Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 2

Biological

Commentary Psychiatry:
CNNI

Cognitive Training in Addiction: Does It Have


Clinical Potential?
Reinout W. Wiers

Two broad classes of cognitive training can be distinguished drinkers with relatively strong positive alcohol associations, in
that have been tested as (additional) interventions in addiction whom an effect was found for WM training on successful
and other psychopathology (1): training of general cognitive reduction of alcohol use (4), and for AUD patients with strong
abilities, such as working memory (WM) or inhibition, of which delay discounting (who tend to prefer immediate rewards over
an interesting example can be found by Snider et al. (2) in the larger future rewards), who showed a stronger effect on this
present issue, and training in which reactions to disorder- variable (2). This latter finding partly replicated an earlier
relevant stimuli are trained, different varieties of cognitive finding, where patients addicted to stimulants showed a
bias modification (CBM). What is the current state of affairs decrease in delay discounting after WM training versus control
regarding their effectiveness, and what do we know regarding training (3). In the present study (2), this was further elaborated
working mechanisms and for whom it may work? by showing rate-dependent WM training effects on episodic
Snider et al. (2) did not find overall effects on the targeted future thinking, which involves vividly imagining future events.
addiction (alcohol use disorder [AUD]), which is in line with As Snider et al. (2) eloquently phrase it, addicted patients are
other reports of absence of overall WM training effects on often “trapped in a narrow temporal window,” and WM training
clinical outcomes in addiction (3–5). Therefore, in this article, may be a way to expand this window, which may ultimately
the first reaction to the question above would appear to be a help to find a way out of the addiction and “remember the
clear “no.” However, Snider et al. (2) emphasize that the pre- future.”
sent study was not designed as a clinical trial but instead as a Why then has no study found overall effects on the clinical
“use-inspired basic research study,” with a focus on cognitive outcome measures for WM training in addiction (2–5)? This
rather than clinical outcomes [for additional discussion on state of affairs contrasts with findings of the other branch of
experimental studies vs. clinical trials, see (6)]. Furthermore, cognitive training (CBM) in AUD, where several studies in
they focused on rate-dependent cognitive outcomes, which patients found clinically relevant effects including less relapse
implies that the baseline scores are considered in assessing a year after treatment discharge [see (6) for a recent review]. As
training effects. One issue with this correlation is that the noted by Snider et al. (2), a general problem with cognitive
change score already contains the baseline score (i.e., training of the first type (training of general abilities) is gener-
the outcome score minus the baseline score), and therefore the alization. From Thorndike on, the existence of “far transfer” has
use of the Oldham correlation is recommended and the been questioned; generalization of training efforts are typically
application of this method in this literature is a step forward. only found to domains that share features with the trained task
Despite the overall negative results regarding main effects on and not in domains that do not (7). I would argue that from this
clinical outcomes (2–5), some positive outcomes have been perspective, the finding that WM training has a rate-dependent
reported, which suggests that WM training may work for a positive effect on delay discounting and episodic future
subgroup of people suffering from addiction: for problem thinking should be regarded as an instance of close rather than

Figure 1. Different types of cognitive training in


addiction.

SEE CORRESPONDING ARTICLE ON PAGE 160

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bpsc.2017.12.008 ª 2017 Society of Biological Psychiatry. Published by Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved. 101
ISSN: 2451-9022 Biological Psychiatry: Cognitive Neuroscience and Neuroimaging February 2018; 3:101–102 www.sobp.org/BPCNNI
Biological
Psychiatry:
CNNI Commentary

