Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 11

Received: 24 February 2017 Revised: 15 November 2017 Accepted: 16 November 2017

DOI: 10.1002/smi.2794

RESEARCH ARTICLE

Economic strain and support in couple: The mediating role of


positive emotions
Petruta P. Rusu1,2 | Peter Hilpert3 | Mariana Falconier4 | Guy Bodenmann1

1
Department of Psychology, University of
Zurich, Zurich, Switzerland Abstract
2
Department of Educational Sciences, This study examined positive emotions as mediating mechanisms in the association between eco-
University “Ștefan cel Mare” of Suceava, nomic strain and spouses' supportive behaviour. Data were collected from 295 married couples
Suceava, Romania living in Romania. Results from the Actor–Partner Mediator Model indicated that economic strain
3
Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral had a negative indirect effect on spouses' supportive dyadic coping due to its negative association
Science, University of Washington, Seattle,
Washington, USA
with partners' positive emotions (joy, contentment, and pride). For both partners, positive emo-
4 tions decreased when they experienced economic strain, which in turn reduced supportive dyadic
Department of Human Development,
Virginia Polytechnic Institute and State coping in couples. These findings have theoretical implications in explaining the association of
University, Falls Church, Virginia, USA economic strain with partners' positive emotions and behaviours and also clinical implications
Correspondence for practitioners working with couples experiencing economic strain.
Petruta P. Rusu, Department of Educational
Sciences, University “Ștefan cel Mare” of
KEY W ORDS
Suceava, Romania.
Email: petruta.rusu@usm.ro dyadic coping, economic strain, married couples, positive emotions, support
Funding information
Swiss Agency for Development and Coopera-
tion (SDC‐CRUS), Grant/Award Number:
SCIEX Project12.237

1 | I N T RO D U CT I O N Horne, & Galovan, 2016). Moreover, there is longitudinal research indi-


cating that lack of employment was more strongly related to a
Economic strain has been found to negatively affect both individual decrease in positive emotions than to an increase in negative emotions
well‐being and couples' functioning. At an individual level, economic and psychological symptoms such as depression and anxiety (Huppert
strain has been linked to depression (Dew & Yorgason, 2010) and anx- & Whittington, 2003).
iety (Falconier, 2010), whereas at the couple level, it has been associ- On the basis of previous studies showing that stress depletes
ated with relationship dissatisfaction, instability (e.g., Archuleta, Britt, spouses' personal and relational resources, we suggest that marital
Tonn, & Grable, 2011), negative interactions such as conflict (Kwon, economic strain is linked to a decrease in wives' and husbands' positive
Rueter, Lee, Koh, & Ok, 2003), psychological aggression (Falconier & emotions, which, in turn, might be related to a decrease in the level of
Epstein, 2010), hostility (Ponnet, Wouters, Goedemé, & Mortelmans, support provided to the partner. The rationale for the association
2013), and demand–withdraw patterns (Falconier & Epstein, 2011). between positive emotions and supportive behaviour in couples stems
Thus far, there is limited research on the explanatory mechanisms from the broaden‐and‐build theory indicating that positive emotions
between economic strain and partners' interactions, and previous build social resources (Fredrickson, 2001) and from research showing
studies focused more on changes in negative emotional states such specific evidence of the association between positive emotions and
as depression or anxiety as mediators (Falconier, 2010; Helms et al., positive interpersonal outcomes in romantic relationships (see, for
2014). review, Ramsey & Gentzler, 2015). Additional studies suggest that
However, stressful contexts affect relationship well‐being not only the presence of positive emotions is not just the absence of negative
by creating additional problems and negative changes but also by emotions (Huppert & Whittington, 2003; Karademas, 2007) and posi-
depleting spouses' positive resources to cope with external stressors tive emotions have distinct implications on close relationships, above
and to engage in positive relationship behaviours (Neff & Karney, and beyond the absence of negative emotions (see for review Yee,
2017). Supporting this model, recent studies showed that economic Gonzaga, & Gable, 2014). A better understanding of the mechanisms
strain was associated with a decrease in supportive dyadic coping relating economic strain to positive constructs may improve the inter-
(SDC) in couples over time (Clavél, Cutrona, & Russell, 2017; Johnson, vention strategies of clinicians working with low‐income couples, by

Stress and Health. 2017;1–11. wileyonlinelibrary.com/journal/smi Copyright © 2017 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. 1
2 RUSU ET AL.

