Analysis of Self Feeding in Children With Feeding Disorders (2014)

You might also like

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 14

JOURNAL OF APPLIED BEHAVIOR ANALYSIS 2014, 47, 710–722 NUMBER 4 (WINTER)

ANALYSIS OF SELF-FEEDING IN CHILDREN WITH


FEEDING DISORDERS
KRISTI M. RIVAS, CATHLEEN C. PIAZZA, HENRY S. ROANE, VALERIE M. VOLKERT,
VICTORIA STEWART, HEATHER J. KADEY, AND REBECCA A. GROFF
UNIVERSITY OF NEBRASKA MEDICAL CENTER’S MUNROE-MEYER INSTITUTE

In the current investigation, we evaluated a method for increasing self-feeding with 3 children with a
history of food refusal. The children never (2 children) or rarely (1 child) self-fed bites of food when
the choice was between self-feeding and escape from eating. When the choice was between self-
feeding 1 bite of food or being fed an identical bite of food, self-feeding was low (2 children) or
variable (1 child). Levels of self-feeding increased for 2 children when the choice was between self-
feeding 1 bite of food or being fed multiple bites of the same food. For the 3rd child, self-feeding
increased when the choice was between self-feeding 1 bite of food or being fed multiple bites of a less
preferred food. The results showed that altering the contingencies associated with being fed
increased the probability of self-feeding, but the specific manipulations that produced self-feeding
were unique to each child.
Key words: choice, concurrent operants, feeding disorder, food refusal, food selectivity, pediatric
feeding disorders, response effort, self-feeding

Much is known about the developmental children with feeding disorders (e.g., Cooper
progression of self-feeding skills in children who et al., 1999; Kerwin, Ahearn, Eicher, & Burd,
eat typically (e.g., Carruth, Ziegler, Gordon, & 1995; Vaz, Volkert, & Piazza, 2011). For example,
Hendricks, 2004). For example, before 7 months Kerwin et al. (1995) evaluated the effects of
of age, most children who eat typically are bottle response effort on levels of acceptance and
and spoon fed by a caregiver. The vast majority of consumption in children with feeding disorders.
children who eat typically will use their hands to In Study 1, the feeder presented the child with
grasp pieces of food by 7 to 11 months of age and varying amounts of food on a spoon, which was
will eat with a spoon by 19 to 24 months, and this conceptualized as the response-effort manipula-
progression will occur in the absence of formal- tion, and acceptance resulted in access to toys and
ized intervention (Carruth & Skinner, 2002). praise. In general, as response effort increased with
Surprisingly little is known, however, about the increasing food volume, the probability of eating
emergence of self-feeding in children who have decreased and the probability of refusal increased.
been diagnosed with feeding disorders. That is, In Study 2, feeders used putative escape extinction
does self-feeding emerge in children with feeding in the form of nonremoval of the spoon or
disorders in the absence of formalized interven- physical guidance to increase acceptance. Kerwin
tion as it does in typically eating children, or is the et al. hypothesized that implementation of escape
motivation to self-feed affected by their historical extinction increased the aversiveness of refusal. As
avoidance of eating? the aversiveness of refusal increased, the probabil-
One way of answering this question is to ity of refusal decreased, and the probability of
examine variables that affect the responding of eating increased.
Vaz et al. (2011) applied the same conceptual
Address correspondence to Cathleen C. Piazza, Munroe- framework used by Kerwin et al. (1995) to
Meyer Institute, University of Nebraska Medical Center, increase the self-feeding of one child with a
985450 Nebraska Medical Center, Omaha, Nebraska 68198
(e-mail: cpiazza@unmc.edu). history of food selectivity. In baseline, the child
doi: 10.1002/jaba.170 did not eat when he had the choice of feeding

710
SELF-FEEDING 711

himself a target food or engaging in refusal METHOD


behavior. Following baseline, Vaz et al. identified Participants, Setting, and Materials
several low-preference foods, called avoidance
Three children who had been admitted to an
foods, which the feeder used in the treatment
outpatient pediatric feeding disorders program
manipulation. During treatment, self-feeding
participated. A goal of the admission was to
increased when the child had the choice of
increase self-feeding, which was the focus of the
feeding himself one bite of a target food or having
current investigation. Based on direct observation
the caregiver feed him one bite of the target food
and caregiver report, all children had the skills to
and five bites of an avoidance food.
self-feed, but none self-fed consistently at home
Vaz et al. (2011) conceptualized their manipu-
or in the clinic.
lation in terms of the response effort and quality of
The participants were Tina, Seth, and Brian for
reinforcement associated with responding. The
Experiment 1 and Brian for Experiment 2. Tina
response-effort manipulation was that self-feeding
was a 3-year-old girl who had been diagnosed
was associated with consumption of a single bite of
with status post short-gut syndrome secondary to
food and being fed was associated with consump-
gastroschisis, which resulted in a small-bowel
tion of six bites of food. Consistent with Kerwin
transplant. Before admission, she was 100%
et al. (1995), Vaz et al. hypothesized that the larger
dependent on gastrostomy (G-) tube feeding and
volume of food was more effortful to consume
parenteral nutrition; she consumed nothing by
because it required more responses. The qualitative
mouth. Seth was a 3-year-old boy who had been
manipulation was that self-feeding was associated
diagnosed with gastroesophageal reflux disease
with consumption of the relatively more preferred
and failure to thrive. Before admission, he was
target food and being fed was associated with the
100% dependent on G-tube feeding; he con-
relatively less preferred avoidance food. The
sumed nothing by mouth. Brian was a 2.5-year-
limitation of Vaz et al. was that the necessity of
old boy who had been diagnosed with pervasive
both the response-effort and quality manipulations
developmental disorder not otherwise specified
to increase self-feeding was unclear.
and no other significant medical history. Before
In the current investigation, we extended the
admission, he consumed most types of Stage 2
results of Kerwin et al. (1995) and Vaz et al.
and 3 jarred baby foods except vegetables. He
(2011) by evaluating a method for increasing self-
refused foods of any other texture. He received
feeding with three children with a history of food
100% of his liquids via baby bottle.
refusal. Before this investigation, we treated these
Sessions for Tina and Seth were conducted in
children’s feeding problems with nonremoval of
the clinic in rooms (4 m by 4 m) with one-way
the spoon with the caregiver feeding the child.
observation panels and sound monitoring. Ses-
Next, we gave each child the opportunity to
sions for Brian were conducted in the home, and
self-feed or to escape eating, and all children
sessions were observed via telehealth. Materials
frequently chose to escape eating and infrequent-
included utensils, bowls, seating equipment, food
ly chose to self-feed. We then eliminated escape
trays, gloves, timers, and laptop computers.
from eating and gave the child a choice between
being fed and self-feeding. Under this arrange-
ment, the children did not self-feed consistently. Response Measurement and Reliability
We then manipulated the number of bites (Study Observers used laptop computers to score self-
1) or the number of bites and type of food (Study fed acceptance for each bite presentation when the
2) associated with being fed until the child’s child picked up the spoon of food and placed
responding shifted from preference for being fed the entire bite past the plane of the lips and into
to preference for self-feeding. the mouth within 5 s (Tina) or 30 s (Seth and
712 KRISTI M. RIVAS et al.

