Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 12

585727

research-article2015
PSPXXX10.1177/0146167215585727Personality and Social Psychology BulletinSczesny et al.

Article

Personality and Social

Beyond Sexist Beliefs: How Do People


Psychology Bulletin
2015, Vol. 41(7) 943­–954
© 2015 by the Society for Personality
Decide to Use Gender-Inclusive and Social Psychology, Inc
Reprints and permissions:

Language? sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.nav
DOI: 10.1177/0146167215585727
pspb.sagepub.com

Sabine Sczesny1, Franziska Moser2, and Wendy Wood3

Abstract
When people use generic masculine language instead of more gender-inclusive forms, they communicate gender stereotypes
and sometimes exclusion of women from certain social roles. Past research related gender-inclusive language use to sexist
beliefs and attitudes. Given that this aspect of language use may be transparent to users, it is unclear whether people explicitly
act on these beliefs when using gender-exclusive language forms or whether these are more implicit, habitual patterns. In two
studies with German-speaking participants, we showed that spontaneous use of gender-inclusive personal nouns is guided
by explicitly favorable intentions as well as habitual processes involving past use of such language. Further indicating the joint
influence of deliberate and habitual processes, Study 2 revealed that language-use intentions are embedded in explicit sexist
ideologies. As anticipated in our decision-making model, the effects of sexist beliefs on language emerged through deliberate
mechanisms involving attitudes and intentions.

Keywords
gender stereotypes, language production, grammatical gender, gender-inclusive language, sexism

Received August 21, 2014; revision accepted April 12, 2015

The last half decade saw a cultural shift in the gender termi- social categorizations and hierarchies. In this way, it contrib-
nology in language use in many nations. For example, the utes to the construction and communication of gender (Maass
use of generic masculine pronouns such as he, him, and his & Arcuri, 1996). For instance, reading a personal noun, such
in English-language books decreased greatly across the 20th as engineer, makes readers think of a male rather than a
century (Twenge, Campbell, & Gentile, 2012). Similarly, female person, whereas kindergarten teacher evokes expec-
between the 1960s and 1990s, spoken language in Australian tations of a woman rather than a man (e.g., for Spanish:
radio programs and parliamentary debates decreased in the Carreiras, Garnham, Oakhill, & Cain, 1996; German: Irmen,
frequency of use of such pronouns and increased slightly in 2007; Italian: Cacciari & Padovani, 2007).
use of he or she and more markedly in the frequency of use Along with demonstrating the deleterious effects of sexist
of the singular they (Pauwels, 2003). In part, the switch to language, research has linked gender-exclusive language
gender-inclusive language comes from the prescriptive lan- with sexist beliefs and attitudes (Swim, Mallett, & Stangor,
guage requirements of scientific and other professional asso- 2004). Yet, existing accounts do not specify the psychologi-
ciations, along with government mandates, which cal mechanisms by which sexist beliefs/attitudes influence
increasingly specify that writing avoids sexist terminology language use, and several different mechanisms could be at
and instead uses gender-inclusive language terms (e.g., work. People may make explicit choices about how to repre-
American Psychological Association, 2009; UNESCO, sent women and men in forms of speech, and it is possible
1999). Outside of these formal requirements, however, peo- that use of gender-inclusive language stems mostly from
ple have a choice about whether to spontaneously use gen-
der-inclusive language in daily life. 1
University of Bern, Switzerland
Understanding the psychological mechanisms behind 2
University of Applied Sciences and Arts Northwestern Switzerland
gender-inclusive language use is important because interper- 3
University of Southern California, Los Angeles, USA
sonal communication contributes to gender stereotyping via
Corresponding Author:
written words, spoken utterances, and the mass media. As a
Sabine Sczesny, Department of Psychology, University of Bern,
tool of social practice, language functions as a device not Fabrikstrasse 8, 3012 Bern, Switzerland.
only for transferring information but also for expressing Email: sabine.sczesny@psy.unibe.ch

Downloaded from psp.sagepub.com at UNIV OF HARTFORD on July 13, 2015


944 Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 41(7)

such deliberate decisions. However, it is also possible that more traditional gender-role beliefs) interpreted a social tar-
less deliberate mechanisms such as habits formed from past get’s personality as more masculine in response to occupa-
language use influence gendered expressions, even if those tional titles with a man-suffix (e.g., chairman) than in
expressions are not entirely consistent with actors’ deliberate response to a suffixless term (e.g., chair) and as less mascu-
intentions. Either or both of these mechanisms could be line in reaction to occupational titles with a person-suffix
responsible for carrying the effects of sexist beliefs to lan- (e.g., chairperson). In these ways, the use of gendered pro-
guage behavior. The current research examines these poten- nouns and nouns conveys meaning about the persons
tial deliberate and habitual mechanisms and identifies involved and their likely dispositions.
components of these mechanisms that help to explain previ-
ous relations between sexist beliefs and attitudes and (lack
of) use of gender-inclusive language.
Use of Gender-Inclusive Language
The present research first tested a model of spontaneous Research to date on the determinants of gender-inclusive lan-
language use that includes habitual mechanisms that arise guage use has emphasized the gender-related belief systems
from past repetition as well as deliberate mechanisms of that can lead to people to adopt certain language forms. For
intentional choice. Second, we tested the extent to which example, male and female college students possessing more
these habitual and deliberate psychological mechanisms are favorable attitudes toward gender equality also expressed
responsible for the effects of sexist ideologies on gender- greater favorability toward using language terms such as
inclusive language choices. “flight attendant” instead of “stewardess” (Douglas & Sutton,
2014; Parks & Roberton, 2004, 2008; Sarrasin, Gabriel, &
Gygax, 2012).
Consequences of Gender-Exclusive In other research, gender ideologies proved to guide
Language Use actual language use. For example, Rubin, Greene, and
The generic use of masculine forms prevailing in many lan- Schneider’s (1994) participants wrote letters in response to a
guages has far-reaching consequences in restricting the fictional university proposal for mandatory drug testing, and
degree of female visibility. Gender-exclusive language has those possessing a stronger masculine gender-role orientation
these broad, powerful effects in part because it is a relatively were more likely to use gender-exclusive language. Other
subtle means of conveying information about women and research has linked sexist attitudes to language use. In illus-
men. For example, female job applicants perceived a lack of tration, Jacobson and Insko’s (1985) participants completed
fit between themselves and potential position openings when sentences by choosing among pronouns and nouns (e.g., he,
job advertisements (e.g., Bem & Bem, 1973) and job inter- she, he/she, the lawyer, the client). Participants with stronger
views (Stout & Dasgupta, 2011) used masculine forms (e.g., sexist attitudes chose non-sexist pronouns less frequently
English: he/masc.; German: Lehrer/teacher, masc.) rather than participants with less sexist attitudes. In another demon-
than gender-inclusive expressions (e.g., English: he and she/ stration, Swim et al.’s (2004) participants described how they
masc. and fem.; German: Lehrer oder Lehrerin/teacher, masc. would act as the main character in three scenarios involving a
or teacher, fem.). Also, in public opinion polls (Stahlberg & nurse, a business executive, and a professor, and those who
Sczesny, 2001) and court decisions (Hamilton, Hunter, & endorsed modern sexist beliefs used more sexist pronouns to
Stuart-Smith, 1992), masculine formulations may bias out- refer to the persons in these stories. Given that men score
comes in favor of men to the disadvantage of women. higher on instrumentality and on sexist attitudes, men also
Furthermore, Italian language descriptions of occupations have been found to use more masculine generic pronouns
(e.g., lawyer) with feminized terms implied lower compe- than do women (Pauwels, 2003; Rubin et al., 1994).
tence than gender-inclusive or masculine terms (Merkel, Taken together, this research indicates that individual dif-
Maass, & Frommelt, 2012). Some researchers have argued ferences in gendered belief systems, including gender-role
that the use of gender-exclusive language (e.g., using he to identity and the endorsement of modern sexist beliefs, are
indicate he or she) represents a kind of group-based ostracism associated with the use of gender-inclusive language. In fact,
(Stout & Dasgupta, 2011). Specifically, in reaction to gender- sexist language has been considered an example of subtle
exclusive language (he) during a mock job interview, women sexism (Swim et al., 2004) and is known to contribute to
experienced a lower sense of belonging, less motivation, and gender stereotyping (e.g., Maass & Arcuri, 1996).
less expected identification than women exposed to gender-
inclusive (he or she) or gender-neutral (one) language. Overview of Present Research and
Due to the close link between language and cognitive rep-
resentations, language use activates associated cognitive
Hypotheses
concepts and schemata and may thereby perpetuate stereo- The present research investigated how people deliberately or
typical thinking and expectations (cf. Stahlberg, Braun, habitually act on their beliefs about gender so as to use or not
Irmen, & Sczesny, 2007). For instance, McConnell and Fazio use gender-inclusive language. We focused on the use of per-
(1996) found that perceivers (especially those who reported sonal nouns, which is a central issue in debates about language

