Westen - 1990 - Object - Relations - and - Social - Cognition - in - Borderlin

You might also like

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 11

See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.

net/publication/232418278

Object Relations and Social Cognition in Borderlines, Major Depressives, and


Normals: A Thematic Apperception Test Analysis

Article in Psychological Assessment · December 1990


DOI: 10.1037/1040-3590.2.4.355

CITATIONS READS
228 3,387

5 authors, including:

Drew Westen Kevin Kerber


Emory University University of Michigan
176 PUBLICATIONS 21,601 CITATIONS 14 PUBLICATIONS 1,054 CITATIONS

SEE PROFILE SEE PROFILE

All content following this page was uploaded by Kevin Kerber on 30 May 2014.

The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file.


Psychological Assessment: Copyright 1990 by the American Psychological Association, Inc.
A Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology 1040-3590/90/$00.75
1990, Vol. 2, No. 4, 355-364

Object Relations and Social Cognition in Borderlines, Major Depressives,


and Normals: A Thematic Apperception Test Analysis
Drew Westen and Naomi Lohr
Departments of Psychology and Psychiatry
University of Michigan
Kenneth R. Silk, Laura Gold, and Kevin Kerber
Department of Psychiatry
University of Michigan

This study compared reliably diagnosed borderline personality disorder patients (n = 35) with
major depressives (n = 25) and normals (n = 30) on 4 dimensions of object relations and social
cognition coded from Thematic Apperception Test (TAT) responses: Complexity of Representa-
tions of People, Affect-Tone of Relationship Paradigms (malevolent to benevolent), Capacity for
Emotional Investment in Relationships, and Understanding of Social Causality. As predicted,
borderlines scored significantly lower on all 4 scales than did normals and lower on Affect-Tone
and Capacity for Emotional Investment than did nonborderline major depressives. Borderlines
also produced more pathological responses than did both groups on every scale, indicating more
poorly differentiated representations, grossly illogical attributions, malevolent expectations, and
need-gratifying relationship paradigms. The results suggest the importance of distinguishing sev-
eral interdependent but distinct cognitive-affective dimensions of object relations and the poten-
tial utility of assessing object relations and social cognition from TAT responses.

The psychological processes underlying the interpersonal pa- fective processes underlying interpersonal functioning in pa-
thology of patients with borderline personality disorder (BPD), tients with BPD.
that is, their distorted object relations, have been conceptual- Many of these dimensions have been studied empirically by
ized in various ways by different theorists. Most argue that a developmental researchers, particularly those interested in so-
disturbance in the first 3 years of life leads to the continued use cial cognition (for a review, see Shantz, 1983). Like object rela-
of developmentally primitive modes of relating in adulthood tions theorists, social-cognition researchers have focused on
(Kernberg, 1975; Masterson, 1976). Although theorists and cli- representational processes underlying interpersonal function-
nicians (see Greenberg & Mitchell, 1983) often speak of levels of ing (see Horowitz, 1987; Singer & Kolligian, 1987; Westen, in
object relations as unitary phenomena, from developmentally press-a). Social-cognition research comes from a different intel-
immature to mature, the term object relations refers to a conger- lectual tradition, studies normative rather than pathological
ies of cognitive and affective functions and structures, includ- processes, uses experimental rather than clinical methods, fo-
ing ways of representing people and relationships, rules of infer- cuses on molecular rather than molar processes, and relies on
ence for interpreting the causes of people's feelings, behaviors, different metaphors of the mind than does psychoanalytic ob-
interpersonal wishes, conflicts, and so forth. The concept of ject-relations theory. Nevertheless, this literature addresses is-
general levels of object relations is clinically an indispensable sues such as the nature and development of causal attributions
heuristic, but these levels should be understood as being com- (Ruble & Rholes, 1981) and person-schemas (Horowitz, 1987;
posed of several interdependent but distinct developmental Livesley & Bromley, 1973; Markus & Wurf, 1987; Singer & Kol-
lines that differ in their maturity and quality among individ- ligian, 1987) in ways that are likely to lead to refinements of
uals as well as within a single individual at any given time (Wes- object-relations models (Westen, 1989; in press-a, in press-b).
ten, 1989, 1990, in press-b). The aim of the present study is to Until recently (see Bell, Billington, Cicchetti, & Gibbons,
explore the nature of different dimensions of the cognitive-af- 1988; Burke, Summers, Selinger, & Polonus, 1986; Westen, Lu-
dolph, Lerner, Ruffins, & Wiss, 1990), empirical studies of ob-
ject relations in borderline patients have relied exclusively on
human-figure responses on the Rorschach test. Nearly all of
This research was supported by a grant from the Department of
those studies have compared borderline patients (often diag-
Psychiatry, University of Michigan.
We thank Alfred Kellam, Natasha Lifton, and John Boekamp for nosed with unreported or marginal reliability and validity)
help in coding the data. with schizophrenics. By and large, borderlines typically score
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to higher than schizophrenics on overall developmental level as
Drew Westen, University of Michigan, Department of Psychology,580 measured on Blatt's Rorschach measure for object relations
Union Drive, Ann Arbor, Michigan 48109-1346 (Blatt, Brenneis, & Shimek, 1976), though findings on particu-
355
356 WESTEN, LOHR, SILK, GOLD, KERBER