Brian) of the initial presentation. We decreased occurrence agreement was 87%, 87%, and 98%;
the time requirement for self-fed acceptance for nonoccurrence agreement was 83%, 88%, and
Tina because we observed that Seth and Brian 98%; and total agreement was 92%, 93%, and
sometimes waited until the end of the 30-s 99% for Tina, Seth, and Brian, respectively. For
interval to self-feed the bite. Therefore, we the stimulus preference assessment, we calculated
shortened the latency for Tina to decrease the agreement as occurrence (both observers scored
interpresentation interval and prevent long the behavior during the trial) plus nonoccurrence
pauses in this interval, which made the meal (both observers did not score the behavior during
more efficient. (We conducted the self-feeding a trial) agreements divided by occurrence plus
analysis with Seth and Brian before we conducted nonoccurrence agreements plus disagreements
it with Tina.) Observers scored a presentation and converted this ratio to a percentage. Mean
when the caregiver placed a bowl with the spoon interobserver agreement was 97% for consump-
of food in front of the child. Data for self-fed tion and 100% for avoidance for Brian during the
acceptance were converted to a percentage after stimulus preference assessment.
dividing occurrences of self-fed acceptance by
number of presentations. General Procedure
During the food preference assessment (Brian Tina’s and Seth’s mothers and Brian’s paternal
only), observers scored consumption when Brian grandmother served as feeders and are henceforth
had no food larger than a pea in his mouth 30 s referred to as caregivers. Before this analysis,
after the entire bite entered the mouth, unless the children received feeding treatment in intensive
absence of food in the mouth was due to outpatient and outpatient clinics (Tina and
expulsion (spitting out the food). Observers Brian) and day-treatment and outpatient clinics
scored avoidance when Brian contacted the (Seth) for 10, 7, and 4.5 months for Tina, Seth,
feeder’s hand or arm or the spoon, covered his and Brian, respectively. As a result, we had
mouth, or engaged in negative vocalizations such conducted caregiver training on feeding protocol
as screaming, crying, or saying “get it away” while implementation for approximately 174, 45, and
the feeder presented the bite. 74 hr with the caregivers of Tina, Seth, and Brian,
Two observers recorded data simultaneously respectively, before the initiation of the self-
but independently during 75%, 24%, and 45% feeding analysis.
of self-feeding sessions for Tina, Seth, and Brian, Caregivers selected target foods before the start
respectively, and 100% of stimulus preference of the analysis from a list of 68 possible foods.
assessment trials for Brian. We calculated occur- Target foods were the focus of the self-feeding
rence agreement as occurrence agreements (both analysis; that is, they were the foods that the
observers scored a self-fed acceptance) divided caregiver wanted the child to self-feed. The target
by occurrence agreements plus disagreements foods were carrots, chicken nuggets, green beans,
(one observer scored and the other did not score a hot dogs, mashed potatoes, sweet potatoes, and
self-fed acceptance) and converted this ratio to a tuna for Tina; bread, broccoli and cheese, carrots,
percentage. We calculated nonoccurrence agree- cheese, chicken, green beans, hot dogs, mashed
ment as nonoccurrence agreements (both ob- potatoes, peaches, pears, and waffles for Seth;
servers did not score a self-fed acceptance) divided and baked beans, baked potato, bananas, carrots,
by nonoccurrence agreements plus disagreements chicken, corn, green beans, macaroni and cheese,
and converted this ratio to a percentage. We mandarin oranges, mashed potatoes, peaches,
calculated total agreement as agreements divided pears, rice, scrambled eggs, soup beans, and
by agreements plus disagreements and converted spaghetti in Study 1 and the same foods except for
the ratio to a percentage. For self-fed acceptance, scrambled eggs in Study 2 for Brian.
SELF-FEEDING 713