Downloaded from psp.sagepub.com at UNIV OF HARTFORD on July 13, 2015


Sczesny et al. 945

and gender (e.g., chairman vs. chairperson vs. chair). Fishbein and Ajzen’s (2011) reasoning, these belief systems
Typically, forms of personal nouns are considered gender- should contribute to people’s intentions to use gender-inclu-
inclusive when they prompt a balanced representation of sive language because they influence specific beliefs about
men and women. the outcomes of using such language terms. In addition, broad
Suggesting that people carry out their gender ideologies by social ideologies might influence the habits people form
making deliberate decisions to use gender-inclusive language, because they influence the behaviors that people repeat in
Cralley and Ruscher (2005) found that non-sexist men used stable contexts. Thus, we anticipated that these sexist beliefs
gender-inclusive language primarily when they were not cog- would indirectly influence gender-inclusive language use by
nitively busy with another task. Thus, such language use affecting attitudes, norms, and perceived behavioral control,
appeared to require explicit, intentional decision making. which, in turn, would influence intentions and habits (i.e., fre-
Following Fishbein and Ajzen’s (2011) classic model of quency of past behavior and experienced automaticity).
action control, people deliberately form intentions to use gen-
der-inclusive language when they (a) hold favorable attitudes Study 1
toward the behavior, (b) perceive supportive subjective
norms, in which significant others favor this behavior, and (c) Method
perceive high behavioral control or ease in performing the
Participants. All participants were German native speakers
behavior. Gendered belief systems, including sexist beliefs,
from Switzerland and Germany, either living in Switzerland
represent broad background factors that contribute to the atti-
or Germany. Both samples were recruited via mailing lists,
tudes and social norms that guide behavioral intentions.
and the study was conducted online.
Action itself is then the product of favorable intentions along
At Time 1, the questionnaire was finished by 438 Swiss
with high perceived control over the behavior.
and German participants (1,083 opened the study’s website;
Language use also may be guided by less deliberate
575 started the questionnaire). Altogether, 438 participants
mechanisms, perhaps activated habitually by environmental
finished the questionnaire at Time 1. At Time 2 (2 weeks
cues (Ouellette & Wood, 1998; Verplanken & Aarts 1999).
later), all participants who had finished the first question-
Habits form as people repeatedly perform the same behavior
naire were provided with the link to answer the second ques-
and learn associations between the behavior and recurring
tionnaire. At Time 2, 346 participants answered the survey.
features of the context, including physical location, time of
From this sample, 45 participants were excluded because
day, and preceding actions in a sequence. In Triandis’s (1977)
their responses could not be tied to their Time 1 scores; 14
terms, they are situation-specific learned sequences of acts
because they either were non-native German speakers, do
that have become automatic and, therefore, require little not live in Switzerland/Germany, or passed their school years
mental effort (see Gardner & Tang, 2014; Wood & Neal, in a non-German speaking country; and 9 participants
2007). Habitual forms of speech could develop through imi- because they had identified the aim of the study.
tation and shared social norms or could initially be practiced The final sample consisted of 278 participants (206
deliberately until they become habitual. Regardless of their females, 72 males; age range = 15-60 years; M = 29.99; SD
origins, once habits have formed, they tend to be brought to = 9.59; men: M = 32.29, SD = 10.35; women: M = 29.18,
mind automatically and to be performed with minimal input SD = 9.20). The sample consisted of 46% employed per-
from intentions and attitudes (Neal, Wood, Labrecque, & sons, 49% students, and 5% unreported occupation. The
Lally, 2012; Neal, Wood, Wu, & Kurlander, 2011). response rate was 27% (278 of 1,083 who opened the
In two studies,1 we evaluated the role of deliberate and website).
habitual factors in predicting the use of gender-inclusive lan-
guage. Study 1 was designed to explore the deliberate and Gender-inclusive language use. To measure the use of gen-
more habitual mechanisms contributing to language use. We der-inclusive language, we used a fill-in-the-blanks task:
expected that favorable attitudes, supportive subjective Participants received 10 short texts in which some nouns
norms, and a higher degree of perceived behavioral control were represented only by the initial letter. Empty lines were
would predict more favorable intentions and stronger habits, provided in the margin to write down the missing words.
which, in turn, would increase use of gender-inclusive lan- This method was chosen to avoid inferences about the length
guage. This proposed mediational model advances theory by of the missing words. Participants were first provided with a
providing novel insight into the psychological pathways by practice text with already filled-in blanks and then with the
which people spontaneously choose language terms. 10 texts. One example of such a text reads as follows:
Study 2 addresses the broader issue of how people’s gen-
der ideologies guide their language use through the deliber- In Switzerland those c. . . . a. . . . designated as entitled to vote
ate and habitual mechanisms identified in Study 1. We who possess certain political rights. In most cases suffrage
evaluated these ideas by including scales assessing a variety coincides with the right to vote. Citizens of age who are at least
of sexist ideologies (i.e., modern sexism, Swim, Aikin, Hall, 18 years old and are Swiss nationals a. . . . entitled to vote. The
& Hunter, 1995; neosexism, Tougas, Brown, Beaton, & Joly, voting papers are sent via p. . . . to the respective households.
1995; ambivalent sexism, Glick & Fiske, 1996). Following (Solution: citizens, are, are, post)

