Dohsa Hu

You might also like

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 10

Dohsa-hou training improves bimanual

coordination among children with


Down Syndrome
Ebrahim Norouzi1 , Mehran Soleymani2 and Rasool Abedanzadeh3
1
Sleep Disorders Research Centre, Kermanshah University of Medical Sciences, Kermanshah, Kermanshah,
Iran; 2Department of Psychology, Faculty of Education and Psychology, Azarbaijan Shahid Madani University,
East Azarbaijan Province, Iran; 3Department of Sport Psychology, Faculty of Sport Sciences, Shahid Chamran
University of Ahvaz, Ahvaz, Iran

A child with Down syndrome (DS) is physically characterized by muscle hypotonia, joint instability, and poor
motor coordination. Here, we tested whether Dohsa-hou training could improve motor coordination among
children with DS, compared to a control condition. Forty children with DS were randomly assigned either to
Dohsa-hou training or to a control condition. All participants completed a bimanual coordination test, at the
following time points: baseline, seven weeks later at completion of the intervention, and again 4 weeks later
at follow-up. Bimanual coordination accuracy and consistency improved from baseline to intervention com-
pletion and to follow-up, but only in the Dohsa-hou training, compared to the control group. The findings
suggest that among children with DS and compared to a control condition, Dohsa-hou training has the
potential to enhance the bimanual coordination, thus contributing to improved motor control of children
with DS.
Keywords: Children with Down syndrome, Dohsa-hou training, muscle relaxation, motor control, coordination pattern

Introduction fitness such as lower aerobic capacity and lower muscu-


Down syndrome (DS) is one of the most common pedi- lar strength (Capio et al. 2018, Stuberg et al. 2001).
atric diseases caused by autosomal chromosomal abnor- Most interestingly, also compared to children with intel-
mality. DS is a genetic disorder, and its prevalence is lectual disabilities, children with DS are reported to
about one per 1000 live births (Siebra and Siebra have lower motor functions (Fenwick 2018, Gonzalez-
2018). Children with DS are the largest group of chil- Ag€uero et al. 2010, Rao 2011).
dren with distinct clinical symptoms (Patterson 2009). More specifically, previous studies showed that chil-
Further, DS is associated with physical growth delays, dren with DS suffer from impairments of motor-percep-
and mild to moderate intellectual disability (Capio tual skills and motor control (Siebra and Siebra 2018),
et al. 2018). and indeed, the kinesthetic perception problem is one of
There is evidence that shows, compared to typically the specific characteristics of children and youths with
developing children (TDC), children with DS have DS (Siebra and Siebra 2018). This kinesthetic percep-
more difficulties in motor performance such as poor tion problem presents in the impaired ability in motor
balance control, reduced postural tone, immaturity of
tasks and also integrating information processing during
motor skill patterns and motor development delays
motor behavior. Further, their hypotonicity has a nega-
(Block 1991, Capio et al. 2018, Stuberg et al. 2001,
tive influence on their proprioceptive feedback, which
Uyanik et al. 2003). Compared to TDC, in children
is vital for optimal motor control (Aparicio and Bala~na
with DS motor functions such as balance performance
2009). Most importantly, their inefficiency of co-con-
and also fundamental movement skills like throwing,
traction during motor coordination patterns results in
running and jumping are 50-70% lower (Shumway-
poor motor coordination performance (Siebra and
Cook and Woollacott 1985). Further, compared to
TDC, children with DS suffer from reduced physical Siebra 2018).
In children with DS, along with internal condition,
Correspondence to: Ebrahim Norouzi, Department of Motor Behavior, inadequate environmental stimuli were reported as the
Faculty of Sport Sciences, Urmia University, Urmia, Iran. Email: eb.nor-
ouzi@urmia.ac.ir, ebrahim.norouzi68@gmail.com one of reasons for motor development delays, reduced

# The British Society of Developmental Disabilities 2022


DOI 10.1080/20473869.2022.2052415 International Journal of Developmental Disabilities 2023 VOL. 69 NO. 6 926
E. Norouzi et al. Dohsa-hou training improved accuracy and consistency of bimanual coordination among children with Down syndrome