Children participated in weekly 1- to 1.5-hr each five-bite session, the caregiver presented
appointments. During each appointment, care- three of the foods once and one of the foods twice,
givers conducted multiple five-bite sessions with one bite at a time. The caregiver presented the
brief breaks between sessions. The mean number bite by placing a spoon with food in a bowl on a
of self-feeding sessions conducted per appoint- table or tray in front of the child accompanied by
ment was 4.4 for Tina, 3.8 for Seth, and 8.8 for a prompt to “take a bite” approximately every 30 s
Brian. Before each appointment, the therapist if the child did not self-feed the bite or 30 s after
who coordinated the treatment selected at least the previous bite entered the child’s mouth. The
four foods from the list of caregiver-identified caregiver delivered praise (e.g., “Good job taking
target foods to present during sessions. The your bite”) after self-fed acceptance. After the bite
therapist ensured that the caregiver presented entered the child’s mouth, the caregiver activated
every target food in each phase of the analysis a timer for 30 s. When the 30-s interval elapsed,
with approximately equal numbers of presenta- the caregiver implemented a mouth check by
tions of each food within the phase. The therapist saying “show me” while modeling an open mouth.
told the caregivers which foods to bring to the If the child did not open his or her mouth in
appointment. For Tina, the caregiver presented response to the verbal and model prompt, the
the same four target foods in each session, with at caregiver used a rubber-coated baby spoon to
least one food from the food groups of protein, prompt the child to open his or her mouth by
starch, and vegetable (e.g., chicken, potatoes, inserting the spoon just past the plane of the
green beans, tuna) because she could not eat fruit child’s lips and turning the spoon 90°. The
due to her medical condition. For Seth and caregiver delivered praise (e.g., “Good job
Brian, one target food from the food groups of swallowing your bite”) if no food larger than a
protein, starch, fruit, and vegetable (e.g., chicken, pea was in the child’s mouth after the first 30-s
potatoes, peaches, green beans) were presented. check for each bite and then presented the next
The caregiver presented target foods in a bite. The caregiver did not provide praise if the
random order across sessions. The texture of absence of food was due to expulsion. The
presented food was pureed table food for Tina caregiver delivered a prompt to “finish swallowing
and Seth and finely chopped table food for Brian. your bite” if the child had food larger than a pea in
The bolus volume for target and avoidance foods his or her mouth at the 30-s check and then
(Brian only) was 1.75 cc on a small Maroon immediately presented the next bite. If the child
spoon for Tina, 2 cc on a toddler spoon for Seth, was packing, which was defined as food larger than
and 4 cc on a toddler spoon for Brian. These were a pea in the mouth at the 30-s check, the caregiver
the textures, utensils, and bolus sizes we had conducted checks after the caregiver had presented
presented in the previous treatment of the child’s the last (fifth) bite of the session and prompted the
feeding problem. child to swallow every 30 s until no food larger
Before the start of sessions, the caregiver than a pea remained in the child’s mouth.
explained the contingencies to the child (e.g., “If General baseline procedure. The purpose of
you take your bite, I will say, ‘good job.’ If you the baseline was to establish levels of self-fed
don’t take your bite, I will feed you two bites of acceptance when the child had the choice of self-
the same food.”). Explanation of the contingen- feeding or refusing the bite of target food. The
cies occurred once per appointment unless the caregiver followed the general procedure. If the
contingencies changed during the appointment. child did not self-feed the bite, the bowl remained
In this case, the caregiver explained the new on the table or tray for 30 s. After 30 s, the
contingencies to the child before beginning caregiver removed the bowl and presented the
sessions with the changed contingencies. During next bite.
714 KRISTI M. RIVAS et al.

General treatment procedure. The caregiver Design


followed the general procedure with the following For the treatment-condition labels, we use the
modifications. If the child did not self-feed the acronyms TF and AF to represent the words
bite, the caregiver used nonremoval of the spoon target food and avoidance food, respectively. Each
with re-presentation of expelled bites (Hoch, label contains two acronyms with accompanying
Babbitt, Coe, Krell, & Hackbert, 1994) to feed numbers to the left and right of a colon (e.g.,
the number and type of foods specified by the 1TF:3TF). The label to the left of the colon is
arranged contingency described below. During always 1TF, indicating that the caregiver pre-
nonremoval of the spoon, the caregiver held the sented the child with one bite of target food to
spoon at the child’s lips until she could deposit self-feed. The label to the right indicates the
the bite into the child’s mouth. In conditions in number of bites and type of food the caregiver fed
which the caregiver fed the child multiple bites to the child if the child did not self-feed. For
consecutively, she fed the child the bite that was example, in the 1TF:2AF condition, the caregiver
in the bowl in front of the child and the additional presented the child with one bite of the target
bites from a bowl of identical target food located food (1TF), and if the child did not self-feed the
on a table next to her. She fed the bites one after bite of target food, the caregiver fed the child two
the other and activated the mouth-check timer bites of the avoidance food (2AF). In Study 1, we
after the prescribed number and type of bites had evaluated the effectiveness of treatment in an
entered the child’s mouth. ABCAC design for Tina, an ABCDAD design for
The caregiver re-presented expelled bites each Seth, and an ABCDEF design for Brian. The
time an expulsion occurred. During re-presenta- baseline was A, B was 1 TF:1TF, C was 1TF:2TF,
tion, the caregiver used the spoon to scoop up the D was 1TF:3TF, E was 1TF:4TF, and F was
expelled food from the child’s face or bib and 1TF:5TF. In Study 2, we evaluated the effective-
placed it back into the child’s mouth using the ness of treatment in an ABCDEAE design for
nonremoval procedure. If the caregiver could not Brian. The baseline was A, B was 1TF:1AF, C was
re-present the bite that had been in the child’s 1TF:2AF, D was 1TF:3AF, and E was 1TF:4AF.
mouth (e.g., it fell on the floor), the caregiver
obtained food from the bowl for the re-
STUDY 1: BITE-NUMBER
presentation. If the caregiver had to re-present
MANIPULATION WITH TARGET FOOD
food from the bowl, she estimated the amount of
food that the child expelled and re-presented the Baseline. The caregiver followed the general
estimated amount. If the child expelled more baseline procedure.
than one food type (e.g., tuna and peas at the Bite-number manipulation with target food.
same time), the caregiver obtained a small The caregiver followed the general treatment
amount of each food type from the bowls until procedure. If the child did not self-feed the bite of
the total bolus equaled the estimated amount of target food independently, the caregiver fed
food that had been expelled. the child the prescribed number of bites of
The caregiver continued to re-present the the identical target food. For example, in the
expelled bite until the child swallowed. Tina and 1TF:2TF condition, if the child did not accept
Brian did not expel during the self-feeding the bite of chicken independently, the caregiver
analysis. Seth expelled six times in Session 12 fed the child two bites of chicken. If the child’s
and once in Session 30. Thus, re-presentation did self-fed acceptance did not increase across
not affect session length or prevent the caregiver sessions based on visual inspection of the data,
from presenting the next bite on 99% of the number of caregiver-fed bites increased from
presented bites for Seth. phase to phase until the child began self-feeding
SELF-FEEDING 715