Downloaded from psp.sagepub.com at UNIV OF HARTFORD on July 13, 2015


946 Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 41(7)

In each text, one of the four blanks required a personal Descriptive and injunctive norms were measured by five
noun, which could be used either in the masculine form items (adopted from Smith et al., 2008), and the factor analy-
(Wähler/voters, masc., Bürger/citizens, masc., Schüler/stu- sis did not allow us to discriminate between these. It revealed
dents, masc., Redakteure/editors, masc., Sportler/athletes, one positive and one negative factor including ratings of
masc. Mitarbeiter/colleagues, masc., Teilnehmer/partici- “Most of the persons who are important to me” (a) “use gen-
pants, masc., Feuerwehrmänner/firemen, masc., Freunde/ der-inclusive language,” (b) “endorse gender-inclusive lan-
friends, masc., Angestellte/employees, masc.) or in a gender- guage,” (c) “disapprove of gender-inclusive language
inclusive form (such as Wähler und Wählerinnen /voters, (reverse coded),” (d) “prefer that I use gender-inclusive lan-
masc. and voters, fem.). guage,” and (e) “disapprove of me using gender-inclusive
To avoid contextual influences, the scenario contexts language (reverse coded).” These were combined into one
were neutral regarding gender-related information and were scale that captured positive norms (α = .88; three items; items
equally typical for women and men (data on distributions of a, b, and d) and another that captured negative norms
women and men in the respective roles were taken from (α = .69; two items; items c and e).
www.bfs.admin.ch). The three other gaps in each text served Perceived behavioral control was measured with seven
as distracters and required different word classes (e.g., verbs, items (adopted from Ajzen & Madden, 1986; Smith et al.,
adjectives). 2008) tapping whether gender-inclusive language (a) “is dif-
We coded participants’ answers as representing either ficult for me to use,” (b) “is complicated to use,” (c) ”is com-
gender-inclusive language (i.e., feminine-masculine word- plicated and impairs writing and speaking for me,” (d) “is
pairs, neutralization) or gender-exclusive language (i.e., something I can’t use under time pressure,” (e) “would be
masculine forms). These codes were based on published easier to use if there were information about the benefits of
guidelines for gender-fair language (e.g., Braun, 2000; its use,” (f) “would be easier for me to use if there were easy
Schweizerische Bundeskanzlei, 2009). Thus, the use of gen- rules and spelling,” and (g) “would be difficult for me if it
der-inclusive language across all texts varied from 0 to 10 were an obligation.” After recoding, the items were compiled
gender-inclusive nouns. into a composite score in which higher numbers reflect
greater control (α = .79). One additional item (i.e., “I can use
Deliberate predictors of language use. To introduce the gender-inclusive language if I want to do so”) was not
attitude object of gender-inclusive language, different pos- included because it did not load substantially (>.30) on the
sibilities of referring to persons in German were described, factors.
such as masculine generics (e.g., die Lehrer/teachers, masc.), Intention to use gender-inclusive language was assessed
masculine-feminine word-pairs (e.g., Lehrer und Lehrerin- with three items: “Gender-inclusive language is something
nen/teachers, masc. and teachers, fem.), or neutral forms that” (a) “I would use in the future,” (b) “I would use in the
(e.g., person). Deliberative predictors, namely, attitudes, future in written language,” and (c) “I would use orally in the
norms, and perceived control, were assessed with 22 ran- future.” Reliability was good (α = .89), and these items were
domly ordered items.2 All items were answered on rating compiled into a single score.
scales ranging from 1 (strongly disagree) to 7 (strongly
agree). A factor analysis (principal axis) using direct obli- Habitual predictors of language use. Frequency of past
min rotation (see Fabrigar, Wegener, MacCallum, & Strahan, behavior was assessed with the following two items: (a) “I
1999) revealed that all items loaded on one of the four pre- always used gender-inclusive language in the past months”
dicted factors (i.e., >.30 on one of the factors; see supple- and (b) “I never used it in the past months (reverse coded).”
mental material). Experienced automaticity was assessed by adapting 10
Positive attitudes toward gender-inclusive language were of the original 12 items from the self-reported habit index
measured by five items (adopted from earlier studies, e.g., (Verplanken & Orbell, 2003; see also Gardner, de Bruijn,
Knussen, Yule, MacKenzie, & Wells, 2004): (a) “It is person- & Lally, 2011, for a review and meta-analysis of applica-
ally important to me to use gender-inclusive language,” (b) tions of the self-report habit index). Participants indicated
“I don’t like gender-inclusive language,” (c) “My feelings whether “Gender-inclusive language is something that” (a)
toward gender-inclusive language are positive,” (d) “I am “I use automatically,” (b) “I do without thinking,” (c) “would
interested in gender-inclusive language,” and (e) “Gender- require effort not to do,” (d) “belongs to my routine,” (e) “I
inclusive language is an issue of personal importance for start doing before I realize I’m doing it,” (f) “I would find it
me.” Four additional items assessed if gender-inclusive lan- hard not to do,” (g) “I have no need to think about doing,”
guage was something that (f) “means to not discriminate and (h) “is typically me.”
against women,” (g) “does not convey any advantages A factor analysis (principal axis) with direct oblimin rota-
(reverse coded),” (h) “produces more gender equality.” After tion on the 12 habit items confirmed separate factors for fre-
removing one item (i.e., “makes text cumbersome and quency of past behavior and for experienced automaticity
impairs readability”) that did not load unambiguously on the (all items loaded >.30 on one of the two factors; see supple-
factors, the remaining eight items were reliable (α = .92) and mental material). Two items (i) “I do frequently” and (j) “I
were combined into a single scale. have been doing for a long time” were excluded because they

Downloaded from psp.sagepub.com at UNIV OF HARTFORD on July 13, 2015


Sczesny et al. 947

Table 1. Study 1: Means, Standard Deviations, Correlations of Predictors and Use of Gender-Inclusive Language (N = 278).

