Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 3

treatments, thus falsifying the psychody- theory, often stated in authoritative tones, that the mental health professions

professions have come to


namic theory of symptom formation. present a picture of psychodynamic treatment believe that these fictions, created as straw
Tryon (2010) observed that “thera- that is unrecognizable to me and to other men by people who did not practice psy-
pists, and the therapeutic approaches that contemporary psychodynamic practitioners. chodynamic therapy, were psychodynamic
currently divide us, differ only with regard Several of the comments about my article therapy.
to what is to be learned and how it is to be perpetuate this tradition and, I am sorry to I do not believe that such fictions were
acquired” (p. 10, italics in original). This say, introduce disinformation into the pages deliberately disingenuous; I think they
makes learning and memory basic to our of the American Psychologist. Before ad- were often sincerely believed to represent
science and profession and should motivate dressing some specifics, I want to say a few psychodynamic practice. By the late 20th
us to search for mechanisms that underlie words about my understanding of how such century, however, the divisions between
all effective psychological interventions misrepresentations can find their way into psychodynamic therapists and academic
(e.g., Tryon, 2005; Tryon & McKay, 2009; scholarly academic journals. psychologists were so great that there was
Tryon & Misurell, 2008). Psychoanalysis developed outside of no opportunity for corrective feedback.
the academic world. Until the late 1980s, The fictions about psychodynamic therapy
REFERENCES most training in psychoanalysis and psy- had taken on a life of their own and were
Blagys, M. D., & Hilsenroth, M. J. (2000). Dis-
chodynamic therapy took place in free- simply passed on as truths by multiple gen-
tinctive features of short-term psychodynamic- standing institutes that were open to med- erations of university professors to multiple
interpersonal psychotherapy: A review of the ical doctors and closed to psychologists. generations of students. My psychody-
comparative psychotherapeutic process litera- There was little exchange of information namic colleagues who teach in psychology
ture. Clinical Psychology: Science & Practice, between the psychoanalytic and academic graduate programs can attest to how exten-
7, 167–188. doi:10.1093/clipsy/7.2.167 worlds, and most academic psychologists sive are the misunderstandings that stu-
Bordin, E. S. (1979). The generalizability of the were, as a result, entirely unaware of the dents, especially those who majored in psy-
psychoanalytic concept of the working alli- ongoing evolution of psychodynamic the- chology, have been taught as received
ance. Psychotherapy: Theory, Research and
Practice, 16, 252–260. doi:10.1037/h0085885
ory and practice. What little they did know truths about the psychodynamic tradition.
Shedler, J. (2010). The efficacy of psychody- of psychodynamic thinking was often Over the past two decades or so, a
namic therapy. American Psychologist, 65, based on limited reading of works that “master narrative” has emerged in the ac-
98 –109. doi:10.1037/a0018378 were generations out of date and read out ademic world that psychodynamic therapy
Tryon, W. W. (2005). Possible mechanisms for of context. The development of psychoan- has somehow been disproven and that CBT
why desensitization and exposure therapy alytic knowledge did not end with Freud has been scientifically tested against it and
work. Clinical Psychology Review, 25, 67–95. any more than the development of physics found superior. In the prevailing academic
doi:10.1016/j.cpr.2004.08.005 ended with Newton or the development of climate, the steadily accumulating scien-
Tryon, W. W. (2008). Whatever happened to
symptom substitution? Clinical Psychology the behavioral tradition in psychology tific evidence for psychodynamic therapy
Review, 28, 963–968. doi:10.1016/j.cpr.2008 ended with Watson. has been repeatedly overlooked. To aca-
.02.003 The medical monopoly on psychoan- demics who were exposed to fictions about
Tryon, W. W. (2010). Learning as core of psy- alytic education, and the arrogance of some psychodynamic approaches, it may have
chological science and clinical practice. The within the psychoanalytic community seemed unthinkable that psychodynamic
Behavior Therapist, 33, 10 –11. when it was the dominant therapy para- concepts and treatments could possibly be
Tryon, W. W., & McKay, D. (2009). Memory digm (including a dismissive attitude to- supported by science, and if that was un-
modification as an outcome variable in anxi-
ward empirical research), provoked under- thinkable, there was no need to examine
ety disorder treatment. Journal of Anxiety Dis-
orders, 23, 546 –556. doi:10.1016/j.janxdis standable resentment among academics. A scientific evidence concerning them. Also
.2008.11.003 culture developed within academic psy- overlooked was the fact that CBT and other
Tryon, W. W., & Misurell, J. R. (2008). Disso- chology that disparaged psychoanalytic ap- “empirically supported therapies” were al-
nance induction and reduction: A possible proaches, or rather, the stereotypes and car- most never tested against actual psychody-
principle and connectionist mechanism for icatures that it mistook for psychoanalytic namic therapy—most of the research sim-
why therapies are effective. Clinical Psychol- approaches. The self-imposed exile of psy- ply compared these treatments with no
ogy Review, 28, 1297–1309. doi:10.1016/ choanalysis from the academic world also treatment, or with minimal, essentially
j.cpr.2008.06.003
left a void, into which were born the alter- sham treatments that were not designed to
natives of cognitive and behavioral thera- be seriously competitive approaches (what
Correspondence concerning this comment pies. my colleague Drew Westen has called “in-
should be addressed to Warren W Tryon, De- It became a convention for cognitive- tent-to-fail” conditions, which are often
partment of Psychology, Fordham University, behavior therapy (CBT) theorists and re- misleadingly labeled in the research litera-
441 E. Fordham Road, Dealy Hall, Bronx, NY searchers to begin articles, papers, and ture as “treatment as usual”). Moreover, the
10458-5198. E-mail: wtryon@fordham.edu books with comments about how psycho- studies that did directly compare CBT with
analytic therapy would supposedly deal legitimate psychodynamic therapies found
DOI: 10.1037/a0022654
with a problem, and then proceed to de- no advantage for CBT. No one took notice:
bunk the psychoanalytic approach and The “master narrative” may have been too
Science or Ideology? show how much more reasonable and help- compelling to academics whose careers,
ful CBT was. From the perspective of those reputations, and funding had now become
of us who practice psychodynamic therapy, tied to CBT and related “empirically sup-
Jonathan Shedler they were debunking fictions of their own ported therapies.”
University of Colorado School of invention; they did not actually know how Three of the four comments on my
Medicine psychodynamic clinicians thought or prac- article appear to have the intent of reas-
The academic psychology literature is filled ticed. After more than two decades of such serting the master narrative by creating a
with pronouncements about psychodynamic writings, many people inside and outside smokescreen of doubt and confusion