consistently, or we reached a maximum of (Miller, 1968; Perone & Baron, 1980). In the
1TF:5TF. We did not go beyond 1TF:5TF investigations by Miller and Perone and Baron, at
because the amount of food that the child would least one of the response choices was arbitrary (e.g.,
have consumed at higher ratios would have been pulling a knob). Those investigations focused on
unacceptably large. describing the basic effects of alterations of effort on
Mean session length in minutes was 4.3 for responding, which suggested that effortful respond-
baseline, 4.7 for 1TF:1TF, and 3.7 for 1TF:2TF ing is aversive. By contrast, the current investigation
for Tina; 4.3 for baseline, 3.9 for 1TF:1TF, 4.8 focused on applying those basic findings to the
for 1TF:2TF, and 4.0 for 1TF:3TF for Seth; and socially important behavior of self-feeding. Similar-
4.3 for baseline, 4.3 for 1TF:1TF, 4.2 for ly, Patel, Piazza, Layer, Coleman, and Swartzwelder
1TF:2TF, 4.5 for 1TF:3TF, 5.4 for 1TF:4TF, (2005) showed that as texture decreased, packing
and 5.9 for 1TF:5TF for Brian. Mean target-bite decreased and vice versa. Patel et al. hypothesized
presentation per minute was 1.2 for baseline, 1.1 that participants lacked the oral motor skills to
for 1TF:1TF, and 1.3 for 1TF:2TF for Tina; 1.9 manipulate higher textures of food; therefore,
for baseline, 1.3 for 1TF:1TF, 1.0 for 1TF:2TF, decreasing the texture of presented food decreased
and 1.4 for 1TF:3TF for Seth; and 1.2 for the response effort associated with eating.
baseline, 1.2 for 1TF:1TF, 1.2 for 1TF:2TF, 1.1 Although the probability of self-feeding increased
for 1TF:3TF, 0.94 for 1TF:4TF, and 0.84 for for Tina and Seth as the number of caregiver-fed
1TF:5TF for Brian. These data suggest that the bites increased, Brian did not self-feed consistently,
caregiver was able to present target bites and the even when the ratio of self-fed to caregiver-fed bites
child accepted caregiver-fed bites at approximate- was 1:5. These results suggest that response-effort
ly the same rate across conditions for Tina and manipulations may increase self-feeding in some
Seth. For Brian, presentation of target bites was children with feeding disorders, but not others.
delayed slightly relative to baseline during the
1TF:4TF and 1TF:5TF conditions.
STUDY 2: BITE-NUMBER AND FOOD-
Results and Discussion TYPE MANIPULATION
The results for percentage of self-fed accep- The purpose of Study 2 was to examine an
tance for Tina (top), Seth (middle), and Brian adjunctive method of increasing self-feeding for
(bottom) appear in Figure 1. During baseline, Brian. First, we identified a low-preference food
Tina infrequently and Seth and Brian never self- (the avoidance food). Next, we increased the ratio
fed bites. The probability of self-fed acceptance of self-fed to caregiver-fed bites as in Study 1, but
increased when the ratio of self-fed to caregiver- the caregiver-fed bites were presentations of the
fed bites was 1:2 for Tina and 1:3 for Seth. Brian’s avoidance food instead of the target food.
self-feeding never increased to clinically accept- Stimulus preference assessment. The purpose of
able levels. this assessment was to identify an avoidance
Kerwin et al. (1995) increased volume of food by food to use in treatment. We conducted a paired-
increasing bite size, the response-effort manipula- stimulus preference assessment based on Fisher
tion, and observed decreases in acceptance. We et al. (1992) to determine Brian’s relative
extended Kerwin et al. in that we increased volume preference for the 16 foods we used in Study 1.
by increasing bite number to decrease one response We used the general procedure described above
and increase an alternative response. These results with the following modifications. The caregiver
are consistent with basic studies on response effort presented two different foods by placing two
in that increasing response effort for one behavior bowls with one bite of food on a spoon in each
shifted responding to an alternative behavior bowl on the tray in front of Brian with the
716 KRISTI M. RIVAS et al.