focusing on cultivating partners' positive emotions in everyday life and However, particular contextual factors (such as economic strain)
enhancing partners' supportive behaviours. The current study used might negatively affect couples' interactions, which would have detri-
data from couples living in Romania, a country with a high proportion mental effects on the relationship in the long term. Contextual influ-
of people at risk of poverty and social exclusion (Randall & Corp, 2014). ences on marriage (demands and resources present in the
environment of a marriage) have been negatively related to spouses'
coping skills and effective interactions (Clavél et al., 2017; Johnson
1.1 | Economic context in Romania et al., 2016; Karney & Bradbury, 2005; Neff & Karney, 2017). High
levels of chronic stress (such as financial strain) are in general associ-
The communist period (1948–1989) and the postcommunist transition
ated with more challenges for couples (e.g., more working hours and
to capitalism have left persistent negative economic implications on
lack of money for child care) and negatively affect couples' ability to
people's lives in Romania. The global economic crisis of 2008, followed
cope effectively with these additional stressors. Moreover,
by the government austerity programme in 2010 negatively affected
Bodenmann et al. (2015) found that stress negatively influenced par-
Romanians' everyday life once again. The average household income
ticularly social support provided by men in couples, and this was
was the lowest from all the European Union countries in 2012 (Randall
explained by the fact that stressful environments deplete the emo-
& Corp, 2014). However, there is a lack of empirical studies examining
tional resources of partners to remain connected and not by the insuf-
the influence of economic strain on couples' interactions. Thus far,
ficient skills of men to be supportive. Similarly, McNulty (2016)
research has found that financial problems predict conflict and depres-
emphasizes the role of contextual factors in interpersonal relationships
sion in Romanian families (Robila & Krishnakumar, 2005). Further
and suggests that external and stressful circumstances limit partners'
investigations are still needed, using a dyadic approach (collecting data
self‐regulatory resources.
from both partners) and explaining the mechanisms involved in the
Economic strain, in particular, has been found to decrease people's
association between economic strain and relational behaviours.
ability to provide social and emotional support to their partners (e.g.,
providing help, expressing care, giving encouragement, showing affec-
tion, and positive dyadic coping [DC]) and to increase undermining
1.2 | Economic strain and supportive dyadic coping
behaviours (e.g., criticizing, insulting, and negative DC; Karademas &
in couples Roussi, 2016; Simons, Lorenz, Wu, & Conger, 1993; Vinokur, Price, &
Economic strain is experienced when an individual perceives that he or Caplan, 1996). A daily diary study indicated that individuals experienc-
she does not have enough or adequate resources to face the economic ing negative daily financial events reported more negative interper-
demands of a situation (Lazarus & Folkman, 1984), creating concerns sonal events on the same day (with family members and with a
and worries (Voydanoff & Donnelly, 1988). According to the sys- romantic partner; Sturgeon, Zautra, & Okun, 2014). In a Romanian
temic–transactional model (STM; Bodenmann, 1997), one partner's National Survey (Barometrul de Opinie Publica, 2007), both men and
stress cannot be understood without considering the other partner's women reported economic strain as being the most important variable
stress. STM is an extension of Lazarus and Folkman's stress transac- in generating family conflicts and the financial situation was found to
tional theory to the context of couples' relationships and provides a be one of the main explanatory variables of marital satisfaction. More-
framework for understanding stress, such as economic strain, and cop- over, Karademas and Roussi (2016) found that DC mediated the asso-
ing processes in couples. STM argues that stress influences a partner's ciation between economic strain and marital satisfaction in a sample of
relational behaviours through its harmful effect on a partner's individ- Greek couples. Consistent with these findings, the present study
ual well‐being. Stress occurring outside of the relationship (external sought to further examine if economic strain could influence positive
stress) spills over into the couple's relationship; decreases the amount behaviours in couples, comprising partners' willingness to provide sup-
of time that partners spend together, the quality of communication, port to each other during times of stress (SDC).
and marital satisfaction (Bodenmann, 2005); and increases negative
interactions between partners (Neff & Karney, 2009). On the basis of
STM, one partner's stress always becomes the other partner's stress
1.3 | Economic strain and positive emotions
as a result of crossover effects and/or because both partners are Empirical evidence suggests that poverty is negatively related to hap-
affected by common stressors (e.g., reduced family income or savings). piness and life satisfaction both within and across countries (see for
The association between both partners' economic strain levels has review Haushofer & Fehr, 2014). In the last two decades, researchers
actually received empirical support in past research (e.g., Falconier & have started to analyse the role of positive emotions in addition to
Epstein, 2011; Mauno & Kinnunen, 2002). those of negative emotions in the stress process. Folkman (2008) pro-
In addition to linking stress processes between partners, STM also posed a revision of the original stress and coping model (Lazarus &
focuses on what partners do to support each other to cope with stress. Folkman, 1984) by incorporating positive emotions. Several studies
STM has defined SDC as the support or assistance that one partner showed that stress negatively affected positive emotions (Gloria,
provides to the other to cope with stress (Bodenmann, 1997). SDC Faulk, & Steinhardt, 2013; Schiffrin & Nelson, 2010), whereas others
may involve emotion‐focused (e.g., being understanding and empathic) found that stress increased negative affect but did not decrease posi-
and problem‐focused (e.g., providing solutions) responses. Research tive affect (Hamama, Ronen, Shachar, & Rosenbaum, 2013). However,
has found that SDC was positively associated with relationship satis- few studies examined explicitly the influence of economic strain on
faction (e.g., Fife, Weaver, Cook, & Stump, 2013; Rusu, 2016). positive emotions. Gallo and Matthews (2003) developed the Reserve
RUSU ET AL. 3

Capacity Model for explaining the dynamic associations of socio‐eco- social support in couples and found that participants who experienced
nomic status with positive and negative emotions, social and personal financial strain were perceived as being less supportive to their part-
resources, and health outcomes. According to this model, a low socio‐ ners (Clavél et al., 2017). However, there remains a need for further
economic status leads to more negative and less positive emotions and understanding of the effect of economic strain on positive outcomes
depletes individual personal and psychosocial resources (such as dispo- in couples, and the current study will examine the influence of eco-
sitional optimism, self‐esteem, and social support). Gallo and Matthews nomic strain on SDC through positive emotions.
(2003) suggested that people from low‐socio‐economic‐status envi- According to the broaden‐and‐build theory (Fredrickson, 2001),
ronments are less likely to experience positive emotions and studies the experience of positive emotions broadens people's thoughts and
concerning socio‐economic status and emotions should include mea- actions (e.g., to play, to be creative, to savour current life circum-
sures of positive emotions. Studies based on the Reserve Capacity stances, and to share achievements with others), which in turn builds
Model have found that positive and negative emotions are differen- intrapersonal and social benefits, such as social support and positive
tially affected by socio‐economic status. Specifically, socio‐economic relations with others (Fredrickson, Cohn, Coffey, Pek, & Finkel,
status was associated with lower levels of positive emotions in daily 2008). Positive emotions and emotion regulation have been found to
life, but not higher levels of negative affect (Gallo, Bogart, Vranceanu, predict disclosure, greater enjoyment of social activities, interpersonal
& Matthews, 2005). Similarly, previous research found that unemploy- sensitivity, prosocial tendencies, greater feelings of self–other overlap,
ment was strongly associated with a decrease in positive affectivity and complex understanding of others (Lopes, Salovey, Côté, Beers, &
and well‐being but had less influence on negative emotions and psy- Petty, 2005; Waugh & Fredrickson, 2006). Similarly, exchanging posi-
chological symptoms (Huppert & Whittington, 2003). In the family psy- tive emotions in close relationships (as a result of positive events that
chology context, economic strain was negatively related to parental happened to one's partner) has been found to be positively associated
positivity (including positive emotions, self‐esteem, and mastery; Jeon with commitment, relationship satisfaction, and relationship stability
& Neppl, 2016). Consistent with the Reserve Capacity Model and on (Aron, Norman, Aron, McKenna, & Heyman, 2000; Gable, Gonzaga, &
the basis of previous studies concerning economic strain and positive Strachman, 2006). Furthermore, existing studies found that positive
emotions, the present study attempts to extend existing findings by emotions have important and distinctive consequences on psycholog-
investigating the associations between family economic strain, positive ical health. For example, positive emotions but not negative emotions
emotions, and support in couples. have been found to be related to resilience (Cohn et al., 2009; Tugade
& Fredrickson, 2004), self–other overlap (Waugh & Fredrickson, 2006),
and life satisfaction (Cohn et al., 2009). In addition, experimental stud-
1.4 | Positive emotions as mediators between
ies showed that positive emotions have an “undoing” function on the
economic strain and supportive dyadic coping negative consequences of stress, by being associated with downregu-
To date, several studies have found that individual variables explained lation of negative emotions, which is relevant to coping (Tugade &
the negative influence of stress in general on couples' relationship Fredrickson, 2004).
quality. Neff and Karney (2004, 2009) showed that stress decreases In examining economic strain in association with positive emo-
partners' willingness to express affection and support, consumes tions, previous studies showed negative associations of economic
partners' resources, and in consequence reduces their ability to engage strain with daily feelings of happiness and excitement (Gallo et al.,
in positive relationship behaviour. However, only a few studies have 2005); joy, optimism, and self‐esteem (Jeon & Neppl, 2016); and feel-
examined potential mechanisms by which economic strain, in particu- ings of satisfaction, hopefulness, and confidence (Huppert &
lar, is associated with partners' behaviours. Conger, Rueter, and Whittington, 2003). Consistent with these studies, our study focused
Conger (2000) found that negative emotions determined by economic on three positive emotions, joy, contentment, and pride, as they have
stress have been associated with negative marital interactions, as been shown to relate to stress and socio‐economic status and have
depressed and frustrated partners will engage more in conflicts and been defined in relationship to environmental resources (Shiota,
will offer less emotional support to their spouse. Keltner, & John, 2006). Joy is defined as a reward‐oriented emotion,
The association between positive and negative emotions in times which arises in safe contexts and reflects an improvement of environ-
of stress might be affected by the level of stress (Zautra, Affleck, mental resources (Fredrickson, 1998; Shiota et al., 2006). Joy has an
Tennen, Reich, & Davis, 2005). Research showed that in times of low important role in both formation and development of close relation-
stress, positive and negative emotions are relatively uncorrelated, but ships. Displays of joy contribute to mate selection, attachment, self‐
positive emotions are especially beneficial for coping on days when expansion (see Shiota, Campos, Keltner, & Hertenstein, 2004), and
people experience higher levels of stress, daily positive emotions being commitment in romantic relationships (Aron et al., 2000). Contentment
associated with the regulation of negative emotions (Zautra et al., is defined by the satisfaction of one's needs (Shiota et al., 2006), arises
2005). Similarly, there is support from other studies on the role of pos- in situations perceived as safe, and “prompts individuals to savor their
itive emotions in enhancing resilience and well‐being in stressful situa- current life circumstances and recent successes” (Fredrickson, 1998, p.
tions (Cohn, Fredrickson, Brown, Mikels, & Conway, 2009; 8). Both joy and contentment have been related to a better stress reg-
Fredrickson, Tugade, Waugh, & Larkin, 2003; Gloria et al., 2013). In ulation (Fredrickson, 2001). Pride is a positive emotion arising from
addition, the influence of stress on emotions might be dependent not achievements attributed to one's abilities or efforts and reflects a
only on the level of stress but also on the type of stress. One recent social evaluation of self compared with others (Shiota et al., 2006; Wil-
longitudinal study examined how stressors differentially influence liams & DeSteno, 2008). As in the case of joy and contentment, pride
4 RUSU ET AL.

