Booij 2006 Inflection and Derivation Elsevier

You might also like

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 9

Provided for non-commercial research and educational use only.

Not for reproduction or distribution or commercial use

This article was originally published in the Encyclopedia of Language & Linguistics,
Second Edition, published by Elsevier, and the attached copy is provided by Elsevier
for the author's benefit and for the benefit of the author's institution, for non-
commercial research and educational use including without limitation use in
instruction at your institution, sending it to specific colleagues who you know, and
providing a copy to your institution’s administrator.

All other uses, reproduction and distribution, including without limitation commercial
reprints, selling or licensing copies or access, or posting on open internet sites, your
personal or institution’s website or repository, are prohibited. For exceptions,
permission may be sought for such use through Elsevier's permissions site at:

http://www.elsevier.com/locate/permissionusematerial

Booij G (2006), Inflection and Derivation. In: Keith Brown, (Editor-in-Chief)


Encyclopedia of Language & Linguistics, Second Edition, volume 5, pp. 654-661.
Oxford: Elsevier.
654 Inference: Abduction, Induction, Deduction

Any thorough account of natural language under- Chomsky N (1975). Introduction. The logical structure of
standing uses all three kinds of inference. (It must linguistic theory. New York: Plenum Press.
also recognize defeasible pragmatic inferences of Einstein A (1973). Ideas and opinions. Laurel edn. New
the Gricean kind; see Sperber and Wilson, 1995; York: Dell [First published 1954].
Harman G (1999). Reasoning, meaning, and mind. Oxford:
Harman, 1999; Levinson, 2000.)
Clarendon Press [Online at http://www.oxfordscholar-
ship.com].
Hodges W (1977). Logic. Harmondsworth: Penguin.
See also: Abduction (01390); Aristotle and Linguistics; Aris- Kneale W & Kneale M (1962). The development of logic.
totle and the Stoics on Language; Categorial Grammars: Oxford: Clarendon Press.
Deductive Approaches; Default Semantics; Implicature; Levinson S C (2000). Presumptive meanings: the theory of
Metalanguage versus Object Language; Monotonicity generalized conversational implicature. Cambridge, MA:
and Generalized Quantifiers; Nonmonotonic Inference. MIT Press.
Mill J S (1970). A system of logic: ratiocinative and induc-
tive (8th edn.). London: Longman [First published 1872,

y
first edn 1843].
Bibliography
Peirce C S (1940). The philosophy of Peirce: selected

op
Allan K (2003). Linguistic metatheory. Language Sciences writings, Buchler J (ed.). London: Routledge and
25, 533–60. Kegan Paul.
Aristotle (1984). The complete works of Aristotle. The Poincaré H (1946). The foundations of science: science and

C
revised Oxford translation, Barnes J (ed.). Bollingen hypothesis, the value of science, science and method,
Series 71. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press. Halstead G B (trans.). Lancaster, PA: The Science Press.
Bacon F (1620). Novum organum. London: John Bill. Sperber D & Wilson D (1995). Relevance: communication

al
Bloomfield L (1933). Language. New York: Holt, Rinehart and cognition (2nd edn.). Oxford: Basil Blackwell [First
and Winston. on edn., 1986].
rs
Inflection and Derivation
Pe

G Booij, Vrije Universiteit Amsterdam, Amsterdam, of affixation, the attachment of the suffix -er to a base
The Netherlands lexeme.
ß 2006 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved. The distinction between inflection and derivation is
primarily a functional one: it refers to different func-
's

tions of morphological processes, the creation of


different forms of lexemes versus the creation of dif-
Introduction
or

ferent lexemes. The formal means for these different


Inflection and derivation are traditional notions in functions may be the same, as is the case for the
the domain of morphology, the subdiscipline of lin- English examples used here: in both inflection and
th

guistics that deals with the internal structure of derivation, use is made of the process of suffixation.
words. Inflection deals with the different forms of a Another morphological process that may be used in
Au

word. For instance, the English words walk, walks, both inflection and derivation is vowel alternation.
walked, and walking are considered different word For instance, vowel alternation is found in the past
forms of the lexeme WALK. The notion ‘lexeme’ is used tense form of to fall, which has the form fell. The latter
for the more abstract notion ‘word’ and is represented form is also that of the related causative verb for to
by means of small capitals. In a dictionary of English, fall, the verb to fell. Thus, both inflection and deriva-
these four different word forms do not receive sepa- tion are formally different from compounding, the
rate lexical entries, but are dealt with in one entry, morphological process in which lexemes are concate-
that for the verbal lexeme WALK. The noun walker, on nated to form new complex words. The cover term
the other hand, is not considered as a form of the for derivation and compounding is word formation.
lexeme WALK, but as a different lexeme, with a differ- The English plural noun walkers can be decom-
ent meaning and a different lexical category (it is a posed morphologically as follows: walk-er-s. This
noun). It usually has its own lexical entry in a dictio- word consists of the stem walker and the plural end-
nary. The lexeme WALKER is considered to be the prod- ing -s. Thus, the stem of a word is the word form
uct of derivation, the creation of a new lexeme minus its inflectional markers. The stem walker in
through the application of a morphological process turn consists of a simplex stem (that is, a root) and