motor control and poor perceptual-motor skills (Bull physical, emotional, and behavioral assessments. In the
2011). Therefore, diagnosis and use of interventional Dohsa-hou training appropriate tasks are implemented
programs might be helpful for improving the percep- with the support of a trainer. Through the mutual efforts
tual-motor skills and motor control among children in the trainer-trainee pair, the trainee can develop a
with DS (Aparicio and Bala~na 2009). Considering the greater sense of self-control (Imura et al. 2016). Indeed,
many complications of drug therapy, it is reasonable to it remains unclear whether and to what extent interven-
use non-pharmacological methods that can improve tions such as mindfulness, biofeedback and muscle
motor abilities in children with DS (Fidler and Nadel relaxation are superior to Dohsa-hou (Kabir et al. 2018,
2007, Parham et al. 2007). Such an interventional pro- Naruse 1992, Poursadoughi et al. 2015). Compared to
gram is the Dohsa-hou training. Dohsa-hou training is a other training such as mindfulness, biofeedback, and
Japanese method of rehabilitation, which involves both muscle relaxation, Dohsa-hou training has a distinct
physical and mental training (Naruse 1992). character. In Dohsa-hou training, the role of the therap-
Specifically, Dohsa-hou training programs are based on ist is most important. For instance, therapist must help
three elements including will, effort and movement clients activate their inner power and respond to Dohsa-
(Naruse 1992, Poursadoughi et al. 2015). Further, the hou interventions elements such as grasping objects,
Dohsa-hou training can be divided into two parts; psy- gravity force and body posture. In other words, in
chological (including try and will) and physiological Dohsa-hou training participants were not asked to run a
(including physical training). With the Dohsa-hou train- training sessions independently and alone (Naruse
ing, people learn to respond to their own physical and 1992). In general, Dohsa-hou is a mutual act between
psychological experiences (Naruse 1992, Poursadoughi the trainer and the trainee, which makes trainer-trainee
et al. 2015). Dohsa-hou training leads to attention con- communication very important (Imura et al. 2016).
trol and to the consciousness of the body's senses. This Given that individuals with DS appear to have more
attention control and the awareness of body segments difficulties to verbally express their thoughts and feel-
might enhance motor performance accuracy ings compared to healthy controls (Vicari 2006),
(Poursadoughi et al. 2015). Dohsa-hou appears particularly appropriate, as this
Dohsa-hou is a process which consists of inner psy- technique does not focus on verbal communication and
chological activities and of bodily movements. When might be more appropriate due to the lower verbal
we wish to move a body part, we achieve this bodily demands (Naruse 1992, Uyanik et al. 2003a).
movement according to our own intention (Kabir et al. Children with DS have more impaired motor coord-
2018, Poursadoughi et al. 2015). In other words, ination (Mcloughlin et al. 2011). To illustrate, Macias
Dohsa-hou helps to express the psychological and phys- et al. (2017) observed poor visual-motor coordination
ical states of a person. A further aim of Dohsa-hou is in individuals with DS. Further, Fidler et al. (2005)
the enhancement of people’s self-control regarding their reported lower scores of motor coordination in children
psychological and physical activities (Imura et al. with DS, compared to children with typical develop-
2016). This can be seen in Dohsa-hou training for chil- ment. Latash et al. (2008) showed that people with DS
dren with cerebral palsy in which an increased sense of suffered from deficits in motor control and coordin-
body awareness and enhancement in motor performance ation. However, early rehabilitation can help some chil-
was observed (Dadkhah 1998). dren with DS attain proper psycho-physical
Dohsa-hou training has also been effective in reduc- development and motor coordiantion (Habib-Hasan
ing hyperactivity symptoms, mental retardation, behav- et al. 2020, Palisano et al. 2001, Walker et al. 2020).
ior disorders and cerebral palsy, as well as improving Such psycho-physical rehabilitation is Dohsa-hou,
the social skills among children with DS (Imura et al. which is commonly include physical and psychological
2016) and autism spectrum disorder (Kabir et al. 2018, approaches (Imura et al. 2016). An important part of
Poursadoughi et al. 2015). Further, Dohsa-hou training Dohsa-hou training is the physical activity component,
improved psychological dimensions such as depression which is aimed at improving self-esteem, creativity and
and stress (Kabir et al. 2018, Naruse 1992, perceptual-motor skills (Naruse 1992, Poursadoughi
Poursadoughi et al. 2015). However, the effect of et al. 2015). Although previous studies have been con-
Dohsa-hou training on motor behavior in individuals ducted to improve motor skills and balance among chil-
with DS has not been investigated extensively (Imura dren with DS (Gonzalez-Ag€uero et al. 2010), further
et al. 2016), and so far it remains unclear, which inter- research is needed to determine the effects of rehabilita-
ventions might be most beneficial for improving motor tion programs specifically regarding bimanual coordin-
function of children with DS (Bull 2011). In addition, ation. Bimanual movements require a continuous
people with Down syndrome are gentle in nature and updating of the perception-action cycle to maintain the
easily develop friendly relationships (Reynolds and accuracy of motor behavior (Kelso 1984, 1997). Daily
Fletcher-Janzen 2007). For the mentioned reasons, we motor tasks such as buttoning up a shirt and tying shoe-
hold that focusing on Dohsa-hou which underlies the laces, often demand using our hands simultaneously

International Journal of Developmental Disabilities 2023 VOL. 69 NO. 6 927


E. Norouzi et al. Dohsa-hou training improved accuracy and consistency of bimanual coordination among children with Down syndrome

Figure 1. CONSORT participant flow diagram.

rather than separately, thus necessitating performance Dohsa-hou training on longer term retention bimanual
of bimanual coordination movements (Norouzi et al. coordination in a sample of children with DS, compared
2018). Moreover, reception and integration of sensory to a control condition.
feedbacks are impaired among people with DS (Uyanik The following two hypotheses were tested. First,
et al. 2003). This is most important because sensory based on previous research (Dadkhah 1998, Fujino
feedback is needed to successfully complete bimanual 2017, Gonzalez-Ag€uero et al. 2010, Kabir et al. 2018,
coordination tasks (Salter et al. 2004). However, little Naruse 1992, Poursadoughi et al. 2015), we expected
attention has been paid to bimanual coordination in that, participation in Dohsa-hou training would have a
people with DS. Furthermore, it is confirmed that more positive impact on learning and longer term reten-
bimanual coordination and motor control have import- tion (follow-up) of bimanual coordination accuracy and
ant roles in increase physical activity levels (Gonzalez- second, on the learning and longer term retention of
Ag€ uero et al. 2010, Rao 2011). Therefore, this is the bimanual coordination consistency compared to a con-
reason why children with DS would need an interven- trol condition.
tion to target this bimanual coordination skill.
Additionally, knowledge of motor control impairments Methods
of children with DS are important for psychologists and Participants
occupational therapists (Fidler et al. 2005). Further, A total of 40 children with DS (mean age: 8.12 years;
motor control impairments in children with DS have SD ¼ 1.87) took part in the present study. Inclusion cri-
been neglected in most studies (Fenwick 2018, Rao teria were: (1) children diagnosed with DS based on
2011). Furthermore, research on motor control and genetic testing for Trisomy 21; (2) age between 6 and
learning in children with DS is limited and most 10 years; (3) a diagnosis of poor motor coordination
importantly longer term retention has largely (based on Movement Assessment Battery (MAB) crite-
neglected (de Mello Monteiro et al. 2017, de Menezes ria; Manual dexterity total  22), and (4) the ability to
et al. 2015). Accordingly, the main purpose of the pre- follow a minimum of two-step instructions. In the pre-
sent study was to examine the effects of a 7-week sent study we used Kaufman Brief Intelligence Test,

928 International Journal of Developmental Disabilities 2023 VOL. 69 NO. 6


E. Norouzi et al. Dohsa-hou training improved accuracy and consistency of bimanual coordination among children with Down syndrome

Table 1. Content of the sessions on Dohsa-hou intervention.

