Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 14

Memory & Cognition (2022) 50:641–654

https://doi.org/10.3758/s13421-021-01211-z

Episodic-semantic interactions in spontaneous thought


Magda Jordão 1 & Peggy L. St. Jacques 2

Accepted: 15 July 2021 / Published online: 9 August 2021


# The Psychonomic Society, Inc. 2021

Abstract
The distinction between a semantic memory system, encompassing conceptual knowledge, and an episodic memory system,
characterized by specific episodes, is one of the most important theoretical proposals in cognitive science. However, the
distinction between systems has rarely been discussed in relation to spontaneous thought that comes to mind with reduced
cognitive effort and intentionality. In this review, we propose that the growing research on spontaneous thought can contribute
to current discussions on the interaction between the episodic and semantic systems. Firstly, we review research that shows that,
as in deliberate retrieval, spontaneous thoughts are influenced by both episodic and semantic memory, as reflected by the mix of
semantic and episodic elements in descriptions of spontaneous thoughts, as well as semantic priming effects in spontaneous
thoughts. We integrate the current evidence based on the interplay between cues and semantic activation. Namely, we suggest
that cues are key to access episodic memory and modulate the frequency of spontaneous thought, while semantic activation
modulates the content of spontaneous thought. Secondly, we propose that spontaneous retrieval is a privileged area to explore the
question of functional independence between systems, because it provides direct access to the episodic system. We review the
evidence for spontaneous thought in semantic dementia, which suggests that episodic and semantic systems are functionally
independent. We acknowledge the scarcity of evidence and suggest that future studies examine the contents of spontaneous
thought descriptions and their neural correlates to test the functional relationship and inform the interaction between episodic and
semantic systems.

Keywords Spontaneous thought . Involuntary autobiographical memory . Priming . Episodic memory . Semantic memory

Introduction Maillet & Schacter, 2016) and in a handbook entirely dedicat-


ed to the area (Christoff & Fox, 2018). The involvement of the
Over the past 15 years, a new area of research has emerged episodic and semantic memory systems has been an important
that focuses on spontaneous thoughts, generally defined as part of this discussion, as authors attempt to explain how
mental contents that come to mind with reduced effort, inten- spontaneous thoughts occur (Berntsen & Hall, 2004;
tion and control (Christoff et al., 2016; Cole & Kvavilashvili, Mildner & Tamir, 2019; Mills et al., 2018) (for proposals
2019). Recently, there has been an effort to systematize the less focused on the episodic and semantic memory systems,
growing evidence on spontaneous thought, in publications see also Barzykowski, Radel, et al., 2019b; Mazzoni, 2019;
suggesting alternative theoretical frameworks (Ciaramelli & Vannucci et al., 2019). Here, we focus on how research on
Treves, 2019; Cole & Kvavilashvili, 2021; Mildner & spontaneous thought about the past and/or future1 (and previ-
Tamir, 2019), reviews examining age-related differences in ous considerations about its reliance on the episodic and se-
spontaneous thoughts (Jordão, Ferreira-Santos, et al., 2019; mantic memory systems) can contribute to the discussion of
the episodic-semantic interaction, namely to the functional
independence between systems.
* Magda Jordão
magda.jordao@gmail.com
1
Please note that spontaneous thought may be atemporal or about the present
1 as described by participants (e.g., Jordão et al., 2020; Jordão, Pinho, & St.
Academic Unit for Ageing and Stroke Research, Bradford Institute
for Health Research, Bradford Teaching Hospitals NHS Foundation Jacques, 2019; Vannucci et al., 2017). Conceptually, there is a variety of
Trust and University of Leeds, Bradford, UK thoughts that may occur spontaneously, and have been associated with the
default-mode network, that may be atemporal or restricted to the present, such
2
Department of Psychology, University of Alberta, as metacognitive thoughts or appraisals of one’s own mental state (for different
Edmonton, Canada types of mental content see, e.g., O’Callaghan et al., 2015).
642 Mem Cogn (2022) 50:641–654

Most of the previous research discussed here has focused episodic memory, respectively, and were first captured by the
on spontaneous thoughts about the past, and to a lesser extent, groundbreaking work of Endel Tulving (Tulving, 1983).
about the future. Following from the definition of spontaneous Semantic memory is defined as knowledge-of-the-world,
thought mentioned above, these are mental contents about the without a specific time or sense of personal experience asso-
past and future that are experienced as less dependent on ef- ciated, whereas episodic memory is defined by a personal
fort, intention, and control (Christoff et al., 2016; Cole & experience of mentally travelling in time to a particular situa-
Kvavilashvili, 2019). This broad definition includes involun- tion in the past (Tulving, 2002), and supports mental time
tary autobiographical memories and future thoughts, which travel to the future (Tulving, 2005). Episodic memory is relat-
are defined as past events and images of the future that come ed, but distinct from autobiographical memory, which is de-
to mind without a previous attempt to recall or imagine (e.g., fined as being about one’s own experiences, and will some-
Berntsen et al., 2015). For that reason, research on involuntary times overlap with instances of episodic memory that involve
autobiographical memory and future thoughts is considered personal experiences (e.g., Conway, 2005; Wheeler et al.,
an important part of this discussion, and henceforth referred 1997). However, autobiographical memory may also take
to as spontaneous past and future thought. the form of knowledge about oneself, without a sense of trav-
The current review is organized around four main sections. elling in time to a particular experience, being closer to se-
First, we provide an introduction to the episodic-semantic inter- mantic than episodic memory (Wheeler et al., 1997).
action debate, defining the key concepts, summarizing recent Autobiographical memory is an important concept in the
theoretical proposals, and highlighting the question of the func- current review for several reasons. First, autobiographical
tional dependence (or independence) between episodic and se- memory research has been pivotal in the episodic-semantic
mantic memory systems. Second, we show that in spontaneous interaction discussion, by showing that there are personal
as in deliberate thought, there is evidence for an interaction be- memories with both semantic and episodic characteristics
tween episodic and semantic memory systems. To do this, we (Renoult et al., 2012). Secondly, theories of autobiographical
review research related to semantic priming and the descriptions memory have suggested hierarchical structures that describe
of spontaneous thought. Despite the interaction between episodic episodic and semantic memory interactions for personal infor-
and semantic systems, we conclude that the overall evidence mation (e.g., Conway, 2005). Finally, spontaneous thoughts
indicates that direct access to the episodic memory system is at have been shown to be frequently about self-related content
the core of spontaneous past and future thought occurrence. and thus closely linked to autobiographical memory
Third, we suggest that studying spontaneous past and future (D’Argembeau, 2018). Throughout the review we develop
thought is a unique avenue to explore the episodic-semantic in- these aspects, in which episodic, semantic, and autobiograph-
teraction. To analyze the functional independence of the episodic ical memory intersect.
memory system in spontaneous retrieval, we review evidence on The episodic-semantic distinction was first proposed as a
semantic dementia that suggests past and future episodic heuristic that led to important new avenues of research, but
thoughts can be accessed spontaneously when the semantic Tulving soon came to believe that these represented, in fact,
memory system is compromised. In conclusion, we highlight functionally and structurally distinct systems (for a historical
the need for future research and detail ways in which studies review, see Renoult & Rugg, 2020), distinguishable based on
on spontaneous thought could provide key information on how what they do and how they do it (Tulving, 2002), and with
episodic and semantic elements intertwine. particular neural substrates and behavioral or cognitive corre-
lates (Tulving, 1985). The distinction between episodic and
semantic memory was initially met with resistance, but found
Theoretical contextualization support in evidence from studies of amnesic patients and brain
activation in healthy individuals (for a review, see Tulving,
In our daily life, we make use of our memories in different 2002). Currently, a renewed interest is emerging in the ques-
ways. For example, when looking at a cup, most of us can tions about the episodic-semantic distinction and, more impor-
recall what the purpose of a cup is and how to use it. tantly, the interaction between the episodic and semantic
Sometimes, a particular cup may also prompt us to remember memory systems (Irish & Vatansever, 2020; Renoult et al.,
the circumstances in which that cup came to be in our posses- 2012; Renoult et al., 2016; Renoult et al., 2019) – to what
sion – a gift from our mother on our first day in a new house of extent do these systems interact? What type of interaction
our own – and take us back to that moment in time. The first exists? Does the interaction blur the distinction between sys-
example highlights that memory reflects knowledge of the tems? Is there a continuum between them? “A continuum of
world, whereas the second example shows that memory is what?” (Tulving, 1983, p. 67). In the next section, we explore
specific and gives us information about what, where, when, these questions, focusing first on defining the episodic-
and who, and is likely accompanied by a sense of travelling semantic distinction, in Tulving’s terms, and his understand-
back in time. These memories are instances of semantic and ing of the interaction between these systems. Then, we
Mem Cogn (2022) 50:641–654 643