Encyclopedia of Language & Linguistics (2006), vol. 5, pp. 654–661


Inflection and Derivation 655

the derivational suffix -er. The distinction between feature. For instance, there is no overt number mark-
word and stem is the main formal correlate of the ing for English singular nouns. There are also nouns
distinction between inflection and derivation. Nor- that, for semantic reasons, do not have plural forms,
mally, the creation of new lexemes is performed on such as milk and abstractness. These nouns must nev-
the basis of the stem form of a lexeme, not on the basis ertheless be considered singular nouns because they
of one of its inflectional forms. For instance, the Ital- trigger singular number agreement with verbs. In con-
ian noun macchina ‘car’ consists of a stem macchin- trast, no obligatory morphological expression is
and an inflectional ending -a. The word macchin-ista involved in using the agent noun for the verb walk,
‘driver’ is derived by adding the suffix -ista to the stem. that is, the word walker, with the suffix -er that creates
If we used the full word form macchina, the wrong de-verbal agent nouns. The use of this word is a choice
form macchina-ista would have been derived. made by the language user for purely semantic rea-
A consequence of the distinction between inflection sons. Hence, we consider walker a case of derivation.
and derivation is that we expect inflection to be pe-
Syntactic Relevance
ripheral to derivation; derivation creates stems from

y
stems, and subsequently, inflection creates concrete A second criterion for the demarcation of inflection

op
word forms from stems. This expectation is confirmed and derivation is that of syntactic relevance; inflec-
by Greenberg’s Universal 28, which reads as follows: tion has been defined as that part of morphology that
is relevant to syntax (Anderson, 1992). Particular
Universal 28. If both the derivation and the inflection

C
word forms may be required by syntactic context.
follow the root, or they both precede the root, the deri- This is the case for syntactic configurations in which
vation is always between the root and the inflection
agreement between constituents is required. The rule
(Greenberg, 1963: 93).

al
of subject–verb agreement in English, for instance,
Three different issues concerning the distinction
onindicates that a specific verb form is required by the
between inflection and derivation are topics of debate subject of the clause, a verb form with the same prop-
among linguists. The first issue is that of the formal erties for the categories number and person. In Dutch
criteria for distinguishing between these two notions. and German (Standard German), a pre-nominal ad-
jective must agree in gender, number, and definiteness
rs
How do we know whether a certain morphological
process belongs to the domain of inflection or to that with the nominal phrase of which it forms a part.
of derivation? Is there a sharp boundary between the Noun phrases may have to carry a specific case
Pe

two categories? The second issue is that linguists have depending on their syntactic function. In Latin, the
discussed which properties are typical for inflection subject is marked by nominative case and the direct
and which ones for derivation. For instance, it has object by accusative case. In the case of rection (or
been claimed that inflection is more productive than government), words require a specific form of the
's

derivation. The third issue is that of the position of words in the phrase that they govern. German pre-
inflection and derivation in the grammar. Do they positions, for instance, require a specific case form of
or

belong to the same morphological module of the the words in the nominal phrases that they govern.
grammar or are they different modules? These three This does not mean, however, that inflection is
always governed by syntax. In Latin, for instance,
th

issues will be dealt with in the following sections of


this article. the accusative form of the word Roma ‘Rome’ is
Romam. This is the form to be used when this noun
Au

functions as the direct object of a clause, but it can


Formal Criteria also function as an adverbial phrase, with the meaning
‘to Rome.’ In the latter case, the accusative form is not
Obligatoriness
required by syntactic context, but a choice made by
A first criterion for distinguishing between inflection the language user in order to express a particular piece
and derivation is that inflection is obligatory, whereas of information. This is called semantic case. The same
derivation is optional. For instance, each English noun applies to the use of plural number for the noun BOOK
must be marked as either singular or plural. Hence, the in John read these books. The choice for the plural
category number is an obligatory category of English, form of book is not dictated by the syntactic context.
and hence inflectional. Similarly, in languages with It should be realized, however, that one cannot
case systems, each noun must be marked for a specific claim that derivation has no relevance to syntax
case. In most languages, verbs are marked obligatorily whatsoever. Since derivation may indicate a change
for a specific tense and often for person and number of of syntactic category, this fact in itself is already of
the subject of the clause. Note that this is true even if relevance to syntax. In the case of derivation of verbs,
there is no overt marking for a particular inflectional the derived verb may also have a specific syntactic

