Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 9

Swiss Journal of Psychology, 72 (3), 2013, 149–157

A. Vedes
Sw issJ. Psychol. et©al.:
73 (3) Dyadic
2013 Coping
Verlag Inventory
Hans Huber, in Portuguese
Hogrefe AG , Bern

Original Communication

Psychometric Properties and


Validity of the Dyadic Coping
Inventory in Portuguese
Ana Vedes1,3, Fridtjof W. Nussbeck2, Guy Bodenmann3,

io s
ut of
Wolfgang Lind1, and Ana Ferreira1

n
rib ro
1
((Department?)), University of Lisbon, Portugal
2

st p
((Department?)), University of Bielefeld, Germany
3
((Department?)), University of Zurich, Switzerland
di ted
Abstract. Several studies have shown that dyadic coping, that is, the way couples cope with stress, plays a unique role in intimate
or c
relationships. The aim of this study is to validate the Portuguese version of the Dyadic Coping Inventory (DCI; Bodenmann, 2008) by
analyzing the factorial structure and psychometric properties of the DCI as well as its convergent and criterion validity. A multigroup
t f rre

confirmatory factor analysis was conducted in a community sample of 605 participants living in Portugal who had been in a close
relationship for at least 2 years. The findings support the hypothesized five-factor structure for the DCI and self- and other-perception,
and a two-factor structure for joint dyadic coping. The psychometric properties of the DCI and its criterion validity with other relationship
no nco

measures (RAS and B-SRQ) were good. These results add an important contribution to the determination of the criterion validity of the
DCI with measures of other crucial domains of couples’ functioning (Shared Meaning, Conflict Processes, and Quality of Sexuality,
Passion, and Romance); to the cross-cultural validation [OK??]of the DCI; and to the demonstration that negative DC has a significant
effect on intimate relationship processes, which contradicts the findings of previous studies. Moreover, it provides an additional tool for
U

working with Portuguese-speaking individuals all over the world.

Keywords: dyadic coping, Dyadic Coping Inventory, psychometric properties, intimate relationships

In the past two decades, stress and coping within intimate reasons: (1) There is presently no measure of DC in Portu-
relationships have received increased attention in psycho- guese; (2) Portuguese is the sixth most-often spoken language
logical research and interventions. Stress is a well-known in the world, with over 200 million native speakers widely
risk factor for individuals, but also for couples, as it pro- distributed in many parts of the world (Wolfram Alpha,
motes poor subjective well-being, poor relationship func- 2011); (3) the divorce rate in Portugal is very high (in 2009,
tioning, and a higher risk of dissolution of the relationship 66% of all marriages ended in divorce; Pordata, 2009); and
(Randall & Bodenmann, 2009). On the other hand, good (4) many Portuguese live as immigrant workers in foreign
dyadic coping (DC) has proved to be a protective factor countries, which may make them more vulnerable to stress.
that is associated with better relationship quality, less neg- The Portuguese version of the DCI may help us to better
ative interaction between partners, fewer psychological understand the process of DC among Portuguese-speaking
problems, better physical well-being, and greater relation- individuals. This understanding in turn may help mental
ship stability (see Bodenmann, Randall, & Cutrona, health professionals in their work with this population.
2012[not in refs, or in press?]; Revenson, Kayser, &
Bodenmann, 2005). Interestingly, DC affects well-being
above and beyond individual coping strategies (Falconier,
Nussbeck, & Bodenmann, 2012; Papp & Witt, 2010).
The Systemic-Transactional Dyadic
Given that this construct is highly relevant for understanding
intimate processes and outcomes, this study aims to validate Coping Model
the Portuguese version of the Dyadic Coping Inventory
(DCI) developed by Bodenmann (2008). It is important to Bodenmann’s (1997, 2005) concept of DC is a genuine sys-
have a Portuguese version of this questionnaire for several temic-transactional approach, extending Lazarus’ stress

DOI 10.1024/1421-0185/a000108 Swiss J. Psychol. 73 (3) © 2013 Verlag Hans Huber, Hogrefe AG, Bern
150 A. Vedes et al.: Dyadic Coping Inventory in Portuguese

and coping theory (Lazarus & Folkman, 1984) to a dyadic statistically analyzed, only three DCI validation studies
perspective. It considers not only support from the partner, have been published to date. In the evaluation study, using
but also joint DC as a response to stress. According to exploratory factor analysis (EFA), Bodenmann found a
Bodenmann’s model, DC is a circular interpersonal process four-factor structure for the self- and partner domains of
involving both individual and partner appraisals, common DC (i.e., Stress Communication, Supportive DC, Delegat-
goals, and resources. This process begins with the verbal ed DC, Negative DC) and a one-factor domain for Joint
or nonverbal communication of specific stress signals by DC. Thus, although Bodenmann’s theory of DC proposes
one partner and is followed by the perception and interpre- that Supportive and Joint DC can be emotion- or problem-
tation of these signs by the other partner – and then his/her focused, they load on the same factor in the statistical anal-
reactions. These reactions can be (1) ignoring (due to lack ysis of the questionnaire. Also, Negative DC, albeit theo-
of motivation or skill deficits), (2) stress contagion, or (3) retically distinguished in different forms, statistically con-
DC by engaging in supportive or joint DC (e.g., Boden- stitutes a one-factor construct.
mann, 1995[not in refs], 2005). One validation study conducted an EFA and a confirma-
Positive forms of DC are: supportive DC (problem- or tory factor analysis (CFA), whereby the CFA was only at

io s
emotion-focused), which refers to assistance from the non- the subscale level, comparing three language groups in
stressed partner to the other (e.g., practical advice, help Switzerland (Italian, French, and German; see Ledermann

ut of
with daily tasks, empathy, allying, exploration of solu- et al., 2010). This study, after excluding items 2 and 3 were

n
rib ro
tions); delegated DC, which results from the stressed part- in the French and Italian groups, replicated the structure
ner explicitly asking the other to take over some of his/her found in the evaluation study.