Variable M SD 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8
1. Positive attitudes 3.63 1.60 — .56** −.20** .28** .58** .67** .76** .37**
2. Positive norms 3.90 1.54 — −.17** .19** .43** .51** .53** .25**
3. Negative norms 2.31 1.24 — −.13* −.22** −.14* −.23** −.12*
4. Perceived behavioral control 4.48 1.24 — .37** .39** .32** .09
5. Frequency of past behavior 4.75 1.80 — .62** .73** .40**
6. Experienced automaticity 3.23 1.65 — .69** .33**
7. Intention 4.35 1.76 — .41**
8. Gender-inclusive language use 4.55 2.93 —

Note. Scales ranging from 1 = strongly disagree to 7 = strongly agree; higher scores on scales indicate more approval of the respective statement; use of
gender-inclusive language varied from 0 to 10 gender-inclusive personal nouns.
*p < .05. **p < .01.

did not load unambiguously on one factor. Thus, the 2 items On average, participants used gender-inclusive language
assessing frequency of past behavior had sufficient reliabil- forms in about 4 of the 10 texts. No gender-inclusive forms
ity (α = .72) and were combined into a composite score. were used by 3% of the respondents, 11.2% used gender-
Also, the 8 experienced automaticity items showed good inclusive forms once, 20.5% twice, 13.3% three times, 8.3%
reliability (α = .95), and scores were compiled into a four times, 7.2% five times, 4.3% six times, 7.2% seven
composite. times, 11.9% eight times, 10.1% nine times, and 3.2% used
gender-inclusive forms in all 10 blanks.
Procedure. Study 1 was introduced as a “Survey on Attitudes
Toward Social Communication.” Participants completed two Predicting gender-inclusive language use. We tested mediation
questionnaires at two time points, separated by a 2-week effects as ways to account for the relations between attitudes,
interval. Participants first indicated a personal code at the norms, and perceived behavioral control with gender-inclu-
beginning of each survey. sive language use. To document which deliberate and habitual
At Time 1, an explanation of gender-inclusive language factors—intention, frequency of past behavior, and experi-
was given (see above). Then all predictors, intention, and enced automaticity—were responsible for carrying effects of
socio-demographic data were assessed. attitudes, norms, and perceived behavioral control to gender-
Time 2 started with a distracter task in which participants inclusive language use, we conducted a mediational analysis
completed a questionnaire regarding attitudes, norms, behav- by means of structural equation modeling (SEM), which
ioral control, and so on of social media use (24 items adapted allowed us to control for potential biases in the estimates due
from the questionnaire on gender-inclusive language use; see to measurement error (Kenny, Kashy, & Bolger, 1998). For
Time 1). Then participants worked on the fill-in-the-blanks the computations, we used Amos 22 (Arbuckle, 2013). The
task to assess their gender-inclusive language use. At the end model was exactly identified (df = 0) and thus had a perfect fit
of Time 2, participants had the possibility to win a voucher (chi-square = 0). Estimations were based on the covariance
and were thanked. Debriefing was sent to all participants matrix and the maximum likelihood method.
when the survey was completed. We predicted the extent of gender-inclusive language at
Time 2 from the predictors—attitudes, norms, perceived
behavioral control—and the mediators—intention, fre-
Results quency of past behavior, experienced automaticity—assessed
Descriptive results. The means and standard deviations of all at Time 1. The results of the mediational analysis—standard-
variables in Study 1 and their correlations are displayed in ized values of the coefficients—are displayed in Figure 1.
Table 1. We did not find any significant gender differences First, gender-inclusive language use was significantly
in these variables (seven t tests for independent samples, predicted by frequency of past behavior (p = .012) and mar-
Bonferroni corrected alpha). ginally by intentions (p = .067). Second, we tested for the
On average, participants reported moderately positive significance of mediation using the Sobel Test (Baron &
attitudes toward the use of gender-inclusive language along Kenny, 1986). The results revealed the following indirect
with moderately positive norms, perceived control, fre- effects on gender-inclusive language use: Attitudes influ-
quency of past use, experienced automaticity, and favorable enced gender-inclusive language use through the frequency
intentions. Participants reported low negative norms regard- of past behavior (p = .017) and marginally through intentions
ing gender-inclusive language. (p = .069). The effects of positive norms on gender-inclusive

Downloaded from psp.sagepub.com at UNIV OF HARTFORD on July 13, 2015


948 Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 41(7)

Positive
Attitudes .64
.14 Intention
-.07

.11
Positive .43
Norms .18
.13 Frequency of Gender-
.21
-.08 Past Behavior Inclusive
.21 Language Use

Negative .51
Norms
.19
Experienced
.22 Automaticity

Perceived
Behavioral
Control

Figure 1. Study 1: Mediation model for gender-inclusive language use.


Note. Significant paths are indicated as lines (p < .05), marginally significant paths in broken lines (p < .10).

language use were marginally mediated by frequency of past That is, to what extent do people draw on sexist/non-sexist
behavior (p = .085). Finally, the effects of perceived behav- beliefs to make deliberate decisions about gender-inclusive
ioral control on gender-inclusive language use were signifi- language use and to what extent do these beliefs work
cantly mediated by frequency of past behavior (p = .029). through a more habitual process?

Discussion Study 2
Study 1 revealed that gender-inclusive language is a product Study 2 was designed to replicate our model and evaluate
of both deliberate and habitual factors. Our design separated how the psychological determinants of gender-inclusive lan-
by 2 weeks the assessments of these psychological predictors guage emerge from a range of subtle sexism measures, includ-
from actual language use, allowing us to draw especially clear ing modern sexism, neosexism, and ambivalent sexism. These
conclusions about the ways that these factors guide language measures were designed to reflect the complexities of wom-
choice. As anticipated, participants were more likely to use en’s contemporary roles: Modern sexism comprises beliefs in
gender-inclusive language at the 2-week follow-up session which individuals disavow women’s present-day discrimina-
when they had used it frequently in the past and thus had tion, reject their demands for political and economic power,
formed language-use habits. They also used gender-inclusive and disapprove of policies designed to promote gender equal-
nouns (marginally) more often when they had expressed ity (Swim et al., 1995). Neosexism describes the conflict
explicit intentions to do so. Surprisingly, experienced auto- between egalitarian values and negative feelings toward
maticity, which reflects perceptions of how mindlessly par- women, including the belief that it is important to maintain the
ticipants use gender-inclusive language, was not a predictor status-quo in women’s and men’s roles (Tougas et al., 1995).
of language use, but instead, the effects were largely sub- Ambivalent sexism toward women addresses the coexistence
sumed by the other deliberate and habitual predictors. Thus, of positive and negative feelings that reflects ambivalence
this language form had multiple determinants, reflecting that (Glick & Fiske, 1996). Besides hostile sexism toward women,
participants used gender-inclusive language partially in a benevolent sexism is defined as “a set of interrelated attitudes
mindless way and partially by making deliberate decisions. toward women that are sexist in terms of viewing women ste-
Given the broadly deleterious effects of gender-exclusive lan- reotypically and in restricted roles, but that are subjectively
guage that we identified in the introduction to this article, positive in feeling tone” (Glick & Fiske, 1996, p. 491). All
such deliberate decisions might seem surprising. sexism measures are well established, reliable, and valid, and
To understand the broader motivations behind language they all correlate with traditional measures of attitudes toward
use, Study 2 evaluated the extent to which sexist ideological women’s rights and responsibilities, but each captures unique
beliefs influence language and additionally tracked whether aspects above and beyond traditional measures of overt sex-
these effects implicate habitual or intentional mechanisms. ism (see Kite, 2001, for an overview).