Encyclopedia of Language & Linguistics (2006), vol. 5, pp. 654–661


656 Inflection and Derivation

valence. For instance, the derivation of causative Participles can function as pronominal adjectives
verbs leads to the creation of transitive verbs that while at the same time keeping their verbal proper-
indicate the obligatory presence of a direct object, as ties, as illustrated by the following German example
in John whitened the walls. Hence, the derivation of (Haspelmath, 1996: 44):
causative verbs has syntactic relevance. The differ-
(2) der im Wald laut singende Wanderer
ence with inflection is that the choice of a particular the in.the forest loud singing hiker
derived word is not governed by syntactic context. ‘the hiker who is singing loud in the forest’
The present participle singend is inflected as a pre-
Change of Syntactic Category
nominal adjective with the inflectional ending -e,
A third criterion of demarcation is that derivation whereas its co-occurrence with a locative adverbial
may change the syntactic category of its input im Wald and the adverbial laut indicates that it still
forms, whereas inflection is always category-neutral. has verbal valence as well. Thus, inflection may have
This criterion reflects the idea that inflection creates effects on syntactic category. In contrast to the situa-

y
forms of the same lexeme, unlike derivation, which tion for derivation, in inflection the syntactic category

op
determines the lexical category of the lexemes it of its inputs is preserved.
creates. For instance, the Dutch diminutive suffix
-(t)je is considered to be a derivational suffix since Paradigms
it always creates nouns, whatever the lexical cat-

C
Inflection is typically associated with paradigms.
egory of its input: hond ‘N, dog’ - hond-je ‘N, A paradigm is an abstract pattern of cells, with each
doggy’; blond ‘A, blond’ - blond-je ‘N, blond girl’; cell having a particular value for one or more inflec-

al
speel ‘V, to play’ - speel-tje ‘N, toy.’ tional categories such as number, case, tense, or as-
Note that this does not indicate that derivation pect. For each lexeme of a particular lexical category,
must be category-changing. When we add the suffix
on the cells of the paradigm are filled with particular
-er to the noun London, we derive a word of the same word forms. This paradigmatic organization of the
nominal category. Yet, this is a clear case of derivation inflectional forms of a lexeme reflects the idea that
since Londoner and London are two different lex- inflection is obligatory in the sense discussed above.
rs
emes. They also belong to two different subcategories Of course, one may also organize the set of deriva-
of nouns: whereas London is a proper name that tionally related lexemes into a paradigm. For in-
Pe

cannot be preceded by a determiner (*the London), stance, for English verbs one might assume a
Londoner is a count noun (the Londoners). paradigmatic cell for ‘agent noun.’ However, there
The evaluative morphology of some languages is a are many English verbs for which this cell might be
problem for this criterion. In Italian, for instance, hard or impossible to fill, as is the case for verbs such
's

evaluative suffixes create lexemes of the same lexical as to die and to fall. This stands in contrast with
category as their inputs: tavolo ‘N, table’ – tavolino inflection where the cells of paradigms are (almost)
or

‘N, small table’; giallo ‘A, yellow’ – giall-ino ‘A, yel- always filled. An exception to this generalization is
lowish’ (Scalise, 1986). That is, this suffix is transpar- that nouns may lack plural forms, mainly for seman-
ent to the lexical category of its inputs. Yet it looks like tic reasons. In Russian, some verbs have defective
th

derivation because it creates new words with mean- paradigms, with one or more empty cells.
ings that are distinct from those of the input words. The notions of ‘suppletion’ and ‘periphrasis’ that
Au

Inversely, there are forms of inflection that are not are used in the analysis of inflectional systems also
completely category-neutral. This applies, for in- reflect the idea of obligatoriness and the idea of a
stance, to the infinite forms of verbs such as partici- paradigmatic organization of inflectional forms. For
ples and infinitives (Haspelmath, 1996). Infinitives, instance, if we say that went is the suppletive past tense
for instance, are forms of verbal lexemes. Yet they form of to go, this presupposes that the past-tense cells
exhibit both verbal and nominal properties. They can of the verb to go must be filled by some word form.
combine with determiners and adjectives, which is The same applies to worse, which is considered the
typical of nouns. At the same time, they exhibit verbal suppletive comparative form of bad. On the other
properties such as co-occurrence with prepositionless hand, there is no particular reason that we would
nominal phrases, as in the following Dutch sentence: want to consider thief as the suppletive agent noun of
(1) Het je moeder cadeautjes gev-en the verb to steal, instead of the derived noun stealer.
the your mother presents give.INF We speak of periphrasis if one or more cells of a
moet afgelopen zijn paradigm cannot be filled by a particular word form.
should finished be Instead, a combination of words must be used. For
‘You should stop giving presents to your mother’ instance, in Latin there are no synthetic forms for the