st p
ordinary tasks and functions; and joint DC, which occurs Another validation study was done in a Northern Italian
when both partners are directly affected by the stressful sample (Donato et al., 2009) with a 41-item version of the
event and engage in a symmetric or complementary, prob-di ted
lem- or emotion-focused process of coping (e.g., joint in-
DCI. The authors used CFA and did not completely repli-
cate the original structure, but rather reported a five-factor
formation/solution seeking, sharing of feelings, mutual solution (i.e., Stress Communication, emotion- and prob-
or c
commitment, or relaxation together). Negative DC in- lem-focused Supportive DC, Delegated DC, and Negative
t f rre

cludes hostile DC (distant or criticizing support, minimiz- DC) for self- and partner domains and a three-factor solu-
ing the severity of the partner’s stress), ambivalent DC (un- tion for Joint DC (problem-focused, seeking closeness, and
motivated provision of support), and superficial DC (vague relaxation). The most recent validation study also used a
no nco

or uninspired support; Bodenmann, 2005). CFA to analyze the DCI structure in a Latin American im-
Further strengths of Bodenmann’s model of DC are as migrant sample (Falconier et al., 2012) and found the same
follows: (1) A questionnaire (DCI) has been developed that five-factor solution for self- and partner domains and a two-
U

assesses the different dimensions of the construct, (2) the factor structure for Joint DC (i.e., emotion- and problem-
model has been evaluated in different contexts and with focused), after excluding six items (2, 3, 15, 17, 18, 26).
large samples in different countries, and (3) the model has Therefore, the empirical distinction between emotion-
preventive and clinical implications as reflected in Couples and problem-focused DC found in some studies is in line
Coping Enhancement Training (CCET; Bodenmann & with Bodenmann’s theoretical model, though it has only
Shantinath, 2004) and Coping-Oriented Couples Therapy been found in the Latin American and Italian samples and
(COCT; Bodenmann, 2010). In the context of intimate re- in studies using a confirmatory approach. Findings con-
lationships, DC is not a selfless behavior, but rather a cou- cerning the criterion validity of the DCI have been highly
ple’s mutual engagement to reduce a partner’s or the cou- consistent. Relationship satisfaction is positively associat-
ple’s stress. It serves to foster a feeling of we-ness and, thus, ed with positive DC and negatively associated with Nega-
to protect and boost the relationship quality (Bodenmann, tive DC (Bodenmann, 2005). In addition, positive forms of
2005; Vedes, Nussbeck, Bodenmann, Lind, & Ferreira, DC have been found to be associated with constructive
2013[not in refs]). communication in couples (Bodenmann, 2008; Ledermann
et al., 2010), effective individual coping strategies (Falco-
nier et al., 2012; Papp & Witt, 2010), constructive conflict
resolution (Falconier et al., 2012), and stronger perceptions
The DCI and Validation Studies of “we-ness” (Vedes, Nussbeck, Bodenmann, Lind, & Fer-
reira, 2013[not in refs]). In addition, the DCI has demon-
The nine subscales of the DCI assess three domains of DC strated good predictive explanatory power, being able to
and evaluate discrepancies regarding equity, congruence, predict relationship quality and stability over a 2- to 5-year
and reciprocity between the two partners. These measures period (Bodenmann & Cina, 2006; Bodenmann, Pihet, &
have important implications for couples interventions and Kayser, 2006).
research (Bodenmann, 2008). Almost all reported studies, as in other studies in the
Apart from the evaluation study with 2,399 Swiss par- context of intimate relationships (see Karney & Bradbury,
ticipants presented in the German DCI manual (Boden- 2010), analyzed convenience samples, which limits the
mann, 2008), in which the original structure of the DCI was generalizability of the findings. All of the validation study