Session Content of sessions


1st to 2nd Relationship building with trainees. Relaxation tasks in form of
twisting trunk activities and active horizontal relaxation.
3rd to 4th Sitting crossed leged (Agura-Zai) for relaxation, bending forward
and returning straight to straighten the curvy back portions.
This task helps the trainer check how flexible the trainee's hip
joints are and if they can keep their back and head upright.
5th to 6th Kneeling (Hiza-dachi) tasks for balancing and body image. This
task is important for testing flexibility. This task is sometimes
also applied to one knee.
7th to 8th Opening of the shoulders (Kata-biraki). In this task, the trainer
opens the trainee's shoulders and helps him to relax them.
This task is performed while the trainee is sitting on a chair.
9th to 10th Shisei (posture making) for attainment of straight and stable
sitting, kneeling, and walking with coinciding images of the
patient himself and in others’ perception
11th to 12th Leaning back (Se-sorase). In this task, the trainee sits cross-
legged and leaning back against the trainer's knees. The trainer
also sits cross-legged and supports the trainee's torso and
head with his/her hands.
13th to 14th Twisting of the torso (Kukan-no-hineri). This task is performed on
the floor while the trainee lies on his side. The trainer helps the
trainee twist his torso so that the back of his shoulder reaches
the floor.
15th to 16th Arm lifting Dohsa-hou exercises are performed lying down and in
a sitting posture. The trainers help the trainee raise his arm by
following the imagery line of the arm's proper movement.
17th to 21th The trainer and trainee review all of the tasks, which they had
previously performed.

Second Edition (KBIT-II) as a customized battery of groups ¼ 2, number of measurements ¼ 3, correlation


Arizona Cognitive Test Battery (ACTB) to examine among repeated measures ¼ 0.50) in mixed ANOVA, at
intelligence quotient (IQ) in children with DS least 28 participants are required (14 per group).
(Kaufman and Kaufman 2004). Exclusion criteria were: A computer-generated random-number sequence was
(1) neuromusculoskeletal disorders or severe sensory prepared, and tickets were consecutively numbered, put
impairments; (2) intake of arousal medications or sub- in a ballot-box, and drawn by an independent researcher
stances; (3) lower and upper extremities orthopedic not further involved in the study. Once a ticket was
problems (orthopedic surgery one year before interven- drawn, it was put aside.
tion); (4) injection of botulism toxin 6 months before
intervention; (5) uncorrected visual impairment and (6) Intervention
absenteeism in educational sessions (more than Trained clinical psychologists led the Dohsa-hou train-
2 sessions). ing sessions, which lasted between 45 and 60 min. The
Participants’ parents were fully informed about the training was implemented three times per week under
aims and the procedure of the study, and the anonymous supervision of a clinical psychologist in small groups of
data handling. They all signed the written informed con- six to nine participants for 7 consecutive weeks. The
sent sheet and children gave their assent (verbally or by training procedure followed the manual of Dohsa-hou
gesture) to participate in the present study. At baseline, (Naruse 1992). The focus was on the subjects' desire
bimanual coordination accuracy and consistency were (1) to move or relax their body voluntarily, (2) to
tested. Next, the participating children were randomly increase their awareness of moving or relaxing alone,
assigned to one of the following conditions: (1) Dohsa- and (3) to help the children to convert the extension or
hou training, and (2) control condition. The intervention flexion pattern of a particular postural deviation to a
lasted 7 weeks including three training sessions per week. correct pattern. The principles of the training tasks
Participants’ bimanual accuracy and consistency were focused on relaxation of body (especially the rigid
tested immediately after completion of the intervention, parts), gait, standing on the knees, and lying down, all
as well as after a 4-week follow-up period (Figure 1). naturally imposed. The content of the single Dohsa-hou
The Review Board of the … … . University ( … ., … .) intervention sessions is described in more detail in
approved the study, which was performed in accordance Table 1.
with the ethical principles laid down in the seventh and All intervention activities were performed in a slow
current edition (2013) of the Declaration of Helsinki. pace; in doing so, the children learned to judge and
A power analysis (using GPower 3.1 software) cope with the information of body movements, and to
indicated that in order to detect an effect of moderate monitor a movement. Relaxation tasks were performed
magnitude (f ¼ 0.25; a-error ¼ 0.05, power ¼ 0.8, in lying positions through twisting the trunk, active

International Journal of Developmental Disabilities 2023 VOL. 69 NO. 6 929


E. Norouzi et al. Dohsa-hou training improved accuracy and consistency of bimanual coordination among children with Down syndrome