has not only intrapersonal but also interpersonal consequences, being men had completed some post‐high‐school education; 43.69% of
related to stronger interpersonal connections, by increasing the disclo- women and 30.66% of men had a bachelor's degree; and 8.87% of
sure about an achievement (Lewis & Sullivan, 2005). women and 9.40% of men had a postgraduate education. According
On the basis of prior studies and consistent with the Reserve to occupational status, 79.3% of the men and 79% of the women were
Capacity Model, we hypothesized that financial strain would be associ- employed; 14.2% of the men and 14.6% of the women were unem-
ated with less joy, contentment, and pride. We further predicted that ployed; 0.3% of the men and 2.7% of the women were still students;
these positive emotions would be associated with SDC in couples. and 2.7% of the men and 2.4% of the women were retired. The
Positive emotions have been found to broaden coping resources socio‐economic status of the sample, defined by family income, was
(Tugade, 2011) and have been conceptualized as self‐resources, facili- representative of Romania; 2% had a very low family income, 43.4%
tating self‐regulation (Tice, Baumeister, & Zhang, 2004). However, had a low family income, 44% had a moderate family income, and
considering that stress may impair individual and couple resources, 8.6% had a high family income. The majority of participants were
we assume that a decrease in partners' positive behaviours (such as Christian‐Orthodox (89.25%), and the rest were Pentecostal (4.69%),
SDC) during economic strain might be explained by a decrease in pos- Evangelist (2.73%), Catholic (1.87%), Adventist (0.85%), and Baptist
itive emotions. (0.34%).

1.5 | Hypotheses 2.2 | Procedure


On the basis of the aforementioned negative associations of economic Couples were recruited by students attending an educational science
strain with positive emotions (Gallo et al., 2005; Jeon & Neppl, 2016) programme from a public Romanian university. Students were
and positive coping behaviours in couple (Clavél et al., 2017; instructed about the study's purpose and were asked to distribute
Karademas & Roussi, 2016), we first hypothesized that economic the questionnaires to married couples (every student received ques-
strain at the dyadic level will be negatively related to male and female tionnaires for two couples). For data collection, students received aca-
positive emotions (joy, contentment, and pride; H1a) and to male and demic course credits. To ensure the independency and privacy of the
female SDC behaviours (H1b). Further, according to the broaden‐ reports, students were instructed to deliver the envelopes separately
and‐build theory of positive emotions (Fredrickson, 2001) and given to each spouse. Students received questionnaires for 390 couples,
prior empirical research regarding the interpersonal benefits of positive and 331 of them were returned. We excluded the questionnaires filled
emotions (Fredrickson et al., 2008; Gable et al., 2006), we predicted out by only one partner (21 couples) and the questionnaires with iden-
that positive emotions experienced by each partner (joy, contentment, tical answers for both spouses on all measures (15 couples). Overall,
and pride) would be positively associated with own and partner SDC 295 couples (N = 590 individuals) with complete data were included
(H2). The prediction of both actor and partner effects is based on the in the study. Couples agreed to participate in the study on a voluntary
STM (Bodenmann, 2005) and studies that have used this model and basis. In the final sample, we had couples from eight different counties
indicated that individual resources of one partner influence both own in Romania. All participants signed a consent form to participate in the
and partner dyadic behaviours (Herzberg, 2013; Papp & Witt, 2010). study. Participants were asked to complete the questionnaires without
Finally, given the STM assumption that individual resources mediate consulting the partner and to return them in enclosed envelopes. Cou-
the link between external stress and relationship outcomes, we ples were not reimbursed for their participation.
assumed that each partner's positive emotions (joy, contentment, and
pride) would mediate the association of economic strain with own
SDC (actor–actor mediation) and with partner SDC (actor–partner
2.3 | Measures
mediation; H3). Participants completed a demographic information form that
requested information on age, gender, marriage duration, number of
children, and education.
2 | METHOD
2.3.1 | Economic strain
2.1 | Sample The Family Economic Strain Scale (FESS; Hilton & Devall, 1997) was
The data from the present study were collected from a community used to assess economic strain. The FESS is a 13‐item questionnaire
sample of 295 Romanian married couples, approximately half from an that measures each partner's individual subjective experience of finan-
urban area (50.2%) and half from a rural area (49.8%). Men's mean cial strain by asking respondents to rate the frequency with which they
age was 39.30 (SD = 9.34, range = 21–66), and women's mean age experience various situations indicative of financial strain (e.g., “In gen-
was 36.14 years (SD = 9.37, range = 20–64). On average, the marriage eral, it is hard for me and my family to live on our present income”) on a
duration was 13.05 years (SD = 9.30; range = 0.25–40 years), and part- 5‐point Likert scale ranging from 1 (never) to 5 (almost always). The final
ners had been in the relationship on average for 15 years (SD = 8.64). item is different from the rest of the scale because it asks participants
Couples had on average 1.43 children (SD = 1.14, range = 0–8 chil- to evaluate family income in comparison to other families from the
dren). Regarding their educational level, 5.46% of women and 6.96% same country also on a 5‐point Likert scale ranging from 1 (far below
of men had finished middle school; 29.35% of women and 41.11% of average) to 5 (far above average). We excluded two items referring to
men had a high school diploma; 11.85% of women and 12.63% of financial stress in relation to children, because 22% of the couples in
RUSU ET AL. 5