Swiss J. Psychol. 73 (3) © 2013 Verlag Hans Huber, Hogrefe AG, Bern
A. Vedes et al.: Dyadic Coping Inventory in Portuguese 151

samples (including ours) were quite similar concerning re- Measures


lationship length, mean age, education level, number of
children, and recruitment strategy. Specifically, our sample DCI (Bodenmann, 2008)
is a national sample, more heterogeneous concerning in-
The DCI is a self-report inventory with 37 items, measured
come, although the ratio between groups is more unequal
on a five-point Likert scale (1 = very rarely to 5 = very
but still adequate. Thus, the results of DCI validation stud-
often). The original DCI has nine subscales: Stress Com-
ies can only be generalized to the age range, income, SES,
munication by Self, Stress Communication by Partner (or
and relationship satisfaction of the population of partici-
Other), Supportive DC by Self, Supportive DC by Partner,
pants who volunteer to take part in the studies.
Negative DC by Self, Negative DC by Partner, Delegated
DC by Self, Delegated DC by Partner, and Joint DC. The
first eight subscales describe behavior shown either by one-
self or by one’s partner, whereas the Joint DC subscale de-
Purpose scribes behavior shown jointly by both partners. All nine
subscales can be used to describe DC behavior. In addition,

io s
This study investigates the psychometric properties of the two items (36 and 37) evaluate how satisfied individuals

ut of
Portuguese version of the DCI. We inspect the factor struc- are with their DC, though these items are not used to de-
ture, hypothesizing that we will result in a five-factor solu- scribe DC behavior itself. The mean of all 35 items (recod-

n
rib ro
tion for the partner domains of DC and a two-factor solu- ing items 7, 10, 11, 15, 22, 25, 26, 27) serves as the total
tion for joint DC due to cultural similarities between Italy score for the DCI. The DCI has shown good reliabilities in

st p
and Portugal as well as with Spanish-speaking migrants in previous validation studies (see Bodenmann, 2008; Donato
the United States. Furthermore, we examine the internal et al., 2009; Falconier et al., 2012; Ledermann et al., 2010).
di ted
reliability of the DCI subscales as well as their convergent
and criterion validity, not only with measures of relation-
or c
ship satisfaction, but also with measures of Shared Mean- Relationship Assessment Scale (RAS; Hendrick,
ing, Conflict Processes, and Quality of Sexuality, Ro- Dicke, & Hendrick, 1998)
t f rre

mance, and Passion. In addition, we explore gender differ-


ences. The RAS is a seven-item measure that assesses a couple’s
no nco

relationship satisfaction using a five-point Likert scale. The


Portuguese version of the RAS (Santos et al., 2000[not in
refs]; revised by Lind, 2008) has shown excellent psycho-
Method
U

metric properties in several samples. In the present study,


Cronbach’s α was .92 for men and .94 for women.
Participants
A group of 605 individuals living in Portugal (23.6% male Brief Sound Relationship Questionnaires (B-SRQ;
and 76.4% female) participated in this study. The women’s Gottman & Gottman, 2006)
mean age was 36.0 (SD = 9.7), and the men’s mean age was
40.7 (SD = 10.5). All participants had been in a close rela- The B-SRQ is a short version of the original Sound Rela-
tionship for at least 2 years. The majority of participants tionship House Questionnaires, developed on the basis of
were married (74.2%); the remaining 25.8% had been co- Gottman’s theory (Gottman, 1999). These true-false scales
habitating for at least 2 years. On average, married partic- were developed to assess the domains of couple function-
ipants had been together for 11.5 years (SD = 10.6) and ing relevant to quality, stability, and communication pat-
cohabitating participants for 5.5 years (SD = 4.0). Also, terns within marriage. In the present study, we used the
69.8% of the participants had children (Mdn = 2). Partici- following established domains (with five items each): (1)
pants were on average satisfied with their relationship (M Shared Meaning (subscales Rituals, Roles, Goals, and
= 3.9, SD = .79, min = 1.57, max = 5, for men; M = 3.9, Symbols), (2) Constructive Conflict Processes (subscales
SD = .87, min = 1.43, max = 5 for women). Accepting Influence, Effective Repair Attempts, and Com-
As to education, 45.5% of men and 49.8% of women promise), and (3) Destructive Conflict Processes (subscales
had a college degree. For the majority of women Harsh Startup, the Four Horsemen, Gridlock on Perpetual
(38.3%[not a majority]), the monthly income was be- Issues, Flooding, and Negative Sentiment Override).
tween e 500 and e 999; for the majority of men In addition, we administered 10 items (1, 4, 6, 7, 8, 9,
(33.6%[not a majority]), it was between e 1000 and 11, 12, 13, 14) from the long-version subscale called Qual-
e 1499. On the lower end, 4.2% of men and 12.8% of ity of Sexuality, Romance, and Passion to cover that aspect
women earned less than e 500 per month. In Portugal, the of relationship quality. We selected the items on the basis
minimum living wage per month is e 485 (Federation of of two criteria: (1) focus on “we-ness” and (2) no redun-
European Employers, 2011). dancy with other included items. All domains used in North

Swiss J. Psychol. 73 (3) © 2013 Verlag Hans Huber, Hogrefe AG, Bern
152 A. Vedes et al.: Dyadic Coping Inventory in Portuguese