orientation from midline. Data were sampled using a


microprocessor (80486) with a sampling rate of 50 Hz.
Lab Windows software (National Instrument
Corporation, version 2.2.1) initiated and terminated 20-
s trials and also provided data capture and recording of
limb position (Norouzi et al. 2018). We encouraged
children with DS to perform anti-phase pattern as
quickly as possible during the experiment. Children
with DS received a general orientation to the task. The
task required them to grasp two handles attached to the
moving slides and displace them horizontally in the
left-right dimension (wrist extension and flexion).
While grasping the two handles, participants produced
Figure 2. The bimanual wrist coordination task.
a180 relative phase (anti-phase) pattern. Moreover,
horizontal relaxation, and lifting the arms upward, visual feedback (seeing their own hands) was available
downward and in different directions (Imura for all participants.
et al. 2016). We used the absolute error for coordination accuracy
The control condition session was designed as an and standard deviation of relative phase as a marker for
active control condition. Children assigned to the con- within subject variability. These calculations were done
trol condition met three times per week in small groups on the continuous relative phase time vector (with a
of six to nine participants for 7 consecutive weeks (dur- Bartlett). A MATLAB procedure for this purpose has
ation: 45–60 min per session). During the meetings, been developed. The MATLAB inputs had the two time
children engaged in socio-therapeutic group events such series vectors that represent the hand movements.
as playing board games, and checking blood pressure, Before the calculation of relative phase we normalized
which were organized by social workers and psycholo- the amplitude and speed of hands. The motion data
gists from the same hospital center. were low-pass filtered (second-order Butterworth with
To assess bimanual coordination, we used the fol- cut-off frequency at 8 Hz, with zero-lag).
lowing valid and reliable procedure (Kelso 1984, 1997): The position signals were smoothed with a symmet-
Participants sat on an adjustable chair at a table covered rical Bartlett (triangular) filter. Velocity time series
by a grown laminated poster board (50 cm deep and were derived from the position signal using a two-point
86 cm wide); see Figure 2. Attached in parallel to the central difference algorithm and then smoothed with a
slides were linear potentiometers (Bourns Instruments, Bartlett window. The Bartlett has a triangular window
Riverside, CA), which encoded the displacement of the with the zero end points. The smoothed position and
handle over a 20-s trial. In the present study, we used a velocity time series were then used to calculate each
bimanual coordination task that consisted of flexion and component of the near-continuous phase state for each
extension movements with both wrists in an anti-phase trial according to the formula:
mode. The continuous nature of the bimanual coordin-
ation movement in the present study requires partici- uR ¼ tan 1 fðdXR =dtÞ=XR g
pants to control the limb extensively in an online
manner, through visual and proprioception feedback where uR is the phase of the right wrist in each sample,
loops. Wrist movements were permitted in only the XR is the position of the right wrist rescaled to the inter-
extension and flexion orientation from midline. Data val, f1, 1g for each cycle of oscillation and (dXR/dt)
were sampled using a microprocessor (80486) with a is its normalized instantaneous velocity. The same for-
sampling rate of 50 Hz. (National Instrument mula was used to calculate uL from the position and
Corporation, version 2.2.1) initiated and terminated 20- velocity signals of the left wrist. The relative phase (u)
s trials and also provided data capture and recording of between the two wrists, was then expressed as:
limb position (Norouzi et al. 2018). The metronome
paced the required speed or frequency of limb move- u ¼ uRuL
ment beginning at a slow speed equivalent to a fre-
quency of 30 beat in minute for 20 s. After completion The mean absolute error of relative phase (AEu)
of the 20-s trial at slow speed, the same required coord- reflected the deviation from the target relative phase
ination task was paced at a medium metronome fre- 180 for the anti-phase mode (“coordination accuracy”).
quency (60 beat in minute), and subsequently at a fast The standard deviation of relative phase (SDu) referred
metronome frequency (90 beat in minute). Wrist move- to the spread of relative phase measures around the
ments were permitted in only the extension and flexion mean (“coordination consistency”).

930 International Journal of Developmental Disabilities 2023 VOL. 69 NO. 6


E. Norouzi et al. Dohsa-hou training improved accuracy and consistency of bimanual coordination among children with Down syndrome

Table 2. Descriptive and statistical overview of socio-demographic information, separately for the Dohsa-hou
training and control groups.

Group
Statistics
Dimension Dohsa-hou training Control condition
N 20 20
Age (years) 8.2 (1.65) 8.4 (1.49) t(38) ¼ 1.32, p ¼ 0.65, d ¼ 0.20
Age range (years) 6–10 6-10
Score on the IQ test 81.62 (1.14) 80.20 (2.9) t (38) ¼ 1.43, p ¼ 0.12, d ¼ 0.21

