw and Riskind 1983). From the medical point of view, both the AET and the
profile methods allow comparisons of constraints and aptitudes when required
(Wagner 1985). The Nordic questionnaire is an illustrative presentation of
ergonomic workplace analysis (Ahonen, Launis and Kuorinka 1989), which covers
the following aspects:

· work space

· general physical activity

· lifting activity

· work postures and movements

· accident risk

· job content

· job restrictiveness

· worker’s communication and personal contacts

· decision-making

· repetitiveness of the work


· attentiveness

· lighting conditions

· thermal environment

· noise.

Among the shortcomings of the general-purpose checklist format employed in


ergonomic job analysis are the following:

· With some exceptions (e.g., the AET, and the Nordic questionnaire), there is a
general lack of ergonomics norms and protocols of evaluation with respect to the
different aspects of work and environment.

· There are dissimilarities in the overall construction of the checklists as regards


means of determining the characteristics of working conditions, the quotation
form, criteria and methods of testing.

· The evaluation of physical workload, work postures and work methods is


limited on account of lack of precision in the analysis of work operations, with
reference to the scale of relative levels of stress.

· The principal criteria of assessment of the worker’s mental load are the degree
of complexity of the task, the attention required by the task and the execution of
mental skills. The existing checklists refer less to underuse of abstract thought
mechanisms than to overuse of concrete thought mechanisms.

· In most checklists, methods of analysis attach major importance to the job as a


position as opposed to the analysis of work, worker-machine compatibility, and so
forth. The psycho-sociological determinants, which are fundamentally subjective
and contingent, are less emphasized in the ergonomics checklists.

A systematically constructed checklist obliges us to investigate the factors of work


conditions which are visible or easy to modify, and permits us to engage in a social
dialogue between employers, job holders and others concerned. One should
exercise a degree of caution towards the illusion of simplicity and efficiency of the
checklists, and towards their quantifying and technical approaches as well.
Versatility in a checklist or questionnaire can be achieved by including specific
modules to suit specific objectives. Therefore, the choice of variables is very much
linked to the purpose for which the work systems are to be analysed and this
determines the general approach for construction of a user-friendly checklist.

The suggested “Ergonomics Checklist” may be adopted for various applications.


Data collection and computerized processing of the checklist data are relatively
straightforward, by responding to the primary and secondary statements (q.v.). The
Summary Evaluation Sheet may be used for profiling and clustering of a selected
group of items, which may form the basis for decisions on work systems. The
process of analysis is often time-consuming and the users of these instruments
must have a sound training in ergonomics both theoretical and practical, in the
evaluation of work systems.

ERGONOMICS CHECKLIST
A broad guideline for a modular-structured work systems checklist is suggested here, covering five major
aspects (mechanistic, biological, perceptual/motor, technical and psychosocial). Weighting of the modules
varies with the nature of the job(s) to be analysed, the specific features of the country or population under
study, organizational priorities and the intended use of the results of the analysis. Respondents mark the
"primary statement" as Yes/No. "Yes" answers indicate the apparent absence of a problem, although the
advisability of further careful scrutiny should not be ruled out. "No" answers indicate a need for an ergonom
evaluation and improvement. Responses to "secondary statements" are indicated by a single digit on the
severity of agreement/disagreement scale illustrated below

0 Do not know or not applicable

1 Strongly disagree

2 Disagree

3 Neither agree nor disagree

4 Agree

5 Strongly agree

A. Organization, worker and the task Your


answers/rating

The checklist designer may provide a sample drawing/photograph of work and workplace under study

1. Description of organization and functions.

_______________________________________________________________________________________
_

_______________________________________________________________________________________
_

_______________________________________________________________________________________
_

_______________________________________________________________________________________
_

_______________________________________________________________________________________
_

2. Worker characteristics: A brief account of the work group.

_______________________________________________________________________________________
_

_______________________________________________________________________________________
_

_______________________________________________________________________________________
_

_______________________________________________________________________________________
_

_______________________________________________________________________________________
_

3. Task description: List activities and materials in use. Give some indication of the work hazards.

_______________________________________________________________________________________
_

_______________________________________________________________________________________
_

_______________________________________________________________________________________
_

_______________________________________________________________________________________
_

_______________________________________________________________________________________
_

B. Mechanistic aspect Your


answers/rating

I. Job Specialization

4. Tasks/work patterns are simple and uncomplicated. Yes/No

If No, rate the following: (Enter 0-5)

4.1 Job assignment is specific to the operative. O

4.2 Tools and methods of work are specialized to the purpose of the job. O

4.3 Production volume and quality of work. O


4.4 Job holder performs multiple tasks. O

II. Skill Requirement

5. Job requires simple motor act. Yes/No

If No, rate the following: (Enter 0-5)

5.1 Job requires knowledge and skilful ability. O

5.2 Job demands training for skill acquisition. O

5.3 Worker makes frequent mistakes at work. O

5.4 Job demands frequent rotation, as directed. O

5.5 Work operation is machine paced/assisted by automation. O

Remarks and suggestions for improvement. Items 4 to 5.5:

_______________________________________________________________________________________
_

_______________________________________________________________________________________
_

_______________________________________________________________________________________
_

_______________________________________________________________________________________
_

_______________________________________________________________________________________
_

O Analyst’s rating O Worker’s rating

C. Biological aspect Your


answers/rating

III. General Physical Activity

6. Physical activity is entirely determined and regulated by the worker. Yes/No

If No, rate the following: (Enter 0-5)

6.1 Worker maintains target-oriented pace. O

6.2 Job implies frequently repeated movements. O

6.3 Cardiorespiratory demand of the job: O

sedentary/light/moderate/heavy/ extremely heavy


(What are the heavy work elements?):

_______________________________________________________________________________________
_

_______________________________________________________________________________________
_

_______________________________________________________________________________________
_

_______________________________________________________________________________________
_

_______________________________________________________________________________________
_

(Enter 0-5)

6.4 Job demands high muscular strength exertion. O

6.5 Job (operation of handle, steering wheel, pedal brake) is predominantly O


static work.

6.6 Job requires fixed working position (sitting or standing). O

IV. Manual Materials Handling (MMH)

Nature of objects handled: animate/inanimate, size and shape.

_______________________________________________________________________________________
_

7. Job requires minimal MMH activity. Yes/No

If No, specify the work:

7.1 Mode of work: (circle one)

pull/push/turn/lift/lower/carry

(Specify repetition cycle):

_______________________________________________________________________________________
_

7.2 Load weight (kg): (circle one)

5-10, 10-20, 20-30, 30-40, >>40.

7.3 Subject-load horizontal distance (cm): (circle one)

<25, 25-40, 40-55, 55-70, >70.


7.4 Subject-load height: (circle one)

ground, knee, waist, chest, shoulder level.

(Enter 0-5)

7.5 Clothing restricts MMH tasks. O

8. Task situation is free from risk of bodily injury. Yes/No

If No, rate the following: (Enter 0-5)

8.1 Task can be modified to reduce the load to be handled. O

8.2 Materials can be packed in standard sizes. O

8.3 Size/position of handles on objects may be improved. O

8.4 Workers do not adopt safer methods of load handling. O

8.5 Mechanical aids may reduce bodily strains. O

List each item if hoists or other handling aids are available.

Suggestions for improvement, Items 6 to 8.5:

_______________________________________________________________________________________
_

_______________________________________________________________________________________
_

_______________________________________________________________________________________
_

_______________________________________________________________________________________
_

_______________________________________________________________________________________
_

V. Workplace/Workspace Design

Workplace may be diagrammatically illustrated, showing human reach and clearance:


9. Workplace is compatible with human dimensions. Yes/No

If No, rate the following: (Enter 0-5)

9.1 Work distance is away from normal reach in the horizontal or vertical plane O
(>60 cm).

9.2 Height of work desk/equipment is fixed or minimally adjustable. O

9.3 No space for subsidiary operations (e.g., inspection and maintenance). O

9.4 Workstations have obstacles, protruding parts or sharp edges. O

9.5 Work surface floors are slippery, uneven, cluttered or unstable. O

10. Seating arrangement is adequate (e.g., comfortable chair, good postural Yes/No
support).

If No, the causes are: (Enter 0-5)

10.1 Seat dimensions (e.g., seat height, back rest) mismatch with human O
dimensions.

10.2 Minimum adjustability of seat. O

10.3 Workseat provides no hold/support (e.g., by means of vertical edges/extra O


stiff covering) to work with the machinery.

10.4 Absence of vibration damping mechanism in the workseat. O

11. Sufficient auxiliary support is available for safety at the workplace. Yes/No

If No, mention the following: (Enter 0-5)

11.1 Non-availability of storage space for tools, personal articles. O

11.2 Doorways, entrance/exit routes, or corridors are restricted. O

11.3 Design mismatch of handles, ladders, staircases, handrails. O

11.4 Handholds and footholds demand awkward position of limbs. O

11.5 Supports are unrecognizable by their place, form or construction O

11.6 Limited use of gloves/footwear to work and operate equipment controls. O

Suggestions for improvement, Items 9 to 11.6:

_______________________________________________________________________________________
_

_______________________________________________________________________________________
_

_______________________________________________________________________________________
_

_______________________________________________________________________________________
_

_______________________________________________________________________________________
_

VI. Work Posture

12. Job allows a relaxed work posture. Yes/No

If No, rate the following: (Enter 0-5)

12.1 Working with arms above shoulder and/or away from the body. O

12.2 Hyperextension of wrist and demand of high strength. O

12.3 Neck/shoulder are not maintained at an angle of about 15°. O

12.4 Back bent and twisted. O

12.5 Hips and legs are not well supported in seated position. O

12.6 One-sided and unsymmetrical movement of the body. O

12.7 Mention reasons of forced posture: (1) machine location (2) seat design, (3)
equipment handling, (4) workplace/workspace

12.8 Specify OWAS code. (For a detailed description of the OWAS method
refer to Karhu et al. 1981.)

_______________________________________________________________________________________
_

Suggestions for improvement, Items 12 to 12.7:

_______________________________________________________________________________________
_

_______________________________________________________________________________________
_

_______________________________________________________________________________________
_

_______________________________________________________________________________________
_
_______________________________________________________________________________________
_

VII. Work Environment

(Give measurements where possible)

NOISE

[Identify noise sources, type and duration of exposure; refer to ILO 1984 code].

13. Noise level is below the maximum sound level recommended. Yes/No

(Use the following table.)


Rating Work requiring no Work requiring Work requiring
verbal communication verbal concentration
communication
1 under 60 dBA under 50 dBA under 45 dBA
2 60-70 dBA 50-60 dBA 45-55 dBA
3 70-80 dBA 60-70 dBA 55-65 dBA
4 80-90 dBA 70-80 dBA 65-75 dBA
5 over 90 dBA over 80 dBA over 75 dBA

Source: Ahonen et al. 1989.

Give your agreement/disagreement score (0-5)

14. Damaging noises are suppressed at the source. Yes/No

If No, rate countermeasures: (Enter 0-5)

14.1 No effective sound isolation present. O

14.2 Noise emergency measures are not taken (e.g., restriction of working time, O
use of personal ear defenders/protectors).

15. CLIMATE

Specify climatic condition.

Temperature ____

Humidity ____

Radiant Temperature ____

Draughts ____
16. Climate is comfortable. Yes/No

If No, rate the following: (Enter 0-5)

16.1 Temperature sensation (circle one):

cool/slightly cool/neutral/warm/very hot

16.2 Ventilation devices (e.g., fans, windows, air conditioners) are not adequate. O

16.3 Non-execution of regulatory measures on exposure limits (if available, O


please elaborate).

16.4 Workers do not wear heat protective/assistive clothing. O

16.5 Drinking fountains of cool water are not available nearby. O

17. LIGHTING

Workplace/machine(s) are sufficiently illuminated at all times. Yes/No

If No, rate the following: (Enter 0-5)

17.1 Illumination is sufficiently intense. O

17.2 Illumination of work area is adequately uniform. O

17.3 Flicker phenomena are minimal or absent. O

17.4 Shadow formation is nonproblematical. O

17.5 Annoying reflected glares are minimal or absent. O

17.6 Colour dynamics (visual accentuation, colour warmth) are adequate. O

18. DUST, SMOKE, TOXICANTS

Environment is free from excessive dust, fumes and toxic substances. Yes/No

If No, rate the following: (Enter 0-5)

18.1 Ineffective ventilation and exhaust systems to carryoff fumes, smoke and O
dirt.

18.2 Lack of protection measures against emergencyrelease and contact with O


dangerous/toxic substances.

List the chemical toxicants:

_______________________________________________________________________________________
_

18.3 Monitoring of the workplace for chemical toxicants is not regular. O

18.4 Non-availability of personal protective measures (e.g., gloves, shoes, mask, O


apron).

19. RADIATION

Workers are effectively protected against radiation exposure. Yes/No

If No, mention the exposures (Enter 0-5)

(see ISSA checklist, Ergonomics):

19.1 UV radiation (200 nm - 400 nm). O

19.2 IR radiation (780 nm - 100 µm). O

19.3 Radioactivity/x-ray radiation (<200 nm). O

19.4 Microwaves (1 mm - 1 m). O

19.5 5 Lasers (300 nm - 1.4 µm). O

19.6 Others (mention):

_______________________________________________________________________________________
_

_______________________________________________________________________________________
_

_______________________________________________________________________________________
_

_______________________________________________________________________________________
_

_______________________________________________________________________________________
_

20. VIBRATION

Machine can be operated without vibration transmission to the operator’s Yes/No


body.

If No, rate the following: (Enter 0-5)

20.1 Vibration is transmitted to the whole body via the feet. O

20.2 Vibration transmission occurs through the seat (e.g., mobile machines that O
are driven with operator seated).

20.3 Vibration is transmitted through the hand-arm system (e.g., power-driven O


handtools, machines driven when operator is walking).

20.4 Prolonged exposure to continuous/repetitive source of vibration. O


20.5 Vibration sources cannot be isolated or eliminated. O

20.6 Identify the sources of vibration.

Comments and suggestions, items 13 to 20:

_______________________________________________________________________________________
_

_______________________________________________________________________________________
_

_______________________________________________________________________________________
_

_______________________________________________________________________________________
_

_______________________________________________________________________________________
_

VIII. Work Time Schedule

Indicate work time: work hours/day/week/year, including seasonal work and shift system.

21. Pressure of work time is minimum. Yes/No

If No, rate the following: (Enter 0-5)

21.1 Job requires night work. O

21.2 Job involves overtime/extra work time. O

Specify average duration:

_______________________________________________________________________________________
_

21.3 Heavy tasks are unevenly distributed throughout the shift. O

21.4 People work at a predetermined pace/time limit. O

21.5 Fatigue allowances/work-rest patterns are not sufficientlyincorporated (use O


cardio-respiratory criteria on work severity).

Comments and suggestions, items 21 to 21.5:

_______________________________________________________________________________________
_

_______________________________________________________________________________________
_
_______________________________________________________________________________________
_

_______________________________________________________________________________________
_

_______________________________________________________________________________________
_

O Analyst’s rating O Worker’s rating

D. Perceptual/motor aspect Your


answers/rating

IX. Displays

22. Visual displays (gauges, meters, warning signals) are easy to read. Yes/No

If No, rate the difficulties: (Enter 0-5)

22.1 Insufficient lighting (refer to item No. 17). O

22.2 Awkward head/eye positioning for visual line. O

22.3 Display style of numerals/numerical progression creates confusion and O


causes reading errors.

22.4 Digital displays are not available for accurate reading. O

22.5 Large visual distance for reading precision. O

22.6 Displayed information is not easily understood. O

22.7 Displayed information changes before an action can be taken. O

23. Emergency signals/impulses are easily recognizable. Yes/No

If No, assess the reasons:

23.1 Signals (visual/auditory) do not conform to the work process. O

23.2 Flashing signals are out of visual field. O

23.3 Auditory display signals are not audible. O

24. Groupings of the display features are logical. Yes/No

If No, rate the following:

24.1 Displays are not distinguished by form, position, colour or tone. O

24.2 Frequently used and critical displays are removed from the central line of O
vision.
X. Controls

25. Controls (e.g., switches, knobs, cranes, driving wheels, pedals) are easy to Yes/No
handle.

If No, the causes are: (Enter 0-5)

25.1 Hand/foot control positions are awkward. O

25.2 Handedness of the controls/tools is incorrect. O

25.3 Dimensions of controls do not match the operating body part. O

25.4 Controls require high actuating force. O

25.5 Controls require high precision and speed. O

25.6 Controls are not shape-coded for good grip. O

25.7 Controls are not colour/symbol-coded for identification. O

25.8 Controls cause unpleasant feeling (warmth, cold, vibration). O

26. Displays and controls (combined) are compatible with easyand Yes/No
comfortable human reactions.

If No, rate the following: (Enter 0-5)

26.1 Placements are not sufficiently close to each other. O

26.2 Display/controls are not sequentially arranged for functions/frequency of O


use.

26.3 Display/control operations are successive, without enough time span to O


complete operation (this creates sensory overloading).

26.4 Disharmony in movement direction of display/control (e.g., leftward O


control movement does not give leftward unit movement).

Comments and suggestions, items 22 to 26.4:

_______________________________________________________________________________________
_

_______________________________________________________________________________________
_

_______________________________________________________________________________________
_

_______________________________________________________________________________________
_

_______________________________________________________________________________________
_

O Analyst’s rating O Worker’s rating

E. Technical aspect Your


answers/rating

XI. Machinery

27. Machine (e.g., conveyer trolley, lifting truck, machine tool) is easy to drive Yes/No
and work with.

If No, rate the following: (Enter 0-5)

27.1 Machine is unstable in operation. O

27.2 Poor maintenance of the machinery. O

27.3 Driving speed of the machine cannot be regulated. O

27.4 Steering wheels/handles are operated, from standing position. O

27.5 Operating mechanisms hamper body movements in the workspace. O

27.6 Risk of injury due to lack of machine guarding. O

27.7 Machinery is not equipped with warning signals. O

27.8 Machine is poorly equipped for vibration damping. O

27.9 Machine noise levels are above legal limits (refer to items No. 13 and 14). O

27.10 Poor visibility of machine parts and adjacent area (refer to items No. 17 O
and 22).

XII. Small Tools/Implements

28. Tools/implements provided to the operatives are comfortable to work with. Yes/No

If No, rate the following: (Enter 0-5)

28.1 Tool/implement has no carrying strap/back frame. O

28.2 Tool cannot be used with alternate hands. O

28.3 Heavy weight of the tool causes hyperextension of the wrist. O

28.4 Form and position of the handle are not designed for convenient grip. O

28.5 Power-driven tool is not designed for two-hand operation. O

28.6 Sharp edges/ridges of the tool/equipment can cause injury. O

28.7 Harnesses (gloves, etc.) are not regularly used in operating vibrating tool. O
28.8 Noise levels of power-driven tool are above acceptable limits (refer to item O
No. 13).

Suggestions for improvement, items 27 to 28.8:

_______________________________________________________________________________________
_

_______________________________________________________________________________________
_

_______________________________________________________________________________________
_

_______________________________________________________________________________________
_

_______________________________________________________________________________________
_

XIII. Work Safety

29. Machine safety measures are adequate to prevent accidents and health Yes/No
hazards.

If No, rate the following: (Enter 0-5)

29.1 Machine accessories cannot be fastened and removed easily. O

29.2 Dangerous points, moving parts and electrical installations are not O
adequately guarded.

29.3 Direct/indirect contact of body parts with machinery can cause hazards. O

29.4 Difficulty in inspection and maintenance of the machine. O

29.5 No clear instructions available for machine operation, maintenance and O


safety.

Suggestions for improvement, items 29 to 29. 5:

_______________________________________________________________________________________
_

_______________________________________________________________________________________
_

_______________________________________________________________________________________
_

_______________________________________________________________________________________
_

_______________________________________________________________________________________
_

O Analyst’s rating O Worker’s rating

F. Psychosocial aspect Your


answers/rating

XIV. Job Autonomy

30. Job allows autonomy (e.g., freedom regarding method of work, Yes/No
performance conditions, time schedule, quality control).

If No, the possible causes are: (Enter 0-5)

30.1 No discretion on the starting/finishing times of the job. O

30.2 No organizational support as regards calling for assistance at work. O

30.3 Insufficient number of people for the task (teamwork). O

30.4 Rigidity in work methods and conditions. O

XV. Job Feedback (Intrinsic and Extrinsic)

31. Job allows direct feedback of information as to the quality and quantity of Yes/No
one’s performance.

If No, the reasons are: (Enter 0-5)

31.1 No participative role in task information and decision making. O

31.2 Constraints of social contact due to physical barriers. O

31.3 Communication difficulty due to high noise level. O

31.4 Increased attentional demand in machine pacing. O

31.5 Other people (managers, co-workers) inform the worker as to his/her O


effectiveness of job performance.

XVI. Task Variety/Clarity

32. Job has a variety of tasks and calls for spontaneity on the part of the Yes/No
worker.

If No, rate the following: (Enter 0-5)

32.1 Job roles and goals are ambiguous. O

32.2 Job restrictiveness is imposed by a machine, process or work group. O


32.3 Worker-machine relation arouses conflict as to behaviour to be evinced by O
operator.

32.4 Restricted level of stimulation (e.g., unchanging visual and auditory O


environment).

32.5 High level of boredom on the job. O

32.6 Limited scope for job enlargement. O

XVII. Task Identity/Significance

33. Worker is given a batch of tasks and arranges his or her own schedule to Yes/No
complete the work (e.g., one plans and executes the job and inspects and
manages the products).

Give your agreement/disagreement score (0-5) O

34. Job is significant in the organization. It provides acknowledgement and Yes/No


recognition from others.

(Give your agreement/disagreement score) O

XVIII Mental Overload/Underload


.

35. Job consists of tasks for which clear communication and unambiguous Yes/No
information support systems are available.

If No, rate the following: (Enter 0-5)

35.1 Information supplied in connection with the job is extensive. O

35.2 Information handling under pressure is required (e.g., emergency O


manoeuvering in process control).

35.3 High information-handling workload (e.g., difficult positioning task-no O


special motivation required).

35.4 Occasional attention is directed to information other than that needed for O
the actual task.

35.5 Task consists of repetitive simple motor act, with superficial attention O
needed.

35.6 Tools/equipment are not pre-positioned to avoid mental delay. O

35.7 Multiple choices are required in decision making and judging risks. O

(Comments and suggestions, items 30 to 35.7)

_______________________________________________________________________________________
_
_______________________________________________________________________________________
_

_______________________________________________________________________________________
_

_______________________________________________________________________________________
_

_______________________________________________________________________________________
_

XIX. Training and Promotion

36. Job has opportunities for associated growth in competence and task Yes/No
accomplishment.

If No, the possible causes are: (Enter 0-5)

36.1 No opportunity for advancement to higher levels. O

36.2 No periodic training for operators, specific to jobs. O

36.3 Training programs/tools are not easy to learn and use. O

36.4 No incentive pay schemes. O

XX. Organizational Commitment

37. Defined commitment towards organizational effectiveness, and physical, Yes/No


mental and social well-being.

Assess the degree to which the following are made available: (Enter 0-5)

37.1 Organizational role in individual role conflicts and ambiguities. O

37.2 Medical/administrative services for preventive intervention in the case of O


work hazards.

37.3 Promotional measures to control absenteeism in work group. O

37.4 Effective safety regulations. O

37.5 Labour inspection and monitoring of better work practices. O

37.6 Follow-up action for accident/injury management. O

(Now go to the Summary Evaluation Sheet)

SUMMARY EVALUATION SHEET


A. Brief Description of Organization, Worker Characteristics and Task Description

__________________________________________________________________
______________

__________________________________________________________________
______________

__________________________________________________________________
______________

__________________________________________________________________
______________

__________________________________________________________________
______________

Severity Agreement
Modules Sections No. of 0 1 2 3 4 5 Relative
rated Items Severity
(%)
B. Mechanistic I. Job Specialization 4
II. Skill Requirement 5
C. Biological III. General Physical Activity 5
IV. Manual Materials 6
Handling
V. Workplace/Workplace 15
Design
VI. Work Posture 6
VII. Work Environment 28
VIII. Work Time Schedule 5
D. IX. Displays 12
Perceptual/motor
X. Controls 10
E. Technical XI. Machinery 10
XII. Small Tools/Implements 8
XIII. Work Safety 5
F. Psychosocial XIV. Job Autonomy 5
XV. Job Feedback 5
XVI. Task Variety/Clarity 6
XVII. Task 2
Identity/Significance
XVIII. Mental 7
Overload/Underload
XIX. Training and Promotion 4
XX. Organizational 6
Commitment

Overall Assessment

Severity Agreement of the Modules Remarks


A O
B O
C O
D O
E O
F O
Work Analyst:

ANTHROPOMETRY
Melchiorre Masali*

*This article is adapted from the 3rd edition of the Encyclopaedia of Occupational
Health and Safety.

Anthropometry is a fundamental branch of physical anthropology. It represents the


quantitative aspect. A wide system of theories and practice is devoted to defining
methods and variables to relate the aims in the different fields of application. In the
fields of occupational health, safety and ergonomics anthropometric systems are
mainly concerned with body build, composition and constitution, and with the
dimensions of the human body’s interrelation to workplace dimensions, machines,
the industrial environment and clothing.

Anthropometric variables

An anthropometric variable is a measurable characteristic of the body that can be


defined, standardized and referred to a unit of measurement. Linear variables are
generally defined by landmarks that can be precisely traced on the body.
Landmarks are generally of two types: skeletal-anatomical, which maybe found
and traced by feeling bony prominences through the skin, and virtual landmarks
that are simply found as maximum or minimum distances using the branches of a
caliper.
Anthropometric variables have both genetic and environmental components and
may be used to define individual and population variability. The choice of
variables must be related to the specific research purpose and standardized with
other research in the same field, as the number of variables described in the
literature is extremely large, up to 2,200 having been described for the human
body.

Anthropometric variables are mainly linear measures, such as heights, distances


from landmarks with subject standing or seated in standardized posture; diameters,
such as distances between bilateral landmarks; lengths, such as distances between
two different landmarks; curved measures, namely arcs, such as distances on the
body surface between two landmarks; and girths, such as closed all-around
measures on body surfaces, generally positioned at at least one landmark or at a
defined height.

Other variables may require special methods and instruments. For instance skinfold
thickness is measured by means of special constant pressure calipers. Volumes are
measured by calculation or by immersion in water. To obtain full information on
body surface characteristics, a computer matrix of surface points may be plotted
using biostereometric techniques.

Instruments

Although sophisticated anthropometric instruments have been described and used


with a view to automated data collection, basic anthropometric instruments are
quite simple and easy to use. Much care must be taken to avoid common errors
resulting from misinterpretation of landmarks and incorrect postures of subjects.

The standard anthropometric instrument is the anthropometer—a rigid rod 2 metres


long, with two counter-reading scales, with which vertical body dimensions, such
as heights of landmarks from floor or seat, and transverse dimensions, such as
diameters, can be taken.

Commonly the rod can be split into 3 or 4 sections which fit into one another. A
sliding branch with a straight or curved claw makes it possible to measure
distances from the floor for heights, or from a fixed branch for diameters. More
elaborate anthropometers have a single scale for heights and diameters to avoid
scale errors, or are fitted with digital mechanical or electronic reading devices
(figure 29.7).

Figure 29.7 An anthropometer


A stadiometer is a fixed anthropometer, generally used only for stature and
frequently associated with a weight beam scale.

For transverse diameters a series of calipers may be used: the pelvimeter for
measures up to 600 mm and the cephalometer up to 300 mm. The latter is
particularly suitable for head measurements when used together with a sliding
compass (figure 29.8).

Figure 29.8 Grip strength of pliers jaws exerted by male and female users as a function
of handle separation

The foot-board is used for measuring the feet and the head-board provides
cartesian co-ordinates of the head when oriented in the “Frankfort plane” (a
horizontal plane passing through porion and orbitale landmarks of the head).The
hand may be measured with a caliper, or with a special device composed of five
sliding rulers.

Skinfold thickness is measured with a constant-pressure skinfold caliper generally


with a pressure of 9.81 x 104 Pa (the pressure imposed by a weight of 10 g on an
area of 1 mm2).

For arcs and girths a narrow, flexible steel tape with flat section is used. Self-
straightening steel tapes must be avoided.

Systems of variables

A system of anthropometric variables is a coherent set of body measurements to


solve some specific problem.

In the field of ergonomics and safety the main problem is fitting equipment and
work space to humans and tailoring clothes to the right size.

Equipment and work space require mainly linear measures of limbs and body
segments that can easily be calculated from landmark heights and diameters,
whereas tailoring sizes are based mainly on arcs, girths and flexible tape lengths.
Both systems may be combined according to need.

In any case it is absolutely necessary to have a precise space reference for each
measurement. The landmarks must therefore be linked by heights and diameters
and every arc or girth must have a defined landmark reference. Heights and slopes
must be indicated.

In a particular survey the number of variables has to be limited to the minimum so


as to avoid undue stress on the subject and operator.

A basic set of variables for work space has been reduced to 33 measured variables
(figure 29.9) plus 20 derived by simple calculation. For a general purpose military
survey, Hertzberg and co-workers use 146 variables. For clothes and general
biological purposes the Italian Fashion Board (Ente Italiano della Moda) uses a set
of 32 general purpose variables and 28 technical ones. The German norm (DIN 61
516) of control body dimensions for clothes includes 12 variables. The
recommendation of the International Organization for Standardization (ISO) for
anthropometry includes a core list of 36 variables (see table 29.1). The
International Data on Anthropometry tables published by the ILO list 19 body
dimensions for the populations of 20 different regions of the world (Jürgens, Aune
and Pieper 1990).

Figure 29.9 Basic set of anthropometric variables


Table 29.1 Basic anthropometric core list

1.1 Forward reach (to hand grip with subject standing upright against a wall)

1.2 Stature (vertical distance from floor to head vertex)

1.3 Eye height (from floor to inner eye corner)

1.4 Shoulder height (from floor to acromion)

1.5 Elbow height (from floor to radial depression of elbow)

1.6 Crotch height (from floor to pubic bone)

1.7 Finger tip height (from floor to grip axis of fist)

1.8 Shoulder breadth (biacromial diameter)

1.9 Hip breadth, standing (the maximum distance across hips)

2.1 Sitting height (from seat to head vertex)

2.2 Eye height, sitting (from seat to inner corner of the eye)

2.3 Shoulder height, sitting (from seat to acromion)

2.4 Elbow height, sitting (from seat to lowest point of bent elbow)

2.5 Knee height (from foot-rest to the upper surface of thigh)


2.6 Lower leg length (height of sitting surface)

2.7 Forearm-hand length (from back of bent elbow to grip axis)

2.8 Body depth, sitting (seat depth)

2.9 Buttock-knee length (from knee-cap to rearmost point of buttock)

2.10 Elbow to elbow breadth (distance between lateral surface of the elbows)

2.11 Hip breadth, sitting (seat breadth)

3.1 Index finger breadth, proximal (at the joint between medial and proximal phalanges)

3.2 Index finger breadth, distal (at the joint between distal and medial phalanges)

3.3 Index finger length

3.4 Hand length (from tip of middle finger to styloid)

3.5 Hand breadth (at metacarpals)

3.6 Wrist circumference

4.1 Foot breadth

4.2 Foot length

5.1 Heat circumference (at glabella)

5.2 Sagittal arc (from glabella to inion)

5.3 Head length (from glabella to opisthocranion)

5.4 Head breadth (maximum above the ear)

5.5 Bitragion arc (over the head between the ears)

6.1 Waist circumference (at the umbilicus)

6.2 Tibial height (from the floor to the highest point on the antero-medial margin of the
glenoid of the tibia)

6.3 Cervical height sitting (to the tip of the spinous process of the 7th cervical vertebra).

Source: Adapted from ISO/DP 7250 1980).

Precision and errors


Precision of living body dimensions must be considered in a stochastic manner,
because the human body is highly unpredictable, both as a static and as a dynamic
structure.

A single individual may grow or change in muscularity and fatness; undergo


skeletal changes as a consequence of ageing, disease or accidents; or modify
behaviour or posture. Different subjects differ by proportions, not only by general
dimensions. Tall stature subjects are not mere enlargements of short ones;
constitutional types and somatotypes probably vary more than general dimensions.

The use of mannequins, particularly those representing the standard 5th, 50th and
95th percentiles for fitting trials may be highly misleading, if body variations in
body proportions are not taken into consideration.

Errors result from misinterpretation of landmarks and incorrect use of instruments


(personal error), imprecise or inexact instruments (instrumental error), or changes
in subject posture (subject error—this latter may be due to difficulties of
communication if the cultural or linguistic background of the subject differs from
that of the operator).

Statistical treatment

Anthropometric data must be treated by statistical procedures, mainly in the field


of inference methods applying univariate (mean, mode, percentiles, histograms,
variance analysis, etc.), bivariate (correlation, regression) and multivariate
(multiple correlation and regression, factor analysis, etc.) methods. Various
graphical methods based on statistical applications have been devised to classify
human types (anthropometrograms, morphosomatograms).

Sampling and survey

As anthropometric data cannot be collected for the whole population (except in the
rare case of a particularly small population), sampling is generally necessary. A
basically random sample should be the starting point of any anthropometric survey.
To keep the number of measured subjects to a reasonable level it is generally
necessary to have recourse to multiple-stage stratified sampling. This allows the
most homogeneous subdivision of the population into a number of classes or strata.

The population may be subdivided by sex, age group, geographical area, social
variables, physical activity and so on.

Survey forms have to be designed keeping in mind both measuring procedure and
data treatment. An accurate ergonomic study of the measuring procedure should be
made in order to reduce the operator’s fatigue and possible errors. For this reason
variables must be grouped according to the instrument used and ordered in
sequence so as to reduce the number of body flexions the operator has to make.
To reduce the effect of personal error, the survey should be carried out by one
operator. If more than one operator has to be used, training is necessary to assure
replicability of measurements.

Population anthropometrics

Disregarding the highly criticized concept of “race”, human populations are


nevertheless highly variable in size of individuals and in size distribution.
Generally human populations are not strictly Mendelian; they are commonly the
result of admixture. Sometimes two or more populations, with different origins and
adaptation, live together in the same area without interbreeding. This complicates
the theoretical distribution of traits. From the anthropometric viewpoint, sexes are
different populations. Populations of employees may not correspond exactly to the
biological population of the same area as a consequence of possible aptitudinal
selection or auto-selection due to job choice.

Populations from different areas may differ as a consequence of different


adaptation conditions or biological and genetic structures.

When close fitting is important a survey on a random sample is necessary.

Fitting trials and regulation

The adaptation of work space or equipment to the user may depend not only on the
bodily dimensions, but also on such variables as tolerance of discomfort and nature
of activities, clothing, tools and environmental conditions. A combination of a
check list of relevant factors, a simulator and a series of fitting trials using a sample
of subjects chosen to represent the range of body sizes of the expected user
population can be used.

The aim is to find tolerance ranges for all subjects. If the ranges overlap it is
possible to select a narrower final range which is not outside the tolerance limits of
any subject. If there is no overlap it will be necessary to make the structure
adjustable or to provide it in different sizes. If more than two dimensions are
adjustable a subject may not be able to decide which of the possible adjustments
will fit him best.

Adjustability can be a complicated matter, especially when uncomfortable postures


result in fatigue. Precise indications must therefore be given to the user who
frequently knows little or nothing about his own anthropometric characteristics. In
general an accurate design should reduce the need for adjustment to the minimum.
In any case it should constantly be kept in mind what is involved is
anthropometrics, not merely engineering.

Dynamic anthropometrics
Static anthropometrics may give wide information about movement if an adequate
set of variables has been chosen. Nevertheless when movements are complicated
and a close fit with the industrial environment is desirable, as in most user-machine
and human-vehicle interfaces, an exact survey of postures and movements is
necessary. This may be done with suitable mock-ups that allow tracing of reach
lines or by photography. In this case a camera fitted with telephoto lens and an
anthropometric rod, placed in the sagittal plane of the subject, allows standardized
photographs with little distortion of image. Small labels on subjects’ articulations
make exact tracing of movements possible.

Another way of studying movements is to formalize postural changes according to


a series of horizontal and vertical planes passing through the articulations. Again,
using computerized human models with computer-aided design (CAD) systems is
a feasible way to include dynamic anthropometrics in ergonomic workplace
design.

MUSCULAR WORK
Juhani Smolander and Veikko Louhevaara

Muscular Work in Occupational Activities

In industrialized countries around 20% of workers are still employed in jobs


requiring muscular effort (Rutenfranz et al. 1990). The number of conventional
heavy physical jobs has decreased, but, on the other hand, many jobs have become
more static, asymmetrical and stationary. In developing countries, muscular work
of all forms is still very common.

Muscular work in occupational activities can be roughly divided into four groups:
heavy dynamic muscle work, manual materials handling, static work and repetitive
work. Heavy dynamic work tasks are found in forestry, agriculture and the
construction industry, for example. Materials handling is common, for example, in
nursing, transportation and warehousing, while static loads exist in office work, the
electronics industry and in repair and maintenance tasks. Repetitive work tasks can
be found in the food and wood-processing industries, for example.

It is important to note that manual materials handling and repetitive work are
basically either dynamic or static muscular work, or a combination of these two.

Physiology of Muscular Work

Dynamic muscular work

In dynamic work, active skeletal muscles contract and relax rhythmically. The
blood flow to the muscles is increased to match metabolic needs. The increased
blood flow is achieved through increased pumping of the heart (cardiac output),
decreased blood flow to inactive areas, such as kidneys and liver, and increased
number of open blood vessels in the working musculature. Heart rate, blood
pressure, and oxygen extraction in the muscles increase linearly in relation to
working intensity. Also, pulmonary ventilation is heightened owing to deeper
breathing and increased breathing frequency. The purpose of activating the whole
cardio-respiratory system is to enhance oxygen delivery to the active muscles. The
level of oxygen consumption measured during heavy dynamic muscle work
indicates the intensity of the work. The maximum oxygen consumption (VO2max)
indicates the person’s maximum capacity for aerobic work. Oxygen consumption
values can be translated to energy expenditure (1 litre of oxygen consumption per
minute corresponds to approximately 5 kcal/min or 21 kJ/min).

In the case of dynamic work, when the active muscle mass is smaller (as in the
arms), maximum working capacity and peak oxygen consumption are smaller than
in dynamic work with large muscles. At the same external work output, dynamic
work with small muscles elicits higher cardio-respiratory responses (e.g., heart
rate, blood pressure) than work with large muscles (figure 29.10).

Figure 29.10 Static versus dynamic work

Static muscle work


In static work, muscle contraction does not produce visible movement, as, for
example, in a limb. Static work increases the pressure inside the muscle, which
together with the mechanical compression occludes blood circulation partially or
totally. The delivery of nutrients and oxygen to the muscle and the removal of
metabolic end-products from the muscle are hampered. Thus, in static work,
muscles become fatigued more easily than in dynamic work.

The most prominent circulatory feature of static work is a rise in blood pressure.
Heart rate and cardiac output do not change much. Above a certain intensity of
effort, blood pressure increases in direct relation to the intensity and the duration of
the effort. Furthermore, at the same relative intensity of effort, static work with
large muscle groups produces a greater blood pressure response than does work
with smaller muscles. (See figure 29.10 .)

In principle, the regulation of ventilation and circulation in static work is similar to


that in dynamic work, but the metabolic signals from the muscles are stronger, and
induce a different response pattern.

Consequences of Muscular Overload in Occupational Activities

The degree of physical strain a worker experiences in muscular work depends on


the size of the working muscle mass, the type of muscular contractions (static,
dynamic), the intensity of contractions, and individual characteristics.

When muscular workload does not exceed the worker’s physical capacities, the
body will adapt to the load and recovery is quick when the work is stopped. If the
muscular load is too high, fatigue will ensue, working capacity is reduced, and
recovery slows down. Peak loads or prolonged overload may result in organ
damage (in the form of occupational or work-related diseases). On the other hand,
muscular work of certain intensity, frequency, and duration may also result in
training effects, as, on the other hand, excessively low muscular demands may
cause detraining effects. These relationships are represented by the so-called
expanded stress-strain concept developed by Rohmert (1984) (figure 29.11).

Figure 29.11 The expanded stress-strain model modified from Rohmert (1984)
In general, there is little epidemiological evidence that muscular overload is a risk
factor for diseases. However, poor health, disability and subjective overload at
work converge in physically demanding jobs, especially with older workers.
Furthermore, many risk factors for work-related musculoskeletal diseases are
connected to different aspects of muscular workload, such as the exertion of
strength, poor working postures, lifting and sudden peak loads.

One of the aims of ergonomics has been to determine acceptable limits for
muscular workloads which could be applied for the prevention of fatigue and
disorders. Whereas the prevention of chronic effects is the focus of epidemiology,
work physiology deals mostly with short-term effects, that is, fatigue in work tasks
or during a work day.

Acceptable Workload in Heavy Dynamic Muscular Work

The assessment of acceptable workload in dynamic work tasks has traditionally


been based on measurements of oxygen consumption (or, correspondingly, energy
expenditure). Oxygen consumption can be measured with relative ease in the field
with portable devices (e.g., Douglas bag, Max Planck respirometer, Oxylog,
Cosmed), or it can be estimated from heart rate recordings, which can be made
reliably at the workplace, for example, with the SportTester device. The use of
heart rate in the estimation of oxygen consumption requires that it be individually
calibrated against measured oxygen consumption in a standard work mode in the
laboratory, i.e., the investigator must know the oxygen consumption of the
individual subject at a given heart rate. Heart rate recordings should be treated with
caution because they are also affected by such factors as physical fitness,
environmental temperature, psychological factors and size of active muscle mass.
Thus, heart rate measurements can lead to overestimates of oxygen consumption in
the same way that oxygen consumption values can give rise to underestimates of
global physiological strain by reflecting only energy requirements.

Relative aerobic strain (RAS) is defined as the fraction (expressed as a percentage)


of a worker’s oxygen consumption measured on the job relative to his or her
VO2max measured in the laboratory. If only heart rate measurements are available, a
close approximation to RAS can be made by calculating a value for percentage
heart rate range (% HR range) with the so-called Karvonen formula as in figure
29.12 .

Figure 29.12 Analysis of acceptable workloads

VO2max is usually measured on a bicycle ergometer or treadmill, for which the


mechanical efficiency is high (20-25%). When the active muscle mass is smaller or
the static component is higher, VO2max and mechanical efficiency will be smaller
than in the case of exercise with large muscle groups. For example, it has been
found that in the sorting of postal parcels the VO2max of workers was only 65% of
the maximum measured on a bicycle ergometer, and the mechanical efficiency of
the task was less than 1%. When guidelines are based on oxygen consumption, the
test mode in the maximal test should be as close as possible to the real task. This
goal, however, is difficult to achieve.

According to Åstrand’s (1960) classical study, RAS should not exceed 50% during
an eight-hour working day. In her experiments, at a 50% workload, body weight
decreased, heart rate did not reach steady state and subjective discomfort increased
during the day. She recommended a 50% RAS limit for both men and women.
Later on she found that construction workers spontaneously chose an average RAS
level of 40% (range 25-55%) during a working day. Several more recent studies
have indicated that the acceptable RAS is lower than 50%. Most authors
recommend 30-35% as an acceptable RAS level for the entire working day.

Originally, the acceptable RAS levels were developed for pure dynamic muscle
work, which rarely occurs in real working life. It may happen that acceptable RAS
levels are not exceeded, for example, in a lifting task, but the local load on the back
may greatly exceed acceptable levels. Despite its limitations, RAS determination
has been widely used in the assessment of physical strain in different jobs.

In addition to the measurement or estimation of oxygen consumption, other useful


physiological field methods are also available for the quantification of physical
stress or strain in heavy dynamic work. Observational techniques can be used in
the estimation of energy expenditure (e.g., with the aid of the Edholm scale)
(Edholm 1966). Rating of perceived exertion (RPE) indicates the subjective
accumulation of fatigue. New ambulatory blood pressure monitoring systems allow
more detailed analyses of circulatory responses.

Acceptable Workload in Manual Materials Handling

Manual materials handling includes such work tasks as lifting, carrying, pushing
and pulling of various external loads. Most of the research in this area has focused
on low back problems in lifting tasks, especially from the biomechanical point of
view.

A RAS level of 20-35% has been recommended for lifting tasks, when the task is
compared to an individual maximum oxygen consumption obtained from a bicycle
ergometer test.

Recommendations for a maximum permissible heart rate are either absolute or


related to the resting heart rate. The absolute values for men and women are 90-
112 beats per minute in continuous manual materials handling. These values are
about the same as the recommended values for the increase in heart rate above
resting levels, that is, 30 to 35 beats per minute. These recommendations are also
valid for heavy dynamic muscle work for young and healthy men and women.
However, as mentioned previously, heart rate data should be treated with caution,
because it is also affected by other factors than muscle work.
The guidelines for acceptable workload for manual materials handling based on
biomechanical analyses comprise several factors, such as weight of the load,
handling frequency, lifting height, distance of the load from the body and physical
characteristics of the person.

In one large-scale field study (Louhevaara, Hakola and Ollila 1990) it was found
that healthy male workers could handle postal parcels weighing 4 to 5 kilograms
during a shift without any signs of objective or subjective fatigue. Most of the
handling occurred below shoulder level, the average handling frequency was less
than 8 parcels per minute and the total number of parcels was less than 1,500 per
shift. The mean heart rate of the workers was 101 beats per minute and their mean
oxygen consumption 1.0 l/min, which corresponded to 31% RAS as related to
bicycle maximum.

Observations of working postures and use of force carried out for example
according to OWAS method (Karhu, Kansi and Kuorinka 1977), ratings of
perceived exertion and ambulatory blood pressure recordings are also suitable
methods for stress and strain assessments in manual materials handling.
Electromyography can be used to assess local strain responses, for example in arm
and back muscles.

Acceptable Workload for Static Muscular Work

Static muscular work is required chiefly in maintaining working postures. The


endurance time of static contraction is exponentially dependent on the relative
force of contraction. This means, for example, that when the static contraction
requires 20% of the maximum force, the endurance time is 5 to 7 minutes, and
when the relative force is 50%, the endurance time is about 1 minute.

Older studies indicated that no fatigue will be developed when the relative force is
below 15% of the maximum force. However, more recent studies have indicated
that the acceptable relative force is specific to the muscle or muscle group, and is 2
to 5% of the maximum static strength. These force limits are, however, difficult to
use in practical work situations because they require electromyographic recordings.

For the practitioner, fewer field methods are available for the quantification of
strain in static work. Some observational methods (e.g., the OWAS method) exist
to analyse the proportion of poor working postures, that is, postures deviating from
normal middle positions of the main joints. Blood pressure measurements and
ratings of perceived exertion may be useful, whereas heart rate is not so applicable.

Acceptable Workload in Repetitive Work

Repetitive work with small muscle groups resembles static muscle work from the
point of view of circulatory and metabolic responses. Typically, in repetitive work
muscles contract over 30 times per minute. When the relative force of contraction
exceeds 10% of the maximum force, endurance time and muscle force start to
decrease. However, there is wide individual variation in endurance times. For
example, the endurance time varies between two to fifty minutes when the muscle
contracts 90 to 110 times per minute at a relative force level of 10 to 20% (Laurig
1974).

It is very difficult to set any definitive criteria for repetitive work, because even
very light levels of work (as with the use of a microcomputer mouse) may cause
increases in intramuscular pressure, which may sometimes lead to swelling of
muscle fibres, pain and reduction in muscle strength.

Repetitive and static muscle work will cause fatigue and reduced work capacity at
very low relative force levels. Therefore, ergonomic interventions should aim to
minimize the number of repetitive movements and static contractions as far as
possible. Very few field methods are available for strain assessment in repetitive
work.

Prevention of Muscular Overload

Relatively little epidemiological evidence exists to show that muscular load is


harmful to health. However, work physiological and ergonomic studies indicate
that muscular overload results in fatigue (i.e., decrease in work capacity) and may
reduce productivity and quality of work.

The prevention of muscular overload may be directed to the work content, the
work environment and the worker. The load can be adjusted by technical means,
which focus on the work environment, tools, and/or the working methods. The
fastest way to regulate muscular workload is to increase the flexibility of working
time on an individual basis. This means designing work-rest regimens which take
into account the workload and the needs and capacities of the individual worker.

Static and repetitive muscular work should be kept at a minimum. Occasional


heavy dynamic work phases may be useful for the maintenance of endurance type
physical fitness. Probably, the most useful form of physical activity that can be
incorporated into a working day is brisk walking or stair climbing.

Prevention of muscular overload, however, is very difficult if a worker’s physical


fitness or working skills are poor. Appropriate training will improve working skills
and may reduce muscular loads at work. Also, regular physical exercise during
work or leisure time will increase the muscular and cardio-respiratory capacities of
the worker.