American studies (Gottman, 2003; Ryan & Gottman, 2000) respect to emotion- and problem-focused Joint DC. After
and all domains of the Portuguese version, which we used, identifying the factorial structure, additional invariance re-
have shown good reliabilities. In this study, Cronbach’s α strictions on model parameters were implemented until the
for the Quality of Sexuality, Romance, and Passion sub- best model was found. Due to unequal group size, in a first
scale was .90 for men and .89 women; for Shared Meaning step, all models were run separately for male and female
was .90 for both sexes; for Constructive Conflict Processes participants. In a second step, the loadings and intercepts
was .82 for men and .80 for women; and for Destructive of male participants were restricted to the female partici-
Conflict Processes was .94 for both sexes. pants’ values. All models showed adequate fit to the data,
so only the results of multigroup models are reported.
Cronbach’s α was used to estimate the reliabilities of the
Procedure scales and subscales. Spearman correlations were estimat-
ed to analyze the convergent validity of the subscales,
A native speaker of Portuguese translated the English ver- whereby moderate to high (but not perfect) correlations
sion of the DCI into Portuguese, and a bilingual profession- were expected. Criterion validity was examined by com-

io s
al translator backtranslated it into English. These two ver- paring the DCI (sub)scales to the RAS and B-SRQ sub-
sions were then compared to ensure the accuracy of the scales. Finally, scale means were compared across sexes.

ut of
translation. Several pretests were conducted (Portuguese

n
rib ro
version available upon request) to ensure that the partici-
pants completely understood the scale.

st p
Individuals were recruited by flyers distributed in differ-
ent settings (e.g., schools, family and children’s associa- Results
di ted
tions, hospitals, cultural associations). The flyers invited
people to voluntarily participate in our online research DCI Structure
study by filling out a few questionnaires about intimate re-
or c
lationships. The inclusion criteria were: (1) being married The results of the fit indices showed that the hypothesized
t f rre

or having cohabitated for at least 2 years; (2) living in Por- five-factor models for self- and other perceptions fit the
tugal; (3) being at least 18 years old; and (4) providing data acceptably. The AIC indicated that the five-factor so-
informed consent. lutions fit the data better than the four-factor solution did.
no nco

The restricted two-factor solution for Joint DC outper-


formed the one-factor and unrestricted two-factor solution
Analytic Strategy in terms of model fit (as shown in Table 1). Imposing meas-
U

urement invariance restrictions (i.e., requiring loading pa-


Based on the hypothesized five-factor structure (i.e., Stress rameters and intercepts to be equal across sexes) yielded
Communication, emotion- and problem-focused Support- acceptably well-fitting models (as compared to uncon-
ive DC, Delegated DC, and Negative DC) for self- and strained models as well). The application of additional re-
other perceptions and the two-factor structure for Joint DC strictions (factorial means, variances, or covariances) re-
(i.e., emotion- and problem-focused) for both sexes, we sulted in a significant decrease in model fit. Thus, men and
conducted a multigroup confirmatory factor analysis. Sex women appear to have used the DCI in the same manner.
was chosen as a grouping variable to detect possible differ- As illustrated in Figure 1, standardized factor loadings
ences between male and female participants. All analyses for self-perceptions ranged from .45 to .88 for men and
were run using Mplus6 (Muthén & Muthén, 1998–2010) from .49 to .89 for women. For other perceptions, the factor
using a robust version of the maximum likelihood estimator loadings ranged from .52 to .91 for men and from .51 to
with mean- and variance-adjusted χ² statistics (MLMV es- .94 for women. For Joint DC, the factor loadings ranged
timator). Goodness of fit was determined using the χ² test. from .46 to .94 for men and from .52 to .92 for women.
Bentler’s comparative fit index (CFI), the root mean square Item 32 (“We engage in a serious discussion about the
residual of approximation (RMSEA), and the standardized problem, and we think about what must be done”) showed
root mean square residual (SRMR) were used to assess lower factor loadings (.46 for men and .52 for women) than
goodness-of-model fit (whereby, following Hu and Bentler, in other DCI validation studies. This may be due to the fact
1999, CFI > .95; RMSEA < .06; SRMR < .08 were consid- that, in Portuguese, “discussion” has an ambiguous mean-
ered to indicate good model fit). The Akaike information ing and can be interpreted as being either positive (con-
criterion (AIC) was used to assess the goodness-of-model structive conversation) or negative (arguments). So, we
fit of nonnested models. suggest that the word “discussion” be replaced by “conver-
For each perspective (own DC and other DC), two mul- sation” in future studies. As in other studies, items 2 and
tigroup models were run: a four-factor (as originally pro- 17 showed relatively low factor loadings (.45 and .52 for
posed for DCI) and a five-factor solution (as found in non- men; .49 and .51 for women), but we do not recommend
Swiss samples). For joint DC, a one-factor and a two-factor dropping them since the model fits ranged from acceptable
model were run to test whether participants differed with to very good.

Swiss J. Psychol. 73 (3) © 2013 Verlag Hans Huber, Hogrefe AG, Bern
A. Vedes et al.: Dyadic Coping Inventory in Portuguese 153

Table 1
Fit indices of the models tested for multigroup analyses
Models χ² (p) df CFI RMSEA AIC SRMR pc
ab
1. Self DC 4 F MG 173.074 (p = .000) 78 .915 .063 21639 .065
2. Self DC 5 F MGab 164.855 (p = .000) 87 .931 .054 21551 .060 .92
a
3. Other DC 4 F MG 216.105 (p = .000) 91 .940 .067 22890 .056
4. Other DC 5 F MGa 184.315 (p = .000) 88 .954 .060 22822 .054 .88
5. Joint DC 1 F MGa 160.237 (p = .000) 15 .898 .898 8074 .059
6. Joint DC 2 F MGa 13.832 (p = .311) 12 .999 .022 7906 .018 .53
Notes. DC = dyadic coping; DDC = delegated DC; F = factors; [Please explain all of the abbreviations used in the table]. aFactor loadings
and intercepts constrained to be identical across sexes. bTo locally identify the model, the unstandardized factor loading of Item 30 was fixed
to 1. cp value of the χ² difference test of the model with identical factor loadings and intercepts against the completely unconstrained model
using the DIFFTEST option in Mplus. p values are only reported for the best-fitting models.

io s
ut of
n
rib ro
st p
di ted
or c
t f rre
no nco
U

Figure 1. Constrained standardized factor loadings for men’s and women’s DC for a five-factor model (self and other)
and a 2-factor model for joint DC. All factor loadings and intercepts are statistically significant at the .05 level.