highlight new evidence that has emerged about the interaction by functional neuroimaging (e.g., Burianova et al., 2010; Kim,
and new theoretical perspectives developed to accommodate 2016), and (3) the crucial supporting role of semantic memory
it. in episodic future thinking (Irish & Piguet, 2013). It is possible
that the two systems work so closely and dynamically that
The episodic-semantic distinction: The question of they cannot be distinguished. Alternatively, it is possible that
functional independence and a new focus on the the current evidence reflects episodic and semantic memory
interaction systems being independently engaged during the same task.
One particular difficulty in understanding the episodic-
What does it mean to say that the episodic and semantic memory semantic interaction relates to the process of selecting a par-
systems are independent? First, Tulving clearly notes what it ticular memory. Most of the previous research relies on mem-
does not mean – that the two memory systems are cut off from ory tasks in which participants are asked to remember specific
one another. Since the first formulations of Tulving’s theoretical episodes and will frequently rely on their deliberate attempt to
proposal the interaction between systems has been acknowl- remember (but, see Ball, 2007; Barzykowski, Niedźwieńska,
edged: the “[episodic memory] operations require, but go be- & Mazzoni, 2019a). Theory and evidence (e.g., Conway,
yond, the semantic memory system” (Tulving, 2002, p. 5). 2005; Conway & Pleydell-Pearce, 2000; Prebble et al.,
Tulving distinguished the interaction of the two systems at 2013) suggest that retrieval of a particular episode relies on
encoding compared to retrieval. At encoding, when we are learn- progressively moving through a knowledge hierarchy that
ing new information, the semantic memory system is necessary starts by accessing more semanticized forms of autobiograph-
for events to then be encoded in the episodic memory system. In ical memory, including lifetime periods and general events,
simpler terms – if we cannot learn the concept of a train (semantic before accessing specific episodes that constitute episodic
memory system), how can we learn the experience of travelling memory. A similar hierarchical process has been suggested
in one for the first time (episodic memory system)? At retrieval, to be the basis of episodic future thinking (Conway et al.,
however, the relationship between the two systems can be differ- 2019; Irish, 2016). This contrasts with past and future
ent. When information already learned comes to mind, this can thoughts that come to mind without previous intent (e.g.,
involve both semantic and episodic memory systems, or only Berntsen & Jacobsen, 2008). In the following sections, we
one of them (Tulving & Markowitsch, 1998), that is, the systems argue that considering episodic-semantic memory in past
can function independently. This clearly does not mean that the and future spontaneous thoughts can provide a unique way
systems are mutually exclusive and will not interact, but that they to understand the functional interdependence of these systems,
do not necessarily have to. Additionally, this means that although when compared to the large majority of research in this area
one system may function worse when the other is compromised, focused on deliberate retrieval.
they should not be affected to the same degree (Tulving, 1983).
This prediction was analyzed in case studies of patients who
endured damage in the neural structures that support episodic Spontaneous thought
memory (e.g., hippocampal damage), in particular the case of and the episodic-semantic interaction
patient KC (Rosenbaum et al., 2005). Of note, KC showed pre-
served general and personal knowledge acquired before neural Spontaneous past thought was recognized since the earliest
damage, but was unable to recall and re-experience personal experimental research on memory, and was described by
experiences, in line with a selective damage in the episodic mem- Ebbinghaus in 1885 as coming to mind “with apparent spon-
ory system and the functional independence of systems. taneity and without any act of the will” (Ebbinghaus, 2013, p.
Functional independence was further supported by evidence 1). Only much later, however, the first experimental attempts
from neural activation that points to the brain lateralization of to understand this phenomenon were developed, based on
functions associated predominantly with semantic versus episod- concepts from involuntary autobiographical memory (for a
ic memory (Habib et al., 2003, but see more recent evidence review, see Berntsen, 2009) and daydreaming research
below), and other cases of developmental (vs. acquired) amnesia (Singer, 1975); and the discussion about how to better under-
(Baddeley et al., 2001). Recently, alternative findings and ideas stand spontaneous thought is still ongoing (Christoff et al.,
have renewed the interest in these systems and focused on the 2018; Seli, Kane, et al., 2018; Seli, Kane, Metzinger, et al.,
episodic-semantic interaction. 2018). Here, we define spontaneous thought based on the
Renoult et al. (2019) recently reviewed the literature on the reduced effort and intention with which it comes to mind (in
episodic-semantic interaction and concluded that the systems line with Cole & Kvavilashvili, 2019). The less effortful na-
are "inextricably intertwined" (p. 1052). In particular, they ture of spontaneous past and future thought is supported by
discussed evidence showing (1) the mix of semantic and epi- evidence demonstrating that (1) spontaneous past and future
sodic elements in personal narratives (personal semantics, thoughts come to mind faster than voluntary past and future
Renoult et al., 2012), (2) the neural activation overlap shown thoughts (Cole et al., 2016; Schlagman & Kvavilashvili,
644 Mem Cogn (2022) 50:641–654

2008), (2) children between 3 and 4 years old report sponta- thought that it elicits, and can explain the occurrence of spon-
neous past and future thoughts, despite strategic cognitive taneous past thoughts in both naturalistic (e.g., Berntsen,
ability not being developed at that age (Caza & Atance, 1996; Berntsen, 1998) and laboratory settings (e.g., Berntsen
2019; Hjuler et al., 2021; Krøjgaard et al., 2014; Krøjgaard et al., 2013; Schlagman & Kvavilashvili, 2008). Supporting
et al., 2017; Sonne et al., 2019, 2020), (3) older adults with this idea, several studies have shown that spontaneous past
dementia report detailed spontaneous past thoughts, despite and future thoughts are frequently associated with a specific
significant executive function decline (Miles et al., 2013), cue identified by participants as triggering the thought (e.g.,
and (4) neural correlates of spontaneous past thoughts show Berntsen & Hall, 2004; Cole et al., 2016; Plimpton et al.,
a reduced involvement of areas associated with strategic pro- 2015; Schlagman et al., 2007; but see Warden et al., 2019),
cessing, compared to voluntary memory (N. M. Hall et al., and the thoughts that are elicited are specific in nature (e.g.,
2008; S. A. Hall et al., 2014). Berntsen & Hall, 2004; Berntsen & Jacobsen, 2008; Cole
In the absence of an effortful strategic search, several et al., 2016; Johannessen & Berntsen, 2010; Schlagman &
mechanisms have been suggested to explain how spontaneous Kvavilashvili, 2008), suggesting that cues that trigger sponta-
past and future thoughts occur, including processes of cue- neous thoughts are highly discriminable. Additionally, the
item discriminability (Berntsen, 2009; Staugaard & frequency of spontaneous past thoughts has also been success-
Berntsen, 2014) and, more recently, semantic to autobio- fully manipulated via cue-item discriminability (Berntsen
graphical memory priming (Mace et al., 2019). These mech- et al., 2013; Staugaard & Berntsen, 2014). For example,
anisms emphasize the role of the episodic and semantic mem- Berntsen et al. (2013) manipulated cue-item discriminability
ory systems, respectively, and are thus particularly important by systematically pairing unique or repeated cues (sounds)
for the discussion of the episodic-semantic interaction. and scenes (pictures). These stimuli were presented first in
an encoding task, which was followed by a separate task in
Direct access to the episodic memory system: The cue- which sounds were presented on their own. According to the
item discriminability mechanism cue-item discriminability mechanism, the authors predicted
that a unique cue coupled with a unique scene (e.g., the sound
Cue-item discriminability refers to the ability of a cue to isolate a of a particular dog barking in an area of the town you visited
specific memory (Rubin, 1995), and is particularly important to o nc e ) w o u l d c o r r es p o nd t o th e h i g h e s t l e v e l o f
explain the emergence of spontaneous past and future thoughts discriminability, and more frequently result in a spontaneous
(Berntsen, 2009). This mechanism was first suggested to explain past thought when the sound cue is presented on its own.
spontaneous past thoughts and builds upon the encoding speci- Other pairings of unique and repeated cues and scenes
ficity principle, which states that an episodic memory is more would produce lower levels of discriminability and result in
likely to be retrieved in the presence of a retrieval cue with which fewer spontaneous past thoughts. Consistent with their
it overlaps (Moscovitch, 1995; Tulving & Thomson, 1973). This prediction, Berntsen et al. (2013) showed that spontaneous
overlap provides direct access to specific events or the episodic past thoughts were more frequent for unique cues. In sum,
memory system (Conway, 2005; Conway & Pleydell-Pearce, these findings provide strong evidence for cue-item discrimi-
2000). However, the ability of a cue to isolate a specific memory nability as a key mechanism to explain the emergence of
does not depend only on an overlap between the cue and the spontaneous past and future thoughts.
memory, but also on the unique relationship between them. This Cue-item discriminability might occur through pattern
has been suggested by the concept of cue overload (Watkins & completion, whereby a part of a memory prompts the emer-
Watkins, 1975), which refers to a decrease in the probability of a gence of related information and of the memory as a whole, a
cue to elicit a memory as the number of different memories mechanism that has recently been suggested to explain the
associated with the cue increases (for a similar demonstration, emergence of spontaneous thoughts (Mills et al., 2018).
named as the fan effect, see Anderson, 1974). For example, Although this evidence indicates that spontaneous retrieval
London bridge may be uniquely associated with the memory processes favor access to the episodic memory system, re-
of walking around London during a single visit to the city. search on the impact of semantic priming raises new questions
However, if one lives in London and crosses the London bridge about this conclusion, as we review in the next section.
daily, it will overlap with numerous memories. In both situations,
there is an overlap between the cue and the memory, but only in Semantic modulation: Semantic information in
the first case is there a unique link between the two. The unique spontaneous thought, priming effects and semantic-
link is likely to isolate a specific memory, which supports the to-autobiographical memory activation
view that cue-item discriminability provides direct access to the
episodic system. Not all spontaneous past and future thoughts are triggered by
The cue-item discriminability mechanism refers to the need an identifiable cue and refer to a specific event. For example,
for a unique and distinctive link between the cue and the Faber and D’Mello (2018) analyzed free descriptions of
Mem Cogn (2022) 50:641–654 645