Data analysis measurement points (significant Time  Group inter-


After examining the normality of the data, we compared action). In other words, improvement of coordination
age and IQ scores between the two groups via a series accuracy (i.e. lower AEu scores) was observed in
of t-tests. Next, two analyses of variance (ANOVA) for Dohsa-hou training condition (Figure 3). Effect size (ES)
repeated measures were performed with the factors calculations and t tests corrections were used to examine
Time (baseline, study end, follow-up), Group (Dohsa- the interaction effects. ES calculations and t tests correc-
hou training vs. controls), and time by group inter- tions showed that in the Dohsa-hou training group error
action, with absolute error of relative phase (AEu) and relative phase decreased substantially from baseline to
standard deviation of relative phase (SDu) as dependent post-intervention (large ES ¼ 2.58, t(19) ¼ 8.96, p ¼ .01),
variables. Post-hoc analyses were performed using and from baseline to follow-up (large ES ¼ 2.08,
Bonferroni corrections for p-values. Due to deviations t(19) ¼ 4.25, p ¼ .01). Within the control group, relative
from sphericity, the repeated measures ANOVAs were phase error remained unchanged from baseline to post-
performed using Greenhouse–Geisser corrected degrees intervention (small ES ¼ 0.43, t(19) ¼ 1.85, p ¼ .07), and
of freedom, though the original degrees of freedom are from baseline to follow-up (small ES ¼ 0.41,
reported with the relevant Greenhouse–Geisser epsilon t(19) ¼ 1.67, p ¼ .11). Effect size calculations comparing
value (e). For the interpretation of the ANOVAs, effect means of the Dohsa-hou training and the control group
sizes were reported via partial eta squared (gp2) values, showed that relative phase error did not differ at baseline
with 0.01  gp2  0.059 indicating small, 0.06  gp2  (small ES ¼ 0.32, t(38) ¼ 0.77, p ¼ .44), but the groups
0.139 indicating medium, and gp2  0.14 indicating did differ at post-intervention (large ES ¼ 2.87,
large group differences. For single t-tests, Cohen’s d t(38) ¼ 7.33, p ¼ .01) and at follow-up (large ES ¼ 1.83,
effect sizes were reported. Effect sizes can be evaluated t(38) ¼ 4.21, p ¼ .001).
as trivial (0–0.19), small (0.20–0.49), medium A similar pattern of results was observed for the
(0.50–0.79) and large (0.80) (Cohen, 1992). The level SDu scores (Table 3). Across the sample as a whole,
of significance was set at alpha ¼ 0.05 across all analy- SDu scores decreased from baseline to study comple-
ses, and all statistics were processed using SPSSV 20.0
R
tion and to follow-up (significant time effect).
(IBM Corporation, Armonk, N.Y., USA) for However, the significant effect of Time was due
Apple McIntoshV.
R
entirely to a reduction in relative phase error in the
Dohsa-hou training group, whereas error remained sta-
Results ble in the control group across all measurement points
Table 2 reports the descriptive and inferential statistical (significant time  group interaction). Moreover, the
indices of the sociodemographic data and IQ scores for lower SDu scores (i.e. higher consistency) were
the Dohsa-hou training and control group. The two observed in the Dohsa-hou training condition (Figure
groups did not differ with respect to age and IQ scores. 4). ES calculations showed that in the Dohsa-hou train-
The absolute error of relative phase (AEu) and ing group SDu scores decreased substantially from
standard deviation of relative phase (SDu) for bimanual baseline to post-intervention (large ES ¼ 2.32,
coordination at baseline, post-intervention and follow- t(19) ¼ 7.61, p ¼ .01), and from baseline to follow-up
up are shown in Figures 3 and 4, separately for children (large ES ¼ 1.53, t(19) ¼ 3.84, p ¼ .01). Within the con-
assigned to the intervention and control conditions. trol group, relative phase error remained unchanged
Table 3 also displays the inferential statistics of the from baseline to post-intervention (small ES ¼ 0.38,
repeated measures ANOVAs, whereas Cohen’s ds are t(19) ¼ 1.51, p ¼ .14), and from baseline to follow-up
reported in text. (small ES ¼ 0.39, t(19) ¼ 1.37, p ¼ .18). ES calculations
As shown in Table 3, across the whole sample, AEu comparing means of the Dohsa-hou training and the
scores decreased from baseline to study completion and control group showed that SDu scores did not differ at
to follow-up (significant Time effect). However, the sig- baseline (small ES ¼ 0.41, t(38) ¼ 0.63, p ¼ .53), but the
nificant effect of Time was due entirely to a reduction in groups did differ at post-intervention (large ES ¼ 2.19,
AEu scores in the Dohsa-hou training group, whereas t(38) ¼ 4.67, p ¼ .01) and at follow-up (large ES ¼ 1.51,
error remained unchanged in the control group across all t(38) ¼ 3.74, p ¼ .01).

International Journal of Developmental Disabilities 2023 VOL. 69 NO. 6 931


E. Norouzi et al. Dohsa-hou training improved accuracy and consistency of bimanual coordination among children with Down syndrome

Figure 3. Bar chart showing the group means for relative phase errors (AEu scores) in the anti-phase mode across the two
group and three assessment (p < 0.05, p < 0.01).

Figure 4. Bar chart showing the group means for relative phase variability (SDu scores) in the anti-phase mode across the
two group and three assessments (p < 0.05, p < 0.01).

Table 3. Descriptive statistics for the accuracy, and consistency of bimanual coordination, separately for groups and
measurement points, and tests for time  group interaction effects.

Group Time Time  group interaction


F gp 2
F gp 2
F gp2
Absolute error of relative phase (AEu) 18.03 0.32 39.70 0.51 30.00 0.44
Standard deviation of relative phase (SDu) 10.64 0.21 19.55 0.34 18.89 0.33
Notes: Degrees of freedom: Time: (1, 38), Group: (1, 38), Time  Group (1, 38).
p < 0.05.
p < 0.01.
p < 0.001.

Discussion standard deviation error of relative phase of bimanual


The key findings of the present study are that among coordination, compared to a control condition. Most
children with DS, a 21-session Dohsa-hou training pro- importantly, such motor control improvements were
gram reduced absolute error of relative phase and maintained 4 weeks after completion of the intervention

932 International Journal of Developmental Disabilities 2023 VOL. 69 NO. 6


E. Norouzi et al. Dohsa-hou training improved accuracy and consistency of bimanual coordination among children with Down syndrome