Swiss J. Psychol. 73 (3) © 2013 Verlag Hans Huber, Hogrefe AG, Bern
154 A. Vedes et al.: Dyadic Coping Inventory in Portuguese

Table 2
Descriptive statistics, reliabilities, and mean differences
Scales Men Women
M (SD) α M (SD) α M-W U (p)
Stress Communication S 3.57 (0.68) .64 3.87 (0.72) .73 .000
O 3.61 (0.77) .78 3.47 (0.87) .77 .060
Emot. Foc. Supp. DC S 4.04 (0.74) .81 4.11 (0.75) .73 .241
O 3.89 (0.91) .88 3.78 (1.04) .92 .373
Probl. Foc. Supp. DC S 3.70 (0.82) .75 3.74 (0.83) .63 .745
O 3.44 (0.97) .84 3.41 (1.07) .81 .891
Delegated DC S 3.56 (0.87) .82 3.78 (0.87) .75 .008
O 3.36 (0.95) .70 3.36 (1.08) .84 .388
Negative DC S 1.95 (0.73) .75 1.89 (0.77) .77 .329

io s
O 2.16 (0.90) .81 2.34 (0.93) .80 .031

ut of
Emot. Foc. Joint DC 2.97 (1.23) .88 3.06 (1.26) .83 .504

n
rib ro
Probl. Foc. Joint DC 3.57 (0.87) .80 3.60 (0.91) .80 .549
Evaluation of DC 3.70 (1.16) .97 3.60 (1.24) .97 .461

st p
Global DC 3.69 (0.59) .94 3.71 (0.64) .95 .717
Notes. DC = Dyadic coping, [Please explain all of the abbreviations used in the table]; M-W U = Mann-Whitney U test; N = 143 men and
462 women. di ted
or c
Table 3
t f rre

Correlations among DCI Scales for men and women


Scales 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14
no nco

1 Stress Com. (S) .46 .46 .49 .40 .45 .33 .52 –.30 –.32 .38 .48 .45 .62
2 Stress Com. (O) .46 .51 .55 .54 .50 .47 .43 –.24 –.44 .50 .54 .53 .72
3 Emot. Foc. Supp. DC (S) .57 .46 .59 .67 .53 .49 .37 –.52 –.44 .51 .57 .55 .73
U

4 Emot. Foc. Supp. DC (O) .52 .53 .58 .43 .77 .30 .66 –.46 –.72 .62 .68 .80 .88
5 Probl. Foc. Supp. DC (S) .47 .55 .62 .54 .43 .55 .35 –.30 –.33 .40 .51 .42 .62
6 Probl.Foc. Supp. DC (O) .43 .61 .52 .71 .59 .31 .63 –.44 –.64 .54 .62 .75 .81
7 Delegated DC (S) .29 .45 .51 .44 .53 .39 .28 –.26 –.19 .32 .37 .30 .49
8 Delegated DC (O) .48 .45 .42 .60 .47 .62 .39 –.27 –.54 .49 .55 .63 .71
9 Negative DC (S) –.18* –.09 –.41 –.26 –.23 –.24 –.23 –.24 .52 –.34 –.32 –.45 –.58*
10 Negative DC (O) –.17* –.30 –.31 –.55 –.27 –.39 –.18* –.42 .57 –.50 –.52 –.70 –.77
11 Emot. Foc. Joint DC .38 .52 .40 .63 .48 .54 .40 .46 –.30 –.41 .63 .72 .73
12 Probl. Foc. Joint DC .45 .60 .46 .68 .63 .65 .43 .51 –.26 –.45 .56 .69 .79
13 Evaluation DC .38 .49 .50 .77 .50 .68 .39 .57 –.38 –.52 .73 .65 .85
14 Global DC .60 .68 .69 .84 .68 .78 .57 .71 –.50 –.67 .73 .78 .80
Notes. DC = Dyadic coping, [Please explain all of the abbreviations used in the table]; N = 143 men and 462 women. White cells indicate
correlations between variables in the male reports; light gray cells indicate correlations between variables in the female reports. All correlations
are significant at p < .01 with the exception of the items marked with an asterisk (*), which are significant at p < .05, and the bold values that
are ns.