this sample did not have children. The total score of the scale was cal- of dyadic data and to specify the effects within person (actor effects)
culated by averaging the values for the other 10 items. The final com- and across partners (partners effects), as well as to include mediation
parison item was kept. Hilton and Devall (1997) reported internal variables in the model (see Figure 1). Considering that economic strain
consistency for FESS ranging from .92 to .95. Internal consistency in is a common stressor shared by wives and husbands, we followed the
this study was α = .92 for males and α = .94 for females. conceptual framework of Westman, Vinokur, Hamilton, and Roziner
(2004) and we computed economic strain as a latent factor indicated
2.3.2 | Supportive dyadic coping by wives' and husbands' scores on this measure. For positive emotions
The Dyadic Coping Inventory (DCI, Romanian version; Rusu, Hilpert, and SDC, we included in the model individual scores of both partners,
Turliuc, & Bodenmann, 2016) was used to measure SDC by oneself. resulting in this way a hybrid model (Ledermann & Kenny, 2012). First,
The DCI measures the frequency with which partners engage in vari- we estimated a partial mediation model for distinguishable dyads
ous aspects of DC (i.e., stress communication, delegated, supportive, (Model 1). Second, considering that the direct paths from economic
and negative DC) on a 5‐point Likert type scale ranging from 1 (very strain to SDC were statistically not significant in the structural model,
rarely) to 5 (very often). The DCI factorial structure has been confirmed we tested the more parsimonious full mediation model, without the
in the Romanian population (Rusu et al., 2016). The SDC by Oneself direct paths from economic strain to SDC (Model 2). Third, we tested
Scale used in the current study includes four items, two of them mea- for gender differences by constraining corresponding paths to be equal
suring emotion‐focused SDC (e.g., “I show empathy and understanding (Model 3, Model 4, and Model 5).
to my partner”) and the other two measuring problem‐focused SDC (e.
g., “I try to analyze the situation together with my partner in an objec- 2.4.1 | Control variables
tive manner and help him/her to understand and change the problem”).
Past research suggested that economic strain, positive emotions, and
In the current study, the internal consistency of the SDC scale was
SDC are influenced by demographic variables such as duration of mar-
α = .74 for men's SDC by oneself and α = .72 for women's SDC by
riage, education, or the number of children (Karney & Bradbury, 1995,
oneself.
2005). Considering that from our main variables, only economic strain
was affected by marriage duration in the structural model, we statisti-
2.3.3 | Positive emotions (joy, contentment, and pride) cally controlled for the influence of marriage duration on economic
In the current study, we used joy, contentment, and pride subscales of strain.
the Dispositional Positive Emotions Scale (DPES; Shiota et al., 2006). We relied on common fit indices: chi‐square, comparative fit index
Respondents reported their level of agreement with each item on a (CFI), standardized root mean square residual (SRMR), and the root
7‐point Likert‐type scale ranging from 1 (strongly disagree) to 7 (strongly mean square residual of approximation (RMSEA; Schermelleh‐Engel,
agree). The internal consistency of the Dispositional Positive Emotions Moosbrugger, & Müller, 2003). We used SPSS 22 for descriptive statis-
Scale subscales reported by Shiota et al. (2006) was α = .82 for joy, tics and MPlus 7.11 (Muthén & Muthén, 1998–2014) for the structural
α = .92 for contentment, and α = .80 for pride. The joy subscale is a model. We used full information maximum likelihood estimator and
six‐item questionnaire that measures a dispositional tendency to feel bootstrap option in MPlus to compute model parameters and standard
joy in life (e.g., “I often feel bursts of joy”). In our sample, Cronbach's errors.
alpha was α = .84 for men's joy and α = .85 for women's joy. The con-
tentment subscale is a five‐item questionnaire that measures a dispo-
sitional tendency to feel contentment in general (e.g., “When I think 3 | RESULTS
about my life I experience a deep feeling of contentment”). In our
study, Cronbach's alpha was α = .90 for men's contentment and
3.1 | Descriptive statistics
α = .83 for women's contentment. The pride subscale is a five‐item
questionnaire that measures a dispositional tendency to feel pride (e. Means, standard deviations, and the results of the t tests for paired

g., “I am proud of myself and my accomplishments”). In our study, samples for each of the study variables are presented in Table 1. Over-

Cronbach's alpha was α = .82 for both men's pride and women's pride. all, participants reported moderate levels of economic strain

All measures were completed in the Romanian language. The (MWomen = 2.64; MMen = 2.49; range = 1–5), relatively high levels of

English versions of FESS and DPES were first translated into Romanian positive emotions (for joy MWomen = 4.67, MMen = 4.48; for content-

and then back translated into English by two independent translators ment MWomen = 5.06, MMen = 4.95; for pride MWomen = 5.29,

(one English teacher and one researcher), following the recommenda- MMen = 5.35, range = 1–7), and coping behaviours (for SDC by oneself

tions of Sireci, Yang, Harter, and Ehrlich (2006). Both the original MWomen = 3.83, MMen = 3.78; range = 1–5). In addition, women

English version and the English version resulting from the back transla- reported higher levels of economic strain and joy than men, but the

tion were compared, and the discrepancies were analysed by the trans- effect size of these differences was small.

lators and solved by mutual agreement. Table 2 provides information about the intercorrelations among
the study variables. Economic strain was significantly negatively corre-
lated with positive emotions and SDC in both husbands and wives
2.4 | Analytic strategy
(supporting H1a and H1b). As expected (H2), all three positive emo-
In order to test our hypotheses, we computed a dyadic mediation tions (joy, contentment, and pride) were significantly positively corre-
model. This approach allowed us to control for the interdependence lated with SDC. Results also show significant positive associations
6 RUSU ET AL.

FIGURE 1 Unstandardized coefficients for the final mediation model. Actor and partner effects were constrained to equality across gender.
Marriage duration was included in the model as a control variable, but not depicted for clarity. The multigroup analysis of the model revealed
no significant differences between couples with and without children. Note: n = 295 women and 295 men, **p < .01 (two‐tailed)

TABLE 1 Descriptive statistics and paired‐sample t test for path model has been tested and showed acceptable fit indices, Model 2:
model variables χ2(52) = 125.36, p = .00, CFI = 0.96, RMSEA = 0.06, 90% CI [0.05,
Variable Mean SD Mean difference t Cohen's d 0.08], SRMR = 0.04. In order to test for gender differences, we have