lar subscales have been less consistent (Gartner, Hurt, & Investment. The scales were also able to predict social adjust-
Gartner, 1989; Hymowitz, Hunt, Cart, Hurt, & Spear, 1983; ment as measured by Weissman's Social Adjustment Scale
Lerner & St. Peter, 1984; Spear & Sugarman, 1984). Lerner and (Weissman & Bothwell, 1976) in both clinical and nonclinical
St. Peter (1984) and Stuart et al. (1990) found that borderlines samples. Two developmental studies have documented develop-
tend to produce malevolent, idiosyncratic, but cognitive-devel- mental differences using both TAT and interview responses in
opmentally advanced representations of people's intentions on all measures except Affect-Tone, with one study comparing
the Rorschach. This finding is not entirely consistent with devel- second and fifth graders and the other comparing early and late
opmental object-relations theory: Malevolence is not likely to adolescents (Westen, Klepser, et al., 1989). Borderline adoles-
reflect a normal developmental stage, and cognitively advanced cents have been shown to differ from psychiatric and normal
representations, even if perceived in a manner suggestive of comparison subjects on the four scales (Westen, Ludolph,
marginal reality testing, are not easily construed as reflecting Lerner, et al., 1990), and borderline adults and adolescents have
preoedipal regression or fixation. been shown to differ from each other in ways suggestive of
When multiple facets of object relations are clearly differen- needed refinements in theory (Westen, Ludolph, Silk, et al.,
tiated, many hypotheses about their developmental course and 1990). Schneider (1990) has recently assessed these dimensions
their quality in severe character pathology can be studied em- from psychotherapy transcripts using an analogous instrument
pirically. This study focuses on four dimensions of object rela- for narrative data and found increases in complexity o frepresen-
tions and social cognition, as measured from Thematic Apper- tations and capacity for emotional investment over the course
ception Test (TAT; Murray, 1938) responses: Complexity of Rep- of brief psychoanalytic psychotherapy and at follow-up. Inter-
resentations of People (tendency to represent people in nal consistency (Cronbach's alpha) has varied depending on the
complex ways and to distinguish clearly their subjective experi- number of TAT responses and between-subject variance, rang-
ence and points of view), Affect-Tone of Relationship Para- ing from .59 to .77 in clinical samples using five to seven cards.
digms (affective quality of the object world or interpersonal In the present study, we compared the TAT responses of a
expectations, from malevolent to benevolent), Capacity for sample of BPD patients with responses of patients with major
Emotional Investment in Relationships and Moral Standards depressive disorder (MDD) and normals. Hypotheses were the
(need-gratifying orientation to the social world versus invest- following. Bordedines should have lower mean scores and a
ment in values, ideals, and committed relationships), and Un- higher percentage of pathological (Level 1) responses on all four
derstanding of Social Causality (tendency to attribute causes of scales than normals. On Affect-Tone of Relationship Para-
behaviors, thoughts, and emotions in a complex, accurate, and digms and Capacity for Emotional Investment, borderlines
psychologically minded way). Each dimension is assessed with should have lower mean scores than major depressives and
a five-level scale (Westen, Lohr, Silk, & Kerber, 1985). With the should have a higher percentage of Level 1 scores, reflecting
exception of Affect-Tone, the scales attempt to measure devel- expectations of malevolence and a need-gratifying orientation
opmental dimensions; Level I is relatively primitive, and Level 5 to the social world. Although major depressives should not
is mature. differ from borderlines in mean Complexity of Representa-
The TAT is an excellent test for assessing object relations and tions of People or Social Causality because of the cognitive
social cognition because, unlike the Rorschach (1942) blots, the constriction characteristic of depressives, which should apply
stimulus is unambiguously social, and subjects are likely to pro- to their representations of people as well, borderlines should
vide enough detail in describing characters and relationships as have more poorly differentiated characters (scored Level 1 on
to provide considerable access to cognitive-affective patterns Complexity of Representations) and grossly illogical causal se-
related to interpersonal functioning. The measures used here quences (scored Level I on Social Causality) than major depres-
were derived from clinical observation, object-relations theory sives. This pattern is expected to hold for borderlines who are
and research (see Blatt & Lerner, 1983; Greenberg & Mitchell, concurrently in a major depressive episode as compared with
1983; Thompson, 1981; Urist, 1980), and research in develop- nonborderline major depressives as well.
mental social cognition (Bogen, 1982; Damon, 1977; Selman,
1980; Shantz, 1983). Recently completed research has begun
validating the measures with both normal and clinical popula-
Method
tions. Research with undergraduates has found significant
correlations between the TAT measures and analogous mea-
sures devised for use with interview data, such as psychother- Subjects
apy transcripts and research interviews (e.g., complexity of TAT
characters correlates with complexity of people in descriptions The patients used as subjects for this study were drawn from two
of actual interpersonal episodes; see Barends, Westen, Leigh, inpatient units of the University of Michigan Medical Center. To be
Silbert, & Byers, 1990; Leigh, Westen, Barends, & Mendel, considered for inclusion in the study, as an initial screening procedure,
potential subjects had to meet at least two criteria of the third edition
1989). This research has also found predicted correlations be-
of the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-II1
tween the four TAT scales and validated instruments, such as or DSM-111-R [revised ]; American Psychiatric Association, 1980,
Blatt, Wein, Chevron, & Quinlan's (1979) measures of complex- 1987) for BPD or schizotypal personality disorder or three such criteria
ity and affective quality of parental representations applied to for major depressive episode on admission. Exclusion criteria included
descriptions of significant others. For example, as predicted, chronic psychosis or medical problems that would prohibit a 2-week
Blatt's "conceptual level" measure correlates with Complexity drug-free period or would confound biological test results.
and Social Causality but not with Affect-Tone or Emotional Ninety-two percent of eligible subjects consented to be considered
OBJECT RELATIONS IN BORDERLINES 357