Encyclopedia of Language & Linguistics (2006), vol. 5, pp. 654–661


Inflection and Derivation 657

passive perfect of verbs. Whereas the third-person Another characteristic of inflection is that inflec-
singular present-passive form of the verb laudare ‘to tional paradigms may make use of more than one
praise’ is laudatur ‘(s)he is praised,’ the perfect coun- stem for computing the different forms of a lexeme.
terpart cannot be created by morphology and is That is, there is formal variation without concomitant
expressed by a combination of a participle and a grammatical or semantic differences. The Latin verb
form of the word esse ‘to be’: laudatus est ‘(s)he has ponere ‘to put’ has three different stem forms, pone-,
been praised.’ posu-, and posit-, as in pone-o ‘I put,’ posu-i ‘I have
Related to the notion paradigm is the observation put,’ and posit-us ‘put, past participle.’ This is no
on inflection that many languages have different in- exclusive property of inflection: stem allomorphy
flectional classes for the paradigms of lexemes. In also occurs in derivation, as illustrated by the English
Latin, for instance, there are five different declension- word pair drama/dramatic, where the adjective
al classes for nouns. This means that there are five has been derived from a second stem form dramat-
different ways of computing the different word forms (Aronoff, 1994).
of the cells of a Latin noun. Many languages have

y
different conjugations for the computation of verbal Semantic Differences

op
word forms. This illustrates that the relation between
form and morphosyntactic information in the realm Is there any difference between the semantic cate-
of inflection might be quite complicated. Another gories expressed by inflection and those expressed

C
example of this complex relation between form and by derivation? Inflection is used cross-linguistically
meaning in the domain of inflection is that more than for a number of categories:
one inflectional property is often expressed by one . Nouns: number, gender, definiteness, case

al
morpheme. In Indo-European languages, case and . Verbs: valency, tense, aspect, mood, person, num-
number are usually expressed by the same morpheme. on ber, gender
The morpheme -i of Latin hort-i ‘garden, GEN.SG’ . Adjectives: degree, number, gender, case, definite-
expresses both number and case. It is therefore a case ness.
of multiple exponence: two morphosyntactic proper-
ties are expressed by one and the same morpheme. Derivation, on the other hand, is used for a much
rs
The derivational morphology of Indo-European lan- wider range of semantic categories. In this respect,
guages, on the other hand, is usually agglutinative, derivation is closer to the lexical expression of
Pe

with each property being expressed by a separate meaning than inflection.


morpheme. Thus, inflection and derivation may differ Bybee (1985) has proposed to interpret the seman-
as to their formal morphological properties. tic difference between inflection and derivation in
These distinguishing formal properties of inflection terms of the notion ‘semantic relevance’. ‘‘A meaning
's

have led to formal models of inflection in which element is relevant to another meaning element if the
the paradigm plays a central role such as the Word- semantic content of the first directly affects or modi-
or

and-Paradigm Theory of Matthews (1972) and the fies the semantic content of the second’’ (Bybee, 1985:
realizational theories of inflectional morphology of 13). Bybee argued that there are two factors that
Anderson (1992) and Stump (2001). In realizational determine whether a certain notion is expressed in-
th

theories, inflectional rules are seen as operations that flectionally or derivationally: relevance and generali-
spell out the phonological form of a lexeme for each ty. The less relevant a category is for the root, and the
Au

combination of morphosyntactic features that may be more general it is, the better it lends itself to inflec-
assigned to that lexeme. That is, they indicate how a tional expression. The inverse applies to derivation.
particular array of morphosyntactic features is real- For instance, the meaning element ‘causative’ is usu-
ized. It is an open issue in present-day morphological ally expressed by derivational means (if not by lexical
theory to what extent the realizational model is an means) because that meaning is very relevant to the
appropriate model for derivational morphology as meaning of the stem. On the other hand, an inflec-
well. In many ways, derivation is structurally similar tional category, such as tense, is not so much of direct
to compounding, which suggests a primarily syntag- relevance to verbal stems, but locates the state of
matic analysis of derived words, with affixes being affairs expressed by a clause with respect to the time
concatenated to stems. For instance, there is a strong of speaking. Hence, tense does not modify the mean-
structural similarity between compounds and English ing of the verbal stem and has a deictic function
derived words such as help-ful and category-wise, instead. Correlating with this semantic distinction,
and the meaning of these words can be derived we see that the morphological expression of tense in
straightforwardly from those of their constituent paradigms is quite general, whereas the morphologi-
morphemes. cal expression of causative may be possible for a