Internal Consistency and Gender Differences cantly higher perception of Stress Communication by Self
and Delegated DC by Self and Negative DC by Partner than
As shown in Table 2, all subscales (with the exception of male participants.
male’s Stress Communication by Self, α = .64, and fe-
male’s own problem-focused DC, α = .63) and the total
score showed at least acceptable reliabilities (Cronbach’s Convergent Validity of the Subscales
α ranging from .70 to .97).
Regarding gender differences, the Mann-Whitney U- In order to inspect the convergent validity of the subscales,
tests revealed that female participants reported a signifi- we calculated their Spearman correlations. As Table 3

Swiss J. Psychol. 73 (3) © 2013 Verlag Hans Huber, Hogrefe AG, Bern
A. Vedes et al.: Dyadic Coping Inventory in Portuguese 155

Table 4
Correlations of the DCI scales with RAS and B-SRQ domains
Scales RAS B-SRQ
Rel. Satisfaction Sex, Rom. & Pass. Const. Conf. Dest. Conf. Shar. Mean.
Men Women Men Women Men Women Men Women Men Women
Stress Com. (S) .41 .42 .43 .30 .33 .29 –.36 –.28 .37 .37
Stress Com. (O) .41 .50 .47 .45 .46 .41 –.37 –.37 .46 .50
Emotion-Foc. Supp. DC (S) .48 .57 .38 .41 .38 .39 –.44 –.37 .37 .50
Emotion-Foc. Supp. DC (O) .70 .76 .64 .57 .64 .65 –.66 –.65 .61 .70
Problem-Foc. Supp. DC (S) .42 .39 .42 .29 .33 .24 –.33 –.21 .41 .36
Problem-Foc. Supp. DC (O) .53 .69 .50 .49 .52 .56 –.46 –.53 .55 .63
Delegated DC (S) .27 .31 .34 .24 .27 .18 –.24 –.17 .30 .25
Delegated DC (O) .50 .60 .39 .44 .43 .49 –.42 –.50 .48 .55

io s
Negative DC (S) –.34 –.50 –.27 –.35 –.32 –.34 .40 .43 –.38 –.43

ut of
Negative DC (O) –.52 –.64 –.37 –.49 –.57 –.55 .58 –.50 –.55 –.62

n
rib ro
Emotion-Foc. Joint DC .58 .66 .69 .68 .63 .56 –.59 –.49 .65 .62
Problem-Foc. Joint DC .53 .60 .53 .49 .51 .51 –.44 –.47 .51 .59

st p
Evaluation DC .75 .80 .68 .62 .70 .68 –.70 –.67 .74 .75
Global DC .70 .80 .65 .63 .68 .64 –.67 –.63 .70 .75
di ted
Notes. DC = dyadic coping, [Please explain all of the abbreviations used in the table]; All correlations are significant at p < .01; N = 143
men and 462 women.
or c
t f rre

shows, most of the correlations among the 13 subscales Sexuality, Romance and Passion; and Shared Meaning.
ranged from r = .30 to .80, indicating that subscales share Negative DC is negatively associated with these variables.
common variance but do not correlate perfectly with each
no nco

other. The smallest correlation coefficients were found for


men’s Negative DC with their own and their partner’s pos-
itive coping behaviors. Discussion
U

In this study, we investigated the factorial structure of the


Portuguese version of the DCI, its psychometrics proper-
Criterion Validity ties, and its validity. Overall, our findings support the va-
lidity of the instrument and confirm our hypotheses: (1) We
Criterion validity was determined using Spearman correla- found a five-factor structure for self- and partner percep-
tions between DCI subscales and the total score with rela- tions and two factors for Joint DC. (2) The Portuguese ver-
tionship satisfaction; Quality of Sexuality, Romance, and sion of the DCI yielded good internal consistency as well
Passion; Constructive Conflict Processes; Destructive as convergent and criterion validity. (3) The measurement
Conflict Processes; and Shared Meaning (see Table 4). models were comparable for women and men, meaning
Considering all scales except the evaluation score, correla- that men’s and women’s item scores can be compared to
tions ranged from r = .17 to r = .76 for the subscales and each other. (4) We also found the DCI to be sensitive to
from r = .63 to r = .80 for the total score. The correlations mean and covariance differences across gender.
between the total DC score and the other scores were rela- Our findings confirm the hypothesized five-factor struc-
tively high for both male and female participants. The cor- ture of the DCI, suggesting that the factorial structure of
relations were slightly higher for relationship satisfaction the DCI is different in samples outside Switzerland and
and Shared Meaning. when CFA is used. The reason for this finding is unclear
With respect to the subscales and the evaluation scale, since studies using CFA in Swiss samples had not been
the correlations of the emotion-focused Supportive DC by performed previously, so we do not know the extent to
Partner and Joint DC subscales with the criterion variables which our findings are a result of the type of analysis. In
were the highest, followed by Negative DC and problem- addition, it would be premature to interpret them as cultural
focused Supportive DC by Partner. The lowest correlations differences since cross-cultural and standardization studies
were found for the self-perception subscales, especially for have not yet been conducted. Contrary to previous valida-
Delegated DC by Self. As expected, positive strategies of tion studies, we did not omit any items of the DCI.
DC are strongly associated with higher (positive) values of We found good reliabilities for all subscales, although
Constructive Conflict; relationship satisfaction; Quality of previous studies had found lower Cronbach’s α values for

Swiss J. Psychol. 73 (3) © 2013 Verlag Hans Huber, Hogrefe AG, Bern
156 A. Vedes et al.: Dyadic Coping Inventory in Portuguese