POSTURES AT WORK
Ilkka Kuorinka

A person’s posture at work—the mutual organization of the trunk, head and


extremities—can be analysed and understood from several points of view. Postures
aim at advancing the work; thus, they have a finality which influences their nature,
their time relation and their cost (physiological or otherwise) to the person in
question. There is a close interaction between the body’s physiological capacities
and characteristics and the requirement of the work.

Musculoskeletal load is a necessary element in body functions and indispensable in


well-being. From the standpoint of the design of the work, the question is to find
the optimal balance between the necessary and the excessive.

Postures have interested researchers and practitioners for at least the following
reasons:

1. A posture is the source of musculoskeletal load. Except for relaxed standing,


sitting and lying horizontally, muscles have to create forces to balance the posture
and/or control movements. In classical heavy tasks, for example in the construction
industry or in the manual handling of heavy materials, external forces, both
dynamic and static, add to the internal forces in the body, sometimes creating high
loads which may exceed the capacity of the tissues. (See figure 29.13 .) Even in
relaxed postures, when muscle work approaches zero, tendons and joints may be
loaded and show signs of fatigue. A job with low apparent loading—an example
being that of a microscopist—may become tedious and strenuous when it is carried
out over a long period of time.

Figure 29.13 Too high hand positions or forward bending are amont the most common
ways of creating “static” load
2. Posture is closely related to balance and stability. In fact, posture is controlled
by several neural reflexes where input from tactile sensations and visual cues from
the surroundings play an important role. Some postures, like reaching objects from
a distance, are inherently unstable. Loss of balance is a common immediate cause
of work accidents. Some work tasks are performed in an environment where
stability cannot always be guaranteed, for example, in the construction industry.

3. Posture is the basis of skilled movements and visual observation. Many tasks
require fine, skilled hand movements and close observation of the object of the
work. In such cases, posture becomes the platform of these actions. Attention is
directed to the task, and the postural elements are enlisted to support the tasks: the
posture becomes motionless, the mus-cular load increases and becomes more
static. A French research group showed in their classical study that immobility and
musculoskeletal load increased when the rate of work increased (Teiger, Laville
and Duraffourg 1974).

4. Posture is a source of information on the events taking place at work.


Observing posture may be intentional or unconscious. Skilful supervisors and
workers are known to use postural observations as indicators of the work process.
Often, observing postural information is not conscious. For example, on an oil
drilling derrick, postural cues have been used to communicate messages between
team members during different phases of a task. This takes place under conditions
where other means of communication are not possible.

Safety, Health and Working Postures

From a safety and health point of view, all the aspects of posture described above
may be important. However, postures as a source of musculoskeletal illnesses such
as low back diseases have attracted the most attention. Musculoskeletal problems
related to repetitive work are also connected to postures.

Low back pain (LBP) is a generic term for various low back diseases. It has many
causes and posture is one possible causal element. Epidemiological studies have
shown that physically heavy work is conducive to LBP and that postures are one
element in this process. There are several possible mechanisms which explain why
certain postures may cause LBP. Forward bending postures increase the load on
the spine and ligaments, which are especially vulnerable to loads in a twisted
posture. External loads, especially dynamic ones, such as those imposed by jerks
and slipping, may increase the loads on the back by a large factor.

From a safety and health standpoint, it is important to identify bad postures and
other postural elements as part of the safety and health analysis of work in general.

Recording and Measuring Working Postures

Postures can be recorded and measured objectively by the use of visual observation
or more or less sophisticated measuring techniques. They can also be recorded by
using self-rating schemes. Most methods consider posture as one of the elements in
a larger context, for example, as part of the job content—as do the AET and
Renault’s Les profils des postes (Landau and Rohmert 1981; RNUR 1976)—or as
a starting point for biomechanical calculations that also take into account other
components.

In spite of the advancements in measuring technology, visual observation remains,


under field conditions, the only practicable means of systematically recording
postures. However, the precision of such measurements remains low. In spite of
this, postural observations can be a rich source of information on work in general.

The following short list of measuring methods and techniques presents selected
examples:

1. Self-reporting questionnaires and diaries. Self-reporting questionnaires and


diaries are an economical means of collecting postural information. Self-reporting
is based on the perception of the subject and usually deviates greatly from
“objectively” observed postures, but may still convey important information about
the tediousness of the work.

2. Observation of postures. The observation of postures includes the purely


visual recording of the postures and their components as well as methods in which
an interview completes the information. Computer support is usually available for
these methods. Many methods are available for visual observations. The method
may simply contain a catalogue of actions, including postures of the trunk and
limbs (e.g., Keyserling 1986; Van der Beek, Van Gaalen and Frings-Dresen
1992) .The OWAS method proposes a structured scheme for the analysis, rating
and evaluation of trunk and limb postures designed for field conditions (Karhu,
Kansi and Kuorinka 1977). The recording and analysis method may contain
notation schemes, some of them quite detailed (as with the posture targeting
method, by Corlett and Bishop 1976), and they may provide a notation for the
position of many anatomical elements for each element of the task (Drury 1987).

3. Computer-aided postural analyses. Computers have aided postural analyses in


many ways. Portable computers and special programs allow easy recording and
fast analysis of postures. Persson and Kilbom (1983) have developed the program
VIRA for upper-limb study; Kerguelen (1986) has produced a complete recording
and analysis package for work tasks; Kivi and Mattila (1991) have designed a
computerized OWAS version for recording and analysis.

Video is usually an integral part of the recording and analysis process. The US
National Institute for Occupational Safety and Health (NIOSH) has presented
guidelines for using video methods in hazard analysis (NIOSH 1990).

Biomechanical and anthropometrical computer programs offer specialized tools for


analysing some postural elements in the work activity and in the laboratory (e.g.,
Chaffin 1969).

Factors Affecting Working Postures

Working postures serve a goal, a finality outside themselves. That is why they are
related to external working conditions. Postural analysis that does not take into
account the work environment and the task itself is of limited interest to
ergonomists.

The dimensional characteristics of the workplace largely define the postures (as in
the case of a sitting task), even for dynamic tasks (for example, the handling of
material in a confined space). The loads to be handled force the body into a certain
posture, as does the weight and nature of the working tool. Some tasks require that
body weight be used to support a tool or to apply force on the object of the work,
as shown, for example in figure 29.14 .

Figure 29.14 Ergonomic aspects of standing


When joints are stretched uncomfortably, pressure can cause considerable fatigue.
Standing on one leg can lead to a load pressure on the hip-joint which is equal to
two and a half times the weight of the body. A good example of how this can
occur is found in cases where the standing worker has to operate a badly
positioned foot pedal.

Individual differences, age and sex influence postures. In fact, it has been found
that a “typical” or “best” posture, for example in manual handling, is largely
fiction. For each individual and each working situation, there are a number of
alternative “best” postures from the standpoint of different criteria.

Job Aids and Supports for Working Postures

Belts, lumbar supports and orthotics have been recommended for tasks with a risk
of low back pain or upper-limb musculoskeletal injuries. It has been assumed that
these devices give support to muscles, for example, by controlling intra-abdominal
pressure or hand movements. They are also expected to limit the range of
movement of the elbow, wrist or fingers. There is no evidence that modifying
postural elements with these devices would help to avoid musculoskeletal
problems.

Postural supports in the workplace and on machinery, such as handles, supporting


pads for kneeling, and seating aids, may be useful in alleviating postural loads and
pain.
Safety and Health Regulations concerning Postural Elements

Postures or postural elements have not been subject to regulatory activities per se.
However, several documents either contain statements which have a bearing on
postures or include the issue of postures as an integral element of a regulation. A
complete picture of the existing regulatory material is not available. The following
references are presented as examples.

1. The International Labour Organization published a Recommendation in 1967


on maximum loads to be handled. Although the Recommendation does not
regulate postural elements as such, it has a significant bearing on postural strain.
The Recommendation is now outdated but has served an important purpose in
focusing attention on problems in manual material handling.

2. The NIOSH lifting guidelines (NIOSH 1981), as such, are not regulations
either, but they have attained that status. The guidelines derive weight limits for
loads using the location of the load—a postural element—as a basis.

3. In the International Organization for Standardization as well as in the


European Community, ergonomics standards and directives exist which contain
matter relating to postural elements (CEN 1990 and 1991).

BIOMECHANICS
Frank Darby

Aims and Principles

Biomechanics is a discipline that approaches the study of the body as though it


were solely a mechanical system: all parts of the body are likened to mechanical
structures and are studied as such. The following analogies may, for example, be
drawn:

· bones: levers, structural members

· flesh: volumes and masses

· joints: bearing surfaces and articulations

· joint linings: lubricants

· muscles: motors, springs

· nerves: feedback control mechanisms

· organs: power supplies


· tendons: ropes

· tissue: springs

· body cavities: balloons.

The main aim of biomechanics is to study the way the body produces force and
generates movement. The discipline relies primarily on anatomy, mathematics and
physics; related disciplines are anthropometry (the study of human body
measurements), work physiology and kinesiology (the study of the principles of
mechanics and anatomy in relation to human movement).

In considering the occupational health of the worker, biomechanics helps to


understand why some tasks cause injury and ill health. Some relevant types of
adverse health effect are muscle strain, joint problems, back problems and fatigue.

Back strains and sprains and more serious problems involving the intervertebral
discs are common examples of workplace injuries that can be avoided. These often
occur because of a sudden particular overload, but may also reflect the exertion of
excessive forces by the body over many years: problems may occur suddenly or
may take time to develop. An example of a problem that develops over time is
“seamstress’s finger”. A recent description describes the hands of a woman who,
after 28 years of work in a clothing factory, as well as sewing in her spare time,
developed hardened thickened skin and an inability to flex her fingers (Poole
1993). (Specifically, she suffered from a flexion deformity of the right index
finger, prominent Heberden’s nodes on the index finger and thumb of the right
hand, and a prominent callosity on the right middle finger due to constant friction
from the scissors.) X-ray films of her hands showed severe degenerative changes in
the outermost joints of her right index and middle fingers, with loss of joint space,
articular sclerosis (hardening of tissue), osteophytes (bony growths at the joint) and
bone cysts.

Inspection at the workplace showed that these problems were due to repeated
hyperextension (bending up) of the outermost finger joint. Mechanical overload
and restriction in blood flow (visible as a whitening of the finger) would be
maximal across these joints. These problems developed in response to repeated
muscle exertion in a site other than the muscle.

Biomechanics helps to suggest ways of designing tasks to avoid these types of


injuries or of improving poorly designed tasks. Remedies for these particular
problems are to redesign the scissors and to alter the sewing tasks to remove the
need for the actions performed.

Two important principles of biomechanics are:


1. Muscles come in pairs. Muscles can only contract, so for any joint there must
be one muscle (or muscle group) to move it one way and a corresponding muscle
(or muscle group) to move it in the opposite direction. Figure 29.15 illustrates the
point for the elbow joint.

Figure 29.15 Skeletal muscles occur in pairs in order to initiate or reverse a movement

2. Muscles contract most efficiently when the muscle pair is in relaxed balance.
The muscle acts most efficiently when it is in the midrange of the joint it flexes.
This is so for two reasons: first, if the muscle tries to contract when it is shortened,
it will pull against the elongated opposing muscle. Because the latter is stretched, it
will apply an elastic counterforce that the contracting muscle must
overcome. Figure 29.16 shows the way in which muscle force varies with muscle
length.

Figure 29.16 Muscle tension varies with muscle length


Second, if the muscle tries to contract at other than the midrange of the movement
of the joint, it will operate at a mechanical disadvantage. Figure 29.17 illustrates
the change in mechanical advantage for the elbow in three different positions.

Figure 29.17 Optimal positions for joint movement


An important criterion for work design follows from these principles: Work should
be arranged so that it occurs with the opposing muscles of each joint in relaxed
balance. For most joints, this means that the joint should be at about its midrange
of movement.

This rule also means that muscle tension will be at a minimum while a task is
performed. One example of the infringement of the rule is the overuse syndrome
(RSI, or repetitive strain injury) which affects the muscles of the top of the forearm
in keyboard operators who habitually operate with the wrist flexed up. Often this
habit is forced on the operator by the design of the keyboard and workstation.

Applications

The following are some examples illustrating the application of biomechanics.

The optimum diameter of tool handles

The diameter of a handle affects the force that the muscles of the hand can apply to
a tool. Research has shown that the optimum handle diameter depends on the use
to which the tool is put. For exerting thrust along the line of the handle, the best
diameter is one that allows the fingers and thumb to assume a slightly overlapping
grip. This is about 40 mm. To exert torque, a diameter of about 50-65 mm is
optimal. (Unfortunately, for both purposes most handles are smaller than these
values.)

The use of pliers

As a special case of a handle, the ability to exert force with pliers depends on the
handle separation, as shown in figure 29.18 .

Figure 29.18 Grip strength of pliers jaws exerted by male and female users as a
function of handle separation

Seated posture

Electromyography is a technique that can be used to measure muscle tension. In a


study of the tension in the erector spinae muscles (of the back) of seated subjects, it
was found that leaning back (with the backrest inclined) reduced the tension in
these muscles. The effect can be explained because the backrest takes more of the
weight of the upper body.

X-ray studies of subjects in a variety of postures showed that the position of


relaxed balance of the muscles that open and close the hip joint corresponds to a
hip angle of about 135°. This is close to the position (128°) naturally adopted by
this joint in weightless conditions (in space). In the seated posture, with an angle of
90° at the hip, the hamstring muscles that run over both the knee and hip joints
tend to pull the sacrum (the part of the vertebral column that connects with the
pelvis) into a vertical position. The effect is to remove the natural lordosis
(curvature) of the lumbar spine; chairs should have appropriate backrests to correct
for this effort.

Screwdriving

Why are screws inserted clockwise? The practice probably arose in unconscious
recognition that the muscles that rotate the right arm clockwise (most people are
right-handed) are larger (and therefore more powerful) that the muscles that rotate
it anticlockwise.

Note that left-handed people will be at a disadvantage when inserting screws by


hand. About 9% of the population are left-handed and will therefore require special
tools in some situations: scissors and can openers are two such examples.

A study of people using screwdrivers in an assembly task revealed a more subtle


relation between a particular movement and a particular health problem. It was
found that the greater the elbow angle (the straighter the arm), the more people had
inflammation at the elbow. The reason for this effect is that the muscle that rotates
the forearm (the biceps) also pulls the head of the radius (lower arm bone) onto the
capitulum (rounded head) of the humerus (upper arm bone). The increased force at
the higher elbow angle caused greater frictional force at the elbow, with
consequent heating of the joint, leading to the inflammation. At the higher angle,
the muscle also had to pull with greater force to effect the screwing action, so a
greater force was applied than would have been required with the elbow at about
90°. The solution was to move the task closer to the operators to reduce the elbow
angle to about 90°.

The cases above demonstrate that a proper understanding of anatomy is required


for the application of biomechanics in the workplace. Designers of tasks may need
to consult experts in functional anatomy to anticipate the types of problems
discussed. (The Pocket Ergonomist (Brown and Mitchell 1986) based on
electromyographical research, suggests many ways of reducing physical
discomfort at work.)

Manual Material Handling

The term manual handling includes lifting, lowering, pushing, pulling, carrying,
moving, holding and restraining, and encompasses a large part of the activities of
working life.

Biomechanics has obvious direct relevance to manual handling work, since


muscles must move to carry out tasks. The question is: how much physical work
can people be reasonably expected to do? The answer depends on the
circumstances; there are really three questions that need to be asked. Each one has
an answer that is based on scientifically researched criteria:
1. How much can be handled without damage to the body (in the form, for
example, of muscle strain, disc injury or joint problems)? This is called the
biomechanical criterion.

2. How much can be handled without overexerting the lungs (breathing hard to
the point of panting)? This is called the physiological criterion.

3. How much do people feel able to handle comfortably? This is called the
psychophysical criterion.

There is a need for these three different criteria because there are three broadly
different reactions that can occur to lifting tasks: if the work goes on all day, the
concern will be how the person feels about the task—the psychophysical criterion;
if the force to be applied is large, the concern would be that muscles and joints are
not overloaded to the point of damage—the biomechanical criterion; and if the rate
of work is too great, then it may well exceed the physiological criterion, or the
aerobic capacity of the person.

Many factors determine the extent of the load placed on the body by a manual
handling task. All of them suggest opportunities for control.

Posture and Movements

If the task requires a person to twist or reach forward with a load, the risk of injury
is greater. The workstation can often be redesigned to prevent these actions. More
back injuries occur when the lift begins at ground level compared to mid-thigh
level, and this suggests simple control measures. (This applies to high lifting as
well.)
The load.

The load itself may influence handling because of its weight and its location. Other
factors, such as its shape, its stability, its size and its slipperiness may all affect the
ease of a handling task.
Organization and environment.

The way work is organized, both physically and over time (temporally), also
influences handling. It is better to spread the burden of unloading a truck in a
delivery bay over several people for an hour rather than to ask one worker to spend
all day on the task. The environment influences handling—poor light, cluttered or
uneven floors and poor housekeeping may all cause a person to stumble.
Personal factors.
Personal handling skills, the age of the person and the clothing worn also can
influence handling requirements. Education for training and lifting are required
both to provide necessary information and to allow time for the development of the
physical skills of handling. Younger people are more at risk; on the other hand,
older people have less strength and less physiological capacity. Tight clothing can
increase the muscle force required in a task as people strain against the tight cloth;
classic examples are the nurse’s smock uniform and tight overalls when people do
work above their heads.

Recommended Weight Limits

The points mentioned above indicate that it is impossible to state a weight that will
be “safe” in all circumstances. (Weight limits have tended to vary from country to
country in an arbitrary manner. Indian dockers, for example, were once “allowed”
to lift 110 kg, while their counterparts in the former People’s Democratic Republic
of Germany were “limited” to 32 kg.) Weight limits have also tended to be too
great. The 55 kg suggested in many countries is now thought to be far too great on
the basis of recent scientific evidence. The National Institute for Occupational
Safety and Health (NIOSH) in the United States has adopted 23 kg as a load limit
in 1991 (Waters et al. 1993).

Each lifting task needs to be assessed on its own merits. A useful approach to
determining a weight limit for a lifting task is the equation developed by NIOSH:

RWL = LC × HM × VM × DM × AM × CM × FM

Where

RWL = recommended weight limit for the task in question

HM = the horizontal distance from the centre of gravity of the load to the midpoint
between the ankles (minimum 15 cm, maximum 80 cm)

VM = the vertical distance between the centre of gravity of the load and the floor
at the start of the lift (maximum 175 cm)

DM = the vertical travel of the lift (minimum 25 cm, maximum 200 cm)

AM = asymmetry factor–the angle the task deviates from straight out in front of
the body

CM = coupling multiplier–the ability to get a good grip on the item to be lifted,


which is found in a reference table

FM = frequency multipliers–the frequency of the lifting.


All variables of length in the equation are expressed in units of centimetres. It
should be noted that 23 kg is the maximum weight that NIOSH recommends for
lifting. This has been reduced from 40 kg after observation of many people doing
many lifting tasks has revealed that the average distance from the body of the start
of the lift is 25 cm, not the 15 cm assumed in an earlier version of the equation
(NIOSH 1981).
Lifting index.

By comparing the weight to be lifted in the task and the RWL, a lifting index (LI)
can be obtained according to the relationship:

LI=(weight to be handled)/RWL.

Therefore, a particularly valuable use of the NIOSH equation is the placing of


lifting tasks in order of severity, using the lifting index to set priorities for action.
(The equation has a number of limitations, however, that need to be understood for
its most effective application. See Waters et al. 1993).

Estimating Spinal Compression Imposed by the Task

Computer software is available to estimate the spinal compression produced by a


manual handling task. The 2D and 3D Static Strength Prediction Programs from
the University of Michigan (“Backsoft”) estimate spinal compression. The inputs
required to the program are:

· the posture in which the handling activity is performed

· the force exerted

· the direction of the force exertion

· the number of hands exerting the force

· the percentile of the population under study.

The 2D and 3D programs differ in that the 3D software allows computations


applying to postures in three dimensions. The program output gives spinal
compression data and lists the percentage of the population selected that would be
able to do the particular task without exceeding suggested limits for six joints:
ankle, knee, hip, first lumbar disc-sacrum, shoulder and elbow. This method also
has a number of limitations that need to be fully understood in order to derive
maximum value out of the program.

GENERAL FATIGUE
Étienne Grandjean*

*This article is adapted from the 3rd edition of the Encyclopaedia of Occupational
Health and Safety.

The two concepts of fatigue and rest are familiar to all from personal experience.
The word “fatigue” is used to denote very different conditions, all of which cause a
reduction in work capacity and resistance. The very varied use of the concept of
fatigue has resulted in an almost chaotic confusion and some clarification of
current ideas is necessary. For a long time, physiology has distinguished between
muscular fatigue and general fatigue. The former is an acute painful phenomenon
localized in the muscles: general fatigue is characterized by a sense of diminishing
willingness to work. This article is concerned only with general fatigue, which may
also be called “psychic fatigue” or “nervous fatigue” and the rest that it
necessitates.

General fatigue may be due to quite different causes, the most important of which
are shown in figure 29.19 . The effect is as if, during the course of the day, all the
various stresses experienced accumulate within the organism, gradually producing
a feeling of increasing fatigue. This feeling prompts the decision to stop work; its
effect is that of a physiological prelude to sleep.

Figure 29.19 Diagrammatic presentation of the cumulative effect of the everyday causes
of fatigue
Fatigue is a salutary sensation if one can lie down and rest. However, if one
disregards this feeling and forces oneself to continue working, the feeling of
fatigue increases until it becomes distressing and finally overwhelming. This daily
experience demonstrates clearly the biological significance of fatigue which plays
a part in sustaining life, similar to that played by other sensations such as, for
example, thirst, hunger, fear, etc.

Rest is represented in figure 29.19 as the emptying of a barrel. The phenomenon


of rest can take place normally if the organism remains undisturbed or if at least
one essential part of the body is not subjected to stress. This explains the decisive
part played on working days by all workbreaks, from the short pause during work
to the nightly sleep. The simile of the barrel illustrates how necessary it is for
normal living to reach a certain equilibrium between the total load borne by the
organism and the sum of the possibilities for rest.

Neurophysiological interpretation of fatigue


The progress of neurophysiology during the last few decades has greatly
contributed to a better understanding of the phenomena triggered off by fatigue in
the central nervous system.

The physiologist Hess was the first to observe that electrical stimulation of certain
of the diencephalic structures, and more especially of certain of the structures of
the medial nucleus of the of the thalamus, gradually produced an inhibiting effect
which showed itself in a deterioration in the capacity for reaction and in a tendency
to sleep. If stimulation was continued for a certain time, general relaxation was
followed by sleepiness and finally by sleep. It was later proved that, starting from
these structures, an active inhibition may extend to the cerebral cortex where all
conscious phenomena are centred. This is reflected not only in behaviour, but also
in the electrical activity of the cerebral cortex. Other experiments have also
succeeded in initiating inhibitions from other sub-cortical regions.

The conclusion which can be drawn from all these studies is that there are
structures located in the diencephalon and mesencephalon which represent an
effective inhibiting system and which trigger off fatigue with all its accompanying
phenomena.

Inhibition and activation

Numerous experiments performed on animals and humans have shown that the
general disposition of them both to reaction depends not only on this system of
inhibition, but essentially also on a system functioning in an antagonistic manner,
known as the reticular ascending system of activation. We know from experiments
that the reticular formation contains structures which control the degree of
wakefulness, and consequently the general dispositions to reaction. Nervous links
exist between these structures and the cerebral cortex where the activating
influences are exerted on the consciousness. Moreover, the activating system
receives stimulations from the sensory organs. Other nervous connections transmit
impulses from the cerebral cortex—the area of perception and thought—to the
activation system. On the basis of these neurophysiological concepts it can be
established that external stimuli as well as influences originating in the areas of
consciousness may, in passing through the activating system, stimulate a
disposition to reaction.

In addition, many other investigations make it possible to conclude that stimulation


of the activating system frequently spreads also from the vegetative centres, and
cause the organism to orient towards an expenditure of energy, towards work,
struggle, flight, etc. (ergotropic conversion of the internal organs). Conversely, it
appears that stimulation of the inhibiting system within the sphere of the vegetative
nervous system causes the organism to tend towards rest, reconstitution of its
reserves of energy, phenomena of assimilation (trophotropic conversion).
By synthesis of all these neurophysiological findings the following conception of
fatigue can be established: the state and feeling of fatigue are conditioned by the
functional reaction of the consciousness in the cerebral cortex, which is, in turn,
governed by two mutually antagonistic systems—the inhibiting system and the
activating system. Thus, the disposition of humans to work depends at each
moment on the degree of activation of the two systems: if the inhibiting system is
dominant, the organism will be in a state of fatigue; when the activating system is
dominant, it will exhibit an increased disposition to work.

This psychophysiological conception of fatigue makes it possible to understand


certain of its symptoms which are sometimes difficult to explain. Thus, for
example, a feeling of fatigue may disappear suddenly when some unexpected
outside event occurs or when emotional tension develops. It is clear in both these
cases that the activating system has been stimulated. Conversely, if the
surroundings are monotonous or work seems boring, the functioning of the
activating system is diminished and the inhibiting system becomes dominant. This
explains why fatigue appears in a monotonous situation without the organism
being subjected to any workload.

Figure 29.20 depicts diagrammatically the notion of the mutually antagonistic


systems of inhibition and activation.

Figure 29.20 Diagrammatic presentation of the control of disposition to work by means


of inhibiting and activating systems

Clinical fatigue
It is a matter of common experience that pronounced fatigue occurring day after
day will gradually produce a state of chronic fatigue. The feeling of fatigue is then
intensified and comes on not only in the evening after work but already during the
day, sometimes even before the start of work. A feeling of malaise, frequently of
an emotive nature, accompanies this state. The following symptoms are often
observed in persons suffering from fatigue: heightened psychic emotivity
(antisocial behaviour, incompatibility), tendency to depression (unmotivated
anxiety), and lack of energy with loss of initiative. These psychic effects are often
accompanied by an unspecific malaise and manifest themselves by psychosomatic
symptoms: headaches, vertigo, cardiac and respiratory functional disturbances, loss
of appetite, digestive disorders, insomnia, etc.

In view of the tendency towards morbid symptoms which accompany chronic


fatigue, it may justly be called clinical fatigue. There is a tendency towards
increased absenteeism, and particularly to more absences for short periods. This
would appear to be caused both by the need for rest and by increased morbidity.
The state of chronic fatigue occurs particularly among persons exposed to psychic
conflicts or difficulties. It is sometimes very difficult to distinguish the external
and internal causes. In fact it is almost impossible to distinguish cause and effect in
clinical fatigue: a negative attitude towards work, superiors or workplace may just
as well be the cause of clinical fatigue as the result.

Research has shown that the switchboard operators and supervisory personnel
employed in telecommunications services exhibited a significant increase in
physiological symptoms of fatigue after their work (visual reaction time, flicker
fusion frequency, dexterity tests). Medical investigations revealed that in these two
groups of workers there was a significant increase in neurotic conditions,
irritability, difficulty in sleeping and in the chronic feeling of lassitude, by
comparison with a similar group of women employed in the technical branches of
the postal, telephone and telegraphic services. The accumulation of symptoms was
not always due to a negative attitude on the part of the women affected towards
their job or their working conditions.

Preventive Measures

These is no panacea for fatigue but much can be done to alleviate the problem by
attention to general working conditions and the physical environment at the
workplace. For example much can be achieved by the correct arrangement of hours
of work, provision of adequate rest periods and suitable canteens and rest rooms;
adequate paid holidays should also be given to workers. The ergonomic study of
the workplace can also help in the reduction of fatigue by ensuring that seats,
tables and work benches are of suitable dimensions and that the work flow is
correctly organized. In addition, noise control, air-conditioning, heating,
ventilation and lighting may all have a beneficial effect on delaying the onset of
fatigue in workers.
Monotony and tension may also be alleviated by a controlled use of colour and
decoration in the surroundings, intervals of music and sometimes breaks for
physical exercises for sedentary workers. Training of workers and in particular of
supervisory and management staff also play an important part.

FATIGUE AND RECOVERY


Rolf Helbig and Walter Rohmert

Fatigue and recovery are periodic processes in every living organism. Fatigue can
be described as a state which is characterized by a feeling of tiredness combined
with a reduction or undesired variation in the performance of the activity (Rohmert
1973).

Not all the functions of the human organism become tired as a result of use. Even
when asleep, for example, we breathe and our heart is pumping without pause.
Obviously, the basic functions of breathing and heart activity are possible
throughout life without fatigue and without pauses for recovery.

On the other hand, we find after fairly prolonged heavy work that there is a
reduction in capacity—which we call fatigue. This does not apply to muscular
activity alone. The sensory organs or the nerve centres also become tired. It is,
however, the aim of every cell to balance out the capacity lost by its activity, a
process which we call recovery.

Stress, Strain, Fatigue and Recovery

The concepts of fatigue and recovery at human work is closely related to the
ergonomic concepts of stress and strain (Rohmert 1984) (figure 29.21).

Figure 29.21 Stress, strain and fatigue


Stress means the sum of all parameters of work in the working system influencing
people at work, which are perceived or sensed mainly over the receptor system or
which put demands on the effector system. The parameters of stress result from the
work task (muscular work, non-muscular work—task-oriented dimensions and
factors) and from the physical, chemical and social conditions under which the
work has to be done (noise, climate, illumination, vibration, shift work, etc.—
situation-oriented dimensions and factors).

The intensity/difficulty, the duration and the composition (i.e., the simultaneous
and successive distribution of these specific demands) of the stress factors results
in an combined stress, which all the exogenous effects of a working system exert
on the working person. This combined stress can be actively coped with or
passively put up with, specifically depending on the behaviour of the working
person. The active case will involve activities directed towards the efficiency of the
working system, while the passive case will induce reactions (voluntary or
involuntary), which are mainly concerned with minimizing stress. The relation
between the stress and activity is decisively influenced by the individual
characteristics and needs of the working person. The main factors of influence are
those that determine performance and are related to motivation and concentration,
and those related to disposition, which can be referred to as abilities and skills.

The stresses relevant to behaviour, which are manifest in certain activities, cause
individually different strains. The strains can be indicated by the reaction of
physiological or biochemical indicators (e.g., raising the heart rate) or it can be
perceived. Thus, the strains are susceptible to “psycho-physical scaling”, which
estimates the strain as experienced by the working person. In a behavioural
approach, the existence of strain can also be derived from an activity analysis. The
intensity with which indicators of strain (physiological-biochemical, behaviouristic
or psycho-physical) react depends on the intensity, duration and combination of
stress factors as well as on the individual characteristics, abilities, skills and needs
of the working person.

Despite constant stresses the indicators derived from the fields of activity,
performance and strain may vary over time (temporal effect). Such temporal
variations are to be interpreted as processes of adaptation by the organic systems.
The positive effects cause a reduction of strain/improvement of activity or
performance (e.g., through training). In the negative case, however, they will result
in increased strain/reduced activity or performance (e.g., fatigue, monotony).

The positive effects may come into action if the available abilities and skills are
improved in the working process itself, e.g., when the threshold of training
stimulation is slightly exceeded. The negative effects are likely to appear if so-
called endurance limits (Rohmert 1984) are exceeded in the course of the working
process. This fatigue leads to a reduction of physiological and psychological
functions, which can be compensated by recovery.
To restore the original performance rest allowances or at least periods with less
stress are necessary (Luczak 1993).

When the process of adaptation is carried beyond defined thresholds, the employed
organic system may be damaged so as to cause a partial or total deficiency of its
functions. An irreversible reduction of functions may appear when stress is far too
high (acute damage) or when recovery is impossible for a longer time (chronic
damage). A typical example of such damage is noise-induced hearing loss.

Models of Fatigue

Fatigue can be many-sided, depending on the form and combi-nation of strain, and
a general definition of it is yet not possible. The biological proceedings of fatigue
are in general not measurable in a direct way, so that the definitions are mainly
oriented towards the fatigue symptoms. These fatigue symptoms can be divided,
for example, into the following three categories.

1. Physiological symptoms: fatigue is interpreted as a decrease of functions of


organs or of the whole organism. It results in physiological reactions, e.g., in an
increase of heart rate frequency or electrical muscle activity (Laurig 1970).

2. Behavioural symptoms: fatigue is interpreted mainly as a decrease of


performance parameters. Examples are increasing errors when solving certain
tasks, or an increasing variability of performance.

3. Psycho-physical symptoms: fatigue is interpreted as an increase of the feeling


of exertion and deterioration of sensation, depending on the intensity, duration and
composition of stress factors.

In the process of fatigue all three of these symptoms may play a role, but they may
appear at different points in time.

Physiological reactions in organic systems, particularly those involved in the work,


may appear first. Later on the feelings of exertion may be affected. Changes in
performance are manifested generally in a decreasing regularity of work or in an
increasing quantity of errors, although the mean of the performance may not yet be
affected. On the contrary, with appropriate motivation the working person may
even try to maintain performance through will-power. The next step may be a clear
reduction of performance ending with a breakdown of performance. The
physiological symptoms may lead to a breakdown of the organism including
changes of the structure of personality and in exhaustion. The process of fatigue is
explained in the theory of successive destabilization (Luczak 1983).

The principal trend of fatigue and recovery is shown in figure 29.22 .


Figure 29.22 Principal trend of fatigue and recovery

Prognosis of Fatigue and Recovery

In the field of ergonomics there is a special interest in predicting fatigue dependent


on the intensity, duration and composition of stress factors and to determine the
necessary recovery time. Table 29.2 shows those different activity levels and
consideration periods and possible reasons of fatigue and different possibilities of
recovery.

Table 29.2 Fatigue and recovery dependent on activity levels

Level of activity Period Fatigue from Recovery by


Work life Decades Overexertion for Retirement
decades
Phases of work life Years Overexertion for years Holidays
Sequences of work Months/weeks Unfavourable shift Weekend, free days
shifts regimes
One work shift One day Stress above Free time, rest periods
endurance limits
Tasks Hours Stress above Rest period
endurance limits
Part of a task Minutes Stress above Change of stress
endurance limits factors

In ergonomic analysis of stress and fatigue for determining the necessary recovery
time, considering the period of one working day is the most important. The
methods of such analyses start with the determination of the different stress factors
as a function of time (Laurig 1992) (figure 29.23).

Figure 29.23 Stress as a function of time

The stress factors are determined from the specific work content and from the
conditions of work. Work content could be the production of force (e.g., when
handling loads), the coordination of motor and sensory functions (e.g., when
assembling or crane operating), the conversion of information into reaction (e.g.,
when controlling), the transformations from input to output information (e.g., when
programming, translating) and the production of information (e.g., when designing,
problem solving). The conditions of work include physical (e.g., noise, vibration,
heat), chemical (chemical agents) and social (e.g., colleagues, shift work) aspects.

In the easiest case there will be a single important stress factor while the others can
be neglected. In those cases, especially when the stress factors results from
muscular work, it is often possible to calculate the necessary rest allowances,
because the basic concepts are known.

For example, the sufficient rest allowance in static muscle work depends on the
force and duration of muscular contraction as in an exponential function linked by
multiplication according to the formula:

with
R.A. = Rest allowance in percentage of t

t = duration of contraction (working period) in minutes

T = maximal possible duration of contraction in minutes

f = the force needed for the static force and

F = maximal force.

The connection between force, holding time and rest allowances is shown in figure
29.24 .

Figure 29.24 Percentage rest allowances for various combinations of holding forces and
time

Similar laws exist for heavy dynamic muscular work (Rohmert 1962), active light
muscular work (Laurig 1974) or different industrial muscular work (Schmidtke
1971). More rarely you find comparable laws for non-physical work, e.g., for
computing (Schmidtke 1965). An overview of existing methods for determining
rest allowances for mainly isolated muscle and non-muscle work is given by
Laurig (1981) and Luczak (1982).

More difficult is the situation where a combination of different stress factors exists,
as shown in figure 29.25 , which affect the working person simultaneously (Laurig
1992).
Figure 29.25 The combination of two stress factors

The combination of two stress factors, for example, can lead to different strain
reactions depending on the laws of combination. The combined effect of different
stress factors can be indifferent, compensatory or cumulative.
In the case of indifferent combination laws, the different stress factors have an
effect on different subsystems of the organism. Each of these subsystems can
compensate for the strain without the strain being fed into a common subsystem.
The overall strain depends on the highest stress factor, and thus laws of
superposition are not needed.

A compensatory effect is given when the combination of different stress factors


leads to a lower strain than does each stress factor alone. The combination of
muscular work and low temperatures can reduce the overall strain, because low
temperatures allow the body to lose heat which is produced by the muscular work.

A cumulative effect arises if several stress factors are superimposed, that is, they
must pass through one physiological “bottleneck”. An example is the combination
of muscular work and heat stress. Both stress factors affect the circulatory system
as a common bottleneck with resultant cumulative strain.

Possible combination effects between muscle work and physical conditions are
described in Bruder (1993) (see table 29.3).

Table 29.3 Rules of combination effects of two stress factors on strain

Cold Vibration Illumination Noise


Heavy dynamic work – + 0 0
Active light muscle work + + 0 0
Static muscle work + + 0 0

0 indifferent effect; + cumulative effect; – compensatory effect.

Source: Adapted from Bruder 1993.

For the case of the combination of more than two stress factors, which is the
normal situation in practice, only limited scientific knowledge is available. The
same applies for the successive combination of stress factors, (i.e., the strain effect
of different stress factors which affect the worker successively). For such cases, in
practice, the necessary recovery time is determined by measuring physiological or
psychological parameters and using them as integrating values.

MENTAL WORKLOAD
Winfried Hacker

Mental Versus Physical Workload


The concept of mental workload (MWL) has become increasingly important since
modern semi-automated and computerized technologies may impose severe
requirements on human mental or information-processing capabilities within both
manufacturing and administrative tasks. Thus, especially for the domains of job
analysis, evaluation of job requirements and job design, the conceptualization of
mental workload has become even more important than that of traditional physical
workload.

Definitions of Mental Workload

There is no agreed-upon definition of mental workload. The main reason is that


there are at least two theoretically well-based approaches and definitions: (1)
MWL as viewed in terms of the task requirements as an independent, external
variable with which the working subjects have to cope more or less efficiently, and
(2) MWL as defined in terms of an interaction between task requirements and
human capabilities or resources (Hancock and Chignell 1986; Welford 1986;
Wieland-Eckelmann 1992).

Although arising from different contexts, both approaches offer necessary and
well-founded contributions to different problems.

The requirements resources interaction approach was developed within the context
of personality-environment fit/misfit theories which try to explain interindividually
differing responses to identical physical and psychosocial conditions and
requirements. Thus, this approach may explain individual differences in the
patterns of subjective responses to loading requirements and conditions, for
example, in terms of fatigue, monotony, affective aversion, burnout or diseases
(Gopher and Donchin 1986; Hancock and Meshkati 1988).

The task requirements approach was developed within those parts of occupational
psychology and ergonomics which are predominantly engaged in task design,
especially in the design of new and untried future tasks, or so-called prospective
task design. The background here is the stress-strain concept. Task requirements
constitute the stress and the working subjects try to adapt to or to cope with the
demands much as they would to other forms of stress (Hancock and Chignell
1986). This task requirements approach tries to answer the question of how to
design tasks in advance in order to optimize their later impact on the—often still
unknown—employees who will accomplish these future tasks.

There are at least a few common characteristics of both conceptualizations of


MWL.

1. MWL mainly describes the input aspects of tasks, that is to say, the
requirements and demands made by the tasks on the employees, which might be
used in forecasting the task outcome.
2. The mental aspects of MWL are conceptualized in terms of information
processing. Information processing includes cognitive as well as
motivational/volitional and emotional aspects, since the persons always will
evaluate the demands which they have to cope with and, thus, will self-regulate
their effort for processing.

3. Information-processing integrates mental processes, representations (for


example, knowledge, or mental models of a machine) and states (for example,
states of consciousness, degrees of activation and, less formally, mood).

4. MWL is a multidimensional characteristic of task requirements, since every


task varies in a couple of interrelated but nevertheless distinct dimensions which
separately must be dealt with in task design.

5. MWL will have a multidimensional impact which at least will determine (a)
behaviour, for example, the strategies and the resulting performance, (b) perceived,
subjective short-term well-being with consequences for health in the long run, and
(c) psycho-physiological processes, for example, alterations of blood pressure at
work, which may become long-term effects of a positive kind (promoting, say,
fitness improvement) or of a negative kind (involving impairments or ill-health).

6. From the point of view of task design, MWL should not be minimized—as
would be necessary in the case of carcinogenic air pollution—but optimized. The
reason is that demanding mental task requirements are inevitable for well-being,
health promotion and qualification since they offer the necessary activating
impulses, fitness prerequisites and learning/training options. Missing demands on
the contrary may result in deactivation, loss of physical fitness, de-qualification
and deterioration of so-called intrinsic (task content-dependent) motivation.
Findings in this area led to the technique of health and personality promoting task
design (Hacker 1986).

7. MWL therefore, in any case, must be dealt with in task analysis, task
requirement evaluation as well as in corrective and prospective task design.

Theoretical Approaches: Requirement-Resources Approaches

From the person-environment fit point of view, MWL and its consequences may be
roughly categorized—as is shown in figure 29.26—into underload, properly fitting
load, and overload. This categorization results from the relationships between task
requirements and mental capabilities or resources. Task requirements may exceed,
fit with or fail to be satisfied by the resources. Both types of misfit may result from
quantitative or qualitative modes of misfit and will have qualitatively differing, but
in any case negative, consequences (see figure 29.26).

Figure 29.26 Types and consequences of requirements-resources relationships


Some theories attempt to define MWL starting from the resource or capacity side
of the requirements, namely, resources relationships. These resource theories might
be subdivided into resource volume and resource allocation theories (Wieland-
Eckelmann 1992). The amount of available capacity may come from a single
source (single resource theories) which determines processing. The availability of
this resource varies with arousal (Kahneman 1973). Modern multiple resource
theories suppose a set of relatively independent processing resources. Thus,
performance will depend on the condition whether the same resource or different
resources are required simultaneously and concurrently. Different resources are,
for example, encoding, processing or responding resources (Gopher and Donchin
1986; Welford 1986). The most critical problem for these types of theories is the
reliable identification of one or more well-defined capacities for qualitatively
different processing operations.

Resource allocation theories suppose qualitatively changing processing as a


function of varying strategies. Depending on the strategies, differing mental
processes and representations may be applied for task accomplishment. Thus, not
the volume of stable resources but flexible allocation strategies become the key
point of interest. Again, however, essential questions—especially concerning the
methods of diagnosis of the strategies—remain to be answered.

Assessment of MWL: using requirement-resource approaches

A strict measurement of MWL at present would be impossible since well-defined


units of measurement are lacking. But, to be sure, the conceptualization and the
instruments for an assessment should meet the general quality criteria of diagnostic
approaches, which have objectivity, reliability, validity and usefulness. However,
as of now, only a little is known about the overall quality of proposed techniques
or instruments.

There are a sizeable number of reasons for the remaining difficulties with assessing
MWL according to the requirement-resource approaches (O’Donnell and
Eggemeier 1986). An attempt at MWL assessment has to cope with questions like
the following: is the task self-intended, following self-set goals, or is it directed
with reference to an externally defined order? Which type of capacities (conscious
intellectual processing, application of tacit knowledge, etc.) are required, and are
they called upon simultaneously or sequentially? Are there different strategies
available and, if so, which ones? Which coping mechanisms of a working person
might be required?

The most often discussed approaches try to assess MWL in terms of:

1. required effort (effort assessment) approaches applying—in some versions


psychophysiologically validated—scaling procedures such as those offered by
Bartenwerfer (1970) or Eilers, Nachreiner and Hänicke (1986), or

2. occupied or, vice versa, residual mental capacity (mental capacity assessment)
approaches applying the traditional dual task techniques as, for example, discussed
by O’Donnell and Eggemeier (1986).

Both approaches are heavily dependent on the assumptions of single resource


theories and consequently have to struggle with the above-mentioned questions.

Effort assessment. Such effort assessment techniques as, for example, the scaling
procedure applied to a perceived correlate of the general central activation,
developed and validated by Bartenwerfer (1970), offer verbal scales which may be
completed by graphic ones and which grade the unidimensionally varying part of
the perceived required effort during task accomplishment. The subjects are
requested to describe their perceived effort by means of one of the steps of the
scale provided.

The quality criteria mentioned above are met by this technique. Its limitations
include the unidimensionality of the scale, covering an essential but questionable
part of perceived effort; the limited or absent possibility of forecasting perceived
personal task outcomes, for example, in terms of fatigue, boredom or anxiety; and
especially the highly abstract or formal character of effort which will identify and
explain nearly nothing of the content-dependent aspects of MWL as, for example,
any possible useful applications of the qualification or the learning options.

Mental capacity assessment. The mental capacity assessment consists of the dual
task techniques and a related data interpretation procedure, called the performance
operating characteristic (POC). Dual task techniques cover several procedures.
Their common feature is that subjects are requested to perform two tasks
simultaneously. The crucial hypothesis is: the less an additional or secondary task
in the dual task situation will deteriorate in comparison with the base-line single
task situation, the lower the mental capacity requirements of the primary task, and
vice versa. The approach is now broadened and various versions of task
interference under dual task conditions are investigated. For example, the subjects
are instructed to perform two tasks concurrently with graded variations of the
priorities of the tasks. The POC curve graphically illustrates the effects of possible
dual-task combinations arising from sharing limited resources among the
concurrently performed tasks.

The critical assumptions of the approach mainly consist in the suggestions that
every task will require a certain share of a stable, limited conscious (versus
unconscious, automated, implicit or tacit) processing capacity, in the hypothetical
additive relationship of the two capacity requirements, and in the restriction of the
approach to performance data only. The latter might be misleading for several
reasons. First of all there are substantial differences in the sensitivity of
performance data and subjectively perceived data. Perceived load seems to be
determined mainly by the amount of required resources, often operationalized in
terms of working memory, whereas performance measures seem to be determined
predominantly by the efficiency of the sharing of resources, depending on
allocation strategies (this is dissociation theory; see Wickens and Yeh 1983).
Moreover, individual differences in information processing abilities and
personality traits strongly influence the indicators of MWL within the subjective
(perceived), performance and psychophysiological areas.

Theoretical Approaches: Task Requirement Approaches

As has been shown, task requirements are multidimensional and, thus, may not be
described sufficiently by means of only one dimension, whether it be the perceived
effort or the residual conscious mental capacity. A more profound description
might be a profile-like one, applying a theoretically selected pattern of graded
dimensions of task characteristics. The central issue is thus the conceptualization of
“task”, especially in terms of task content, and of “task accomplishment”,
especially in terms of the structure and phases of goal-oriented actions. The role of
the task is stressed by the fact that even the impact of contextual conditions (like
temperature, noise or working hours) on the persons are task-dependent, since they
are mediated by the task acting as a gate device (Fisher 1986). Various theoretical
approaches sufficiently agree regarding those critical task dimensions, which offer
a valid prediction of the task outcome. In any case, task outcome is twofold, since
(1) the intended result must be achieved, meeting the performance-outcome
criteria, and (2) a number of unintended personal short-term and cumulative long-
term side effects will emerge, for example fatigue, boredom (monotony),
occupational diseases or improved intrinsic motivation, knowledge or skills.

Assessment of MWL. With task requirement approaches, action-oriented


approaches like those of complete versus partialized actions or the motivation
potential score (for an elaboration of both see Hacker 1986), propose as
indispensable task characteristics for analysis and evaluation at least the following:

· temporal and procedural autonomy regarding decisions on self-set goals and,


consequently, transparency, predictability and control of the work situation

· number and variety of subtasks (especially concerning preparation,


organization and checking of the results of implementation) and of actions
accomplishing these subtasks (i.e., whether such actions involve cyclical
completeness versus fragmentation)

· variety (“level”) of action-regulating mental processes and representations.


These may be mentally automated or routinized ones, knowledge-based “if-then”
ones or intellectual and problem-solving ones. (They may also be characterized by
hierarchical completeness as opposed to fragmentation)

· required cooperation

· long-term learning requirements or options.

The identification of these task characteristics requires the joint procedures of


job/task analysis, including document analyses, observations, interviews and group
discussions, which must be integrated in a quasi-experimental design (Rudolph,
Schönfelder and Hacker 1987). Task analysis instruments which may guide and
assist the analysis are available. Some of them assist only the analysis (for
example, NASA-TLX Task Load Index, Hart and Staveland, 1988) while others
are useful for evaluation and design or redesign. An example here is the TBS-GA
(Tätigkeitsbewertungs System für geistige Arbeit [Task Diagnosis Survey—Mental
Work]); see Rudolph, Schönfelder and Hacker (1987).

VIGILANCE
Herbert Heuer
The concept of vigilance refers to a human observer’s state of alertness in tasks
that demand efficient registration and processing of signals. The main
characteristics of vigilance tasks are relatively long durations and the requirement
to detect infrequent and unpredictable target stimuli (signals) against a background
of other stimulus events.