for inclusion in the research. While consenting subjects were drug-free, verbal productivity or motivation and to elicit adequate material for
they were administered the Diagnostic Interview for Borderlines (DIB; scoring (and for clinical use in cases in which the testing was used for
Gunderson, Kolb, & Austin, 1981), which has been shown in several clinical purposes). Testers were unaware of DIB diagnosis.
studies to predict clinician diagnosis of BPD using DSM-IIIor DSM-
II1-R criteria with sensitivity and specificity above 80% (Armelius,
Jullgran, & Renberg, 1985). Interrater reliability (K= .78) by our group Measures
has been described elsewhere (Cornell, Silk, Ludolph, & Lohr, 1983),
and reliability has been maintained through periodic retraining and Seven TAT cards (Cards 1, 2, 3BM, 4, 13MF, 15, and 18GF) were
assessment. Diagnosis of depression was made by Research Diagnos- coded on the four dimensions outlined above: Complexity of Represen-
tic Criteria (RDC; Spitzer, Endicott, Gibbon, & Robbins, 1975), a pre- tations of People, Affect-Tone of Relationship Paradigms, Capacity for
cursor to DSM-III for research purposes, by a member of the research Emotional Investmem in Relationships, and Understanding of Social
team, based on interviews with the patient and the patient's primary Causality. Each scale has five levels; Level I represents the lowest level
therapist. Interrater reliability on the diagnosis of depression was .92 response, and Level 5, the highest level response. Scoring requires
(weighted kappa; Cohen, 1968), with a range of pairwise reliability of training using an extensive manual. A brief description of the levels of
.88 to .94. As in previous research using the DIB, patients were admit- each scale is provided in Table 2.
ted to the BPD cohort by obtaining a DIB score of>~7. Nonbordedine Complexity of Representations of People. Although object-relations
MDD comparison subjects scored ~<5 on the DIB and met RDC crite- theorists vary widely in their particular models of the development of
ria for major depressive disorder. All subjects scoring 6 on the DIB self- and object-representations, they are largely in agreement about
were eliminated, as decided in advance, to minimize group overlap. three developmental phenomena. First, development of representa-
For particular data analyses, two groups of borderline patients were tions is characterized by increasing differentiation among representa-
distinguished: (a) those who met DIB criteria for borderline but failed tions, in which the points of view of self and others are gradually more
to meet RDC criteria for MDD and (b) those who met criteria for both clearly distinguished. Second, representations gradually become more
disorders, who were comorbid for MDD. complex, internally differentiated, and integrated as children mature.
Normal comparison subjects were obtained by advertising in news- Third, whereas young children tend to split their representations of
papers in two cities, in two university in-house newsletters, and on two people by affective valence, having difficulty forming representations
radio stations. The advertisement requested paid volunteers who are that include both positive and negative attributes, both children and
healthy and reasonably satisfied with life. Potential subjects were ad- adults are able to integrate more complex, ambivalent, or multivalent
ministered a 10-rain semistructured telephone screening interview representations. While challenging some of the developmental timeta-
that assessed physical health, mood, current drug use, use of leisure bles, research in developmental psychology has largely documented
time, consistency and satisfaction at work, establishment of stable rela- these developmental changes. (Harter, 1986; Shantz, 1983; Westen,
tionships, ability to make future plans, and history of psychotherapy. 1989, 1990). Research on the development of representations of self
The purpose of the interview was to screen out potential subjects with and others (Damon & Hart, 1982; Livesley & Bromley, 1973; Rosen-
serious illnesses or severe psychopathology. Subjects who passed the berg, 1979) suggests a shift from concrete, transitory, relatively unor-
telephone screening were asked to sign a consent form and to take the ganized, and often contradictory representations in the preschool
Minnesota Multiphasic Personality Inventory (MMPI, Hathaway & years to unidimensional traits in late childhood, to a focus on internal
McKinley, 1940) and the Rosenberg Self-Esteem Inventory (Rosen- processes, including unconscious personality processes, in adoles-
berg, 1957). Inclusion criteria consisted of an MMPI in which K- cence. The measure of complexity of representations of people for use
corrected scores on all scales (except Masculinity-Femininity) were with the TAT was designed to assess what might be called "working"
less than two standard deviations from the mean, and at least 8 of the representations of self and others (i.e., the momentarily activated repre-
10 items on the self-esteem inventory were in the direction of high sentations that guide conscious and unconscious information process-
self-esteem. The MMPI criteria, like criteria on the self-esteem inven- ing and behavior). The notion of working representations is related to
tory, were chosen to rule out serious psychopathology and are mark- the "internal working models" (Bowlby, 1969) receiving considerable
edly different from published reports of depressed and borderline focus in attachment research (Kobak & Seeery, 1988; Main, Kaplan, &
MMPI profiles. On follow-up 1 to 2 years later, normal subjects were Cassidy, 1985).
also administered the DIB; only I subject scored above a 3 (M = 2.02), Affect-Tone of Relationship Paradigms. A critical dimension of the
and none received any points for depression on the Affects sections of representations underlying interpersonal functioning is the Affect-
the DIB, corroborating that the normal sample did not overlap with Tone of Relationship Paradigms. From a psychoanalytic perspective,
either clinical sample. Table I describes subject characteristics. this can be conceptualized as the affective coloring of the object world,
ranging from malevolent to benevolent. The concept of Affect-Tone
has been implicitly used by theoreticians of severe personality dis-
Procedures orders (e.g., Klein, 1948), particularly BPD (Gunderson, 1984; Kern-
berg, 1975; Masterson, 1976). From a social-cognitive perspective, this
The subjects were administered a variety of biological and psycholog- construct can be conceptualized as the affective quality of interper-
ical tests and interviews as part of a larger project on the relation sonal expectancies. Relevant research has focused on hostile or malevo-
between BPD and mood disorder. A series of TAT cards was adminis- lent attributions in aggressive boys (Dodge & Somberg, 1987), dis-
tered in standardized sequence by members of the research team or by tressed marital couples (Fincham, Beach, & Baucom, 1987), trauma
clinical psychology interns (graduate students), following specific in- victims (Janoff-Bulman, 1989), and abusive mothers (Larrance &
structions for administration, including order of presentation of cards Twentyman, 1983). The measure of Affect-Tone of Relationship Para-
and instructions for prompting. For each card, subjects were asked to digms for use with the TAT was designed to assess the extent to which
tell a story, including what was happening in the picture, what led up to the person expects relationships to be destructive and threatening or
it, and what was going to happen. Subjects who did not spontaneously safe and enriching.
describe what the character or characters were thinking and feeling Capacity for Emotional Investment in Relationships and Moral Stan-
were asked to do so. lfa subject provided an incomplete response (omit- dards. Despite their differences, object-relations theorists posit a de-
ting or minimally elaborating any of the elements of the story), testers velopmental movement from a need-gratifying pattern of emotional
inquired about missing elements. This was done to minimize biases of investment in people (often referred to as narcissistic), in which rela-
358 WESTEN, LOHR, SILK, GOLD, KERBER

Table 1
Means and Standard Deviations o f Subject Characteristics

Age HDRS score SES


Sex
Group n M SD (% female) M SD M SD

BPD total 37 28.24 9.03 73.00 13.65 7.43 4.24 2.57


BPD/non-MDD 19 25.68 7.96 68.40 10.29 6.08 3.18 2.56
BPD/MDD 18 30.94 9.50 77.80 17.00 7.27 5.07 2.34
MDD 25 40,20 11.93 72.00 17.24 5.57 6.17 1.98
Normal 30 32,30 11.35 60.00 1.25 1.35 6.77 1.48

Note. HDRS = Hamilton Depression Rating Scale (Hamilton, 1960); SES = socioeconomic status (Hol-
lingshead & Redlich, 1958); BPD = borderline personality disorder; MDD = major depressive disorder.