Economic strain restricted the paths in the association between economic strain and
positive emotions, Model 3: χ2(53) = 128.36, p = .00, CFI = 0.96,
Women 2.64 0.96 −0.15** −3.99 0.23
RMSEA = 0.07, 90% CI [0.05, 0.08], SRMR = 0.04; the actor effects
Men 2.49 0.91
in the association between positive emotions and SDC, Model 4:
Joy
χ2(54) = 128.48, p = .00, CFI = 0.96, RMSEA = 0.06, 90% CI [0.05,
Women 4.67 1.00 −0.19** −3.48 0.20
0.08], SRMR = 0.04; and then the partner effects between emotions
Men 4.48 1.00
and SDC, Model 5: χ2(55) = 128.70, p = .00, CFI = 0.96, RMSEA = 0.06,
Contentment
90% CI [0.05, 0.08], SRMR = 0.04. The full mediation model with
Women 5.06 1.16 −0.10 −1.57 0.08
restricted actor and partner effects across gender (Model 5) was the
Men 4.95 1.10
best fitting model. Given that χ2 is sensitive to sample size, we applied
Pride
the rule that χ2/df should be smaller than 3 (Schermelleh‐Engel et al.,
Women 5.29 0.87 0.05 0.9 0.04
2003).
Men 5.35 0.87
In order to analyse gender differences, we computed models
Supportive dyadic coping
including constraints for corresponding paths. Specifically, we imposed
Women 3.83 0.61 −0.05 −1.33 0.07
equality constraints across gender on the effect of economic strain on
Men 3.78 0.63
positive emotions (Model 3), the actor effects of positive emotions on
Note. n = 295 women and 295 men, df = 293. SDC (Model 4), and the partner effects of positive emotions on SDC
**p < .01 (two‐tailed). (Model 5). The results showed that the constraints did not significantly
worsen the models, indicating no gender differences (Table 3). The full
mediation model with restrictions across gender is depicted in Figure 1
. This is the most parsimonious model, indicating that there are no dif-
between the two partners' levels of economic strain (r = .76, p < .001), ferences across gender in the actor and partner effects and the medi-
joy (r = .56, p < .001), contentment (r = .47, p < .001), pride (r = .31, ation paths.
p < .001), emotion‐focused SDC (r = .37, p < .001), and problem‐
focused SDC (r = .33, p < .001).

3.3 | Direct effects


3.2 | Path analysis results 3.3.1 | Economic strain and positive emotions
The mediation model included economic strain, positive emotions, and As presented in Table 4, the direct paths from economic strain to
SDC as latent variables. Table 3 provides an overview of the models female and male positive emotions were statistically significant (as a
that have been tested. Model 1 (partial mediation) had acceptable fit further support of H1a). In our first hypothesis, we assumed a negative
indices: χ (50) = 124.95, p = .00, CFI = 0.96, RMSEA = 0.07, 90% CI
2 association between economic strain and positive emotions. Results
[0.05, 0.08], SRMR = 0.04. Considering that the direct effect of eco- showed that economic strain was significantly negatively related to
nomic strain on SDC was statistically not significant, a full mediation both partners' positive emotions (b = −0.52, p < .001).
RUSU ET AL. 7

TABLE 2 Correlations among path model variables


Variables 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12

1. Male economic strain —


2. Female economic strain .76** —
3. Male joy −.37** −.34** —
4. Female joy −.38** −.46** .56** —
5. Male contentment −.42** −.39** .67** .41** —
6. Female contentment −.39** −.50** .37** .57** .47** —
7. Male pride −.31** −.25** .42** .23** .58** .27** —
8. Female pride −.18** −.27** .28** .48** .31** .53** .31** —
9. Male emotion‐focused SDC oneself −.21** −.23** .28** .19** .32** .26** .37** .23** —
10. Female emotion‐focused SDC oneself −.24** −.30** .26** .33** .37** .38** .27** .31** .37** —
11. Male problem‐focused SDC oneself −.14* −.11 .26** .23** .24** .07 .17** .06 .49** .26** —
12. Female problem‐focused SDC oneself −.15* −.20** .19** .32** .17** .28** .20** .28** .32** .52** .33** —
13. Marriage duration .30** .32** −.19** −.29** −.07 −.15** .02 −.10 −.01 −.13* −.04 −.07

Note. n = 295 men and 295 women. SDC = supportive dyadic coping.
*p < .05 (two‐tailed).
**p < .01 (two‐tailed).

TABLE 3 Goodness‐of‐fit indices for unrestricted and restricted models across gender
Models χ2 (df) p Δχ2 (df) p CFI AIC BIC RMSEA

1. Partial mediation model 124.95 (50) .00 — 0.96 7165.321 7365.868 .07
2. Full mediation model 125.36 (52) .00 0.41 (2) .81 0.96 7161.738 7345.917 .06
3. Restricted paths economic strain: positive emotions across gender 128.36 (53) .00 3 (1) .08 0.96 7162.740 7343.236 .07
4. Restricted actor effects positive emotions: SDC across gender 128.48 (54) .00 0.12 (1) .72 0.96 7160.854 7337.666 .06
5. Restricted partner effects positive emotions: SDC across gender 128.70 (55) .00 0.22 (1) .63 0.96 7159.080 7332.208 .06

Note. AIC = Akaike information criterion; BIC = Bayesian information criterion; CFI = comparative fit index; RMSEA = root mean square residual of approx-
imation; SDC = supportive dyadic coping.

TABLE 4 Parameter estimates for direct and indirect paths of the mediation model

95% bootstrap confidence intervals


b SE p Lower 2.5% Upper 2.5%

Direct effects

MD → ES 0.02 0.00 .00 0.01 0.04


Actor effects

ES → PE −0.52 0.06 .00 −0.66 −0.39

PE → SDC 0.29 0.04 .00 0.19 0.38


Partner effects

PE → SDC 0.26 0.04 .00 0.17 0.35


Indirect effects
Actor–actor mediation
ES → PE → SDC −0.15 0.02 .00 −0.21 −0.10
Actor–partner mediation
ES → PE → SDC −0.13 0.02 .00 −0.19 −0.08

Note. Parameter estimates are unstandardized coefficients. ES = economic strain; MD = marriage duration; PE = positive emotions; SDC = supportive dyadic
coping.

3.3.2 | Economic strain and supportive dyadic coping emotions as mediators. Therefore, the more parsimonious model, with-
The direct paths from economic strain to SDC (H1b) were statistically out the direct path from economic strain to SDC, was preferred over
not significant in the structural model, when we included positive the less parsimonious model.
8 RUSU ET AL.

3.3.3 | Positive emotions and supportive dyadic coping The direct negative association between economic strain and SDC
The findings on the effects of positive emotions on SDC (Table 4) indi- (H1b) was supported by correlation coefficients for both husbands and
cated that female and male positive emotions had a significant direct wives. These findings coincided with similar results from previous
effect on their own SDC (b = 0.29, p < .001), as a further support of studies on the negative association between economic strain and
H2. In addition to actor effects, partner effects were also significant; partners' social and emotional support (Simons et al., 1993; Vinokur
female and male positive emotions had a direct positive effect on their et al., 1996) and expanded prior research that found positive associa-
partner SDC (b = 0.26, p < .001). tions between economic strain and relationship negative behaviours
(Falconier & Epstein, 2010). However, in the path analytical model,
the direct effect of economic strain on SDC was not significant; this
3.4 | Indirect effects association was fully mediated by positive emotions.