Vigilance Tasks

The prototypical task for vigilance research was that of radar operators.
Historically, their apparently unsatisfactory performance during the Second World
War has been a major impetus for the extensive study of vigilance. Another major
task requiring vigilance is industrial inspection. More generally, all kinds of
monitoring tasks which require the detection of relatively infrequent signals
embody the risk of failures to detect and to respond to these critical events.

Vigilance tasks make up a heterogeneous set and vary on several dimensions, in


spite of their common characteristics. An obviously important dimension is the
overall stimulus rate as well as the rate of target stimuli. It is not always possible to
define stimulus rate unambiguously. This is the case in tasks which require the
detection of target events against continuously presented background stimuli, as in
detecting critical values on a set of dials in a monitoring task. A less obviously
important distinction is that between successive-discrimination tasks and
simultaneous-discrimination tasks. In simultaneous-discrimination tasks both target
stimuli and background stimuli are present at the same time, while in successive-
discrimination tasks one is presented after the other so that some demands on
memory are made. Although most vigilance tasks require detection of visual
stimuli, stimuli in other modalities have also been studied. Stimuli can be confined
to a single spatial location, or there can be different sources for target stimuli.
Target stimuli can differ from background stimuli by physical characteristics, but
also by more conceptual ones (like a certain pattern of meter readings that can
differ from other patterns). Of course, the conspicuousness of targets can vary:
some can be detected easily, while others may be hard to discriminate from
background stimuli. Target stimuli can be unique or there can be sets of target
stimuli without well-defined boundaries to set them off from background stimuli,
as is the case in many industrial inspection tasks. This list of dimensions on which
vigilance tasks differ can be expanded, but even this length of the list suffices to
emphasize the heterogeneity of vigilance tasks and thus the risks involved in
generalizing certain observations across the full set.

Performance Variations and the Vigilance Decrement

The most frequently used performance measure in vigilance tasks is the proportion
of target stimuli, for example, faulty products in industrial inspection, that have
been detected; this is an estimate of the probability of so-called hits. Those target
stimuli that remain unnoticed are called misses. Although the hit rate is a
convenient measure, it is somewhat incomplete. There is a trivial strategy that
allows one to achieve 100% hits: one only has to classify all stimuli as targets.
However, the hit rate of 100% is then accompanied by a false-alarm rate of 100%,
that is, not only the target stimuli are correctly detected, but the background stimuli
are incorrectly “detected” as well. This line of reasoning makes it quite clear that
whenever there are false alarms at all, it is important to know their proportion in
addition to the hit rate. Another measure for performance in a vigilance task is the
time needed to respond to target stimuli (response time).

Performance in vigilance tasks exhibits two typical attributes. The first one is the
low overall level of vigilance performance. It is low in comparison with an ideal
situation for the same stimuli (short observation periods, high readiness of the
observer for each discrimination, etc.). The second attribute is the so-called
vigilance decrement, the decline of performance in the course of the watch which
can start within the first few minutes. Both these observations refer to the
proportion of hits, but they have also been reported for response times. Although
the vigilance decrement is typical of vigilance tasks, it is not universal.

In investigating the causes of poor overall performance and vigilance decrements, a


distinction will be made among concepts that are related to the basic characteristics
of the task and concepts that are related to organismic and task-unrelated
situational factors. Among the task-related factors strategic and non-strategic ones
can be distinguished.

Strategic processes in vigilance tasks

The detection of a signal like a faulty product is partly a matter of the observer’s
strategy and partly a matter of the signal’s discriminability. This distinction is
based on the theory of signal detection (TSD), and some basics of the theory need
to be presented in order to highlight the distinction’s importance. Consider a
hypothetical variable, defined as “evidence for the presence of a signal”. Whenever
a signal is presented, this variable takes on some value, and whenever a
background stimulus is presented, it takes on a value that is lower on the average.
The value of the evidence variable is assumed to vary across repeated presentations
of the signal. Thus it can be characterized by a so-called probability density
function as is illustrated in figure 29.27. Another density function characterizes the
values of the evidence variable upon presentation of a background stimulus. When
the signals are similar to the background stimuli, the functions will overlap, so that
a certain value of the evidence variable can originate either from a signal or from a
background stimulus. The particular shape of the density functions of figure 29.27
is not essential for the argument.

Figure 29.27 Thresholds and discriminability


The detection response of the observer is based on the evidence variable. It is
assumed that a threshold is set so that a detection response is given whenever the
value of the evidence variable is above the threshold. As is illustrated in figure
29.27 , the areas under the density functions to the right of the threshold
correspond to the probabilities of hits and false alarms. In practice, estimates of the
separation of the two functions and the location of the threshold can be derived.
The separation of the two density functions characterizes the discriminability of the
target stimuli from the background stimuli, while the location of the threshold
characterizes the observer’s strategy. Variation of the threshold produces a joint
variation of the proportions of hits and false alarms. With a high threshold, the
proportions of hits and false alarms will be small, while with a low threshold the
proportions will be large. Thus, selection of a strategy (placement of the threshold)
essentially is the selection of a certain combination of hit rate and false-alarm rate
among the combinations that are possible for a certain discriminability.

Two major factors that influence the location of the threshold are payoffs and
signal frequency. The threshold will be set to lower values when there is much to
gain from a hit and little to lose from a false alarm, and it will be set to higher
values when false alarms are costly and the benefits from hits are small. A low
threshold setting can also be induced by a high proportion of signals, while a low
proportion of signals tends to induce higher threshold settings. The effect of signal
frequency on threshold settings is a major factor for the low overall performance in
terms of the proportion of hits in vigilance tasks and for the vigilance decrement.

An account of the vigilance decrement in terms of strategic changes (threshold


changes) requires that the reduction of the proportion of hits in the course of the
watch is accompanied by a reduction of the proportion of false alarms. This is, in
fact, the case in many studies, and it is likely that the overall poor performance in
vigilance tasks (in comparison with the optimal situation) does also result, at least
partly, from a threshold adjustment. In the course of a watch the relative frequency
of detection responses comes to match the relative frequency of targets, and this
adjustment implies a high threshold with a relatively small proportion of hits and a
relatively small proportion of false alarms as well. Nevertheless there are vigilance
decrements that result from changes in discriminability rather than from changes in
threshold settings. These have been observed mainly in successive-discrimination
tasks with a relatively high rate of stimulus events.

Nonstrategic processes in vigilance tasks

Although part of the overall poor performance in vigilance tasks and many
instances of the vigilance decrement can be accounted for in terms of strategic
adjustments of the detection threshold to low signal rates, such an account is not
complete. There are changes in the observer during a watch that can reduce the
discriminability of stimuli or result in apparent threshold shifts that cannot be
considered as adaptation to the task characteristics. In the more than 40 years of
vigilance research a number of nonstrategic factors that contribute to poor overall
performance and to the vigilance decrement have been identified.
A correct response to a target in a vigilance task requires a sufficiently precise
sensory registration, an appropriate threshold location, and a link between the
perceptual processes and the associated response-related processes. During the
watch the observers have to maintain a certain task set, a certain readiness to
respond to target stimuli in a certain way. This is a nontrivial requirement, because
without a particular task set no observer would respond to target stimuli in the way
required. Two major sources of failures are thus inaccurate sensory registration and
lapses in the readiness to respond to target stimuli. Major hypotheses to account for
such failures will be briefly reviewed.

Detection and identification of a stimulus are faster when there is no temporal or


spatial uncertainty about its appearance. Temporal and/or spatial uncertainty is
likely to reduce vigilance performance. This is the essential prediction of
expectancy theory. Optimal preparedness of the observer requires temporal and
spatial certainty; obviously vigilance tasks are less than optimal in this respect.
Although the major focus of expectancy theory is on the overall low performance,
it can also serve to account for parts of the vigilance decrement. With infrequent
signals at random intervals, high levels of preparedness might initially exist at
times when no signal is presented; in addition, signals will be presented at low
levels of preparedness. This discourages occasional high levels of preparedness in
general so that whatever benefits accrue from them will vanish in the course of a
watch.

Expectancy theory has a close relation to attentional theories. Variants of


attentional theories of vigilance, of course, are related to dominant theories of
attention in general. Consider a view of attention as “selection for processing” or
“selection for action”. According to this view, stimuli are selected from the
environment and processed with high efficiency whenever they serve the currently
dominant action plan or task set. As already said, selection will benefit from
precise expectations about when and where such stimuli will occur. But stimuli
will only be selected if the action plan—the task set—is active. (Drivers of cars, for
example, respond to traffic lights, other traffic, etc.; passengers don’t do so
normally, although both are in almost the same situation. The critical difference is
that between the task sets of the two: only the driver’s task set requires responses
to traffic lights.)

Selection of stimuli for processing will suffer when the action plan is temporarily
deactivated, that is, when the task set is temporarily absent. Vigilance tasks
embody a number of features that discourage continuous maintenance of the task
set, like short cycle times for processing stimuli, lack of feedback and little
motivational challenge by apparent task difficulty. So-called blockings can be
observed in almost all simple cognitive tasks with short cycle times like simple
mental arithmetic or rapid serial responses to simple signals. Similar blockings
occur in the maintenance of the task set in a vigilance task as well. They are not
immediately recognizable as delayed responses because responses are infrequent
and targets that are presented during a period of absent task set may no longer be
there when the absence is over, so that a miss will be observed instead of a delayed
response. Blockings become more frequent with time spent on task. This can give
rise to the vigilance decrement. There may be additional reasons for temporary
lapses in the availability of the appropriate task set, for example, distraction.

Certain stimuli are not selected in the service of the current action plan, but by
virtue of their own characteristics. These are stimuli that are intense, novel, moving
toward the observer, have an abrupt onset or for any other reason might require
immediate action no matter what the current action plan of the observer is. There is
little risk of not detecting such stimuli. They attract attention automatically, as is
indicated, for example, by the orienting response, which includes a shift of the
direction of the gaze toward the stimulus source. However, answering an alarm bell
is not normally considered a vigilance task. In addition to stimuli that attract
attention by their own characteristics, there are stimuli that are processed
automatically as a consequence of practice. They seem to “pop out” from the
environment. This kind of automatic processing requires extended practice with a
so-called consistent mapping, that is, a consistent assignment of responses to
stimuli. The vigilance decrement is likely to be small or even absent once
automatic processing of stimuli has been developed.

Finally, vigilance performance suffers from a lack of arousal. This concept refers
in a rather global manner to the intensity of neural activity, ranging from sleep
through normal wakefulness to high excitement. One of the factors that is thought
to affect arousal is external stimulation, and this is fairly low and uniform in most
vigilance tasks. Thus, the intensity of central nervous system activity can decline
overall over the course of a watch. An important aspect of arousal theory is that it
links vigilance performance to various task-unrelated situational factors and factors
related to the organism.

The Influence of Situational and Organismic Factors

Low arousal contributes to poor performance in vigilance tasks. Thus performance


can be enhanced by situational factors that tend to enhance arousal, and it can be
reduced by all measures that reduce the level of arousal. On balance, this
generalization is mostly correct for the overall performance level in vigilance tasks,
but the effects on the vigilance decrement are absent or less reliably observed
across different kinds of manipulation of arousal.

One way to raise the level of arousal is the introduction of additional noise.
However, the vigilance decrement is generally unaffected, and with respect to
overall performance the results are inconsistent: enhanced, unchanged and reduced
performance levels have all been observed. Perhaps the complex nature of noise is
relevant. For example, it can be affectively neutral or annoying; it cannot only be
arousing, but also be distracting. More consistent are the effects of sleep
deprivation, which is “de-arousing”. It generally reduces vigilance performance
and has sometimes been seen to enhance the vigilance decrement. Appropriate
changes of vigilance performance have also been observed with depressant drugs
like benzodiazepines or alcohol and stimulant drugs like amphetamine, caffeine or
nicotine.

Individual differences are a conspicuous feature of performance in vigilance tasks.


Although individual differences are not consistent across all sorts of vigilance
tasks, they are fairly consistent across similar ones. There is only little or no effect
of sex and general intelligence. With respect to age, vigilance performance
increases during childhood and tends to decline beyond the age of sixty. In
addition there is a good chance that introverts will show better performance than
extroverts.

The Enhancement of Vigilance Performance

The existing theories and data suggest some means to enhance vigilance
performance. Depending on the amount of specificity of the suggestions, it is not
difficult to compile lists of various lengths. Some rather broad suggestions are
given below that have to be fitted to specific task requirements. They are related to
the ease of perceptual discriminations, the appropriate strategic adjustments, the
reduction of uncertainty, the avoidance of the effects of attentional lapses and the
maintenance of arousal.

Vigilance tasks require discriminations under non-optimal conditions. Thus one is


well advised in making the discriminations as easy as possible, or the signals as
conspicuous as possible. Measures related to this general goal can be
straightforward (like appropriate lighting or longer inspection times per product) or
more sophisticated, including special devices to enhance the conspicuousness of
targets. Simultaneous comparisons are easier than successive ones, so the
availability of a reference standard can be helpful. By means of technical devices it
is sometimes possible to present the standard and the object to be examined in
rapid alternation, so that differences will appear as motions in the display or other
changes for which the visual system is particularly sensitive.

To counteract the strategic changes of the threshold that lead to a relatively low
proportion of correct detections of targets (and for making the task less boring in
terms of the frequency of actions to be taken) the suggestion has been made to
introduce fake targets. However, this seems not to be a good recommendation.
Fake targets will increase the proportion of hits overall, but at the cost of more
frequent false alarms. In addition, the proportion of undetected targets among all
stimuli that are not responded to (the outgoing faulty material in an industrial
inspection task) will not necessarily be reduced. Better suited seems to be explicit
knowledge about the relative importance of hits and false alarms and perhaps other
measures to obtain an appropriate placement of the threshold for deciding between
“good” and “bad”.

Temporal and spatial uncertainty are important determinants of poor vigilance


performance. For some tasks spatial uncertainty can be reduced by way of defining
a certain position of the object to be inspected. However, little can be done about
temporal uncertainty: the observer would be unnecessary in a vigilance task if the
occurrence of a target could be signalled in advance of its presentation. One thing
that can be done in principle, however, is to mix objects to be inspected if faults
tend to occur in bunches; this serves to avoid very long intervals without targets as
well as very short intervals.

There are some obvious suggestions for the reduction of attentional lapses or at
least their impact on performance. By proper training, some kind of automatic
processing of targets can perhaps be obtained provided that the background and
target stimuli are not too variable. The requirement for a sustained maintenance of
the task set can be avoided by means of frequent short breaks, job rotation, job
enlargement or job enrichment. Introduction of variety can be as simple as having
the inspector himself or herself getting the material to be inspected from a box or
other location. This also introduces self-pacing, which may help in avoiding signal
presentations during temporary deactivations of the task set. Sustained
maintenance of task set can be supported by means of feedback, indicated interest
by supervisors and operator’s awareness of the importance of the task. Of course,
accurate feedback of performance level is not possible in typical vigilance tasks;
however, even inaccurate or incomplete feedback can be helpful as far as the
observer’s motivation is concerned.

There are some measures that can be taken to maintain a sufficient level of arousal.
Continuous use of drugs may exist in practice, but is never found among
recommendations. Some background music can be useful, but can also have an
opposite effect. Social isolation during vigilance tasks should mostly be avoided,
and during times of day with low levels of arousal like the late hours of the night,
supportive measures such as short watches are particularly important.

MENTAL FATIGUE
Peter Richter

Mental strain is a normal consequence of the coping process with mental workload
(MWL). Long-term load or a high intensity of job demands can result in short-term
consequences of overload (fatigue) and underload (monotony, satiation) and in
long-term consequences (e.g., stress symptoms and work-related diseases). The
maintenance of the stable regulation of actions while under strain can be realized
through changes in one’s action style (by variation of strategies of information-
seeking and decision-making), in the lowering of the level of need for achievement
(by redefinition of tasks and reduction of quality standards) and by means of a
compensatory increase of psychophysiological effort and afterwards a decrease of
effort during work time.

This understanding of the process of mental strain can be conceptualized as a


transactional process of action regulation during the imposition of loading factors
which include not only the negative components of the strain process but also the
positive aspects of learning such as accretion, tuning and restructuring and
motivation (see figure 29.28).

Figure 29.28 Components of the process of strain and its consequences


Mental fatigue can be defined as a process of time-reversible decrement of
behavioural stability in performance, mood and activity after prolonged working
time. This state is temporarily reversible by changing the work demands, the
environmental influences or stimulation and is completely reversible by means of
sleep.
Mental fatigue is a consequence of performing tasks with a high level of difficulty
that involve predominantly information processing and/or are of protracted
duration. In contrast with monotony, the recovery of the decrements is time-
consuming and does not occur suddenly after changing task conditions. Symptoms
of fatigue are identified on several levels of behavioural regulation: dis-regulation
in the biological homeostasis between environment and organism, dis-regulation in
the cognitive processes of goal-directed actions and loss of stability in goal-
oriented motivation and achievement level.

Symptoms of mental fatigue can be identified in all subsystems of the human


information processing system:

· perception: reduced eye movements, reduced discrimination of signals,


threshold deterioration

· information processing: extension of decision time, action slips, decision


uncertainty, blockings, “risky strategies” in action sequences, disturbances in
sensorimotor coordination of movements

· memory functions: prolongation of information in ultrashort-term storages,


disturbances in the rehearsal processes in short-term memory, delay in information
transmission in long-term memory and in memory searching processes.

Differential Diagnostic of Mental Fatigue

Sufficient criteria exist to differentiate amongst menta fatigue, monotony, mental


satiation and stress (in a narrow sense) (table 29.4).

Table 29.4 Differenting among several negative consequences of mental strain

Criteria Mental fatigue Monotony Satiation Stress


Key condition Poor fit in terms Poor fit in terms Loss of Goals perceived
of overload of underload perceived sense as threatening
preconditions preconditions of tasks
Mood Tiredness Tiredness with Irritability Anxiety, threat
without boredom boredom aversion
exhaustion
Emotional Neutral Neutral Increased Increased
evaluation affective anxiety
aversion
Activation Continuously Not Increased Increased
decreased continuously
decreased
Recovery Time-consuming Suddenly after ? Long-term
task alternation disturbances in
recovery
Prevention Task design Enrichment of Goal-setting Job redesign,
training, short- job content programmes and conflict and
break system job enrichment stress
management

Degrees of Mental Fatigue

The well-described phenomenology of mental fatigue (Schmidtke 1965), many


valid methods of assessment and the great amount of experimental and field results
offer the possibility of an ordinal scaling of degrees of mental fatigue (Hacker and
Richter 1994). The scaling is based on the individual’s capacity to cope with
behavioural decrements:

Level 1: Optimal and efficient performance: no symptoms of decrement in


performance, mood and activation level.

Level 2: Complete compensation characterized by increased peripheral psycho-


physiological activation (e.g., as measured by electromyogram of finger muscles),
perceived increase of mental effort, increased variability in performance criteria.

Level 3: Labile compensation additional to that described in level 2: action slips,


perceived fatigue, increasing (compensatory) psycho-physiological activity in
central indicators, heart rate, blood pressure.

Level 4: Reduced efficiency additional to that described in level 3: decrease of


performance criteria.

Level 5: Yet further functional disturbances: disturbances in social relationships


and cooperation at workplace; symptoms of clinical fatigue like loss of sleep
quality and vital exhaustion.

Prevention of Mental Fatigue

The design of task structures, environment, rest periods during working time and
sufficient sleep are the ways to reduce symptoms of mental fatigue in order that no
clinical consequences will occur:

1. Changes in the structure of tasks. Designing of preconditions for adequate


learning and task structuring is not only a means of furthering the development of
efficient job structures, but is also essential for the prevention of a misfit in terms
of mental overload or underload:
· Information processing burdens can be relieved by developing efficient
internal task representations and organization of information. The resulting
enlargement of cognitive capacity will match information needs and resources
more aptly.

· Human-centred technologies with high compatibility between the order of


information as it is presented and the required task (Norman 1993) will reduce the
mental effort necessary for information recoding and, in consequence, relieve
symptoms of fatigue and stress.

· Well-balanced coordination of different levels of regulations (as they apply to


skills, rules and knowledge) may reduce effort and, moreover, increase human
reliability (Rasmussen 1983).

· Training workers in goal-directed action sequences in advance of actual


problems will lighten their sense of mental effort by making their jobs clearer,
more predictable and more evidently under their control. Their
psychophysiological activation level will be effectively reduced.

2. Introduction of systems of short-term breaks during work. The positive effects


of such breaks depend on the observance of some preconditions. More short breaks
are more efficient than fewer long breaks; effects depend on a fixed and therefore
anticipatable time schedule; and the content of the breaks should have a
compensatory function to the physical and mental job demands.

3. Sufficient relaxation and sleep. Special employee-assistant programmes and


stress-management techniques may support the ability of relaxation and the
prevention of the development of chronicle fatigue (Sethi, Caro and Schuler 1987).

WORK ORGANIZATION
Eberhard Ulich and Gudela Grote

Design of Production Systems

Many companies invest millions in computer-supported production systems and at


the same time do not make full use of their human resources, whose value can be
significantly increased through investments in training. In fact, the use of qualified
employee potential instead of highly complex automation can not only, in certain
circumstances, significantly reduce investment costs, it can also greatly increase
flexibility and system capability.

Causes of Inefficient Use of Technology


The improvements which investments in modern technology are intended to make
are frequently not even approximately achieved (Strohm, Kuark and Schilling
1993; Ulich 1994). The most important reasons for this are due to problems in the
areas of technology, organization and employee qualifications.

Three main causes can be identified for problems with technology:

1. Insufficient technology. Because of the rapidity of technological changes, new


technology reaching the market has sometimes undergone inadequate continuous
usability tests, and unplanned downtime can result.

2. Unsuitable technology. Technology developed for large companies is often


not suitable for smaller companies. When a small firm introduces a production
planning and control system developed for a large company, it may deprive itself
of the flexibility necessary for its success or even survival.

3. Excessively complex technology. When designers and developers use their


entire planning knowledge to realize what is technically feasible without taking
into account the experience of those involved in production, the result can be
complex automated systems which are no longer easy to master.

Problems with organization are primarily attributable to continuous attempts at


implementing the latest technology in unsuitable organizational structures. For
instance, it makes little sense to introduce third, fourth and fifth generation
computers into second generation organizations. But this is exactly what many
companies do (Savage and Appleton 1988). In many companies, a radical
restructuring of the organization is a precondition for the successful use of new
technology. This particularly includes an examination of the concepts of
production planning and control. Ultimately, local self-control by qualified
operators can in certain circumstances be significantly more efficient and
economical than a technically highly developed production planning and control
system.

Problems with the qualifications of employees primarily arise because a large


number of companies do not recognize the need for qualification measures in
conjunction with the introduction of computer-supported production systems. In
addition, training is too frequently regarded as a cost factor to be controlled and
minimized, rather than as a strategic investment. In fact, system downtime and the
resulting costs can often be effectively reduced by allowing faults to be diagnosed
and remedied on the basis of operators’ competence and system-specific
knowledge and experience. This is particularly the case in tightly coupled
production facilities (Köhler et al. 1989). The same applies to introducing new
products or product variants. Many examples of inefficient excessive technology
use testify to such relationships.
The consequence of the analysis briefly presented here is that the introduction of
computer-supported production systems only promises success if it is integrated
into an overall concept which seeks to jointly optimize the use of technology, the
structure of the organization and the enhancement of staff qualifications.

From the Task to the Design of Socio-Technical Systems

Work-related psychological concepts of production design are based on the


primacy of the task. On the one hand, the task forms the interface between
individual and organization (Volpert 1987). On the other hand, the task links the
social subsystem with the technical subsystem. “The task must be the point of
articulation between the social and technical system—linking the job in the
technical system with its correlated role behaviour, in the social system”
(Blumberg 1988).

This means that a socio-technical system, for example a production island, is


primarily defined by the task which it has to perform. The distribution of work
between human and machine plays a central role, because it decides whether the
person “functions” as the long arm of the machine with a function leftover in an
automation “gap” or whether the machine functions as the long arm of the person,
with a tool function supporting human capabilities and competence. We refer to
these opposing positions as “technology-oriented” and “work-oriented” (Ulich
1994).

The Concept of Complete Task

The principle of complete activity (Hacker 1986) or complete task plays a central
role in work-related psychological concepts for defining work tasks and for
dividing up tasks between human and machine. Complete tasks are those “over
which the individual has considerable personal control” and that “induce strong
forces within the individual to complete or to continue them”. Complete tasks
contribute to the “development of what has been described ... as ‘task
orientation’—that is, a state of affairs in which the individual’s interest is aroused,
engaged and directed by the character of the task” (Emery 1959). Figure 29.29
summarizes characteristics of completeness which must be taken into account for
measures geared towards work-oriented design of production systems.

Figure 29.29 Characteristics of complete tasks


Illustrations of concrete consequences for production design arising from the
principle of the complete task are the following:

1. The independent setting of objectives, which can be incorporated into higher-


order goals, requires turning away from central planning and control in favour of
decentralized shop-floor control, which provides the possibility of making self-
determined decisions within defined periods of time.

2. Self-determined preparation for action, in the sense of carrying out planning


functions, requires the integration of work preparation tasks on the shop-floor.

3. Selecting methods means, for example, allowing a designer to decide whether


he or she wishes to use the drawing board instead of an automated system (such as
a CAD application) to perform certain subtasks, provided that it is ensured that
data required for other parts of the process are entered in the system.
4. Performance functions with process feedback for correcting actions where
appropriate require in the case of encapsulated work processes “windows to the
process” which help to minimize process distance.

5. Action control with feedback of results means that shop-floor workers take on
the function of quality inspection and control.

These indications of the consequences arising from realizing the principle of the
complete task make two things clear: (1) in many cases—probably even the
majority of cases—complete tasks in the sense described in figure 29.29 can only
be structured as group tasks on account of the resulting complexity and the
associated scope; (2) restructuring of work tasks—particularly when it is linked to
introducing group work—requires their integration into a comprehensive
restructuring concept which covers all levels of the company.

The structural principles which apply to the various levels are summarized in table
29.5 .

Table 29.5 Work-oriented principles for production structuring

Organizational level Structural principle


Company Decentralization
Organizational unit Functional integration
Group Self-regulation1
Individual Skilled production work1

1
Taking into account the principle of differential work design.

Source: Ulich 1994.

Possibilities for realizing the principles for production structuring outlined in table
29.5 are illustrated by the proposal for restructuring a production company shown
in figure 29.30 . This proposal, which was unanimously approved both by those
responsible for production and by the project group formed for the purpose of
restructuring, also demonstrates a fundamental turning away from Tayloristic
concepts of labour and authority divisions. The examples of many companies show
that the restructuring of work and organization structures on the basis of such
models is able to meet both work psychological criteria of promoting health and
personality development and the demand for long-term economic efficiency (see
Ulich 1994).

Figure 29.30 Proposal for restructuring a production company


The line of argument favoured here—only very briefly outlined for reasons of
space—seeks to make three things clear:

1. Concepts like the ones mentioned here represent an alternative to “lean


production” in the sense described by Womack, Jones and Roos (1990). While in
the latter approach “every free space is removed” and extreme breaking down of
work activities in the Tayloristic sense is maintained, in the approach being
advanced in these pages, complete tasks in groups with wide-ranging self-
regulation play a central role.

2. Classical career paths for skilled workers are modified and in some cases
precluded by the necessary realization of the functional integration principle, that
is, with the reintegration on the shop-floor of what are known as indirectly
productive functions, such as shop-floor work preparation, maintenance, quality
control and so forth. This requires a fundamental reorientation in the sense of
replacing the traditional career culture with a competence culture.
3. Concepts such as those mentioned here mean a fundamental change to
corporate power structures which must find their counterpart in the development of
corresponding possibilities for participation.

Workers’ Participation

In the previous sections types of work organization were described that have as one
basic characteristic the democratization at lower levels of an organization’s
hierarchy through increased autonomy and decision latitude regarding work
content as well as working conditions on the shop-floor. In this section,
democratization is approached from a different angle by looking at participative
decision-making in general. First, a definitional framework for participation is
presented, followed by a discussion of research on the effects of participation.
Finally, participative systems design is looked at in some detail.

Definitional framework for participation

Organizational development, leadership, systems design, and labour relations are


examples of the variety of tasks and contexts where participation is considered
relevant. A common denominator which can be regarded as the core of
participation is the opportunity for individuals and groups to promote their interests
through influencing the choice between alternative actions in a given situation
(Wilpert 1989). In order to describe participation in more detail, a number of
dimensions are necessary, however. Frequently suggested dimensions are (a)
formal-informal, (b) direct-indirect, (c) degree of influence and (d) content of
decision (e.g., Dachler and Wilpert 1978; Locke and Schweiger 1979). Formal
participation refers to participation within legally or otherwise prescribed rules
(e.g., bargaining procedures, guidelines for project management), while informal
participation is based on non-prescribed exchanges, for example, between
supervisor and subordinate. Direct participation allows for direct influence by the
individuals concerned, whereas indirect participation functions through a system of
representation. Degree of influence is usually described by means of a scale
ranging from “no information to employees about a decision”, through “advance
information to employees” and “consultation with employees” to “common
decision of all parties involved”. As regards the giving of advance information
without any consultation or common decision-making, some authors argue that this
is not a low level of participation at all, but merely a form of “pseudo-
participation” (Wall and Lischeron 1977). Finally, the content area for participative
decision-making can be specified, for example, technological or organizational
change, labour relations, or day-to-day operational decisions.

A classification scheme quite different from those derived from the dimensions
presented so far was developed by Hornby and Clegg (1992). Based on work by
Wall and Lischeron (1977), they distinguish three aspects of participative
processes:
1. the types and levels of interactions between the parties involved in a decision

2. the flow of information between the participants

3. the nature and degree of influence the parties exert on each other.

They then used these aspects to complement a framework suggested by Gowler


and Legge (1978), which describes participation as a function of two
organizational variables, namely, type of structure (mechanistic versus organic)
and type of process (stable versus unstable). As this model includes a number of
assumptions about participation and its relationship to organization, it cannot be
used to classify general types of participation. It is presented here as one attempt to
define participation in a broader context (see table 29.6). (In the last section of this
article, Hornby and Clegg’s study (1992) will be discussed, which also aimed at
testing the model’s assumptions.)

Table 29.6 Participation in organizational context

Organizational structure
Organizational Mechanistic Organic
processes
Stable Regulated Open
Interaction: vertical/command Interaction: lateral/consultative
Information flow: non-reciprocal Information flow: reciprocal
Influence: asymmetrical Influence: asymmetrical
Unstable Arbitrary Regulated
Interaction: ritualistic/random Interaction: intensive/random
Information flow: Information flow:
non-reciprocal/sporadic reciprocal/interrogative
Influence: authoritarian Influence: paternalistic

Source: Adapted from Hornby and Clegg 1992.

An important dimension usually not included in classifications for participation is


the organizational goal behind choosing a participative strategy (Dachler and
Wilpert 1978). Most fundamentally, participation can take place in order to comply
with a democratic norm, irrespective of its influence on the effectiveness of the
decision-making process and the quality of the decision outcome and
implementation. On the other hand, a participative procedure can be chosen to
benefit from the knowledge and experience of the individuals involved or to ensure
acceptance of a decision. Often it is difficult to identify the objectives behind
choosing a participative approach to a decision and often several objectives will be
found at the same time, so that this dimension cannot be easily used to classify
participation. However, for understanding participative processes it is an important
dimension to keep in mind.

Research on the effects of participation

A widely shared assumption holds that satisfaction as well as productivity gains


can be achieved by providing the opportunity for direct participation in decision-
making. Overall, research has supported this assumption, but the evidence is not
unequivocal and many of the studies have been criticized on theoretical and
methodological grounds (Cotton et al. 1988; Locke and Schweiger 1979; Wall and
Lischeron 1977). Cotton et al. (1988) argued that inconsistent findings are due to
differences in the form of participation studied; for instance, informal participation
and employee ownership are associated with high productivity and satisfaction
whereas short-term participation is ineffective in both respects. Although their
conclusions were strongly criticized (Leana, Locke and Schweiger 1990), there is
agreement that participation research is generally characterized by a number of
deficiencies, ranging from conceptual problems like those mentioned by Cotton et
al. (1988) to methodological issues like variations in results based on different
operationalizations of the dependent variables (e.g., Wagner and Gooding 1987).

To exemplify the difficulties of participation research, the classic study by Coch


and French (1948) is briefly described, followed by the critique of Bartlem and
Locke (1981). The focus of the former study was overcoming resistance to change
by means of participation. Operators in a textile plant where frequent transfers
between work tasks occurred were given the opportunity to participate in the
design of their new jobs to varying degrees. One group of operators participated in
the decisions (detailed working procedures for new jobs and piece rates) through
chosen representatives, that is, several operators of their group. In two smaller
groups, all operators participated in those decisions and a fourth group served as
control with no participation allowed. Previously it had been found in the plant that
most operators resented being transferred and were slower in relearning their new
jobs as compared with learning their first job in the plant and that absenteeism and
turnover among transferred operators was higher than among operators not recently
transferred.

This occurred despite the fact that a transfer bonus was given to compensate for the
initial loss in piece-rate earnings after a transfer to a new job. Comparing the three
experimental conditions it was found that the group with no participation remained
at a low level of production—which had been set as the group standard—for the
first month after the transfer, while the groups with full participation recovered to
their former productivity within a few days and even exceeded it at the end of the
month. The third group that participated through chosen representatives did not
recover as fast, but showed their old productivity after a month. (They also had
insufficient material to work on for the first week, however.) No turnover occurred
in the groups with participation and little aggression towards management was
observed. The turnover in the participation group without participation was 17%
and the attitude towards management was generally hostile. The group with no
participation was broken up after one month and brought together again after
another two and one-half months to work on a new job, and this time they were
given the opportunity to participate in the design of their job. They then showed
the same pattern of recovery and increased productivity as the groups with
participation in the first experiment. The results were explained by Coch and
French on the basis of a general model of resistance to change derived from work
by Lewin (1951, see below).

Bartlem and Locke (1981) argued that these findings could not be interpreted as
support for the positive effects of participation because there were important
differences between the groups as regards the explanation of the need for changes
in the introductory meetings with management, the amount of training received,
the way the time studies were carried out to set the piece rate, the amount of work
available and group size. They assumed that perceived fairness of pay rates and
general trust in management contributed to the better performance of the
participation groups, not participation per se.

In addition to the problems associated with research on the effects of participation,


very little is known about the processes that lead to these effects (e.g., Wilpert
1989). In a longitudinal study on the effects of participative job design, Baitsch
(1985) described in detail processes of competence development in a number of
shop-floor employees. His study can be linked to Deci’s (1975) theory of intrinsic
motivation based on the need for being competent and self-determining. A
theoretical framework focusing on the effects of participation on the resistance to
change was suggested by Lewin (1951) who argued that social systems gain a
quasi-stationary equilibrium which is disturbed by any attempt at change. For the
change to be successfully carried through, forces in favour of the change must be
stronger than the resisting forces. Participation helps in reducing the resisting
forces as well as in increasing the driving forces because reasons for resistance can
be openly discussed and dealt with, and individual concerns and needs can be
integrated into the proposed change. Additionally, Lewin assumed that common
decisions resulting from participatory change processes provide the link between
the motivation for change and the actual changes in behaviour.

Participation in systems design

Given the—albeit not completely consistent—empirical support for the


effectiveness of participation, as well as its ethical underpinnings in industrial
democracy, there is widespread agreement that for the purposes of systems design
a participative strategy should be followed (Greenbaum and Kyng 1991;
Majchrzak 1988; Scarbrough and Corbett 1992). Additionally, a number of case
studies on participative design processes have demonstrated the specific
advantages of participation in systems design, for example, regarding the quality of
the resulting design, user satisfaction, and acceptance (i.e., actual use) of the new
system (Mumford and Henshall 1979; Spinas 1989; Ulich et al. 1991).

The important question then is not the if, but the how of participation. Scarbrough
and Corbett (1992) provided an overview of various types of participation in the
various stages of the design process (see table 29.7). As they point out, user
involvement in the actual design of technology is rather rare and often does not
extend beyond information distribution. Participation mostly occurs in the latter
stages of implementation and optimization of the technical system and during the
development of socio-technical design options, that is, options of organizational
and job design in combination with options for the use of the technical system.

Table 29.7 User participation in the technology process

Phases of technology process Type of participation

Formal Informal

Design Trade union consultation User redesign

Photocopying

Implementation New technology agreements Skills bargaining

Collective bargaining Negotiation

User cooperation

Use Job design Information job redesign and


work practices

Quality circles

Adapted from Scarbrough and Corbett 1992.

Besides resistance in managers and engineers to the involvement of users in the


design of technical systems and potential restrictions embedded in the formal
participation structure of a company, an important difficulty concerns the need for
methods that allow the discussion and evaluation of systems that do not yet exist
(Grote 1994). In software development, usability labs can help to overcome this
difficulty as they provide an opportunity for early testing by future users.

In looking at the process of systems design, including participative processes,


Hirschheim and Klein (1989) have stressed the effects of implicit and explicit
assumptions of system developers and managers about basic topics such as the
nature of social organization, the nature of technology and their own role in the
development process. Whether system designers see themselves as experts,
catalysts or emancipators will greatly influence the design and implementation
process. Also, as mentioned before, the broader organizational context in which
participative design takes place has to be taken into account. Hornby and Clegg
(1992) provided some evidence for the relationship between general organizational
characteristics and the form of participation chosen (or, more precisely, the form
evolving in the course of system design and implementation). They studied the
introduction of an information system which was carried out within a participative
project structure and with explicit commitment to user participation. However,
users reported that they had had little information about the changes supposed to
take place and low levels of influence over system design and related questions
like job design and job security. This finding was interpreted in terms of the
mechanistic structure and unstable processes of the organization that fostered
“arbitrary” participation instead of the desired open participation (see table 29.6).

In conclusion, there is sufficient evidence demonstrating the benefits of


participative change strategies. However, much still needs to be learned about the
underlying processes and influencing factors that bring about, moderate or prevent
these positive effects.

SLEEP DEPRIVATION
Kazutaka Kogi

Healthy individuals regularly sleep for several hours every day. Normally they
sleep during the night hours. They find it most difficult to remain awake during the
hours between midnight and early morning, when they normally sleep. If an
individual has to remain awake during these hours either totally or partially, the
individual comes to a state of forced sleep loss, or sleep deprivation, that is usually
perceived as tiredness. A need for sleep, with fluctuating degrees of sleepiness, is
felt which continues until sufficient sleep is taken. This is the reason why periods
of sleep deprivation are often said to cause a person to incur sleep deficit or sleep
debt.

Sleep deprivation presents a particular problem for workers who cannot take
sufficient sleep periods because of work schedules (e.g., working at night) or, for
that matter, prolonged free-time activities. A worker on a night shift remains sleep-
deprived until the opportunity for a sleep period becomes available at the end of
the shift. Since sleep taken during daytime hours is usually shorter than needed, the
worker cannot recover from the condition of sleep loss sufficiently until a long
sleep period, most likely a night sleep, is taken. Until then, the person accumulates
a sleep deficit. (A similar condition—jet lag—arises after travelling between time
zones that differ by a few hours or more. The traveller tends to be sleep-deprived
as the activity periods in the new time zone correspond more clearly to the normal
sleep period in the originating place.) During the periods of sleep loss, workers feel
tired and their performance is affected in various ways. Thus various degrees of
sleep deprivation are incorporated into the daily life of workers having to work
irregular hours and it is important to take measures to cope with unfavourable
effects of such sleep deficit. The main conditions of irregular working hours that
contribute to sleep deprivation are shown in table 29.8 .

Table 29.8 Main conditions of irregular working hours which contribute to sleep
deprivation of various degrees

Irregular working hours Conditions leading to sleep deprivation


Night duty No or shortened night-time sleep
Early morning or late evening duty Shortened sleep, disrupted sleep
Long hours of work or working two shifts Phase displacement of sleep
together
Straight night or early morning shifts Consecutive phase displacement of sleep
Short between-shift period Short and disrupted sleep
Long interval between days off Accumulation of sleep shortages
Work in a different time zone No or shortened sleep during the “night”
hours in the originating place (jet lag)
Unbalanced free time periods Phase displacement of sleep, short sleep

In extreme conditions, sleep deprivation may last for more than a day. Then
sleepiness and performance changes increase as the period of sleep deprivation is
prolonged. Workers, however, normally take some form of sleep before sleep
deprivation becomes too protracted. If the sleep thus taken is not sufficient, the
effects of sleep shortage still continue. Thus, it is important to know not only the
effects of sleep deprivation in various forms but also the ways in which workers
can recover from it.

The complex nature of sleep deprivation is shown by figure 29.31 , which depicts
data from laboratory studies on the effects of two days of sleep deprivation
(Fröberg 1985).

Figure 29.31 Perfomance, sleep ratings and physiological variables of a group of


subjects exposed to two nights of sleep deprivation
The data show three basic changes resulting from prolonged sleep deprivation:

1. There is a general decreasing trend in both objective performance and


subjective ratings of performance efficiency.

2. The decline in performance is influenced by the time of day. This cycling


decline is correlated with those physiological variables which have a circadian
cycling period. Performance is better in the normal activity phase when, for
example, adrenaline excretion and body temperature are higher than those in the
period originally assigned to a normal night’s sleep, when the physiological
measures are low.

3. Self-ratings of sleepiness increase with time of continuous sleep deprivation,


with a clear cyclic component associated with time of day.

The fact that the effects of sleep deprivation are correlated with physiological
circadian rhythms helps us to understand its complex nature (Folkard and
Akerstedt 1992). These effects should be viewed as a result of a phase shift of the
sleep-wakefulness cycle in one’s daily life.

The effects of continuous work or sleep deprivation thus include not only a
reduction in alertness but decreased performance capabilities, increased probability
of falling asleep, lowered well-being and morale and impaired safety. When such
periods of sleep deprivation are repeated, as in the case of shift workers, their
health may be affected (Rutenfranz 1982; Koller 1983; Costa et al. 1990). An
important aim of research is thus to determine to what extent sleep deprivation
damages the well-being of individuals and how we can best use the recovery
function of sleep in reducing such effects.
Effects of Sleep Deprivation

During and after a night of sleep deprivation, the physiological circadian rhythms
of the human body seem to remain sustained. For example, the body temperature
curve during the first day’s work among night-shift workers tends to keep its basic
circadian pattern. During the night hours, the temperature declines towards early
morning hours, rebounds to rise during the subsequent daytime and falls again after
an afternoon peak. The physiological rhythms are known to get “adjusted” to the
reversed sleep-wakefulness cycles of night-shift workers only gradually in the
course of several days of repeated night shifts. This means that the effects on
performance and sleepiness are more significant during night hours than in the
daytime. The effects of sleep deprivation are therefore variably associated with the
original circadian rhythms seen in physiological and psychological functions.

The effects of sleep deprivation on performance depend on the type of the task to
be performed. Different characteristics of the task influence the effects (Fröberg
1985; Folkard and Monk 1985; Folkard and Akerstedt 1992). Generally, a complex
task is more vulnerable than a simpler task. Performance of a task involving an
increasing number of digits or a more complex coding deteriorates more during
three days of sleep loss (Fröberg 1985; Wilkinson 1964). Paced tasks that need to
be responded to within a certain interval deteriorate more than self-paced tasks.
Practical examples of vulnerable tasks include serial reactions to defined
stimulations, simple sorting operations, the recording of coded messages, copy
typing, display monitoring and continuous inspection. Effects of sleep deprivation
on strenuous physical performance are also known. Typical effects of prolonged
sleep deprivation on performance (on a visual task) is shown in figure 29.32
(Dinges 1992). The effects are more pronounced after two nights of sleep loss (40-
56 hours) than after one night of sleep loss (16-40 hours).

Figure 29.32 Regression lines fit to response speed (the reciprocal of response times) on
a 10-minute simple, unprepared visual task administered repeatedly to healthy young
adults during no sleep loss (5-16 hours), one night of sleep loss (16-40 hours) and two
nights of sleep loss (40-56 hours)
The degree to which the performance of tasks is affected also appears to depend on
how it is influenced by the “masking” components of the circadian rhythms. For
example, some measures of performance, such as five-target memory search tasks,
are found to adjust to night work considerably more quickly than serial reaction
time tasks, and hence they may be relatively unimpaired on rapidly rotating shift
systems (Folkard et al. 1993). Such differences in the effects of endogenous
physiological body clock rhythms and their masking components must be taken
into account in considering the safety and accuracy of performance under the
influence of sleep deprivation.

One particular effect of sleep deprivation on performance efficiency is the


appearance of frequent “lapses” or periods of no response (Wilkinson 1964;
Empson 1993). These performance lapses are short periods of lowered alertness or
light sleep. This can be traced in records of videotaped performance, eye
movements or electroencephalograms (EEGs). A prolonged task (one-half hour or
more), especially when the task is replicated, can more easily lead to such lapses.
Monotonous tasks such as repetitions of simple reactions or monitoring of
infrequent signals are very sensitive in this regard. On the other hand, a novel task
is less affected. Performance in changing work situations is also resistant.

While there is evidence of a gradual arousal decrease in sleep deprivation, one


would expect less affected performance levels between lapses. This explains why
results of some performance tests show little influence of sleep loss when the tests
are done in a short period of time. In a simple reaction time task, lapses would lead
to very long response times whereas the rest of the measured times would remain
unchanged. Caution is thus needed in interpreting test results concerning sleep loss
effects in actual situations.
Changes in sleepiness during sleep deprivation obviously relate to physiological
circadian rhythms as well as to such lapse periods. Sleepiness sharply increases
with time of the first period of night-shift work, but decreases during subsequent
daytime hours. If sleep deprivation continues to the second night sleepiness
becomes very advanced during the night hours (Costa et al. 1990; Matsumoto and
Harada 1994). There are moments when the need for sleep is felt to be almost
irresistible; these moments correspond to the appearance of lapses, as well as to the
appearance of interruptions in the cerebral functions as evidenced by EEG records.
After a while, sleepiness is felt to be reduced, but there follows another period of
lapse effects. If workers are questioned about various fatigue feelings, however,
they usually mention increasing levels of fatigue and general tiredness persisting
throughout the sleep deprivation period and between-lapse periods. A slight
recovery of subjective fatigue levels is seen during the daytime following a night
of sleep deprivation, but fatigue feelings are remarkably advanced in the second
and subsequent nights of continued sleep deprivation.

During sleep deprivation, sleep pressure from the interaction of prior wakefulness
and circadian phase may always be present to some degree, but the lability of state
in sleepy subjects is also modulated by context effects (Dinges 1992). Sleepiness is
influenced by the amount and type of stimulation, the interest afforded by the
environment and the meaning of the stimulation to the subject. Monotonous
stimulation or that requiring sustained attention can more easily lead to vigilance
decrement and lapses. The greater the physiological sleepiness due to sleep loss,
the more the subject is vulnerable to environmental monotony. Motivation and
incentive can help override this environmental effect, but only for a limited period.

Effects of Partial Sleep Deprivation and Accumulated Sleep Shortages

If a subject works continuously for a whole night without sleep, many performance
functions will have definitely deteriorated. If the subject goes to the second night
shift without getting any sleep, the performance decline is far advanced. After the
third or fourth night of total sleep deprivation, very few people can stay awake and
perform tasks even if highly motivated. In actual life, however, such conditions of
total sleep loss rarely occur. Usually people take some sleep during subsequent
night shifts. But reports from various countries show that sleep taken during
daytime is almost always insufficient to recover from the sleep debt incurred by
night work (Knauth and Rutenfranz 1981; Kogi 1981; ILO 1990). As a result, sleep
shortages accumulate as shift workers repeat night shifts. Similar sleep shortages
also result when sleep periods are reduced on account of the need to follow shift
schedules. Even if night sleep can be taken, sleep restriction of as little as two
hours each night is known to lead to an insufficient amount of sleep for most
persons. Such sleep reduction can lead to impaired performance and alertness
(Monk 1991).
Examples of conditions in shift systems which contribute to accumulation of sleep
shortages, or partial sleep deprivation, are given in table 29.8 . In addition to
continued night work for two or more days, short between-shift periods, repetition
of an early start of morning shifts, frequent night shifts and inappropriate holiday
allotment accelerate the accumulation of sleep shortages.

The poor quality of daytime sleep or shortened sleep is important, too. Daytime
sleep is accompanied by an increased frequency of awakenings, less deep and
slow-wave sleep and a distribution of REM sleep different from that of normal
night-time sleep (Torsvall, Akerstedt and Gillberg 1981; Folkard and Monk 1985;
Empson 1993). Thus a daytime sleep may not be as sound as a night sleep even in
a favourable environment.

This difficulty of taking good quality sleep due to different timing of sleep in a
shift system is illustrated by figure 29.33 which shows the duration of sleep as a
function of the time of sleep onset for German and Japanese workers based on
diary records (Knauth and Rutenfranz 1981; Kogi 1985). Due to circadian
influence, daytime sleep is forced to be short. Many workers may have split sleep
during the daytime and often add some sleep in the evening where possible.