Downloaded from psp.sagepub.com at UNIV OF HARTFORD on July 13, 2015


Sczesny et al. 949

Method Table 2. We did not find any significant gender differences


in any of these variables (10 t tests for independent samples,
Participants. Study 2 was also conducted online with partici- Bonferroni corrected alpha).
pants who were native German speakers from Switzerland and On average, participants reported moderately positive atti-
Germany, recruited via mailing lists. At Time 1, 274 partici- tudes toward the use of gender-inclusive language, positive
pants completed the questionnaire (554 opened the site; 407 norms, moderately high perceived control, frequent past
started the questionnaire). All participants who had finished the behavior, high experienced automaticity, and favorable inten-
first questionnaire received the link to answer the second ques- tions. Participants also reported relatively low endorsement
tionnaire (Time 2, 2 weeks later; for details see Study 1). At of negative norms regarding gender-inclusive language.
Time 2, 241 participants completed the questionnaire. From Participants used gender-inclusive language forms in
this sample, we excluded 29 participants because we could not about 4 of the 10 texts on average. Specifically, 10.3% of the
link their data with Time 1 data; 7 participants because they respondents did not use any gender-inclusive forms, 25.1%
were non-native German speakers, did not live in Switzerland/ used gender-inclusive forms once, 16.3% twice, 10.8% three
Germany or passed their school years in a non-German speak- times, 9.4% four times, 4.4% five times, 2% six times, 4.9%
ing country; and 2 participants because they identified the aim seven times, 5.4% eight times, 8.9% nine times, and 2.5%
of the study. The final sample consisted of 203 participants used gender-inclusive forms for all 10 blanks (N = 203).
(141 females, 62 males; age range = 18-54 years; M = 25.00;
SD = 6.91; men: M = 24.82, SD = 5.95; women: M = 25.08, SD Predicting gender-inclusive language use. We again used
= 7.31). Of these, 69% were students, 27% employed persons, SEM (see Study 1) to predict the extent of gender-inclusive
and 5% did not indicate occupation. The response rate was language at Time 2 from the predictors—attitudes, norms,
37% (203 of 554 who opened the study’s website). perceived behavioral control—and the mediators—inten-
tion, frequency of past behavior, experienced automaticity—
Gender-inclusive language use. As in Study 1, we assessed assessed at Time 1. The model was exactly identified (df =
gender-inclusive language use with same 10 fill-in-the- 0) and thus had a perfect fit (chi-square = 0). The results of
blanks items at Time 2. the mediational analysis—standardized values of the coef-
ficients—are displayed in Figure 2.
Deliberate and habitual predictors of language use. The First, as hypothesized, language use was significantly
Time 1 questionnaire included 34 randomly ordered items. predicted by intentions (p = .021) and marginally by fre-
All items were answered on rating scales ranging from 1 quency of past behavior (p = .093). In addition, positive atti-
(strongly disagree) to 7 (strongly agree). Two factor analyses tudes directly predicted the gender-inclusive language use
(principal axis) with direct oblimin rotation revealed almost (p = .005). The Sobel tests revealed that effects of attitudes
the same results as in Study 1 (see supplemental material). on gender-inclusive language use were significantly medi-
Thus, we formed scales representing positive attitudes ated by intentions (p = .022). Also, the effects of positive
toward gender-inclusive language (α = .92; 8 items), positive norms on gender-inclusive language use were predicted by
norms (α = .88; 3 items), negative norms (α = .73; 2 items), intentions (p = .043). In this study, frequency of past behav-
perceived behavioral control (α = .80; 7 items), frequency ior was not predicted by the other variables in the model.
of past behavior (3 items; α = .81; in addition to the 2 items
in Study 1 in the present study, the item “I do frequently” The impact of sexist beliefs on gender-inclusive language
was also included in this scale based on the respective factor use. The three sexist belief measures—modern sexism, neo-
analysis), and experienced automaticity (α = .94; 7 items; in sexism, and ambivalent sexism (measured at Time 1)—cor-
contrast to Study 1, 2 items were not included because they related significantly with gender-inclusive language use at
loaded substantially on two factors: “That belongs to my rou- Time 2 (ranging from −.32 to −.43), replicating previous
tine” and “I have been doing for a long time”). work linking sexist beliefs to gender-inclusive language
(Swim et al., 2004).
Sexist beliefs. We measured sexist beliefs at Time 1 with To identify which of the potential mediational variables—
41 items, including a German version by Swim, Becker, and attitudes, norms, perceived behavioral control—were
DeCoster (2014) that included the measurement of modern responsible for carrying the effects of sexist beliefs to inten-
sexism (8 items; α = .82; Swim et al., 1995), neosexism (11 tions, frequency of past behavior, and experienced automa-
items; α = .80; Tougas et al., 1995), and ambivalent sexism ticity, and thus in turn for guiding gender-inclusive language
(11 items to measure hostile and 11 items to measure benevo- use, we conducted another mediational analysis by means of
lent sexism; α = .90; Glick & Fiske, 1996). SEM that included the three sexist beliefs as predictor vari-
ables. The model was again exactly identified (df = 0) and
thus had a perfect fit (chi-square = 0). Estimations were
Results
based on the covariance matrix and the maximum likelihood
Descriptive results. The means and standard deviations of all method. The results of this mediational analysis—standard-
variables in Study 2 and their correlations are displayed in ized values of the coefficients—are displayed in Figure 3.

Downloaded from psp.sagepub.com at UNIV OF HARTFORD on July 13, 2015


950 Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 41(7)

Table 2. Study 2: Means, Standard Deviations, Correlations of Predictors, and Use of Gender-Inclusive Language (N = 203).

Variable M SD 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11
1. Positive attitudes 3.62 1.73 — .57** −.28** .21** .65** .66** −.47** −.45** −.35** .79** .54**
2. Positive norms 3.49 1.52 — −.34** .14* .59** .51** −.23** −.27** −.34** .60** .39**
3. Negative norms 2.70 1.48 — −.20** −.29** −.19** .15* .17* .10 −.28** −.13
4. Perceived behavioral control 4.20 1.28 — .35** .35** −.08 −.10 −.10 .24** .19**
5. Frequency of past behavior 3.92 1.75 — .68** −.33** −.36** −.31** .77** .50**
6. Experienced automaticity 2.74 1.50 — −.23** −.20** −.19** .69** .40**
7. Modern sexism 3.30 1.03 — .64** .39** −.36** −.32**
8. Neosexism 2.37 0.80 — .56** −.38** −.34**
9. Ambivalent sexism 3.10 0.99 — −.32** −.43**
10. Intention 3.87 1.75 — .55**
11. Gender-inclusive language use 3.43 2.99 —

Note. Scales ranging from 1 = strongly disagree to 7 = strongly agree; higher scores on scales indicate more approval of the respective statement; use of
gender-inclusive language varied from 0 to 10 gender-inclusive personal nouns.
*p < .05. **p < .01.

Positive
Attitudes .65
Intention
.21

.07 .27
Positive .26
.43
Norms
.31 Gender-
Frequency of .16
Inclusive
Past Behavior
.21 Language Use

Negative .51
Norms .21

Experienced
.23 Automaticity

Perceived
Behavioral
Control

Figure 2. Study 2: Mediation model for gender-inclusive language use.