For our main hypothesis (H3), we assumed that positive emotions In our second hypothesis, we assumed a positive association

explained the influence of economic strain on SDC. The results between positive emotions and SDC. The results showed significant

(Table 4) indicated that positive emotions (joy, contentment, and pride) actor effects; men and women's positive emotions were positively

mediated both the actor and partner effects of economic strain on related to their own SDC. Moreover, we found significant partner

SDC. Specifically, female and male economic strain had a significant effects; men and women's positive emotions had a positive effect on

indirect effect on their own SDC through their own positive emotions their partner SDC. Therefore, partners scoring highly on positive emo-

(b = −0.15, p < .001), confirming the actor–actor mediation. In addition, tions were more likely to report providing partner support in couples.

we found significant actor–partner mediation. Specifically, female and These findings were in line with other research showing a positive

male positive emotions mediated the association between economic association between the ability to manage emotions and perceived

strain and their partner SDC (b = −0.13, p < .001). social support (Lopes, Salovey, & Straus, 2003). In addition to previous
studies indicating a positive association between positive emotions
and better individual adjustment to stress (Gloria et al., 2013; Tugade
4 | DISCUSSION & Fredrickson, 2004), we found that increasing positive emotions can
also promote DC in couples. These findings highlighted the role of pos-
It has been illustrated that economic strain predicts negative relation- itive emotions in building social resources (Fredrickson, 2001) and
ship outcomes, such as poor relationship satisfaction, instability, and were consistent with past research relating positive emotions with
conflict (e.g., Archuleta et al., 2011; Dew & Yorgason, 2010). Several positive relations, social support, commitment, and relationship satis-
studies have looked into the spillover processes of economic strain faction (Aron et al., 2000; Fredrickson et al., 2008; Gable et al., 2006).
on relationship functioning, and they have identified negative variables The results from our study supported the mediation hypothesis
(e.g., depression and anxiety; Falconier, 2010) as explaining mecha- (H3), indicating that positive emotions (joy, contentment, and pride)
nisms. However, less is known about the influence of economic strain explained the influence of economic strain on their own SDC (actor–
on positive emotions and positive relationship behaviours. Following actor mediation) and on their partner's SDC (actor–partner mediation).
STM (Bodenmann, 1997), the aim of this study was to examine Both wives and husbands, under economic strain, experienced a
whether economic strain is associated with declines in SDC in couples decrease in their feelings of joy, contentment, and pride, which in turn
directly and indirectly by negatively affecting partners' positive negatively affected provided SDC. These findings corroborated prior
emotions. Below is a discussion of the direct and indirect effects found theory and research underlying stress, emotions, and social outcomes
in this study. (Bodenmann, 1997; Fredrickson, 2001; Fredrickson et al., 2008). First,
The hypothesized negative direct association between economic our results provided support to STM (Bodenmann, 1997), which con-
strain and positive emotions (H1a) was supported by both correlation siders that external stress has a negative influence on relational behav-
coefficients and path effects. The findings indicated that men and iours directly or indirectly through a negative influence on individual
women's economic strain was significantly negatively related to their variables. Accordingly, in the current study, the negative influence of
own dispositional positive emotions (i.e., joy, contentment, and pride). economic strain on spouses' SDC was explained by the influence of
These findings were in line with the Reserve Capacity Model (Gallo & economic strain on positive emotions. Second, our study was an exten-
Matthews, 2003) and studies suggesting a negative influence of eco- sion of the broaden‐and‐build theory of positive emotions
nomic strain on positive emotions and well‐being (Gallo et al., 2005; (Fredrickson, 2001) in the context of couples' interaction. Our out-
Jeon & Neppl, 2016). Moreover, our results supplemented existing comes suggest that a lower level of positive emotions induced by
findings that showed that economic strain increased the experience financial strain has negative consequences on partners' social
of negative emotions (i.e., depression and anxiety, Falconier, 2010) resources affecting the support offered to the partner to cope with
and provided support to Zautra et al.'s (2005) assumption that highly stress. In addition, our results supported and extended findings
stressful situations are associated with a decrease of positive emo- reported in a previous study on the impact of financial strain on
tions. Importantly, couples coping with economic strain not only are depression and social support. Vinokur et al. (1996) showed that
in the situation of having additional problems within the relationship depressive symptoms of a partner confronting financial strain damp-
(such as depression, anxiety, and conflict) but also have fewer ened the support provided to their unemployed spouse. According to
resources (positive emotions and SDC) for a successful adaptation to the model proposed by Neff and Karney (2017), we may assume that
these difficulties. economic stress diverts spouses' resources from being connected with
RUSU ET AL. 9

their partners to outside stressors, being thus unable to provide emo- psychological symptoms but also on positive well‐being and increasing
tional and operational support in couples. Shah, Mullainathan, and the experience of positive emotions. A specific intervention, designed
Shafir (2012) revealed in a series of experiments that economic strain for couples experiencing economic strain, is the programme
consumes people's attentional resources; a focus on financial difficul- TOGETHER (Falconier, 2015), which focuses on improving partners'
ties (such as daily basic expenses) will determine them to neglect other individual resources (such as individual coping and problem‐solving
important problems. By analysing positive emotions and SDC, our find- strategies) and dyadic skills (such as communication and SDC). More-
ings offer more light on the spillover of financial strain in couple over, our results support the importance of including positive emotions
relationships. in couple and family therapy. Specific interventions from Positive Fam-
In the present study, we also found gender differences in ily Therapy (Conoley, Plumb, Hawley, Spaventa‐Vancil, & Hernández,
experiencing economic strain and joy, but the effect size of these dif- 2015) could be used with couples under economic strain in order to
ferences was small. Despite the traditional gender role orientation, facilitate partners' positive emotions (e.g., joy, contentment, and pride).
assigning breadwinning and decision‐making responsibilities to men, The therapist may encourage partners to practise capitalization (dis-
wives in our study reported higher levels of economic strain than their closing positive events and expressing positive affects about those
husbands. Even if the effect size is small, this finding is consistent with events) and visualization of successful outcomes (see Conoley et al.,
the results reported in other studies (Falconier, 2010) and might be 2015). However, in addition to focusing on individual and dyadic skills
explained by the fact that women are primarily responsible for family in couple therapy, Neff and Karney (2017) suggest that contextual
care and for managing household expenses. Another gender difference stressors should be addressed directly in the case of highly vulnerable
was related to the dispositional tendency to feel joy in everyday life. couples (such as specific governmental policies and interventions to
This difference supported the literature finding women to be more provide instrumental support).
likely than men to express positive emotions related to others, such
as joy (Alexander & Wood, 2000). 4.2 | Limitations
The findings of our study should be interpreted in light of some limita-
4.1 | Implications tions. One limitation of the current study is that negative emotions
have not been measured, and thus, we could not control for them in
Identifying positive emotions as mediating mechanisms between
the structural model. In addition, the cross‐sectional data limited the
economic strain and SDC has both theoretical and clinical implica-
possibility to analyse the association between study variables over
tions. From a theoretical point of view, our results contribute to a
time and increased the possibility of other causal associations in our
better understanding of the mechanisms involving the spillover
models. The self‐report measures and the procedure of completion
effects of economic strain into relationships. The results of the pres-
of the questionnaires by couples at home increased the probability of
ent study confirmed the STM (Bodenmann, 1997), showing that an
socially desirable answers. Further research should consider a longitu-
external stressor (e.g., economic strain) can influence couples' inter-
dinal or a daily diary design to analyse the influence of economic strain
action (SDC) through its influence on the individual level (positive
on emotions and relationship behaviours over time. Despite these lim-
emotions).
itations, our study contributes to a better understanding of the mech-
From a practical point of view, our findings have implications for
anisms relating economic strain to couple behaviours. Furthermore,
the psychotherapy of couples confronted with economic strain. The
this research is one of the first exploring the negative influence of eco-
current study indicated a link between contextual, intrapersonal, and
nomic strain on positive individual and relationship outcomes.
interpersonal variables. The subjective evaluation of the contextual
factors (such as family economic strain) had a negative influence on
ACKNOWLDEGEMENTS
individual variables (everyday positive emotions, such as joy, content-
ment, and pride), and this affected spouses' interactions with their This study was supported by a grant funded by the Swiss Agency for
partners (SDC by oneself). Cultivating positive emotions in everyday Development and Cooperation (SDC‐CRUS) awarded to the first
life and enhancing a partner's emotion‐focused and problem‐focused author (SCIEX Project12.237).
SDC can help married couples in confronting financial strain.
Clinicians may help couples to be prepared to find more ways of ORCID
experiencing daily positive emotions and supportive behaviours for Petruta P. Rusu http://orcid.org/0000-0003-0868-6054
each other, particularly during vulnerable times of economic strain.
The finding that economic strain was negatively related to positive RE FE RE NC ES
emotions and support in couples suggests the importance of address- Alexander, M. G., & Wood, W. (2000). Women, men, and positive emotions:
ing not only negative aspects but also positive individual and dyadic A social role interpretation. Gender and Emotion: Social Psychological
factors within the context of working with couples under stress. Con- Perspectives, 189–210.