Figure 29.33 Mean sleep length as a function of the time of sleep onset. Comparison of
data from German and Japanese shift workers.
In real-life settings, shift workers take a variety of measures to cope with such
accumulation of sleep shortages (Wedderburn 1991). For example, many of them
try to sleep in advance before a night shift or have a long sleep after it. Although
such efforts are by no means entirely effective to offset the effects of sleep deficit,
they are made quite deliberately. Social and cultural activities may be restricted as
part of coping measures. Outgoing free-time activities, for example, are undertaken
less frequently between two night shifts. Sleep timing and duration as well as the
actual accumulation of sleep deficit thus depend on both job-related and social
circumstances.

Recovery from Sleep Deprivation and Health Measures

The only effective means of recovering from sleep deprivation is to sleep. This
restorative effect of sleep is well known (Kogi 1982). As recovery by sleep may
differ according to its timing and duration (Costa et al. 1990), it is essential to
know when and for how long people should sleep. In normal daily life, it is always
the best to take a full night’s sleep to accelerate the recovery from sleep deficit but
efforts are usually made to minimize sleep deficit by taking sleep at different
occasions as replacements of normal night sleeps of which one has been deprived.
Aspects of such replacement sleeps are shown in table 29.9 .

Table 29.9 Aspects of advance, anchor & retard sleeps taken as replacement of normal
night sleep

Aspect Advance sleep Anchor sleep Retard sleep


Occasion Before a night shift Intermittent night After a night shift
work
Between night shifts Between night shifts
During a night shift
Before early morning After prolonged
work Alternate-day work evening work

Late evening naps Prolonged freetime Daytime naps

Naps taken
informally
Duration Usually short Short by definition Usually short but
longer after late
evening work
Quality Longer latency of Short latency Shorter latency for
falling asleep REM sleep
Poor mood on rising
Poor mood on rising Increased
Sleep stages similar awakenings
Reduced REM sleep to initial part of a
normal night sleep
Slow-wave sleep Increased REM sleep
dependent on prior
wakefulness Increased slow-wave
sleep after long
wakefulness
Interaction with Disrupted rhythms; Conducive to Disrupted rhythms;
circadian rhythms relatively faster stabilizing original slow adjustment
adjustment rhythms

To offset night sleep deficit, the usual effort made is to take daytime sleep in
“advance” and “retard” phases (i.e., before and after night-shift work). Such a sleep
coincides with the circadian activity phase. Thus the sleep is characterized by
longer latency, shortened slow-wave sleep, disrupted REM sleep and disturbances
of one’s social life. Social and environmental factors are important in determining
the recuperative effect of a sleep. That a complete conversion of circadian rhythms
is impossible for a shift worker in a real-life situation should be borne in mind in
considering the effectiveness of the recovery functions of sleep.

In this respect, interesting features of a short “anchor sleep” have been reported
(Minors and Waterhouse 1981; Kogi 1982; Matsumoto and Harada 1994). When
part of the customary daily sleep is taken during the normal night sleep period and
the rest at irregular times, the circadian rhythms of rectal temperature and urinary
secretion of several electrolytes can retain a 24-hour period. This means that a
short night-time sleep taken during the night sleep period can help preserve the
original circadian rhythms in subsequent periods.

We may assume that sleeps taken at different periods of the day could have certain
complementary effects in view of the different recovery functions of these sleeps.
An interesting approach for night-shift workers is the use of a night-time nap
which usually lasts up to a few hours. Surveys show this short sleep taken during a
night shift is common among some groups of workers. This anchor-sleep type
sleep is effective in reducing night work fatigue (Kogi 1982) and may reduce the
need of recovery sleep. Figure 29.34 compares the subjective feelings of fatigue
during two consecutive night shifts and the off-duty recovery period between the
nap-taking group and the non-nap group (Matsumoto and Harada 1994). The
positive effects of a night-time nap in reducing fatigue was obvious. These effects
continued for a large part of the recovery period following night work. Between
these two groups, no significant difference was found upon comparing the length
of the day sleep of the non-nap group with the total sleeping time (night-time nap
plus subsequent day sleep) of the nap group. Therefore a night-time nap enables
part of the essential sleep to be taken in advance of the day sleep following night
work. It can therefore be suggested that naps taken during night work can to a
certain extent aid recovery from the fatigue caused by that work and accompanying
sleep deprivation (Sakai et al. 1984; Saito and Matsumoto 1988).
Figure 29.34 Mean scores for subjective feelings of fatigue during two consecutive night
shifts and the off-duty recovery period for nap and no-nap groups

It must be admitted, however, that it is not possible to work out optimal strategies
that each worker suffering from sleep deficit can apply. This is demonstrated in the
development of international labour standards for night work that recommend a set
of measures for workers doing frequent night work (Kogi and Thurman 1993). The
varied nature of these measures and the trend towards increasing flexibility in shift
systems clearly reflect an effort to develop flexible sleep strategies (Kogi 1991).
Age, physical fitness, sleep habits and other individual differences in tolerance
may play important roles (Folkard and Monk 1985; Costa et al. 1990; Härmä
1993). Increasing flexibility in work schedules in combination with better job
design is useful in this regard (Kogi 1991).
Sleep strategies against sleep deprivation should be dependent on type of working
life and be flexible enough to meet individual situations (Knauth, Rohmert and
Rutenfranz 1979; Rutenfranz, Knauth and Angersbach 1981; Wedderburn 1991;
Monk 1991). A general conclusion is that we should minimize night sleep
deprivation by selecting appropriate work schedules and facilitate recovery by
encouraging individually suitable sleeps, including replacement sleeps and a sound
night-time sleep in the early periods after sleep deprivation. It is important to
prevent the accumulation of sleep deficit. The period of night work which deprives
workers of sleep in the normal night sleep period should be as short as possible.
Between-shift intervals should be long enough to allow a sleep of sufficient length.
A better sleep environment and measures to cope with social needs are also useful.
Thus, social support is essential in designing working time arrangements, job
design and individual coping strategies in promoting the health of workers faced
with frequent sleep deficit.

WORKSTATIONS
Roland Kadefors

An Integrated Approach in the Design of Workstations

In ergonomics, the design of workstations is a critical task. There is general


agreement that in any work setting, whether blue-collar or white-collar, a well-
designed workstation furthers not only the health and well-being of the workers,
but also productivity and the quality of the products. Conversely, the poorly
designed workstation is likely to cause or contribute to the development of health
complaints or chronic occupational diseases, as well as to problems with keeping
product quality and productivity at a prescribed level.

To every ergonomist, the above statement may seem trivial. It is also recognized
by every ergonomist that working life worldwide is full of not only ergonomic
shortcomings, but blatant violations of basic ergonomic principles. It is clearly
evident that there is a widespread unawareness with respect to the importance of
workstation design among those responsible: production engineers, supervisors and
managers.

It is noteworthy that there is an international trend with respect to industrial work


which would seem to underline the importance of ergonomic factors: the
increasing demand for improved product quality, flexibility and product delivery
precision. These demands are not compatible with a conservative view regarding
the design of work and workplaces.

Although in the present context it is the physical factors of workplace design that
are of chief concern, it should be borne in mind that the physical design of the
workstation cannot in practice be separated from the organization of work. This
principle will be made evident in the design process described in what follows. The
quality of the end result of the process relies on three supports: ergonomic
knowledge, integration with productivity and quality demands, and participation.
The process of implementation of a new workstation must cater to this integration,
and it is the main focus of this article.

Design considerations

Workstations are meant for work. It must be recognized that the point of departure
in the workstation design process is that a certain production goal has to be
achieved. The designer—often a production engineer or other person at middle-
management level—develops internally a vision of the workplace, and starts to
implement that vision through his or her planning media. The process is iterative:
from a crude first attempt, the solutions become gradually more and more refined.
It is essential that ergonomic aspects be taken into account in each iteration as the
work progresses.

It should be noted that ergonomic design of workstations is closely related to


ergonomic assessment of workstations. In fact, the structure to be followed here
applies equally to the cases where the workstation already exists or when it is in a
planning stage.

In the design process, there is a need for a structure which ensures that all relevant
aspects be considered. The traditional way to handle this is to use checklists
containing a series of those variables which should be taken into account.
However, general purpose checklists tend to be voluminous and difficult to use,
since in a particular design situation only a fraction of the checklist may be
relevant. Furthermore, in a practical design situation, some variables stand out as
being more important than others. A methodology to consider these factors jointly
in a design situation is required. Such a methodology will be proposed in this
article.

Recommendations for workstation design must be based on a relevant set of


demands. It should be noted that it is in general not enough to take into account
threshold limit values for individual variables. A recognized combined goal of
productivity and conservation of health makes it necessary to be more ambitious
than in a traditional design situation. In particular, the question of musculoskeletal
complaints is a major aspect in many industrial situations, although this category of
problems is by no means limited to the industrial environment.

A Workstation Design Process

Steps in the process


In the workstation design and implementation process, there is always an initial
need to inform users and to organize the project so as to allow for full user
participation and in order to increase the chance of full employee acceptance of the
final result. A treatment of this goal is not within the scope of the present treatise,
which concentrates on the problem of arriving at an optimal solution for the
physical design of the workstation, but the design process nonetheless allows the
integration of such a goal. In this process, the following steps should always be
considered:

1. collection of user-specified demands

2. prioritizing of demands

3. transfer of demands into (a) technical specifications and (b) specifications in


user terms

4. iterative development of the workstation’s physical layout

5. physical implementation

6. trial period of production

7. full production

8. evaluation and identification of rest problems.

The focus here is on steps one through five. Many times, only a subset of all these
steps is actually included in the design of workstations. There may be various
reasons for this. If the workstation is a standard design, such as in some VDU
working situations, some steps may duly be excluded. However, in most cases the
exclusion of some of the steps listed would lead to a workstation of lower quality
than what can be considered acceptable. This can be the case when economic or
time constraints are too severe, or when there is sheer neglect due to lack of
knowledge or insight at management level.

Collection of user-specified demands

It is essential to identify the user of the workplace as any member of the production
organization who may be able to contribute qualified views on its design. Users
may include, for instance, the workers, the supervisors, the production planners
and production engineers, as well as the safety steward. Experience shows clearly
that these actors all have their unique knowledge which should be made use of in
the process.

The collection of the user-specified demands should meet a number of criteria:


1. Openness. There should be no filter applied in the initial stage of the process.
All points of view should be noted without voiced criticism.

2. Non-discrimination. Viewpoints from every category should be treated


equally at this stage of the process. Special consideration should be given to the
fact that some persons may be more outspoken than others, and that there is a risk
that they may silence some of the other actors.

3. Development through dialogue. There should be an opportunity to adjust and


develop the demands through a dialogue between participants of different
backgrounds. Prioritizing should be addressed as part of the process.

4. Versatility. The process of collection of user-specified demands should be


reasonably economical and not require the involvement of specialist consultants or
extensive time demands on the part of the participants.

The above set of criteria may be met by using a methodology based on quality
function deployment (QFD) according to Sullivan (1986). Here, the user demands
may be collected in a session where a mixed group of actors (not more than eight
to ten people) is present. All participants are given a pad of removable self-sticking
notes. They are asked to write down all workplace demands which they find
relevant, each one on a separate slip of paper. Aspects relating to work
environment and safety, productivity and quality should be covered. This activity
may continue for as long as found necessary, typically ten to fifteen minutes. After
this session, one after the other of the participants is asked to read out his or her
demands and to stick the notes on a board in the room where everyone in the group
can see them. The demands are grouped into natural categories such as lighting,
lifting aids, production equipment, reaching requirements and flexibility demands.
After the completion of the round, the group is given the opportunity to discuss and
to comment on the set of demands, one category at a time, with respect to
relevance and priority.

The set of user-specified demands collected in a process such as the one described
in the above forms one of the bases for the development of the demand
specification. Additional information in the process may be produced by other
categories of actors, for example, product designers, quality engineers, or
economists; however, it is vital to realize the potential contribution that the users
can make in this context.

Prioritizing and demand specification

With respect to the specification process, it is essential that the different types of
demands be given consideration according to their respective importance;
otherwise, all aspects that have been taken into account will have to be considered
in parallel, which may tend to make the design situation complex and difficult to
handle. This is why checklists, which need to be elaborate if they are to serve the
purpose, tend to be difficult to manage in a particular design situation.

It may be difficult to devise a priority scheme which serves all types of


workstations equally well. However, on the assumption that manual handling of
materials, tools or products is an essential aspect of the work to be carried out in
the workstation, there is a high probability that aspects associated with
musculoskeletal load will be at the top of the priority list. The validity of this
assumption may be checked in the user demand collection stage of the process.
Relevant user demands may be, for instance, associated with muscular strain and
fatigue, reaching, seeing, or ease of manipulation.

It is essential to realize that it may not be possible to transform all user-specified


demands into technical demand specifications. Although such demands may relate
to more subtle aspects such as comfort, they may nevertheless be of high relevance
and should be considered in the process.

Musculoskeletal load variables

In line with the above reasoning, we shall here apply the view that there is a set of
basic ergonomic variables relating to musculoskeletal load which need to be taken
into account as a priority in the design process, in order to eliminate the risk of
work-related musculosketal disorders (WRMDs). This type of disorder is a pain
syndrome, localized in the musculoskeletal system, which develops over long
periods of time as a result of repeated stresses on a particular body part (Putz-
Anderson 1988). The essential variables are (e.g., Corlett 1988):

· muscular force demand

· working posture demand

· time demand.

With respect to muscular force, criteria setting may be based on a combination of


biomechanical, physiological and psychological factors. This is a variable that is
operationalized through measurement of output force demands, in terms of handled
mass or required force for, say, the operation of handles. Also, peak loads in
connection with highly dynamic work may have to be taken into account.

Working posture demands may be evaluated by mapping (a) situations where the
joint structures are stretched beyond the natural range of movement, and (b) certain
particularly awkward situations, such as kneeling, twisting, or stooped postures, or
work with the hand held above shoulder level.
Time demands may be evaluated on the basis of mapping (a) short-cycle, repetitive
work, and (b) static work. It should be noted that static work evaluation may not
exclusively concern maintaining a working posture or producing a constant output
force over lengthy periods of time; from the point of view of the stabilizing
muscles, particularly in the shoulder joint, seemingly dynamic work may have a
static character. It may thus be necessary to consider lengthy periods of joint
mobilization.

The acceptability of a situation is of course based in practice on the demands on


the part of the body that is under the highest strain.

It is important to note that these variables should not be considered one at a time
but jointly. For instance, high force demands may be acceptable if they occur only
occasionally; lifting the arm above shoulder level once in a while is not normally a
risk factor. But combinations among such basic variables must be considered. This
tends to make criteria setting difficult and involved.

In the Revised NIOSH equation for the design and evaluation of manual handling
tasks (Waters et al. 1993), this problem is addressed by devising an equation for
recommended weight limits which takes into account the following mediating
factors: horizontal distance, vertical lifting height, lifting asymmetry, handle
coupling and lifting frequency. In this way, the 23-kilogram acceptable load limit
based on biomechanical, physiological and psychological criteria under ideal
conditions, may be modified substantially upon taking into account the specifics of
the working situation. The NIOSH equation provides a base for evaluation of work
and workplaces involving lifting tasks. However, there are severe limitations as to
the usability of the NIOSH equation: for instance, only two-handed lifts may be
analysed; scientific evidence for analysis of one-handed lifts is still inconclusive.
This illustrates the problem of applying scientific evidence exclusively as a basis
for work and workplace design: in practice, scientific evidence must be merged
with educated views of persons who have direct or indirect experience of the type
of work considered.

The cube model

Ergonomic evaluation of workplaces, taking into account the complex set of


variables which need to be considered, is to a large extent a communications
problem. Based on the prioritizing discussion described above, a cube model for
ergonomic evaluation of workplaces was developed (Kadefors 1993). Here the
prime goal was to develop a didactic tool for communication purposes, based on
the assumption that output force, posture and time measures in a great majority of
situations constitute interrelated, prioritized basic variables.

For each one of the basic variables, it is recognized that the demands may be
grouped with respect to severity. Here, it is proposed that such a grouping may be
made in three classes: (1) low demands, (2) medium demands or (3) high demands.
The demand levels may be set either by using whatever scientific evidence is
available or by taking a consensus approach with a panel of users. These two
alternatives are of course not mutually exclusive, and may well entail similar
results, but probably with different degrees of generality.

As noted above, combinations of the basic variables determine to a large extent the
risk level with respect to the development of musculoskeletal complaints and
cumulative trauma disorders. For instance, high time demands may render a
working situation unacceptable in cases where there are also at least medium level
demands with respect to force and posture. It is essential in the design and
assessment of workplaces that the most important variables be considered jointly.
Here a cube model for such evaluation purposes is proposed. The basic variables—
force, posture and time—constitute the three axes of the cube. For each
combination of demands a subcube may be defined; in all, the model incorporates
27 such subcubes (see figure 29.35).

Figure 29.35 The "cube model" for ergonomics assessment. Each cube represents a
combination of demands relating to force, posture and time. Light: acceptable
combination; gray: conditionally acceptable; black: unacceptable
An essential aspect of the model is the degree of acceptability of the demand
combinations. In the model, a three-zone classification scheme is proposed for
acceptability: (1) the situation is acceptable, (2) the situation is conditionally
acceptable or (3) the situation is unacceptable. For didactic purposes, each subcube
may be given a certain texture or colour (say, green-yellow-red). Again, the
assessment may be user-based or based on scientific evidence. The conditionally
acceptable (yellow) zone means that “there exists a risk of disease or injury that
cannot be neglected, for the whole or a part of the operator population in question”
(CEN 1994).

In order to develop this approach, it is useful to consider a case: the evaluation of


load on the shoulder in moderately paced one-handed materials handling. This is a
good example, since in this type of situation, it is normally the shoulder structures
that are under the heaviest strain.

With respect to the force variable, classification may be based in this case on
handled mass. Here, low force demand is identified as levels below 10% of
maximal voluntary lifting capacity (MVLC), which amounts to approximately 1.6
kg in an optimal working zone. High force demand requires more than 30%
MVLC, approximately 4.8 kg. Medium force demand falls in between these limits.
Low postural strain is when the upper arm is close to the thorax. High postural
strain is when humeral abduction or flexion exceeds 45°. Medium postural strain is
when the abduction/flexion angle is between 15° and 45°. Low time demand is
when the handling occupies less than one hour per working day on and off, or
continuously for less than 10 minutes per day. High time demand is when the
handling takes place for more than four hours per working day, or continuously for
more than 30 minutes (sustained or repetitively). Medium time demand is when the
exposure falls between these limits.

In figure 29.35 , degrees of acceptability have been assigned to combinations of


demands. For instance, it is seen that high time demands may only be combined
with combined low force and postural demands. Moving from unacceptable to
acceptable may be undertaken by reducing demands in either dimension, but
reduction in time demands is the most efficient way in many cases. In other words,
in some cases workplace design should be altered, in other cases it may be more
efficient to change the organization of work.

Using a consensus panel with a set of users for definition of demand levels and
classification of degree of acceptability may enhance the workstation design
process considerably, as considered below.

Additional variables

In addition to the basic variables considered above, a set of variables and factors
characterizing the workplace from an ergonomics point of view has to be taken
into account, depending upon the particular conditions of the situation to be
analysed. They include:

· precautions to reduce risks for accidents

· specific environmental factors such as noise, lighting and ventilation

· exposure to climatic factors

· exposure to vibration (from hand-held tools or whole body)

· ease of meeting productivity and quality demands.


To a large extent these factors may be considered one at a time; hence the checklist
approach may be useful. Grandjean (1988) in his textbook covers the essential
aspects that usually need to be taken into account in this context. Konz (1990) in
his guidelines provides for workstation organization and design a set of leading
questions focusing on worker-machine interfacing in manufacturing systems.

In the design process followed here, the checklist should be read in conjunction
with the user-specified demands.

A Workstation Design Example: Manual Welding

As an illustrative (hypothetical) example, the design process leading to


implementation of a workstation for manual welding (Sundin et al. 1994) is
described here. Welding is an activity frequently combining high demands for
muscular force with high demands for manual precision. The work has a static
character. The welder is often doing welding exclusively. The welding work
environment is generally hostile, with a combination of exposure to high noise
levels, welding smoke and optical radiation.

The task was to devise a workplace for manual MIG (metal inert gas) welding of
medium size objects (up to 300 kg) in a workshop environment. The workstation
had to be flexible since there was a variety of objects to be manufactured. There
were high demands for productivity and quality.

A QFD process was carried out in order to provide a set of workstation demands in
user terms. Welders, production engineers and product designers were involved.
User demands, which are not listed here, covered a wide range of aspects including
ergonomics, safety, productivity and quality.

Using the cube model approach, the panel identified, by consensus, limits between
high, moderate and low load:

1. Force variable. Less than 1 kg handled mass is termed a low load, whereas
more than 3 kg is considered a high load.

2. Postural strain variable. Working positions implying high strain are those
involving elevated arms, twisted or deep forward-flexed positions, and kneeling
positions, and also include situations where the wrist is held in extreme
flexion/extension or deviation. Low strain occurs where the posture is straight
upright standing or sitting and where hands are in optimal working zones.

3. Time variable. Less than 10% of the working time devoted to welding is
considered low demand, whereas more than 40% of total working time is termed
high demand. Medium demands occur when the variable falls between the limits
given above, or when the situation is unclear.
It was clear from assessment using the cube model (figure 29.35) that high time
demands could not be accepted if there were concurrent high or moderate demands
in terms of force and postural strain. In order to reduce these demands, mechanized
object handling and tool suspension was deemed a necessity. There was consensus
developed around this conclusion. Using a simple computer-aided design (CAD)
program (ROOMER), an equipment library was created. Various workplace station
layouts could be developed very easily and modified in close interaction with the
users. This design approach has significant advantages compared with merely
looking at plans. It gives the user an immediate vision of what the intended
workplace may look like.

Figure 29.36 shows the welding workstation arrived at using the CAD system. It is
a workplace which reduces the force and posture demands, and which meets nearly
all the residual user demands put forward.

Figure 29.36 A CAD version of a workstation for manual welding, arrived at in the
design process

On the basis of the results of the first stages of the design process, a welding
workplace (figure 29.37) was implemented. Assets of this workplace include:

Figure 29.37 The welding workstation implemented


1. Work in the optimized zone is facilitated using a computerized handling
device for welding objects. There is an overhead hoist for transportation purposes.
As an alternative, a balanced lifting device is supplied for easy object handling.

2. The welding gun and grinding machine are suspended, thus reducing force
demands. They can be positioned anywhere around the welding object. A welding
chair is supplied.

3. All media come from above, which means that there are no cables on the
floor.

4. The workstation has lighting at three levels: general, workplace and process.
The workplace lighting comes from ramps above the wall elements. The process
lighting is integrated in the welding smoke ventilation arm.

5. The workstation has ventilation at three levels: general displacement


ventilation, workplace ventilation using a movable arm, and integrated ventilation
in the MIG welding gun. The workplace ventilation is controlled from the welding
gun.

6. There are noise-absorbing wall elements on three sides of the workplace. A


transparent welding curtain covers the fourth wall. This makes it possible for the
welder to keep informed of what happens in the workshop environment.

In a real design situation, compromises of various kinds may have to be made, due
to economic, space and other constraints. It should be noted, however, that licensed
welders are hard to come by for the welding industry around the world, and they
represent a considerable investment. Nearly no welders go into normal retirement
as active welders. Keeping the skilled welder on the job is beneficial for all parties
involved: welder, company and society. For instance, there are very good reasons
why equipment for object handling and positioning should be an integral
constituent of many welding workplaces.

Data for Workstation Design

In order to be able to design a workplace properly, extensive sets of basic


information may be needed. Such information includes anthropometric data of user
categories, lifting strength and other output force capacity data of male and female
populations, specifications of what constitutes optimal working zones and so forth.
In the present article, references to some key papers are given.

The most complete treatment of virtually all aspects of work and workstation
design is probably still the textbook by Grandjean (1988). Information on a wide
range of anthropometric aspects relevant to workstation design is presented by
Pheasant (1986). Large amounts of biomechanical and anthropometric data are
given by Chaffin and Andersson (1984). Konz (1990) has presented a practical
guide to workstation design, including many useful rules of thumb. Evaluation
criteria for the upper limb, particularly with reference to cumulative trauma
disorders, have been presented by Putz-Anderson (1988). An assessment model for
work with hand tools was given by Sperling et al. (1993). With respect to manual
lifting, Waters and co-workers have developed the revised NIOSH equation,
summarizing existing scientific knowledge on the subject (Waters et al. 1993).
Specification of functional anthropometry and optimal working zones have been
presented by, for example, Rebiffé, Zayana and Tarrière (1969) and Das and Grady
(1983a, 1983b). Mital and Karwowski (1991) have edited a useful book reviewing
various aspects relating in particular to the design of industrial workplaces.

The large amount of data needed to design workstations properly, taking all
relevant aspects into account, will make necessary the use of modern information
technology by production engineers and other responsible people. It is likely that
various types of decision-support systems will be made available in the near future,
for instance in the form of knowledge-based or expert systems. Reports on such
developments have been given by, for example, DeGreve and Ayoub (1987),
Laurig and Rombach (1989), and Pham and Onder (1992). However, it is an
extremely difficult task to devise a system making it possible for the end-user to
have easy access to all relevant data needed in a specific design situation.

TOOLS
T.M. Fraser

Commonly a tool comprises a head and a handle, with sometimes a shaft, or, in the
case of the power tool, a body. Since the tool must meet the requirements of
multiple users, basic conflicts can arise which may have to be met with
compromise. Some of these conflicts derive from limitations in the capacities of
the user, and some are intrinsic to the tool itself. It should be remembered,
however, that human limitations are inherent and largely immutable, while the
form and function of the tool are subject to a certain amount of modification. Thus,
in order to effect desirable change, attention must be directed primarily to the form
of the tool, and, in particular, to the interface between the user and the tool, namely
the handle.

The Nature of Grip

The widely accepted characteristics of grip have been defined in terms of a power
grip, a precision grip and a hook grip, by which virtually all human manual
activities can be accomplished.

In a power grip, such as is used in hammering nails, the tool is held in a clamp
formed by the partially flexed fingers and the palm, with counterpressure being
applied by the thumb. In a precision grip, such as one uses when adjusting a set
screw, the tool is pinched between the flexor aspects of the fingers and the
opposing thumb. A modification of the precision grip is the pencil grip, which is
self-explanatory and is used for intricate work. A precision grip provides only 20%
of the strength of a power grip.

A hook grip is used where there is no requirement for anything other than holding.
In the hook grip the object is suspended from the flexed fingers, with or without
the support of the thumb. Heavy tools should be designed so that they can be
carried in a hook grip.

Grip Thickness

For precision grips, recommended thicknesses have varied from 8 to 16


millimetres (mm) for screwdrivers, and 13 to 30 mm for pens. For power grips
applied around a more or less cylindrical object, the fingers should surround more
than half the circumference, but the fingers and thumb should not meet.
Recommended diameters have ranged from as low as 25 mm to as much as 85 mm.
The optimum, varying with hand size, is probably around 55 to 65 mm for males,
and 50 to 60 mm for females. Persons with small hands should not perform
repetitive actions in power grips of diameter greater than 60 mm.

Grip Strength and Hand Span

The use of a tool requires strength. Other than for holding, the greatest requirement
for hand strength is found in the use of cross-lever action tools such as pliers and
crushing tools. The effective force in crushing is a function of the grip strength and
the required span of the tool. The maximum functional span between the end of the
thumb and the ends of the grasping fingers averages about 145 mm for men and
125 mm for women, with ethnic variations. For an optimal span, which ranges
from 45 to 55 mm for both men and women, the grip strength available for a single
short-term action ranges from about 450 to 500 newtons for men and 250 to 300
newtons for women, but for repetitive action the recommended requirement is
probably closer to 90 to 100 newtons for men, and 50 to 60 newtons for women.
Many commonly used clamps or pliers are beyond the capacity of one-handed use,
particularly in women.

When a handle is that of a screwdriver or similar tool the available torque is


determined by the user’s ability to transmit force to the handle, and thus is
determined by both the coefficient of friction between hand and handle and the
diameter of the handle. Irregularities in the shape of the handle make little or no
difference to the ability to apply torque, although sharp edges can cause discomfort
and eventual tissue damage. The diameter of a cylindrical handle that allows the
greatest application of torque is 50 to 65 mm, while that for a sphere is 65 to 75
mm.

Handles

Shape of handle

The shape of a handle should maximize contact between skin and handle. It should
be generalized and basic, commonly of flattened cylindrical or elliptical section,
with long curves and flat planes, or a sector of a sphere, put together in such a
manner as to conform to the general contours of the grasping hand. Because of its
attachment to the body of a tool, the handle may also take the form of a stirrup, a
T-shape or an L-shape, but the portion that contacts the hand will be in the basic
form.

The space enclosed by the fingers is, of course, complex. The use of simple curves
is a compromise intended to meet the variations represented by different hands and
different degrees of flexion. In this regard, it is undesirable to introduce any
contour matching of flexed fingers into the handle in the form of ridges and
valleys, flutings and indentations, since, in fact, these modifications would not fit a
significant number of hands and might indeed, over a prolonged period, cause
pressure injury to the soft tissues. In particular, recesses of greater that 3 mm are
not recommended.

A modification of the cylindrical section is the hexagonal section, which is of


particular value in the design of small calibre tools or instruments. It is easier to
maintain a stable grip on a hexagonal section of small calibre than on a cylinder.
Triangular and square sections have also been used with varying degrees of
success. In these cases, the edges must be rounded to avert pressure injury.
Grip Surface and Texture

It is not by accident that for millennia wood has been the material of choice for tool
handles other than those for crushing tools like pliers or clamps. In addition to its
aesthetic appeal, wood has been readily available and easily worked by unskilled
workers, and has qualities of elasticity, thermal conductivity, frictional resistance
and relative lightness in relation to bulk that have made it very acceptable for this
and other uses.

In recent years, metal and plastic handles have become more common for many
tools, the latter in particular for use with light hammers or screwdrivers. A metal
handle, however, transmits more force to the hand, and preferably should be
encased in a rubber or plastic sheath. The grip surface should be slightly
compressible, where feasible, nonconductive and smooth, and the surface area
should be maximized to ensure pressure distribution over as large an area as
possible. A foam rubber grip has been used to reduce the perception of hand
fatigue and tenderness.

The frictional characteristics of the tool surface vary with the pressure exerted by
the hand, with the nature of the surface and contamination by oil or sweat. A small
amount of sweat increases the coefficient of friction.

Length of handle

The length of the handle is determined by the critical dimensions of the hand and
the nature of the tool. For a hammer to be used by one hand in a power grip, for
example, the ideal length ranges from a minimum of about 100 mm to a maximum
of about 125 mm. Short handles are unsuitable for a power grip, while a handle
shorter than 19 mm cannot be properly grasped between thumb and forefinger and
is unsuitable for any tool.

Ideally, for a power tool, or a hand saw other than a coping or fret saw, the handle
should accommodate at the 97.5th percentile level the width of the closed hand
thrust into it, namely 90 to 100 mm in the long axis and 35 to 40 mm in the short.

Weight and Balance

Weight is not a problem with precision tools. For heavy hammers and power tools
a weight between 0.9 kg and 1.5 kg is acceptable, with a maximum of about 2.3 kg.
For weights greater than recommended, the tool should be supported by
mechanical means.

In the case of a percussion tool such as a hammer, it is desirable to reduce the


weight of the handle to the minimum compatible with structural strength and have
as much weight as possible in the head. In other tools, the balance should be evenly
distributed where possible. In tools with small heads and bulky handles this may
not be possible, but the handle should then be made progressively lighter as the
bulk increases relative to the size of the head and shaft.

Significance of Gloves

It is sometimes overlooked by tool designers that tools are not always held and
operated by bare hands. Gloves are commonly worn for safety and comfort. Safety
gloves are seldom bulky, but gloves worn in cold climates may be very heavy,
interfering not only with sensory feedback but also with the ability to grasp and
hold. The wearing of woollen or leather gloves can add 5 mm to hand thickness
and 8 mm to hand breadth at the thumb, while heavy mittens can add as much as
25 to 40 mm respectively.

Handedness

The majority of the population in the western hemisphere favours the use of the
right hand. A few are functionally ambidextrous, and all persons can learn to
operate with greater or less efficiency with either hand.

Although the number of left-handed persons is small, wherever feasible the fitting
of handles to tools should make the tool workable by either left-handed or right-
handed persons (examples would include the positioning of the secondary handle
in a power tool or the finger loops in scissors or clamps) unless it is clearly
inefficient to do so, as in the case of screw-type fasteners which are designed to
take advantage of the powerful supinating muscles of the forearm in a right-handed
person while precluding the left-hander from using them with equal effectiveness.
This sort of limitation has to be accepted since the provision of left-hand threads is
not an acceptable solution.

Significance of Gender

In general, women tend to have smaller hand dimensions, smaller grasp and some
50 to 70% less strength than men, although of course a few women at the higher
percentile end have larger hands and greater strength than some men at the lower
percentile end. As a result there exists a significant although undetermined number
of persons, mostly female, who have difficulty in manipulating various hand tools
which have been designed with male use in mind, including in particular heavy
hammers and heavy pliers, as well as metal cutting, crimping and clamping tools
and wire strippers. The use of these tools by women may require an undesirable
two-handed instead of single-handed function. In a mixed-gender workplace it is
therefore essential to ensure that tools of suitable size are available not only to
meet the requirements of women, but also to meet those of men who are at the low
percentile end of hand dimensions.
Special considerations

The orientation of a tool handle, where feasible, should allow the operating hand to
conform to the natural functional position of the arm and hand, namely with the
wrist more than half-supinated, abducted about 15° and slightly dorsiflexed, with
the little finger in almost full flexion, the others less so and the thumb adducted
and slightly flexed, a posture sometimes erroneously called the handshake position.
(In a handshake the wrist is not more than half-supinated.) The combination of
adduction and dorsiflexion at the wrist with varying flexion of the fingers and
thumb generates an angle of grasp comprising about 80° between the long axis of
the arm and a line passing through the centre point of the loop created by the
thumb and index finger, that is, the transverse axis of the fist.

Forcing the hand into a position of ulnar deviation, that is, with the hand bent
towards the little finger, as is found in using a standard pliers, generates pressure
on the tendons, nerves and blood vessels within the wrist structure and can give
rise to the disabling conditions of tenosynovitis, carpal tunnel syndrome and the
like. By bending the handle and keeping the wrist straight, (that is, by bending the
tool and not the hand) compression of nerves, soft tissues and blood vessels can be
avoided. While this principle has been long recognized, it has not been widely
accepted by tool manufacturers or the using public. It has particular application in
the design of cross-lever action tools such as pliers, as well as knives and
hammers.

Pliers and cross-lever tools

Special consideration must be given to the shape of the handles of pliers and
similar devices. Traditionally pliers have had curved handles of equal length, the
upper curve approximating the curve of the palm of the hand and the lower curve
approximating the curve of the flexed fingers. When the tool is held in the hand,
the axis between the handles is in line with the axis of the jaws of the pliers.
Consequently, in operation, it is necessary to hold the wrist in extreme ulnar
deviation, that is, bent towards the little finger, while it is being repeatedly rotated.
In this position the use of the hand-wrist-arm segment of the body is extremely
inefficient and very stressful on the tendons and joint structures. If the action is
repetitive it may give rise to various manifestations of overuse injury.

To counter this problem a new and ergonomically more suitable version of pliers
has appeared in recent years. In these pliers the axis of the handles is bent through
approximately 45° relative to the axis of the jaws. The handles are thickened to
allow a better grasp with less localized pressure on the soft tissues. The upper
handle is proportionately longer with a shape that fits into, and around the ulnar
side of, the palm. The forward end of the handle incorporates a thumb support. The
lower handle is shorter, with a tang, or rounded projection, at the forward end and a
curve conforming to the flexed fingers.
While the foregoing is a somewhat radical change, several ergonomically sound
improvements can be made in pliers relatively easily. Perhaps the most important,
where a power grip is required, is in the thickening and slight flattening of the
handles, with a thumb support at the head-end of the handle and a slight flare at the
other end. If not integral to the design, this modification can be achieved by
encasing the basic metal handle with a fixed or detachable non-conductive sheath
made of rubber or an appropriate synthetic material, and perhaps bluntly roughened
to improve the tactile quality. Indentation of the handles for fingers is undesirable.
For repetitive use it may be desirable to incorporate a light spring into the handle to
open it after closing.

The same principles apply to other cross-lever tools, particularly with respect to
change in the thickness and flattening of the handles.

Knives

For a general purpose knife, that is, one that is not used in a dagger grasp, it is
desirable to include a 15° angle between handle and blade to reduce the stress on
joint tissues. The size and shape of handles should conform in general to that for
other tools, but to allow for different hand sizes it has been suggested that two
sizes of knife handle should be supplied, namely one to fit the 50th to 95th
percentile user, and one for the 5th to 50th percentile. To allow the hand to exert
force as close to the blade as possible the top surface of the handle should
incorporate a raised thumb rest.

A knife guard is required to prevent the hand from slipping forward onto the blade.
The guard may take several forms, such as a tang, or curved projection, about 10 to
15 mm in length, protruding downwards from the handle, or at right angles to the
handle, or a bail guard comprising a heavy metal loop from front to rear of the
handle. The thumb rest also acts to prevent slippage.

The handle should conform to general ergonomic guidelines, with a yielding


surface resistant to grease.

Hammers

The requirements for hammers have been largely considered above, with the
exception of that relating to bending the handle. As noted above, forced and
repetitive bending of the wrist may cause tissue damage. By bending the tool
instead of the wrist this damage may be reduced. With respect to hammers various
angles have been examined, but it would appear that bending the head downward
between 10° and 20° may improve comfort, if it does not actually improve
performance.

Screwdrivers and scraping tools


The handles of screwdrivers and other tools held in a somewhat similar manner,
such as scrapers, files, hand chisels and so on, have some special requirements.
Each at one time or another is used with a precision grip or a power grip. Each
relies on the functions of the fingers and the palm of the hand for stabilization and
the transmission of force.

The general requirements of handles have already been considered. The most
common effective shape of a screwdriver handle has been found to be that of a
modified cylinder, dome-shaped at the end to receive the palm, and slightly flared
where it meets the shaft to provide support to the ends of the fingers. In this
manner, torque is applied largely by way of the palm, which is maintained in
contact with the handle by way of pressure applied from the arm and the frictional
resistance at the skin. The fingers, although transmitting some force, occupy more
of a stabilizing role, which is less fatiguing since less power is required. Thus the
dome of the head becomes very important in handle design. If there are sharp
edges or ridges on the dome or where the dome meets the handle, then either the
hand becomes callused and injured, or the transmission of force is transferred
towards the less efficient and more readily fatigued fingers and thumb. The shaft is
commonly cylindrical, but a triangular shaft has been introduced which provides
better support for the fingers, although its use may be more fatiguing.

Where the use of a screwdriver or other fastener is so repetitive as to comprise an


overuse injury hazard the manual driver should be replaced with a powered driver
slung from an overhead harness in such a manner as to be readily accessible
without obstructing the work.

Saws and power tools

Hand saws, with the exception of fret saws and light hacksaws, where a handle like
that of a screwdriver is most appropriate, commonly have a handle which takes the
form of a closed pistol grip attached to the blade of the saw.

The handle essentially comprises a loop into which the fingers are placed. The loop
is effectively a rectangle with curved ends. To allow for gloves it should have
internal dimensions of approximately 90 to 100 mm in the long diameter and 35 to
40 mm in the short. The handle in contact with the palm should have the flattened
cylindrical shape already mentioned, with compound curves to reasonably fit the
palm and the flexed fingers. The width from outer curve to inner curve should be
about 35 mm, and the thickness not more than 25 mm.

Curiously, the function of grasping and holding a power tool is very similar to that
of holding a saw, and consequently a somewhat similar type of handle is effective.
The pistol grip common in power tools is akin to an open saw handle with the sides
being curved instead of being flattened.
Most power tools comprise a handle, a body and a head. Placement of the handle is
significant. Ideally handle, body and head should be in line so that the handle is
attached at the rear of the body and the head protrudes from the front. The line of
action is the line of the extended index finger, so that the head is eccentric to the
central axis of the body. The centre of mass of the tool, however, is in front of the
handle, while the torque is such as to create a turning movement of the body which
the hand must overcome. Consequently it would be more appropriate to place the
primary handle directly under the centre of mass in such a way that, if necessary,
the body juts out behind the handle as well as in front. Alternatively, particularly in
a heavy drill, a secondary handle can be placed underneath the drill in such a
manner that the drill can be operated with either hand. Power tools are normally
operated by a trigger incorporated into the upper front end of the handle and
operated by the index finger. The trigger should be designed to be operated by
either hand and should incorporate an easily reset latching mechanism to hold the
power on when required.

CONTROLS, INDICATORS AND PANELS


Karl H. E. Kroemer

In what follows, three of the most important concerns of ergonomic design will be
examined: first, that of controls, devices to transfer energy or signals from the
operator to a piece of machinery; second, indicators or displays, which provide
visual information to the operator about the status of the machinery; and third, the
combination of controls and displays in a panel or console.

Designing for the Sitting Operator

Sitting is a more stable and less energy-consuming posture than standing, but it
restricts the working space, particularly of the feet, more than standing. However,
it is much easier to operate foot controls when sitting, as compared to standing,
because little body weight must be transferred by the feet to the ground.
Furthermore, if the direction of the force exerted by the foot is partly or largely
forward, provision of a seat with a backrest allows the exertion of rather large
forces. (A typical example of this arrangement is the location of pedals in an
automobile, which are located in front of the driver, more or less below seat
height.) Figure 29.38 shows schematically the locations in which pedals may be
located for a seated operator. Note that the specific dimensions of that space
depend on the anthropometry of the actual operators.

Figure 29.38 Preferred and regular workspace for feet (in centimetres)
The space for the positioning of hand-operated controls is primarily located in front
of the body, within a roughly spherical contour that is centred at either the elbow,
at the shoulder, or somewhere between those two body joints. Figure 29.39 shows
schematically that space for the location of controls. Of course, the specific
dimensions depend on the anthropometry of the operators.

Figure 29.39 Preferred and regular workspace for hands (in centimetres)

The space for displays and for controls that must be looked at is bounded by the
periphery of a partial sphere in front of the eyes and centred at the eyes. Thus, the
reference height for such displays and controls depends on the eye height of the
seated operator and on his or her trunk and neck postures. The preferred location
for visual targets closer than about one metre is distinctly below the height of the
eye, and depends on the closeness of the target and on the posture of the head. The
closer the target, the lower it should be located, and it should be in or near the
medial (mid-sagittal) plane of the operator.

It is convenient to describe the posture of the head by using the “ear-eye line”
(Kroemer 1994a) which, in the side view, runs through the right ear hole and the
juncture of the lids of the right eye, while the head is not tilted to either side (the
pupils are at the same horizontal level in the frontal view). One usually calls the
head position “erect” or “upright” when the pitch angle P (see figure 29.40)
between the ear-eye line and the horizon is about 15°, with the eyes above the
height of the ear. The preferred location for visual targets is 25°-65° below the ear-
eye line (LOSEE in figure 29.40), with the lower values preferred by most people
for close targets that must be kept in focus. Even though there are large variations
in the preferred angles of the line of sight, most subjects, particularly as they
become older, prefer to focus on close targets with large LOSEE angles.

Figure 29.40 Ear-eye line

Designing for the Standing Operator

Pedal operation by a standing operator should be seldom required, because


otherwise the person must spend too much time standing on one foot while the
other foot operates the control. Obviously, simultaneous operation of two pedals by
a standing operator is practically impossible. While the operator is standing still,
the room for the location of foot controls is limited to a small area below the trunk
and slightly in front of it. Walking about would provide more room to place pedals,
but that is highly impractical in most cases because of the walking distances
involved.
The location for hand-operated controls of a standing operator includes about the
same area as for a seated operator, roughly a half sphere in front of the body, with
its centre near the shoulders of the operator. For repeated control operations, the
preferred part of that half sphere would be its lower section. The area for the
location of displays is also similar to the one suited to a seated operator, again
roughly a half sphere centred near the operator’s eyes, with the preferred locations
in the lower section of that half sphere. The exact locations for displays, and also
for controls that must be seen, depends on the posture of the head, as discussed
above.

The height of controls is appropriately referenced to the height of the elbow of the
operator while the upper arm is hanging from the shoulder. The height of displays
and controls that must be looked at is referred to the eye height of the operator.
Both depend on the operator’s anthropometry, which may be rather different for
short and tall persons, for men and women, and for people of different ethnic
origins.

Foot-operated Controls

Two kinds of controls should be distinguished: one is used to transfer large energy
or forces to a piece of machinery. Examples of this are the pedals on a bicycle or
the brake pedal in a heavier vehicle that does not have a power-assist feature. A
foot-operated control, such as an on-off switch, in which a control signal is
conveyed to the machinery, usually requires only a small quantity of force or
energy. While it is convenient to consider these two extremes of pedals, there are
various intermediate forms, and it is the task of the designer to determine which of
the following design recommendations apply best among them.

As mentioned above, repeated or continual pedal operation should be required only


from a seated operator. For controls meant to transmit large energies and forces,
the following rules apply:

· Locate pedals underneath the body, slightly in front, so that they can be
operated with the leg in a comfortable position. The total horizontal displacement
of a reciprocating pedal should normally not exceed about 0.15 m. For rotating
pedals, the radius should also be about 0.15 m. The linear displacement of a
switch-type pedal may be minimal and should not exceed about 0.15 m.

· Pedals should be so designed that the direction of travel and the foot force are
approximately in the line extending from the hip through the ankle joint of the
operator.

· Pedals that are operated by flexion and extension of the foot in the ankle joint
should be so arranged that in the normal position the angle between the lower leg
and the foot is approximately 90°; during operation, that angle may be increased to
about 120°.

· Foot-operated controls that simply provide signals to the machinery should


normally have two discrete positions, such as ON or OFF. Note, however, that
tactile distinction between the two positions may be difficult with the foot.

Selection of Controls

Selection among different sorts of controls must be made according to the


following needs or conditions:

· Operation by hand or foot

· Amounts of energies and forces transmitted

· Applying “continuous” inputs, such as steering an automobile

· Performing “discrete actions,” for example, (a) activating or shutting down


equipment, (b) selecting one of several distinct adjustments, such as switching
from one TV or radio channel to another, or (c) carrying out data entry, as with a
keyboard.

The functional usefulness of controls also determines selection procedures. The


main criteria are as follows:

· The control type shall be compatible with stereotypical or common


expectations (for instance, using a push-button or toggle switch to turn on an
electric light, not a rotary knob).

· Size and motion characteristics of the control shall be compatible with


stereotypical experience and past practice (for instance, providing a large steering
wheel for the two-handed operation of an automobile, not a lever).

· The direction of operation of a control shall be compatible with stereotypical or


common expectations (for instance, an ON control is pushed or pulled, not turned
to the left).

· Hand operation is used for controls that require small force and fine
adjustment, while foot operation is suitable for gross adjustments and large forces
(however, consider the common use of pedals, particularly accelerator pedals, in
automobiles, which does not comply with this principle).

· The control shall be “safe” in that it cannot be operated inadvertently nor in


ways that are excessive or inconsistent with its intended purpose.
Table 29.10 and table 29.11 help in the selection of proper controls. However,
note that there are few “natural” rules for selection and design of controls. Most
current recommendations are purely empirical and apply to existing devices and
Western stereotypes.

Table 29.10 Control movements and expected effects

Direction of control movement


Function Up Right Forward Clockwise Press, Down Left Rearward
Squeeze
On +3 + + + - +3
Off + - -
Right + -
Left +
Raise + -
Lower - +
Retract - +
Extend + -
Increase - - + -
Decrease - - +
Open -
Value
Close + -
Value

Blank: Not applicable; + Most preferred; – less preferred.


1
With trigger-type control. 2 With push-pull switch. 3 Up in the United States,
down in Europe.

Source: Modified from Kroemer 1995.

Table 29.11 Control-effect relations of common hand controls

Effect Key Toggle Push Bar Round Thumbwheel Thumbwheel


- switch - knob knob discrete continuous
loc butto
k n
Select ON/OFF + + + =
Select – + +
ON/STANDBY/O
FF
Select = – +
OFF/MODE1/MO
DE2
Select one – +
function of
several related
functions
Select one of three +
or more discrete
alternatives
Select operating + + –
condition
Engage or
disengage
Select one of +
mutually
exclusive
functions
Set value on scale + –
Select value in + + +
discrete steps

Blank: Not applicable; +: Most preferred; –: Less preferred; = Least preferred.


1
Estimated (no experiments known).

Source: Modified from Kroemer 1995.