thoughts coming to mind while reading a text or watching a et al. (2019) tested the impact of semantic memory prim-
video, and found that around 14% fell on a semantic memory ing on spontaneous past thoughts and found an increase in
category, which included general knowledge like facts, mean- the number of spontaneous past thoughts that were related
ing and concepts (“I thought about a scene from both “The to the content of the priming. Relatedly, semantic person-
Mummy” and “The Return of the Mummy”, p. 5). In a recent al knowledge can also prime deliberate retrieval of auto-
study, looking at descriptions of spontaneous thoughts, we biographical memories (Sheldon et al., 2020).
found a mix of semantic and episodic elements (Jordão Semantic-to-autobiographical priming has been demon-
et al., 2020). These results mirror evidence from deliberate strated using a variety of priming tasks, suggesting the
retrieval showing that descriptions of autobiographical mem- robustness and generalizability of this effect. For
ories involve both semantic and episodic information (Levine example, Mace et al. (2019) used a semantic priming task
et al., 2002). Moreover, we found that, based on inter-coder that consisted of presenting single words of different
agreement, some spontaneous thoughts were better character- levels of familiarity and asking participants to subjective-
ized as repeated or extended personal events. Thus, we also ly rate the degree of familiarity. Some of the words were
found evidence for personal semantics in past and future spon- selected because they are frequently associated with per-
taneous thoughts. Personal semantics refer to memories that sonal experiences (e.g., summer), and thus were expected
do not fit in episodic or semantic memory but include variable to be effective primes for autobiographical memories. To
elements of both (Renoult et al., 2012). If we understand per- elicit and record spontaneous thoughts, they used a stan-
sonal semantics as evidence for a continuum between seman- dard vigilance task paradigm in which participants were
tic and episodic memory systems (Renoult et al., 2012; presented with groups of horizontal or vertical lines and
Renoult et al., 2016; Renoult et al., 2019), our results suggest asked to respond when the lines were presented vertically
that a similar continuum operates in spontaneous thoughts. (based on Schlagman & Kvavilashvili, 2008). The presen-
Another example of spontaneous thoughts that do not tation also included word cues that the participants were
refer to specific events are spontaneous semantic memo- asked to ignore but which have been shown to trigger
ries, consisting of fragments of semantic knowledge, spontaneous thoughts. When spontaneous thoughts oc-
which have been suggested to rely on semantic priming, curred, the participants stopped the task and wrote them
in particular, long-term priming (Kvavilashvili & down. Mace et al. (2019) found an increase in the number
Mandler, 2004). This possibility is in line with semantic of spontaneous past thoughts in which content was related
priming research, which shows that priming effects can be to the primed concepts, and no differences in the number
long-lasting and persist in long-term memory (for a of spontaneous thoughts overall. In a second study, the
review, see Joordens & Becker, 1997). Several studies authors used a lexical decision task as semantic memory
have found that priming can influence spontaneous past priming to minimize the possibility of autobiographical
thoughts both inside (Barzykowski & Niedźwieńska, memory activation during the priming task, and
2018; study 2, Mace & Petersen, 2020) and outside the replicated these findings. Mace and Unlu (2020) also rep-
lab (Mace, 2005), and can have durable effects that last licated the priming effect with sentences and pictures,
several days. In these studies, priming was based on vol- supporting the idea that the effect occurs in various
untarily recalling personal specific memories about a par- contexts involving language and visual perception.
ticular time period (e.g., high school). After this, sponta- Semantic priming effects are also evident when more
neous past thoughts were recorded and compared between abstract concepts are primed. For example, Vannucci
participants who performed the priming or a control task. et al. (2019) showed a priming effect using left- and
The results showed no changes in the overall frequency of right-pointing arrows as priming material. The authors
spontaneous past thoughts (Barzykowski & proposed that left-pointing arrows should prime the con-
Niedźwieńska, 2018; studies 2 and 3, Mace, 2005), or cept of the past and lead to more spontaneous past
spontaneous task-unrelated thoughts (study 2, Mace & thoughts, while right-pointing arrows should prime the
Petersen, 2020), but spontaneous past thoughts about the concept of the future and lead to more spontaneous future
primed time period were more frequent. This effect is thoughts. To test this idea, the authors embedded left or
thought to rely on spreading of activation within the au- right-pointing arrows in the vigilance task previously de-
tobiographical memory system, which would include ele- scribed. They found a greater number of spontaneous past
ments from both episodic and semantic memory systems thoughts when participants were primed with left-pointing
(Mace, 2010). More recently, it has been shown that spon- arrows, and more spontaneous future thoughts with right-
taneous past thoughts may be primed not only by other pointing arrows. Similar to the studies previously de-
autobiographical memories, but also by semantic memory, scribed, the overall number of spontaneous thoughts
in a process called semantic-to-autobiographical memory remained unchanged by the priming procedure. Across
priming (Mace et al., 2019; Mace & Unlu, 2020). Mace all of these studies priming increased spontaneous
646 Mem Cogn (2022) 50:641–654

thoughts related with the content of the primed concepts, (2009, pp. 107–110) suggested that there are several factors
but not the overall frequency of spontaneous past or task- that may influence how cues discriminate particular memo-
unrelated thoughts.2 ries, such as motivation and how recent a memory is (i.e.,
In sum, the evidence reviewed here demonstrates that spon- recency). These factors would moderate the availability of
taneous thoughts are also influenced by the semantic memory certain memories and thus the likelihood these are recalled
system. In fact, Mace and Unlu (2020) suggested that in relation to environmental cues. For example, if I am moti-
semantic-to-autobiographical memory priming is a ubiquitous vated to start exercising, seeing an advert about trainers may
phenomenon, responsible for the majority of spontaneous past remind me of an event in which my friend told me about
thoughts in daily life. The essential role of semantic memory online physical exercise videos. This does not mean that those
has been similarly emphasized for spontaneous thoughts trainers were particularly related to the memory and discrim-
(Mildner & Tamir, 2019), where it has been suggested that inated it from all other “trainer-related” memories I have.
the semantic memory system is a scaffold for spontaneous However, the fact that I am motivated to start exercising acti-
thought. vates memories about that goal, such as the event of talking to
my friend, which in interaction with the cue, becomes the
Episodic and semantic elements of spontaneous most distinctive memory. Thus, memories related to personal
thought goals would be more activated and, because of that, more
readily available (e.g., Johannessen & Berntsen, 2010). If a
It is clear from the above evidence that both episodic and cue is related to several competing memories, the availability
semantic memory play a role in spontaneous thought. of the memory would interact with the cue to isolate that
Namely, the cue-item discriminability mechanism activates memory, rendering its spontaneous retrieval more likely. We
episodic memory by eliciting specific spontaneous thoughts, found evidence for this mechanism in spontaneous thoughts
while semantic memory activation changes the content of by priming personal goals (Jordão, Pinho, et al., 2019). After
spontaneous thoughts. Cue-item discriminability seems to be priming, participants performed a vigilance task including
particularly important for determining if a spontaneous past or common words and, when randomly probed, described their
future thought occurs. In addition, the frequency of spontane- spontaneous thoughts. We found an increase in spontaneous
ous thought about the past is influenced by other mechanisms future thought after priming, consistent with the close relation-
such as attention and intention (Barzykowski & ship between personal goals and future thoughts
Niedźwieńska, 2016; Barzykowski, Niedźwieńska, et al., (D’Argembeau, 2016). Importantly, we also showed that the
2019; Vannucci et al., 2014). The evidence reviewed here also majority of spontaneous thoughts were cued by the words in
indicates that spontaneous thoughts are frequently related to the vigilance task. Again, this was not due to the content of the
the primed materials, indicating that semantic priming influ- cues, which were not specifically about personal goals or the
ences the content of spontaneous thoughts but does not influ- future, but rather the interaction of the priming activation with
ence the occurrence of spontaneous thoughts.3 As we review the cues.
below, the role of semantic priming and cue-item discrimina- It is possible that semantic priming plays a similar role to
bility in spontaneous thought is better understood as part of an motivational priming by making priming-related contents
interaction that involves both semantic and episodic memory more available and consequently more likely to be distin-
systems. guished from other thoughts when a cue is encountered (for
We suggest that a parsimonious way of explaining the im- similar reasoning, see Barzykowski & Niedźwieńska, 2018).
pact of semantic priming in spontaneous thought is based on Indeed, when studying the effect of priming personal goals in
how cue-item discriminability interacts with the availability of spontaneous thought, we found this exact pattern: the thoughts
particular memories through spreading activation. Berntsen were related to an identifiable cue in the majority of cases, and
priming influenced content but not overall frequency (Jordão,
2
The absence of an effect of priming on the overall frequency of spontaneous Pinho, et al., 2019). In other studies involving semantic prim-
past and task-unrelated thoughts is not an indication that the semantic-to- ing (Mace et al., 2019; Mace & Petersen, 2020), the number of
autobiographical priming does not occur. In fact, an effect on content is pre-
cisely what the authors predicted, as clearly stated in their hypothesis that
primed spontaneous past thoughts for which participants iden-
“semantic activations cause activations of associated autobiographical memo- tified a cue was not reported. However, the authors analyzed
ries (e.g., reading the word garden activates a pool of personal memories different types of cues (more or less related with the original
surrounding this concept), and this semantic-to-autobiographical priming pro-
priming stimuli), which suggests that a considerable amount
cess subsequently influences the content of voluntarily and involuntarily re-
trieved autobiographical memories” (Mace et al., 2019, p. 302). of primed spontaneous thoughts were associated with a cue.
3
McVay and Kane (2013) found an increase in spontaneous thoughts when This would be in line with an interaction between cue and
priming participants’ personal goals but in this case the priming was based on priming in which the cue remains a key element in spontane-
providing cues related to participants’ personal goals. The increased occur-
rence of these thoughts would thus still be in line with cue-item discriminabil- ous thought occurrence. Moreover, semantic-to-
ity rather than with an effect of previous activation. autobiographical priming did not change the specific nature
Mem Cogn (2022) 50:641–654 647