program. The present findings add to the current litera- psychological experiences. Awareness of the body that
ture in an important way in that Dohsa-hou training results from the Dohsa-hou training might lead to atten-
proved to be successfully applicable to children with tion control and more consciousness of the body's per-
DS. Such findings are relevant because children with ception (Dadkhah 1998, Fujino 2017). This attention
DS are at increased risk for reduced motor control, and control enhancement might lead to improved motor
to our knowledge few programs or counseling options function, especially motor coordination. It is confirmed
have been established for this specific population to that bimanual coordination is strongly affected by per-
enhance motor control and prevent motor function ceptual skills and sensory feedback from executive
impairments so far. We believe that the Dohsa-hou limbs (Salter et al. 2004). Further, perceptual and sen-
training intervention has the potential to help psycholo- sory processes are highly dependent on experience and
gists and coaches improve motor control of children training. It is believed that training interventions
with DS. improve perceptions and sensory feedback inputs
Our first hypothesis was that, compared to a control (Salter et al. 2004). Likewise, Dohsa-hou training might
condition, Dohsa-hou training would reduce absolute enhance the child's body perception (Imura et al. 2016)
error of relative phase over time, and this expectation and thereby improve the motor control and bimanual
was fully confirmed. The present results therefore con- coordination (Fujino 2017, Gonzalez-Ag€uero et al.
cur with numerous previous studies (Dadkhah 1998, 2010, Naruse 1992, Poursadoughi et al. 2015).
Gonzalez-Ag€ uero et al. 2010, Naruse 1992, Another explanation for the decrease of bimanual
Poursadoughi et al. 2015). Further, according to the coordination errors might be the muscle relaxation fol-
second hypothesis we expected that compared to a con- lowing Dohsa-hou training (Fujino 2017). Other studies
trol condition, a Dohsa-hou training intervention would show that Dohsa-hou training improves muscle relax-
lead to reductions in standard deviation of relative ation and proprioception (Kabir et al. 2018).
phase error. This hypothesis was also supported. The Additionally, it is claimed that such improved muscle
present results thus are in accord with previous findings relaxation and proprioception might lead to changes in
related to this hypothesis (Dadkhah 1998, Fujino 2017, behavioral and motor responses to environmental stim-
Gonzalez-Ag€ uero et al. 2010, Kabir et al. 2018, Naruse uli and sensory feedback (Kabir et al. 2018).
1992, Poursadoughi et al. 2015). However, the present Accordingly, it is confirmed that optimal bimanual
results do expand upon previous results, in that Dohsa- coordination depends on a higher proprioception sense
hou interventions have the potential to impact the motor (Salter et al. 2004); therefore, Dohsa-hou training might
control of children with DS successfully and positively have led to improved bimanual coordination in children
through physical and mental training including muscle with DS. Further, the hypotonicity affects children with
relaxation. Dohsa-hou has proven effective for motor DS's movement, coordination, and lead to difficulty in
rehabilitation because it helps the children promotes simultaneous activation of homologous muscle groups
motor behavioral development such as maintain proper thus lead to the decreased in motor control. Most
sitting and standing postures as well as walking skills importantly, their inefficiency of co-contraction during
(Fujino 2017). As Dohsa-hou interventions directly tar- motor coordination patterns results in poor motor
get the children body, it is generally considered a phys- coordination performance (Siebra and Siebra 2018).
ical and exercise-based rehabilitation method (Imura The kinesthetic perception problem refer to difficulties
et al. 2016). or dysfunctions in tactile (proprioception)/kinesthetic
The results of present study show that after a well- (movement) processing system in which resulted in
designed Dohsa-hou training the motor coordination of problems during performing motor tasks such as note-
children with DS is improved. This finding is in accord- taking, manipulating buttons or tools and equipment.
ance with Fujino (2017), who concluded that under- This kinesthetic perception problem presents in the
going Dohsa-hou training has a positive impact on impaired ability in motor tasks and also integrating
motor function. Dohsa-hou training decreased muscle information processing during motor performance
tension and increased awareness of body senses (Aparicio and Bala~na 2009). Moreover, coupling
(Dadkhah 1998). In Dohsa-hou training, the body is between the limbs and co-contraction rely on feedback
considered as a key to understanding the problems and from tactile/kinesthetic processing system.
treatment processes of disability (Naruse 1992, Another interpretation of the effectiveness of Dohsa-
Dadkhah 1998). Thus, Dohsa-hou training increases the hou training can be considered to be the type of inter-
interaction between mind and body (Fujino 2017). In vention. There is evidence that children with DS do not
addition, while traditional interventions appear to have sufficient ability to express their inner feeling and
improve single and disparate parts of the body (Rao experiences verbally (Bull 2011). Therefore, Dohsa-hou
2011), Dohsa-hou training claims to impact the entire training, which emphasizes the non-verbal conscious-
body (Fujino 2017). Dohsa-hou training improves the ness of the body and is performed by the active trainer
individuals’ skill to respond to their own physical and (Fujino 2017), might be more beneficial to these

International Journal of Developmental Disabilities 2023 VOL. 69 NO. 6 933


E. Norouzi et al. Dohsa-hou training improved accuracy and consistency of bimanual coordination among children with Down syndrome