Figure 29.41 presents examples of “detent” controls, characterized by discrete


detents or stops in which the control comes to rest. It also depicts typical
“continuous” controls where the control operation may take place anywhere within
the adjustment range, without the need to be set in any given position.

Figure 29.41 Some examples of "detent" and "continuous" controls


The sizing of controls is largely a matter of past experiences with various control
types, often guided by the desire to minimize the needed space in a control panel,
and either to allow simultaneous operations of adjacent controls or to avoid
inadvertent concurrent activation. Furthermore, the choice of design characteristics
will be influenced by such considerations as whether the controls are to be located
outdoors or in sheltered environments, in stationary equipment or moving vehicles,
or may involve the use of bare hands or of gloves and mittens. For these
conditions, consult readings at the end of the chapter.

Several operational rules govern the arrangement and grouping of controls. These
are listed in table 29.12 . For more details, check the references listed at the end of
this section and Kroemer, Kroemer and Kroemer-Elbert (1994).

Table 29.12 Rules for arrangement of controls

Locate for the Controls shall be oriented with respect to the operator. If the
ease of operation operator uses different postures (such as in driving and
operating a backhoe), the controls and their associated
displays shall move with the operator so that in each posture
their arrangement and operation is the same for the operator.
Primary controls The most important controls shall have the most
first advantageous locations to make operation and reaching easy
for the operator.
Group related Controls that are operated in sequence, that are related to a
controls together particular function, or that are operated together, shall be
arranged in functional groups (together with their associated
displays). Within each functional group, controls and
displays shall be arranged according to operational
importance and sequence.
Arrange for If operation of controls follows a given pattern, controls
sequential shall be arranged to facilitate that sequence. Common
operation arrangements are left-to-right (preferred) or top-to-bottom,
as in printed materials of the Western world.
Be consistent The arrangement of functionally identical or similar controls
shall be the same from panel to panel.
Dead-operator If the operator becomes incapacitated and either lets go of a
control control, or continues to hold on to it, a “deadman” control
design shall be utilized which either turns the system to a
non-critical operation state or shuts it down.
Select codes There are numerous ways to help identify controls, to
appropriately indicate the effects of the operation and to show their status.
Major coding means are:

–Location–Shape–Size–Mode of operation– Labels –


Colours–Redundancy

Source: Modified from Kroemer, Kroemer and Kroemer-Elbert 1994. Reproduced


by permission of Prentice-Hall. All rights reserved.

Preventing Accidental Operation

The following are the most important means to guard against inadvertent activation
of controls, some of which may be combined:

· Locate and orient the control so that the operator is unlikely to strike it or move
it accidentally in the normal sequence of control operations.

· Recess, shield or surround the control by physical barriers.

· Cover the control or guard it by providing a pin, a lock or other means that
must be removed or broken before the control can be operated.

· Provide extra resistance (by viscous or coulomb friction, by spring-loading or


by inertia) so that an unusual effort is required for actuation.

· Provide a “delaying” means so that the control must pass through a critical
position with an unusual movement (such as in the gear shift mechanism of an
automobile).

· Provide interlocking between controls so that prior operation of a related


control is required before the critical control can be activated.
Note that these designs usually slow the operation of controls, which may be
detrimental in case of an emergency.

Data Entry Devices

Nearly all controls can be used to enter data on a computer or other data storage
device. However, we are most used to the practice of using a keyboard with push-
buttons. On the original typewriter keyboard, which has become the standard even
for computer keyboards, the keys were arranged in a basically alphabetic sequence,
which has been modified for various, often obscure, reasons. In some cases, letters
which frequently follow each other in common text were spaced apart so that the
original mechanical type bars might not entangle if struck in rapid sequence.
“Columns” of keys run in roughly straight lines, as do the “rows” of keys.
However, the fingertips are not aligned in such manners, and do not move in this
way when digits of the hand are flexed or extended, or moved sideways.

Many attempts have been made over the last hundred years to improve keying
performance by changing the keyboard layout. These include relocating keys
within the standard layout, or changing the keyboard layout altogether. The
keyboard has been divided into separate sections, and sets of keys (such as
numerical pads) have been added. Arrangements of adjacent keys may be changed
by altering spacing, offset from each other or from reference lines. The keyboard
may be divided into sections for the left and right hand, and those sections may be
laterally tilted and sloped and slanted.

The dynamics of the operation of push-button keys are important for the user, but
are difficult to measure in operation. Thus, the force-displacement characteristics
of keys are commonly described for static testing, which is not indicative of actual
operation. By current practise, keys on computer keyboards have fairly little
displacement (about 2 mm) and display a “snap-back” resistance, that is, a decrease
in operation force at the point when actuation of the key has been achieved. Instead
of separate single keys, some keyboards consist of a membrane with switches
underneath which, when pressed in the correct location, generate the desired input
with little or no displacement felt. The major advantage of the membrane is that
dust or fluids cannot penetrate it; however, many users dislike it.

There are alternatives to the “one key-one character” principle; instead, one can
generate inputs by various combinatory means. One is “chording”, meaning that
two or more controls are operated simultaneously to generate one character. This
poses demands on the memory capabilities of the operator, but requires the use of
only very few keys. Other developments utilize controls other than the binary
tapped push button, replacing it by levers, toggles or special sensors (such as an
instrumented glove) which respond to movements of the digits of the hand.
By tradition, typing and computer entry have been made by mechanical interaction
between the operator’s fingers and such devices as keyboard, mouse, track ball or
light pen. Yet there are many other means to generate inputs. Voice recognition
appears one promising technique, but other methods can be employed. They might
utilize, for example, pointing, gestures, facial expressions, body movements,
looking (directing one’s gaze), movements of the tongue, breathing or sign
language to transmit information and to generate inputs to a computer. Technical
development in this area is very much in flux, and as the many nontraditional input
devices used for computer games indicate, acceptance of devices other than the
traditional binary tap-down keyboard is entirely feasible within the near future.
Discussions of current keyboard devices have been provided, for example, by
Kroemer (1994b) and McIntosh (1994).

Displays

Displays provide information about the status of equipment. Displays may apply to
the operator’s visual sense (lights, scales, counters, cathode-ray tubes, flat panel
electronics, etc.), to the auditory sense (bells, horns, recorded voice messages,
electronically generated sounds, etc.) or to the sense of touch (shaped controls,
Braille, etc.). Labels, written instructions, warnings or symbols (“icons”) may be
considered special kinds of displays.

The four “cardinal rules” for displays are:

1. Display only that information which is essential for adequate job


performance.

2. Display information only as accurately as is required for the operator’s


decisions and actions.

3. Present information in the most direct, simple, understandable and usable


form.

4. Present information in such a way that failure or malfunction of the display


itself will be immediately obvious.

The selection of either an auditory or visual display depends on the prevailing


conditions and purposes. The objective of the display may be to provide:

· historical information about the past state of the system, such as the course run
by a ship

· status information about the current state of the system, such as the text already
input into a word processor or the current position of an airplane
· predictive information, such as on the future position of a ship, given certain
steering settings

· instructions or commands telling the operator what to do, and possibly how to
do it.

A visual display is most appropriate if the environment is noisy, the operator stays
in place, the message is long and complex, and especially if it deals with the spatial
location of an object. An auditory display is appropriate if the workplace must be
kept dark, the operator moves around, and the message is short and simple,
requires immediate attention, and deals with events and time.

Visual Displays

There are three basic types of visual displays: (1)The check display indicates
whether or not a given condition exists (for example a green light indicates normal
function). (2)The qualitative display indicates the status of a changing variable or
its approximate value, or its trend of change (for example, a pointer moves within a
“normal” range). (3) The quantitative display shows exact information that must be
ascertained (for example, to find a location on a map, to read text or to draw on a
computer monitor), or it may indicate an exact numerical value that must be read
by the operator (for example, a time or a temperature).

Design guidelines for visual displays are:

· Arrange displays so that the operator can locate and identify them easily
without unnecessary searching. (This usually means that the displays should be in
or near the medial plane of the operator, and below or at eye height.)

· Group displays functionally or sequentially so that the operator can use them
easily.

· Make sure that all displays are properly illuminated or illuminant, coded and
labelled according to their function.

· Use lights, often coloured, to indicate the status of a system (such as ON or


OFF) or to alert the operator that the system, or a subsystem, is inoperative and that
special action must be taken. Common meanings of light colours are listed
in figure 29.42 . Flashing red indicates an emergency condition that requires
immediate action. An emergency signal is most effective when it combines sounds
with a flashing red light.

Figure 29.42 Colour coding of indicator lights


For more complex and detailed information, especially quantitative information,
one of four different kinds of displays are traditionally used: (1) a moving pointer
(with fixed scale), (2) a moving scale (with fixed pointer), (3) counters or (4)
“pictorial” displays, especially computer-generated on a display monitor. Figure
29.43 lists the major characteristics of these display types.

Figure 29.43 Characteristics of displays


It is usually preferable to use a moving pointer rather than a moving scale, with the
scale either straight (horizontally or vertically arranged), curved or circular. Scales
should be simple and uncluttered, with graduation and numbering so designed that
correct readings can be taken quickly. Numerals should be located outside the scale
markings so that they are not obscured by the pointer. The pointer should end with
its tip directly at the marking. The scale should mark divisions only so finely as the
operator must read. All major marks should be numbered. Progressions are best
marked with intervals of one, five or ten units between major marks. Numbers
should increase left to right, bottom to top or clockwise. For details of dimensions
of scales refer to standards such as those listed by Cushman and Rosenberg 1991
or Kroemer 1994a.

Starting in the 1980s, mechanical displays with pointers and printed scales were
increasingly replaced by “electronic” displays with computer-generated images, or
solid-state devices using light-emitting diodes (see Snyder 1985a). The displayed
information may be coded by the following means:

· shapes, such as straight or circular

· alphanumeric, that is, letters, numbers, words, abbreviations

· figures, pictures, pictorials, icons, symbols, in various levels of abstraction,


such as the outline of an airplane against the horizon

· shades of black, white or gray

· colours.

Unfortunately, many electronically generated displays have been fuzzy, often


overly complex and colourful, hard to read, and required exact focusing and close
attention, which may distract from the main task, for example, driving a car. In
these cases the first three of the four “cardinal rules” listed above were often
violated. Furthermore, many electronically generated pointers, markings and
alphanumerics did not comply with established ergonomic design guidelines,
especially when generated by line segments, scan lines or dot matrices. Although
some of these defective designs were tolerated by the users, rapid innovation and
improving display techniques allows many better solutions. However, the same
rapid development leads to the fact that printed statements (even if current and
comprehensive when they appear) are becoming obsolete quickly. Therefore, none
are given in this text. Compilations have been published by Cushman and
Rosenberg (1991), Kinney and Huey (1990), and Woodson, Tillman and Tillman
(1991).

The overall quality of electronic displays is often wanting. One measure used to
assess the image quality is the modulation transfer function (MTF) (Snyder
1985b). It describes the resolution of the display using a special sine-wave test
signal; yet, readers have many criteria regarding the preference of displays (Dillon
1992).
Monochrome displays have only one colour, usually either green, yellow, amber,
orange or white (achromatic). If several colours appear on the same chromatic
display, they should be easily discriminated. It is best to display not more than
three or four colours simultaneously (with preference being given to red, green,
yellow or orange, and cyan or purple). All should strongly contrast with the
background. In fact, a suitable rule is to design first by contrast, that is, in terms of
black and white, and then to add colours sparingly.

In spite of the many variables that, singly and interacting with each other, affect the
use of complex colour display, Cushman and Rosenberg (1991) compiled
guidelines for use of colour in displays; these are listed in figure 29.44 .

Figure 29.44 Guidelines for use of colours in displays

Other suggestions are as follows:

· Blue (preferably desaturated) is a good colour for backgrounds and large


shapes. However, blue should not be used for text, thin lines or small shapes.

· The colour of alphanumeric characters should contrast with that of the


background.
· When using colour, use shape as a redundant cue (e.g., all yellow symbols are
triangles, all green symbols are circles, all red symbols are squares). Redundant
coding makes the display much more acceptable for users who have colour-vision
deficiencies.

· As the number of colours is increased, the sizes of the colour-coded objects


should also be increased.

· Red and green should not be used for small symbols and small shapes in
peripheral areas of large displays.

· Using opponent colours (red and green, yellow and blue) adjacent to one
another or in an object/background relationship is sometimes beneficial and
sometimes detrimental. No general guidelines can be given; a solution should be
determined for each case.

· Avoid displaying several highly saturated, spectrally extreme colours at the


same time.

Panels of Controls and Displays

Displays as well as controls should be arranged in panels so they are in front of the
operator, that is, close to the person’s medial plane. As discussed earlier, controls
should be near elbow height, and displays below or at eye height, whether the
operator is sitting or standing. Infrequently operated controls, or less important
displays, can be located further to the sides, or higher.

Often, information on the result of control operation is displayed on an instrument.


In this case, the display should be located close to the control so that the control
setting can be done without error, quickly and conveniently. The assignment is
usually clearest when the control is directly below or to the right of the display.
Care must be taken that the hand does not cover the display when operating the
control.

Popular expectancies of control-display relations exist, but they are often learned,
they may depend on the user’s cultural background and experience, and these
relationships are often not strong. Expected movement relationships are influenced
by the type of control and display. When both are either linear or rotary, the
stereotypical expectation is that they move in corresponding directions, such as
both up or both clockwise. When the movements are incongruent, in general the
following rules apply:

· Clockwise for increase. Turning the control clockwise causes an increase in the
displayed value.
· Warrick’s gear-slide rule. A display (pointer) is expected to move in the same
direction as does the side of the control close to (i.e., geared with) the display.

The ratio of control and display displacement (C/D ratio or D/C gain) describes
how much a control must be moved to adjust a display. If much control movement
produces only a small display motion, once speaks of a high C/D ratio, and of the
control as having low sensitivity. Often, two distinct movements are involved in
making a setting: first a fast primary (“slewing”) motion to an approximate
location, then a fine adjustment to the exact setting. In some cases, one takes as the
optimal C/D ratio that which minimizes the sum of these two movements.
However, the most suitable ratio depends on the given circumstances; it must be
determined for each application.

Labels and Warnings

Labels

Ideally, no label should be required on equipment or on a control to explain its use.


Often, however, it is necessary to use labels so that one may locate, identify, read
or manipulate controls, displays or other equipment items. Labelling must be done
so that the information is provided accurately and rapidly. For this, the guidelines
in table 29.13 apply.

Table 29.13 Guidelines for labels

Orientation A label and the information printed on it shall be


oriented horizontally so that it can be read quickly
and easily. (Note that this applies if the operator is
used to reading horizontally, as in Western
countries.)
Location A label shall be placed on or very near the item that it
identifies.
Standardization Placement of all labels shall be consistent throughout
the equipment and system.
Equipment functions A label shall primarily describe the function (“what
does it do”) of the labelled item.
Abbreviations Common abbreviations may be used. If a new
abbreviation is necessary, its meaning should be
obvious to the reader. The same abbreviation shall be
used for all tenses and for the singular and plural
forms of a word. Capital letters shall be used, periods
normally omitted.
Brevity The label inscription shall be as concise as possible
without distorting the intended meaning or
information. The texts shall be unambiguous,
redundancy minimized.
Familiarity Words shall be chosen, if possible, that are familiar to
the operator.
Visibility and The operator shall be able to be read easily and
legibility accurately at the anticipated actual reading distances,
at the anticipated worst illumination level, and within
the anticipated vibration and motion environment.
Important factors are: contrast between the lettering
and its background; the height, width, strokewidth,
spacing and style of letters; and the specular
reflection of the background, cover or other
components.
Font and size Typography determines the legibility of written
information; it refers to style, font, arrangement and
appearance.

Source: Modified from Kroemer, Kroemer and Kroemer-Elbert 1994 (reproduced


by permission of Prentice-Hall; all rights reserved).

Font (typeface) should be simple, bold and vertical, such as Futura, Helvetica,
Namel, Tempo and Vega. Note that most electronically generated fonts (formed by
LED, LCD or dot matrix) are generally inferior to printed fonts; thus, special
attention must be paid to making these as legible as possible.

· The height of characters depends on the viewing distance:

viewing distance 35 cm, suggested height 22 mm

viewing distance 70 cm, suggested height 50 mm

viewing distance 1 m, suggested height 70 mm

viewing distance 1.5 m, suggested height at least 1 cm.

· The ratio of strokewidth to character height should be between 1:8 to 1:6 for
black letters on white background, and 1:10 to 1:8 for white letters on black
background.

· The ratio of character width to character height should be about 3:5.

· The space between letters should be at least one stroke width.

· The space between words should be at least one character width.


· For continuous text, mix upper- and lower-case letters; for labels, use upper-
case letters only.

Warnings

Ideally, all devices should be safe to use. In reality, often this cannot be achieved
through design. In this case, one must warn users of the dangers associated with
product use and provide instructions for safe use to prevent injury or damage.

It is preferable to have an “active” warning, usually consisting of a sensor that


notices inappropriate use, combined with an alerting device that warns the human
of an impending danger. Yet, in most cases, “passive” warnings are used, usually
consisting of a label attached to the product and of instructions for safe use in the
user manual. Such passive warnings rely completely on the human user to
recognize an existing or potential dangerous situation, to remember the warning,
and to behave prudently.

Labels and signs for passive warnings must be carefully designed by following the
most recent government laws and regulations, national and international standards,
and the best applicable human engineering information. Warning labels and
placards may contain text, graphics, and pictures—often graphics with redundant
text. Graphics, particularly pictures and pictograms, can be used by persons with
different cultural and language backgrounds, if these depictions are selected
carefully. However, users with different ages, experiences, and ethnic and
educational backgrounds, may have rather different perceptions of dangers and
warnings. Therefore, design of a safe product is much preferable to applying
warnings to an inferior product.

INFORMATION PROCESSING AND DESIGN


Andries F. Sanders

In designing equipment it is of the utmost importance to take full account of the


fact that a human operator has both capabilities and limitations in processing
information, which are of a varying nature and which are found on various levels.
Performance in actual work conditions strongly depends on the extent to which a
design has either attended to or ignored these potentials and their limits. In the
following a brief sketch will be offered of some of the chief issues. Reference will
be made to other contributions of this volume, where an issue will be discussed in
greater detail.

It is common to distinguish three main levels in the analysis of human information


processing, namely, the perceptual level, the decision level and the motor level.
The perceptual level is subdivided into three further levels, relating to sensory
processing, feature extraction and identification of the percept. On the decision
level, the operator receives perceptual information and chooses a reaction to it
which is finally programmed and actualized on the motor level. This describes only
the information flow in the simplest case of a choice reaction. It is evident, though,
that perceptual information may accumulate and be combined and diagnosed
before eliciting an action. Again, there may arise a need for selecting information
in view of perceptual overload. Finally, choosing an appropriate action becomes
more of a problem when there are several options some of which may be more
appropriate than others. In the present discussion, the emphasis will be on the
perceptual and decisional factors of information processing.

Perceptual Capabilities and Limits

Sensory limits

The first category of processing limits is sensory. Their relevance to information


processing is obvious since processing becomes less reliable as information
approaches threshold limits. This may seem a fairly trivial statement, but
nonetheless, sensory problems are not always clearly recognized in designs. For
example, alphanumerical characters in sign posting systems should be sufficiently
large to be legible at a distance consistent with the need for appropriate action.
Legibility, in turn, depends not only on the absolute size of the alphanumericals but
also on contrast and—in view of lateral inhibition—also on the total amount of
information on the sign. In particular, in conditions of low visibility (e.g., rain or
fog during driving or flying) legibility is a considerable problem requiring
additional measures. More recently developed traffic signposts and road markers
are usually well designed, but signposts near and within buildings are often
illegible. Visual display units are another example in which sensory limits of size,
contrast and amount of information play an important role. In the auditory domain
some main sensory problems are related to understanding speech in noisy
environments or in poor quality audio transmission systems.

Feature extraction

Provided sufficient sensory information, the next set of information processing


issues relates to extracting features from the information presented. Most recent
research has shown ample evidence that an analysis of features precedes the
perception of meaningful wholes. Feature analysis is particularly useful in locating
a special deviant object amidst many others. For instance, an essential value on a
display containing many values may be represented by a single deviant colour or
size, which feature then draws immediate attention or “pops out”. Theoretically,
there is the common assumption of “feature maps” for different colours, sizes,
forms and other physical features. The attention value of a feature depends on the
difference in activation of the feature maps that belong to the same class, for
example, colour. Thus, the activation of a feature map depends on the
discriminability of the deviant features. This means that when there are a few
instances of many colours on a screen, most colour feature maps are about equally
activated, which has the effect that none of the colours pops out.

In the same way a single moving advertisement pops out, but this effect disappears
altogether when there are several moving stimuli in the field of view. The principle
of the different activation of feature maps is also applied when aligning pointers
that indicate ideal parameter values. A deviation of a pointer is indicated by a
deviant slope which is rapidly detected. If this is impossible to realize, a dangerous
deviation might be indicated by a change in colour. Thus, the general rule for
design is to use only a very few deviant features on a screen and to reserve them
only for the most essential information. Searching for relevant information
becomes cumbersome in the case of conjunctions of features. For example, it is
hard to locate a large red object amidst small red objects and large and small green
objects. If possible, conjunctions should be avoided when trying to design for
efficient search.

Separable versus integral dimensions

Features are separable when they can be changed without affecting the perception
of other features of an object. Line lengths of histograms are a case in point. On the
other hand, integral features refer to features which, when changed, change the
total appearance of the object. For instance, one cannot change features of the
mouth in a schematic drawing of a face without altering the total appearance of the
picture. Again, colour and brightness are integral in the sense that one cannot
change a colour without altering the brightness impression at the same time. The
principles of separable and integral features, and of emergent properties evolving
from changes of single features of an object, are applied in so-called integrated or
diagnostic displays. The rationale of these displays is that, rather than displaying
individual parameters, different parameters are integrated into a single display, the
total composition of which indicates what may be actually wrong with a system.

Data presentation in control rooms is still often dominated by the philosophy that
each individual measure should have its own indicator. Piecemeal presentation of
the measures means that the operator has the task of integrating the evidence from
the various individual displays so as to diagnose a potential problem. At the time of
the problems in the Three Mile Island nuclear power plant in the United States
some forty to fifty displays were registering some form of disorder. Thus, the
operator had the task of diagnosing what was actually wrong by integrating the
information from that myriad of displays. Integral displays may be helpful in
diagnosing the kind of error, since they combine various measures into a single
pattern. Different patterns of the integrated display, then, may be diagnostic with
regard to specific errors.

A classical example of a diagnostic display, which has been proposed for nuclear
control rooms, is shown in figure 29.45 . It displays a number of measures as
spokes of equal length so that a regular polygon always represents normal
conditions, while different distortions may be connected with different types of
problems in the process.

Figure 29.45 In the normal situation all parameter values are equal, creating a
hexagon. In the deviation, some of the values have changed creating a specific
distortion.

Not all integral displays are equally discriminable. To illustrate the issue, a positive
correlation between the two dimensions of a rectangle creates differences in
surface, while maintaining an equal shape. Alternatively, a negative correlation
creates differences in shape while maintaining an equal surface. The case in which
variation of integral dimensions creates a new shape has been referred to as
revealing an emergent property of the patterning, which adds to the operator’s
ability to discriminate the patterns. Emergent properties depend upon the identity
and arrangement of parts but are not identifiable with any single part.

Object and configural displays are not always beneficial. The very fact that they
are integral means that the characteristics of the individual variables are harder to
perceive. The point is that, by definition, integral dimensions are mutually
dependent, thus clouding their individual constituents. There may be circumstances
in which this is unacceptable, while one may still wish to profit from the diagnostic
patternlike properties, which are typical for the object display. One compromise
might be a traditional bar graph display. On the one hand, bar graphs are quite
separable. Yet, when positioned in sufficiently close vicinity, the differential
lengths of the bars may together constitute an object-like pattern which may well
serve a diagnostic aim.

Some diagnostic displays are better than others. Their quality depends on the
extent that the display corresponds to the mental model of the task. For example,
fault diagnosis on the basis of distortions of a regular polygon, as in figure
29.45 [ERG45FE], may still bear little relationship to the domain semantics or to
the concept of the operator of the processes in a power plant. Thus, various types
of deviations of the polygon do not obviously refer to a specific problem in the
plant. Therefore, the design of the most suitable configural display is one that
corresponds to the specific mental model of the task. Thus it should be emphasized
that the surface of a rectangle is only a useful object display when the product of
length and width is the variable of interest!

Interesting object displays stem from three-dimensional representations. For


instance, a three-dimensional representation of air traffic—rather than the
traditional two-dimensional radar representation—may provide the pilot with a
greater “situational awareness” of other traffic. The three-dimensional display has
been shown to be much superior to a two-dimensional one since its symbols
indicate whether another aircraft is above or below one’s own.

Degraded conditions

Degraded viewing occurs under a variety of conditions. For some purposes, as with
camouflage, objects are intentionally degraded so as to prevent their identification.
On other occasions, for example in brightness amplification, features may become
too blurred to allow one to identify the object. One research issue has concerned
the minimal number of “lines” required on a screen or “the amount of detail”
needed in order to avoid degradation. Unfortunately, this approach to image quality
has not led to unequivocal results. The problem is that identifying degraded stimuli
(e.g., a camouflaged armoured vehicle) depends too much on the presence or
absence of minor object-specific details. The consequence is that no general
prescription about line density can be formulated, except for the trivial statement
that degradation decreases as the density increases.
Features of alphanumeric symbols

A major issue in the process of feature extraction concerns the actual number of
features which together define a stimulus. Thus, the legibility of ornate characters
like Gothic letters is poor because of the many redundant curves. In order to avoid
confusion, the difference between letters with very similar features—like the i and
the l, and the c and the e—should be accentuated. For the same reason, it is
recommended to make the stroke and tail length of ascenders and descenders at
least 40% of the total letter height.

It is evident that discrimination among letters is mainly determined by the number


of features which they do not share. These mainly consist of straight line and
circular segments which may have horizontal, vertical and oblique orientation and
which may differ in size, as in lower- and upper-case letters.

It is obvious that, even when alphanumericals are well discriminable, they may
easily lose that property in combination with other items. Thus, the digits 4 and 7
share only a few features but they do not do well in the context of larger otherwise
identical groups (e.g., 384 versus 387) There is unanimous evidence that reading
text in lower case is faster than in capitals. This is usually ascribed to the fact that
lower case letters have more distinct features (e.g., dog, cat versus DOG, CAT).
The superiority of lower case letters has not only been established for reading text
but also for road signs such as those used for indicating towns at the exits of
motorways.

Identification

The final perceptual process is concerned with identification and interpretation of


percepts. Human limits arising on this level are usually related to discrimination
and finding the appropriate interpretation of the percept. The applications of
research on visual discrimination are manifold, relating to alphanumerical patterns
as well as to more general stimulus identification. The design of brake lights in
cars will serve as an example of the last category. Rear-end accidents account for a
considerable proportion of traffic accidents, and are due in part to the fact that the
traditional location of the brake light next to the rear lights makes it poorly
discriminable and therefore extends the driver’s reaction time. As an alternative, a
single light has been developed which appears to reduce the accident rate. It is
mounted in the centre of the rear window at approximately eye level. In
experimental studies on the road, the effect of the central braking light appears to
be less when subjects are aware of the aim of the study, suggesting that stimulus
identification in the traditional configuration improves when subjects focus on the
task. Despite the positive effect of the isolated brake light, its identification might
still be further improved by making the brake light more meaningful, giving it the
form of an exclamation mark, “!”, or even an icon.
Absolute judgement

Very strict and often counterintuitive performance limits arise in cases of absolute
judgement of physical dimensions. Examples occur in connection with colour
coding of objects and the use of tones in auditory call systems. The point is that
relative judgement is far superior to absolute judgement. The problem with
absolute judgement is that the code has to be translated into another category. Thus
a specific colour may be linked with an electrical resistance value or a specific tone
may be intended for a person for which an ensuing message is meant. In fact,
therefore, the problem is not one of perceptual identification but rather of response
choice, which will be discussed later in this article. At this point it suffices to
remark that one should not use more than four or five colours or pitches so as to
avoid errors. When more alternatives are needed one may add extra dimensions,
like loudness, duration and components of tones.

Word reading

The relevance of reading separate word units in traditional print is demonstrated by


various widely experienced evidence, such as the fact that reading is very much
hampered when spaces are omitted, printing errors remain often undetected, and it
is very hard to read words in alternating cases (e.g., ALTeRnAtInG). Some
investigators have emphasized the role of word shape in reading word units and
suggested that spatial frequency analysers may be relevant in identifying word
shape. In this view meaning would be derived from total word shape rather than by
letter-by-letter analysis. Yet, the contribution of word shape analysis is probably
limited to small common words—articles and endings—which is consistent with
the finding that printing errors in small words and endings have a relatively low
probability of detection.

Text in lower case has an advantage over upper case which is due to a loss of
features in the upper case. Yet, the advantage of lower case words is absent or may
even be reversed when searching for a single word. It could be that factors of letter
size and letter case are confounded in searching: Larger-sized letters are detected
more rapidly, which may offset the disadvantage of less distinctive features. Thus,
a single word may be about equally legible in upper case as in lower case, while
continuous text is read faster in lower case. Detecting a SINGLE capital word
amidst many lower case words is very efficient, since it evokes pop-out. An even
more efficient fast detection can be achieved by printing a single lower case word
in bold, in which case the advantages of pop-out and of more distinctive features
are combined.

The role of encoding features in reading is also clear from the impaired legibility of
older low-resolution visual display unit screens, which consisted of fairly rough
dot matrices and could portray alphanumericals only as straight lines. The common
finding was that reading text or searching from a low-resolution monitor was
considerably slower than from a paper-printed copy. The problem has largely
disappeared with the present-day higher-resolution screens. Besides letter form
there are a number of additional differences between reading from paper and
reading from a screen. The spacing of the lines, the size of the characters, the type
face, the contrast ratio between characters and background, the viewing distance,
the amount of flicker and the fact that changing pages on a screen is done by
scrolling are some examples. The common finding that reading is slower from
computer screens—although comprehension seems about equal—may be due to
some combination of these factors. Present-day text processors usually offer a
variety of options in font, size, colour, format and style; such choices could give
the false impression that personal taste is the major reason.

Icons versus words

In some studies the time taken by a subject in naming a printed word was found to
be faster than that for a corresponding icon, while both times were about equally
fast in other studies. It has been suggested that words are read faster than icons
since they are less ambiguous. Even a fairly simple icon, like a house, may still
elicit different responses among subjects, resulting in response conflict and, hence,
a decrease in reaction speed. If response conflict is avoided by using really
unambiguous icons the difference in response speed is likely to disappear. It is
interesting to note that as traffic signs, icons are usually much superior to words,
even in the case where the issue of understanding language is not seen as a
problem. This paradox may be due to the fact that the legibility of traffic signs is
largely a matter of the distance at which a sign can be identified. If properly
designed, this distance is larger for symbols than for words, since pictures can
provide considerably larger differences in shape and contain less fine details than
words. The advantage of pictures, then, arises from the fact that discrimination of
letters requires some ten to twelve minutes of arc and that feature detection is the
initial prerequisite for discrimination. At the same time it is clear that the
superiority of symbols is only guaranteed when (1) they do indeed contain little
detail, (2) they are sufficiently distinct in shape and (3) they are unambiguous.

Capabilities and Limits for Decision

Once a precept has been identified and interpreted it may call for an action. In this
context the discussion will be limited to deterministic stimulus-response relations,
or, in other words, to conditions in which each stimulus has its own fixed response.
In that case the major problems for equipment design arise from issues of
compatibility, that is, the extent to which the identified stimulus and its related
response have a “natural” or well-practised relationship. There are conditions in
which an optimal relation is intentionally aborted, as in the case of abbreviations.
Usually a contraction like abrvtin is much worse than a truncation like abbrev.
Theoretically, this is due to the increasing redundancy of successive letters in a
word, which allows “filling out” final letters on the basis of earlier ones; a
truncated word can profit from this principle while a contracted one cannot.

Mental models and compatibility

In most compatibility problems there are stereotypical responses derived from


generalized mental models. Choosing the null position in a circular display is a
case in point. The 12 o’clock and 9 o’clock positions appear to be corrected faster
than the 6 o’clock and 3 o’clock positions. The reason may be that a clockwise
deviation and a movement in the upper part in the display are experienced as
“increases” requiring a response that reduces the value. In the 3 and 6 o’clock
positions both principles conflict and they may therefore be handled less
efficiently. A similar stereotype is found in locking or opening the rear door of a
car. Most people act on the stereotype that locking requires a clockwise movement.
If the lock is designed in the opposite way, continuous errors and frustration in
trying to lock the door are the most likely result.

With respect to control movements the well-known Warrick’s principle on


compatibility describes the relation between the location of a control knob and the
direction of the movement on a display. If the control knob is located to the right of
the display, a clockwise movement is supposed to move the scale marker up. Or
consider moving window displays. According to most people’s mental model, the
upward direction of a moving display suggests that the values go up in the same
way in which a rising temperature in a thermometer is indicated by a higher
mercury column. There are problems in implementing this principle with a “fixed
pointer-moving scale” indicator. When the scale in such an indicator moves down,
its value is intended to increasing. Thus a conflict with the common stereotype
occurs. If the values are inverted, the low values are on the top of the scale, which
is also contrary to most stereotypes.

The term proximity compatibility refers to the correspondence of symbolic


representations to people’s mental models of functional or even spatial
relationships within a system. Issues of proximity compatibility are more pressing
as the mental model of a situation is more primitive, global or distorted. Thus, a
flow diagram of a complex automated industrial process is often displayed on the
basis of a technical model which may not correspond at all with the mental model
of the process. In particular, when the mental model of a process is incomplete or
distorted, a technical representation of the progress adds little to develop or correct
it. A daily-life example of poor proximity compatibility is an architectural map of a
building that is intended for viewer orientation or for showing fire escape routes.
These maps are usually entirely inadequate—full of irrelevant details—in
particular for people who have only a global mental model of the building. Such
convergence between map reading and orientation comes close to what has been
called “situational awareness”, which is particularly relevant in three-dimensional
space during an air flight. There have been interesting recent developments in
three-dimensional object displays, representing attempts to achieve optimal
proximity compatibility in this domain.

Stimulus-response compatibility

An example of stimulus-response (S-R) compatibility is typically found in the case


of most text processing programs, which assume that operators know how
commands correspond to specific key combinations. The problem is that a
command and its corresponding key combination usually fail to have any pre-
existing relation, which means that the S-R relations must be learned by a
painstaking process of paired-associate learning. The result is that, even after the
skill has been acquired, the task remains error-prone. The internal model of the
program remains incomplete since less practised operations are liable to be
forgotten, so that the operator can simply not come up with the appropriate
response. Also, the text produced on the screen usually does not correspond in all
respects to what finally appears on the printed page, which is another example of
inferior proximity compatibility. Only a few programs utilize a stereotypical spatial
internal model in connection with stimulus-response relations for controlling
commands.

It has been correctly argued that there are much better pre-existing relations
between spatial stimuli and manual responses—like the relation between a pointing
response and a spatial location, or like that between verbal stimuli and vocal
responses. There is ample evidence that spatial and verbal representations are
relatively separate cognitive categories with little mutual interference but also with
little mutual correspondence. Hence, a spatial task, like formatting a text, is most
easily performed by spatial mouse-type movement, thus leaving the keyboard for
verbal commands.

This does not mean that the keyboard is ideal for carrying out verbal commands.
Typing remains a matter of manually operating arbitrary spatial locations which
are basically incompatible with processing letters. It is actually another example of
a highly incompatible task which is only mastered by extensive practise, and the
skill is easily lost without continuous practice. A similar argument can be made for
shorthand writing, which also consists of connecting arbitrary written symbols to
verbal stimuli. An interesting example of an alternative method of keyboard
operation is a chording keyboard.

The operator handles two keyboards (one for the left and one for the right hand)
both consisting of six keys. Each letter of the alphabet corresponds to a chording
response, that is, a combination of keys. The results of studies on such a keyboard
showed striking savings in the time needed for acquiring typing skills. Motor
limitations limited the maximal speed of the chording technique but, still, once
learned, operator performance approached the speed of the conventional technique
quite closely.
A classical example of a spatial compatibility effect concerns the traditional
arrangements of stove burner controls: four burners in a 2 × 2 matrix, with the
controls in a horizontal row. In this configuration, the relations between burner and
control are not obvious and are poorly learned. However, despite many errors, the
problem of lighting the stove, given time, can usually be solved. The situation is
worse when one is faced with undefined display-control relations. Other examples
of poor S-R compatibility are found in the display-control relations of video
cameras, video recorders and television sets. The effect is that many options are
never used or must be studied anew at each new trial. The claim that “it is all
explained in the manual”, while true, is not useful since, in practice, most manuals
are incomprehensible to the average user, in particular when they attempt to
describe actions using incompatible verbal terms.

Stimulus-stimulus (S-S) and response-response (R-R) compatibility

Originally S-S and R-R compatibility were distinguished from S-R compatibility.
A classical illustration of S-S compatibility concerns attempts in the late forties to
support auditory sonar by a visual display in an effort to enhance signal detection.
One solution was sought in a horizontal light beam with vertical perturbations
travelling from left to right and reflecting a visual translation of the auditory
background noise and potential signal. A signal consisted of a slightly larger
vertical perturbation. The experiments showed that a combination of the auditory
and visual displays did not do better than the single auditory display. The reason
was sought in a poor S-S compatibility: the auditory signal is perceived as a
loudness change; hence visual support should correspond most when provided in
the form of a brightness change, since that is the compatible visual analogue of a
loudness change.

It is of interest that the degree of S-S compatibility corresponds directly to how


skilled subjects are in cross-modality matching. In a cross-modality match,
subjects may be asked to indicate which auditory loudness corresponds to a certain
brightness or to a certain weight; this approach has been popular in research on
scaling sensory dimensions, since it allows one to avoid mapping sensory stimuli
to numerals. R-R compatibility refers to correspondence of simultaneous and also
of successive movements. Some movements are more easily coordinated than
others, which provides clear constraints for the way a succession of actions—for
example, successive operation of controls—is most efficiently done.

The above examples show clearly how compatibility issues pervade all user-
machine interfaces. The problem is that the effects of poor compatibility are often
softened by extended practice and so may remain unnoticed or underestimated.
Yet, even when incompatible display-control relations are well-practised and do
not seem to affect performance, there remains the point of a larger error
probability. The incorrect compatible response remains a competitor for the correct
incompatible one and is likely to come through on occasion, with the obvious risk
of an accident. In addition, the amount of practice required for mastering
incompatible S-R relations is formidable and a waste of time.

Limits of Motor Programming and Execution

One limit in motor programming was already briefly touched upon in the remarks
on R-R compatibility. The human operator has clear problems in carrying out
incongruent movement sequences, and in particular, changing from the one to
another incongruent sequence is hard to accomplish. The results of studies on
motor coordination are relevant to the design of controls in which both hands are
active. Yet, practice can overcome much in this regard, as is clear from the
surprising levels of acrobatic skills.

Many common principles in the design of controls derive from motor


programming. They include the incorporation of resistance in a control and the
provision of feedback indicating that it has been properly operated. A preparatory
motor state is a highly relevant determinant of reaction time. Reacting to an
unexpected sudden stimulus may take an additional second or so, which is
considerable when a fast reaction is needed—as in reacting to a lead car’s brake
light. Unprepared reactions are probably a main cause of chain collisions. Early
warning signals are beneficial in preventing such collisions. A major application of
research on movement execution concerns Fitt’s law, which relates movement,
distance and the size of the target that is aimed at. This law appears to be quite
general, applying equally to an operating lever, a joystick, a mouse or a light pen.
Among others, it has been applied to estimate the time needed to make corrections
on computer screens.

There is obviously much more to say than the above sketchy remarks. For instance,
the discussion has been almost fully limited to issues of information flow on the
level of a simple choice reaction. Issues beyond choice reactions have not been
touched upon, nor problems of feedback and feed forward in the ongoing
monitoring of information and motor activity. Many of the issues mentioned bear a
strong relation to problems of memory and of planning of behaviour, which have
not been addressed either. More extensive discussions are found in Wickens
(1992), for example.

DESIGNING FOR SPECIFIC GROUPS


Joke H. Grady-van den Nieuwboer

In designing a product or an industrial process, one focuses on the “average” and


“healthy” worker. Information regarding human abilities in terms of muscular
strength, bodily flexibility, length of reach, and many other characteristics is for
the most part derived from empirical studies carried out by military recruitment
agencies, and reflects measured values valid for the typical young male in his
twenties. But working populations, to be sure, consist of people of both sexes and a
broad range of ages, to say nothing of a variety of physical types and abilities,
levels of fitness and health, and functional capacities. A classification of the
varieties of functional limitation among people as outlined by the World Health
Organization is given in the accompanying box. At present, industrial design for
the most part takes insufficient account of the general abilities (or inabilities, for
that matter) of workers at large, and should take as its point of departure a broader
human average as a basis for design. Clearly, a suitable physical load for a 20-year-
old may exceed the capacity to manage of a 15-year-old or a 60-year-old. It is the
business of the designer to consider such differences not only from the point of
view of efficiency, but with a eye to the prevention of job-related injury and
illness.

The International Classification of Functional Limitation in People

The WHO (World Health Organization) introduced in 1980 a classification of functional


limitation in people; the ICIDH (International Classification Impairment, Disability and
Handicap). In this classification a difference is made between illness, limitations and handicap.

This reference model was created to facilitate international communication. The model was
presented on the one hand to offer a reference framework for policy makers and on the other
hand, to offer a reference framework for doctors diagnosing people suffering from the
consequences of illness.

Why this reference framework? It arose with the aim of trying to improve and increase the
participation of people with long-term limited abilities. Two aims are mentioned:

· the rehabilitation perspective, i.e., the reintegration of people into society, whether this means
work, school, household, etc.

· the prevention of illness and where possible the consequences of illness e.g., disability and
handicap.

As of January 1st, 1994 the classification is official. The activities that have followed, are
widespread and especially concerned with issues such as: information and educational measures
for specific groups; regulations for the protection of workers; or, for instance, demands that
companies should employ, for example, at least 5 per cent of workers with a disability. The
classification itself leads in the long term to integration and non-discrimination.

Illness

Illness strikes each of us. Certain illnesses can be prevented, others not. Certain illnesses can be
cured, others not. Where possible illness should be prevented and if possible cured.

Impairment

Impairment means every absence or abnormality of a psychological, physiological or anatomic


structure or function.

Being born with three fingers instead of five does not have to lead to disability. The capabilities
of the individual, and the degree of manipulation possible with the three fingers, will determine
whether or not the person is disabled. When, however, a fair amount of signal processing is not
possible on a central level in the brain, then impairment will certainly lead to disability as at
present there is no method to “cure” (solve) this problem for the patient.

Disability

Disability describes the functional level of an individual having difficulty in task performance
e.g., difficulty standing up from their chair. These difficulties are of course related to the
impairment, but also to the circumstances surrounding it. A person who uses a wheelchair and
lives in a flat country like the Netherlands has more possibilities for self-transportation than the
same person living in a mountainous area like Tibet.

Handicap

When the problems are placed on a handicap level, it can be determined in which field the main
problems are effective e.g., immobility or physical dependency. These can affect work
performance; for example the person may not be able to get themselves to work; or, once at
work, might need assistance in personal hygiene, etc.

A handicap shows the negative consequences of disability and can only be solved by taking the
negative consequences away.

Summary and conclusions

The above-mentioned classification and the policies thereof offer a well defined international
workable framework. Any discussion on designing for specific groups will need such a
framework in order to define our activities and try to implement these thoughts in design.

The progress of technology has brought about the state of affairs that, of all the
workplaces in Europe and North America, 60% involve the seated position. The
physical load in work situations is now on average far less than before, but many
worksites, nonetheless, call for physical loads that cannot be sufficiently reduced to
fit human physical capabilities; in some developing countries, the resources of
current technology are simply not available to relieve the human physical burden
to any appreciable extent. And in technologically advanced countries, it is still a
common problem that a designer will adapt his or her approach to constraints
imposed by product specifications or production processes, either slighting or
leaving out human factors related to disability and the prevention of harm due to
the workload. With respect to these aims, designers have to be educated to devote
attention to all such human factors, expressing the results of their study in a
product requirements document (PRD). The PRD contains the system of demands
which the designer has to meet in order to achieve both the expected product
quality level and the satisfaction of human capability needs in the production
process. While it is unrealistic to demand a product that matches a PRD in every
respect, given the need of unavoidable compromises, the design method suited to
the closest approach to this goal is the system ergonomic design (SED) method, to
be discussed following a consideration of two alternative design approaches.

Creative Design

This design approach is characteristic of artists and others involved in the


production of work of a high order of originality. The essence of this design
process is that a concept is worked out intuitively and through “inspiration”,
allowing problems to be dealt with as they arise, without conscious deliberation
beforehand. Sometimes, the outcome will not resemble the initial concept, but
nonetheless represents what the creator regards as his or her authentic product. Not
seldom, too, the design is a failure. Figure 29.46 illustrates the route of creative
design.

Figure 29.46 Creative design

System Design
System design arose from the need to predetermine the steps in design in a logical
order. As design becomes complex, it has to be subdivided into subtasks.
Designers or subtask teams thus become interdependent, and design becomes the
job of a design team rather than an individual designer. Complementary expertise
is distributed through the team, and design assumes an interdisciplinary character.

System design is oriented to the optimal realization of complex and well-defined


product functions through the selection of the most appropriate technology; it is
costly, but the risks of failure are considerably reduced as compared with less
organized approaches. The efficacy of the design is measured against the goals
formulated in the PRD.

The way in which the specifications formulated in the PRD are of the first
importance. Figure 29.47 illustrates the relationship between the PRD and other
parts of the system design process.

Figure 29.47 System design


As this scheme shows, the input of the user is neglected. Only at the end of the
design process can the user criticize the design. This is unhelpful to both producer
and user, since one has to wait for the next design cycle (if there is one) before
errors can be corrected and modifications made. Furthermore, user feedback is
seldom systematized and imported into a new PRD as a design influence.

System ergonomic design (SED)

SED is a version of system design adapted to ensure that the human factor is
accounted for in the design process. Figure 29.48 illustrates the flow of user input
into the PRD.

Figure 29.48 System ergonomic design


In system ergonomic design, the human being is considered part of the system:
design specification changes are, in fact, made in consideration of the worker’s
abilities with respect to cognitive, physical and mental aspects, and the method
lends itself as an efficient design approach for any technical system where human
operators are employed.

For example, to examine the implications of the worker’s physical abilities, task-
allocation in the design of the process will call for a careful selection of tasks to be
performed by the human operator or by the machine, each task being studied for its
aptness to machine or human treatment. Clearly, the human worker will be more
effective at interpreting incomplete information; machines however calculate much
more rapidly with prepared data; a machine is the choice for lifting heavy loads;
and so forth. Furthermore, since the user-machine interface can be tested at the
prototype phase, one can eliminate design errors that would otherwise untimely
manifest themselves at the phase of technical functioning.

Methods in User Research


No “best” method exists, nor any source of formulae and sure and certain
guidelines, according to which design for disabled workers ought to be undertaken.
It is a rather a common-sense business of making as exhaustive search of all
obtainable knowledge relevant to the problem and of implementing it to its most
evident best effect.

Information can be assembled from sources such as the following:

· The literature of research results.

· Direct observation of the disabled person at work and description of his or her
particular work difficulties. Such observation should be made at a point in the
worker’s schedule when he or she can be expected to be subject to fatigue—the
end of a work shift, perhaps. The point is that any design solutions should be
adapted to the most arduous phase of the work process, or else such phases may
fail to be performed adequately (or at all) owing to the worker’s capacity having
been physically exceeded.

· The interview. One has to be aware of the possibly subjective responses which
the interview per se may have the effect of eliciting. It is a far better approach that
the interview technique be combined with observation. Disabled persons
sometimes hesitate to discuss their difficulties, but when workers are aware that the
investigator is willing to exert special thoroughness on their behalf, their reticence
will diminish. This technique is time-consuming, but quite worthwhile.

· Questionnaires. An advantage of the questionnaire is that it can be distributed


to large groups of respondents and at the same time gather data of as specific a sort
as one wishes to provide for. The questionnaire must, however, be constructed
upon the basis of representative information pertaining to the group to which it will
be administered. This means that the type of information to be sought must be
obtained on the basis of interviews and observations carried out among a sample of
workers and specialists that ought to be reasonably restricted as to size. In the case
of disabled persons, it is sensible to include among such a sample the physicians
and therapists who are involved with prescribing special aids for disabled persons
and have examined them regarding their physical capabilities.

· Physical measurements. Measurements obtained from instruments in the field


of bio-instrumentation (e.g., the activity level of muscles, or the amount of oxygen
consumed in a given task) and by anthropometrical methods (e.g., the linear
dimensions of body elements, the range of motion of limbs, muscular strength) are
of indispensable value in human-oriented work designs.