sidering that both negative and positive emotions are influenced by Archuleta, K. L., Britt, S. L., Tonn, T. J., & Grable, J. E. (2011). Financial sat-
stressful contexts (Neff & Karney, 2017) and they have distinct effects isfaction and financial stressors in marital satisfaction. Psychological
on relationship outcomes (Yee et al., 2014), we support the view of Reports, 108(2), 563–576.

Huppert and Whittington (2003) that psychological therapies should Aron, A., Norman, C. C., Aron, E. N., McKenna, C., & Heyman, R. E. (2000).
focus not only on negative well‐being and decreasing the negative Couples' shared participation in novel and arousing activities and
10 RUSU ET AL.

experienced relationship quality. Journal of Personality and Social Psy- September 11th, 2001. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
chology, 78(2), 273–284. 84(2), 365–376.
Barometrul de Opinie Publica (Barometer for Public Opinion) (2007). Gable, S. L., Gonzaga, G. C., & Strachman, A. (2006). Will you be there for
Bucharest, Romania. Retrieved from http://www.fundatia.ro/ me when things go right? Supportive responses to positive event dis-
barometrul‐de‐opinie‐publica‐viata‐cuplu‐mai‐2007 closures. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 91(5), 904–917.
Bodenmann, G. (1997). Dyadic coping: A systemic–transactional view of Gallo, L. C., Bogart, L. M., Vranceanu, A. M., & Matthews, K. A. (2005).
stress and coping among couples: Theory and empirical findings. Euro- Socioeconomic status, resources, psychological experiences, and emo-
pean Review of Applied Psychology, 47(2), 137–141. tional responses: A test of the reserve capacity model. Journal of
Bodenmann, G. (2005). Dyadic coping and its significance in marital func- Personality and Social Psychology, 88(2), 386.
tioning. In T. A. Revenson, K. Kayser, & G. Bodenmann (Eds.), Couples Gallo, L. C., & Matthews, K. A. (2003). Understanding the association
coping with stress: Emerging perspectives on dyadic coping (pp. 33–49). between socioeconomic status and physical health: Do negative emo-
Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. tions play a role? Psychological Bulletin, 129(1), 10.
Bodenmann, G., Meuwly, N., Germann, J., Nussbeck, F. W., Heinrichs, M., & Gloria, C. T., Faulk, K. E., & Steinhardt, M. A. (2013). Positive affectivity pre-
Bradbury, T. N. (2015). Effects of stress on the social support provided dicts successful and unsuccessful adaptation to stress. Motivation and
by men and women in intimate relationships. Psychological Science, Emotion, 37(1), 185–193.
26(10), 1584–1594.
Hamama, L., Ronen, T., Shachar, K., & Rosenbaum, M. (2013). Links
Clavél, F. D., Cutrona, C. E., & Russell, D. W. (2017). United and divided by
between stress, positive and negative affect, and life satisfaction among
stress: How stressors differentially influence social support in African
teachers in special education schools. Journal of Happiness Studies,
American couples over time. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin,
14(3), 731–751.
43(7), 1050–1064.
Haushofer, J., & Fehr, E. (2014). On the psychology of poverty. Science,
Cohn, M. A., Fredrickson, B. L., Brown, S. L., Mikels, J. A., & Conway, A. M.
344(6186), 862–867.
(2009). Happiness unpacked: Positive emotions increase life satisfac-
tion by building resilience. Emotion, 9(3), 361–368. Helms, H. M., Supple, A. J., Su, J., Rodriguez, Y., Cavanaugh, A. M., &
Hengstebeck, N. D. (2014). Economic pressure, cultural adaptation
Conger, K. J., Rueter, M. A., & Conger, R. D. (2000). The role of economic
stress, and marital quality among Mexican‐origin couples. Journal of
pressure in the lives of parents and their adolescents: The family stress
Family Psychology, 28(1), 77.
model.
Conoley, C. W., Plumb, E. W., Hawley, K. J., Spaventa‐Vancil, K. Z., & Herzberg, P. Y. (2013). Coping in relationships: The interplay between indi-
Hernández, R. J. (2015). Integrating positive psychology into family vidual and dyadic coping and their effects on relationship satisfaction.
therapy positive family therapy. The Counseling Psychologist, 43(5), Anxiety, Stress, and Coping, 26(2), 136–153.
703–733. Hilton, J. M., & Devall, E. L. (1997). The Family Economic Strain Scale:
Dew, J. P., & Yorgason, J. (2010). Economic pressure and marital conflict in Development and evaluation of the instrument with single‐and two‐
retirement‐aged couples. Journal of Family Issues, 31(2), 164–188. parent families. Journal of Family and Economic Issues, 18(3), 247–271.

Falconier, M. K. (2010). Female anxiety and male depression: Links Huppert, F. A., & Whittington, J. E. (2003). Evidence for the independence
between economic strain and psychological aggression in Argentinean of positive and negative well‐being: Implications for quality of life
couples. Family Relations, 59(4), 424–438. assessment. British Journal of Health Psychology, 8(1), 107–122.