tionships with others are valued primarily for the gratification, secu- Coders received the stories typed one to a page in random order, so
rity, or benefits they afford, to mature object relations based on mutual that rating multiple stories in the same protocol would be entirely
love, respect, and concern for others who are valued for their specific independent. Reliability was computed using Pearson's r, with Spear-
attributes (e.g., Fairbairn, 1954). Research in developmental psychol- man-Brown correction for double coding. Uncorrected pairwise reli-
ogy on children's conceptions of friendship, justice, convention, author- abilities ranged from .88 to .95. Corrected average reliabilities for the
ity, and morality tend to support this view; again, however, these litera- four scales were as follows: Complexity of Representations, .94; Af-
tures suggest a much longer maturational process than described by fect-Tone, .97; Emotional Investment, .94; Social Causality, .95. lntra-
psychoanalytic theory, which proposes that need-gratifying object rela- class correlation coefficients yielded similar reliability estimates.
tions are transcended by the end of the oedipal period (see Damon,
1977; Rest, 1983; Selman, 1980; Shantz, 1983; Westen, 1989,1990). The
measure for assessing capacity for emotional investment in relation- Results
ships and moral standards reflects a developmental model aimed at
integrating cognitive-developmental theories and research with ob- All groups (BPD/non-MDD, B P D / M D D , M D D , and nor-
ject-relations theory and clinical observation (Westen, 1985). It at- mal) were primarily female, with no significant sex differences
tempts to assess investment in relationships and values rather than a m o n g them. As could be expected from differences in (a) age
simply knowledge of them; many sociopaths, for example, "know" so- o f onset o f the disorders, (b) the disorders' relative effects on
cial rules but lack an affective-motivational investment in them. occupational functioning, and (c) diagnostic criteria, the groups
Understanding of Social Causality Clinical experience with patients differed significantly in age, F(3, 88) = 7.16, p = .0002, socioeco-
with BPD suggests that these patients tend to make highly idiosyncra-
n o m i c status (SES; Hollingshead & Redlich, 1958), F(3, 61) =
tic, illogical, and inaccurate attributions of people's intentions. One
8.51, p = .0001, and severity o f depression as assessed by the
could probably posit a "borderline attributional style" (Westen, in
press-c), characterized by egocentric attributions, expectations of ma- H a m i l t o n Rating Scale for Depression ( H R S D ; H a m i l t o n ,
levolence in interpersonal relations, a tendency to make peculiar and t960), F(3, 63) = 24.43, p = .0001. As will be addressed below,
inaccurate attributions, and a tendency to make affect-centered attri- these group differences did not account for significant find-
butions (i.e., attributions that are congruent with mood or affective ings.
valence of representations, rather than more cognitively based attribu- To avoid the possibility o f spurious findings reflecting po-
tions). Extensive research in the development of understanding of so- tentially intercorrelated measures, TAT scores were entered
cial causality in children (Chandler, Paget, & Koch, 1978; Piaget, 1951; into a multivariate ANOVA. Mean scores on each scale were
Ruble & Rholes, 1981 ; Selman, 1980) suggests a number of developmen- then analyzed by ANOVA, with planned pairwise comparisons
tal shifts in the way children infer causality in the social realm. These
by t test with Bonferroni correction for the number o f tests on
include increased complexity, abstractness, accuracy, internality (i.e.,
each variable. Presence or absence o f pathological responses
focus on internal psychological processes rather than on surface-level,
observable, behavioral causes), and understanding of unconscious (Level 1) was also coded as a dichotomous variable on each TAT
processes. Bogen (1982) assessed dimensions such as understanding of response for each scale; to aggregate the data and control for
reciprocal causal influences of characters in stories children tell. The missing data points, percentage o f pathological responses per
measure of understanding of social causality for use with TAT re- subject was calculated for each variable. These data were then
sponses was designed to assess the logic, complexity, and accuracy of treated as continuous and analyzed by ANOVA.
attributions. A multivariate analysis using TAT scores as dependent vari-
Coding and Interrater Reliability All cards were coded indepen- ables was significant, F(8, 172) = 2.74, p = .007. Table 3 reports
dently by two raters on each scale. Two advanced graduate students in the results o f univariate ANOVAs for m e a n scores on all four
clinical psychology and two B.A.-level research assistants, who had variables. As can be seen, the three groups differed on all four
been trained extensively using detailed scoring manuals, coded the
variables as predicted. Planned comparisons (one-tailed t tests
data for the study. Coders met at regular intervals to discuss indepen-
with Bonferroni adjustment) demonstrated highly significant
dently scored responses in order to prevent coder drift and to resolve
discrepancies. Because coding extended over several months, and differences between borderlines and normals on all four scales,
coders' time commitments varied, each coder was trained to code all and clear differences between the borderlines and major de-
four scales; different pairs of coders thus scored different subsets of the pressives on the two scales on which they were hypothesized to
data on the various scales. differ--Affect-Tone and Emotional Investment.
OBJECT RELATIONS IN BORDERLINES 359

Table 2
Brief Synposis of Measures of Object Relations and Social Cognition
Scale

Complexity of Affect-Tone of Relationship Capacity for Emotional Understanding of Social


Level Representations of People Paradigms Investment Causality

1 People are not clearly Malevolent representations; Need-gratifying orientation; Noncausal or grossly illogical
differentiated; gratuitous violence or gross profound self- depictions of psychological
confusion of points of negligence by significant preoccupation, and interpersonal events.
view. others.
2 Simple, unidimensional Representation of relationships Limited investment in people, Rudimentary understanding
representations; focus as hostile, empty, or relationships, and moral of social causality; minor
on actions; traits are capricious but not standards; conflicting logic errors or unexplained
global and univalent, profoundly malevolent; interests recognized, but transitions; simple
profound loneliness or gratification remains stimulus-response
disappointment in primary aim; moral causality.
relationships. standards minimally
developed or followed to
avoid punishment.
3 Minor elaboration of Mixed representations with Conventional investment in Complex, accurate situational
mental life or mildly negative tone. people and moral causality and rudimentary
personality. standards; stereotypic understandingof the role
compassion, mutuality, or of thoughts and feelings in
helping orientation; guilt mediating action.
at moral transgressions.
Expanded appreciation of Mixed representations with Mature, committed Expanded appreciation o f the
complexity of neutral or balanced tone. investment in relationships role of mental processes
subjective experience and values; mutual empathy in generating thoughts,
and personality and concern; commitment feelings, behaviors, and
dispositions; absence of to abstract values. interpersonal interactions.
representations
integrating life history,
complex subjectivity,
and personality
processes.
Complex representations, Predominantly positive Autonomous self hood in the Complex appreciation of the
indicating representations; benign and context of committed role of mental processes
understanding of enriching interactions. relationships; recognition in generating thoughts,
interaction of enduring of conventional nature of feelings, behaviors, and
and momentary moral rules in the context interpersonal interactions;
psychological of carefully considered understanding of
experience; standards or concern for unconscious motivational
understanding of concrete people or processes.
personality as system relationships.
of processes interacting
with each other and the
environment.

Table 4 reports the percentage of Level I (hypothesized to be on Axis II (and do not differ significantly in HRSD depression
pathological) responses per protocol by diagnosis. As can be scores). To test this hypothesis, we first ran a multivariate analy-
seen in the table, again the groups differed significantly on all sis to determine whether differences on the TAT measure still
four variables, with significant differences found between the emerge when dividing the sample into four groups: borderlines
borderlines and both comparison groups on every variable ex- without major depression, borderlines with major depression,
cept Complexity, on which the borderlines produced more nonborderline major depressives, and normals. The multivar-
poorly differentiated responses than major depressives, but the iate analysis was significant, F(12, 225.2) = 1.97, p = .03, and
differences did not reach a significant level. The significance the general pattern of ANOVA findings was similar to the
values reported here represent relatively conservative estimates, three-group analysis. The specific comparison of borderline
because the Bonferroni correction was technically not neces- patients comorbid for major depression with nonborderline
sary due to the highly significant multivariate analysis. major depressives (one-tailed t tests) is presented in Table 5. As
An even more conservative test of the differences between can be seen, the basic pattern found with the entire borderline
the borderlines and the major depressives would control for sample, in which borderlines have lower mean scores on Af-
depression by comparing only those BPDs who were comorbid fect-Tone and Emotional Investment and a higher percentage of
for major depression with MDDs; such a comparison would pathological responses on all four scales, was largely con-
contrast patients who share an Axis I diagnosis and differ only firmed. The significance values are, of course, slightly lower
360 WESTEN, LOHR, SILK, GOLD, KERBER