The methods described above are some of the various ways of gathering data about
people. Methods exist, too, to evaluate user-machine systems. One of these—
simulation—is to construct a realistic physical copy. The development of a more or
less abstract symbolic representation of a system is an example of modelling. Such
expedients, of course, are both useful and necessary when the actual system or
product is not in existence or not accessible to experimental manipulation.
Simulation is more often used for training purposes and modelling for research. A
mock-up is a full-size, three-dimensional copy of the designed workplace
composed, where necessary, of improvised materials, and is of great use in testing
design possibilities with the proposed disabled worker: in fact, the majority of
design problems can be identified with the aid of such a device. Another advantage
to this approach is that the motivation of the worker grows as he or she participates
in the design of his or her own future workstation.

Analysis of Tasks

In the analysis of tasks, different aspects of a defined job are subject to analytical
observation. These manifold aspects include posture, routing of work
manipulations, interactions with other workers, handling tools and operating
machines, the logical order of subtasks, the efficiency of operations, static
conditions (a worker may have to perform tasks in the same posture over a long
time or with high frequency), dynamic conditions (calling for numerous varying
physical conditions), material environmental conditions (as in a cold
slaughterhouse) or non-material conditions (as with stressful work surroundings or
the organization of the work itself).

Work design for the disabled person has, then, to be founded on a thorough task
analysis as well as a full examination of the functional abilities of the disabled
person. The basic design approach is a crucial issue: it is more efficient to
elaborate all possible solutions for the problem in hand without prejudice than to
produce a single design concept or a limited number of concepts. In design
terminology, this approach is called making a morphological overview. Given the
multiplicity of original design concepts, one can proceed to an analysis of the pro
and con features of each possibility with respect to material use, construction
method, technical production features, ease of manipulation, and so on. It is not
unprecedented that more than one solution reaches the prototype stage and that a
final decision is made at a relatively late phase in the design process.

Although this may seem a time-consuming way to realize design projects, in fact
the extra work it entails is compensated for in terms of fewer problems
encountered in the developmental stage, to say nothing that the result—a new
workstation or product—will have embodied a better balance between the needs of
the disabled worker and the exigencies of the working environment. Unfortunately,
the latter benefit rarely if ever reaches the designer in terms of feedback.

Product Requirements Document (PRD) and Disability


After all information relating to a product has been assembled, it should be
transformed into a description not only of the product but of all those demands
which may be made of it, regardless of source or nature. These demands may of
course be divided along various lines. The PRD should include demands relating to
user-operator data (physical measurements, range of motion, range of muscular
strength, etc.), technical data (materials, construction, production technique, safety
standards, etc.), and even conclusions arising out of market feasibility studies.

The PRD forms the designer’s framework, and some designers regard it as an
unwelcome restriction of their creativity rather than as a salutary challenge. In
view of the difficulties at times accompanying the execution of a PRD, it should
always be borne steadily in mind that a design failure causes distress for the
disabled person, who may relinquish his or her efforts to succeed in the
employment arena (or else fall helpless victim to the progress of the disabling
condition), and additional costs for redesign as well. To this end, technical
designers should not operate alone in their design work for the disabled, but should
cooperate with whatever disciplines are needed for securing the medical and
functional information to set up an integrated PRD as a framework for the design.

Prototype Testing

When a prototype is built, it should be tested for errors. Error testing should be
carried out not only from the point of view of the technical system and subsystems,
but also with a view to its usability in combination with the user. When the user is
a disabled person, extra precautions have to be taken. An error to which an
unimpaired worker may successfully respond in safety may not afford the disabled
worker the opportunity of avoiding harm.

Prototype testing should be carried out on a small number of disabled workers


(except in the case of a unique design) according to a protocol matched to the
PRD. Only by such empirical testing can the degree to which the design meets the
demands of the PRD be adequately judged. Although results on small numbers of
subjects may not be generalizable to all cases, they do supply valuable information
for the designer’s use in either the final design or in future designs.

Evaluation

The evaluation of a technical system (a work situation, machine or tool) should be


judged on its PRD, not by questioning the user or even by attempting comparisons
of alternative designs with respect to physical performance. For instance, the
designer of a specific knee brace, basing his or her design on research results that
show unstable knee joints to exhibit a delayed hamstring reaction, will create a
product that compensates for this delay. But another brace may have different
design aims. Yet present evaluation methods show no insight as to when to
prescribe what kind of knee brace to which patients under what conditions—
precisely the sort of insight a health professional needs when prescribing technical
aids in the treatment of disabilities.

Current research aims at making this sort of insight possible. A model used to
obtain insight into those factors which actually determine whether or not a
technical aid ought to be used, or whether or not a worksite is well designed and
equipped for the disabled worker is the Rehabilitation Technology Useability
Model (RTUM). The RTUM model offers a framework to use in evaluations of
existing products, tools or machines, but can also be used in combination with the
design process as shown in figure 29.49 .

Figure 29.49 Rehabilitation Technology Useability Model (RTUM) in combination with


the system ergonomic design approach

Evaluations of existing products reveal that as regards technical aids and worksites,
the quality of PRDs is very poor. At some times, the product requirements are not
recorded properly; at others they are not developed to a useful extent. Designers
simply must learn to start documenting their product requirements, including those
relevant to disabled users. Note that, as figure 29.49 shows, RTUM, in conjunction
with SED, offers a framework that includes the requirements of disabled users.
Agencies responsible for prescribing products for their users must request industry
to evaluate those product before marketing them, a task in essence impossible in
the absence of product requirement specifications; figure 29.49 also shows how
provision can be made to ensure that the end result can be evaluated as it should
(on a PRD) with the help of the disabled person or group for whom the product is
intended. It is up to national health organizations to stimulate designers to abide by
such design standards and to formulate appropriate regulations.

CULTURAL DIFFERENCES
Houshang Shahnavaz

Culture and technology are interdependent. While culture is indeed an important


aspect in technology design, development and utilization, the relationship between
culture and technology is, however, extremely complex. It needs to be analysed
from several perspectives in order to be considered in the design and application of
technology. Based on his work in Zambia, Kingsley (1983) divides technological
adaptation into changes and adjustments at three levels: that of the individual, of
the social organization and of the cultural value system of the society. Each level
possesses strong cultural dimensions which require special design considerations.

At the same time, technology itself is an inseparable part of culture. It is built,


wholly or in part, around the cultural values of a particular society. And as part of
culture, technology becomes an expression of that society’s way of life and
thinking. Thus, in order for technology to be accepted, utilized and acknowledged
by a society as its own, it must be congruent to the overall image of that society’s
culture. Technology must complement culture, not antagonize it.

This article will deal with some of the intricacies concerning cultural
considerations in technology designs, examining the current issues and problems,
as well as the prevailing concepts and principles, and how they can be applied.

Definition of Culture

The definition of the term culture has been debated at length amongst sociologists
and anthropologists for many decades. Culture can be defined in many terms.
Kroeber and Kluckhohn (1952) reviewed over a hundred definitions of culture.
Williams (1976) mentioned culture as one of the most complicated words in the
English language. Culture has even been defined as the entire way of life of people.
As such, it includes their technology and material artefacts—anything one would
need to know to become a functioning member of the society (Geertz 1973). It may
even be described as “publicly available symbolic forms through which people
experience and express meaning” (Keesing 1974). Summing it up, Elzinga and
Jamison (1981) put it aptly when they said that “the word culture has different
meanings in different intellectual disciplines and systems of thought”.

Technology: Part and Product of Culture

Technology can be considered both as part of culture and its product. More than 60
years ago the noted sociologist Malinowsky included technology as part of the
culture and gave the following definition: “culture comprises inherited artefacts,
goods, technical processes, ideas, habits and values.” Later, Leach (1965)
considered technology as a cultural product and mentioned “artefacts, goods and
technical processes” as “products of culture”.

In the technological realm, “culture” as an important issue in the design,


development and utilization of technical products or systems has been largely
neglected by many suppliers as well as receivers of technology. One major reason
for this neglect is the absence of basic information on cultural differences.

In the past, technological changes have led to significant changes in social life and
organization and in people’s value systems. Industrialization has made deep and
enduring changes in the traditional lifestyles of many previously agricultural
societies since such lifestyles were largely regarded as incompatible with the way
industrial work should be organized. In situations of large cultural diversity, this
has led to various negative socio-economic outcomes (Shahnavaz 1991). It is now
a well-established fact that simply to impose a technology on a society and believe
that it will be absorbed and utilized through extensive training is wishful thinking
(Martin et al. 1991).

It is the responsibility of the technology designer to consider the direct and indirect
effects of the culture and to make the product compatible with the cultural value
system of the user and with its intended operating environment.

The impact of technology for many “industrially developing countries” (IDCs) has
been much more than improvement in efficiency. Industrialization was not just
modernization of the production and service sectors, but to some extent
Westernization of the society. Technology transfer is, thus, also cultural transfer.

Culture, in addition to religion, tradition and language, which are important


parameters for technology design and utilization, encompasses other aspects, such
as specific attitudes towards certain products and tasks, rules of appropriate
behaviour, rules of etiquette, taboos, habits and customs. All these must be equally
considered for optimum design.

It is said that people are also products of their distinctive cultures. Nevertheless,
the fact remains that world cultures are very much interwoven due to human
migration throughout history. It is small wonder that there exist more cultural than
national variations in the world. Nevertheless, some very broad distinctions can be
made regarding societal, organizational and professional culture-based differences
that could influence design in general.

Constraining Influences of Culture

There is very little information on both theoretical and empirical analyses of the
constraining influences of culture on technology and how this issue should be
incorporated in the design of hardware and software technology. Even though the
influence of culture on technology has been recognized (Shahnavaz 1991;
Abeysekera, Shahnavaz and Chapman 1990; Alvares 1980; Baranson 1969), very
little information is available on the theoretical analysis of cultural differences with
regard to technology design and utilization. There are even fewer empirical studies
that quantify the importance of cultural variations and provide recommendations
on how cultural factors should be considered in the design of product or system
(Kedia and Bhagat 1988). Nevertheless, culture and technology can still be studied
with some degree of clarity when viewed from different sociological viewpoints.

Culture and Technology: Compatibility and Preference

Proper application of a technology depends, to a large extent, on the compatibility


of the user’s culture with the design specifications. Compatibility must exist at all
levels of culture—at the societal, organizational and professional levels. In turn,
cultural compatibility can have strong influence on a people’s preferences and
aptness to utilize a technology. This question involves preferences relating to a
product or system; to concepts of productivity and relative efficiency; to change,
achievement and authority; as well as to the manner of technology utilization.
Cultural values can thus affect people’s willingness and ability to select, to use and
to control technology. They have to be compatible in order to be preferred.

Societal culture

As all technologies are inevitably associated with sociocultural values, the cultural
receptivity of the society is a very important issue for the proper functioning of a
given technological design (Hosni 1988). National or societal culture, which
contributes to the formation of a collective mental model of people, influences the
entire process of technology design and application, which ranges from planning,
goal setting and defining design specifications, to production, management and
maintenance systems, training and evaluation. Technology design of both hardware
and software should, therefore, reflect society-based cultural variations for
maximum benefit. However, defining such society-based cultural factors for
consideration in the design of technology is a very complicated task. Hofstede
(1980) has proposed four dimensional framework variations of national-based
culture.
1. Weak versus strong uncertainty avoidance. This concerns a people’s desire to
avoid ambiguous situations and to what extent their society has developed formal
means (such as rules and regulations) to serve this purpose. Hofstede (1980) gave,
for example, high uncertainty avoidance scores to countries like Japan and Greece,
and low scores to Hong Kong and Scandinavia.

2. Individualism versus collectivism. This pertains to the relationship between


individuals and organizations in the society. In individualistic societies, the
orientation is such that each person is expected to look after his or her own
interests. In contrast, in a collectivist culture, social ties between people are very
strong. Some examples of individualistic countries are the United States and Great
Britain while Colombia and Venezuela can be considered as having collectivist
cultures.

3. Small versus large power distance. A large “power distance” characterizes


those cultures where the less powerful individuals accept the unequal distribution
of power in a culture, as well as the hierarchies in the society and its organizations.
Examples of large power distance countries are India and the Philippines. Small
power distances are typical of countries like Sweden and Austria.

4. Masculinity versus femininity. Cultures that put more emphasis on material


achievements are regarded as belonging to the former category. Those giving more
value to quality of life and other less tangible outcomes belong to the latter.

Glenn and Glenn (1981) have also distinguished between “abstractive” and
“associative” tendencies in a given national culture. It is argued that when people
of an associative culture (like those from Asia) approach a cognitive problem, they
put more emphasis on context, adapt a global thinking approach and try to utilize
association among various events. Whereas in the Western societies, a more
abstractive culture of rational thinking predominates. Based on these cultural
dimensions, Kedia and Bhagat (1988) have developed a conceptual model for
understanding cultural constraints on technology transfer. They have developed
various descriptive “propositions” which provide information on different
countries’ cultural variations and their receptivity with regard to technology.
Certainly many cultures are moderately inclined to one or the other of these
categories and contain some mixed features.

Consumers’ as well as producers’ perspectives upon technological design and


utilization are directly influenced by the societal culture. Product safety standards
for safeguarding consumers as well as work-environment regulations, inspection
and enforcement systems for protecting the producers are to a large extent the
reflection of the societal culture and value system.

Organizational culture
A company’s organization, its structure, value system, function, behaviour, and so
on, are largely cultural products of the society in which it operates. This means that
what happens within an organization is mostly a direct reflection of what is
happening in the outside society (Hofstede 1983). The prevailing organizations of
many companies operating in the IDCs are influenced both by the characteristics of
the technology producer country as well as those of the technology recipient
environment. However, the reflection of the societal culture in a given organization
can vary. Organizations interpret the society in terms of their own culture, and their
degree of control depends, among other factors, on the modes of technology
transfer.

Given the changing nature of organization today, plus a multicultural, diverse


workforce, adapting a proper organizational programme is more important than
ever before to a successful operation (an example of a workforce diversity
management programme is described in Solomon (1989)).

Professional culture

People belonging to a certain professional category may use a piece of technology


in a specific fashion. Wikström et al. (1991), in a project aimed to develop hand
tools, have noted that despite the designers’ assumption of how plate shares are to
be held and used (i.e., with a forward holding grip and the tool moving away from
one’s own body), the professional tinsmiths were holding and using the plate share
in a reversed manner, as shown in figure 29.50 . They concluded that tools should
be studied in the actual field conditions of the user population itself in order to
acquire relevant information on the tools characteristics.

Figure 29.50 The use of plate share tools by professional tinsmiths in practice (the
reversed grip)
Using Cultural Features for Optimum Design

As implied by the foregoing considerations, culture provides identity and


confidence. It forms opinions about the objectives and characteristics of a “human-
technology system” and how it should operate in a given environment. And in any
culture, there are always some features that are valuable with regard to
technological progress. If these features are considered in the design of software
and hardware technology, they can act as the driving force for technology
absorption in the society. One good example is the culture of some southeast Asian
countries largely influenced by Confucianism and Buddhism. The former
emphasizes, among other things, learning and loyalty, and considers it a virtue to
be able to absorb new concepts. The latter teaches the importance of harmony and
respect for fellow human beings. It is said that these unique cultural features have
contributed to the provision of the right environment for the absorption and
implementation of advanced hardware and organizational technology furnished by
the Japanese (Matthews 1982).

A clever strategy would thus make the best use of the positive features of a
society’s culture in promoting ergonomic ideas and principles. According to
McWhinney (1990) “the events, to be understood and thus used effectively in
projection, must be embedded in stories. One must go to varying depths to unleash
founding energy, to free society or organization from inhibiting traits, to find the
paths along which it might naturally flow. . . . Neither planning nor change can be
effective without embedding it consciously in a narrative.”

A good example of cultural appreciation in designing management strategy is the


implementation of the “seven tools” technique for quality assurance in Japan. The
“seven tools” are the minimum weapons a samurai warrior had to carry with him
whenever he went out to fight. The pioneers of “quality control circles”, adapting
their nine recommendations to a Japanese setting, reduced this number in order to
take advantage of a familiar term—“the seven tools”—so as to encourage the
involvement of all employees in their quality work strategy (Lillrank and Kano
1989).

However, other cultural features may not be beneficial to technological


development. Discrimination against women, the strict observation of a caste
system, racial or other prejudice, or considering some tasks as degrading, are a few
examples that can have a negative influence on technology development. In some
traditional cultures, men are expected to be the primary wage-earners. They
become accustomed to regarding the role of women as equal employees, not to
mention as supervisors, with insensitivity or even hostility. Withholding equal
employment opportunity from women and questioning the legitimacy of women’s
authority is not appropriate to the current needs of organizations, which require
optimum utilization of human resources.

With regard to task design and job content, some cultures consider tasks like
manual labour and service as degrading. This may be attributed to past experiences
linked to colonial times regarding “master-slave relationships”. In some other
cultures, strong biases exist against tasks or occupations associated with “dirty
hands”. These attitudes are also reflected in lower-than-average pay scales for
these occupations. In turn, these have contributed to shortages of technicians or
inadequate maintenance resources (Sinaiko 1975).

Since it usually takes many generations to change cultural values with respect to a
new technology, it would be more cost-effective to fit the technology to the
technology recipient’s culture, taking cultural differences into consideration in the
design of hardware and software.

Cultural Considerations in Product and System Designing

By now it is obvious that technology consists both of hardware and software.


Hardware components include capital and intermediary goods, such as industrial
products, machinery, equipment, buildings, workplaces and physical layouts, most
of which chiefly concern the micro-ergonomics domain. Software pertains to
programming and planning, management and organizational techniques,
administration, maintenance, training and education, documentation and services.
All these concerns fall under the heading of macro-ergonomics.

A few examples of cultural influences that require special design consideration


from the micro- and macro-ergonomic point of view are given below.

Micro-ergonomic issues
Micro-ergonomics is concerned with the design of a product or system with the
objective of creating a “usable” user-machine-environment interface. The major
concept of product design is usability. This concept involves not only the
functionality and reliability of the product, but issues of safety, comfort and
enjoyment as well.

The user’s internal model (i.e., his or her cognitive or mental model) plays an
important role in usability design. To operate or control a system efficiently and
safely, the user must have an accurate representative cognitive model of the system
in use. Wisner (1983) has stated that “industrialization would thus more or less
require a new kind of mental model.” In this view, formal education and technical
training, experience as well as culture are important factors in determining the
formation of an adequate cognitive model.

Meshkati (1989), in studying the micro- and macro-ergonomic factors of the 1984
Union Carbide Bhopal accident, highlighted the importance of culture on the
Indian operators’ inadequate mental model of the plant operation. He stated that
part of the problem may have been due to “the performance of poorly trained Third
World operators using advanced technological systems designed by other humans
with much different educational backgrounds, as well as cultural and psychosocial
attributes.” Indeed, many design usability aspects at the micro-interface level are
influenced by the user’s culture. Careful analyses of the user’s perception,
behaviour and preferences would lead to a better understanding of the user’s needs
and requirements for designing a product or system that is both effective and
acceptable.

Some of these culture-related micro-ergonomic aspects are the following:

1. Interface design. Human emotion is an essential element of product design. It


is concerned in such factors as colour and shape (Kwon, Lee and Ahn 1993;
Nagamachi 1992). Colour is regarded as the most important factor to do with
human emotions with regard to product design. The product’s colour treatment
reflects the psychological and sentimental dispositions of the users, which differ
from country to country. The symbolism of colour may also differ. For example,
the colour red, which indicates danger in Western countries, is an auspicious token
in India (Sen 1984) and symbolizes joy or happiness in China. Pictorial signs and
symbols that are used in many different applications for public accommodations
are strongly culture related. Western pictorial information, for example, is difficult
to interpret by non-Western people (Daftuar 1975; Fuglesang 1982).

2. Control/display compatibility. Compatibility is a measure of how well spatial


movements of control, display behaviour or conceptual relationships meet human
expectations (Staramler 1993). It refers to the user’s expectation of the stimulus-
response relationship, which is a fundamental ergonomic issue for safe and
efficient operation of a product or system. A compatible system is one which
considers people’s common perceptual-motor behaviour (i.e., their population-
stereotype). However, like other human behaviour, perceptual-motor behaviour
may also be influenced by culture. Hsu and Peng (1993) compared American and
Chinese subjects regarding control/burner relationships in a four-burner stove.
Different population-stereotype patterns were observed. They conclude that
population stereotypes regarding control/burner linkages were culturally different,
probably as a result of differences in reading or scanning habits.

3. Workplace design. An industrial workstation design aims to eliminate harmful


postures and improve user performance in relation to the user’s biological needs,
preferences and task requirements. People from different cultures may prefer
different types of sitting posture and work heights. In Western countries, work
heights are set near the seated elbow height for maximum comfort and efficiency.
However, in many parts of the world people sit on the floor. Indian workers, for
example, prefer squatting or sitting cross-legged to standing or to sitting on a chair.
In fact it has been observed that even when chairs are provided, the operators still
prefer to squat or sit cross-legged on the seats. Daftuar (1975) and Sen (1984) have
studied the merits and implications of the Indian sitting posture. After describing
the various advantages of sitting on the floor, Sen stated that “as a large population
of the world market covers societies where squatting or sitting on the ground
predominate, it is unfortunate that up to now no modern machines have been
designed to be used in this way.” Thus, variations in preferred posture should be
considered in machine and workplace design in order to improve the operator’s
efficiency and comfort.

4. Design of protective equipment. There exist both psychological and physical


constraints with regard to wearing protective clothing. In some cultures, for
example, jobs requiring the use of protective wear may be regarded as common
labour, suitable only for unskilled workers. Consequently, protective equipment is
usually not worn by engineers at workplaces in such settings. Regarding physical
constraints, some religious groups, obliged by their religion to wear a head
covering (like the turbans of Indian Sikhs or the head covers of Muslim women)
find it difficult to wear, for example, protective helmets. Therefore, special designs
of protective wear are needed to cope with such cultural variations in protecting
people against work-environmental hazards.

Macro-ergonomic issues

The term macro-ergonomics refers to the design of software technology. It


concerns the proper design of organizations and management systems. Evidence
exists showing that because of differences in culture, sociopolitical conditions and
educational levels, many successful managerial and organizational methods
developed in industrialized countries cannot be successfully applied to developing
countries (Negandhi 1975). In most IDCs, an organizational hierarchy
characterized by a down-flow of authority structure within the organization is a
common practice. It has little concern for Western values such as democracy or
power sharing in decision-making, which are regarded as key issues in modern
management, being essential for proper utilization of human resources as regards
intelligence, creativity, problem solving potential and ingenuity.

The feudal system of social hierarchy and its value system are also widely
practised in most industrial workplaces in the developing countries. These make a
participatory management approach (which is essential for the new production
mode of flexible specialization and the motivation of the workforce) a difficult
endeavour. However, there are reports confirming the desirability of introducing
autonomous work systems even in these cultures Ketchum 1984).

1. Participatory ergonomics. Participatory ergonomics is a useful macro-


ergonomics approach for solving various work-related problems (Shahnavaz,
Abeysekera and Johansson 1993; Noro and Imada 1991; Wilson 1991). This
approach, mostly used in industrialized countries, has been applied in different
forms depending on the organizational culture in which it has been implemented.
In a study, Liker, Nagamachi and Lifshitz (1988) compared participatory
ergonomics programmes in two US and two Japanese manufacturing plants which
were aiming to reduce physical stress on workers. They concluded that an
“effective participatory ergonomics programme can take many forms. The best
programme for any plant in any culture may depend on its own unique history,
structure and culture.”

2. Software systems. Societal and organizational culture-based differences


should be considered in designing a new software system or introducing a change
in the organization. With respect to information technology, De Lisi (1990)
indicates that networking capabilities will not be realized unless the networks fit
the existing organizational culture.

3. Work organization and management. In some cultures, the family is so


important an institution that it plays a prominent role in work organization. For
example, among some communities in India, a job is generally regarded as a
family responsibility and is collectively performed by all family members
(Chapanis 1975).

4. Maintenance system. Design of maintenance programmes (both preventive


and regular) as well as housekeeping are other examples of areas in which work
organization should be adapted to cultural constraints. The traditional culture
among the sort of agricultural societies predominant in many IDCs is generally not
compatible with the requirements of industrial work and how activities are
organized. Traditional agricultural activity does not require, for example, formal
maintenance programming and precision work. It is for the most part not carried
out under time pressure. In the field, it is usually left to the recycling process of
nature to take care of maintenance and housekeeping work. The design of
maintenance programmes and housekeeping manuals for industrial activities
should thus take these cultural constraints into account and provide for adequate
training and supervision.

Zhang and Tyler (1990), in a case study related to the successful establishment of a
modern telephone cable production facility in China supplied by a US firm (the
Essex Company) stated that “both parties realize, however, that the direct
application of American or Essex management practices was not always practical
nor desirable due to cultural, philosophical, and political differences. Thus the
information and instructions provided by Essex was often modified by the Chinese
partner to be compatible with the conditions existing in China.” They also argued
that the key to their success, despite cultural, economic and political differences,
was both parties’ dedication and commitment to a common goal as well as the
mutual respect, trust, and friendship which transcended any differences between
them.

Design of shift and work schedules are other examples of work organization. In
most IDCs there are certain sociocultural problems associated with shift work.
These include poor general living and housing conditions, lack of support services,
a noisy home environment and other factors, which require the design of special
shift programmes. Furthermore, for female workers, a working day is usually much
longer than eight hours; it consists of not only the actual time spent working, but
also the time spent on travelling, working at home and taking care of children and
elderly relatives. In view of the prevailing culture, shift and other work design
requires special work-rest schedules for effective operation.

Flexibility in work schedules to allow cultural variances such as an after-lunch nap


for Chinese workers and religious activities for Muslims are further cultural
aspects of work organization. In the Islamic culture, people are required to break
from work a few times a day to pray, and to fast for one month each year from
sunrise to sunset. All these cultural constraints require special work organizational
considerations.

Thus, many macro-ergonomic design features are closely influenced by culture.


These features should be considered in the design of software systems for effective
operation.

Conclusion: Cultural Differences in Design

Designing a usable product or system is not an easy task. There exists no absolute
quality of suitability. It is the designer’s task to create an optimum and harmonic
interaction between the four basic components of the human-technology system:
the user, the task, the technological system and the operating environment. A
system may be fully usable for one combination of user, task and environmental
conditions but totally unsuitable for another. One design aspect which can greatly
contribute to the design’s usability, whether it is a case of a single product or a
complex system, is the consideration of cultural aspects which have a profound
influence on both the user and the operating environment.

Even if a conscientious engineer designs a proper human-machine interface for use


in a given environment, the designer is often unable to foresee the effects of a
different culture on the product’s usability. It is difficult to prevent possible
negative cultural effects when a product is used in an environment different from
that for which it was designed. And since there exist almost no quantitative data
regarding cultural constraints, the only way the engineer can make the design
compatible with regard to cultural factors is to actively integrate the user
population in the design process.

The best way to consider cultural aspects in design is for the designer to adapt a
user-centred design approach. True enough, the design approach adapted by the
designer is the essential factor that will instantly influence the usability of the
designed system. The importance of this basic concept must be recognized and
implemented by the product or system designer at the very beginning of the design
life cycle. The basic principles of user-centred design can thus be summarized as
follows (Gould and Lewis 1985; Shackel 1986; Gould et al. 1987; Gould 1988;
Wang 1992):

1. Early and continual focus on user. The user should be an active member of the
design team throughout the whole product development life cycle (i.e., predesign,
detail design, production, verification and product improvement phase).

2. Integrated design. The system should be considered as a whole, ensuring a


holistic design approach. This means that all aspects of the system’s usability
should be evolved in parallel by the design team.

3. Early and continuous user testing. User reaction should be tested using
prototypes or simulations while carrying out real work in the real environment
from early development stage to the final product.

4. Iterative design. Designing, testing and redesigning are repeated in regular


cycles until satisfactory usability results are achieved.

In the case of designing a product on a global scale, the designer has to consider
the needs of consumers around the world. In such a case, access to all actual users
and operating environments may not be possible for the purpose of adopting a
user-centred design approach. The designer has to use a broad range of
information, both formal and informal, such as literature reference material,
standards, guidelines, and practical principles and experience in making an
analytical evaluation of the design and has to provide sufficient adjustability and
flexibility in the product in order to satisfy the needs of a wider user population.
Another point to consider is the fact that designers can never be all-knowing. They
need input from not only the users but also other parties involved in the project,
including managers, technicians, and repair and maintenance workers. In a
participatory process, people involved should share their knowledge and
experiences in developing a usable product or system and accept collective
responsibility for its functionality and safety. After all, everyone involved has
something at stake.

ELDERLY WORKERS
Antoine Laville and Serge Volkoff

The status of ageing workers varies according to their functional condition, which
itself is influenced by their past working history. Their status also depends on the
work post that they occupy, and the social, cultural and economic situation of the
country in which they live.

Thus, workers who have to perform much physical labour are also, most often,
those who have had the least schooling and the least occupational training. They
are subject to exhausting work conditions, which can cause disease, and they are
exposed to the risk of accidents. In this context, their physical capacity is very
likely to decline towards the end of their active life, a fact that makes them more
vulnerable at work.

Conversely, workers who have had the advantage of lengthy schooling, followed
by occupational training that equips them for their work, in general practise trades
where they can put to use the knowledge thus acquired and progressively widen
their experience. Often they do not work in the most harmful occupational
environments and their skills are recognized and valued as they grow older.

During a period of economic expansion and shortage of labour, ageing workers are
recognized as having the qualities of “occupational conscientiousness”, being
regular in their work, and being able to keep up their know-how. In a period of
recession and unemployment, there will be greater emphasis on the fact that their
work performance falls short of that of younger people and on their lower capacity
to adapt to changes in work techniques and organization.

Depending on the countries concerned, their cultural traditions and their mode and
level of economic development, consideration for ageing workers and solidarity
with them will be more or less evident, and their protection will be more or less
assured.

The time dimensions of the age/work relationship


The relationship between ageing and work covers a great diversity of situations,
which can be considered from two points of view: on the one hand, work appears
to be a transformation factor for the worker throughout his or her active life, the
transformations being either negative (e.g., wear and tear, decline in skills,
illnesses and accidents) or positive (e.g., acquisition of knowledge and experience);
on the other hand, work reveals the changes connected with age, and this results in
marginalization and even exclusion from the production system for older workers
exposed to demands at work that are too great for their declining capacity, or on
the contrary allows for progress in their working career if the content of the work is
such that a high value is placed on experience.

Advancing age therefore plays the role of a “vector” on which events in life are
registered chronologically, both at and outside work. Around this axis are hinged
processes of decline and building, which are very variable from one worker to
another. In order to take into account the problems of ageing workers in the design
of work situations, it is necessary to take into account both the dynamic
characteristics of changes connected with age and the variability of these changes
among individuals.

The age/work relationship can be considered in the light of a threefold evolution:

1. Work evolves. Techniques change; mechanization, automation,


computerization and methods of information transfer, among other factors, tend or
will tend to become more generalized. New products make their appearance, others
disappear. New risks are revealed or extended (e.g., radiation and chemical
products), others become less prominent. Work organization, labour management,
the distribution of tasks and the work schedules are transformed. Some production
sectors develop, while others decline. From one generation to another, the work
situations encountered during the active life of the worker, the demands that they
make and the skills that they require are not the same.

2. Working populations change. Age structures are modified in accordance with


demographic changes, the means of entering or retiring from work and attitudes
towards employment. Women’s share in the working population continues to
evolve. Genuine upheavals are occurring in the field of education, occupational
training and access to the health system. All these transformations are at the same
time producing generation-related and period-related effects which obviously
influence the age/work relationship and which can to a certain extent be
anticipated.

3. Finally—a point that deserves emphasis—individual changes are in progress


throughout one’s working life, and the adjustment between the characteristics of
particular work and those of the people who carry it out is therefore frequently
called into question.
Some processes of organic ageing and their relationship to work

The main organic functions involved in work decline in an observable way from
the ages of 40 or 50, after some of them have undergone development up to the
ages of 20 or 25.

In particular, a decline with age is observed in maximum muscular strength and


range of joint movement. The reduction in strength is in the order of 15 to 20%
between the ages of 20 and 60. But this is only an overall trend, and the variability
among individuals is considerable. Moreover, these are maximum capacities; the
decline is much less for more moderate physical demands.

One function that is very sensitive to age is regulation of posture. This difficulty is
not very apparent for common and stable working positions (standing or sitting)
but it becomes obvious in situations of disequilibrium that require precise
adjustments, strong muscular contraction or joint movements at extreme angles.
These problems become more severe when the work has to be carried out on
unstable or slippery supports, or when the worker suffers a shock or unexpected
jolt. The result is that accidents due to loss of balance become more frequent with
age.

Sleep regulation becomes less reliable from the ages of 40 to 45 onwards. It is


more sensitive to changes in working schedules (such as night work or shift work)
and to disturbing environments (e.g., noise or lighting). Changes in the length and
quality of sleep follow.

Thermoregulation also becomes more difficult with age, and this causes older
workers to have specific problems with regard to work in heat, particularly when
physically intense work has to be carried out.

Sensory functions begin to be affected very early, but the resulting deficiencies are
rarely marked before the ages of 40 to 45. Visual function as a whole is affected:
there is a reduction in the amplitude of accommodation (which can be corrected
with appropriate lenses), and also in the peripheral visual field, perception of
depth, resistance to glare and light transmission through the crystalline lens. The
resulting inconvenience is noticeable only in particular conditions: in poor lighting,
near sources of glare, with objects or texts of very small size or badly presented,
and so on.

The decline in auditory function affects the hearing threshold for high frequencies
(high-pitched sounds), but it reveals itself particularly as difficulty in
discriminating sound signals in a noisy environment. Thus, the intelligibility of the
spoken word becomes more difficult in the presence of ambient noise or strong
reverberation.
The other sensory functions are, in general, little affected at this time of life.

It can be seen that, in a general way, organic decline with age is noticeable
particularly in extreme situations, which should in any case be modified to avoid
difficulties even for young workers. Moreover, ageing workers can compensate for
their deficiencies by means of particular strategies, often acquired with experience,
when the work conditions and organization permit: the use of additional supports
for unbalanced postures, lifting and carrying loads in such a way as to reduce
extreme effort, organizing visual scanning so as to pinpoint useful information,
among other means.

Cognitive ageing: slowing down and learning

As regards cognitive functions, the first thing to note is that work activity brings
into play basic mechanisms for receiving and processing information on the one
hand, and on the other, knowledge acquired throughout life. This knowledge
concerns mainly the meaning of objects, signals, words and situations
(“declarative” knowledge), and ways of doing things (“procedural” knowledge).

Short-term memory allows us to retain, for some dozens of seconds or for some
minutes, useful information that has been detected. Processing of this information
is carried out by comparing it with knowledge that has been memorized on a
permanent basis. Ageing acts on these mechanisms in various ways: (1) by virtue
of experience, it enriches knowledge, the capacity to select in the best way both
useful knowledge and the method of processing it, especially in tasks that are
carried out fairly frequently, but (2) the time taken to process this information is
lengthened owing both to ageing of the central nervous system, and to more fragile
short-term memory.

These cognitive functions depend very much on the environment in which the
workers have lived, and therefore on their past history, their training, and the work
situations which they have had to face. The changes that occur with age are
therefore manifested in extremely varied combinations of phenomena of decline
and reconstruction, in which each of these two factors may be more or less
accentuated.

If in the course of their working lives workers have received only brief training,
and if they have had to carry out relatively simple and repetitive tasks, their
knowledge will be limited and they have difficulties when confronted with new or
relatively unfamiliar tasks. If, moreover, they have to perform work under marked
time constraints, the changes that have occurred in their sensory functions and the
slowing down of their information processing will handicap them. If, on the other
hand, they have had lengthy schooling and training, and if they have had to carry
out a variety of tasks, they will thereby have been able to enhance their skills so
that the sensory or cognitive deficiencies associated with age will be largely
compensated for.

It is therefore easy to understand the role played by continued training in the work
situation of ageing workers. Changes in work make it necessary more and more
often to have recourse to periodic training, but older workers rarely receive it.
Firms frequently do not consider it worthwhile to give training to a worker nearing
the end of his or her active life, particularly as learning difficulties are thought to
increase with age. And the workers themselves hesitate to undergo training, fearing
that they will not succeed, and not always seeing very clearly the benefits that they
could derive from training.

In fact, with age, the manner of learning is modified. Whereas a young person
records the knowledge transmitted to him, an older person needs to understand
how this knowledge is organized in relation to what he or she already knows, what
is its logic, and what is its justification for work. He or she also needs time to learn.
Therefore one response to the problem of training older workers is, in the first
place, to use different teaching methods, according to each person’s age,
knowledge and experience, with, in particular, a longer training period for older
people.

Ageing of men and women at work

Age differences between men and women are found at two different levels. At the
organic level, life expectancy is generally greater for women than for men, but
what is called life expectancy without disability is very close for the two sexes—up
to 65 to 70 years. Beyond that age, women are generally at a disadvantage.
Moreover, women’s maximum physical capacity is on average 30% less than
men’s, and this difference tends to persist with advancing age, but the variability in
the two groups is wide, with some overlap between the two distributions.

At the level of the working career there are great differences. On average, women
have received less training for work than men when they start their working life,
they most often occupy posts for which fewer qualifications are needed, and their
working careers are less rewarding. With age they, therefore, occupy posts with
considerable constraints, such as time constraints and repetitiveness of the work.
No sexual difference in the development of cognitive capacity with age can be
established without reference to this social context of work.

If the design of work situations is to take account of these gender differences,


action must be taken especially in favour of the initial and continuing vocational
training of women and constructing work careers that increase women’s
experiences and enhance their value. This action must, therefore, be taken well
before the end of their active lives.
Ageing of working populations: the usefulness of collective data

There are at least two reasons for adopting collective and quantitative approaches
with respect to the ageing of the working population. The first reason is that such
data will be necessary in order to evaluate and foresee the effects of ageing in a
workshop, a service, a firm, a sector or a country. The second reason is that the
main components of ageing are themselves phenomena subject to probability: all
workers do not age in the same way or at the same rate. It is therefore by means of
statistical tools that various aspects of ageing will sometimes be revealed,
confirmed or assessed.

The simplest instrument in this field is the description of age structures and of their
evolution, expressed in ways relevant to work: economic sector, trade, group of
jobs, and so on.

For example, when we observe that the age structure of a population in a


workplace remains stable and young, we may ask which characteristics of the work
could play a selective role in terms of age. If, on the contrary, this structure is
stable and older, the workplace has the function of receiving people from other
sectors of the firm; the reasons for these movements are worth studying, and we
should equally verify whether the work in this workplace is suited to the
characteristics of an ageing workforce. If, finally, the age structure shifts regularly,
simply reflecting recruitment levels from one year to another, we probably have a
situation where people “grow old on site”; this sometimes requires special study,
particularly if the annual number of recruitments is tending to decline, which will
shift the overall structure towards higher age groups.

Our understanding of these phenomena can be enhanced if we have quantitative


data on working conditions, on the posts currently occupied by the workers and (if
possible) on the posts that they no longer occupy. The work schedules, the
repetitiveness of work, the nature of the physical demands, the work environment,
and even certain cognitive components, can be the subject of queries (to be asked
of the workers) or of evaluations (by experts). It is then possible to establish a
connection between the characteristics of the present work and of past work, and
the age of the workers concerned, and so to elucidate the selection mechanisms to
which the work conditions can give rise at certain ages.

These investigations can be further improved by also obtaining information on the


health status of the workers. This information can be derived from objective
indicators such as the work accident rate or sickness absence rate. But these
indicators often require considerable care as regards methodology, because
although they do indeed reflect health conditions that may be work-related, they
also reflect the strategy of all those concerned with occupational accidents and
absence due to illness: the workers themselves, the management and the physicians
can have various strategies in this regard, and there is no guarantee that these
strategies are independent of the worker’s age. Comparisons of these indicators
between ages are therefore often complex.

Recourse will therefore be had, when possible, to data arising from self-evaluation
of health by the workers, or obtained during medical examinations. These data may
relate to diseases whose variable prevalence with age needs to be better known for
purposes of anticipation and prevention. But the study of ageing will rely above all
on the appreciation of conditions that have not reached the disease stage, such as
certain types of functional deterioration: (e. g., of the joints—pain and limitation of
sight and hearing, of the respiratory system) or else certain kinds of difficulty or
even incapacity (e. g. in mounting a high step, making a precise movement,
maintaining equilibrium in an awkward position).

Relating data concerning age, work and health is therefore at the same time a
useful and complex matter. Their use permits various types of connections to be
revealed (or their existence to be presumed). It may be a case of simple causal
relationships, with some requirement of the work accelerating a type of decline in
the functional state as age advances. But this is not the most frequent case. Very
often, we shall be led to appreciate simultaneously the effect of an accumulation of
constraints on the a set of health characteristics, and at the same time the effect of
selection mechanisms in accordance with which workers whose health has declined
may find that they are excluded from certain kinds of work (what the
epidemiologists call the “healthy worker effect”).

In this way we can evaluate the soundness of this collection of relationships,


confirm certain fundamental knowledge in the sphere of psychophysiology, and
above all obtain information that is useful for devising preventive strategies as
regards ageing at work.

Some types of action

Action to be undertaken to maintain ageing workers in employment, without


negative consequences for them, must follow several general lines:

1. One must not consider this age group as a category apart, but must instead
consider age as one factor of diversity among others in the active population;
protective measures that are too targeted or too accentuated tend to marginalize and
weaken the position of the populations concerned.

2. One should anticipate individual and collective changes related to age, as well
as changes in work techniques and organization. The management of human
resources can be effectively carried out only over time, so as to prepare appropriate
adjustments in work careers and training. The design of work situations can then
take account at the same time of the available technical and organizational
solutions and the characteristics of the (future) population concerned.
3. The diversity of individual development throughout working life should be
taken into consideration, so as to create conditions of equivalent diversity in work
careers and situations.

4. Attention should be devoted to favouring the process of building up skills and


attenuating the process of decline.

On the basis of these few principles, several types of immediate action can first be
defined. The highest priority of action will concern working conditions that are
capable of posing particularly acute problems for older workers. As mentioned
earlier, postural stresses, extreme exertion, strict time constraints (e.g., as with
assembly-line work or the imposition of higher output goals), harmful
environments (temperature, noise) or unsuitable environments (lighting
conditions), night work and shift work are examples.

Systematic pinpointing of these constraints in posts that are (or may be) occupied
by older workers allows an inventory to be drawn up and priorities to be
established for action. This pinpointing can be carried out by means of empirical
inspection checklists. Of equal use will be analysis of worker activity, which will
permit observation of their behaviour to be linked with the explanations that they
give of their difficulties. In these two cases, measures of effort or of environmental
parameters may complete the observations.

Beyond this pinpointing, the action to be taken cannot be described here, since it
will obviously be specific to each work situation. The use of standards may
sometimes be useful, but few standards take account of specific aspects of ageing,
and each one is concerned with a particular domain, which tends to give rise to
thinking in an isolated fashion about each component of the activity under study.

Apart from the immediate measures, taking ageing into account implies longer-
range thinking directed towards working out the widest possible flexibility in the
design of work situations.

Such flexibility must first be sought in the design of work situations and
equipment. Restricted space, nonadjustable tools, rigid software, in short, all the
characteristics of the situation that limit the expression of human diversity in the
carrying out of the task are very likely to penalize a considerable proportion of
older workers. The same is true of the more constraining types of organization: a
completely predetermined distribution of tasks, frequent and urgent deadlines, or
too numerous or too strict orders (these, of course, must be tolerated when there are
essential requirements relating to the quality of production or the safety of an
installation). The search for such flexibility is, therefore, the search for varied
individual and collective adjustments that can facilitate the successful integration
of ageing workers into the production system. One of the conditions for the success
of these adjustments is obviously the establishment of work training programmes,
provided for workers of all ages and geared to their specific needs.

Taking ageing into account in the design of work situations thus entails a series of
coordinated actions (overall reduction in extreme stresses, using all possible
strategies for work organization, and continuous efforts to increase skills), which
are all the more efficient and all the less expensive when they are taken over the
long term and are carefully thought out in advance. The ageing of the population is
a sufficiently slow and foreseeable phenomenon for appropriate preventive action
to be perfectly feasible.

WORKERS WITH SPECIAL NEEDS


Joke H. Grady-van den Nieuwboer

Designing for Disabled Persons is Designing for Everyone

There are so many products on the market that readily reveal their unfitness for the
general population of users. What evaluation should one make of a doorway too
narrow to comfortably accommodate a stout person or pregnant woman? Shall its
physical design be faulted if it satisfies all relevant tests of mechanical function?
Certainly such users cannot be regarded as disabled in any physical sense, since
they may be in a state of perfect health. Some products need considerable handling
before one can force them to perform as desired—certain inexpensive can openers
come, not altogether trivially, to mind. Yet a healthy person who may experience
difficulty operating such devices need not be considered disabled. A designer who
successfully incorporates considerations of human interaction with the product
enhances the functional utility of his or her design. In the absence of good
functional design, people with a minor disability may find themselves in the
position of being severely hampered. It is thus the user-machine interface that
determines the value of design for all users.

It is a truism to remind oneself that technology exists to serve human beings; its
use is to enlarge their own capabilities. For disabled persons, this enlargement has
to be taken some steps further. For instance in the 1980s, a good deal of attention
was paid to the design of kitchens for disabled people. The experience gained in
this work penetrated design features for “normal” kitchens; the disabled person in
this sense may be considered a pioneer. Occupationally-induced impairments and
disabilities—one has but to consider the musculoskeletal and other complaints
suffered by those confined to sedentary tasks so common in the new workplace—
similarly call for design efforts aimed not only also preventing the recurrence of
such conditions, but at the development of user-compatible technology adapted to
the needs of workers already affected by work-related disorders.

The Broader Average Person


The designer should not focus on a small, unrepresentative population. Among
certain groups it is most unwise to entertain assumptions concerning similarities
among them. For example, a worker injured in a certain way as an adult may not
necessarily be anthropometrically quite so different from an otherwise comparable,
healthy person, and may be considered as part of the broad average. A young child
so injured will display a considerably different anthropometry as an adult since his
muscular and mechanical development will be steadily and sequentially influenced
by preceding growth stages. (No conclusions as to comparability as adults ought to
be ventured as regards the two cases. They must be regarded as two distinct,
specific groups, only the one being included among the broad average.) But as one
strives for a design suitable for, say, 90% of the population, one should exert
fractionally greater pains to increase this margin to, say, 95%, the point being that
in this way the need for design for specific groups can be reduced.

Another way to approach design for the broader average population is to produce
two products, each one designed roughly to fit the two percentile extremes of
human differences. Two sizes of chair, for instance, might be built, the one with
brackets allowing it to be adjusted in height from 38 to 46 cm, and the other one
from 46 to 54 cm; two sizes of pliers already exist, one fitting larger and average
sizes of men’s hands and the other fitting average women’s hands and hands of
smaller men.

It would be a well-advised company policy to reserve annually a modest amount of


money to have worksites analysed and made more suitable for workers, a move
that would prevent illness and disability due to excessive physical load. It also
increases the motivation of workers when they understand that management is
actively trying to improve their work environment, and more impressively so when
elaborate measures sometimes have to be undertaken: thorough work analysis, the
construction of mock-ups, anthropometrical measurements, and even the specific
design of units for the workers. In a certain company, in fact, the conclusion was
that the units should be redesigned at every worksite because they caused physical
overload in the form of too much standing, there were unsuitable dimensions
associated with the seated positions, and there were other deficiencies as well.

Costs, Benefits and Usability of Design

Cost/benefit analyses are developed by ergonomists in order to gain insight into the
results of ergonomic policies other than those that are economic. In the present
day, evaluation in the industrial and commercial realms includes the negative or
positive impact of a policy on the worker.

Methods of evaluating quality and usability are currently the subject of active
research. The Rehabilitation Technology Useability Model (RTUM), as shown
in figure 29.51 , can be utilized as a model for evaluating the usability of a product
within rehabilitation technology and to illuminate the various aspects of the
product which determine its usability.

Figure 29.51 The Rehabilitation Technology Useability Model (RTUM)

From the strictly economic point of view, the costs of creating a system in which a
given task can be performed or in which a certain product can be made can be
specified; it scarcely needs mentioning that in these terms each company is
interested in a maximum return on its investment. But how can the real costs of
task performance and product manufacturing in relation to financial investment be
determined when one takes into account the varying exertions of workers’
physical, cognitive and mental systems? In fact, the judging of human performance
itself is, among other factors, based on the workers’ perception of what has to be
done, their view of their own value in doing it, and their opinion of the company. It
is actually the intrinsic satisfaction with work that is the norm of value in this
context, and this satisfaction, together with the aims of the company, constitute
one’s reason for performing. Worker well-being and performance are thus based on
a wide spectrum of experiences, associations and perceptions that determine
attitudes towards work and the ultimate quality of performance—an understanding
upon which the RTUM model is predicated.