of spontaneous past thoughts – these were still mostly specific, may be directly accessed but does not result in more sponta-
irrespective of priming (with no exceptions in Mace et al., neous thoughts overall. Spontaneous retrieval seems thus par-
2019; Mace & Unlu, 2020). Again, this suggests that semantic ticularly reliant on the episodic memory system and, although
priming does not change the mechanism by which specific semantic modulation may be involved, this does not always
spontaneous past and future thought occurs. appear to be required. These characteristics place spontaneous
An alternative hypothesis is that the presence of cues might thought research in a unique position to explore the functional
limit the impact of semantic priming on the occurrence of independence between episodic and semantic memory sys-
spontaneous thought. In this case, if no cues were present, tems, as detailed in the next section.
there might be an increase in the overall frequency of sponta-
neous thought after priming. This would indicate that seman-
tic priming influences not only the content of spontaneous Spontaneous thought as a new approach
thoughts, but also prompts the emergence of thoughts that to study the functional independence
would not be experienced otherwise. However, current re- between episodic and semantic memory
search does not support this idea. In particular, Stawarczyk systems
et al. (2011) measured the effect of personal goal priming
during a sustained attention task that included only numbers. As reviewed in the previous section, behavioral studies indi-
They found that participants reported more thoughts about the cate that the occurrence of spontaneous thoughts is associated
future, relating to personal goal priming, but the overall num- with a quick and non-effortful access to specific events, which
ber of thoughts did not change. This evidence suggests that involves the episodic memory system. Neuropsychological
priming impacts the content but not the overall frequency of evidence supports the idea that spontaneous thoughts are a
spontaneous thought, irrespective of the presence and type of privileged area to study the functional independence between
cues used in the elicitation task. episodic and semantic memory systems. For example, episod-
One prediction that follows from our proposal is that if ic spontaneous mind wandering is disrupted in patients with
semantic priming influences cue-item discriminability, as hippocampal damage in whom the commonly episodic nature
one of several factors which include recency or motivational of spontaneous thought is replaced by semantic content
biases, then semantic priming should have a greater effect on (McCormick et al., 2018). Similarly, hippocampal atrophy in
spontaneous thought when the cues are more general. When a Alzheimer’s disease has been associated with changes in the
cue is more general, it has the potential to overlap with a normative profile of mind wandering, which, as in spontane-
higher number of memories or thoughts such that semantic ous thought, involves a shift of attention from the environment
priming has a greater impact by increasing the availability of to internal mental contents (Smallwood & Schooler, 2015).
semantically-related thoughts and isolating them. When a cue People living with Alzheimer’s disease experience more
is very specific, it will overlap with fewer thoughts, reducing thoughts about stimuli that are in their environment when
the likelihood of semantic impact. In other words, if a cue is the thought comes to mind, indicating that the mental time-
more strongly linked with a particular thought, the increased travel quality of specific spontaneous thought is not as acces-
availability caused by semantic priming is less important. To sible4 (O’Callaghan et al., 2019). Thus, these findings show
our knowledge there is currently no study that has manipulat- the critical role of the episodic memory system in spontaneous
ed both priming and cues in this way. This could be accom- thought.
plished by analyzing priming effects in paradigms that include With respect to the semantic memory system, we suggest
cues with different levels of specificity, as in Uzer and Brown semantics may modulate the content of spontaneous thoughts
(2017), or that manipulate cue overlap directly, as in Berntsen or be involved in the voluntary elaboration of a thought that
et al. (2013). In Berntsen et al. (2013), a combination of fea- initially comes to mind spontaneously. The semantic memory
tures defines a very specific cue, in contrast with the general system should also be involved in understanding certain cues,
verbal cues used in the vigilance tasks in which the priming when these are more general and conceptual. However, if a
effects have been shown (Jordão, Pinho, et al., 2019; cue does not require extensive semantic processing (e.g., a
Schlagman & Kvavilashvili, 2008).
In conclusion, the present evidence on the episodic and 4
It should be noted that ongoing discussions about the definition of mind
semantic elements of spontaneous thought demonstrates an wandering (Christoff et al., 2018; Seli, Kane, et al., 2018; Seli, Kane,
Metzinger, et al., 2018), and differences in the language and concepts used
interplay between systems. Some spontaneous thoughts are
in mind wandering and involuntary autobiographical memory research, advise
semantic or include semantic elements. At the same time, caution when integrating findings from these areas. For example, although
current evidence underscores the key role of direct access to sometimes mind wandering is assumed to be spontaneous, evidence shows
the episodic memory system in order for a spontaneous that is not always the case (Seli et al., 2015). Another example is the role of
triggers, which is frequently analyzed in involuntary autobiographical memory
thought to occur. The semantic memory system, as in other but not in mind-wandering research (Berntsen, 2019; but see Faber &
types of activation, seems to impact the pool of thoughts that D’Mello, 2018; McVay & Kane, 2013).
648 Mem Cogn (2022) 50:641–654

personal specific cue, like a personal photograph), we would based PM (Kamminga et al., 2014).5 It is possible that
expect that the semantic memory system would not be neces- event-based PM tasks using very specific personal cues may
sary for a spontaneous thought to occur. It is in these cases that bypass these semantic deficits and prompt recall, as in volun-
spontaneous thought can be a unique area in which to test the tary autobiographical memories. However, this possibility has
functional independence between episodic and semantic not yet been explored in semantic dementia research.
memory systems. If the episodic memory system is function- The fact that performance in time-based PM seems to be
ally independent, we should find relatively intact spontaneous relatively spared suggests that at least some forms of sponta-
thoughts when the semantic memory system is compromised neous future thought are preserved when the semantic mem-
such as in semantic dementia. Although no studies have di- ory system deteriorates, again indicating a functional indepen-
rectly analyzed spontaneous thought in semantic dementia, dence of the episodic memory system. Anecdotal descriptions
indirect evidence suggests that spontaneous thoughts are pre- from semantic dementia case studies also supports this empir-
served, and we review this evidence below. ical evidence, with reports that some participants were able to
Voluntary retrieval of specific autobiographical epi- “keep track of appointments” (Hodges et al., 1992). It should
sodes when very specific cues are provided to prompt be noted, however, that the possible strategies used by the
recall and access the episodic memory system directly, participants (e.g., checking a clock) were not analyzed in
might parallel cue-item discriminability mechanisms dur- Kamminga et al. (2014), and it is thus uncertain to what extent
ing spontaneous retrieval to directly access the episodic the preservation of time-based PM may be due to deliberate
memory system. Indeed, personal cues promote fast and strategies used by the participants, in addition to spontaneous
direct retrieval in healthy participants as we would expect retrieval. Nevertheless, these findings raise the possibility that
based on cue-item discriminability (Uzer & Brown, spontaneous future thoughts that consist of relatively simple
2017). In semantic dementia, several studies have shown instructions (buying a present for my mum at the market this
preserved autobiographical memory retrieval for remote afternoon) versus more elaborate future scenarios (imagining
events, when very specific cues such as personal photo- the family singing Happy Birthday to my mum in her party
graphs (Westmacott et al., 2001), or other personal de- this evening) may be accessible even if the semantic memory
tailed information provided by a close person (Moss system is compromised. It is less likely that spontaneous fu-
et al., 2003) are used to prompt recall. Based on these ture thoughts that involve more complex events will be pre-
results, we expect that spontaneous thought about the past served. In fact, in deliberate retrieval, the semantic memory
will not be impaired in semantic dementia, which would system has been found to be an essential scaffold to episodic
be in line with the functional independence of the episod- future thinking, as shown by its impairment in semantic de-
ic memory system. mentia (Irish, 2016). According to the idea that spontaneous
Another area of research that provides indirect evidence is future thoughts are often based on memories of imagined fu-
prospective memory (PM). Memory for future intentions has ture episodes that have been previously contructed in a delib-
been shown to partially rely on spontaneous cognitive pro- erate manner (Cole & Kvavilashvili, 2021; D'Argembeau &
cesses. More specifically, while encoding a future action one Mathy, 2011; Jordão et al., 2020), we would expect similar
intends to perform is certainly intentional, thoughts about deficits in spontaneous retrieval in semantic dementia.
these intentions often come to mind spontaneously in relation Although we found no studies directly analyzing the im-
to a particular event (event-based PM, e.g., Scullin et al., pact of semantic dementia on spontaneous thought, there are
2010) or a particular time (time-based PM, e.g., no reasons to believe that such analysis is methodologically
Kvavilashvili & Fisher, 2007). For these reasons, prospective impossible. At the moment there are several lab paradigms
memory has been identified as a key research area to under- that have been successfully used to elicit spontaneous
stand spontaneous future thought (Cole & Kvavilashvili, thoughts. Some of these paradigms require little semantic pro-
2019; Kvavilashvili & Rummel, 2020). In semantic dementia, cessing and should be adaptable to semantic dementia
time-based but not event-based prospective memory is pre- (Berntsen et al., 2013; Laflamme et al., 2018; O’Callaghan
served (Kamminga et al., 2014). Kamminga et al. (2014) sug- et al., 2015). It would also be possible to adapt more natural-
gested that one reason for this differential pattern of PM in istic paradigms that have been used in other contexts. For
semantic dementia is that event-based PM relies more on se- example, St. Jacques and Schacter (2013) used a museum tour
mantic processing (e.g., “When the alarm rings, please put this
notebook on the floor,” requires an understanding of what an 5
This relationship also explains why a greater involvement of spontaneous
alarm, a notebook and the floor are), whereas in time-based thought in event- compared to time-based PM, does not translate in better
PM the requirement for semantic processing is reduced (the preservation of the first. Namely, the spontaneous thought processes in
trigger for the action is defined by the sense of time, “in 10 event-based PM rely on cues that are processed semantically. In time-based
PM, spontaneous thoughts may or may not be triggered by cues (Kvavilashvili
minutes”). This interpretation is supported by a strong rela- & Fisher, 2007), and are thus not necessarily affected by semantic processing
tionship between semantic naming and event but not time- deficits.
Mem Cogn (2022) 50:641–654 649