152 February–March 2011 ● American Psychologist


(Anestis, Anestis, & Lilienfeld, 2011; eight meta-analyses showing substantial and patient care both in the United States
McKay, 2011; Thombs, Jewett, & Bassel, benefits for psychodynamic therapy re- and abroad.
2011). Intentionally or not, they offer ported in my Table 1 (Shedler, 2010)— has Assuming that the comment’s authors
misinformation, omit crucial information, been the target of criticism and reflects a (Thombs et al., 2011) hold Cuijpers’s meta-
cherry-pick findings to create a misleading computational error. Anestis et al. and analytic research in high regard, they should
impression of the research literature, or im- Thombs et al. are correct in noting that the also be aware of two other meta-analytic
pose a disingenuous double standard with computation method was irregular and pro- studies co-authored by Cuijpers and pub-
respect to what constitutes evidence. Such duced an implausibly large value. However, lished around the same period. One (pub-
tactics seem to emerge predictably when neither of these comments mentions that the lished too late to be included in my article)
the master narrative is at stake, as described authors of that meta-analysis published a cor- showed a large effect size of .69 for psy-
by Wachtel (2010); his elegantly written rection (Leichsenring & Rabung, 2009), chodynamic therapy for the treatment of
exposé of the academic psychotherapy re- more than a year and a half ago, in the depression, further contributing to the lit-
search establishment should be required Journal of the American Medical Associa- erature establishing psychodynamic ther-
reading for every psychologist. tion, and that the corrected, properly calcu- apy as an evidence-based treatment (Dries-
Some examples will illustrate these lated effect size was still an impressive .65— sen et al., 2010). The second found very
issues. Two of the four comments (McKay, comparable to that of any other “empirically substantial publication bias in research
2011; Tryon & Tryon, 2011) cite a meta- supported therapy.” I thus find myself skep- on CBT for depression (Cuijpers, Smit,
analysis indicating that there is no empiri- tical as to whether the intent of the com- Bohlmeijer, Hollon, & Andersson, 2010);
cal support for the concept of “symptom ments’ authors is to accurately describe the the findings indicated that the effect sizes
substitution.” One of them (McKay, 2011) literature and inform readers or to selectively reported in the literature are inflated by
declares that symptom substitution is “a choose findings that bolster the a priori mas- approximately 60% to 75% due to publi-
fundamental axiom according to psychody- ter narrative. cation bias (i.e., studies with less favor-
namic approaches” (p. 147). I want to state, Three of the comments (Anestis et al., able results are either rejected for publi-
categorically and unequivocally, that this 2011; McKay, 2011; Thombs et al., 2011) cation or never submitted for publication
concept has nothing to do with psychody- imply that the methods used in empirical at all). Such a situation may be all but
namic theory or practice. It is irrelevant to studies of psychodynamic therapies are unavoidable when the same individuals
psychodynamic therapy and has no mean- somehow inadequate relative to studies of routinely trade roles as study authors,
ing in the context of contemporary theoret- other evidence-based therapies. To make grant reviewers, journal reviewers, and
ical models (vs. theories that held currency the case, one of them (Thombs et al., 2011) members of academic hiring and promo-
in the psychoanalytic community more tion committees.
cites a meta-analysis by Cuijpers, van
than half a century ago). In fact, most psy- My article (Shedler, 2010) reported
Straten, Bohlmeijer, Hollon, and Ander-
chodynamic practitioners do not even eight meta-analyses, all of which showed