Encyclopedia of Language & Linguistics (2006), vol. 5, pp. 654–661


658 Inflection and Derivation

restricted number of verbal roots only. Inflection of A psycholinguistic difference that is sometimes
the agreement type such as person-number marking claimed to correlate with the distinction between
on verbs and gender marking on adjectives and verbs (regular) inflection and derivation is that between
has no obvious relevance to the meaning of the computation and storage. The idea is the following.
stem, and hence this kind of contextually determined Regular inflectional forms are processed on line: they
morphology is the prototypical case of inflection. are created by the speaker and parsed (decomposed)
According to Bybee (1985), this gradual semantic by the hearer. Hence, they may be assumed to be
difference between derivation and the different kinds computed. Derived words, on the other hand, are
of inflection correlates with the order in which deri- stored as such in the mental lexicon and are retrieved
vational and inflectional elements occur in a word: as wholes from that lexicon in production and per-
derivational elements are closer to the root than in- ception. This assumption concerns the differences in
flectional elements because they have a higher seman- semantic regularity between inflection and derivation
tic relevance. In addition, the order of inflectional observed above; storage is often essential for assigning
elements in an inflectional word form reflects differ- the right meaning to a derived word or for choosing

y
ent degrees of relevance. For instance, since the cat- the correct derived word, whereas regular inflectional

op
egory of aspect is more relevant for the meaning of word forms can be computed. Irregular inflectional
the stem than tense, tense morphemes tend to be forms, on the other hand, must be stored in the lexi-
peripheral to aspectual morphemes. Person and num- con, for obvious reasons. For instance, the past tense

C
ber markings on verbs (necessary for reasons of form of walk is the regular, and hence predictable,
agreement only) are peripheral to tense marking. form walked, whereas the past tense form of the verb
to fall, the word form fell, is not predictable. This idea

al
is referred to as the Dual Mechanism Theory of mor-
Properties of Inflection and Derivation
phological processing (Pinker, 1999; Clahsen, 1999).
Inflectional processes are said to be fully productive,
on The Dual Mechanism Theory is based on the role
whereas derivational processes may exhibit different of frequency in lexical decision tasks. In such tasks,
degrees of productivity. This seems to be a natural subjects must decide whether a particular word
corollary of the fact that inflection is obligatory. (form) is correct. The frequency of occurrence of a
rs
However, this is not completely true, since there word (form) may play a role in performing such tasks.
might be competing inflectional processes with the The response latencies in lexical decision tasks corre-
Pe

same function, some of which may be unproductive. late with frequency: a more frequent word will be
English plural nouns are created by the productive recognized faster and will thus have shorter response
process of suffixation with one of the allomorphs of latencies than a word of low frequency. This frequen-
the suffix /z/, but there are also closed sets of nouns cy effect is interpreted in terms of activation level.
's

with a different plural forms, such as nouns of Greek A word with a high frequency has a higher initial
origin in -on, as in prolegomenon/prolegomena. The level of activation in the mental lexicon than a word
or

past tense of English verbs is normally formed by with low frequency. Frequency effects presuppose the
adding -ed to the verbal stem, but there is also a set storage of the word (forms) involved. Hence, if there
of verbs of which the past tense form is created are no frequency effects for a particular word (form),
th

by means of vowel change, as in ride/rode. Hence, one may conclude that it is not stored in lexical
inflectional rules can be unproductive. memory. Pinker (1999) and Clahsen (1999) argued,
Au

Inflectional forms of lexemes are usually semanti- for English and German, respectively, that regular
cally regular, whereas derived words may exhibit all inflectional forms do not exhibit frequency effects,
sorts of semantic idiosyncrasies. The derived word unlike irregular ones. This is explained by the assump-
baker, for example, does not just denote ‘one who tion that only irregular inflectional forms are stored.
bakes,’ but is used to denote a certain profession. According to Clahsen et al. (2003), derived words
The de-verbal noun dwelling, as in my humble dwell- always show frequency effects, even the regular ones.
ing, does not denote the act of dwelling, but a loca- Hence, derived words must be assumed to always
tion. Semantic idiosyncrasies in inflectional forms are be stored. Hence, the Dual Mechanism Theory should
much rarer, but do exist. For instance, the English be restricted to the domain of inflection.
plural form brethren is irregular, both formally and However, it has been shown in more recent psycho-
semantically, since it does not simply mean ‘brothers,’ linguistic research that regular inflectional forms are
but is used to denote male members of a religious also stored in the lexicon when they have a high fre-
community. In Dutch, the plural form of the noun quency of occurrence (Stemberger and MacWhinney,
letter ‘letter,’ letteren, is used to denote the scientific 1988; Baayen et al., 1997) since such regular forms
domain of arts and humanities. also show frequency effects. In other words, the fact