to create a standardized naturalistic experience and collected contributions to understand the episodic-semantic interaction.
specific visual cues of this experience using a wearable cam- However, there is still scarce evidence about how and how
era. Later, these cues were used to test deliberate retrieval of much do semantic and episodic elements intertwine in spon-
episodic memory against similar photographs. It would be taneous thought descriptions. Methodological factors seem to
possibile to integrate these photographs in a vigilance task in be partially responsible for this. First, it is common to measure
which participants are not asked to voluntarily recall any the level of specificity of spontaneous thoughts by asking
memories, but are asked to identify occasions in which they participants to classify them as specific versus general events,
come to mind spontaneously. This would allow measurement or as a specific, repeated or extended event (e.g., Barzykowski
of the preservation or deterioration of spontaneous thought in & Niedźwieńska, 2018; Cole et al., 2016; Kvavilashvili &
semantic dementia compared to controls, and provide key Schlagman, 2011; Plimpton et al., 2015; Schlagman et al.,
evidence for the present discussion about the functional inde- 2009; Schlagman & Kvavilashvili, 2008). A minority of stud-
pendence of the episodic memory system. ies used participants’ descriptions to determine the specificity
The potential of spontaneous thought research to inform of spontaneous thoughts based on categories, such as dichot-
the episodic-semantic interaction debate is not restricted to omously distinguishing episodic versus semantic thoughts
studying semantic dementia patients. Another important area (McCormick et al., 2018; Song & Wang, 2012), autobio-
of research with healthy participants has to do with the mix of graphical versus semantic memories (Faber & D’Mello,
episodic and semantic information. Based on facilitated access 2018), or a more diverse number of categories from general
to the episodic memory system under spontaneous retrieval, knowledge to specific events with detail (El Haj et al., 2012;
we would expect differences in this episodic-semantic mix, Jordão et al., 2020). In contrast, in deliberate retrieval, much
compared to deliberate retrieval. For example, older adults of the research on episodic and semantic information relies on
frequently show a distinct pattern of episodic and semantic a detailed analysis of participants’ descriptions, based on the
elements in autobiographical retrieval that includes an in- autobiographical interview (AI, Levine et al., 2002). The AI
crease of semantic information compared to younger adults provides a reliable method to analyze episodic and semantic
(Levine et al., 2002). However, when we looked at episodic elements within one description. In this analysis, an event that
specificity between age groups in spontaneous retrieval, we is specific can still include a variable proportion of episodic
found no evidence for differences in specificity (Jordão et al., and semantic elements. This level of analysis seems particu-
2020). Thus, the age differences in the episodic-semantic in- larly important in spontaneous thought to identify variations
teraction seem to be related to the deliberate retrieval process of episodic specificity, given that a high number of specific
itself – both younger and older adults deliberately search for events is frequently reported. The AI and related adaptations
more episodic detail, and younger adults are more successful (Addis et al., 2008) have contributed, for example, to the un-
in accessing it, while older adults access more general derstanding of prospection (Miloyan et al., 2019). Some of the
(semantic) information. This highlights that at least some of disadvantages of the AI, namely the fact that it is time and
the semantic information included in memory descriptions labor consuming, have now started to be addressed by semi-
does not require reinstatement of this information, which automatized transcription and coding (Wardell et al., 2020).
would indicate a decreased functional independence between We suggest that one of the key developments in under-
episodic and semantic memory systems (Renoult et al., 2019). standing spontaneous thought and the episodic-semantic in-
Rather, it is attributable to the deliberate process of recalling, teraction will be in adapting a detailed classification system of
which engages additional strategies to access detail, and may episodic and semantic details, such as the AI, to spontaneous
be more or less successful. thoughts. Firstly, this will depend on using open-ended
In the next section we develop suggestions for two areas we methods that allow the collection of descriptions of spontane-
think are particularly important to advance the knowledge in ous thoughts as experienced by the participants. A consider-
this field: the analysis of spontaneous thought content and its able amount of the research on spontaneous thoughts has used
evolution over time, from the moment when it first comes to probes with forced-choice response options, which limits the
mind. opportunity to analyze episodic and semantic elements (as
well as introducing bias; Jordão, Ferreira-Santos, et al.,
2019; Weinstein, 2017). Recently, Jordano and Touron
Future studies (2018) reviewed findings from think-aloud protocols in the
context of spontaneous thought monitoring. We suggest that
Exploring spontaneous thought content: Open-ended think-aloud protocols could also be an important tool to un-
methods and the need for adapted coding systems derstand how episodic and semantic elements interact in spon-
taneous thought. Secondly, the development of a detailed clas-
As exemplified above, the evidence about semantic and epi- sification system of episodic and semantic details will have to
sodic information in spontaneous thought can provide useful be adapted to include implicit information, inferred from what
650 Mem Cogn (2022) 50:641–654

was described. We found the presence of implicit information Conclusion


to be quite frequent in spontaneous thought descriptions
(Jordão et al., 2020), which is reinforced by the fact that in- In this review we explored the episodic-semantic interaction
ferred information is mentioned in other classifications of by focusing on spontaneous thought. We showed that, as in
spontaneous thoughts (Faber & D’Mello, 2018, p. 12). deliberate retrieval, there is an influence of both the episodic
Establishing a standardized way to deal with implicit informa- and semantic systems in spontaneous retrieval. This indicates
tion, and testing its reliability, will be fundamental to the ad- that the two systems interact. To define the type of interaction
vancement of the field. between them, we focused on semantic priming studies, which
revealed that the impact of the semantic memory system has
been experimentally observed as a modulation of content. On
Spontaneous thought evolving over time: Neural the other hand, the episodic memory system seems to be par-
correlates with high time-precision methods ticularly involved in spontaneous thought occurrence, when a
cue leads to direct access of episodic content. Thus, spontane-
The growing literature on the neural mechanisms of sponta- ous thought does not depend on a previous semantic search,
neous thoughts in healthy individuals offers a useful way to and is a privileged research area to analyze the functional
understand this episodic-semantic interaction in spontaneous independence of the episodic system. We reviewed neuropsy-
thought (for a recent review, see Andrews-Hanna et al., 2018). chological investigations of semantic dementia, which suggest
To investigate the episodic-semantic interaction in spontane- spontaneous thought still occurs when the semantic memory
ous thoughts, we suggest that high time- precision methods, system deteriorates, and indicates that the episodic memory
such as electroencephalography (EEG) and the analysis of system can function independently. Our conclusion is in line
event-related potentials (ERPs) will be particularly important, with recent views on the episodic-semantic interaction, which
in two specific ways. Firstly, to distinguish brain activity as- recognize the existence of an interaction while asserting that a
sociated with the emergence versus maintenance of a sponta- certain degree of distinctiveness between the systems is pres-
neous thought. In fact, a very important issue in spontaneous ent (Renoult et al., 2019). Yet, much remains to be understood
thought has to do with distinguishing the processes associated regarding the forms of spontaneous thought that mix episodic
with its emergence and its maintenance over time and semantic elements, and are not easily categorized as one
(Smallwood, 2013). In the present review, we hypothesized or the other. To explore this, we finished by suggesting two
that the semantic memory system modulates the content of concrete avenues for research: developing a coding system for
spontaneous thought but not its occurrence. This idea could episodic specificity in spontaneous thought and analyzing
be tested by using EEG along with vigilance tasks that include event-related potentials of spontaneous thought.
meaningful cues. In this context, the initiation of spontaneous
thoughts is frequently associated with the cues included in the
task, and its time of presentation can be precisely determined
by the experimenter. Thus, ERPs could be used to assess
whether the occurrence of spontaneous thoughts is consistent- References
ly associated with activation in brain regions related with the
episodic memory system, independently of more or less Addis, D. R., Wong, A. T., & Schacter, D. L. (2008). Age-Related
Changes in the Episodic Simulation of Future Events.
semantic-related activation. Psychological Science, 19(1), 33-41. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.
Secondly, analyzing ERPs for spontaneous thoughts would 1467-9280.2008.02043.x
be important to provide an alternative method to examine Anderson, J. R. (1974). Retrieval of propositional information from long-
episodic specificity. In deliberate retrieval, thoughts with dif- term memory. Cognitive Psychology, 6(4), 451-474. https://doi.org/
10.1016/0010-0285(74)90021-8
ferent levels of episodic specificity including general facts,
Andrews-Hanna, J. R., Irving, Z. C., Fox, K. C., Spreng, R. N., &
personal semantics and unique episodic memories have been Christoff, K. (2018). The Neuroscience of Spontaneous Thought:
shown to have a distinguishable EEG profile (Renoult et al., An Evolving Interdisciplinary Field. In K. Christoff & K. C. R.
2016). Thus, we expect that spontaneous thoughts of different Fox (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of spontaneous thought: Mind-
levels of specificity may also be distinguishable based on their wandering, creativity, dreaming (pp. 143-163). Oxford University
Press.
EEG profile. Identifying these profiles would allow for the Baddeley, A., Vargha-Khadem, F., & Mishkin, M. (2001). Preserved
cross validation of episodic specificity between neural data Recognition in a Case of Developmental Amnesia: Implications
and behavioral data based on participants’ descriptions. for the Acquisition of Semantic Memory? Journal of cognitive neu-
From this perspective, ERP analysis and the development of roscience, 13(3), 357-369. https://doi.org/10.1162/
08989290151137403
a coding system for episodic specificity in spontaneous Ball, C. T. (2007). Can we elicit involuntary autobiographical memories
thought, mentioned in the previous section, go hand in hand, in the laboratory? In J. H. Mace (Ed.), Involuntary memory (pp. 127-
and could be an essential stepping stone for future research. 152). Blackwell Publishing.
Mem Cogn (2022) 50:641–654 651