sson (2010) showing that effect sizes for
know what the term means. McKay (2011) consistent and substantial treatment bene-
psychodynamic psychotherapy decrease as
cites Wachtel (1997) to support his asser- fits for psychodynamic therapy. Anestis et
study quality improves and implies that this
tion that symptom substitution is a funda- al. (2011) accuse me of “ignoring crucial
limitation is somehow unique to research
mental axiom, but he fails to mention that findings that run counter to [my] position”
on psychodynamic psychotherapy. How-
Wachtel’s book was actually first published (p. 150), noting that I did not include one
in 1977, more than 30 years ago (Wachtel, ever, Cuipjers himself did not draw this meta-analysis, two decades out of date, that
1977); that Wachtel, a psychoanalytic the- conclusion and does not agree with it; his had not shown such favorable results (the
orist, was arguing against, not for, the con- own conclusion is that the same limitation meta-analysis of Svartberg & Styles,
cept of symptom substitution; and that applies to research on all forms of psycho- 1991). I did not include that particular pub-
Wachtel even then noted that the concept therapy “including CBT, IPT, behavioral lication because it was superseded four
was rarely mentioned in the psychoanalytic activation, etc.” (P. Cuijpers, personal years later by a more rigorous meta-analy-
literature of the time (let alone today; communication, August 11, 2010). The sis that I did include (that of Anderson &
Wachtel, 1997, and P. L. Wachtel, personal most comprehensive meta-analysis to date Lambert, 1995), which reexamined all the
communication, September 7, 2010). In on CBT for depression reported an impres- same studies. The authors of the later meta-
fact, the meta-analysis about symptom sub- sively large effect size (Churchill et al., analysis (whose own allegiance was not,
stitution (Tryon, 2008) begins with the ex- 2001), which I reported in my article with- incidentally, to psychodynamic ap-
plicit statement that the concept was of out editorial comment (Shedler, 2010). proaches) found that the earlier meta-anal-
interest “half a century ago” (p. 963) and However, the authors of that meta-analysis ysis had mistakenly classified as “psy-
long ago faded from attention. Moreover, noted that “the overall quality score of tri- chodynamic” treatments that were not
my article had nothing to do with symp- als appeared to have a considerable effect psychodynamic, including behavioral, cog-
tom substitution. I cannot help wondering . . . . Trials with lower scores demonstrated nitive, and psychoeducational approaches
whether the intent of McKay’s (2011) com- a pronounced and highly significant differ- as well as a bogus treatment that involved
ment is to engage with my arguments in an ence and higher-scoring trials demonstrated only eight 30-minute sessions, which no
intellectually honest way or simply to no significant differences” (Churchill et al., psychodynamic practitioner then or now
throw everything but the kitchen sink that 2001, p. 82). In somewhat simpler language: would consider adequate treatment. I am
might appear to discredit psychodynamic The more rigorously conducted studies thus dismayed to be accused by Anestis et
approaches, whether relevant to my article showed no statistically significant benefits al. (2011) of “glossing over key method-
or not. for CBT. Nevertheless, that meta-analysis ological details” (p. 150).
Two of the comments (Anestis et al., has been subsequently cited repeatedly, Unlike the other comments, the com-
2011; Thombs et al., 2011) note that the with no cautions whatsoever, as providing ment of Tryon and Tryon (2011) appears to
effect size from the meta-analysis by conclusive support for CBT, and it has had be a sincere effort to engage with my ar-
Leichsenring and Rabung (2008)— one of a major influence on health care policies guments. I noted in my article (Shedler,