Table 3
Mean Scores by Diagnosis

Planned comparisons
Diagnosis
Borderlines
Major vs. major Borderlines vs.
Borderlines depressives Normals depressives normals

Scale M SD M SD M SD F stat df T stat df T stat df

Complexity of Representations 2.80 .59 2.76 .57 3.16 .57 4.48** 2,89 .01 60 2.58** 65
Affect-Toneof Relationship
Paradigms 2.53 .59 2.83 .53 2.97 .39 6.22*** 2,89 2.20* 60 3.44**** 65
Capacity for Emotional
Investment 2.18 .54 2.42 .40 2.61 .48 6.31"** 2,89 1.88t 60 3.53**** 65
Understanding of Social
Causality 2.49 .60 2.58 .48 2.90 .54 4.79** 2,89 .07 61 3.01"** 65

Note. Data analyzed by analysis of variance. Planned comparisons analyzed by one-tailed T tests with Bonferroni adjustment for number of
comparisons.
* = p < . 0 5 , **=p<.01, ***=p<.005, ****=p<.001, t = t r e n d ( p ~ - 0 6 ) .

than they were with the total borderline sample because of the parable to those of the normals and higher than those of the
smaller sample size. MDDs.
We similarly analyzed the presence of scores on Levels 4 and To address potential confounding variables, we ran several
5, although we had not formulated specific hypotheses about additional analyses. First, because age and SES differed among
them (and hence they are not summarized in tabular form). We the groups, we correlated these variables with all dependent
did not expect to see a total absence of high-level responses in variables within and between groups. There were no significant
any group, because of our assumption that people do not always correlations with age. Only Complexity of Representations
operate at a single level of object relations and that different correlated with SES (r = .24, p = .03). Although covariation is
cards are likely to activate different issues, relationship para- probably inappropriate because lower SES in the borderline
digms, and levels of functioning. Contrary to current theories, group is likely to be a reflection of diagnosis, to be conservative
we expected that a substantial percentage of borderlines would we covaried for SES in an additional analysis of the complexity
produce complex representations and attributions. In fact, data. The overall ANOVA remained marginally significant,
45.9% of borderlines produced more than one story that scored F(2, 75) = 3.01, p = .055, with BPDs significantly lower than
Level 4 or 5 on Complexity of Representations, which was com- normals but not significantly different from MDDs.

Table 4
Percentage o f Level 1 (Pathological) Responses per Protocol by Diagnosis
Diagnosis Planned comparisons

Major Borderlines vs.


Border- depres- major Borderlines vs.
lines sives Normals depressives normals

Scale M SD M SD M SD F star df T star df T star df

Complexity of
Representations
(Poorly differentiated) 08 .16 03 .07 0.5 .03 3.46* 2,89 1.61 60 2.57** 65
Affect-Toneof
Relationship
Paradigms (Malevolent) 16 .17 07 .11 05 .09 7.74**** 2,89 3.61 *** 60 3.54**** 65
Capacity for
Emotional Investment
(Need-gratifying) 24 .23 14 .18 10 .11 4.95** 2,89 2.11" 60 3.01"** 65
Social Causality
(Grossly illogical) 13 .20 05 .11 0.3 .07 4.17" 2,89 1.99" 60 2.74** 65

Note. Data analyzed by analysis of variance. Planned comparisons analyzed by one-tailed T tests with Bonferroni adjustment for number of
comparisons. Nonparametric analyses yielded similar results.
*=pA.05, **=p<.01, ***=p<.005, ****=p_<.001.
OBJECT RELATIONS IN BORDERLINES 361

Table 5
Mean Scores and Percentage of Level 1 (Pathological) Responses for Major
Depressives With and Without BPD

Percentage of Level 1
Mean scores responses
BPD/ MDD BPD/ MDD
MDD only MDD only
Scale M SD Mean SD T stat M SD M SD T stat
Affect-Tone of Relationship
Paradigms 2.52 .52 2.87 .52 2.20** 18 1.54 06 1.09 2.86***
Complexity of Representations 2.86 .61 2.75 .58 .62 06 1.62 03 .71 .93
Capacity for Emotional
Investment 2.22 .46 2.42 .41 1.51t 23 1.99 14 1.58 1.71"
Understanding of Social
Causality 2.63 .67 2.57 .49 .35 13 2.09 05 1.17 1.64"

Note. Data (planned comparisons) analyzed by t test (one-tailed), dr= 41. BPD = borderline personality
disorder; MDD = major depressive disorder.
*=p_<.05, **=p<.01, ***=p_<.005, t=trend(p<.07).

To address rival hypotheses that the results reflect degree of cognition. The overall pattern of findings suggests that BPDs
psychopathology rather than borderline pathology per se, or are distinguished by poorly differentiated, egocentric represen-
that the findings reflect characteristics of the normals rather tations of people; malevolent expectations of relationships; dif-
than of the borderlines, the following analyses were conducted. ficulty investing in relationships and moral standards; and
Borderlines and depressives were compared on the General idiosyncratic and grossly illogical attributions. It is likely that
Symptomatology Index of the Revised Symptoms Checklist- these characteristics of the object relations and social cognition
90. (SCL-90-R; Derogatis, 1977; the two groups did not differ of borderline patients underlie many of the interpersonal diffi-
significantly, t[29] = .52, p = .61), suggesting that degree of culties of these patients.
psychopathology does not explain the findings. Additionally, A particularly strong finding is that BPDs have a tendency to
we tested for differences on the dependent variables between view the world of people as malevolent, which was replicated in
the depressives and the normals. Whereas the borderlines were a study comparing borderline adolescents to psychiatric and
significantly more pathological than the normals on all eight normal comparison subjects (Westen, Ludolph, Lerner, et aL
variables tested, depressives differed from normals only on the 1990). This finding has emerged consistently in studies using
two variables expected to distinguish the two groups because of various sources of data, including Rorschach responses (Lerner
the cognitive constriction of the MDD group: mean Complex- & St. Peter, 1984; Spear & Sugarman, 1984; Stuart et al., 1990),
ity and mean Social Causality. The two BPD groups (with and early memories (Nigg, Lohr, Westen, Gold, & Silk, 1989), sto-
without MDD) did not differ significantly from each other on ries told in response to the Picture Arrangement subtest of the
any TAT variable. WAIS-R (Segal, Westen, Lohr, Silk, & Cohen, 1989), and the
Another potential confound is that, although the testers were SCL-90-R (notably the Paranoia and Interpersonal Sensitivity
unaware of diagnosis (as during the administration of the TAT), subscales; Benjamin, Silk, Lohr, & Westen, 1989). Together
they may have been able to intuit the group to which the subject these findings suggest that malevolent expectations appear to
belonged (particularly the normals) and thus able to influence be characteristic of borderline patients and thus should be con-
responses by additional prompting. To test for prompting ef- sidered for inclusion as a diagnostic criterion for BPD.
fects, we compared subjects who were tested by administrators Despite a tendency among the BPD subjects in this study to
acquainted with the measures with those unacquainted. Both produce poorly differentiated, extremely egocentric representa-
across and within diagnostic groups, one-tailed t tests revealed tions, a substantial percentage of borderline subjects (45.9%)
no significant differences on any measure. A second analysis showed evidence of a capacity to form more complex represen-
comparing subjects tested by members of the research team tations (Level 4 or 5) on at least two of the seven TAT cards. This
with those not tested by members of the research team simi- finding points to the importance of focusing on the dynamic
larly yielded no differences approaching significance. flux and flow of object relations and on the conditions under
which pathological processes are evoked, rather than strictly on
levels of object relations as monolithic structures. This shift in
Discussion focus would require a greater integration of more classical psy-
choanalytic thinking about momentary dynamic processes and
In general, the results of this study suggest that patients with compromise formations into object-relations concepts. It would
BPD can be successfully discriminated from major depressives also suggest the importance for psychoanalytic theory of wres-
and normals on the basis of their object relations and social tling with contemporary issues in personality psychology re-
362 WESTEN, LOHR, SILK, GOLD, KERBER