Baseline 1 TF:1 TF 1 TF:2 TF BL 1 TF:2 TF


100 (BL)
PERCENTAGE OF BITES WITH
SELF-FED ACCEPTANCE

80

60

40

20
TINA
0
0 10 20 30 40 50 60 70 80 90 100 110 120

1 TF: 1 TF: 1 Target Food (TF):3 TF BL 1 TF:3 TF


BL 1 TF 2 TF
100
PERCENTAGE OF BITES WITH
SELF-FED ACCEPTANCE

80

60

40

20
SETH

0
0
0 5 10 15 20 25 30 35 40 45 50 55 60

BL 1 TF:1 TF 1 TF:2 TF 1 TF:3 TF 1 TF: 1 TF:


4 TF 5 TF
100
PERCENTAGE OF BITES WITH
SELF-FED ACCEPTANCE

80

60
BRIAN

40

20

0
0 5 10 15 20 25 30 35 40 45 50 55 60 65 70 75 80 85 90 95
SESSION

Figure 1. Percentage of bites with self-fed acceptance for Tina (top), Seth (middle), and Brian (bottom) during Study 1.

prompt to “pick one.” The caregiver presented bowl of the nonchosen food, presented the
each food with every other food and selected the chosen food by touching the spoon to Brian’s lips
order of pairings randomly. If Brian chose a food, for 30 s, and deposited the bite if Brian opened
the caregiver provided brief praise, removed the his mouth during the 30-s interval. The caregiver
SELF-FEEDING 717

did not re-present expelled bites. If Brian did not ence for the four less preferred foods. Of the four
choose or accept a bite during the 30-s interval, foods that Brian never consumed, he engaged in
the caregiver removed the bowls and presented the highest levels of avoidance behavior (20% of
the next pair of foods. The results of the trials) with the scrambled eggs; therefore, we
preference assessment identified scrambled eggs chose scrambled eggs as the avoidance food.
as the least preferred food for Brian. Thereafter, During baseline of the self-feeding analysis,
the caregiver presented scrambled eggs only in Brian infrequently self-fed target bites. He
the avoidance procedure and did not present engaged in steadily increasing levels of self-fed
scrambled eggs in the rotation of target foods in acceptance when the ratio of self-fed target bites
baseline or treatment. to caregiver-fed avoidance bites was 1:4.
Baseline. The procedure was identical to that When effort manipulations alone were not
of Study 1, except the caregiver did not present effective for Brian in Study 1, we evaluated his
scrambled eggs. relative preferences for the target foods and
Bite-number and food-type manipulation. The identified his least preferred food. An alternative
procedure was similar to that of Study 1, except method for identifying avoidance foods would be
when Brian did not self-feed the bite, the caregiver to use caregiver report regarding foods the child
fed him the prescribed number of bites of avoidance had avoided in the past or to use foods that are
food. To illustrate, in the 1TF:3AF condition, the consumed less often by the general population.
caregiver presented a bite of mashed potatoes. If Consistent with Vaz et al. (2011), we conceptu-
Brian did not self-feed the bite of mashed potatoes, alized presentation of the least preferred food as a
the caregiver removed the bowl of mashed potatoes quality manipulation. When we altered effort and
and fed him three bites of scrambled eggs. Mean quality of caregiver-fed bites, responding shifted
session length in minutes across phases was 4.2 for to self-feeding when being fed was associated with
baseline, 5.1 for 1TF:1AF, 5.3 for 1TF:2AF, 5.3 for presentation of four bites of the avoidance food.
1TF:3AF, and 4.9 for 1TF:4AF. Mean target-bite These data replicate other studies that show
presentation per minute was 1.2 in baseline, 0.99 that manipulations of the parameters associated
for 1TF:1AF, 0.94 for 1TF:2AF, 0.96 for 1TF:3AF, with responding can be an effective method for
and 1.1 for 1TF:4AF, suggesting that the caregiver changing behavior. For example, Athens and
was able to present target bites and Brian accepted Vollmer (2010) evaluated the effects of alterations
caregiver-fed bites at approximately the same rate of duration of, quality of, and delay to reinforce-
across conditions. ment, and a combination of these parameters
during differential-reinforcement-of-alternative-
Results and Discussion behavior schedules. Although the various manip-
The top panel of Figure 2 shows the percentage ulations in isolation produced some changes in
of consumption (solid bars) and avoidance behavior, the greatest changes occurred when
(dotted bars) during the preference assessment, multiple manipulations occurred simultaneously.
and the bottom panel shows the percentage of Similarly, the greatest and most consistent changes
self-fed acceptance during the self-feeding analy- in behavior for Brian occurred when the caregiver
sis. During the preference assessment, Brian did manipulated both the response effort and the
not consume four foods (chicken, scrambled quality of food associated with being fed.
eggs, creamed corn, baked potato), which
suggested that these foods were less preferred
GENERAL DISCUSSION
than the foods he consumed more frequently
during the assessment. We examined the avoid- These results are important for a number of
ance data to further discriminate Brian’s prefer- reasons. First, the results suggest that a history of a
718 KRISTI M. RIVAS et al.