Negative DC (Donato et al., 2009; Falconier et al., 2012; [abbreviation for what??] (COETT) and coping-oriented
Ledermann et al., 2010). This adds support to the impor- couples therapy (COCT) do.
tance negative DC should have in a couple’s interventions The limitations of this study include the fact that we used
(e.g., by making couples aware of the negative implications a national convenience sample, so generalizations can be
that supporting the partner in a superficial, ambivalent, or made only with caution. Also, the design was cross-sec-
hostile way has for the relationship and by fostering posi- tional, including only self-report measures from individ-
tive forms of DC that match the partner’s needs). DC was uals; and test-retest reliability, discriminate and predictive
significantly correlated not only with relationship satisfac- validity, and treatment sensitivity were not addressed. In
tion, but also with relevant dimensions like Shared Mean- addition, the sample consisted solely of participants resid-
ing, Conflict Processes, and Quality of Sexuality, Ro- ing in Portugal, so that generalizations to other Portuguese-
mance, and Passion. All of these variables have been found speaking cultures or immigrant subcultures in general can-
to be important in the prediction of marital quality and di- not be drawn. Future research should include studies with
vorce (e.g., Gottman & Gottman, 2008). These results are these other cultures.
compatible with previous research, which found substantial Nevertheless, the findings of this study support the no-

io s
associations between DC and couple functioning (Boden- tion that the DCI is a valuable instrument for assessing DC,
mann, 2000; Bodenmann et al., 2006; Falconier et al., a construct now receiving increased attention, in Portu-

ut of
2012) and increase the DCI’s criterion validity. The corre- guese-speaking individuals. Its use will facilitate research

n
rib ro
lations with Shared Meaning and Quality of Sexuality, Ro- on DC as well as clinical and preventive DC interventions
mance, and Passion support the theoretical rationale that in this population.

st p
DC may promote a feeling of “we-ness” (Bodenmann,
2005).
di ted
Interestingly, the correlations for both men and women
were higher for partner’s subscales (especially for emotion-
Acknowledgments

focused support) and for Joint DC (see also Bodenmann, This research was supported by grant SFRH/BD/63182/2009
or c
2000; Falconier et al., 2012). This finding suggests that (1) from the Portuguese Foundation for Science and Technology.
t f rre

the more one partner perceives the other as being support-


ive and responsive (mainly in an emotional way), the more
satisfied he/she is with the relationship and, consequently,
no nco

the more positively he/she perceives his/her partner; (2) the References
more individuals cope together as a unit, the stronger the
feeling of “we-ness” and fulfillment ensue (Patrick, Knee, Bodenmann, G. (1997). Dyadic coping – A systemic-transaction-
U

Canevello, & Lonsbary, 2007; Vedes, Nussbeck, Boden- al view of stress and coping among couples: Theory and em-
mann, Lind, & Ferreira, 2013[not in refs]). These results pirical findings. European Review of Applied Psychology, 47,
agree with previous results stating the relevance of emo- 137–140.
tional and joint DC over other DC processes (e.g., Boden- Bodenmann, G. (2000). Stress und Coping bei Paaren [Stress and
mann & Cina, 2006). coping in couples]. Göttingen: Hogrefe.
Bodenmann, G. (2005). Dyadic coping and its significance for
Unlike other validation studies, but similar to the eval-
marital functioning. In T. Revenson, K. Kayser, & G. Boden-
uation study, the correlations between Negative DC and mann (Eds.), Couples coping with stress: Emerging perspec-
other DCI subscales, as well as with Shared Meaning, Con- tives on dyadic coping (pp. 33–50). Washington, DC: Ameri-
flict Processes, and Quality of Sexuality, Romance, and can Psychological Association.
Passion were (almost) all significant and strong – and in Bodenmann, G. (2008). Dyadisches Coping Inventar (DCI). Test-
the expected direction. As mentioned above, these findings manual [Dyadic Coping Inventory (DCI). Test manual]. Bern,
add additional support for the notion that negative ways of Switzerland: Huber.
supporting the partner are relevant to the quality of the re- Bodenmann, G. (2010). New themes in couple therapy: The role
lationship. of stress, coping and social support. In K. Hahlweg, M. Grawe,
Consistent with findings from previous studies, we & D. H. Baucom (Eds.), Enhancing couples: The shape of cou-
found gender differences (Bodenmann et al., 2006; Falco- ple therapy to come (pp. 142–156). Cambridge, MA: Hogrefe.
nier et al., 2012), with women perceiving themselves as Bodenmann, G., & Cina, A. (2006). Stress and coping among
stable/satisfied, stable/distressed and separated/divorced Swiss
communicating more stress and providing more delegated
couples. Journal of Divorce and Remarriage, 44, 71–89.
DC than their partners, and also perceiving more negative
Bodenmann, G., Pihet, S., & Kayser, K. (2006). The relationship
DC in their partner. This finding suggests that men and between dyadic coping and marital quality: A 2-year longitu-
women seem to have incongruent perceptions of their own dinal study. Journal of Family Psychology, 20, 485–493.
and their partner’s DC. These findings underscore the need [not in text]Bodenmann, G., Randall, A., & Cutrona, C. (in
to attend to gender specificities, to use congruence/discrep- press). Social support and dyadic coping: Commonalities and
ancy measures within[?? for] the couples’ DC, and their differences. [WHERE??]
implications for couples’ bonding, as well as the impor- Bodenmann, G., & Shantinath, S. (2004). The Couples Coping
tance of integrating DC into couple’s interventions like Enhancement Training (CCET): A new approach to prevention