Encyclopedia of Language & Linguistics (2006), vol. 5, pp. 654–661


Inflection and Derivation 659

that a correct inflectional form can be computed ordered in two or more levels. Inflectional rules are
without lexical information does not necessarily indi- located at the last level of the lexical component of
cate that the language user may not also make use of the grammar and word formation at a preceding
his or her vast lexical memory in the case of inflection one. Hence, inflection is predicted to be peripheral
and store and retrieve such complex forms. to derivation. However, irregular inflectional forms
A related psycholinguistic distinction between in- such as the plural form lice of the noun louse may be
flection and derivation is the family size effect formed at an earlier level, because in English, irregu-
(Baayen et al., 1997). In the case of derivation, what lar plurals may occur within compounds, as in
counts in predicting differences in response latency lice-infested. Although it may be true for English
between derived words is not so much the token that only irregular plurals feed word formation, this
frequency of the word forms for that word, but rather restriction does not apply to related languages such
the number of morphologically related words (related as Dutch. For instance, Dutch has compounds such
in terms of derivational processes and compounding), as scholen-gemeenschap ‘lit. school-s community,
that is, the size of its morphological family. The more comprehensive school’ with the regular plural noun

y
family members a word has, the higher its initial level scholen ‘schools’ in first position. This compound

op
of activation in the mental lexicon. contrasts with the Dutch compound school-
Differences between inflection and derivation have gemeenschap ‘school community,’ with the singular
also been found in studies of language users who or stem form of school in first position.

C
suffer from certain forms of aphasia. For instance, Therefore, the hypothesis of level ordering and,
Badecker and Caramazza (1989) investigated the more generally, the hypothesis of Split Morphology
language of an Italian aphatic and discovered that are empirically inadequate as a general theory of

al
he made many inflectional errors, but almost no deri- how inflection and derivation interact, since it cat-
vational ones. On the other hand, there are also on egorically excludes inflectional forms to form inputs
speakers with agrammatism (aphatics with poor syn- for derivation, which is empirically incorrect (Booij,
tax and almost no function words) whose inflectional 1994, 1996). For instance, in many languages parti-
morphology is not affected and also preserved as their ciples, which have both verbal and adjectival proper-
derivational morphology (De Bleser and Bayer, 1988). ties, can be used as stems for de-adjectival word
rs
A survey of possible psycholinguistic differences formation, as in English excitedness, in which the
between inflection and derivation can be found in past participle feeds derivation of nouns by means
Pe

Bertinetto (1995). of the suffix -ness. In Dutch, the comparative forms


of adjectives can be used as stems as well, as in the
Morpheme Order and the Theory verb ver-erg-er ‘to worsen’ derived from the compar-
ative form erg-er of the adjective erg ‘bad’. Since
of Split Morphology
's

comparative forms are usually classified as inflection-


The functional differences between inflection and al forms, this also forms a problem for the Split
or

derivation and their formal correlates have led some Morphology Hypothesis. It has also been observed
linguists to propose the Theory of Split Morphology that plural forms of nouns may function as stems in
(Perlmutter, 1988; Anderson, 1992). In this theory, derivation. This is, for example, the case in Dutch,
th

derivation is assumed to be accounted for by a pre- where the collective suffix -dom is added to plural
syntactic component of the grammar. This component nouns, as in held-en-dom ‘heroism’ with the morpho-
Au

generates derived lexemes in their stem forms, which logical structure ROOT-PLURAL-COLLECTIVE. The root
are then inserted into syntactic structure as created by form is the noun held ‘hero’, which is followed by
the syntactic component. Inflection, on the other the regular plural suffix -en. Similar observations on
hand, is accounted for in a post-syntactic component, Spanish and Portuguese can be found in Rainer
because the choice of the correct inflectional form of a (1996).
lexeme depends on the syntactic context in which it Compounding, another major type of word forma-
occurs. A consequence of this organization of the tion, also appears to use plural nouns as building
grammar is that inflectional elements will be periph- blocks, as in Italian lava-piatti ‘dish-washer,’ a combi-
eral to derivational elements, a prediction that is in nation of the verbal stem lava ‘to wash’ and the plural
accordance with Greenberg’s Universal 28 mentioned noun piatti ‘dishes.’ Note that this compound has a
above. singular meaning even though one of its constituents
An earlier variant of Split Morphology is the theory is plural.
of level ordering, as proposed for English by Kiparsky Some linguists deny an absolute distinction between
(1985). In this approach, all morphology is pre- inflection and derivation and consider them to form
syntactic. However, the set of morphological rules is one continuum (Bybee, 1985; Dressler, 1989). In this