100
CONSUMPTION
90
AVOIDANCE
PERCENTAGE OF TRIALS

80
70
60
50
40 BRIAN
30
20
10
0 Mac & Banana Baked Pears Peaches Spaghetti Mandarin Carrots Mashed Rice Green Tuna Chicken Scrambled Creamed Baked
Cheese Beans Oranges Potatoes Beans Eggs Corn Potato

FOODS
1 Target Food (TF):
1 TF: 1 TF: 4 AF Baseline (BL) 1 TF: 4 Avoidance Food (AF)
1 TF: 3 AF
BL 1 AF 2 AF

100
PERCENTAGE OF BITES WITH

90
SELF-FED ACCEPTANCE

80
70
60
50
40
30
20
10 BRIAN
0
0 10 20 30 40 50 60 70 80 90 100 110 120 130 140 150 160 170 180
SESSION

Figure 2. Percentage of trials with consumption (solid bars) and avoidance (dotted bars) by food during the stimulus
preference assessment (top) and percentage of bites with self-fed acceptance (bottom) for Brian in Study 2.

feeding disorder may be associated with refusal to children in this investigation was similar to our
self-feed in an age-typical manner, at least for the clinical experience that many children with severe
participants in the current investigation. The feeding disorders do not exhibit age-typical self-
children were at or beyond the age (19 to 24 feeding without intervention. The data from the
months) at which self-feeding is expected to current study suggest that this refusal may be a
emerge, yet none exhibited consistent self- function of a motivational deficit.
feeding behavior. At home, they rarely or never The results of the current investigation extend
self-fed by caregiver report, and in the clinic, self- those of Kerwin et al. (1995) and Vaz et al.
feeding was inconsistent. The failure to self-feed (2011). As Kerwin et al. noted, a history of
did not appear to be due to motor limitations, medical problems, oral-motor problems, or both,
because we had observed the children self-feed in and escape from bite presentations may bias the
the past and they were able to self-feed without responding of children with feeding problems in
assistance during treatment. The behavior of the favor of refusal. The data from the current
SELF-FEEDING 719

investigation are consistent with Kerwin et al. in manipulations, and she had participated in
that participants more often refused to eat when feeding therapy for the longest time. Brian began
given the choice between escape from eating and self-feeding with the most manipulations, and
self-feeding in baseline. The children also chose he had participated in feeding therapy for the
to be fed either more often (Seth and Brian) or at shortest time. However, it is not clear whether
least some of the time (Tina) when given the length of feeding therapy had any impact on the
choice between being fed and self-feeding the self-feeding manipulation.
identical bite of food in the 1TF:1TF condition. These results are also important because there
Recall that Vaz et al. used a combination of have been surprisingly few investigations on
effort and quality manipulations to increase self- increasing self-feeding in children with underlying
feeding. However, it was not clear whether feeding disorders. The majority of investigations
both were necessary. The results from the current on self-feeding have involved participants with
investigation suggest that different manipulations disabilities who required training to master the
may be necessary to alter the behavior of different specific skills associated with self-feeding (Collins,
children. Nevertheless, these data provide a Gast, Wolery, Holcombe, & Leatherby, 1991;
model for how contingencies may be manipulat- Leibowitz & Holcer, 1974; Luiselli, 1991, 1993;
ed to increase self-feeding. Piazza, Anderson, & Fisher, 1993; Richman,
The model we used was one in which the first Sonderby, & Kahn, 1980; Sisson & Dixon, 1986;
manipulation was to increase the response effort Stimbert, Minor, & McCoy, 1977; Thompson,
associated with being fed via systematic increases 1977). Self-feeding is a behavior that is important
in the ratio of caregiver-fed to self-fed bites of the for the child’s socialization (i.e., for the child to be
same food. If the response-effort manipulation similar to same-aged peers) and is valued by
was ineffective, we then manipulated both the caregivers (Schulze, Harwood, & Schoelmerich,
effort and quality of food associated with being 2001). If refusal to self-feed is, in fact, a chronic
fed. Although we used the model of response problem for children with feeding problems, then
effort followed by effort and quality manipu- investigations like the current study and those of
lations, other sequences or types of manipulations Luiselli (2000) and Vaz et al. (2011) are important
could be the focus of future research. in providing clinicians with the necessary tools to
One unanswered question is why different address the problem.
manipulations were effective for different chil- The data from the current investigation also
dren. Recall that of the three children, we used are important because few studies have been
the largest bolus size and a higher texture of food conducted using negative reinforcement and
with Brian; thus, eating for him may have been both response-effort and quality manipulations
even more effortful than for Tina and Seth. That to increase a desirable behavior. Manipulations in
may be one reason Brian required manipulations the majority of studies have focused on increasing
of response effort and food quality to increase response effort of an inappropriate behavior to
self-feeding, and this could be a topic for future decrease that inappropriate behavior (e.g., self-
investigations. An alternative explanation is that injury; Hanley, Piazza, Keeney, Blakeley-Smith,
Brian had received the least amount of feeding & Worsdell, 1998; Van Houten, 1993), to shift
therapy of the three participants before the start responding from an inappropriate to a more
of this study. Interestingly, the number and type desirable behavior in the context of concurrent
of manipulations necessary to increase self- reinforcement schedules for both behaviors (e.g.,
feeding were inversely correlated with the self-injury and toy play; Shore, Iwata, DeLeon,
duration of feeding therapy. Of the three Kahng, & Smith, 1997), or to test the efficacy
children, Tina began self-feeding with the fewest of different stimuli as reinforcement during
720 KRISTI M. RIVAS et al.