Swiss J. Psychol. 73 (3) © 2013 Verlag Hans Huber, Hogrefe AG, Bern
A. Vedes et al.: Dyadic Coping Inventory in Portuguese 157

of marital distress based upon stress and coping. Family Rela- di, S., Rossier, J., . . . Iafrate, R. (2010). Psychometrics of the
tions, 53, 477–484. dyadic coping inventory in three language groups. Swiss Jour-
Donato, S., Iafrate, R., Barni, D., Bertoni, A., Bodenmann, G., & nal of Psychology, 69, 201–212.
Gagliardi, S (2009). Measuring dyadic coping: The factorial Lind, W. (2008). Casais biculturais e monoculturais: Diferenças
structure of Bodenmann’s Dyadic Coping Questionnaire in an e recursos [Monocultural and bicultural couples: Differences
Italian sample. Testing, Psychometrics, Methodology in Ap- and resources] (Unpublished doctoral dissertation). University
plied Psychology, 16, 25–47. of Lisbon, Portugal.
Falconier, M., Nussbeck, F., & Bodenmann, G. (2012). Dyadic Muthén, L. K., & Muthén, B. O. (1998/2010). Mplus user’s guide
coping in Latino couples: Validity of the Spanish version of (6th ed.). Los Angeles, CA: Author.
the Dyadic Coping Inventory. Anxiety, Stress, and Coping. Ad- Papp, L. M., & Witt, N. L. (2010). Romantic partners’ individual
vance online publication. doi 10.1080/10615806.2012.699045 coping strategies and dyadic coping: Implications for relation-
Federation of European Employers. (2011). FedEE Review of ship functioning. Journal of Family Psychology, 24, 551–559.
minimum wage rates. Retrieved from Patrick, H., Knee, C. R., Canevello, A., & Lonsbary, C. (2007).
http://www.fedee.com/minwage.html [this page would ap- The role of need fulfillment in relationship functioning and
pear to cost money] well-being: A self-determination theory perspective. Journal
Gottman, J. M. (1999). The marriage clinic: A scientifically based

io s
of Personality and Social Psychology, 92, 434–457.
marital therapy. New York: Norton.

ut of
Pordata, (2009). Número de divórcios por 100 casamentos em
Gottman, J. M. (2003). [title of chapter??]. In Child Trends (Ed.),
Portugal [Number of divorces per 100 marriages in Portugal].
Conceptualizing and measuring “healthy marriages” for em-

n
rib ro
Retrieved from http://www.pordata.pt/Portugal/Nu-
pirical research and evaluation studies: A compendium of
mero+de+divorcios+por+100+casamentos-531
measures – Part II (pp. 25–29, 71–74). Washington, DC: Child

st p
Trends. Randall, A. K., & Bodenmann, G. (2009). The role of stress on
close relationships and marital satisfaction. Clinical Psychol-
Gottman, J. M., & Gottman, J. S. (2006). The Brief Sound Rela-
di ted
tionships Questionnaires. In J. M. Gottman & J. S. Gottman
(Eds.), The art and science of love: A workshop for couples
ogy Review, 29, 105–115.
Revenson, A., Kayser, K., & Bodenmann, G. (2005). Emerging
(pp. 291–300). Seattle, WA: The Gottman Institute. perspectives on couples’ coping with stress. Washington, DC:
or c
Gottman, J. M., & Gottman, J. S. (2008). Gottman method couple American Psychological Association.
t f rre

therapy. In A. S. Gurman (Ed.), Clinical handbook of couple Ryan, K., & Gottman, J. M. (2000). Validation of the Sound Re-
therapy (4th ed., pp. 138–166). New York: Guilford. lationship House Scales. Unpublished manuscript, University
Hendrick, S. S., Dicke, A., & Hendrick, C. (1998). The Relation- of Washington, Seattle, WA.
no nco

ship Assessment Scale. Journal of Social and Personal Rela- Wolfram Alpha. (2011). Portuguese. Retrieved from http://www.
tionships, 15, 137–142. wolframalpha.com/input/?i=portuguese
Hu, L. T., & Bentler, P. M. (1999). Cut-off criteria for fit indexes
in covariance structure analysis: Conventional criteria versus
U

new alternatives. Structural Equation Modelling, 6, 1–55.


Karney, R. B., & Bradbury, N. T. (2010). Intimate relationships. Ana Vedes
New York: Norton.
Lazarus, R. S., & Folkman, S. (1984). Stress, appraisal, and cop- Binzmuhlestrasse 14/23
ing. New York: Springer. 8050 Zurich
Ledermann, T., Bodenmann G., Gagliardi, S., Charvoz, L., Verar- anavedes@gmail.com

Swiss J. Psychol. 73 (3) © 2013 Verlag Hans Huber, Hogrefe AG, Bern

You might also like