Note. Significant paths are indicated as lines (p < .05), marginally significant paths in broken lines (p < .10).

First, the results indicated that only ambivalent sexism of language use mediated by attitudes and intentions—modern
the three sexist beliefs directly predicted gender-inclusive sexism: −.37, CI 95% = [−.67, −.15]; neosexism: −.44, CI
language use (p < .001). Second, to test whether sexist beliefs 95% = [−.81, −.20]; ambivalent sexism: −.26, CI 95% =
indirectly influenced gender-inclusive language use by [−.49, −.11]. In addition, the bootstrapping analysis of the
affecting attitudes, norms, and perceived behavioral control, impact of ambivalent sexism on gender-inclusive language
which in turn influenced intentions and habits, we conducted use was mediated directly by attitudes, −.21, CI 95% =
serial mediation analyses (Hayes, 2013). We used a boot- [−.52, −.01]. The impact of ambivalent sexism on gender-
strapping technique with 5,000 iterations and calculated inclusive language use was mediated by positive norms,
accelerated confidence intervals (CI 95%). which in turn predicted intention, −.25, CI 95% = [−.40,
For all three sexist beliefs, the respective bootstrapping −.15], as well as directly by intention;−.21, CI 95%
analysis revealed an indirect effect on gender-inclusive = [−.52, −.01].

Downloaded from psp.sagepub.com at UNIV OF HARTFORD on July 13, 2015


Sczesny et al. 951

-.30
Positive
-.18 Attitudes
.64
Modern
.21 Intention
Sexism -.13
.07
.21
Positive
-.27 .39 .25
Norms
.31 Frequency of Gender-
Neosexism Past Behavior Inclusive
.21
Language Use

Negative .57
Norms .22

Experienced -.26
Ambivalent
.23 Automaticity
Sexism

Perceived
Behavioral
Control

Figure 3. Study 2: Mediation model for the impact of sexist beliefs on gender-inclusive language use.
Note. The residuals of the mediated variables are correlated but, for the sake of clarity, not indicated. Significant paths are indicated as lines (p < .05),
marginally significant paths in broken lines (p < .10).

Discussion women. In large part, people who hold sexist beliefs make
deliberate decisions to use language that perpetuates gender
Replicating the information-processing findings in Study 1, stereotyping and supports patriarchy. Participants in our
intentions and frequency of past behavior contributed to the research were essentially creating the gendered world that
frequency of gender-inclusive language use. That is, our par- they endorsed in their ideological beliefs.
ticipants used gender-inclusive language to the extent that
they explicitly formed intentions to do so and (marginally) to
the extent that they had repeatedly, habitually done so in the General Discussion
past. Echoing Study 1, the measure of experienced automa-
ticity did not relate to language use. Understanding the use of gender-inclusive language is of
Our central question was the extent to which broader gen- considerable social importance, given the role of reformed
dered ideologies (Jacobson & Insko, 1985; Swim et al., language in reducing gender stereotyping and increasing the
2004) influenced gender-inclusive language use through visibility of women (Stahlberg et al., 2007). Although it is
explicit, deliberate reasoning processes or through the less well documented that gender-inclusive language use is
thoughtful, habitual route. The results clearly targeted delib- linked to sexist beliefs and attitudes (Douglas & Sutton,
erate decision making. That is, participants with stronger 2014; Jacobson & Insko, 1985; Swim et al., 2004), the pres-
sexist beliefs had less favorable attitudes toward using gen- ent research identified the cognitive and motivational pro-
der-inclusive language (and believed that others disapprove cesses by which people use this language in spontaneous
them). Given these attitudes and norms, these participants social communication.
formed less favorable intentions to use gender-inclusive Our studies yielded two clear findings. First, we provided
language. As a result of these deliberate decisions, they used a broad information-processing model outlining the psycho-
such language forms less often on the behavioral measure. logical mechanisms that promote use of gender-inclusive
Thus, gender ideologies directed future speech patterns by language. Some participants in our studies mindlessly used
influencing individual’s explicit decisions. All three sexist sexist language forms as they had in the past. Such habitual
beliefs followed the anticipated sequence in which attitudes language users may be especially likely to rely on whatever
predicted intentions that predicted use. Interestingly, of all is the standard form without considering the possibility of
three sexist beliefs, ambivalent sexism turned out to be the alternative language forms and their implications for social
only ideology that directly predicted language use. change. Other participants used or failed to use gender-inclu-
At a general level, these findings provide an answer to the sive language more deliberately and were acting on their
question of how people continue to use sexist terminology intentions to use or not to use this language form, along with
despite the clear research evidence of deleterious effects for their positive or negative attitudes toward it.

Downloaded from psp.sagepub.com at UNIV OF HARTFORD on July 13, 2015


952 Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 41(7)