If one does not accept this view, it becomes necessary to regard investment only in
relation to doubtful and unspecified results. If ergonomists and physicians wish to
improve the work environment of disabled people—to produce more from machine
operations and enhance the usability of the tools used—they will encounter
difficulties in finding ways to justify the financial investment. Typically, such
justification has been sought in savings realized by prevention of injury and illness
due to work. But if the costs of illness have been borne not by the company but by
the state, they become financially invisible, so to speak, and are not seen as work-
related.

Nevertheless, the awareness that investment in a healthy working environment is


money well spent has been growing with the recognition that the “social” costs of
incapacities are translatable in terms of ultimate costs to a country’s economy, and
that value is lost when a potential worker is sitting about at home making no
contribution to society. Investing in a workplace (in terms of adapting a work
station or providing special tools or perhaps even help in personal hygiene) can not
only reward a person with job satisfaction but can help make him or her self-
sufficient and independent of social assistance.

Cost/benefit analyses can be carried out in order to determine whether special


intervention in the workplace is justified for disabled persons. The following
factors represent sources of data that would form the object of such analyses:

1. Personnel

· Absence. Will the disabled worker have a satisfactory attendance record?

· Is it likely that extra costs may be incurred for special task instruction?

· Are personnel changes called for? Their costs must be considered also.

· Can accident compensation rates be expected to increase?

2. Safety

· Will the job being considered for the disabled worker involve safety
regulations?

· Will special safety regulations be involved?

· Is the work characterized by a considerable frequency of accidents or near


accidents?

3. Medical

· As regards the worker whose disability is being examined with a view to his or
her re-entry into the workplace, the nature and seriousness of the incapacity must
be assessed.

· The extent of the disabled worker’s absence must also be taken into account.
· What is the character and frequency of the worker’s “minor” symptoms, and
how are they to be dealt with? Can the future development of related “minor”
illnesses capable of hampering the worker’s efficiency be foreseen?

As concerns time lost from work, these calculations can be made in terms of
wages, overhead, compensation and lost production. The sort of analyses just
described represents a rational approach by which an organization can arrive at an
informed decision as to whether a disabled worker is better off back on the job and
whether the organization itself will gain by his or her return to work.

In the preceding discussion, designing for the broader population has received a
focus of attention heightened by emphasis on specific design in relation to usability
and the costs and benefits of such design. It is still a difficult task to make the
needed calculations, including all relevant factors, but at present, research efforts
are continuing that incorporate modelling methods in their techniques. In some
countries, for example the Netherlands and Germany, government policy is making
companies more responsible for job-related personal harm; fundamental changes in
regulatory policies and insurance structures are, clearly, to be expected to result
from trends of this sort. It has already become a more or less settled policy in these
countries that a worker who suffers a disabling accident at work should be
provided with an adapted work station or be able to perform other work within the
company, a policy that has made the treatment of the disabled a genuine
achievement in the humane treatment of the worker.

Workers with Limited Functional Capacity

Whether design is aimed at the disabled or at the broader average, it is hindered by


a scarcity of research data. Handicapped people have been the subjects of virtually
no research efforts. Therefore, in order to set up a product requirements document,
or PRD, a specific empirical research study will have to be undertaken in order to
gather that data by observation and measurement.

In gathering the information needed about the disabled worker or user it is


necessary to consider not only the current functional status of the disabled person,
but to make the attempt to foresee whatever changes might be the result of the
progression of a chronic condition. This kind of information can, in fact, be elicited
from the worker directly, or a medical specialist can supply it.

In designing, for instance, a work action to which data about the worker’s physical
strength is relevant, the designer will not choose as a specification the maximum
strength which the disabled person can exert, but will take into account any
possible diminution in strength that a progression in the worker’s condition might
bring about. Thus the worker will be enabled to continue to use the machines and
tools adapted or designed for him or at the work station.
Furthermore, designers should avoid designs that involve manipulations of the
human body at the far extremes of, say, the range of motion of a body part, but
should accommodate their designs to the middle ranges. A simple but very
common illustration of this principle follows. A very common part of the drawers
of kitchen and office cabinets and desks is a handle that has the form of a little
shelf under which one places the fingers, exerting upward and forward force to
open the drawer. This manoeuvre requires 180 degrees of supination (with the
palm of the hand up) in the wrist—the maximum point for the range of this sort of
motion of the wrist. This state of affairs may present no difficulty for a healthy
person, provided that the drawer can be opened with a light force and is not
awkwardly situated, but makes for strain when the action of the drawer is tight or
when the full 180-degree supination is not possible, and is a needless burden on a
disabled person. A simple solution—a vertically placed handle—would be
mechanically far more efficient and more easily manipulated by a larger portion of
the population.

Physical Functioning Ability

In what follows, the three chief areas of limitation in physical functional ability, as
defined by the locomotion system, the neurological system and the energy system,
will be discussed. Designers will gain some insight into the nature of user/worker
constraints in considering the following basic principles of bodily functions.

The locomotion system. This consists of the bones, joints, connective tissues and
muscles. The nature of the joint structure determines the range of motion possible.
A knee joint, for example, shows a different degree of movement and stability than
the joint of the hip or the shoulder. These varying joint characteristics determine
the actions possible to the arms, hands, feet, and so on. There are also different
types of muscle; it is the type of muscle, whether the muscle passes over one or
two joints, and the location of the muscle that determines, for a given body part,
the direction of its movement, its speed, and the strength which it is capable of
exerting.

The fact that this direction, speed and strength can be characterized and calculated
is of great importance in design. For disabled people, one has to take it into
account that the “normal” locations of muscles have been disturbed and that the
range of motion in joints has been changed. In an amputation, for instance, a
muscle may function only partly, or its location may have changed, so that one has
to examine the physical ability of the patient carefully to establish what functions
remain and how reliable they may be. A case history follows.

A 40-year-old carpenter lost his thumb and the third finger of his right hand in an
accident. In an effort to restore the carpenter’s capacity for work, a surgeon
removed one of the patient’s great toes and he replaced the missing thumb with it.
After a period of rehabilitation, the carpenter returned to work but found it
impossible to do sustained work for more than three to four hours. His tools were
studied and found to be unfitted to the “abnormal” structure of his hand. The
rehabilitation specialist, examining the “redesigned” hand from the point of view
of its new functional ability and form was able to have new tools designed that
were more appropriate and usable with respect to the altered hand. The load on the
worker’s hand, previously too heavy, was now within a usable range, and he
regained his ability to continue work for a longer time.

The neurological system. The neurological system can be compared to a very


sophisticated control room, complete with data collectors, whose purpose it is to
initiate and govern one’s movements and actions by interpreting information
relating to those aspects of the body’s components relating to position and
mechanical, chemical and other states. This system incorporates not only a
feedback system (e.g., pain) that provides for corrective measures, but a “feed-
forward” capability which expresses itself anticipatorily so as to maintain a state of
equilibrium. Consider the case of a worker who reflexively acts so as to restore a
posture in order to protect himself from a fall or from contact with dangerous
machine parts.

In disabled persons, the physiological processing of information can be impaired.


Both the feedback and the feed-forward mechanisms of visually impaired people
are weakened or absent, and the same is true, on an acoustic level, among the
hearing-impaired. Furthermore the important governing circuits are interactive.
Sound signals have an effect on the equilibrium of a person in conjunction with
proprioceptive circuits that situate our bodies in space, so to speak, via data
gathered from muscles and joints, with the further help of visual signals. The brain
can function to overcome quite drastic deficiencies in these systems, correcting for
errors in the coding of information and “filling in” missing information. Beyond
certain limits, to be sure, incapacity supervenes. Two case histories follow.

Case 1. A 36-year-old woman suffered a lesion of the spinal cord due to an


automobile accident. She is able to sit up without assistance and can move a
wheelchair manually. Her trunk is stable. The feeling in her legs is gone, however;
this defect includes an inability to sense temperature changes.

She has a sitting workplace at home (the kitchen is designed to allow her to work
in a seated position). The safety measure has been taken of installing a sink in a
position sufficiently isolated that the risk of burning her legs with hot water is
minimized, since her inability to process temperature information in the legs leaves
her vulnerable to being unaware of being burned.

Case 2. A five-year-old boy whose left side was paralysed was being bathed by his
mother. The doorbell rang, the mother left the boy alone to go to the front door,
and the boy, turning on the hot-water tap, suffered burns. For safety reasons, the
bath should have been equipped with a thermostat (preferably one that the boy
could not have overridden).

The energy system. When the human body has to perform physical labour,
physiological changes, notably in the form of interactions in the muscle cells, take
place, albeit relatively inefficiently. The human “motor” converts only about 25%
of its energy supply to mechanical activity, the remainder of the energy
representing thermal losses. The human body is therefore not especially suited to
heavy physical labour. Exhaustion sets in after a certain time, and if heavy labour
has to be performed, reserve energy sources are drawn upon. These sources of
reserve energy are always used whenever work is carried out very rapidly, is
started suddenly (without a warm-up period) or involves heavy exertion.

The human organism obtains energy aerobically (via oxygen in the bloodstream)
and anaerobically (after depleting aerobic oxygen, it calls upon small, but
important reserve units of energy stored in muscle tissue). The need for fresh air
supplies in the workplace naturally draws the focus of discussion of oxygen usage
toward the aerobic side, working conditions that are strenuous enough to call forth
anaerobic processes on a regular basis being extraordinarily uncommon in most
workplaces, at least in the developed countries. The availability of atmospheric
oxygen, which relates so directly to human aerobic functioning, is a function of
several conditions:

· Ambient air pressure (approximately 760 torr, or 21.33 kPa at sea level). High-
altitude task performance can be profoundly affected by oxygen deficiency and is a
prime consideration for workers in such conditions.

· For workers doing heavy labour, ventilation is necessary to ensure refreshment


of the air supply, allowing the volume of air respired per minute to be increased.

· Ambient oxygen makes its way into the bloodstream via the alveoli by
diffusion. At higher blood pressures, the diffusion surface is enlarged and thereby
the oxygen capacity of the blood.

· An increase in oxygen diffusion to the tissues causes an increase of the


diffusion surface and consequently of the oxygen level.

· People with certain heart problems suffer when, with increased cardiac output
(together with the oxygen level), the blood circulation changes in favour of the
muscles.

· By contrast with oxygen, because of the large reserves of glucose, and


especially fat, the energy source (“fuel”) need not be continuously delivered from
the outside. In heavy labour, it is merely glucose, with its high energy value, that is
used. With lighter work, fat is called upon, at a rate varying with the individual. A
brief, general case history follows.

A person suffering from asthma or bronchitis, both of which are diseases affecting
the lungs, causes the worker severe limitation in his or her work. The work
assignment of this worker should be analysed with respect to factors such as
physical load. The environment should be analysed as well: clean ambient air will
contribute substantially to workers’ well-being. Furthermore, the workload should
be balanced through the day, avoiding peak loads.

Specific Design

In some cases, however, there is still a need for specific design, or design for very
small groups. Such a need arises when the tasks to be performed and the
difficulties a disabled person is experiencing are excessively large. If the needed
specific requirements cannot be made with the available products on the market
(even with adaptations), specific design is the answer. Whether this sort of solution
may be costly or cheap (and aside from humanitarian issues) it must be nonetheless
regarded in the light of workability and support to the firm’s viability. A specially
designed worksite is worthwhile economically only when the disabled worker can
look forward to working there for years and when the work he or she does is, in
production terms, an asset to the company. When this is not the case, although the
worker may indeed insist upon his or her right to the job, a sense of realism should
prevail. Such touchy problems should be approached in a spirit of seeking a
solution by cooperative endeavours at communication.

The advantages of specific design are as follows:

· The design is custom made: it fits the problems to be solved to perfection.

· The worker so served can return to work and a life of social participation.

· The worker can be self-sufficient, independent of welfare.

· The costs of any personnel changes that the alternative might involve are
avoided.

The disadvantages of specific design are:

· The design is unlikely to be used for even one other person, let alone a larger
group.

· Specific design is often costly.

· Specifically designed products must often be handmade; savings owing to


mass methods are most often not realizable.
Case 1. For example, there is the case of a receptionist in a wheelchair who had a
speech problem. Her speech difficulty made for rather slow conversations. While
the firm remained small, no problems arose and she continued to work there for
years. But when the firm enlarged, her disabilities began to make themselves
problematic. She had to speak more rapidly and to move about considerably faster;
she could not cope with the new demands. However, solutions to her troubles were
sought and reduced themselves to two alternatives: special technical equipment
might be installed so that the deficiencies that degraded the quality of some of her
tasks could be compensated for, or she could simply choose a set of tasks involving
a more desk-bound workload. She chose the latter course and still works for the
same company.

Case 2. A young man, whose profession was the production of technical drawings,
suffered a high level spinal cord lesion due to diving in shallow waters. His injury
is severe enough for him to require help with all his daily activities. Nevertheless,
with the help of a computer-aided design (CAD) software, he continues to be able
make his living at technical drawing and lives, financially independent, with his
partner. His work space is a study adapted for his needs and he works for a firm
with which he communicates by computer, phone and fax. To operate his personal
computer, he had to have certain adaptations made to the keyboard. But with these
technical assets he can earn a living and provide for himself.

The approach for specific design is not different from other design as described
above. The only insurmountable problem that may arise during a design project is
that the design objective cannot be achieved on purely technical grounds—in other
words, it can’t be done. For example, a person suffering from Parkinson’s disease
is prone, at a certain stage in the progression of his or her condition, to fall over
backwards. An aid which would prevent such an eventuality would of course
represent the desired solution, but the state of the art is not such that such a device
can yet be built.

System Ergonomic Design and Workers with Special Physical Needs

One can treat bodily impairment by medically intervening to restore the damaged
function, but the treatment of a disability, or deficiency in the ability to perform
tasks, can involve measures far less developed in comparison with medical
expertise. As far as the necessity of treating a disability is concerned, the severity
of the handicap strongly influences such a decision. But given that treatment is
called for, however, the following means, taken singly or in combination, form the
choices available to the designer or manager:

· leaving out a task

· compensating for a worker’s deficiency in performing a task element by using


a machine or another person’s help
· differentiation of the task order, that is, dividing the task into more manageable
subtasks

· modification of the tools used in the task

· special design of tools and machines.

From the specific ergonomic point of view, treatment of a disability includes the
following:

· modification of the task

· modification of a tool

· design of new tools or new machines.

The issue of efficacy is always the point of departure in the modification of tools or
machines, and is often related to the costs devoted to the modification in question,
the technical features to be addressed, and the functional changes to be embodied
in the new design. Comfort and attractiveness are qualities that by no means
deserve to be neglected among these other characteristics.

The next consideration relating to design changes to be made to a tool or machine


is whether the device is one already designed for general use (in which case,
modifications will be made to a pre-existing product) or is to be designed with an
individual type of disability in mind. In the latter case, specific ergonomic
considerations must be devoted to each aspect of the worker’s disability. For
example, given a worker suffering from limitations in brain function after a stroke,
impairments such as aphasia (difficulty in communication), a paralysed right arm,
and a spastic paresis of the leg preventing its being moved upwards might require
the following adjustments:

· a personal computer or other device enabling the worker to communicate

· tools that can be operated with the remaining useful arm

· a prosthetic system that would serve to restore the function of the impaired foot
as well as to compensate for the patient’s loss of ability to walk.

Is there any general answer to the question of how to design for the disabled
worker? The system ergonomic design (SED) approach is an eminently suitable
one for this task. Research related to the work situation or to the kind of product at
issue requires a design team for the purpose of gathering special information
relating either to a special group of disabled workers or to the unique case of an
individual user disabled in a particular way. The design team will, by virtue of
including a diversity of qualified people, be in possession of expertise beyond the
technical sort expected of a designer alone; the medical and ergonomic knowledge
shared among them will be as fully applicable as the strictly technical.

Design constraints determined by assembling data related to disabled users are


treated with the same objectivity and in the same analytical spirit as are counterpart
data relating to healthy users. Just as for the latter, one has to determine for
disabled persons their personal patterns of behavioural response, their
anthropometrical profiles, biomechanical data (as to reach, strength, range of
motion, handling space used, physical load and so forth), ergonomic standards and
safety regulations. But one is most regretfully obliged to concede that very little
research indeed is done on behalf of disabled workers. There exist a few studies on
anthropometry, somewhat more on biomechanics in the field of prostheses and
orthoses, but hardly any studies have been carried on physical load capabilities.
(The reader will find references to such material in the “Other relevant reading” list
at the end of this chapter.) And while it is sometimes easy to gather and apply such
data, frequently enough the task is difficult, and in fact, impossible. To be sure, one
must obtain objective data, however strenuous the effort and unlikely the chances
of doing so, given that the numbers of disabled persons available for research is
small. But they are quite often more than willing to participate in whatever
research they are offered the opportunity of sharing in, since there is great
consciousness of the importance of such a contribution towards design and
research in this field. It thus represents an investment not only for themselves but
for the larger community of disabled people.

SYSTEM DESIGN IN DIAMOND MANUFACTURING


Issachar Gilad*

*The author acknowledges the assistance of Mr. E. Messer and Prof. W. Laurig for
their contribution to the biomechanical and design aspects, and to Prof. H. Stein
and Dr. R. Langer for their help with the physiological aspects of the polishing
process. The research was supported by a grant from the Committee for Research
and Prevention in Occupational Safety and Health, Ministry of Labor and Social
Affairs, Israel.

The design of manually operated work benches and working methods in the
diamond polishing industry has not changed for hundreds of years. Occupational
health studies of diamond polishers have identified high rates of musculoskeletal
disorders of the hands and arms, specifically, ulnar neuropathy at the elbow. These
are due to the high musculoskeletal demands placed on the upper body in the
practice of this manually intensive profession. A study conducted at the Technion
Israel Institute of Technology addressed itself to the investigation of the ergonomic
aspects and occupational diseases relating to safety issues among craftsmen in the
diamond polishing industry. The tasks in this industry, with its high demands for
manipulative movements, include movements that require frequent, rapid hand
exertions. An epidemiological review conducted during the years 1989-1992 in the
Israeli diamond industry has pointed out that the manipulative movements
experienced in diamond polishing very often cause serious health problems to the
worker in the upper extremities and in the upper and lower back. When such
occupational hazards affect workers, it produces a chain reaction that eventually
affects the industry’s economy as well.

For thousands of years, diamonds have been objects of fascination, beauty,


richness and capital value. Skillful craftsmen and artists have tried, through the
ages, to create beauty by enhancing the shape and values of this unique form of
hard carbon crystal formation. In contrast to the continuing achievements of artistic
creation with the native stone and the emergence of a great international industry,
very little has been done to improve some questionable working conditions. A
survey of the diamond museums in England, South Africa and Israel allows one to
draw the historical conclusion that the traditional polishing workplace has not
changed for hundreds of years. The typical diamond polishing tools, working
bench and work processes are described by Vleeschdrager (1986), and they have
been found to be universally common to all polishing setups.

Ergonomic evaluation performed at diamond manufacturing setups points to a


great lack of engineering design of the polishing workstation, which causes back
pain and neck and arm stress due to working posture. A micromotion study and
biomechanical analysis of motion patterns involved in the diamond polishing
profession indicate extremely intense hand and arm movements that involve high
acceleration, rapid movement and a great degree of repetitiveness in short-period
cycles. A symptom survey of diamond polishers indicated that 45% of the
polishers were younger than 40 years of age, and although they represent a young
and healthy population, 64% reported pain in the shoulders, 36% pain in the upper
arm and 27% pain in the lower arm. The act of polishing is performed under an
extensive amount of “hand on tool” pressure which is applied to a vibrating
polishing disk.

The first known description of a diamond polishing workstation was given in 1568
by the Italian goldsmith, Benvenuto Cellini, who wrote: “One diamond is rubbed
against another until by mutual abrasion both take a form which the skilled
polisher wishes to achieve.” Cellini’s description could have been written today:
the role of the human operator has not changed over these 400 years. If one
examines the working routines, hand tools and the nature of the decisions involved
in the process one can see that the user-machine relationship has also hardly
changed. This situation is unique among most industries where enormous changes
have occurred with the entry of automation, robotics and computer systems; these
have completely changed the role of the worker in the world today. Yet the
polishing work cycle has been found to be very similar, not only in Europe where
the polishing craft started, but in most industries all over the globe, whether in
advanced facilities in the United States, Belgium or Israel—which specialize in
fancy geometry and higher-value diamond products—or the facilities in India,
China and Thailand, which generally produce popular shapes and mid-value
products.

The polishing process is based on grinding the fixed rough diamond over diamond
dust bonded to the polishing disk’s surface. Owing to its hardness, only grinding
by friction against similar carbon material is effective in manipulating the
diamond’s shape to its geometric and brilliant finish. The workstation hardware is
composed of two basic groups of elements: workstation mechanisms and hand-
held tools. The first group includes an electric motor, which rotates a polishing
disk on a vertical cylindrical shaft, perhaps by a single direct drive; a solid flat
table which surrounds the polishing disk; a bench seat and a source of light. The
hand-held operating tools consist of a diamond holder (or tang) which houses the
rough stone during all polishing phases and is usually held in the left palm. The
work is magnified with a convex lens which is held between the first, second and
third fingers of the right hand and viewed with the left eye. This method of
operation is imposed by a strict training process which in most cases does not take
handedness into account. During work the polisher assumes a reclining posture,
pressing the holder to the grinding disk. This posture requires the support of the
arms on the working table in order to stabilize the hands. As a result, the ulnar
nerve is vulnerable to external lesions due to its anatomical position. Such an
injury is common among diamond polishers and has been accepted as an
occupational disease since the 1950s. The number of polishers worldwide today is
around 450,000, of whom approximately 75% are located in the Far East, primarily
India, which has dramatically expanded its diamond industry in the last two
decades. The act of polishing is done manually, with each of the diamond facets
being produced by polishers who are trained and skilled with respect to a certain
part of the stone’s geometry. The polishers are a clear majority of the diamond
craft force, composing about 80% of the overall industry’s workforce. Therefore,
most of the occupational risks of this industry can be addressed through improving
the operation of the diamond polishing workstation.

Analysis of the motion patterns involved in polishing shows that the polishing
routine consists of two subroutines: a simpler routine called the polish cycle, which
represents the basic diamond polishing operation, and a more important one called
the facet cycle, which involves a final inspection and a change of the stone’s
position in the holder. The overall procedure includes four basic work elements:

1. Polishing. This is simply the actual polishing operation.

2. Inspection. Every few seconds the operator, using a magnifying glass, visually
inspects the progress made on the polished facet.

3. Dop adjustment. An angular adjustment is made to the diamond holder’s head


(dop).
4. Stone change. The act of changing facets, which is done by turning the
diamond through a predetermined angle. It takes about 25 repetitions of these four
elements to polish a diamond’s facet. The number of such repetitions depends upon
such aspects as operator’s age, stone hardness and characteristics, time of day
(owing to operator fatigue), and so on. On average, each repetition takes about four
seconds. A micromotion study as performed on the polishing process and the
methodology used is given by Gilad (1993).

Two of the elements—polishing and inspection—are performed in relatively static


working postures while so-called “hand to polish” (H to P) and “hand to inspect”
(H to I) actions require short and fast movements of the shoulder, elbow and wrist.
Most of the actual movements of both hands are performed by flexion and
extension of the elbow and pronation and supination of the elbow. Body posture
(back and neck) and all other movements except wrist deviation are relatively
unchanged during normal work. The stone holder, which is constructed of a square
cross-sectional steel rod, is held so that it presses on blood vessels and bone, which
can result in a reduction of blood flow to the ring and little fingers. The right hand
holds the magnifying glass all during the polishing cycle, exerting isometric
pressure on the three first fingers. For most of the time the right and left hands
follow parallel movement patterns, while in the “hand to grind” movement the left
hand leads and the right hand starts moving after a short delay, and in the “hand to
inspect” movement the order is reversed. Right-hand tasks involve either holding
the magnifying glass to the inspecting left eye while supporting the left hand
(elbow flexion), or by putting pressure on the diamond holder head for better
grinding (elbow extension). These fast movements result in rapid accelerations and
decelerations that end up in a very precise placing of the stone on the grinding
disk, which requires a high level of manual dexterity. It should be noted that it
takes long years to become proficient to the point where work movements are
almost embedded reflexes executed automatically.

On the face of it, diamond polishing is a simple straightforward task, and in a way
it is, but it requires much skill and experience. In contrast to all other industries,
where raw and processed material is controlled and manufactured according to
exact specifications, the diamond in the rough is not homogeneous and each
diamond crystal, large or small, has to be checked, categorized and treated
individually. Apart from the needed manual skill, the polisher has to make
operational decisions at every polishing phase. As a result of the visual inspection,
decisions must be made on such factors as angular spatial correction—a three-
dimensional judgment—amount and duration of pressure to be applied, angular
positioning of the stone, contact point on the grinding disk, among others. Many
points of significance have to be considered, all in the average time of four
seconds. it is important to understand this decision-making process when
improvements are designed.
Before one can advance to the stage at which motion analysis can be used for
setting better ergonomic design and engineering criteria for a polishing
workstation, one has to be aware of yet further aspects involved in this unique
user-machine system. In this post-automation age, we still find the production part
of the successful and expanding diamond industry almost untouched by the
enormous technological advances made in the last few decades. While almost all
other sectors of industry have undergone continuing technology change that
defined not only production methods but the products themselves, the diamond
industry has remained virtually static. A plausible reason for this stability may be
the fact that neither the product nor the market have changed through the ages. The
design and shapes of diamonds have in practice remained almost unchanged. From
the business point of view, there was no reason to change the product or the
methods. Furthermore, since most of the polishing work is done by subcontracting
to individual workers, the industry had no problem in regulating the labour force,
adjusting the flow of work and the supply of rough diamonds according to market
fluctuations. As long as the production methods do not change, the product will not
change either. Once the use of more advanced technology and automation are
adopted by the diamond industry, the product will change, with a greater variety of
forms available in the market. But a diamond still has a mystic quality that sets it
apart from other products, a value that may well decrease when it comes to be
regarded as merely another mass-produced item. Recently though, market
pressures and the arrival of new production centres, mainly in the Far East, are
challenging the old established European centres. These are forcing the industry to
examine new methods and production systems and the role of the human operator.

When considering improving the polishing workstation, one must look upon it as
part of a user-machine system that is governed by three main factors: the human
factor, the technology factor and the business factor. A new design that takes
account of ergonomic principles will provide a springboard to a better production
cell in the broad sense of the term, meaning comfort over long working hours, a
better quality product and higher production rates. Two different design
approaches have been considered. One involves a redesign of the existing
workstation, with the worker given the same tasks to perform. The second
approach is to look at the polishing task in an unbiased manner, aiming at an
optimal, total station and task design. A total design should not be based on the
present workstation as input but on the future polishing task, generating design
solutions that integrate and optimize the needs of the three above-mentioned
system factors.

At present, the human operator performs most of the tasks involved in the
polishing act. These human-performed tasks rely on “filling” and working
experience. This is a complex psychophysiological process, only partially
conscious, based on trial and error input which enables an operator to execute
complex operations with a good prediction of the outcome. During periodic daily
work cycles of thousands of identical movements, “filling” manifests itself in the
human-automatic operation of motor memory executed with great precision. For
each of these automatic motions, tiny corrections are made in response to feedback
received from the human sensors, like the eyes, and the pressure sensors. In any
future diamond polishing workstation these tasks will continue to be performed in
a different way. As to the material itself, in the diamond industry, by contrast with
most other industries, the relative value of the raw material is very high. This fact
explains the importance of making maximal use of the rough diamond’s volume
(or stone weight) in order to get the largest net stone possible after polishing. This
emphasis is paramount throughout all the stages of diamond processing.
Productivity and efficiency are not measured by reference to time only, but also by
the size and precision achieved.

The four repetitive work elements—“polish”, “hand to inspect”, “inspect” and


“hand to polish”—as performed in the polishing act, can be classified under the
three main task categories: motor tasks for motion elements, visual tasks as sensing
elements, and control and management as decision-content elements. Gilad and
Messer (1992) discuss design considerations for an ergonomic workstation. Figure
29.52 presents an outline of an advanced polishing-cell. Only the general
construction is indicated, since the details of such a design are guarded as a
professionally restricted “know-how”. The term polishing cell is used since this
user-machine system includes a totally different approach to polishing diamonds.
In addition to ergonomic improvements, the system consists of mechanical and
optoelectronic devices that enable the manufacture of three to five stones at the
same time. Parts of visual and control tasks have been transferred to technical
operators and management of the production cell is mediated via a display unit
which provides momentary information about geometry, weight and optional
operation moves in order to support optimal operating acts. Such a design takes the
polishing workstation a few steps ahead into modernization, incorporating an
expert system and a visual control system to replace the human eye in all routine
work. Operators will still be able to intervene at any point, set up data and make
human judgements on machine performance. The mechanical manipulator and the
expert system will form a closed-loop system capable of performing all polishing
tasks. Material handling, quality control and final approval will still reside with the
operator. At this stage of an advanced system, it would be appropriate to consider
the employment of higher technology such as a laser polisher. At present, lasers are
being used extensively to saw and cut diamonds. Using a technologically advanced
system will radically change the human task description. The need for skilled
polishers will diminish until they will deal only with polishing larger, top-valued
diamonds, probably with supervision.

Figure 29.52 Schematic presentation of a polishing-cell


DISREGARDING ERGONOMIC DESIGN
PRINCIPLES: CHERNOBYL
Vladimir M. Munipov

The causes of the 1986 Chernobyl disaster have been variously attributed to the
operating personnel, the plant management, the design of the reactor and the lack
of adequate safety information in the Soviet nuclear industry. This article considers
a number of design faults, operational shortcomings and human errors that
combined in the accident. It examines the sequence of events leading up to the
accident, design problems in the reactor and cooling rods, and the course of the
accident itself. It considers the ergonomics aspects, and expresses the view that the
main cause of the accident was inadequate user-machine interaction. Finally, it
stresses the continuing inadequacies, and emphasizes that unless the ergonomics
lessons are fully learned, a similar disaster could still occur.

The full story of the Chernobyl disaster is yet to be disclosed. To speak candidly,
the truth is still veiled by self-serving reticence, half-truths, secrecy and even
falsehood. A comprehensive study of the causes of the accident appears to be a
very difficult task. The main problem faced by the investigator is the need to
reconstruct the accident and the role of the human factors in it on the basis of the
tiny bits of information that have been made available for study. The Chernobyl
disaster is more than a severe technological accident, part of the reasons for the
disaster also lie with the administration and the bureaucracy. However, the chief
aim of this article is to consider the design faults, the operational shortcomings and
the human errors that combined in the Chernobyl accident.

Who is to blame?

The chief designer for the pressure tube large power boiling water reactors
(RBMK) used at the Chernobyl nuclear power plant (NPP), in 1989, presented his
view on the causes of the Chernobyl accident. He attributed the disaster to the fact
that the personnel failed to observe the correct procedures, or “production
discipline”. He pointed out that the lawyers investigating the accident had arrived
at the same conclusion. According to his view, “the fault lies with the personnel
rather than some design or manufacturing failings.” The research supervisor for the
RBMK development supported this view. The possibility of ergonomic inadequacy
as a causative factor was not considered.

The operators themselves expressed a different opinion. The shift supervisor of the
fourth unit, A.F. Akimov, when dying in a hospital as a result of receiving a dose
of radiation of more than 1,500 rads (R) in a short period of time during the
accident, kept telling his parents that his actions had been correct and he could not
understand what had gone wrong. His persistence reflected absolute trust in a
reactor that was supposedly completely safe. Akimov also said that he had nothing
to blame his crew for. The operators were sure that their actions were in accord
with regulations, and the latter did not mention the eventuality of an explosion at
all. (Remarkably, the possibility of the reactor’s becoming dangerous under certain
conditions was introduced into the safety regulations only after the Chernobyl
accident.) However, in light of design problems revealed subsequently, it is
significant that the operators could not understand why inserting rods into the core
caused such a terrible explosion instead of instantly stopping the nuclear reaction
as designed. In other words, in this case they acted correctly according to the
maintenance instructions and to their mental model of the reactor system, but the
design of the system failed to correspond to that model.

Six persons, representing only the plant management, were convicted, in view of
the human losses, on the grounds of having violated safety regulations for
potentially explosive facilities. The chairman presiding over the court said some
words to the effect of proceeding with the investigations as regards “those who
failed to take measures to improve the plant design”. He also mentioned the
responsibility of department officials, local authorities and medical services. But,
in fact, it was clear that the case was closed. Nobody else was held responsible for
the greatest disaster in the history of nuclear technology.
However, it is necessary to investigate all causative factors that combined in the
disaster to learn important lessons for safe future operation of NPPs.

Secrecy: The information monopoly in research and industry

The failure of the user-machine relationship that resulted in “Chernobyl-86” can be


attributed in some measure to the policy of secrecy—the enforcement of an
information monopoly—that governed technological communication in the Soviet
nuclear energy establishment. A small group of scientists and researchers were
given an exhaustive right to define the basic principles and procedures in nuclear
power, a monopoly reliably protected by the policy of secrecy. As a result,
reassurances by Soviet scientists as regards the absolute safety of NPPs remained
unchallenged for 35 years, and secrecy veiled the incompetence of the civil nuclear
leaders. Incidentally, it became known recently that this secrecy was extended to
information relating to the Three Mile Island accident as well; the operating
personnel of Soviet NPPs were not fully informed about this accident—only
selected items of information, which did not contradict the official view on NPP
safety, were made known. A report on the human engineering aspects of the Three
Mile Island accident, presented by the author of this paper in 1985, was not
distributed to those involved with safety and reliability of NPPs.

No Soviet nuclear accidents were ever made public except for the accidents at the
Armenian and Chernobyl (1982) nuclear power plants, which were casually
mentioned in the newspaper Pravda. By concealing the true state of affairs (thus
failing to make use of lessons based on the accident analyses) the leaders of the
nuclear power industry were setting it straight on the path to Chernobyl-86, a path
that was further smoothed by the fact that a simplified idea of the operator
activities had been implanted and the risk of operating NPPs was underestimated.

As a member of the State Expert Committee on the Consequences of the


Chernobyl accident stated in 1990: “To err no more, we have to admit all our
errors and analyse them. It is essential to determine which errors were due to our
inexperience and which ones were actually a deliberate attempt to hide the truth.”

The Chernobyl Accident of 1986

Faulty planning of the test

On 25 April 1986, the fourth unit of the Chernobyl NPP (Chernobyl 4) was being
prepared for routine maintenance. The plan was to shut the unit down and perform
an experiment involving inoperative safety systems totally deprived of power from
normal sources. This test should have been carried out before the initial Chernobyl
4 startup. However, the State Committee was in such a hurry to start up the unit
that they decided to postpone indefinitely some “insignificant” tests. The
Acceptance Certificate was signed at the end of 1982. Hence, the deputy chief
engineer was acting according to the earlier plan, which presupposed a wholly
inactive unit; his planning and timing of the test proceeded according to this
implicit assumption. This test was in no way carried out on his own initiative.

The programme of the test was approved by the chief engineer. The power during
the test was supposed to be generated from the rundown energy of the turbine rotor
(during its inertia-induced rotation). When still rotating, the rotor provides electric
power generation which could be used in an emergency. Total loss of power at a
nuclear plant causes all mechanisms to stop, including the pumps which provide
for the coolant circulation in the core, which in turn results in core meltdown—a
grave accident. The above experiment was aimed at testing the possibility of using
some other available means—the inertial rotation of the turbine—to produce
power. It is not forbidden to perform such tests at operating plants provided that an
adequate procedure has been developed and additional safety precautions have
been worked out. The programme must ensure that a back-up power supply for the
whole test period is provided. In other words, the loss of power is only implied but
never actualized. The test may be performed only after the reactor is shut down,
that is, when the “scram” button is pushed and the absorbing rods are inserted in
the core. Prior to this, the reactor must be in a stable controlled condition with the
reactivity margin specified in the operating procedure, with at least 28 to 30
absorbing rods inserted in the core.

The programme approved by the chief engineer of the Chernobyl plant satisfied
none of the above requirements. Moreover, it called for the shutting off of the
emergency core cooling system (ECCS), thus jeopardizing the safety of the plant
for the whole test period (about four hours). When developing the programme, the
initiators took into account the possibility of triggering the ECCS, an eventuality
which would have prevented them from completing the rundown test. The bleed-
off method was not specified in the programme since the turbine no longer needed
steam. Clearly, the people involved were completely ignorant of reactor physics.
The nuclear power leaders obviously included similarly unqualified people as well,
which would account for the fact that when the above programme was submitted
for approval to the responsible authorities in January 1986, it was never
commented on by them in any way. The dulled feeling of danger also made its
contribution. Owing to the policy of secrecy surrounding nuclear technology the
opinion had formed that nuclear power plants were safe and reliable, and that their
operation was accident-free. Lack of official response to the programme did not,
however, alert the director of the Chernobyl plant to the possibility of danger. He
decided to proceed with the test using the uncertified programme, even though it
was not permitted.

Change in the test programme

While performing the test, the personnel violated the programme itself, thus
creating further possibilities for an accident. The Chernobyl personnel committed
six gross errors and violations. According to the programme the ECCS was made
inoperative, this being one of the gravest and most fatal errors. The feedwater
control valves had been cut off and locked beforehand so that it would be
impossible even to open them manually. The emergency cooling was deliberately
put out of action in order to prevent possible thermal shock resulting from cold
water entering the hot core. This decision was based on the firm belief that the
reactor would hold out. The “faith” in the reactor was strengthened by the
comparatively trouble-free ten years’ operation of the plant. Even a serious
warning, the partial core meltdown at the first Chernobyl unit in September 1982,
was ignored.

According to the test programme the rotor rundown was to be carried out at a
power level of 700 to 1000 MWth (megawatts of thermal power). Such a rundown
should have been performed as the reactor was being shut down, but the other,
disastrous, way was chosen: to proceed with the test with the reactor still operating.
This was done to ensure the “purity” of the experiment.

In certain operating conditions, it becomes necessary to change or turn off a local


control for clusters of absorbing rods. When turning off one of these local systems
(the means of doing this are specified in the procedure for low-power operation),
the senior reactor control engineer was slow to correct the imbalance in the control
system. As a result, the power fell below 30 MWth which led to fission-product
reactor poisoning (with xenon and iodine). In such an event, it is next to impossible
to restore normal conditions without interrupting the test and waiting a day until
the poisoning is overcome. The deputy chief engineer for operations did not want
to interrupt the test and, by means of shouting at them, forced the control-room
operators to begin raising the power level (which had been stabilized at 200 MWth).
The reactor poisoning continued, but further power increase was impermissible
owing to the small operating reactivity margin of only 30 rods for a large power
pressure-tube reactor (RBMK). The reactor became practically uncontrollable and
potentially explosive because, in trying to overcome the poisoning, the operators
withdrew several rods needed to maintain the reactivity safety margin, thus making
the scram system ineffective. Nevertheless, it was decided to proceed with the test.
Operator behaviour was evidently motivated mainly by the desire to complete the
test as soon as possible.

Problems due to the inadequate design of the reactor and absorbing rods

To give a better understanding of the causes of the accident, it is necessary to point


out the major design deficiencies of the absorbing rods of the control and scram
system. The core height is 7 m, while the absorbing length of the rods amounts to 5
m with 1 m hollow parts above and below it. The bottom ends of the absorbing
rods, which go under the core when fully inserted, are filled with graphite. Given
such a design, the control rods enter the core followed by one-metre hollow parts
and, finally, come the absorbing parts.
At Chernobyl 4 , there were a total of 211 absorbing rods, 205 of which were fully
withdrawn. Simultaneous reinsertion of so many rods initially results in reactivity
overshoot (a peak in fission activity), since at first the graphite ends and hollow
parts enter the core. In a stable controlled reactor such a burst is nothing to worry
about, but in the event of a combination of adverse conditions, such an addition
may prove fatal since it leads to prompt neutron reactor runaway. The immediate
cause of initial reactivity growth was the initiation of water boiling in the core.
This initial reactivity growth reflected one particular drawback: a positive steam
void coefficient, which resulted from the core design. This design deficiency is one
of the faults which caused operator errors.

Grave design faults in the reactor and the absorbing rods actually predetermined
the Chernobyl accident. In 1975, after the accident at the Leningrad plant, and later
on, specialists warned about the possibility of another accident in view of
deficiencies in core design. Six months before the Chernobyl disaster, a safety
inspector at the Kursk plant sent a letter to Moscow in which he pointed out to the
chief researcher and chief designer certain design inadequacies of the reactor and
the control and protection system rods. The State Supervising Committee for
Nuclear Power, however, called his argument groundless.

The course of the accident itself

The course of the events was as follows. With the onset of the reactor coolant
pump cavitation, which led to reduced flow rate in the core, the coolant boiled in
the pressure tubes. Just then, the shift supervisor pushed the button of the scram
system. In response, all the control rods (which had been withdrawn) and the scram
rods dropped into the core. However, first to enter the core were the graphite and
hollow ends of the rods, which cause reactivity growth; and they entered the core
just at the beginning of intensive steam generation. The rise of the core temperature
also produced the same effect. Thus there were combined three conditions
unfavourable for the core. Immediate reactor runaway began. This was due
primarily to gross design deficiencies of the RBMK. It should be recalled here that
the ECCS had been made inoperative, locked and sealed.

The subsequent events are well known. The reactor was damaged. The major part
of the fuel, graphite and other in-core components were blown out. Radiation
levels in the vicinity of the damaged unit amounted to 1,000 to 15,000 R/h,
although there were some more distant or sheltered areas where radiation levels
were considerably lower.

At first the personnel failed to realize what had happened and just kept on saying,
“It is impossible! Everything was done properly.”

Ergonomics considerations in connection with the Soviet report on the accident


The report presented by the Soviet delegation at the International Atomic Energy
Association (IAEA) meeting in summer 1986 evidently gave truthful information
on the Chernobyl explosion, but a doubt keeps on returning as to whether the
emphasis was put in the right places and whether the design inadequacies were not
treated much too gently. The report stated that the behaviour of the personnel was
caused by the desire to complete the test as soon as possible. Judging from the
facts that the personnel violated the procedure for preparing and carrying out tests,
violated the test programme itself, and were careless when performing the reactor
control, it would seem that the operators were not fully aware of the processes
taking place in the reactor and had lost all feeling of danger. According to the
report:

The reactor designers failed to provide safety systems designed to prevent an


accident in the case of deliberate shut-off of the engineered safety means combined
with violations of the operating procedures since they regarded such a combination
as unlikely. Hence the initial cause of the accident was a very unlikely violation of
the operating procedure and conditions by the plant personnel.

It has become known that in the initial text of the report the words “plant
personnel” were followed by the phrase “which showed the design faults of the
reactor and the control and protection system rods”.

The designers considered the interference of “clever fools” in plant control


unlikely, and therefore failed to develop the corresponding engineered safety
mechanisms. Given the phrase in the report stating that the designers considered
the actual combination of events unlikely, some questions arise: Had the designers
considered all possible situations associated with human activity at the plant? If the
answer is positive, then how were they taken into account in the plant design?
Unfortunately, the answer to the first question is negative, leaving areas of user-
machine interaction undetermined. As a result, onsite emergency training and
theoretical and practical training were carried out mainly within a primitive control
algorithm.

Ergonomics was not used when designing computer-assisted control systems and
control rooms for nuclear plants. As a particularly serious example, an essential
parameter indicative of the core state, that is, the number of the control and
protection system rods in the core, was displayed on the control board of
Chernobyl 4 in a manner inappropriate for perception and comprehension. This
inadequacy was overcome only by operator experience in interpreting displays.

Project miscalculations and ignoring human factors had created a delayed-action


bomb. It should be emphasized that the design fault of the core and the control
system served as a fatal basis for further erroneous actions by operators, and thus
the main cause of the accident was the inadequate design of user-machine
interaction. Investigators of the disaster called for “respect to human engineering
and man-machine interaction, it being the lesson Chernobyl taught us.”
Unfortunately, it is difficult to abandon old approaches and stereotyped thinking.

As early as 1976, academician P.L. Kapitza seemed to foresee a disaster for


reasons that might have been relevant to preventing a Chernobyl, but his concerns
were made known only in 1989. In February 1976, US News and World Report, a
weekly news magazine, published a report on the fire at the Browns Ferry nuclear
facility in California. Kapitza was so concerned about this accident that he
mentioned it in his own report, “Global problems and energy”, delivered in
Stockholm in May 1976. Kapitza said in particular:

The accident highlighted the inadequacy of the mathematical methods used to


calculate the probability of such events, since these methods do not take into
account the probability due to human errors. To solve this problem, it is necessary
to take measures to prevent any nuclear accident from taking on a disastrous
course.

Kapitza tried to publish his paper in the magazine Nauka i Zhizn (Science and
Life), but the paper was rejected on the grounds that it was not advisable “to
frighten the public”. The Swedish magazine Ambio had asked Kapitza for his
paper but in the long run did not publish it either.

The Academy of Sciences assured Kapitza that there could be no such accidents in
the USSR and as an ultimate “proof” gave him the just published Safety Rules for
NPPs. These rules contained, for example, such items as “8.1. The actions of the
personnel in case of a nuclear accident are determined by the procedure for dealing
with the consequences of the accident”!

After Chernobyl

As a direct or indirect consequence of the Chernobyl accident, measures are being


developed and put into effect to ensure safe operation of current NPPs and to
improve the design and construction of future ones. In particular, measures have
been taken to make the scram system more fast-operating and to exclude any
possibility of its being deliberately shut off by the personnel. The design of the
absorbing rods has been modified and they have been made more numerous.

Furthermore, the pre-Chernobyl procedure for abnormal conditions instructed


operators to keep the reactor operating, while according to the current one the
reactor must be shut down. New reactors that, basically speaking, are in fact
inherently safe are being developed. There have appeared new areas of research
which were either ignored or non-existent before Chernobyl, including
probabilistic safety analysis and experimental safety bench tests.
However, according to the former USSR Minister of Nuclear Power and Industry,
V. Konovalov, the number of failures, shutdowns and incidents at nuclear power
plants is still high. Studies show that this is due mainly to the poor quality of the
delivered components, to human error and to inadequate solutions by design and
engineering bodies. The quality of construction and installation work leaves much
to be desired as well.

Various modifications and design changes have become common practice. As a


result, and in combination with inadequate training, qualifications of the operating
personnel are low. The personnel have to improve their knowledge and skills in the
course of their work, based on their experience in plant operation.

Ergonomics lessons are still to be learned

Even the most effective, sophisticated safety control system will fail to provide for
plant reliability if human factors are not taken into account. Work is being prepared
for the vocational training of personnel in the All-Union Scientific and Research
Institute of NPPs, and there are plans to considerably enlarge this effort. It should
be admitted, however, that human engineering still is not an integral part of plant
design, construction, testing and operation.

The former USSR Ministry of Nuclear Power replied in 1988 to an official inquiry
that in the period 1990-2000 there was no need for specialists in human
engineering with secondary and higher education as there were no corresponding
requests for such personnel from nuclear plants and enterprises.

To solve many of the problems mentioned in this article it is necessary to carry out
combined research and development involving physicists, designers, industrial
engineers, operating personnel, specialists in human engineering, psychology and
other fields. Organizing such joint work entails great difficulties, one particular
difficulty being the remaining monopoly of some scientists and groups of scientists
on “truth” in the field of nuclear energy and the monopoly of the operating
personnel on the information concerning NPP operation. Without available
comprehensive information, it is impossible to give a human engineering diagnosis
of a NPP and, if necessary, propose ways to eliminate its shortcomings as well as
to develop a system of measures to prevent accidents.

In the NPPs of the former Soviet Union the current means for diagnosis, control
and computerization are far from accepted international standards; plant control
methods are needlessly complicated and confusing; there are no advanced
programmes of personnel training; there is poor support of plant operation by
designers and highly outdated formats for operating manuals.

Conclusions
In September 1990, after further investigations, two former Chernobyl employees
were freed from prison before the end of their terms. Some time later all the
imprisoned operating personnel were freed before the appointed time. Many people
involved with the reliability and safety of NPPs now believe that the personnel had
acted correctly, even though these correct actions resulted in the explosion. The
Chernobyl personnel cannot be held responsible for the unexpected magnitude of
the accident.

In an attempt to identify those who were responsible for the disaster, the court
mainly relied on the opinion of technical specialists who, in this case, were the
designers of Chernobyl nuclear power plant. As a result of this one more important
Chernobyl lesson is learned: As long as the main legal document that is used to
identify responsibility for disasters at such complicated establishments as NPP is
something like maintenance instructions produced and changed exclusively by
designers of these establishments, it is too technically difficult to find the real
reasons for disasters, as well as to take all the necessary precautions to avoid them.