garding person-situation interactions and the importance of Another possible objection is that the findings reflect char-
specifying conditions under which trait-like phenomena be- acteristics of the normal sample, rather than of the borderline
come manifest. Future research should be directed toward un- sample. Differences between the BPD and M D D groups, how-
derstanding the conditions under which BPDs manifest prob- ever, cannot be explained with this rival hypothesis, nor can it
lematic object relations. No one is borderline all the time, yet explain why multiple differences between BPDs and normals
we know little about the activating conditions for pathological were not also present between MDDs and normals. From a
processes. Future research will also need to explore the possibil- psychoanalytic perspective, a substantial percentage of people
ity that different dimensions o f object relations may be in- who are typically used as normals in psychological research
fluenced by different experiences and that pathogenic experi- have considerable character pathology, so that a sample repre-
ences may not all occur during the preoedipal period (Westen, sentative of the population of American adults would not be an
in press-b; Westen, Ludolph, Block, Wixom & Wiss, 1990). appropriate comparison group for a study of patients with per-
The data also suggest that if BPD is a variant of a mood sonality disorders. What we had hoped to select with a brief
disorder, the relationship is not simple. BPDs with major de- screening interview, a self-esteem inventory, and the MMPI was
pression did not significantly differ from BPDs without major a sample of people who seemed relatively intact clinically, re-
depression on any scale; in contrast, the BPD group as a whole ported the same, and were statistically normal on all scales of a
differed from the M D D group, as did the BPD/MDD sub- psychometrically valid and reliable instrument.
group, in nearly every comparison where predicted. In other
words, borderlines with major depression look like borderlines,
not like major depressives. Although there is likely to be a bio- References
logical substrate to some subgroups of patients with BPD, this
substrate is not a simple vulnerability to major depression. Akiskal, H. S. (1981). Subaffective disorders: Dysthymic, cyclothymic,
Finally, this study supports the utility of projective tests, and and biopolar I1 disorders in the "borderline realm:' Psychiatric Clin-
particularly the TAT, in assessing dimensions of object rela- ics of North America. 4, 25-46.
American Psychiatric Association, (1980). Diagnostic and Statistical
tions and social cognition. Since Mischel's (1968) sophisticated
Manual of Mental Disorders (3rd ed.). Washington, DC: Author.
critique of trait psychology and projective testing, generations American Psychiatric Association. (1987). Diagnostic and Statistical
of psychologists, including personality and clinical psycholo- Manual of Mental Disorders (3rd ed. rev.).Washington, DC: Author.
gists, have been trained with a deeply ingrained assumption Armelius, B., Jullgran, G., & Renberg, E. (1985). Borderline diagnosis
that projective techniques are inherently invalid and unreliable. from hospital records: Reliability and validity ofGunderson's DIB.
In the present study, however, four complex coding schemes Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 173, 32-34.
were applied to TAT data with high interrater reliability, and Barends, A., Westen, D., Leigh, J., Silbert, D., & Byers, S. (1990). As-
these were able to document some relatively subtle predicted sessing affect-tone of relationship paradigms from TAT and inter-
differences. view data. Psychological Assessment: A Journal of Consulting and
This study has the following limitations. First, the study in- Clinical Psychology, 2, 329-332.
Bell, M., Billington, R., Cicchetti, D., & Gibbons, J. (1988). Do object
volves the use of TAT measures whose validity and psychomet-
relations deficits distinguish BPD from other diagnostic groups?
ric properties are only beginning to be established, and clearly Journal of Clinical Psychology, 44, 511-516.
results must be interpreted cautiously, Convergent findings us- Benjamin, J., Silk, K. R., Lohr, N. E., & Westen, D. (1989). The relation-
ing other methods and samples have, however, been very con- ship between borderline personality disorder and anxiety disorders.
sistent (Bell et al., 1988; Benjamin et al., 1989; Burke et al., 1986; American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 59, 461-467.
Nigg et al., 1989; Westen, Ludolph, Lerner, et al., 1990). Fur- Blatt, S. J., Brenneis, C. B., & Schimek, J. G. (1976). Normal develop-
ther, we made some very specific predictions in this study, all ment and psychopathological impairment of the concept of the ob-
but one of which were borne out, and it is difficult to conceive ject on the Rorschach. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 85, 364-
of a rival hypothesis based on problems with the measures that 373.
could account for this set of findings as parsimoniously. Blatt, S. J., & Lerner, H. (1983). Investigations in the psychoanalytic
theory of object relations and object representations. In J. Masling
A second limitation is that the groups differed in age and
(Ed.), Empirical studies of psychoanalytic theories (Vol. I, pp. 189-
SES, reflecting group differences inherent in the population. 249). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Neither age nor SES, however, correlated with any dependent Blatt, S. J., Wein, S., Chevron, E. S., & Quinlan, D. M. (1979). Parental
variable except for a low correlation between complexity and representations and depression in normal young adults. Journal of
SES, suggesting that the findings are not reducible to group Abnormal Psychology, 78, 388-397.
differences on these variables. A study comparing borderline Bogen, T. M. (1982). Patterns of developmental change in formal charac-
adolescents to normal and psychiatric comparison subjects in teristics of stories children tell. Unpublished doctoral dissertation,
which age and SES were matched produced a very similar pat- University of Michigan.
tern of results (Westen, Ludolph, Lerner, et al., 1990). The find- Bowlby, J. (1969). Attachment and loss: VoL 1. Attachment. New York:
ings were also not reducible to degree of psychopathology as Basic Books.
Burke, W E, Summers, E, Selinger, D., & Polonus, T. W (1986). The
assessed by the S C L - 9 0 - R ; it would be difficult to argue that comprehensive object relations profile: A preliminary report. Psy-
patients hospitalized for MDD, who are typically debilitated by choanalytic Psychology, 3, 173-185.
their illness and receive multiple discharge diagnoses on both Chandler, M. J., Paget, K. E, & Koch, D. A. (1978). The child's demysti-
Axis ! and Axis II (e.g., avoidant or dependent personality dis- fication of psychological defense mechanisms: A structural and de-
order), have less severe psychopathology than patients hospital- velopmental analysis. Developmental Psychology, 14, 197-205.
ized for BPD. Cohen, J. (I 968). Weighted kappa: Nominal scale agreement with pro-
OBJECT RELATIONS IN BORDERLINES 363