Encyclopedia of Language & Linguistics (2006), vol. 5, pp. 654–661


660 Inflection and Derivation

view, interactions between derivation and inflection In a number of languages, verbs may be preceded
of the kind discussed above come as no surprise since by so-called preverbs, which look like derivational
it is the kind of inflection that is close to derivation prefixes. Classical examples of such word combina-
that may feed derivation. tions are the particle verbs of Germanic languages.
Although the Split Morphology Hypothesis faces For instance, German anrufen ‘to phone’ looks like a
certain problems, we must somehow account for the word since it has a specific idiosyncratic meaning.
fact that derivation usually takes stems as their However, the particle an can be separated from the
inputs, that is, words minus their inflectional ele- verb in main clauses, as in Johann ruft seine Mutter an
ments. As pointed out above, that is the main formal ‘John calls his mother.’ The past participle of anrufen
reason for keeping inflection and derivation apart. In is an-ge-ruf-en, with the inflectional prefix ge- before
order to solve this paradoxical situation, we might the root and after the particle. We might therefore
distinguish between two types of inflection, inherent think that this is a case in which a derivational prefix
inflection and contextual inflection. An example of precedes an inflectional one, thus forming an excep-
inherent inflection is the formation of plural nouns. In tion to Greenberg’s Universal 28. However, anrufen is

y
most contexts, the use of the plural form of a noun is not a word, but a lexicalized word combination, as is

op
not required by syntactic context, but a purely seman- clear from the fact that it can be split in main clauses.
tic choice based on what the language user wants to Consequently, the position of the inflectional prefix
convey. The choice of specific finite forms of verbs, ge- is as expected. Thus, if inflectional elements ap-

C
and of case forms of nouns, on the other hand, is pear in between verbal stems and preverbs, this is no
governed mostly by syntactic context and then qua- counterexample to the generalization that inflection
lifies as contextual inflection. Participles and infini- tends to be peripheral with respect to derivation.

al
tives also qualify as cases of inherent inflection. The Detailed studies of such preverb–verb combinations
crucial observation is that it is only inherent inflection on and their interaction with inflection can be found in
that can feed word formation processes such as deri- Booij and van Marle (2003).
vation and compounding (Booij, 1994, 1996). This In sum, it is only in very specific circumstances that
suggests that inherent inflection is halfway between inflectional morphemes can appear inside derivational
derivation and contextual inflection. This is in line morphemes. Hence, Greenberg’s Universal 28 remains
rs
with the observation that inherent inflection may an important generalization about the relation
have class-changing effects, just like derivation, as between inflection and derivation.
Pe

observed above. In some psycholinguistic experi-


ments, this distinction between inherent and contex-
tual inflection is reflected by differences in frequency See also: Affixation; A-Morphous Morphology; Compound;
effects. For instance, Dutch verbal plurals are differ- Dual-Mechanism Morphology; Internal Modification; Lex-
's

ent from noun plurals as to frequency effects in lexical ical Phonology and Morphology; Morphology and Lan-
guage Processing; Morphotactics; Paradigm Function
decision tasks. In certain tasks, verbs do not show a
Morphology; Productivity.
or

frequency effect, unlike nouns (Baayen et al., 1997,


2003).
Yet, even inherent inflection cannot feed deriva-
th

tion, or more generally, word formation, on all occa- Bibliography


sions. For instance, the Dutch de-nominal suffix -er, Anderson S (1992). A-morphous morphology. Cambridge:
Au

which forms nouns, cannot be attached to plural Cambridge University Press.


nouns. The noun wetenschapp-er ‘scientist’ derived Aronoff M (1994). Morphology by itself: Stems and inflec-
from the noun wetenschap ‘science’ is well formed, tional classes. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
but we cannot add this suffix to the plural noun Baayen R H & Schreuder R (eds.) (2003). Morphological
wetenschapp-en although this is an existing and well- structure in language processing. Berlin/New York:
formed plural noun of Dutch: *wetenschappen-er. Mouton de Gruyter.
The crucial condition in the case of Dutch appears to Baayen H, Dijkstra T & Schreuder R (1997). ‘Singulars and
be that inflectional elements must appear at the right plurals in Dutch: Evidence for a parallel dual route
model.’ Journal of Memory and Language 36, 94–117.
edge of a prosodic word (Booij, 2002). Hence, plural
Baayen R H, Lieber R & Schreuder R (1997). ‘The mor-
suffix can appear word-internally before suffixes phological complexity of simplex nouns.’ Linguistics 35,
such as -achtig ‘-like’ because the stem of such suffixes 861–877.
forms a prosodic word of its own, as in boeken-achtig Baayen H, McQueen J, Dijkstra T & Schreuder R (2003).
‘books-like, bookish’, which consists of the prosodic ‘Frequency effects in regular inflectional morphology:
words (buken) and (Axtex). Therefore, the plural noun Revisiting Dutch plurals.’ In Baayen & Schreuder (eds.)
boeken ‘books’ can appear before the suffix -achtig. 355–390.