Barzykowski, K., & Niedźwieńska, A. (2016). The Effects of Instruction Cole, S. N., & Kvavilashvili, L. (2019). Spontaneous future cognition: the
on the Frequency and Characteristics of Involuntary past, present and future of an emerging topic. Psychological re-
Autobiographical Memories. PLoS ONE, 11(6), e0157121. https:// search, 83(4), 631-650. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-019-
doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0157121 01193-3
Barzykowski, K., & Niedźwieńska, A. (2018). Priming involuntary au- Cole, S. N., & Kvavilashvili, L. (2021). Spontaneous and deliberate fu-
tobiographical memories in the lab. Memory, 26(2), 277-289. ture thinking: a dual process account. Psychological research, 85(2),
https://doi.org/10.1080/09658211.2017.1353102 464-479. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-019-01262-7
Barzykowski, K., Niedźwieńska, A., & Mazzoni, G. (2019a). How inten- Cole, S. N., Staugaard, S. R., & Berntsen, D. (2016). Inducing involun-
tion to retrieve a memory and expectation that a memory will come tary and voluntary mental time travel using a laboratory paradigm.
to mind influence the retrieval of autobiographical memories. Memory & Cognition, 44(3), 376-389. https://doi.org/10.3758/
Consciousness and Cognition, 72, 31-48. https://doi.org/10.1016/j. s13421-015-0564-9
concog.2019.03.011 Conway, M. A. (2005). Memory and the self. Journal of Memory and
Barzykowski, K., Radel, R., Niedźwieńska, A., & Kvavilashvili, L. Language, 53(4), 594-628. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2005.08.
(2019b). Why are we not flooded by involuntary thoughts about 005
the past and future? Testing the cognitive inhibition dependency Conway, M. A., Justice, L. V., & D’Argembeau, A. (2019). The self-
hypothesis. Psychological research, 83(4), 666-683. https://doi. memory system revisited: Past, present, and future. In J. H. Mace
org/10.1007/s00426-018-1120-6 (Ed.), The organization and structure of autobiographical memory
Berntsen, D. (1996). Involuntary autobiographical memories. Applied (pp. 28-51). Oxford University Press.
Cognitive Psychology, 10(5), 435-454. https://doi.org/10.1002/ Conway, M. A., & Pleydell-Pearce, C. W. (2000). The construction of
(SICI)1099-0720(199610)10:5<435::AID-ACP408>3.0.CO;2-L autobiographical memories in the self-memory system.
Berntsen, D. (1998). Voluntary and involuntary access to autobiograph- Psychological Review, 107(2), 261-288. https://doi.org/10.1037/
ical memory. Memory, 6(2), 113-141. https://doi.org/10.1080/ 0033-295X.107.2.261
741942071 D'Argembeau, A., & Mathy, A. (2011). Tracking the construction of
Berntsen, D. (2009). Involuntary Autobiographical Memories: An episodic future thoughts. J Exp Psychol Gen, 140(2), 258-271.
Introduction to the Unbidden Past. Cambridge University Press. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0022581
Berntsen, D. (2019). Spontaneous future cognitions: an integrative re- D’Argembeau, A. (2016). The Role of Personal Goals in Future-Oriented
view. Psychological research, 83(4), 651-665. https://doi.org/10. Mental Time Travel. In M. Kourken, B. K. Stanley, & K. S. Karl
1007/s00426-018-1127-z (Eds.), Seeing the Future: Theoretical Perspective on Future-
Berntsen, D., & Hall, N. M. (2004). The episodic nature of involuntary Oriented Mental Time Travel (pp. 199-214). Oxford University
autobiographical memories. Memory and Cognition, 32(5), 789- Press.
803. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03195869 D’Argembeau, A. (2018). Mind-Wandering and Self-Referential
Berntsen, D., & Jacobsen, A. S. (2008). Involuntary (spontaneous) mental Thought. In K. Christoff & K. C. R. Fox (Eds.), The Oxford
time travel into the past and future. Consciousness and Cognition, Handbook of Spontaneous Thought: Mind-wandering, Creativity,
17(4), 1093-1104. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2008.03.001 Dreaming, and Clinical Disorders. Oxford University Press.
Berntsen, D., Rubin, D. C., & Salgado, S. (2015). The frequency of https://doi.org/10.1093/oxfordhb/9780190464745.013.14
involuntary autobiographical memories and future thoughts in rela- Ebbinghaus, H. (2013). Memory: a contribution to experimental psychol-
tion to daydreaming, emotional distress, and age. Consciousness ogy. Annals of neurosciences, 20(4), 155-156. https://doi.org/10.
and Cognition, 36, 352-372. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog. 5214/ans.0972.7531.200408
2015.07.007 El Haj, M., Fasotti, L., & Allain, P. (2012). The involuntary nature of
Berntsen, D., Staugaard, S. R., & Sorensen, L. M. T. (2013). Why am I music-evoked autobiographical memories in Alzheimer's disease.
remembering this now? Predicting the occurrence of involuntary Consciousness and Cognition, 21(1), 238-246. https://doi.org/10.
(Spontaneous) episodic memories. Journal of Experimental 1016/j.concog.2011.12.005
Psychology: General, 142(2), 426-444. https://doi.org/10.1037/ Faber, M., & D’Mello, S. K. (2018). How the stimulus influences mind
a0029128 wandering in semantically rich task contexts. Cognitive Research:
Burianova, H., McIntosh, A. R., & Grady, C. L. (2010). A common Principles and Implications, 3(1), 35. https://doi.org/10.1186/
functional brain network for autobiographical, episodic, and seman- s41235-018-0129-0
tic memory retrieval. NeuroImage, 49(1), 865-874. https://doi.org/ Habib, R., Nyberg, L., & Tulving, E. (2003). Hemispheric asymmetries
10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.08.066 of memory: the HERA model revisited. Trends in Cognitive
Caza, J. S., & Atance, C. M. (2019). Children’s behavior and spontaneous Sciences, 7(6), 241-245. https://doi.org/10.1016/S1364-6613(03)
talk in a future thinking task. Psychological research, 83(4), 761- 00110-4
773. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-018-1089-1 Hall, N. M., Gjedde, A., & Kupers, R. (2008). Neural mechanisms of
Christoff, K., & Fox, K. C. R. (2018). The Oxford Handbook of voluntary and involuntary recall: A PET study. Behavioural Brain
Spontaneous Thought: Mind-Wandering, Creativity, and Research, 186(2), 261-272. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bbr.2007.08.
Dreaming. Oxford University Press. 026
Christoff, K., Irving, Z. C., Fox, K. C. R., Spreng, R. N., & Andrews- Hall, S. A., Rubin, D. C., Miles, A., Davis, S. W., Wing, E. A., Cabeza,
Hanna, J. R. (2016). Mind-wandering as spontaneous thought: a R., & Berntsen, D. (2014). The neural basis of involuntary episodic
dynamic framework. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 17(11), 718- memories. Journal of cognitive neuroscience, 26(10), 2385-2399.
731. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn.2016.113 https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn_a_00633
Christoff, K., Mills, C., Andrews-Hanna, J. R., Irving, Z. C., Thompson, Hjuler, T. F., Sonne, T., Kingo, O. S., Berntsen, D., & Krøjgaard, P.
E., Fox, K. C. R., & Kam, J. W. Y. (2018). Mind-wandering as a (2021). Real-time assessment of looking time at central environmen-
scientific concept: cutting through the definitional haze. Trends in tal cues for spontaneous recall in 35-month-olds. Cognitive
Cognitive Sciences, 22(11), 957-959. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics. Development, 57, 100995. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogdev.2020.
2018.07.004 100995
Ciaramelli, E., & Treves, A. (2019). A Mind Free to Wander: Neural and Hodges, J. R., Patterson, K., Oxbury, S., & Funnell, E. J. B. (1992).
Computational Constraints on Spontaneous Thought. Frontiers in Semantic dementia: Progressive fluent aphasia with temporal lobe
Psychology. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2019.00039 atrophy. Brain, 115(6), 1783-1806.
652 Mem Cogn (2022) 50:641–654