progressive-ratio schedules of reinforcement (e.g., Another limitation is that the initial treatments
Glover, Roane, Kadey, & Grow, 2008). implemented before the current study to establish
Some limitations of the study are that we did consumption occurred in the context of a
not evaluate a positive reinforcement procedure caregiver feeding the child. It is possible that
for self-feeding before our manipulations. Al- this history of being fed during initial treatment
though a number of studies have shown that biased the children toward being fed and reduced
reinforcement for acceptance and swallowing the probability that they would self-feed. We first
may be an effective treatment for food refusal treated the children in the context of being fed
(e.g., Stark, Bowen, Tyc, Evans, & Passero, 1990; because we hypothesized that being fed was less
Stark et al., 1993; Turner, Sanders, & Wall, effortful than self-feeding and that reducing the
1994), more recent studies have demonstrated effort of eating was important for establishing
that putative escape extinction may be a necessary consumption. It is not clear whether the method
component of treatment for some children (e.g., by which initial acceptance is established in
Bachmeyer et al., 2009; LaRue et al., 2011; children with feeding disorders affects the
Piazza, Patel, Gulotta, Sevin, & Layer, 2003; subsequent probability of self-feeding, but this
Reed et al., 2004). In fact, all of the children in could be a topic to explore in future research.
the current investigation required an escape In conclusion, we evaluated a method to
extinction component in the initial treatment increase self-feeding by three children with a
of their feeding problem that was conducted history of food refusal. We showed that these
before the current investigation. Therefore, we children often preferred to be fed when given a
hypothesized that these children were not likely choice between being fed by a caregiver or self-
to respond to reinforcement procedures for self- feeding. We then manipulated the response effort
feeding. As Kerwin et al. (1995) noted, positive or response effort and quality of food fed by the
reinforcement may be a “luxury” that some caregiver. The results showed that altering the
children with feeding disorders are willing to contingencies associated with being fed increased
forgo if the cost of that luxury is consumption. the probability of self-feeding. Even though the
Said another way, many children with feeding specific manipulations that produced self-feeding
problems would choose not to eat rather than eat were unique to each child, this study provides a
and access highly preferred stimuli, and it often is general model for manipulating concurrent
difficult to identify preferred stimuli that contingencies to increase self-feeding.
compete with escape from eating (Piazza et al.,
2003). Nevertheless, this hypothesis is specula-
tive, and future investigators might explore the REFERENCES
use of positive reinforcement or a combination Athens, E. S., & Vollmer, T. R. (2010). An investigation of
of positive reinforcement, response-effort, and differential reinforcement of alternative behavior
quality manipulations to increase self-feeding. without extinction. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis,
43, 569–589. doi: 10.1901/jaba.2010.43-569
A second limitation is that the data were variable Bachmeyer, M. H., Piazza, C. C., Fredrick, L. D., Reed,
during many of the phases. The caregiver presented G. K., Rivas, K. D., & Kadey, H. J. (2009). Functional
four target foods in each session, and it is possible analysis and treatment of multiply controlled inappro-
priate mealtime behavior. Journal of Applied Behavior
that this variability was due to the child’s relative Analysis, 42, 641–658. doi: 10.1901/jaba.2009.42-641
preferences for individual target foods. However, Carruth, B. R., & Skinner, J. D. (2002). Feeding behaviors
we did not conduct a preference assessment of and other motor development in healthy children (2-24
foods for children in Study 1, and we did not months). Journal of the American College of Nutrition,
21, 88–96. doi: 10.1080/07315724.2002.10719199
examine responding to individual foods. This Carruth, B. R., Ziegler, P. J., Gordon, A., & Hendricks, K.
could be a topic for future investigations. (2004). Developmental milestones and self-feeding
SELF-FEEDING 721