individuals, compared to other verbal interventions such hou training at follow-up. However, outcome variables
as psychological counseling. Most psychological coun- the trajectory of change was toward baseline levels after
seling emphasizes the verbal intervention. However, in 4 weeks, therefore, it is suggested that Dohsa-hou train-
the Dohsa-hou training, using muscle relaxation techni- ing be used continually. Fifth and major limitation of
ques can have more effect on the motor and perceptual current study, we only assessed male children with DS;
aspects of children with DS. Furthermore, it would be accordingly, it is unclear whether the present results
recommend in future studies for a measure to assess would also be obtained for female children with DS.
muscle relaxation following Dohsa-hou training with Sixth, the findings indicate that, among children with
electromyography (EMG) outcomes that may be related DS, Dohsa-hou is able to facilitate motor control and
to bimanual coordination and motor control. learning. Therefore, the findings may be generalizable
Briefly, the relaxation resulting from Dohsa-hou to real life capacities and to other motor control skills
training might improve the process of sustainability
requiring similar levels of visual-motor control. Despite
between the body and the mind, attention control, and
we focused on the measures of change, however a main
eventually motor control (Dadkhah 1998). Further, the
limitation of the present study was that we did not
active involvement of participants in Dohsa-hou train-
assess any phase transition variables of bimanual coord-
ing is an important part of this intervention (Kabir et al.
ination and skills of the child such catch a ball.
2018, Poursadoughi et al. 2015). Moreover, Dohsa-hou
Seventh, multitudes of factors have been identified in
training lead the patients to become aware of their body
movements and adjust them (Kabir et al. 2018). Again, previous research that can influence the improvement
it would be recommend in future studies to assess of motor control such as sensory feedback enhance-
mind-body awareness and attentional control following ment, visual feedback, etc. These factors should be
Dohsa-hou training that may be related to motor coord- more systematically assessed in future studies. Eighth,
ination and control. in future studies, Dohsa-hou training might be com-
Finally, Uyanik et al. (2003) shows that there is a pared with other forms of interventions such as motor-
close relationship between physical activity and life perceptual training, visual training, core stability train-
expectancy in children with DS, which indicates the ing, handwriting training, or clinical interventions with
importance of designing an exercise-based intervention respect to effects on bimanual coordination, motor func-
to increase life expectancy and promote health in chil- tion, psychological and cognitive function among chil-
dren with DS. Quality of life as well as physical and dren with DS. Last, it remains to be determined
psychological outcomes improve in children with DS whether and if so to what extent the present patterns of
undergoing physical training interventions compared to results can be replicated with other children with dis-
children with no physical training interventions (Capio ability populations such as children with autism, or
et al. 2018). For example, Andriolo et al. (2005) attention deficit and hyperactivity disorder.
showed that life expectancy and potential for active
functioning in society were improved after aerobic
Conclusion
exercise training programs (Siebra and Siebra 2018).
Among children with DS, Dohsa-hou training had posi-
This holds even more true, as poor motor control in
tive impacts on accuracy and consistency in bimanual
children with DS can be attributed to their inactive life
coordination, when compared to a control condition.
style (Gonzalez-Ag€uero et al. 2010, Rao 2011).
Accordingly, the present data show that Dohsa-hou Notably, this impact was still apparent four weeks after
training, as a physical activity program, is recom- the intervention had ended. Our findings are important
mended to enhance motor control of children with DS because children with DS are at increased risk of devel-
and consequentially this is a first step toward our ultim- oping motor control disorders, and because the study
ate goal to increase their quality of life. provides a viable means of facilitating and enhancing
The novelty of the findings should be balanced motor control. While our findings support the efficacy
against the following limitations. First, the sample size of Dohsa-hou to improve the bimanual coordination of
was rather small, though we focused on effect sizes male children with DS, the question regarding mecha-
when interpreting our findings as effect size calcula- nisms for the promising findings remains open and war-
tions are not sensitive to sample size. Second, it is con- rants further investigation in future research.
ceivable that other latent but unassessed physiological
and psychological factors might have biased the present
pattern of results in the same or opposite directions. Acknowledgements
Third, we note that there was also a gradual drift We are grateful to Mr. Sajad Darabi (Allameh
upwards for consistency in the control group. This may Tabatabei University) for his skilled technical assist-
be attributable to motor learning over time. Fourth, ance, and Mr. Yazdan Norouzi (Islamic Azad
most participants do meaningfully benefit from Dohsa- University of Kermanshah) for providing the data.

934 International Journal of Developmental Disabilities 2023 VOL. 69 NO. 6


E. Norouzi et al. Dohsa-hou training improved accuracy and consistency of bimanual coordination among children with Down syndrome