February–March 2011 ● American Psychologist 153


2010) that there are seven distinctive fea- Anestis, M. D., Anestis, J. C., & Lilienfeld, S. O. Wachtel, P. L. (2010). Beyond “ESTs”: Prob-
tures of psychodynamic therapy that, in (2011). When it comes to evaluating psy- lematic assumptions in the pursuit of evi-
empirical research, reliably distinguish chodynamic therapy, the devil is in the details. dence-based practice. Psychoanalytic Psy-
psychodynamic therapy from other thera- American Psychologist, 66, 149 –151. doi: chology, 27, 251–272. doi:10.1037/a0020532
10.1037/a0021190
pies. Tyron and Tyron suggest that the
Churchill, R., Hunot, V., Corney, R., Knapp, M.,
seven features are not specifically psy- McGuire, H., Tylee, A., & Wessely, S. Correspondence concerning this comment
chodynamic, but merely common factors in (2001). A systematic review of controlled tri- should be addressed to Jonathan Shedler, De-
all effective therapy. However, the empir- als of the effectiveness and cost-effectiveness partment of Psychiatry, University of Colorado
ical research is unambiguous. It is not that of brief psychological treatments for depres- School of Medicine, Mail Stop A011-04, 13001
some therapists trained in other approaches sion. Health Technology Assessment, 5, East 17th Place, Aurora, CO 80045: E-mail:
do not draw on some of these same fea- 1–173. jonathan@shedler.com
tures or that the features are incompatible Cuijpers, P., Smit, F., Bohlmeijer, E., Hollon,
with other therapy approaches. It is that S. D., & Andersson, G. (2010). Efficacy of
psychodynamic therapists do these seven cognitive– behavioural therapyand other psy- DOI: 10.1037/a0022242
chological treatments for adult depression:
things more regularly, consistently, and
deeply than practitioners of other forms of
Meta-analytic study of publication bias. Brit- Integrative Perspectives on
ish Journal of Psychiatry, 196, 173–178. doi:
therapy. Moreover, psychodynamic theory 10.1192/bjp.bp.109.066001
Acculturation
and treatment models explicitly place these Cuijpers, P., van Straten, A., Bohlmeijer, E.,
features at center stage, in ways that other Hollon, S. D., & Andersson, G. (2010). The Caitlin Killian
approaches do not. They are at the heart of effects of psychotherapy for adult depression Drew University
practice-oriented books and articles on psy- are overestimated: A meta-analysis of study
chodynamic therapy but are scarcely men- quality and effect size. Psychological Medicine, Schwartz, Unger, Zamboanga, and Szapoc-
tioned in the manuals for many of the treat- 40, 211–223. doi:10.1017/S0033291709006114
znik (May–June 2010) are to be com-
ments that are actively promoted and Driessen, E., Cuijpers, P., de Maat, S. C. M.,
Abbass, A. A., de Jonghe, F., & Dekker,
mended for their attempts “to propose an
marketed as “empirically supported thera- expanded, multidimensional model of ac-
J. J. M. (2010). The efficacy of short-term
pies.” psychodynamic psychotherapy for depression: culturation and of the demographic and
What disturbs me about the three A meta-analysis. Clinical Psychology Review, contextual forces that can influence the ac-
other comments is not that the authors dis- 30, 25–36. doi:10.1016/j.cpr.2009.08.010 culturation process” (p. 238). In their arti-
agree with my conclusions but that they Leichsenring, F., & Rabung, S. (2008). Effec- cle, they called attention to key factors
portray themselves as objective investiga- tiveness of long-term psychodynamic psycho- such as the generational status of immi-
tors who desire only to promote good sci- therapy: A meta-analysis. Journal of the grants and their children; the role of lo-
ence. I have little doubt that that is how American Medical Association, 300, 1551–
cation, particularly in ethnic enclaves;
they genuinely see themselves. People 1565. doi:10.1001/jama.300.13.1551
Leichsenring, F., & Rabung, S. (2009). Analyz- and the context of reception that immi-