as far transfer (as it is presented here), which would also fit with cues would make the training more therapeutically meaning-
the observed overlap in “neural machinery” between WM and ful. Moreover, providing patients with feedback on their neu-
episodic future thinking (2). Given these findings and despite rocognitive improvements can have further motivating effects.
the overall lack of clinical effectiveness observed, I agree with Finally, cognitive training could also be combined with
the authors that these generalized effects of delay discounting neurostimulation (10), with transcranial direct current stimula-
and future episodic thinking are also promising from a thera- tion being the most accessible variety for clinical purposes, for
peutic perspective. WM and episodic future thinking are likely which increases in WM and decreases in craving have been
important abilities needed to successfully curb an addiction; reported (10). But like WM training and CBM, it appears to be
setting goals incompatible with continued substance use and crucial to combine these interventions with general therapeutic
vividly imagining the associated desirable outcomes is an interventions that use the increased capacity to disengage
important ingredient of change (8). Furthermore, increased WM from the temptation now to achieve therapeutic goals and
capacity can help to shield these long-term goals from inter- remember the virtues of a future without drugs.
fering cues triggering short-term goals (e.g., substance cues).
This further raises the question of why the observed increases
Acknowledgments and Disclosures
in WM and episodic thinking did not result in effects in treat-
I thank Professors Hilde Huizenga and Han van der Maas for sharing their
ment outcome.
perspective on Oldham’s correlation.
Apart from statistical power issues [the effects of CBM on The author reports no biomedical financial interests or potential conflicts
reduced relapse in AUD patients were observed in much larger of interest.
samples—see (6)], I argue that to make general cognitive
training clinically effective, transfer to the relevant context
needs to be fostered. If you train in chess, you do not get Article Information
better in WM or music (7); if a patient’s WM is successfully From the Addiction Development and Psychopathology (ADAPT) Labora-
increased through training, this is unlikely to directly translate tory, Department of Psychology, University of Amsterdam, Amsterdam, The
Netherlands.
to effects on abstinence from alcohol or other substances. For
Address correspondence to Reinout W. Wiers, Ph.D., University of
the generalized effects found here on delay discounting and Amsterdam, Psychology, Postbus 15916, Amsterdam 1001 NK, The
episodic future thinking (2) to result in clinical effects, the Netherlands; E-mail: r.wiers@uva.nl.
training could first be related to therapeutically relevant goals. Received Dec 22, 2017; accepted Dec 22, 2017.
That would call for future studies aligning WM training with
motivational interventions, such as motivational interviewing
(8), or with other interventions, such as goal management References
training: the widening temporal window of the patient should 1. Wiers RW, Gladwin TE, Hofmann W, Salemink E, Ridderinkhof KR
be used to strengthen long-term goals incompatible with (2013): Cognitive bias modification and cognitive control training in
addiction and related psychopathology: Mechanisms, clinical
continuation of the addiction, and with vividly imagining the
perspectives, and ways forward. Clin Psychol Sci 1:192–212.
associated positive changes. 2. Snider SE, Deshpande HU, Lisinski JM, Koffarnus MN, LaConte SM,
A second possibility to foster generalization is to link the Bickel WK (2018): Working memory training improves alcohol users’
training to addiction-relevant cues. Note that when addiction- episodic future thinking: A rate-dependent analysis. Biol Psychiatry
relevant distractors are used in the training task, this trans- Cogn Neurosci Neuroimaging 3:160–167.
fers WM training to a variety of CBM, which typically contains 3. Bickel WK, Yi R, Landes RD, Hill PF, Baxter C (2011): Remember the
future: Working memory training decreases delay discounting among
addiction-relevant (or disorder-relevant) cues (Figure 1). An
stimulant addicts. Biol Psychiatry 69:260–265.
alternative is to do general training in conjunction with cue 4. Houben K, Wiers RW, Jansen A (2011): Getting a grip on drinking
exposure (9). This is also interesting from a theoretical behavior: Training working memory to reduce alcohol abuse. Psychol
perspective, because cognitive training may interfere with Sci 22:968–975.
memory reconsolidation of drug memories. In addition, this 5. Wanmaker S, Leijdesdorff SMJ, Geraerts E, van de Wetering BJM,
may help generalization to the world full of alcohol cues Renkema PJ, Franken IHA (2017): The efficacy of a working memory
training in substance use patients: A randomized double-blind
outside the clinic. This may be further fostered by using
placebo-controlled clinical trial [published online ahead of print Sep
smartphones, which patients can use to engage in a chal- 21]. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol.
lenging cognitive task when craving and that may be strate- 6. Wiers RW, Boffo M, Field M (in press): What’s in a trial? On the
gically used in tempting situations. Engaging in a demanding importance of distinguishing between experimental lab-studies and
cognitive task counters memory elaboration of indulging in the randomized controlled trials; the case of cognitive bias modification
temptation, an aspect of craving. In any case, for general and alcohol use disorders. J Stud Alcohol Drugs.
7. Sala G, Gobet F (2017): Does far transfer exist? Negative evidence
cognitive training to be therapeutically effective, it seems
from chess, music, and working memory training. Curr Dir Psychol Sci
crucial to foster connections to therapeutic goals so that the 26:515–520.
widened therapeutic horizon established by the training is 8. Miller WR, Rollnick GA (2013): Motivational Interviewing: Helping
indeed used for therapeutic purposes. People Change. New York: Guilford Press.
One other issue regarding the clinical application of cogni- 9. Kaag AM, Goudriaan AE, de Vries TJ, Pattij T, Wiers RW (2017): A high
tive training concerns its repetitive and rather boring nature. working memory load prior to memory retrieval reduces craving in non-
treatment seeking problem drinkers [published online ahead of print
Making training adaptive [as in the experimental conditions of
Nov 27]. Psychopharmacology (Berl).
the studies testing WM training in addiction (2–5)] can partly 10. den Uyl TE, Gladwin TE, Rinck M, Lindenmeyer J, Wiers RW (2016):
counter this problem. However, relating training to therapeutic A clinical trial with combined transcranial direct current stimulation and
goals (as sketched above) and/or including addiction-relevant alcohol approach bias retraining. Addict Biol 22:1632–1640.

102 Biological Psychiatry: Cognitive Neuroscience and Neuroimaging February 2018; 3:101–102 www.sobp.org/BPCNNI

You might also like