Encyclopedia of Language & Linguistics (2006), vol. 5, pp. 654–661


Influence of Literacy on Language Development 661

Badecker W & Caramazza A (1989). ‘A lexical distinction Sprachwissenschaft und Kommunikationsforschung 42,
between inflection and derivation.’ Linguistic Inquiry 20, 3–10.
108–116. Greenberg J (1963). ‘Some universals of grammar with
Bertinetto P M (1995). ‘Compositionality and non- particular reference to the order of meaningful elements.’
compositionality in morphology.’ In Dressler W U & In Universals of language. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Burani C (eds.) Cross-disciplinary approaches to mor- 73–113.
phology. Vienna: Verlag der Oesterreichischen Akademie Hammond M & Noonan M (eds.) (1988). Theoretical
der Wissenschaften. 9–36. morphology. San Diego: Academic Press.
Booij G (1994). ‘Against split morphology.’ In Booij G & Haspelmath M (1996). ‘Wordclass-changing inflection
van Marle J (eds.) Yearbook of Morphology 1993. and morphological theory.’ In Booij & van Marle (eds.)
Dordrecht: Kluwer. 27–50. 43–66.
Booij G (1996). ‘Inherent versus contextual inflection and Kiparsky P (1985). ‘Some consequences of lexical pho-
the split morphology hypothesis.’ In Booij & van Marle nology.’ Phonology Yearbook 2, 85–138.
(eds.) 1–16. Matthews P H (1972). Inflectional morphology: A theoret-
Booij G (2002). ‘Prosodic restrictions on stacking up ical study based on Latin verb conjugation. Cambridge:

y
affixes.’ In Booij G & van Marle J (eds.) Yearbook of Cambridge University Press.
Morphology 2001. Dordrecht: Kluwer. 183–202. Perlmutter D (1988). ‘The split morphology hypothesis:

op
Booij G & van Marle J (eds.) (1996). Yearbook of Mor- Evidence from Yiddish.’ In Hammond & Noonan (eds.)
phology 1995. Dordrecht: Kluwer. 79–100.
Booij G & van Marle J (eds.) (2003). Yearbook of Mor- Pinker S (1999). Words and rules. New York: Basic Books.

C
phology 2003. Dordrecht: Kluwer. Rainer F (1996). ‘Inflection inside derivation: Evidence
Bybee J (1985). Morphology: A study of the relation from Spanish and Portuguese.’ In Booij & van Marle
between meaning and form. Amsterdam/Philadelphia: (eds.) 83–92.

al
Benjamins. Scalise S (1986). ‘Inflection and derivation.’ Linguistics 22,
Clahsen H (1999). ‘Lexical entries and rules of language.’ 561–581.
Behavorial and Brain Sciences 22, 991–1060. Stemberger J P & MacWhinney B (1988). ‘Are inflected
Clahsen H, Sonnenstuhl I & Blevins J P (2003). ‘Deriva-
on forms stored in the lexicon?’ In Hammond & Noonan
tional morphology in the German mental lexicon: (eds.) 101–116.
A dual mechanism account.’ In Baayen & Schreuder Stump G (1990). ‘Breton inflection and the split morpholo-
rs
(eds.) 125–157. gy hypothesis.’ In Hendrick R (ed.) The syntax of the
de Bleser R & Bayer J (1988). ‘On the role of inflectional modern Celtic languages. San Diego: Academic Press.
morphology in agrammatism.’ In Hammond & Noonan 97–119.
Pe

(eds.) 45–69. Stump G (2001). Inflectional morphology: A theory of


Dressler W U (1989). ‘Prototypical differences between paradigm structure. Cambridge: Cambridge University
inflection and derivation.’ Zeitschrift für Phonetik, Press.
's
or
th

Influence of Literacy on Language Development


J Miller, University of Auckland, Auckland, to spend many years subjected to rigorous formal
Au

New Zealand education in which the teaching of complex written


ß 2006 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved. language is central. In spite of this, many literacy
practices are at odds with practices found in formal
education. The differences between spoken and writ-
ten language bear directly on theories of first-
What Is Literacy?
language acquisition. In grammar and vocabulary,
Literacy is a complex phenomenon with linguistic, written language is far more complex than unplanned
social, educational, and political aspects. It includes spoken language. Complex constructions are ac-
the roles of written language in various social groups quired after the age of seven and much later, and the
and settings, the structure of different types of study of literacy and written vs. spoken language does
text, and the tension between nonstandard and stand- not support theories of rich innate linguistic knowl-
ard varieties. A central role belongs to the social edge.
capital residing in the mastery of complex written One view of literacy is that it concerns how chil-
language. Every literate society obliges its children dren learn to connect letters and sounds and that it is

Encyclopedia of Language & Linguistics (2006), vol. 5, pp. 654–661

You might also like