A second important finding from this research involves Acknowledgments


insight into the mechanisms by which sexist ideological We thank Aline Amacker and Sara Matthies for their help in con-
beliefs guide language use. These beliefs worked exclusively ducting this research and Friederike Braun, Michèle Kaufmann, and
through deliberate reasoning. That is, participants who Ulrich Orth for their valuable comments on an earlier version of this
endorsed modern sexism, neosexism, or ambivalent sexism manuscript.
failed to use gender-inclusive language because of their neg-
ative attitudes toward using it along with their intentions not Declaration of Conflicting Interests
to use it. Such individuals might deliberately avoid using The author(s) declared no potential conflicts of interest with respect
gender-inclusive language because they viewed it as a kind to the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article.
of oppressive political correctness, or they might claim
(incorrectly, given social science evidence) that is unneces- Funding
sary because the masculine generic includes both women and The author(s) disclosed receipt of the following financial support for
men. Thus, sexist individuals do not fail to use gender-inclu- the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article: This
sive language just because they cannot be bothered to adopt research was conducted within the Marie Curie Initial Training
new terminology differing from standard masculine forms. Network: Language, Cognition, & Gender (ITN LCG) funded by
Instead, they have made explicit choices not to vary from the European Commission’s Seventh Framework Programme
language forms that reflect the male norm and in which (FP7/2007-2013) under Grant Agreement 237907 (www.itn-lcg.eu).
women are relatively invisible.
Across our studies, the baseline use of gender-inclusive Notes
language among our German and Swiss participants was 1. We also conducted a preliminary study providing initial evi-
quite low. The majority of participants still used masculine dence of the deliberate and habitual factors involved in use of
generics more frequently than gender-inclusive forms. gender-inclusive language. Because these findings were largely
Aggregated across the heterogeneous samples in the present repeated in Study 1, for brevity, we do not discuss them further.
research, masculine generics consistently predominated. In 2. Participants also rated three additional items to capture self-
line with this behavior, participants expressed only moder- identity (e.g., “I’m the type of person who thinks about the
ately positive attitudes, norms, and intentions with respect to effect of using gender-inclusive language,” adopted from Terry,
using gender-inclusive language. That is, in Study 1, inten- Hogg, & White, 1999). When including these items in the factor
analysis, they loaded on the same factor as the attitude items. As
tions were slightly above the midpoint on the scale, and in
the self-identity items are not technically attitude items and the
Study 2, they were slightly below the scale midpoint. results were the same without including these items in the atti-
Furthermore, our participants reported only moderately tude measure, we reported all analyses with the attitude measure
strong habits in the form of past behavior frequency, along without the three self-identity items in the present article.
with modest levels of experienced automaticity of use of
gender-fair language. This limited endorsement and past use Supplemental Material
of inclusive language terms is consistent with the continued
The online supplemental material is available at http://pspb.
gender disparities in women’s access to resources and oppor- sagepub.com/supplemental.
tunities in the economy, education, health, and politics. On
the Global Gender Gap Index (World Economic Forum, References
2013), no country achieved complete equality, with
Germany’s score of .76 and Switzerland’s .77 on a scale from Ajzen, I., & Madden, T. J. (1986). Prediction of goal-directed
behavior: Attitudes, intentions and perceived behavioral con-
1 (complete equality) to 0 (inequality). The evidence of sex-
trol. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 22, 453-474.
ist language-use patterns in the present research is consistent doi:10.1016/0022-1031(86)90045-4
with the continued prevalence of patriarchy in the countries American Psychological Association. (2009). Publication man-
in our sample. ual of the American Psychological Association (6th ed.).
Finally, our evidence that gender-inclusive language Washington, DC: Author.
arises from both deliberate and habitual processes provides Arbuckle, J. L. (2013). Amos 22 user’s guide. Chicago, IL: IBM.
insight into potential mechanisms of change. That is, success- Baron, R. M., & Kenny, D. A. (1986). The moderator-mediator vari-
ful interventions to increase such language use could focus on able distinction in social psychological research: Conceptual,
simple repetition of non-sexist language terms so that these strategic, and statistical considerations. Journal of Personality
become established habits (Wood & Rünger, in press; Koeser, and Social Psychology, 51, 1173-1182. doi:10.1037/0022-
Kuhn, & Sczesny, 2014). In addition, interventions could 3514.51.6.1173
Bem, S. L., & Bem, D. L. (1973). Does sex-biased job advertising
address people’s understanding of the consequences of gender-
“aid and abet” sex discrimination? Journal of Applied Social
inclusive language as a means of altering explicit intentions to Psychology, 3, 6-18. doi:10.1111/j.1559-1816.1973.tb01290.x
use it (Fishbein & Ajzen, 2011; Koeser & Sczesny, 2014). Braun, F. (2000). Mehr Frauen in die Sprachen. Leitfaden zur geschlech-
Interventions that change deliberate decisions not to use such tergerechten Formulierung [More women into language. Guideline
language terms are especially appropriate for addressing lan- for gender-fair formulation]. Kiel: Ministerium für Justiz, Frauen,
guage use of people who endorse sexist ideologies. Jugend und Familie des Landes Schleswig-Holstein.

Downloaded from psp.sagepub.com at UNIV OF HARTFORD on July 13, 2015


Sczesny et al. 953

Cacciari, C., & Padovani, R. (2007). Further evidence on gender of the psychology of women and gender (pp. 215-227). New
stereotype priming in language: Semantic facilitation and inhi- York, NY: Wiley.
bition on Italian role nouns. Applied Psycholinguistics, 28, Knussen, C., Yule, F., MacKenzie, J., & Wells, M. (2004). An
277-293. doi:10.1017/S0142716407070142 analysis of intentions to recycle household waste: The roles
Carreiras, M., Garnham, A., Oakhill, J. V., & Cain, K. (1996). of past behaviour, perceived habit, and perceived lack of
The use of stereotypical gender information in construct- facilities. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 24, 237-246.
ing a mental model: Evidence from English and Spanish. doi:10.1016/j.jenvp.2003.12.001
Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 49A, 639-663. Koeser, S., Kuhn, E. A., & Sczesny, S. (2014). Just reading? How
doi:10.1080/027249896392531 gender-fair language triggers readers’ use of gender-fair forms.
Cralley, E. L., & Ruscher, J. B. (2005). Lady, girl, female, or Journal of Language and Social Psychology. Advance online
woman? Sexism and cognitive busyness predict use of gender- publication. doi:10.1177/0261927X14561119
biased nouns. Journal of Language and Social Psychology, 24, Koeser, S., & Sczesny, S. (2014). Promoting gender-fair language:
300-314. doi:10.1177/0261927X05278391 The impact of arguments on language use, attitudes, and cogni-
Douglas, K. M., & Sutton, R. M. (2014). “A giant leap for mankind” tions. Journal of Language and Social Psychology, 33, 548-
but what about women? The role of system-justifying ideolo- 560. doi:10.1177/0261927X14541280
gies in predicting attitudes toward sexist language. Journal of Maass, A., & Arcuri, L. (1996). Language and stereotyping. In C.
Language and Social Psychology, 33, 667-680. doi:10.1177/ N. Macrae, C. Stangor, & M. Hewstone (Eds.), Stereotypes and
0261927X14538638 stereotyping (pp. 193-226). New York, NY: Guilford Press.
Fabrigar, L. R., Wegener, D. T., MacCallum, R. C., & Strahan, E. McConnell, A. R., & Fazio, R. H. (1996). Women as men and
J. (1999). Evaluating the use of exploratory factor analysis in people: Effects of gender-marked language. Personality
psychological research. Psychological Methods, 4, 272-299. and Social Psychology Bulletin, 22, 1004-1013.
doi:10.1037/1082-989X.4.3.272 doi:10.1177/01461672962210003
Fishbein, M., & Ajzen, I. (2011). Predicting and changing behav- Merkel, E., Maass, A., & Frommelt, L. (2012). Shielding women
ior: A reasoned action approach. New York, NY: Psychology against status loss: The masculine form and its alternatives
Press. in the Italian language. Journal of Language and Social
Gardner, B., de Bruijn, G. J., & Lally, P. (2011). A systematic Psychology, 31, 311-320. doi:10.1177/0261927X12446599
review and meta-analysis of applications of the self-report habit Neal, D. T., Wood, W., Labrecque, J. S., & Lally, P. (2012). How
index to nutrition and physical activity behaviours. Annals of do habits guide behavior? Perceived and actual triggers of hab-
Behavioral Medicine, 42, 174-187. doi:10.1007/s12160-011- its in daily life. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 48,
9282-0 492-498. doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2011.10.011
Gardner, B., & Tang, V. (2014). Reflecting on non-reflective Neal, D. T., Wood, W., Wu, M., & Kurlander, D. (2011). The pull of
action: An exploratory think-aloud study of self-report habit the past: When do habits persist despite conflict with motives?
measures. British Journal of Health Psychology, 19, 258-273. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 37, 1428-1437.
doi:10.1111/bjhp.12060 doi:10.1177/0146167211419863
Glick, P., & Fiske, S. T. (1996). The Ambivalent Sexism Ouellette, J. A., & Wood, W. (1998). Habit and intention in every-
Inventory: Differentiating hostile and benevolent sexism. day life: The multiple processes by which past behavior pre-
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 70, 491-512. dicts future behavior. Psychological Bulletin, 124, 54-74.
doi:10.1037/0022-3514.70.3.491 doi:10.1037/0033-2909.124.1.54
Hamilton, M. C., Hunter, B., & Stuart-Smith, S. (1992). Jury Parks, J. B., & Roberton, M. A. (2004). Attitudes toward women
instructions worded in the masculine generic: Can a woman mediate the gender effect on attitudes toward sexist language.
claim self-defence when “he” is threatened? In J. C. Christer Psychology of Women Quarterly, 28, 233-239. doi:10.1111/
& D. Howard (Eds.), New directions in feminist psychology: j.1471-6402.2004.00140.x
Practice, theory and research (pp. 169-178). New York, NY: Parks, J. B., & Roberton, M. A. (2008). Generation gaps in atti-
Springer. tudes toward sexist/nonsexist language. Journal of Language
Hayes, A. F. (2013). Introduction to mediation, moderation, and and Social Psychology, 27, 276-283. doi:10.1177/02619
conditional process analysis. New York, NY: Guilford Press. 27X08317956
Irmen, L. (2007). What’s in a (role) name? Formal and concep- Pauwels, A. (2003). Linguistic sexism and feminist linguistic activ-
tual aspects of comprehending personal nouns. Journal of ism. In J. Holmes & M. Meyerhoff (Eds.), The handbook of
Psycholinguistic Research, 36, 431-456. doi:10.1007/s10936- language and gender (pp. 550-570). Malden, MA: Blackwell.
007-9053-z Rubin, D. L., Greene, K., & Schneider, D. (1994). Adopting gen-
Jacobson, M. B., & Insko, W. R., Jr. (1985). Use of non-sexist pro- der-inclusive language reforms: Diachronic and synchronic
nouns as a function of one’s feminist orientation. Sex Roles, 13, variation. Journal of Language and Social Psychology, 13,
1-7. doi:10.1007/BF00287456 91-114. doi:10.1177/0261927X94132001
Kenny, D. A., Kashy, D. A., & Bolger, N. (1998). Data analysis in Sarrasin, O., Gabriel, U., & Gygax, P. (2012). Sexism and attitudes
social psychology. In D. T. Gilbert, S. T. Fiske, & G. Lindzey toward gender-neutral language: The case of English, French,
(Eds.), The handbook of social psychology (Vol. 1, pp. 233- and German. Swiss Journal of Psychology, 71, 113-124.
265). New York, NY: McGraw-Hill. doi:10.1024/1421-0185/a000078
Kite, M. E. (2001). Changing times, changing gender roles: Who do Schweizerische Bundeskanzlei. (2009). Geschlechtergerechte
we want women and men to be? In R. Unger (Ed.), Handbook Sprache. Leitfaden zum geschlechtergerechten Formulieren