Disclosure statement review and comparison of the Japanese body movement approach.
Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 12, 21. [29472851
No potential conflict of interest was reported by Kaufman, A. S., and Kaufman, N. L. 2004. K-BIT 2: Kaufman brief
the author(s). intelligence test. Pearson.
Kelso, J. A. 1984. Phase transitions and critical behavior in human
bimanual coordination. The American Journal of Physiology, 246,
ORCID R1000–R1004.
Ebrahim Norouzi http://orcid.org/0000-0003- Kelso, J. A. S. 1997. Relative timing in brain and behavior: some
observations about the generalized motor program and self-organ-
1091-9371 ized coordination dynamics. Human Movement Science, 16,
Rasool Abedanzadeh http://orcid.org/0000-0002- 453–460.
Latash, M., Wood, L., and Ulrich, D. 2008. What is currently known
3629-8465 about hypotonia, motor skill development, and physical activity in
Down syndrome. DSE Library. 23–43.
Macias, A., Caro, K., Castro, L. A., Sierra, V., Ahumada, E. A., and
References Encinas, I. A. 2017, November. Exergames in individuals with
 N., and da Silva,
Andriolo, R. B., El Dib, R., Ramos, L., Atallah, A. down syndrome: a performance comparison between children and
E. M. 2005. Aerobic exercise training programmes for improving adolescents. In International Conference on Smart Objects and
physical and psychosocial health in adults with Down syndrome. Technologies for Social Good. Cham: Springer, Pp. 92–101.
Cochrane database of systematic reviews, (3). Mcloughlin, M. J., Colbert, L. H., Stegner, A. J. and Cook, D. B.
Aparicio, T. S. and Bala~ na, J. M. 2009. A study of early fine motor 2011. Are women with fibromyalgia less physically active than
intervention in Down’s syndrome children. Early Child healthy women? Medicine & Science in Sports & Exercise, 43,
Development and Care, 179, 631–636. 905–912.
Block, M. E. 1991. Motor development in children with Down syn- Naruse, G. 1992. Recent development of Dohsa-hou in Japan. The
drome: A review of the literature. Adapted Physical Activity Journal of Rehabilitation Psychology, 19, 7–11.
Quarterly, 8, 179–209. Norouzi, E., Hossieni, F. and Solymani, M. 2018. Effects of neuro-
Bull, M. J. 2011. Health supervision for children with Down syn- feedback training on performing bimanual coordination in-phase
drome. Pediatrics, 128, 393–406. and anti-phase patterns in children with ADHD. Applied
Capio, C. M., Mak, T. C. T., Tse, M. A. and Masters, R. S. W. Psychophysiology and Biofeedback, 43, 283–292.
2018. Fundamental movement skills and balance of children with Palisano, R. J., Walter, S. D., Russell, D. J., Rosenbaum, P. L.,
Down syndrome. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research, 62, Gemus, M., Galuppi, B. E. and Cunningham, L. 2001. Gross
225–236. motor function of children with Down syndrome: Creation of
Cohen, J. 1992. A power primer. Psychological bulletin, 112, 155. motor growth curves. Archives of Physical Medicine and
Dadkhah, A. 1998. Use of Dohsa-hou, a Japanese psychorehabilita- Rehabilitation, 82, 494–500.
tive program, to guide motor activity of young adults with cere- Parham, L. D., Cohn, E. S., Spitzer, S., Koomar, J. A., Miller, L. J.,
bral palsy. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 86, 243–249. Burke, J. P., Brett-Green, B., Mailloux, Z., May-Benson, T. A.,
de Mello Monteiro, C. B., da Silva, T. D., de Abreu, L. C., Fregni, Roley, S. S., Schaaf, R. C., Schoen, S. A. and Summers, C. A.
F., de Araujo, L. V., Ferreira, F. H. I. B. and Leone, C. J. B. N. 2007. Fidelity in sensory integration intervention research. The
2017. Short-term motor learning through non-immersive virtual
American Journal of Occupational Therapy, 61, 216–227.
reality task in individuals with Down syndrome. BMC Neurology,
Patterson, D. 2009. Molecular genetic analysis of Down syndrome.
17, 1–8.
Human Genetics, 126, 195–214.
de Menezes, L. D. C., Massetti, T., Oliveira, F. R., de Abreu, L. C.,
Poursadoughi, A., Dadkhah, A., Pourmohamadreza-Tajrishi, M. and
Malheiros, S. R. P., Trevizan, I. L., Moriyama, C. H. and de
Biglarian, A. 2015. Psycho-rehabilitation method (Dohsahou) and
Mello Monteiro, C. B. J. I. 2015. Motor learning and virtual real-
quality of life in children with cerebral palsy. Iranian
ity in Down syndrome; a literature review. International Archives
Rehabilitation Journal, 13, 28–33.
of Medicine, 8, 1–11.
Rao, B. K. 2011. Effect of strength and balance training in children
Fenwick, E. 2018. The impact of being a member of a swim team on
individuals with Down syndrome. Williams Honors College, with Down's syndrome: A randomised controlled trial. Clinical
Honors Research Projects. 740. US. https://ideaexchange.uakron. Rehabilitation, 25, 1–16.
edu/honors_research_projects/740. Reynolds, C. R., and Fletcher-Janzen, E. (Eds.). 2007. Encyclopedia
Fidler, D. J., Hepburn, S. L., Mankin, G. and Rogers, S. J. 2005. of Special Education: A Reference for the Education of Children,
Praxis skills in young children with Down syndrome, other devel- Adolescents, and Adults with Disabilities and Other Exceptional
opmental disabilities, and typically developing children. The Individuals, (Vol. 3). John Wiley & Sons.
American Journal of Occupational Therapy, 59, 129–138. Salter, J. E., Wishart, L. R., Lee, T. D. and Simon, D. 2004.
Fidler, D. J. and Nadel, L. 2007. Education and children with Down Perceptual and motor contributions to bimanual coordination.
syndrome: Neuroscience, development, and intervention. Mental Neuroscience Letters, 363, 102–107.
Retardation and Developmental Disabilities Research Reviews, Shumway-Cook, A. and Woollacott, M. H. 1985. Dynamics of pos-
13, 262–271. tural control in the child with Down syndrome. Physical Therapy,
Fujino, H. 2017. Psychological Support for Young Adults with 65, 1315–1322.
Down Syndrome: Dohsa-Hou Program for Maladaptive Behaviors Siebra, C. A. and Siebra, H. A. 2018. Using computational support
and Internalizing Problems. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 1504. in motor ability analysis of individuals with Down syndrome:
Gonzalez-Ag€uero, A., Vicente-Rodrıguez, G., Moreno, L. A., Literature review. Computer Methods and Programs in
Guerra-Balic, M., Ara, I. and Casaj us, J. A. 2010. Health-related Biomedicine, 157, 145–152.
physical fitness in children and adolescents with Down syndrome Stuberg, D., Sanger, W., Naganuma, G., Harris, S. and Tada, W.
and response to training. Scandinavian Journal of Medicine & 2001. Genetic disorders: A pediatric perspective. In: L. B.Jorde, J.
Science in Sports, 20, 716–724. C. Carey, Bamshad, eds. Neurological rehabilitation. 4th ed. St
Habib-Hasan, Z., Sheikh, M. S., Hoodbhoy, Z., Azam, I. and O’Neil, Louis, MO: Mosby.
M. J. J. o. P. R. M. 2020. Early intervention physical therapy Uyanik, M., Bumin, G. and Kayihan, H. 2003. Comparison of differ-
using “parent empowerment program” for children with Down ent therapy approaches in children with Down syndrome.
syndrome in Pakistan: A feasibility study. Journal of Pediatric Pediatrics International, 45, 68–73.
Rehabilitation Medicine, 13, 233–240. Vicari, S. 2006. Motor development and neuropsychological patterns
Imura, O. Furukawa, T. Fujino, H. Sugao, S. Chervenkova, V. in persons with Down syndrome. Behavior Genetics, 36,
Teruta, E., Harada, S., Nihei, Y. and Hibi, Y. 2016. Introduction 355–364.
to Dohsa-hou: an integrated Japanese body-mind therapy. Osaka Walker, B. J., Washington, L., Early, D. and Poskey, G. A. 2020.
University, Japan. Parents' experiences with implementing therapy home programs
Kabir, R. S., Haramaki, Y., Ki, H. and Ohno, H. 2018. Self-active for children with Down syndrome: A scoping review.
relaxation therapy (SART) and self-regulation: A comprehensive Occupational Therapy in Health Care, 34, 85–98.

International Journal of Developmental Disabilities 2023 VOL. 69 NO. 6 935

You might also like