Further, a question still remains as to whether operating personnel should strictly


follow the maintenance instructions in the case of disaster or whether they should
act according to their knowledge, experience or intuition, which may even
contradict the instructions or be unconsciously associated with the threat of severe
punishment.

We must state, regrettably, that the question “Who is guilty of the Chernobyl
accident?” has not been cleared up. Those responsible should be sought among
politicians, physicists, administrators and operators, as well as among development
engineers. Convicting mere “switchmen” as in the Chernobyl case, or having
clergymen sanctify NPPs with holy water, such as was done with the incident-
plagued unit in Smolensk in 1991, cannot be the correct measures to ensure safe
and reliable operation of NPPs.

Those considering the Chernobyl disaster merely an unfortunate nuisance of a sort


which will never happen again, have to realize that one basic human characteristic
is that people do make mistakes—not only operating personnel but also scientists
and engineers. Ignoring ergonomic principles about user-machine interactions in
any technical or industrial field will result in more frequent and more severe errors.

It is therefore necessary to design technical facilities such as NPPs in such a way


that possible errors are discovered before a severe accident can happen. Many
ergonomic principles have been derived trying to prevent errors in the first place,
for instance in the design of indicators and controls. However, still today these
principles are violated in many technical facilities all over the world.

The operating personnel of complex facilities need to be highly qualified, not only
for the routine operations but also in the procedures necessary in the case of a
deviation from normal status. A sound understanding of the physics and the
technologies involved will help the personnel to react better under critical
conditions. Such qualifications can only be attained through intensive training.

The constant improvements of user-machine interfaces in all kinds of technical


applications, often as a result of minor or major accidents, show that the problem
of human errors and thus of user-machine interaction is far from being solved.
Continuous ergonomic research and the consequent application of the obtained
results aimed at making user-machine interaction more reliable is necessary,
especially with technologies that bear a highly destructive power, such as nuclear
power. Chernobyl is a severe warning of what can happen if people—scientists and
engineers, as well as administrators and politicians—disregard the necessity of
including ergonomics in the process of designing and operating complex technical
facilities.

Hans Blix, Director General of the IAEA, has stressed this problem with an
important comparison. It has been said that the problem of war is much too serious
to be left solely to generals. Blix added “that the problems of nuclear power are
much too serious to leave them solely to nuclear experts”.

REFERENCES
Abeysekera, JDA, H Shahnavaz, and LJ Chapman. 1990. Ergonomics in
developing countries. In Advances in Industrial Ergonomics and Safety, edited by
B Das. London: Taylor & Francis.

Ahonen, M, M Launis, and T Kuorinka. 1989. Ergonomic Workplace Analysis.


Helsinki: Finnish Institute of Occupational Health.

Alvares, C. 1980. Homo Faber: Technology and Culture in India, China and the
West from 1500 to Present Day. The Hague: Martinus Nijhoff.

Amalberti, R. 1991. Savoir-faire de l’opérateur: aspects théoriques et pratiques en


ergonomie. In Modèle en analyse du travail, edited by R Amalberti, M de
Montmollin, and J Thereau. Liège: Mardaga.

Amalberti, R, M Bataille, G Deblon, A Guengant, JM Paquay, C Valot, and JP


Menu. 1989. Développement d’aides intelligentes au pilotage: Formalisation
psychologique et informatique d’un modèle de comportement du pologage de
combat engagé en mission de pènètration. Paris: Rapport CERMA.

Åstrand, I. 1960. Aerobic work capacity in men and women with special reference
to age. Acta Physiol Scand 49 Suppl. 169:1-92.

Bainbridge, L. 1981. Le contrôleur de processus. B Psychol XXXIV:813-832.


—. 1986. Asking questions and accessing knowledge. Future Comput Sys 1:143-
149.

Baitsch, C. 1985. Kompetenzentwicklung und partizipative Arbeitsgestaltung.


Bern: Huber.

Banks, MH and RL Miller. 1984. Reliability and convergent validity of the job
component inventory. J Occup Psychol 57:181-184.

Baranson, J. 1969. Industrial Technology for Developing Economies. New York:


Praeger.

Bartenwerfer, H. 1970. Psychische Beanspruchung und Erdmüdung. In Handbuch


der Psychologie, edited by A Mayer and B Herwig. Göttingen: Hogrefe.

Bartlem, CS and E Locke. 1981. The Coch and French study: A critique and
reinterpretation. Hum Relat 34:555-566.

Blumberg, M. 1988. Towards a new theory of job design. In Ergonomics of Hybrid


Automated Systems, edited by W Karwowski, HR Parsaei, and MR Wilhelm.
Amsterdam: Elsevier.

Bourdon, F and A Weill Fassina. 1994. Réseau et processus de coopération dans la


gestion du trafic ferroviaire. Travail Hum. Numéro spécial consacré au travail
collectif.

Brehmer, B. 1990. Towards a taxonomy for microworlds. In Taxonomy for an


Analysis of Work Domains. Proceedings of the First MOHAWC Workshop, edited
by B Brehmer, M de Montmollin and J Leplat. Roskilde: Riso National
Laboratory.

Brown DA and R Mitchell. 1986. The Pocket Ergonomist. Sydney: Group


Occupational Health Centre.

Bruder. 1993. Entwicklung eines wissensbusierten Systems zur


belastungsanalytisch unterscheidbaren Erholungszeit. Düsseldorf: VDI-Verlag.

Caverni, JP. 1988. La verbalisation comme source d’observables pour l’étude du


fonctionnnement cognitif. In Psychologie cognitive: Modèles et méthodes, edited
by JP Caverni, C Bastien, P Mendelson, and G Tiberghien. Grenoble: Presses
Univ. de Grenoble.

Campion, MA. 1988. Interdisciplinary approaches to job design: A constructive


replication with extensions. J Appl Psychol 73:467-481.
Campion, MA and PW Thayer. 1985. Development and field evaluation of an
inter-disciplinary measure of job design. J Appl Psychol 70:29-43.

Carter, RC and RJ Biersner. 1987. Job requirements derived from the Position
Analysis Questionnaire and validity using military aptitude test scores. J Occup
Psychol 60:311-321.

Chaffin, DB. 1969. A computerized biomechanical model-development of and use


in studying gross body actions. J Biomech 2:429-441.

Chaffin, DB and G Andersson. 1984. Occupational Biomechanics. New York:


Wiley.

Chapanis, A. 1975. Ethnic Variables in Human Factors Engineering. Baltimore:


Johns Hopkins University.

Coch, L and JRP French. 1948. Overcoming resistance to change. Hum Relat
1:512-532.

Corlett, EN and RP Bishop. 1976. A technique for assessing postural discomfort.


Ergonomics 19:175-182.

Corlett, N. 1988. The investigation and evaluation of work and workplaces.


Ergonomics 31:727-734.

Costa, G, G Cesana, K Kogi, and A Wedderburn. 1990. Shiftwork: health, sleep


and performance. Frankfurt: Peter Lang.

Cotton, JL, DA Vollrath, KL Froggatt, ML Lengnick-Hall, and KR Jennings. 1988.


Employee participation: Diverse forms and different outcomes. Acad Manage Rev
13:8-22.

Cushman, WH and DJ Rosenberg. 1991. Human Factors in Product Design.


Amsterdam: Elsevier.

Dachler, HP and B Wilpert. 1978. Conceptual dimensions and boundaries of


participation in organizations: A critical evaluation. Adm Sci Q 23:1-39.

Daftuar, CN. 1975. The role of human factors in underdeveloped countries, with
special reference to India. In Ethnic Variable in Human Factor Engineering, edited
by Chapanis. Baltimore: Johns Hopkins University.

Das, B and RM Grady. 1983a. Industrial workplace layout design. An application


of engineering anthropometry. Ergonomics 26:433-447.
—. 1983b. The normal working area in the horizontal plane. A comparative study
between Farley’s and Squire’s concepts. Ergonomics 26:449-459.

Deci, EL. 1975. Intrinsic Motivation. New York: Plenum Press.

Decortis, F and PC Cacciabue. 1990. Modèlisation cognitive et analyse de


l’activité. In Modèles et pratiques de l’analyse du travail, edited by R Amalberti, M
Montmollin, and J Theureau. Brussels: Mardaga.

DeGreve, TB and MM Ayoub. 1987. A workplace design expert system. Int J Ind
Erg 2:37-48.

De Keyser, V. 1986. De l’évolution des métiers. In Traité de psychologie du


travail, edited by C Levy- Leboyer and JC Sperandio. Paris: Presses Universitaires
de France.

—. 1992. Man within the Production Line. Proceedings of the Fourth Brite-EuRam
Conference, 25-27 May, Séville, Spain. Brussels: EEC.

De Keyser, V and A Housiaux. 1989. The Nature of Human Expertise. Rapport


Intermédiaire Politique Scientifique. Liège: Université de Liège.

De Keyser, V and AS Nyssen. 1993. Les erreurs humaines en anesthésie. Travail


Hum 56:243-266.

De Lisi, PS. 1990. Lesson from the steel axe: Culture, technology and
organizational change. Sloan Manage Rev 32:83-93.

Dillon, A. 1992. Reading from paper versus screen: A critical review of the
empirical literature. Ergonomics 35:1297-1326.

Dinges, DF. 1992. Probing the limits of functional capacity: The effects of sleep
loss on short-duration tasks. In Sleep, Arousal, and Performance, edited by RJ
Broughton and RD Ogilvie. Boston: Birkhäuser.

Drury, CG. 1987. A biomechanical evaluation of the repetitive motion injury


potential of industrial jobs. Sem Occup Med 2:41-49.

Edholm, OG. 1966. The assessment of habitual activity. In Physical Activity in


Health and Disease, edited by K Evang and K Lange-Andersen. Oslo:
Universitetterlaget.

Eilers, K, F Nachreiner, and K Hänicke. 1986. Entwicklung und Überprüfung einer


Skala zur Erfassung subjektiv erlebter Anstrengung. Zeitschrift für
Arbeitswissenschaft 40:215-224.
Elias, R. 1978. A medicobiological approach to workload. Note No. 1118-9178 in
Cahiers De Notes Documentaires—Sécurité Et Hygiène Du Travail. Paris: INRS.

Elzinga, A and A Jamison. 1981. Cultural Components in the Scientific Attitude to


Nature: Eastern and Western Mode. Discussion paper No. 146. Lund: Univ. of
Lund, Research Policy Institute.

Emery, FE. 1959. Characteristics of Socio-Technical Systems. Document No. 527.


London: Tavistock.

Empson, J. 1993. Sleep and Dreaming. New York: Harvester Wheatsheaf.

Ericson, KA and HA Simon. 1984. Protocol Analysis: Verbal Reports As Data.


Cambridge, Mass.: MIT Press.

European Committee for Standardization (CEN). 1990. Ergonomic Principles of


the Design of Work Systems. EEC Council Directive 90/269/EEC, The Minimum
Health and Safety Requirements for the Manual Handling of Loads. Brussels:
CEN.

—. 1991. CEN Catalogue 1991: Catalogue of European Standards. Brussels: CEN.

—. 1994. Safety of Machinery: Ergonomic Design Principles. Part 1: Terminology


and General Principles. Brussels: CEN.

Fadier, E. 1990. Fiabilité humaine: méthodes d’analyse et domaines d’application.


In Les facteurs humains de la fiabilité dans les systèmes complexes, edited by J
Leplat and G De Terssac. Marseilles: Octares.

Falzon, P. 1991. Cooperative dialogues. In Distributed Decision Making.


Cognitive Models for Cooperative Works, edited by J Rasmussen, B Brehmer, and
J Leplat. Chichester: Wiley.

Faverge, JM. 1972. L’analyse du travail. In Traité de psychologie appliqueé, edited


by M Reuchlin. Paris: Presses Universitaires de France.

Fisher, S. 1986. Stress and Strategy. London: Erlbaum.

Flanagan, JL. 1954. The critical incident technique. Psychol Bull 51:327-358.

Fleishman, EA and MK Quaintance. 1984. Toxonomies of Human Performance:


The Description of Human Tasks. New York: Academic Press.

Flügel, B, H Greil, and K Sommer. 1986. Anthropologischer Atlas. Grundlagen


und Daten. Deutsche Demokratische Republik. Berlin: Verlag tribüne.
Folkard, S and T Akerstedt. 1992. A three-process model of the regulation of
alertness sleepiness. In Sleep, Arousal and Performance, edited by RJ Broughton
and BD Ogilvie. Boston: Birkhäuser.

Folkard, S and TH Monk. 1985. Hours of work: Temporal factors in work


scheduling . Chichester: Wiley.

Folkard, S, TH Monk, and MC Lobban. 1978. Short and long-term adjustment of


circadian rhythms in “permanent” night nurses. Ergonomics 21:785-799.

Folkard, S, P Totterdell, D Minors and J Waterhouse. 1993. Dissecting circadian


performance rhythms: Implications for shiftwork. Ergonomics 36(1-3):283-88.

Fröberg, JE. 1985. Sleep deprivation and prolonged working hours. In Hours of
Work: Temporal Factors in Work Scheduling, edited by S Folkard and TH Monk.
Chichester: Wiley.

Fuglesang, A. 1982. About Understanding Ideas and Observations on Cross-


Cultural Communication. Uppsala: Dag Hammarskjöld Foundation.

Geertz, C. 1973. The Interpretation of Cultures. New York: Basic Books.

Gilad, I. 1993. Methodology for functional ergonomic evaluation of repetitive


operations. In Advances in Industrial Egonomics and Safety, edited by Nielsen and
Jorgensen. London: Taylor & Francis.

Gilad, I and E Messer. 1992. Biomechanics considerations and ergonomic design


in diamond polishing. In Advances in Industrial Ergonomics and Safety, edited by
Kumar. London: Taylor & Francis.

Glenn, ES and CG Glenn. 1981. Man and Mankind: Conflict and Communication
between Cultures. Norwood, NJ: Ablex.

Gopher, D and E Donchin. 1986. Workload—An examination of the concept. In


Handbook of Perception and Human Performance, edited by K Boff, L Kaufman,
and JP Thomas. New York: Wiley.

Gould, JD. 1988. How to design usable systems. In Handbook of Human


Computer Interaction, edited by M Helander. Amsterdam: Elsevier.

Gould, JD and C Lewis. 1985. Designing for usability: Key principles and what
designers think. Commun ACM 28:300-311.

Gould, JD, SJ Boies, S Levy, JT Richards, and J Schoonard. 1987. The 1984
Olympic message system: A test of behavioral principles of the design. Commun
ACM 30:758-769.
Gowler, D and K Legge. 1978. Participation in context: Towards a synthesis of the
theory and practice of organizational change, part I. J Manage Stud 16:150-175.

Grady, JK and J de Vries. 1994. RAM: The Rehabilitation Technology Acceptance


Model as a Base for an Integral Product Evaluation. Instituut voor Research,
Ontwikkeling en Nascholing in de Gezondheidszorg (IRON) and University
Twente, Department of Biomedical Engineering.

Grandjean, E. 1988. Fitting the Task to the Man. London: Taylor & Francis.

Grant, S and T Mayes. 1991. Cognitive task analysis? In Human-Computer


Interactionand Complex Systems, edited by GS Weir and J Alty. London:
Academic Press.

Greenbaum, J and M Kyng. 1991. Design At Work: Cooperative Design of


Computer Systems. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

Greuter, MA and JA Algera. 1989. Criterion development and job analysis. In


Assessment and Selection in Organizations, edited by P Herlot. Chichester: Wiley.

Grote, G. 1994. A participatory approach to the complementary design of highly


automated work systems. In Human Factors in Organizational Design and
Management, edited by G Bradley and HW Hendrick. Amsterdam: Elsevier.

Guelaud, F, M-N Beauchesne, J Gautrat, and G Roustang. 1977. Pour une analyse
des conditions du travail ouvrier dans l’entreprise. Paris: A. Colin.

Guillerm, R, E Radziszewski, and A Reinberg. 1975. Circadian rhythms of six


healthy young men over a 4-week period with night-work every 48 h and a 2 per
cent Co2 atmosphere. In Experimental Studies of Shiftwork, edited by P
Colquhoun, S Folkard, P Knauth, and J Rutenfranz. Opladen: Westdeutscher
Werlag.

Hacker, W. 1986. Arbeitspsychologie. In Schriften zur Arbeitpsychologie, edited


by E Ulich. Bern: Huber.

Hacker, W and P Richter. 1994. Psychische Fehlbeanspruchung. Ermüdung,


Monotonie, Sättigung, Stress. Heidelberg: Springer.

Hackman, JR and GR Oldham. 1975. Development of the job diagnostic survey. J


Appl Psychol 60:159-170.

Hancock, PA and MH Chignell. 1986. Toward a Theory of Mental Work Load:


Stress and Adaptability in Human-Machine Systems. Proceedings of the IEEE
International Conference On Systems, Man, and Cybernetics. New York: IEEE
Society.
Hancock, PA and N Meshkati. 1988. Human Mental Workload. Amsterdam: North
Holland.

Hanna, A (ed.). 1990. Annual Design Review ID. 37 (4).

Härmä, M. 1993. Individual differences in tolerance to shiftwork: a review.


Ergonomics 36:101-109.

Hart, S and LE Staveland. 1988. Development of NASA-TLX (Task Load Index):


Results of empirical and theoretical research. In Human Mental Work Load, edited
by PA Hancock and N Meshkati. Amsterdam: North Holland.

Hirschheim, R and HK Klein. 1989. Four paradigms of information systems


development. Commun ACM 32:1199-1216.

Hoc, JM. 1989. Cognitive approaches to process control. In Advances in Cognitive


Science, edited by G Tiberghein. Chichester: Horwood.

Hofstede, G. 1980. Culture’s Consequences: International Differences in Work-


Related Values. Beverly Hills, Calif.: Sage Univ. Press.

—. 1983. The cultural relativity of organizational practices and theories. J Int


Stud :75-89.

Hornby, P and C Clegg. 1992. User participation in context: A case study in a UK


bank. Behav Inf Technol 11:293-307.

Hosni, DE. 1988. The transfer of microelectronics technology to the third world.
Tech Manage Pub TM 1:391-3997.

Hsu, S-H and Y Peng. 1993. Control/display relationship of the four-burner stove:
A reexamination. Hum Factors 35:745-749.

International Labour Organization (ILO). 1990.The hours we work: new work


schedules in policy and practice. Cond Wor Dig 9.

International Organization for Standardization (ISO). 1980. Draft Proposal for


Core List of Anthropometric Measurements ISO/TC 159/SC 3 N 28 DP 7250.
Geneva: ISO.

—. 1996. ISO/DIS 7250 Basic Human Body Measurements for Technological


Design. Geneva: ISO.

Japan Industrial Design Promotion Organization (JIDPO). 1990. Good Design


Products 1989. Tokyo: JIDPO.
Jastrzebowski, W. 1857. Rys ergonomiji czyli Nauki o Pracy, opartej naprawdach
poczerpnietych z Nauki Przyrody. Przyoda i Przemysl 29:227-231.

Jeanneret, PR. 1980. Equitable job evaluation and classification with the Position
Analysis Questionnaire. Compens Rev 1:32-42.

Jürgens, HW, IA Aune, and U Pieper. 1990. International data on anthropometry.


Occupational Safety and Health Series. Geneva: ILO.

Kadefors, R. 1993. A model for assessment and design of workplaces for manual
welding. In The Ergonomics of Manual Work, edited by WS Marras, W
Karwowski, and L Pacholski. London: Taylor & Francis.

Kahneman, D. 1973. Attention and Effort. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.

Karhu, O, P Kansi, and I Kuorinka. 1977. Correcting working postures in industry:


A practical method for analysis. Appl Ergon 8:199-201.

Karhu, O, R Harkonen, P Sorvali, and P Vepsalainen. 1981. Observing working


postures in industry: Examples of OWAS application. Appl Ergon 12:13-17.

Kedia, BL and RS Bhagat. 1988. Cultural constraints on transfer of technology


across nations: Implications for research in international and comparative
management. Acad Manage Rev 13:559-571.

Keesing, RM. 1974. Theories of culture. Annu Rev Anthropol 3:73-79.

Kepenne, P. 1984. La charge de travail dans une unité de soins de médecine.


Mémoire. Liège: Université de Liège.

Kerguelen, A. 1986. L’observation systématique en ergonomie: Élaboration d’un


logiciel d’aide au recueil et à l’analyse des données. Diploma in Ergonomics
Thesis, Conservatoire National des Arts et Métiers, Paris.

Ketchum, L. 1984. Sociotechnical design in a third world country: The railway


maintenance depot at Sennar in Sudan. Hum Relat 37:135-154.

Keyserling, WM. 1986. A computer-aided system to evaluate postural stress in the


workplace. Am Ind Hyg Assoc J 47:641-649.

Kingsley, PR. 1983. Technological development: Issues, roles and orientation for
social psychology. In Social Psychology and Developing Countries, edited by
Blacker. New York: Wiley.

Kinney, JS and BM Huey. 1990. Application Principles for Multicolored Displays.


Washington, DC: National Academy Press.
Kivi, P and M Mattila. 1991. Analysis and improvement of work postures in
building industry: Application of the computerized OWAS method. Appl Ergon
22:43-48.

Knauth, P, W Rohmert and J Rutenfranz. 1979. Systemic selection of shift plans


for continuous production with the aid of work-physiological criteria. Appl Ergon
10(1):9-15.

Knauth, P. and J Rutenfranz. 1981. Duration of sleep related to the type of shift
work, in Night and shiftwork: biological and social aspects , edited by A Reinberg,
N Vieux, and P Andlauer. Oxford Pergamon Press.

Kogi, K. 1982. Sleep problems in night and shift work. II. Shiftwork: Its practice
and improvement . J Hum Ergol:217-231.

—. 1981. Comparison of resting conditions between various shift rotation systems


for industrial workers, in Night and shift work. Biological and social aspects ,
edited by A Reinberg, N Vieux, and P Andlauer. Oxford: Pergamon.

—. 1985. Introduction to the problems of shiftwork. In Hours of Work: Temporal


Factors in Work-Scheduling, edited by S Folkard and TH Monk. Chichester:
Wiley.

—. 1991. Job content and working time: The scope for joint change. Ergonomics
34:757-773.

Kogi, K and JE Thurman. 1993. Trends in approaches to night and shiftwork and
new international standards. Ergonomics 36:3-13.

Köhler, C, M von Behr, H Hirsch-Kreinsen, B Lutz, C Nuber, and R Schultz-Wild.


1989. Alternativen der Gestaltung von Arbeits- und Personalstrukturen bei
rechnerintegrierter Fertigung. In Strategische Optionen der Organisations- und
Personalentwicklung bei CIM Forschungsbericht KfK-PFT 148, edited by Institut
für Sozialwissenschaftliche Forschung. Karlsruhe: Projektträgerschaft
Fertigungstechnik.

Koller, M. 1983. Health risks related to shift work. An example of time-contingent


effects of long-term stress. Int Arch Occ Env Health 53:59-75.

Konz, S. 1990. Workstation organization and design. Ergonomics 32:795-811.

Kroeber, AL and C Kluckhohn. 1952. Culture, a critical review of concepts and


definitions. In Papers of the Peabody Museum. Boston: Harvard Univ.

Kroemer, KHE. 1993. Operation of ternary chorded keys. Int J Hum Comput
Interact 5:267-288.
—. 1994a. Locating the computer screen: How high, how far? Ergonomics in
Design (January):40.

—. 1994b. Alternative keyboards. In Proceedings of the Fourth International


Scientific Conference WWDU ‘94. Milan: Univ. of Milan.

—. 1995. Ergonomics. In Fundamentals of Industrial Hygiene, edited by BA


Ploog. Chicago: National Safety Council.

Kroemer, KHE, HB Kroemer, and KE Kroemer-Elbert. 1994. Ergonomics: How to


Design for Ease and Efficiency. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.

Kwon, KS, SY Lee, and BH Ahn. 1993. An approach to fuzzy expert systems for
product colour design. In The Ergonomics of Manual Work, edited by Maras,
Karwowski, Smith, and Pacholski. London: Taylor & Francis.

Lacoste, M. 1983. Des situations de parole aux activités interprétives. Psychol


Franç 28:231-238.

Landau, K and W Rohmert. 1981. AET-A New Job Analysis Method. Detroit,
Mich.: AIIE Annual Conference.

Laurig, W. 1970. Elektromyographie als arbeitswissenschaftliche


Untersuchungsmethode zur Beurteilung von statischer Muskelarbeit. Berlin: Beuth.

—. 1974. Beurteilung einseitig dynamischer Muskelarbeit. Berlin: Beuth.

—. 1981. Belastung, Beanspruchung und Erholungszeit bei energetisch-muskulärer


Arbeit—Literaturexpertise. In Forschungsbericht Nr. 272 der Bundesanstalt für
Arbeitsschutz und Unfallforschung Dortmund. Bremerhaven: Wirtschaftsverlag
NW.

—. 1992. Grundzüge der Ergonomie. Erkenntnisse und Prinzipien. Berlin, Köln:


Beuth Verlag.

Laurig, W and V Rombach. 1989. Expert systems in ergonomics: Requirements


and an approach. Ergonomics 32:795-811.

Leach, ER. 1965. Culture and social cohesion: An anthropologist’s view. In


Science and Culture, edited by Holten. Boston: Houghton Mifflin.

Leana, CR, EA Locke, and DM Schweiger. 1990. Fact and fiction in analyzing
research on participative decision making: A critique of Cotton, Vollrath, Froggatt,
Lengnick-Hall, and Jennings. Acad Manage Rev 15:137-146.

Lewin, K. 1951. Field Theory in Social Science. New York: Harper.


Liker, JK, M Nagamachi, and YR Lifshitz. 1988. A Comparitive Analysis of
Participatory Programs in US and Japan Manufacturing Plants. Ann Arbor, Mich.:
Univ. of Michigan, Center for Ergonomics, Industrial and Operational
Engineering.

Lillrank, B and N Kano. 1989. Continuous Improvement: Quality Control Circles


in Japanese Industries. Ann Arbor, Mich.: Univ. of Michigan, Center for Japanese
Studies.

Locke, EA and DM Schweiger. 1979. Participation in decision making: One more


look. In Research in Organizational Behavior, edited by BM Staw. Greenwich,
Conn.: JAI Press.

Louhevaara, V, T Hakola, and H Ollila. 1990. Physical work and strain involved in
manual sorting of postal parcels. Ergonomics 33:1115-1130.

Luczak, H. 1982. Belastung, Beanspruchung und Erholungszeit bei informatorisch-


mentaler Arbeit — Literaturexpertise. Forschungsbericht der Bundesanstalt für
Arbeitsschutz und Unfallforschung Dortmund . Bremerhaven: Wirtschaftsverlag
NW.

—. 1983. Ermüdung. In Praktische Arbeitsphysiologie, edited by W Rohmert and J


Rutenfranz. Stuttgart: Georg Thieme Verlag.

—. 1993. Arbeitswissenschaft. Berlin: Springer Verlag.

Majchrzak, A. 1988. The Human Side of Factory Automation. San Francisco:


Jossey-Bass.

Martin, T, J Kivinen, JE Rijnsdorp, MG Rodd, and WB Rouse. 1991. Appropriate


automation-integrating technical, human, organization, economic and cultural
factors. Automatica 27:901-917.

Matsumoto, K and M Harada. 1994. The effect of night-time naps on recovery


from fatigue following night work. Ergonomics 37:899-907.

Matthews, R. 1982. Divergent conditions in the technological development of


India and Japan. Lund Letters on Technology and Culture, No. 4. Lund: Univ. of
Lund, Research Policy Institute.

McCormick, EJ. 1979. Job Analysis: Methods and Applications. New York:
American Management Association.

McIntosh, DJ. 1994. Integration of VDUs into the US office work environment. In
Proceedings of the Fourth International Scientific Conference WWDU ‘94. Milan:
Univ. of Milan.
McWhinney. 1990. The Power of Myth in Planning and Organizational Change,
1989 IEEE Technics, Culture and Consequences. Torrence, Calif.: IEEE Los
Angeles Council.

Meshkati, N. 1989. An etiological investigation of micro and macroergonomics


factors in the Bhopal disaster: Lessons for industries of both industrialized and
developing countries. Int J Ind Erg 4:161-175.

Minors, DS and JM Waterhouse. 1981. Anchor sleep as a synchronizer of rhythms


on abnormal routines. Int J Chronobiology : 165-188.

Mital, A and W Karwowski. 1991. Advances in Human Factors/Ergonomics.


Amsterdam: Elsevier.

Monk, TH. 1991. Sleep, Sleepiness and Performance . Chichester: Wiley.

Moray, N, PM Sanderson, and K Vincente. 1989. Cognitive task analysis for a


team in a complex work domain: A case study. Proceedings of the Second
European Meeting On Cognitive Science Approaches to Process Control, Siena,
Italy.

Morgan, CT, A Chapanis, JS III Cork, and MW Lund. 1963. Human Engineering
Guide to Equipment Design. New York: McGraw-Hill.

Mossholder, KW and RD Arvey. 1984. Synthetic validity: A conceptual and


comparative review. J Appl Psychol 69:322-333.

Mumford, E and Henshall. 1979. A Participative Approach to Computer Systems


Design. London: Associated Business Press.

Nagamachi, M. 1992. Pleasantness and Kansei engineering. In Measurement


Standards. Taejon, Korea: Korean Research Institute of Standards and Science
Publishing.

National Institute for Occupational Safety and Health (NIOSH). 1981. Work
Practices Guide for Manual Lifting. Cincinnati, Ohio: US Department of Health
and Human Services.

—. 1990. OSHA Instruction CPL 2.85: Directorate of Compliance Programs:


Appendix C, Guidelines Auggested By NIOSH for Videotape Evaluation of Work
Station for Upper Extremities Cumulative Trauma Disorders. Washington, DC: US
Department of Health and Human Services.

Navarro, C. 1990. Functional communication and problem-solving in a bus traffic-


regulation task. Psychol Rep 67:403-409.
Negandhi, ART. 1975. Modern Organizational Behaviour. Kent: Kent Univ..

Nisbett, RE and TD De Camp Wilson. 1977. Telling more than we know. Psychol
Rev 84:231-259.

Norman, DA. 1993. Things That Make Us Smart. Reading: Addison-Wesley.

Noro, K and AS Imada. 1991. Participatory Ergonomics. London: Taylor &


Francis.

O’Donnell, RD and FT Eggemeier. 1986. Work load assessment methodology. In


Handbook of Perception and Human Performance. Cognitive Processes and
Performance, edited by K Boff, L Kaufman, and JP Thomas. New York: Wiley.

Pagels, HR. 1984. Computer culture: The scientific, intellectual and social impact
of the computer. Ann NY Acad Sci :426.

Persson, J and Å Kilbom. 1983. VIRA—En Enkel Videofilmteknik För


Registrering OchAnalys Av Arbetsställningar Och—Rörelser. Solna, Sweden:
Undersökningsrapport,Arbetraskyddsstyrelsen.

Pham, DT and HH Onder. 1992. A knowledge-based system for optimizing


workplace layouts using a genetic algorithm. Ergonomics 35:1479-1487.

Pheasant, S. 1986. Bodyspace, Anthropometry, Ergonomics and Design. London:


Taylor & Francis.

Poole, CJM. 1993. Seamstress’ finger. Brit J Ind Med 50:668-669.

Putz-Anderson, V. 1988. Cumulative Trauma Disorders. A Manual for


Musculoskeletal Diseases of the Upper Limbs. London: Taylor & Francis.

Rasmussen, J. 1983. Skills, rules, and knowledge: Sinds, signs, symbols and other
distinctions in human performance models. IEEE T Syst Man Cyb 13:257-266.

—. 1986. A framework for cognitive task analysis in systems design. In Intelligent


Decision Support in Process Environments, edited by E Hollnagel, G Mancini, and
DD Woods. Berlin: Springer.

Rasmussen, J, A Pejtersen, and K Schmidts. 1990. In Taxonomy for Analysis of


Work Domains. Proceedings of the First MOHAWC Workshop, edited by B
Brehmer, M de Montmollin and J Leplat. Roskilde: Riso National Laboratory.

Reason, J. 1989. Human Error. Cambridge: CUP.


Rebiffé, R, O Zayana, and C Tarrière. 1969. Détermination des zones optimales
pour l’emplacement des commandes manuelles dans l’espace de travail.
Ergonomics 12:913-924.

Régie nationale des usines Renault (RNUR). 1976. Les profils de poste: Methode
d’analyse des conditions de travail. Paris: Masson-Sirtes.

Rogalski, J. 1991. Distributed decision making in emergency management: Using a


method as a framework for analysing cooperative work and as a decision aid. In
Distributed Decision Making. Cognitive Models for Cooperative Work, edited by J
Rasmussen, B Brehmer, and J Leplat. Chichester: Wiley.

Rohmert, W. 1962. Untersuchungen über Muskelermüdung und Arbeitsgestaltung.


Bern: Beuth-Vertrieb.

—. 1973. Problems in determining rest allowances. Part I: Use of modern methods


to evaluate stress and strain in static muscular work. Appl Ergon 4(2):91-95.

—. 1984. Das Belastungs-Beanspruchungs-Konzept. Z Arb wiss 38:193-200.

Rohmert, W and K Landau. 1985. A New Technique of Job Analysis. London:


Taylor & Francis.

Rolland, C. 1986. Introduction à la conception des systèmes d’information et


panorama des méthodes disponibles. Génie Logiciel 4:6-11.

Roth, EM and DD Woods. 1988. Aiding human performance. I. Cognitive


analysis. Travail Hum 51:39-54.

Rudolph, E, E Schönfelder, and W Hacker. 1987. Tätigkeitsbewertungssystem für


geistige arbeit mit und ohne Rechnerunterstützung (TBS-GA). Berlin:
Psychodiagnostisches Zentrum der Humboldt-Universität.

Rutenfranz, J. 1982. Occupational health measures for night- and shiftworkers. II.
Shiftwork: Its practice and improvement. J Hum Ergol:67-86.

Rutenfranz, J, J Ilmarinen, F Klimmer, and H Kylian. 1990. Work load and


demanded physical performance capacity under different industrial working
conditions. In Fitness for Aged, Disabled, and Industrial Workers, edited by M
Kaneko. Champaign, Ill.: Human Kinetics Books.

Rutenfranz, J, P Knauth, and D Angersbach. 1981. Shift work research issues. In


Biological Rhythms, Sleep and Shift Work , edited by LC Johnson, DI Tepas, WP
Colquhoun, and MJ Colligan. New York: Spectrum Publications Medical and
Scientific Books.
Saito, Y. and K Matsumoto. 1988. Variations of physiological functions and
psychological measures and their relationship on delayed shift of sleeping time.
Jap J Ind Health 30:196-205.

Sakai, K, A Watanabe, N Onishi, H Shindo, K Kimotsuki, H Saito, and K Kogl.


1984. Conditions of night naps effective to facilitate recovery from night work
fatigue. J Sci Lab 60: 451-478.

Savage, CM and D Appleton. 1988. CIM and Fifth Generation Management.


Dearborn: CASA/SME Technical Council.

Savoyant, A and J Leplat. 1983. Statut et fonction des communications dans


l’activité des équipes de travail. Psychol Franç 28:247-253.

Scarbrough, H and JM Corbett. 1992. Technology and Organization. London:


Routledge.

Schmidtke, H. 1965. Die Ermüdung. Bern: Huber.

—. 1971. Untersuchungen über den Erholunggszeitbedarf bei verschiedenen Arten


gewerblicher Tätigkeit. Berlin: Beuth-Vertrieb.

Sen, RN. 1984. Application of ergonomics to industrially developing countries.


Ergonomics 27:1021-1032.

Sergean, R. 1971. Managing Shiftwork. London: Gower Press.

Sethi, AA, DHJ Caro, and RS Schuler. 1987. Strategic Management of


Technostress in an Information Society. Lewiston: Hogrefe.

Shackel, B. 1986. Ergonomics in design for usability. In People and Computer:


Design for Usability, edited by MD Harrison and AF Monk. Cambridge:
Cambridge Univ. Press.

Shahnavaz, H. 1991. Transfer of Technology to Industrially Developing Countries


and Human Factors Consideration TULEÅ 1991: 22, 23024. Luleå Univ., Luleå,
Sweden: Center for Ergonomics of Developing Countries.

Shahnavaz, H, J Abeysekera, and A Johansson. 1993. Solving multi-factorial


work-environment problems through participatory ergonomics: Case study: VDT
operators. In Ergonomics of Manual Work, edited by E Williams, S Marrs, W
Karwowski, JL Smith, and L Pacholski. London: Taylor & Francis.

Shaw, JB and JH Riskind. 1983. Predicting job stress using data from the Position
Analysis Questionnaire (PAQ). J Appl Psychol 68:253-261.
Shugaar, A. 1990. Ecodesign: New products for a greener culture. Int Herald Trib,
17.

Sinaiko, WH. 1975. Verbal factors in human engineering: Some cultural and
psychological data. In Ethnic Variables in Human Factors Engineering, edited by
A Chapanis. Baltimore: Johns Hopkins Univ..

Singleton, WT. 1982. The Body At Work. Cambridge: CUP.

Snyder, HL. 1985a. Image quality: Measures and visual performance. In Flat Panel
Displays and CRTs, edited by LE Tannas. New York: Van Nostrand Reinhold.

—. 1985b. The visual system: Capabilities and limitations. In Flat Panel Displays
and CRTs, edited by LE Tannas. New York: Van Nostrand Reinhold.

Solomon, CM. 1989. The corporate response to work force diversity. Pers J 68:42-
53.

Sparke, P. 1987. Modern Japanese Design. New York: EP Dutton.

Sperandio, JC. 1972. Charge de travail et régulation des processus opératoires.


Travail Hum 35:85-98.

Sperling, L, S Dahlman, L Wikström, A Kilbom, and R Kadefors. 1993. A cube


model for the classification of work with hand tools and the formulation of
functional requirements. Appl Ergon 34:203-211.

Spinas, P. 1989. User oriented software development and dialogue design. In Work
With Computers: Organizational, Management, Stress and Health Aspects, edited
by MJ Smith and G Salvendy. Amsterdam: Elsevier.

Staramler, JH. 1993. The Dictionary of Human Factors Ergonomics. Boca Raton:
CRC Press.

Strohm, O, JK Kuark, and A Schilling. 1993. Integrierte Produktion:


Arbeitspsychologische Konzepte und empirische Befunde, Schriftenreihe Mensch,
Technik, Organisation. In CIM—Herausforderung an Mensch, Technik,
Organisation, edited by G Cyranek and E Ulich. Stuttgart, Zürich: Verlag der
Fachvereine.

Strohm, O, P Troxler and E Ulich. 1994. Vorschlag für die Restrukturierung eines
Produktionsbetriebes. Zürich: Institut für Arbietspsychologie der ETH.

Sullivan, LP. 1986. Quality function deployment: A system to assure that customer
needs drive the product design and production process. Quality Progr :39-50.
Sundin, A, J Laring, J Bäck, G Nengtsson, and R Kadefors. 1994. An Ambulatory
Workplace for Manual Welding: Productivity through Ergonomics. Manuscript.
Göteborg: Lindholmen Development.

Tardieu, H, D Nanci, and D Pascot. 1985. Conception d’un système d’information.


Paris: Editions d’Organisation.

Teiger, C, A Laville, and J Durafourg. 1974. Taches répétitives sous contrainte de


temps et charge de travail. Rapport no 39. Laboratoire de physiologie du travail et
d’ergonomie du CNAM.

Torsvall, L, T Akerstedt, and M. Gillberg. 1981. Age, sleep and irregular


workhours: a field study with EEG recording, catecholamine excretion and self-
ratings. Scand J Wor Env Health 7:196-203.

Ulich, E. 1994. Arbeitspsychologie 3. Auflage. Zürich: Verlag der Fachvereine and


Schäffer-Poeschel.

Ulich, E, M Rauterberg, T Moll, T Greutmann, and O Strohm. 1991. Task


orientation and user-oriented dialogue design. In Int J Human-Computer
Interaction 3:117-144.

United Nations Educational, Scientific and Cultural Organization (UNESCO).


1992. Ergonomics Impact of Science on Society. Vol. 165. London: Taylor &
Francis.

Van Daele, A. 1988. L’écran de visualisation ou la communication verbale?


Analyse comparative de leur utilisation par des opérateurs de salle de contrôle en
sidérurgie. Travail Hum 51(1):65-80.

—. 1992. La réduction de la complexité par les opérateurs dans le contrôle de


processus continus. contribution à l’étude du contrôle par anticipation et de ses
conditions de mise en œuvre. Liège: Université de Liège.

Van der Beek, AJ, LC Van Gaalen, and MHW Frings-Dresen. 1992. Working
postures and activities of lorry drivers: A reliability study of on-site observation
and recording on a pocket computer. Appl Ergon 23:331-336.

Vleeschdrager, E. 1986. Hardness 10: diamonds . Paris.

Volpert, W. 1987. Psychische Regulation von Arbeitstätigkeiten. In


Arbeitspsychologie. Enzklopüdie der Psychologie, edited by U Kleinbeck and J
Rutenfranz. Göttingen: Hogrefe.

Wagner, R. 1985. Job analysis at ARBED. Ergonomics 28:255-273.


Wagner, JA and RZ Gooding. 1987. Effects of societal trends on participation
research. Adm Sci Q 32:241-262.

Wall, TD and JA Lischeron. 1977. Worker Participation: A Critique of the


Literature and Some Fresh Evidence. London: McGraw-Hill.

Wang, WM-Y. 1992. Usability Evaluation for Human-Computer Interaction


(HCI). Luleå, Sweden: Luleå Univ. of Technology.

Waters, TR, V Putz-Anderson, A Garg, and LJ Fine. 1993. Revised NIOSH


equation for the design and evaluation of manual handling tasks. Ergonomics
36:749-776.

Wedderburn, A. 1991. Guidelines for shiftworkers. Bulletin of European Shiftwork


Topics (BEST) No. 3. Dublin: European Foundation for the Improvement of
Living and Working Conditions.

Welford, AT. 1986. Mental workload as a function of demand, capacity, strategy


and skill. Ergonomics 21:151-176.

White, PA. 1988. Knowing more about what we tell: ‘Introspective access’ and
causal report accuracy, 10 years later. Brit J Psychol 79:13-45.

Wickens, C. 1992. Engineering Psychology and Human Performance. New York:


Harper Collins.

Wickens, CD and YY Yeh. 1983. The dissociation between subjective work load
and performance: A multiple resources approach. In Proceedings of the Human
Factors Society 27th Annual Meeting. Santa Monica, Calif.: Human Factors
Society.

Wieland-Eckelmann, R. 1992. Kognition, Emotion und Psychische


Beanspruchung. Göttingen: Hogrefe.

Wikström.L, S Byström, S Dahlman, C Fransson, R Kadefors, Å Kilbom, E


Landervik, L Lieberg, L Sperling, and J Öster. 1991. Criterion for Selection and
Development of Hand Tools. Stockholm: National Institute of Occupational
Health.

Wilkinson, RT. 1964. Effects of up to 60 hours sleep deprivation on different types


of work. Ergonomics 7:63-72.

Williams, R. 1976. Keywords: A Vocabulary of Culture and Society. Glasgow:


Fontana.
Wilpert, B. 1989. Mitbestimmung. In Arbeits- und Organisationspsychologie.
Internationales Handbuch in Schlüsselbegriffen, edited by S Greif, H Holling, and
N Nicholson. Munich: Psychologie Verlags Union.

Wilson, JR. 1991. Participation: A framework and foundation for ergonomics. J


Occup Psychol 64:67-80.

Wilson, JR and EN Corlett. 1990. Evaluation of Human Work: A Practical


Ergonomics Methodology. London: Taylor & Francis.

Wisner, A. 1983. Ergonomics or anthropology: A limited or wide approach to


working condition in technology transfer. In Proceedings of the First International
Conference On Ergonomics of Developing Countries, edited by Shahnavaz and
Babri. Luleå, Sweden: Luleå Univ. of Technology.

Womack, J, T Jones, and D Roos. 1990. The Machine That Changed the World.
New York: Macmillan.

Woodson, WE, B Tillman, and P Tillman. 1991. Human Factors Design


Handbook. New York: McGraw-Hill.

Zhang, YK and JS Tyler. 1990. The establishment of a modern telephone cable


production facility in a developing country. A case study. In International Wire and
Cable Symposium Proceedings. Illinois.

Zinchenko, V and V Munipov. 1989. Fundamentals of Ergonomics. Moscow:


Progress.

OTHER RELEVANT READINGS


Amphoux, L. 1976. Fatigue. Instantanés médicaux, pathologie du travail,
intoxications, maladies par agents physiques. In Encyclopédie Médico-
Chirurgicale. Paris: Les éditions techniques.

Birren, JE and K Warner Schaie. 1990. Handbook of the Psychology of Aging.


New York: Academic Press.

Bourlière, F. 1982. Gérontologie, Biologie Et Clinique. Paris: Flammarion.

Bugard, P, EJ Caille, L Crocq, B Ely, M Henry, L Petrescu, and A Reinberg. 1974.


CIS 76-281 Stress, Fatigue, and Depression in Everyday Life. Vol. 1 and 2. Paris:
Editions Doin.

Carter, FA and EN Corlett. 1982. Accidents and Shiftwork. Dublin: European


Foundation for the Improvement of Living and Working Conditions.
Charness, M. 1985. Aging and Human Performance. New York: Wiley.

Colligan, MJ and RR Rosa. 1990. Shiftwork effects on social and family life. In
Shiftwork, edited by AJ Scott. Philadelphia: Hanley & Belfus.

Davies, BT and H Shahnawaz. 1977. CIS 77-1774 An anthropometer for use in


developing countries. Ergonomics 20(3):317-320.

Drury, CG, B Paramore, HP Van Cott, SM Grey, and EN Corlett. 1987. Task
analysis. In Handbook of Human Factors, edited by G Salvendy. New York:
Wiley.

Fraser, TM. 1989. The Worker At Work. London: Taylor & Francis.

Freivalds, A. 1987. The ergonomics of tools. Int Rev Ergon 1:43-75.

Gadbois, C. 1991. Round the clock operations in hospitals: Shift scheduling, task
demands and work organisation. Abstracts of the International Symposium
Shiftwork and Job Demands, 11-12 July, Paris.

Graf, O. 1922. Über lohnendste Arbeitspausen bei geistiger Arbeit. Psychol


Arbeiten 7:548-611.

Grieco, A and M Masali. 1971. Setting up a method for the measurement of


anthropometric parameters with a view to the ergonomic design of workplaces.
Med Lavoro 62(11):505-531.

Hamelin, P. 1981. Les conditions temporelles de travail des conducteurs routiers et


la sécurité routière. Travail Hum 44:5-21.

International Labour Organization (ILO). 1984. Protection of Workers against


Noise and Vibration in the Working Environment, an ILO Code of Practice.
Geneva: ILO.

ISSA. N.d. W.G. 3 E.: Checklist for a Classification of Machines On the Basis of
Ergonomic Principles, International Section ‘Machine Safety’ On the Prevention of
Occupational Accidents and Diseases. Mannheim: ISSA.

Konz, S. 1979. Handtools. In Work Design Columbus, Ohio: Grid.

McEwan, YW. 1978. Flexible working arrangements in continuous shift


production. Pers Rev :12-19.

Minors, DS and JM Waterhouse. 1983. Circadian rhythm amplitude-is it related to


rhythm adjustment and/or worker motivation? Ergonomics 26:229-241.
Mital, A and A Kilborn. 1992. Design, selection and use of hand tools to alleviate
trauma of the upper extremeties: Part I- Guidelines for the practitioner. Part II- The
scientific basis for the guide. Int J Ind Erg 10:1-21.

Nowak, E. 1978. CIS 78-2068 Determination of the spatial reach area of the arms
for workplace design purposes. Ergonomics 21(7) (July):493-507.

Robinson, PK et al. N.d. Aging and Technological Advances. Presented At NATO


Conference Series III, Human Factors, Volume 24.

Rohmert, W. 1975. Anthropometric elements of ergonomics. Bulletin de


l’AISS:25-35.

—. 1983. Formen menschlicher Arbeit. In Praktische Arbeitsphysiologie, edited by


W Rohmert and J Rutenfranz. Stuttgart: Georg Thieme Verlag.

Tichauer, ER. 1978. The Biomechanical Basis of Ergonomics. New York: Wiley
Interscience.

Vidacek, S, L Kaliterna, and B Radosevic-Vidacek. 1987. Predictive validity of


individualdifference measures for health problems in shiftworkers: Preliminary
results. In Contemporary Advances in Shiftwork Research, edited by A Oginski, J
Pokorski, and J Rutenfranz. Krakow: Medical Academy.

Vieillissement et travail. 1989. Travail Hum 52:1-3.

Webb Associates. 1972. CIS 74-802 Anthropometry for Respirator Sizing.


Cincinnati, Ohio: NIOSH.

Part IV - Tools and Approaches Français

Click the website below to check the full details of the Encyclopedia

http://www.ilocis.org/en/contilo.html

You might also like