speaking from a dominant paradigm often grants enter, including the potential dis-
ing effectiveness of long-term psychodynamic
assume that they are speaking obvious crimination they may face. These vari-
psychotherapy: In reply. Journal of the Amer-
truths, while people in more marginalized ican Medical Association, 301, 932–933. doi: ables are the crucial backdrop for the
groups tend to experience those in power as 10.1001/jama.2009.181 authors’ call to “focus on the higher order
self-justifying, self-serving, and blind to McKay, D. (2011). Methods and mechanisms in construct of receiving-culture acquisition
important information that does not com- the efficacy of psychodynamic psychotherapy. as well as on the individual dimensions
port with their own worldview (much as American Psychologist, 66, 147–148. doi: of this higher order construct—practices,
academic psychologists experienced many 10.1037/a0021195
values, and identifications” (p. 246). As a
of the medical psychoanalysts in the mid- Shedler, J. K. (2010). The efficacy of psychody-
namic psychotherapy. American Psychologist,
sociologist trained in social psychology, I
dle of the 20th century). From my perspec- am pleased by their incorporation of
tive, the authors of these comments essen- 65, 98 –109. doi:10.1037/a0018378
Svartberg, M., & Styles, T.C. (1991). Compara- some of the sociological literature on
tially offer tendentious arguments aimed at these processes. However, I was sur-
tive effects of short-term psychodynamic psy-
promoting an ideological agenda. I would chotherapy: A meta-analysis. Journal of Con- prised by important gaps in their discus-
go so far as to say that their writings betray sulting and Clinical Psychology, 59, 704 – sion of Portes and Rumbaut’s (1996,
a troubling disrespect for scientific evi- 714. doi:10.1037/0022-006X.59.5.704 2006) work and by their neglect of one of
dence. While Anestis et al. (2011), McKay Thombs, B. D., Jewett, L. R., & Bassel, M. the most widely used terms employed by
(2011), and Thombs et al. (2011) imply or (2011). Is there room for criticism of studies of
sociologists to hypothesize outcomes for
explicitly state that I am the one who mar- psychodynamic psychotherapy? American Psy-
chologist, 66, 148 –149. doi:10.1037/a0021248
the very questions Schwartz et al. were
shals evidence selectively, from my angle
Tryon, W. W. (2008). Whatever happened to posing.
of vision they appear to value only evi-
symptom substitution? Clinical Psychology Although Schwartz et al. (2010) thor-
dence that supports an a priori agenda
Review, 28, 963–968. doi:10.1016/j.cpr.2008. oughly detailed Berry’s (1980) four accul-
while ignoring, dismissing, or attacking ev-
02.003 turation models, they failed to discuss
idence that does not. If so, this is not sci- Tryon, W. W., & & Tryon, G. S. (2011). No Portes and Rumbaut’s (1996, 2006) selec-
ence, but ideology masquerading as science. ownership of common factors. American Psy- tive acculturation. In their own four mod-
chologist, 66, 151–152. doi:10.1037/a0021056 els, Portes and Rumbaut discussed whether
REFERENCES Wachtel, P. L. (1977). Psychoanalysis and be-
children of immigrants acculturate or resist
havior therapy: Toward an integration. New
Anderson, E. M., & Lambert, M. J. (1995). York, NY: Basic Books.
acculturation in consonance or in disso-
Short-term dynamically oriented psychother- Wachtel, P. L. (1997). Psychoanalysis, behavior nance with their parents, which in turn af-
apy: A review and meta-analysis. Clinical therapy, and the relational world. Washing- fects their level of acculturative stress, and
Psychology Review, 15, 503–514. doi: ton, DC: Amercian Psychological Associa- thus their mental health outcomes, in part
10.1016/0272-7358(95)00027-M tion. through the potential for role-reversals with

154 February–March 2011 ● American Psychologist

You might also like