vision for scaled disagreement or partial credit. PsychologicalBulle- neurotic, borderline, and schizophrenic patients. Psychiatry, 47, 77-
tin, 70, 213-220. 92.
Cornell, D., Silk, K., Ludolph, P., & Lohr, N. (1983). Test-related reli- Livesley, W J., & Bromley, D. B. (1973). Person perception in childhood
ability of the diagnostic interview for borderlines. Archives of Gen- and adolescents. London: Wiley.
eral Psychiatry, 40, 1307-1310. Main, M., Kaplan, N., & Cassidy, J. 0985). Security in infancy, child-
Damon, W. (1977). The social world of the child. San Francisco: Jossey- hood, and adulthood: A move to the level of representation. In I.
Bass. Bretherton & E. Waters (Eds.), Growing points of attachment theory
Damon, W., & Hart, D. (1982). The development of self-understanding and research(pp. 67-104). Monographs of the Society for Research in
from infancy through adolescence. ChiM Development, 53, 841-864. Child Development, 50, Nos. 1-2.
Derogatis, L. R. (1977). SCL-90-R manual I: Scoring and procedures Markus, H., & Wurf, E. 0987). The dynamic self-concept: A social
manual for the SCL-90-R. Baltimore, MD: Clinical Psychometrics psychological perspective. Annual Review of Psychology 38, 299-
Research Unit. 337.
Dodge, K. A., & Somberg, D. R. 0987). Hostile attributional biases Masterson, J. E (1976). Psychotherapy of the bordedine adult. New
among aggressive boys are exacerbated under conditions of threat to York: Brunner/Mazei.
the self. Child Development, 58, 213-224. Mischel, W. (1968). Personality and assessment. New York: Wiley.
Fairbairn, W. (1954). An object-relations theory of the personality, New Murray, H. 0938). Explorations in personality New York: Oxford Uni-
York: Basic Books. versity Press.
Fincham, E D., Beach, S. R., & Baucom, D. H. 0987). Attributionai Nigg, J., Lohr, N., Westen, D., Gold, L., & Silk, K. 0989). Affective
processes in distressed and nondistressed couples: 4. Self-partner quality of relationships in the early memories of borderlines, depres-
attribution differences. Journal of Personalityand Social Psychology, sives, andnormals. Unpublished manuscript, Department of Psychi-
52, 739-748. atry, University of Michigan.
Gartner, J., Hurt, S. W.,& Gartner, A. (1989). Psychological test signs of Piaget, J. (1951). The language and the thought of the child. New York:
borderline personality disorder: A review of the empirical literature. Humanities Press. (Original work published 1926).
Journal of Personality Assessment, 53, 413-44 I. Rest, J. R. 0983). Morality. In P. Mussen (Ed.), Handbook of child
Greenberg, J. R., & Mitchell, S. A. 0983). Object relations in psychoan- psychology(Vol. 3, pp. 556-629). Cognitivedevelopment, J. H. Flavell
alytic theory Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. & E. M. Markman (Eds.). New York: Wiley.
Gunderson, J. (1984). Borderline personality disorder. Washington, Rorschach, H. (1942). Psychodiagnostics. (E Lemkau & B. Kronen-
DC: American Psychiatric Press. berg, Trans). Berne: Huber (lst German ed. published 1921; U.S.
Gunderson, J. G., Kolb, J. E., & Austin, V.(198 l). The Diagnostic Inter- distributor, Grune & Stratton).
view for Borderline Patients. American Journal of Psychiatry, 138, Rosenberg, M. (1957). Occupations and values. Glencoe, IL: Free Press.
896. Rosenberg, M. 0979). Conceivingthe self. New York: Basic Books.
Hamilton, M. (1960). A rating scale for depression. Journal of Neurol- Ruble, D. N., & Rholes, W. S. (1981). The development of children's
ogy, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry, 23, 56-62. perceptions and attributions about their social world. In J. H. Har-
Harter, S. (1986). Cognitive-developmental processes in the integra- vey, W Wickes, & R. E Kidd (Eds.), New directions in attribution
tion of concepts about emotions and the self. Social Cognition, 4, research (Vol. 3, pp. 3-36). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
119-151. Schneider, E. L. 0990). The effect of brief psychotherapy on the level of
Hathaway, S. R., & McKinley, J. C. (1940). A multiphasic personality the patient's object relations. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, New
schedule (Minnesota): Vol. 1: Construction of the schedule. Journal York University.
of Psychology 10, 249-254. Segal, S., Westen, D., Lohr, N., Silk, K., & Cohen, R. (1989). Assessing
Hollingshead, A. B., & Redlich, E C. (1958). Social class and mental object relations and social cognition in borderlinepersonality disorder
illness: A community study New York: Wiley. from stories told to Picture Arrangement subtest of the WAIS-R. Un-
Horowitz, M. J. (1987). States of mind."Configurational analysis of indi- published manuscript, Department of Psychology, University of
vidual psychology (2nd. ed.). New York: Plenum Press. Michigan.
Hymowitz, P., Hunt, H. E, Carr, A. C., Hurt, S. W., & Spear, W. E. Selman, R. L. (1980). The growth of interpersonal understanding. Devel-
(1983). The WAIS and Rorschach Test in diagnosing borderline per- opmental and clinical analyses. New York: Academic Press.
sonality. Journal of Personality Assessment, 47, 588-596. Shantz, C. U. (1983). Social cognition. In P. Mussen (Ed.), Handbook of
Janoff-Bulman, R. (1989). Assumptive worlds and the stress of trau- child psychology (Vol. 3, pp. 495-555), Cognitive development, J. H.
matic events: Applications of the schema construct. Social Cogni- Flavell & E. M. Markman (Eds.). New York: Wiley.
tion, 7, ! 13-136. Singer, J., & Kolligian, J. 0987). Personality: Developments in the
Kernberg, O. (I 975). Borderlineconditions and pathological narcissism. study of private experience. Annual Review of Psychology, 38, 533-
New York: Jason Aronson. 574.
Klein, M. (1948). Contributions to psycho-analysis, 1921-1945. Lon- Spear, W. E., & Sugarman, A. (1984). Dimensions of internalized object
don: Hogarth Press. relations in borderline and schizophrenic patients. Psychoanalytic
Kobak, R. R., & Sceery, A. (1988). Attachment in late adolescence: Psychology, 1, 113-129.
Working models, affect regulation, and representations of self and Spitzer, R. L., Endicott, J., Gibbon, M., & Robbins, E. (1975). Research
others. ChiM Development, 59, 135-146. diagnostic criteria (RDC). Psychopharmacology Bulletin, 11, 22-24.
Larrance, D. T., & Twentyman, C. T. (1983). Maternal attributions and Stuart, J., Westen, D., Lohr, N., Silk, K., Becker, S., Vorus, N., & Benja-
child abuse. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 92, 449--457. min, J. (1990). Object relations in borderlines, major depressives,
Leigh, J., Westen, D., Barends, A., & Mendel, M. (1989). Assessing and normals: Analysis of Rorschach human figure responses. Jour-
complexity of representations of people from TAT and interviewdata. nal of Personality Assessment, 55, 296-314.
Unpublished manuscript, Department of Psychology, University of Thompson, A. E. (198 ! ). The theory of affect development and maturity:
Michigan. Applications to the TAT. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Univer-
Lerner, H. D., & St. Peter, S. (1984). Patterns of object relations in sity of Michigan.
364 WESTEN, LOHR, SILK, GOLD, KERBER