Irish, M. (2016). Semantic memory as the essential scaffold for future- Kvavilashvili, L., & Schlagman, S. (2011). Involuntary autobiographical
oriented mental time travel. In K. Michaelian, S. B. Klein, & K. K. memories in dysphoric mood: A laboratory study. Memory, 19(4),
Szpunar (Eds.), Seeing the future: Theoretical perspective on future- 331-345. https://doi.org/10.1080/09658211.2011.568495
oriented mental time travel (pp. 389-408). Oxford University Press. Laflamme, P., Seli, P., & Smilek, D. (2018). Validating a visual version
Irish, M., & Piguet, O. (2013). The pivotal role of semantic memory in of the metronome response task. Behavior Research Methods, 50(4),
remembering the past and imagining the future. Front Behav 1503-1514. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13428-018-1020-0
Neurosci, 7, 27. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnbeh.2013.00027 Levine, B., Svoboda, E., Hay, J. F., Winocur, G., & Moscovitch, M.
Irish, M., & Vatansever, D. (2020). Rethinking the episodic-semantic (2002). Aging and autobiographical memory: Dissociating episodic
distinction from a gradient perspective. Current Opinion in from semantic retrieval. Psychology and Aging, 17(4), 677-689.
Behavioral Sciences, 32, 43-49. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cobeha. https://doi.org/10.1037/0882-7974.17.4.677
2020.01.016 Mace, J. H. (2005). Priming involuntary autobiographical memories.
Johannessen, K. B., & Berntsen, D. (2010). Current concerns in involun- Memory, 13(8), 874-884. https://doi.org/10.1080/
tary and voluntary autobiographical memories. Consciousness and 09658210444000485
Cognition, 19(4), 847-860. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2010. Mace, J. H. (2010). Understanding Autobiographical Remembering from
01.009 a Spreading Activation Perspective. In J. H. Mace (Ed.), The Act of
Joordens, S., & Becker, S. (1997). The long and short of semantic prim- Remembering (pp. 181-201). Wiley-Blackwell.
ing effects in lexical decision. J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn, Mace, J. H., McQueen, M. L., Hayslett, K. E., Staley, B. J. A., & Welch,
23(5), 1083-1105. https://doi.org/10.1037//0278-7393.23.5.1083 T. J. (2019). Semantic memories prime autobiographical memories:
Jordano, M. L., & Touron, D. R. (2018). How often are thoughts General implications and implications for everyday autobiographi-
metacognitive? Findings from research on self-regulated learning, cal remembering. Memory & Cognition, 47(2), 299-312. https://doi.
think-aloud protocols, and mind-wandering. Psychonomic Bulletin org/10.3758/s13421-018-0866-9
& Review, 25, 1269-1286. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-018- Mace, J. H., & Petersen, E. P. (2020). Priming autobiographical memo-
1490-1 ries: How recalling the past may affect everyday forms of autobio-
Jordão, M., Ferreira-Santos, F., Pinho, M. S., & St. Jacques, P. L. (2019). graphical remembering. Consciousness and Cognition, 85, 103018.
Meta-analysis of aging effects in mind wandering: Methodological https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2020.103018
and sociodemographic factors. Psychology and Aging, 34(4), 531- Mace, J. H., & Unlu, M. (2020). Semantic-to-autobiographical memory
544. https://doi.org/10.1037/pag0000356 priming occurs across multiple sources: Implications for autobio-
Jordão, M., Pinho, M. S., & St. Jacques, P. L. (2019). Inducing sponta- graphical remembering. Memory & Cognition, 48(6), 931-941.
neous future thoughts in younger and older adults by priming future- https://doi.org/10.3758/s13421-020-01029-1
oriented personal goals. Psychological research, 83, 710- Maillet, D., & Schacter, D. L. (2016). From mind wandering to involun-
726. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-019-01146-w tary retrieval: Age-related differences in spontaneous cognitive pro-
Jordão, M., Pinho, M. S., & St. Jacques, P. L. (2020). The effects of aging cesses. Neuropsychologia, 80, 142-156. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
and an episodic specificity induction on spontaneous task-unrelated neuropsychologia.2015.11.017
thought. PLoS ONE, 15(8), e0237340. https://doi.org/10.1371/ Mazzoni, G. (2019). Involuntary memories and involuntary future think-
journal.pone.0237340 ing differently tax cognitive resources. Psychological research,
Kamminga, J., O'Callaghan, C., Hodges, J. R., & Irish, M. (2014). 83(4), 684-697. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-018-1123-3
Differential prospective memory profiles in frontotemporal demen- McCormick, C., Rosenthal, C. R., Miller, T. D., & Maguire, E. A. (2018).
tia syndromes. J Alzheimers Dis, 38(3), 669-679. https://doi.org/10. Mind-Wandering in People with Hippocampal Damage. Journal of
3233/jad-131118 Neuroscience, 38(11), 2745-2754. https://doi.org/10.1523/
Kim, H. (2016). Default network activation during episodic and semantic JNEUROSCI.1812-17.2018
memory retrieval: A selective meta-analytic comparison. McVay, J. C., & Kane, M. (2013). Dispatching the wandering mind?
Neuropsychologia, 80, 35-46. https://doi.org/10.1016/j. Toward a laboratory method for cuing “spontaneous” off-task
neuropsychologia.2015.11.006 thought. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 1-16. https://doi.org/10.3389/
Krøjgaard, P., Kingo, O. S., Dahl, J. J., & Berntsen, D. (2014). “That one fpsyg.2013.00570
makes things small”: Experimentally induced spontaneous memo- Mildner, J. N., & Tamir, D. I. (2019). Spontaneous thought as an uncon-
ries in 3.5-year-olds. Consciousness and Cognition, 30, 24-35. strained memory process. Trends in Neurosciences, 42(11), 763-
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2014.07.017 777. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tins.2019.09.001
Krøjgaard, P., Kingo, O. S., Jensen, T. S., & Berntsen, D. (2017). By- Miles, A. N., Fischer-Mogensen, L., Nielsen, N. H., Hermansen, S., &
passing strategic retrieval: Experimentally induced spontaneous ep- Berntsen, D. (2013). Turning back the hands of time:
isodic memories in 35- and 46-month-old children. Consciousness Autobiographical memories in dementia cued by a museum setting.
and Cognition, 55, 91-105. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2017. Consciousness and Cognition, 22(3), 1074-1081. https://doi.org/10.
08.001 1016/j.concog.2013.07.008
Kvavilashvili, L., & Fisher, L. (2007). Is time-based prospective remem- Mills, C., Herrera-Bennett, A., Faber, M., & Christoff, K. (2018). Why
bering mediated by self-initiated rehearsals? Role of incidental cues, the Mind Wanders: How Spontaneous Thought’s Default
ongoing activity, age, and motivation. Journal of Experimental Variability May Support Episodic Efficiency and Semantic
Psychology: General, 136(1), 112-132. https://doi.org/10.1037/ Optimization. In K. Christoff & K. C. R. Fox (Eds.), The Oxford
0096-3445.136.1.112 Handbook of Spontaneous Thought: Mind-Wandering, Creativity,
Kvavilashvili, L., & Mandler, G. (2004). Out of one's mind: A study of and Dreaming. Oxford University Press. https://doi.org/10.1093/
involuntary semantic memories. Cognitive Psychology, 48(1), 47- oxfordhb/9780190464745.013.42
94. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0010-0285%2803%2900115-4 Miloyan, B., McFarlane, K., & Vásquez-Echeverría, A. (2019). The
Kvavilashvili, L., & Rummel, J. (2020). On the Nature of Everyday adapted Autobiographical interview: A systematic review and pro-
Prospection: A Review and Theoretical Integration of Research on posal for conduct and reporting. Behavioural Brain Research, 370,
Mind-Wandering, Future Thinking, and Prospective Memory. 111881. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bbr.2019.03.050
Review of General Psychology, 24(3), 210-237. https://doi.org/10. Moscovitch, M. (1995). Recovered consciousness: A hypothesis
1177/1089268020918843 concerning modularity and episodic memory. Journal of Clinical
Mem Cogn (2022) 50:641–654 653