behaviors in infants and toddlers. Journal of the sumption in a child with chronic food refusal. Behavior
American Dietetic Association, 104, 51–56. doi: Modification, 24, 348–358. doi: 10.1177/
10.1016/j.jada.2003.10.019 0145445500243003
Collins, B. C., Gast, D. L., Wolery, M., Holcombe, A., & Miller, L. K. (1968). Escape from an effortful situation.
Leatherby, J. G. (1991). Using constant time delay to Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 11,
teach self-feeding to young students with severe/ 619–627. doi: 10.1901/jeab.1968.11-619
profound handicaps: Evidence of limited effectiveness. Patel, M. R., Piazza, C. C., Layer, S. A., Coleman, R., &
Journal of Developmental and Physical Disabilities, 3, Swartzwelder, D. M. (2005). A systematic evaluation of
157–179. doi: 10.1007/BF01045931 food textures to decrease packing and increase oral
Cooper, L. J., Wacker, D. P., Brown, K., McComas, J. J., Peck, intake in children with pediatric feeding disorders.
S. M., Drew, J., … Kayser, K. (1999). Use of a concurrent Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 38, 89–100. doi:
operants paradigm to evaluate positive reinforcers during 10.1901/jaba.2005.161-02
treatment food refusal. Behavior Modification, 23, 3–40. Perone, M., & Baron, A. (1980). Reinforcement of human
doi: 10.1177/0145445599231001 observing behavior by a stimulus correlated with
Fisher, W., Piazza, C. C., Bowman, L. G., Hagopian, L. P., extinction or increased effort. Journal of the Experimen-
Owens, J. C., & Slevin, I. (1992). A comparison of two tal Analysis of Behavior, 34, 239–261. doi: 10.1901/
approaches for identifying reinforcers for persons with jeab.1980.34-239
severe and profound disabilities. Journal of Applied Piazza, C. C., Anderson, C., & Fisher, W. (1993). Teaching
Behavior Analysis, 25, 491–498. doi: 10.1901/ self-feeding skills to patients with Rett syndrome.
jaba.1992.25-491 Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 35, 991–
Glover, A. C., Roane, H. S., Kadey, H. J., & Grow, L. L. 996. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8749.1993.tb11581.x
(2008). Preference for reinforcers under progressive- Piazza, C. C., Patel, M. R., Gulotta, C. S., Sevin, B. M., &
and fixed-ratio schedules: A comparison of single and Layer, S. A. (2003). On the relative contributions of
concurrent arrangements. Journal of Applied Behavior positive reinforcement and escape extinction in the
Analysis, 41, 163–176. doi: 10.1901/jaba.2008.41-163 treatment of food refusal. Journal of Applied Behavior
Hanley, G. P., Piazza, C. C., Keeney, K. M., Blakeley-Smith, Analysis, 36, 309–324. doi: 10.1901/jaba.2003.36-309
A. B., & Worsdell, A. S. (1998). Effects of wrist weights Reed, G. K., Piazza, C. C., Patel, M. R., Layer, S. A.,
on self-injurious and adaptive behaviors. Journal of Bachmeyer, M. H., Bethke, S. D., & Gutshall, K. A.
Applied Behavior Analysis, 31, 307–310. doi: 10.1901/ (2004). On the relative contributions of noncontingent
jaba.1998.31-307 reinforcement and escape extinction in the treatment of
Hoch, T. A., Babbitt, R. L., Coe, D. A., Krell, D. M., & food refusal. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 37,
Hackbert, L. (1994). Contingency contacting: Com- 27–42. doi.10.1901/jaba.2004.37-27
bining positive reinforcement and escape extinction Richman, J. S., Sonderby, T., & Kahn, J. V. (1980).
procedures to treat persistent food refusal. Behavior Prerequisite vs. in vivo acquisition of self-feeding skill.
Modification, 18, 106–128. doi: 10.1177/ Behaviour Research and Therapy, 18, 327–332. doi:
01454455940181007 10.1016/0005-7967(80)90091-1
Kerwin, M. E., Ahearn, W. H., Eicher, P. S., & Burd, D. M. Schulze, P. A., Harwood, R. L., & Schoelmerich, A. (2001).
(1995). The costs of eating: A behavioral economic Feeding practices and expectations among middle-class
analysis of food refusal. Journal of Applied Behavior Anglo and Puerto Rican mothers of 12-month-old
Analysis, 28, 245–260. doi: 10.1901/jaba.1995.28-245 infants. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 32, 397–
LaRue, R. H., Stewart, V., Piazza, C. C., Volkert, V. M., 406. doi: 10.1177/0022022101032004002
Patel, M. R., & Zeleny, J. (2011). Escape as Shore, B. A., Iwata, B. A., DeLeon, I. G., Kahng, S., & Smith,
reinforcement and escape extinction in the treatment R. G. (1997). An analysis of reinforcer substitutability
of feeding problems. Journal of Applied Behavior using object manipulation and self-injury as competing
Analysis, 44, 719–735. doi: 10.1901/jaba.2011.44-719 responses. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 30, 21–41.
Leibowitz, J. M., & Holcer, P. (1974). Building and doi: 10.1901/jaba.1997.30-21
maintaining self-feeding skills in a retarded child. Sisson, L. A., & Dixon, M. J. (1986). A behavioral approach
American Journal of Occupational Therapy, 28, 545– to the training and assessment of feeding skills in
548. multihandicapped children. Applied Research in Mental
Luiselli, J. K. (1991). Acquisition of self-feeding in a child Retardation, 7, 149–163. doi: 10.1016/0270-3092(86)
with Lowe’s syndrome. Journal of Developmental and 90002-0
Physical Disabilities, 3, 181–189. doi: 10.1007/ Stark, L. J., Bowen, A. M., Tyc, V. L., Evans, S., & Passero,
BF01045932 M. A. (1990). A behavioral approach to increasing
Luiselli, J. K. (1993). Training self-feeding skills in children calorie consumption in children with cystic fibrosis.
who are deaf and blind. Behavior Modification, 17, 457– Journal of Pediatric Psychology, 15, 309–326. doi:
473. doi: 10.1177/01454455930174003 10.1093/jpepsy/15.3.309
Luiselli, J. K. (2000). Cueing, demand fading, and positive Stark, L. J., Knapp, L. G., Bowen, A. M., Powers, S. W.,
reinforcement to establish self-feeding and oral con- Jelalian, E., Evans, S., … Hovell, M. (1993). Increasing
722 KRISTI M. RIVAS et al.

calorie consumption in children with cystic fibrosis: the treatment of children with persistent feeding
Replication with 2-year follow-up. Journal of Applied difficulties. Behavior Change, 11, 242–258.
Behavior Analysis, 26, 435–450. doi: 10.1901/ Van Houten, R. (1993). The use of wrist weights to reduce
jaba.1993.26-435 self-injury maintained by sensory reinforcement.
Stimbert, V. E., Minor, J. W., & McCoy, J. F. (1977). Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 26, 197–203.
Intensive feeding training with retarded children. doi: 10.1901/jaba.1993.26-197
Behavior Modification, 1, 517–530. doi: 10.1177/ Vaz, P. C. M., Volkert, V. M., & Piazza, C. C. (2011). Using
014544557714005 negative reinforcement to increase self-feeding in a child
Thompson, R. J., Jr. (1977). Applied behavior analysis in the with food selectivity. Journal of Applied Behavior
treatment of mealtime tantrums and delay in self- Analysis, 44, 915–920. doi: 10.1901/jaba.2011.44-915
feeding in a multi-handicapped child. Journal of Clinical
Child and Adolescent Psychology, 6, 52–54. doi:
10.1080/15374417709532784 Received December 15, 2011
Turner, K. M. T., Sanders, M. R., & Wall, C. R. (1994). Final acceptance May 5, 2014
Behavioural parent training versus dietary education in Action Editor, MaryLou Kerwin
Copyright of Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis is the property of Wiley-Blackwell and its
content may not be copied or emailed to multiple sites or posted to a listserv without the
copyright holder's express written permission. However, users may print, download, or email
articles for individual use.

You might also like