Urist, J. (1980). Object relations. In R. W Woody (Ed.), Encyclopedia of kamp, J. (in press). Object relations in childhood and adolescence:
clinicalassessment (Vol. 2, pp. 821-833). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass. The development of working representations. Journal of Consulting
Weissman, M., & Bothwell, S. (1976). Self-report version of the Social and Clinical Psychology.
Adjustment Scale. Archives of General Psychiatry, 33, 1111-1115. Westen, D., Lohr, N., Silk, K., & Kerber, K. (1985). Measuring object
Westen, D. (1985). Self and society: Narcissism, collectivism, and the relations and social cognition using the TAT:Scoring manual. Unpub-
development of morals. New York: Cambridge University Press. lished manuscript, University of Michigan.
Westen, D. (1989). Are "primitive" object relations really pre-oedipal? Westen, D., Ludolph, P., Block, M. J., Wixom, J., & Wiss, E C. (1990).
American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 59, 331-345. Developmental history and object relations in psychiatrically dis-
Westen, D (1990). The relations among narcissism, egocentrism, self- turbed adolescent females. American Journal of Psychiatry, 147,
concept, and self-esteem. Psychoanalysis and Contemporary 1061-1068.
Thought, 13, 185-241. Westen, D., Ludolph, E, Lerner, H., Ruffins, S., & Wiss, C. (1990).
Westen, D (in press-a). Social cognition and object relations. Psycholog- Object relations in borderline adolescents. Journal of the American
ical Bulletin. Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 29, 338-348.
Westen, D (in press-b). Toward a revised theory of borderline object Westen, D., Ludolph, E, Silk, K., Kellam, A., Gold, L., & Lohr, N.
relations: Implications of empirical research. International Journal (1990). Object relations in borderline adolescents and adults: Devel-
of Psycho-Analysis. opmental differences. Adolescent Psychiatry, 17, 360-384.
Westen, D (in press-c). Cognitive-behavioral interventions in the psy-
choanalytic psychotherapy of borderline personality disorders. Received November 13, 1989
Clinical Psychology Review. Revision received February 14, 1990
Westen, D., Klepser, J., Ruffins, S., Silverman, M., Lifton, N., & Boe- Accepted March 6, 1990 •

I n s t r u c t i o n s to A u t h o r s

Preparing Manuscripts. Authors should prepare Brief Reports. Psychological Assessment: A Journal
manuscripts according to the Publication Manual of the of Consulting and Clinical Psychology will accept Brief
American Psychological Association (3rd ed.). Typing in- Reports of research studies in clinical assessment and
structions (all copy must be double-spaced) and instruc- evaluation. The procedure is intended to permit the pub-
tions on preparing tables, figures, references, metrics, lication of carefully designed studies of specialized inter-
and abstracts appear in the Manual. Alos, all manusripts est that cannot now be accepted as regular articles be-
are subject to editing for sexist language. For more infor- cause of lack of space. Several pages in each issue may be
mation, refer to Alan E. Kazdin's editorial in the March, devoted to Brief Reports.
1989 issue (pp. 3-5). An author who submits a Brief Report must agree not
to submit the full report to another journal of general
Publication Policy. APA policy prohibits an author circulation. The Brief Report should give a clear, con-
from submitting the same manuscript for concurrent densed summary of the procedure of the study and as
consideration by two or more journals. APA policy also full an account of the results as space permits. Brief Re-
prohibits duplicate publication, that is, publication of a ports should be limited to three printed pages and pre-
manuscript that has already been published in whole or pared according to the following specifications:
in substantial part in another journal. Prior and dupli- To ensure that a Brief Report does not exceed three
cate publication constitute unethical behavior, and au- printed pages, follow these instructions for typing: (a)
thors have an obligation to consult journal editors if Set typewriter to a 55-space (pica) or 66-space (elite) line,
there is any chance or question that the paper might not with 25 lines per page. (b) Type text. (c) Count all lines
be suitable for publication in an APA journal. Authors except abstract (75-100 words), title, and byline. Include
submitting a manuscript previously considered for pub- acknowledgments, heading, tables, and references. If
lication in another APA journal are invited to inform you have exceeded 325 lines, shorten the material.
the Editor, who will then seek to obtain independent This journal does not require an extended report.
reviews, thus avoiding the possibility of repeated review- However, if one is available, the Brief Report must be
ing by the same consultant. Also, authors of manuscripts accompanied by the following footnote, typed on a sepa-
submitted to APA journals are expected to have avail- rate sheet and not counted in the 325-line quota:
able their raw data throughout the editorial review pro- Correspondence concerning this article (and requests for
cess and for at least 5 years after the date of publication. an extended report of this study) should be addressed to
Ethical Standards. Authors will be required to state [give the author~ full name and address].
in writing that they have complied with APA ethical
standards in the treatment of their sample, human or
animal, or to describe the details of treatment. (A copy
Submitting manuscripts. Manuscripts should be sub-
mitted in triplicate, and all copies should be clear, read-
of the APA Ethical Principles may be obtained from the
able, and on paper of good quality. A dot matrix or un-
APA Ethics Office, 1200 Seventeenth Street, NW, Wash-
usual typeface is acceptable only if it is clear and legible.
ington, DC 20036.)
Dittoed and mimeographed copies are not acceptable
Abstracts. Manuscripts of regular articles must be and will not be considered. Authors should keep a copy
accompanied by an abstract containing a maximum of of the manuscript to guard against loss. Mail manu-
960 characters and spaces (which is approximately 100- scripts to the Editor, Alan E. Kazdin, Psychological As-
150 words). Manuscripts o fBriefReports must be accom- sessment: JCCP, Department of Psychology, Yale Univer-
panied by an abstract of 75-100 words. All abstracts sity, P.O. Box 11A Yale Station, New Haven, CT 06520-
must be typed on a separate sheet of paper. 7447.

View publication stats

You might also like