Downloaded from psp.sagepub.com at UNIV OF HARTFORD on July 13, 2015


954 Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 41(7)

im Deutschen [Gender-fair language. Guideline for gender-fair Terry, D. J., Hogg, M. A., & White, K. M. (1999). The theory of
formulation in German]. Retrieved from http://www.bk.admin. planned behaviour: Self-identity, social identity and group
ch/dokumentation/sprachen/04915/05313/index.html norms. British Journal of Social Psychology, 38, 225-244.
Smith, J. R., Terry, D. J., Manstead, A. S. R., Louis, W. R., doi:10.1348/014466699164149
Kotterman, D., & Wolfs, J. (2008). The attitude-behavior rela- Tougas, F., Brown, R., Beaton, A. M., & Joly, S. (1995).
tionship in consumer conduct: The role of norms, past behav- Neosexism: Plus ça change, plus c’est pareil. Personality
ior, and self-identity. The Journal of Social Psychology, 148, and Social Psychology Bulletin, 21, 842-849.
311-333. doi:10.3200/SOCP.148.3.311-334 doi:10.1177/0146167295218007
Stahlberg, D., Braun, F., Irmen, L., & Sczesny, S. (2007). Triandis, H. C. (1977). Interpersonal behaviour. Monterey, CA:
Representation of the sexes in language. In K. Fiedler (Ed.), Brooks.
Social communication. A volume in the series frontiers of Twenge, J. M., Campbell, W. K., & Gentile, B. (2012). Male and
social psychology (pp. 163-187). New York, NY: Psychology female pronoun use in US books reflects women’s status, 1900-
Press. 2008. Sex Roles, 67, 488-493. doi:10.1007/s11199-012-0194-7
Stahlberg, D., & Sczesny, S. (2001). Effekte des generischen United Nations Educational, Scientific and Cultural Organization.
Maskulinums und alternativer Sprachformen auf den gedankli- (1999). Guidelines on gender-neutral language. Paris,
chen Einbezug von Frauen [Effects of masculine generics France: Author. Retrieved from http://unesdoc.unesco.org/
and alternative linguistic forms on the cognitive inclusion images/0011/001149/114950mo.pdf
of women]. Psychologische Rundschau, 52, 131-140. Verplanken, B., & Aarts, H. (1999). Habit, attitude, and planned
doi:10.1026//0033-3042.52.3.131 behavior: Is habit an empty construct or an interesting case
Stout, J. G., & Dasgupta, N. (2011). When he doesn’t mean you: of goal-directed automaticity? European Review of Social
Gender-exclusive language as ostracism. Personality and Social Psychology, 10, 101-134. doi:10.1080/14792779943000035
Psychology Bulletin, 3, 757-769. doi:10.1177/0146167211406434 Verplanken, B., & Orbell, S. (2003). Reflections of past behavior:
Swim, J. K., Aikin, K. J., Hall, W. S., & Hunter, B. A. (1995). A self-report index of habit strength. Journal of Applied Social
Sexism and racism: Old-fashioned and modern prejudices. Psychology, 33, 1313-1330. doi:10.1111/j.1559-1816.2003.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 68, 199-214. tb01951.x
doi:10.1037/0022-3514.68.2.199 Wood, W., & Neal, D. T. (2007). A new look at habits and the
Swim, J. K., Becker, J. C., & DeCoster, J. (2014). Higher order habit-goal interface. Psychological Review, 114, 843-863.
structures for contemporary measures of sexist beliefs. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.114.4.843
Manuscript in preparation. Wood, W., & Rünger, D. (in press). The psychology of habit.
Swim, J. K., Mallett, R., & Stangor, C. (2004). Understanding sub- Annual Review of Psychology.
tle sexism: Detection and use of sexist language. Sex Roles, 51, World Economic Forum. (2013). Retrieved from http://www.
117-128. doi:10.1023/B:SERS.0000037757.731.06 weforum.org/videos/global-gender-gap-report-2013

Downloaded from psp.sagepub.com at UNIV OF HARTFORD on July 13, 2015

You might also like