and Experimental Neuropsychology, 17(2), 276-290. https://doi.org/ Memory, and Cognition, 36(1), 190-203. https://doi.org/10.1037/
10.1080/01688639508405123 a0017732
Moss, H. E., Kopelman, M. D., Cappelletti, M., de Mornay Davies, P., & Seli, P., Carriere, J. S. A., & Smilek, D. (2015). Not all mind wandering is
Jaldow, E. (2003). Lost for Words or Loss of Memories? created equal: dissociating deliberate from spontaneous mind wan-
Autobiographical Memory in Semantic Dementia. Cognitive dering. Psychological research, 79(5), 750-758. https://doi.org/10.
Neuropsychology, 20(8), 703-732. https://doi.org/10.1080/ 1007/s00426-014-0617-x
02643290242000916 Seli, P., Kane, M. J., Metzinger, T., Smallwood, J., Schacter, D. L.,
O’Callaghan, C., Shine, J. M., Hodges, J. R., Andrews-Hanna, J. R., & Maillet, D., . . . Smilek, D. (2018). The family-resemblances frame-
Irish, M. (2019). Hippocampal atrophy and intrinsic brain network work for mind-wandering remains well clad. Trends in Cognitive
dysfunction relate to alterations in mind wandering in neurodegen- Sciences, 22(11), 959-961. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2018.07.
eration. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 116(8), 007
3316. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1818523116 Seli, P., Kane, M. J., Smallwood, J., Schacter, D. L., Maillet, D.,
O’Callaghan, C., Shine, J. M., Lewis, S. J. G., Andrews-Hanna, J. R., & Schooler, J. W., & Smilek, D. (2018). Mind-wandering as a natural
Irish, M. (2015). Shaped by our thoughts – A new task to assess kind: a family-resemblances view. Trends in Cognitive Sciences,
spontaneous cognition and its associated neural correlates in the 22(6), 479-490. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2018.03.010
default network. Brain and Cognition, 93, 1-10. https://doi.org/10. Sheldon, S., Peters, S., & Renoult, L. (2020). Altering access to autobio-
1016/j.bandc.2014.11.001 graphical episodes with prior semantic knowledge. Consciousness
Plimpton, B., Patel, P., & Kvavilashvili, L. (2015). Role of triggers and and Cognition, 86, 103039. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2020.
dysphoria in mind-wandering about past, present and future: A lab- 103039
oratory study. Consciousness and Cognition, 33, 261-276. https:// Singer, J. L. (1975). Navigating the stream of consciousness: Research in
doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2015.01.014 daydreaming and related inner experience. American Psychologist,
Prebble, S. C., Addis, D. R., & Tippett, L. J. (2013). Autobiographical 30(7), 727-738. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0076928
memory and sense of self. Psychological Bulletin, 139(4), 815-840. Smallwood, J. (2013). Distinguishing how from why the mind wanders:
https://doi.org/10.1037/a0030146 A process–occurrence framework for self-generated mental activity.
Renoult, L., Davidson, P. S., Palombo, D. J., Moscovitch, M., & Levine, Psychological Bulletin, 139(3), 519-535. https://doi.org/10.1037/
B. (2012). Personal semantics: at the crossroads of semantic and a0030010
episodic memory. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 16(11), 550-558. Smallwood, J., & Schooler, J. W. (2015). The Science of Mind
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2012.09.003 Wandering: Empirically Navigating the Stream of Consciousness.
Renoult, L., Irish, M., Moscovitch, M., & Rugg, M. D. (2019). From Annual Review of Psychology, 66(1), 487-518. https://doi.org/10.
Knowing to Remembering: The Semantic–Episodic Distinction. 1146/annurev-psych-010814-015331
Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 23(12), 1041-1057. https://doi.org/ Song, X., & Wang, X. (2012). Mind Wandering in Chinese Daily Lives –
10.1016/j.tics.2019.09.008 An Experience Sampling Study. PLoS ONE, 7(9), e44423. https://
Renoult, L., & Rugg, M. D. (2020). An historical perspective on Endel doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0044423
Tulving's episodic-semantic distinction. Neuropsychologia, 139, Sonne, T., Kingo, O. S., Berntsen, D., & Krøjgaard, P. (2019). Thirty-
107366. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2020.107366 five-month-old children have spontaneous memories despite change
Renoult, L., Tanguay, A., Beaudry, M., Tavakoli, P., Rabipour, S., of context for retrieval. Memory, 27(1), 38-48. https://doi.org/10.
Campbell, K., . . . Davidson, P. S. R. (2016). Personal semantics: 1080/09658211.2017.1363243
Is it distinct from episodic and semantic memory? An electrophys- Sonne, T., Kingo, O. S., Berntsen, D., & Krøjgaard, P. (2020). Noting a
iological study of memory for autobiographical facts and repeated difference: change in social context prompts spontaneous recall in
events in honor of Shlomo Bentin.Special Issue: Functional 46-month-olds, but not in 35-month-olds. Psychological research.
Selectivity in Perceptual and Cognitive Systems - A Tribute to https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-020-01310-7
Shlomo Bentin (1946-2012), 83, 242-256. https://doi.org/10.1016/ St. Jacques, P. L., & Schacter, D. L. (2013). Modifying Memory:
j.neuropsychologia.2015.08.013 Selectively Enhancing and Updating Personal Memories for a
Rosenbaum, R. S., Köhler, S., Schacter, D. L., Moscovitch, M., Museum Tour by Reactivating Them. Psychological Science,
Westmacott, R., Black, S. E., . . . Tulving, E. (2005). The case of 24(4), 537-543. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797612457377
K.C.: Contributions of a memory-impaired person to memory theo- Staugaard, S. R., & Berntsen, D. (2014). Involuntary memories of emo-
ry. Neuropsychologia, 43(7), 989-1021. https://doi.org/10.1016/j. tional scenes: the effects of cue discriminability and emotion over
neuropsychologia.2004.10.007 time. Journal of Experimental Psychology. General, 143(5), 1939-
Rubin, D. C. (1995). Memory in oral traditions: The cognitive psychol- 1957. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0037185
ogy of epic, ballads, and counting-out rhymes. Oxford University Stawarczyk, D., Majerus, S., Maj, M., Van der Linden, M., &
Press. D'Argembeau, A. (2011). Mind-wandering: phenomenology and
Schlagman, S., Kliegel, M., Schulz, J., & Kvavilashvili, L. (2009). function as assessed with a novel experience sampling method.
Differential effects of age on involuntary and voluntary autobio- Acta Psychologica, 136(3), 370-381. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
graphical memory. Psychology and Aging, 24(2), 397-411. https:// actpsy.2011.01.002
doi.org/10.1037/a0015785 Tulving, E. (1983). Elements of Episodic Memory. Clarendon Press.
Schlagman, S., & Kvavilashvili, L. (2008). Involuntary autobiographical Tulving, E. (1985). How many memory systems are there? American
memories in and outside the laboratory: How different are they from Psychologist, 40(4), 385-398. https://doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.
voluntary autobiographical memories? Memory and Cognition, 40.4.385
36(5), 920-932. https://doi.org/10.3758/MC.36.5.920 Tulving, E. (2002). Episodic Memory: From Mind to Brain. Annual
Schlagman, S., Kvavilashvili, L., & Schulz, J. (2007). Effects of age on Review of Psychology, 53(1), 1-25. https://doi.org/10.1146/
involuntary autobiographical memories. In J. H. Mace (Ed.), annurev.psych.53.100901.135114
Involuntary memory (pp. 87-112). Blackwell Publishing. Tulving, E. (2005). Episodic Memory and Autonoesis: Uniquely Human?
Scullin, M. K., McDaniel, M. A., & Einstein, G. O. (2010). Control of In H. S. Terrace & J. Metcalfe (Eds.), The missing link in cognition:
cost in prospective memory: Evidence for spontaneous retrieval Origins of self-reflective consciousness. (pp. 3-56). Oxford
processes. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, University Press.
654 Mem Cogn (2022) 50:641–654

Tulving, E., & Markowitsch, H. J. (1998). Episodic and declarative mem- Warden, E. A., Plimpton, B., & Kvavilashvili, L. (2019). Absence of age
ory: Role of the hippocampus. Hippocampus, 8(3), 198-204. https:// effects on spontaneous past and future thinking in daily life.
doi.org/10.1002/(SICI)1098-1063(1998)8:3<198::AID-HIPO2>3. Psychological Research, 83(4), 727-746. https://doi.org/10.1007/
0.CO;2-G s00426-018-1103-7
Tulving, E., & Thomson, D. M. (1973). Encoding specificity and retrieval Watkins, O. C., & Watkins, M. J. (1975). Buildup of proactive inhibition
processes in episodic memory. Psychological Review, 80(5), 352- as a cue-overload effect. Journal of Experimental Psychology:
373. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0020071 Human Learning and Memory, 1(4), 442-452. https://doi.org/10.
Uzer, T., & Brown, N. R. (2017). The effect of cue content on retrieval 1037/0278-7393.1.4.442
from autobiographical memory. Acta psychologica, 172, 84-91. Weinstein, Y. (2017). Mind-wandering, how do I measure thee with
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.actpsy.2016.11.012 probes? Let me count the ways. Behavior Research Methods.
Vannucci, M., Batool, I., Pelagatti, C., & Mazzoni, G. (2014). Modifying https://doi.org/10.3758/s13428-017-0891-9
the frequency and characteristics of involuntary autobiographical Westmacott, R., Leach, L., Freedman, M., & Moscovitch, M. (2001).
memories. PLoS ONE, 9(4), e89582. https://doi.org/10.1371/ Different patterns of autobiographical memory loss in semantic de-
journal.pone.0089582 mentia and medial temporal lobe amnesia: a challenge to consolida-
Vannucci, M., Pelagatti, C., Chiorri, C., & Brugger, P. (2019). Space- tion theory. Neurocase, 7(1), 37-55. https://doi.org/10.1093/neucas/
time interaction: visuo-spatial processing affects the temporal focus 7.1.37
of mind wandering. Psychology Response, 83(4), 698-709. https:// Wheeler, M. A., Stuss, D. T., & Tulving, E. (1997). Toward a theory of
doi.org/10.1007/s00426-018-1080-x episodic memory: The frontal lobes and autonoetic consciousness.
Vannucci, M., Pelagatti, C., & Marchetti, I. (2017). Manipulating cues in Psychological Bulletin, 121(3), 331-354. https://doi.org/10.1037/
mind wandering: Verbal cues affect the frequency and the temporal 0033-2909.121.3.331
focus of mind wandering. Consciousness and Cognition, 53, 61-69.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2017.06.004
Wardell, V., Esposito, C. L., Madan, C. R., & Palombo, D. J. (2020). Publisher’s note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdic-
Semi-automated transcription and scoring of autobiographical mem- tional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
ory narratives. Behavior Research Methods, 53, 507-517. https://
doi.org/10.3758/s13428-020-01437-w

You might also like