Falconier, M. K. (2015). TOGETHER—A couples' program to improve Jeon, S., & Neppl, T. K. (2016). Intergenerational continuity in economic
communication, coping, and financial management skills: Development hardship, parental positivity, and positive parenting: The association
and initial pilot‐testing. Journal of Marital and Family Therapy, 41(2), with child behavior. Journal of Family Psychology, 30(1), 22.
236–250. Johnson, M. D., Horne, R. M., & Galovan, A. M. (2016). The developmental
Falconier, M. K., & Epstein, N. B. (2010). Relationship satisfaction in course of supportive dyadic coping in couples. Developmental Psychol-
Argentinian couples under economic strain: Gender differences in a ogy, 52(12), 2031–2043.
dyadic stress model. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 27(6),
Karademas, E. C. (2007). Positive and negative aspects of well‐being: Com-
781–799.
mon and specific predictors. Personality and Individual Differences, 43(2),
Falconier, M. K., & Epstein, N. B. (2011). Female‐demand/male‐withdraw 277–287.
communication in Argentinian couples: A mediating factor between
Karademas, E. C., & Roussi, P. (2016). Financial strain, dyadic coping, rela-
economic strain and relationship distress. Personal Relationships, 18(4),
tionship satisfaction, and psychological distress: A dyadic mediation
586–603.
study in Greek couples. Stress and Health.
Fife, B. L., Weaver, M. T., Cook, W. L., & Stump, T. T. (2013). Partner inter-
Karney, B. R., & Bradbury, T. N. (1995). The longitudinal course of marital
dependence and coping with life‐threatening illness: The impact on
quality and stability: A review of theory, methods, and research. Psycho-
dyadic adjustment. Journal of Family Psychology, 27(5), 702.
logical Bulletin, 118(1), 3–34.
Folkman, S. (2008). The case for positive emotions in the stress process.
Anxiety, Stress, and Coping, 21(1), 3–14. Karney, B. R., & Bradbury, T. N. (2005). Contextual influences on marriage:
Implications for policy and intervention. Current Directions in Psycholog-
Fredrickson, B. L. (1998). What good are positive emotions? Review of ical Science, 14(4), 171–174.
General Psychology, 2(3), 300–319.
Kwon, H. K., Rueter, M. A., Lee, M. S., Koh, S., & Ok, S. W. (2003). Marital
Fredrickson, B. L. (2001). The role of positive emotions in positive psychol-
relationships following the Korean economic crisis: Applying the family
ogy: The broaden‐and‐build theory of positive emotions. American stress model. Journal of Marriage and Family, 65(2), 316–325.
Psychologist, 56(3), 218–226.
Lazarus, R. S., & Folkman, S. (1984). Stress, appraisal, and coping. New York,
Fredrickson, B. L., Cohn, M. A., Coffey, K. A., Pek, J., & Finkel, S. M. (2008).
NY: Springer.
Open hearts build lives: Positive emotions, induced through loving‐
kindness meditation, build consequential personal resources. Journal Ledermann, T., & Kenny, D. A. (2012). The common fate model for dyadic
of Personality and Social Psychology, 95(5), 1045–1062. data: Variations of a theoretically important but underutilized model.
Journal of Family Psychology, 26(1), 140.
Fredrickson, B. L., Tugade, M. M., Waugh, C. E., & Larkin, G. R. (2003). What
good are positive emotions in crisis? A prospective study of resilience Lewis, M., & Sullivan, M. W. (2005). The development of self‐conscious
and emotions following the terrorist attacks on the United States on emotions. Handbook of Competence and Motivation, 185–201.
RUSU ET AL. 11

Lopes, P. N., Salovey, P., Côté, S., Beers, M., & Petty, R. E. (2005). Emotion Shiota, M. N., Campos, B., Keltner, D., & Hertenstein, M. J. (2004). Positive
regulation abilities and the quality of social interaction. Emotion, 5(1), emotion and the regulation of interpersonal relationships. The Regula-
113. tion of Emotion, 127–155.
Lopes, P. N., Salovey, P., & Straus, R. (2003). Emotional intelligence, person- Shiota, M. N., Keltner, D., & John, O. P. (2006). Positive emotion disposi-
ality, and the perceived quality of social relationships. Personality and tions differentially associated with Big Five personality and
Individual Differences, 35(3), 641–658. attachment style. The Journal of Positive Psychology, 1(2), 61–71.

Mauno, S., & Kinnunen, U. (2002). Perceived job insecurity among dual‐ Simons, R. L., Lorenz, F. O., Wu, C. I., & Conger, R. D. (1993). Social network
earner couples: Do its antecedents vary according to gender, economic and marital support as mediators and moderators of the impact of
sector and the measure used? Journal of Occupational and Organiza- stress and depression on parental behavior. Developmental Psychology,
tional Psychology, 75, 295–314. 29(2), 368.
Sireci, S. G., Yang, Y., Harter, J., & Ehrlich, E. J. (2006). Evaluating guidelines
McNulty, J. K. (2016). Chapter five—Highlighting the contextual nature of
for test adaptations a methodological analysis of translation quality.
interpersonal relationships. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology,
Journal of Cross‐Cultural Psychology, 37(5), 557–567.
54, 247–315.
Sturgeon, J. A., Zautra, A. J., & Okun, M. A. (2014). Associations between
Muthén, L. K., & Muthén, B. O. (1998–2014). Mplus user's guide (Seventh
financial stress and interpersonal events: A daily diary study of mid-
ed.). Los Angeles, CA: Author.
dle‐aged adults and their life circumstances. Psychology and Aging,
Neff, L. A., & Karney, B. R. (2004). How does context affect intimate 29(4), 803.
relationships? Linking external stress and cognitive processes within Tice, D. M., Baumeister, R. F., & Zhang, L. (2004). The role of emotion in
marriage. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 30(2), 134–148. self‐regulation: Differing roles of positive and negative emotion. The
Neff, L. A., & Karney, B. R. (2009). Stress and reactivity to daily relationship regulation of emotion, 213–226.
experiences: How stress hinders adaptive processes in marriage. Journal Tugade, M. M. (2011). Positive emotions and coping: Examining dual‐pro-
of Personality and Social Psychology, 97(3), 435. cess models of resilience. In S. Folkman (Ed.), The Oxford handbook of
Neff, L. A., & Karney, B. R. (2017). Acknowledging the elephant in the room: stress, health, and coping (pp. 186–199). Oxford, UK: Oxford University
How stressful environmental contexts shape relationship dynamics. Press.
Current Opinion in Psychology, 13, 107–110. Tugade, M. M., & Fredrickson, B. L. (2004). Resilient individuals use positive
emotions to bounce back from negative emotional experiences. Journal
Papp, L. M., & Witt, N. L. (2010). Romantic partners' individual coping strat-
of Personality and Social Psychology, 86(2), 320–333.
egies and dyadic coping: Implications for relationship functioning.
Journal of Family Psychology, 24(5), 551. Vinokur, A. D., Price, R. H., & Caplan, R. D. (1996). Hard times and hurtful
partners: How financial strain affects depression and relationship satis-
Ponnet, K., Wouters, E., Goedemé, T., & Mortelmans, D. (2013). Family
faction of unemployed persons and their spouses. Journal of Personality
financial stress, parenting and problem behavior in adolescents: An
and Social Psychology, 71(1), 166.
actor–partner interdependence approach. Journal of Family Issues,
37(4), 574–597. Voydanoff, P., & Donnelly, B. W. (1988). Economic distress, family coping,
and quality of family life. In P. Voydanoff, & L. C. Majka (Eds.), Families
Ramsey, M. A., & Gentzler, A. L. (2015). An upward spiral: Bidirectional and economic distress: Coping strategies and social policy. New perspec-
associations between positive affect and positive aspects of close rela- tives on family (pp. 97–115). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
tionships across the life span. Developmental Review, 36, 58–104.
Waugh, C. E., & Fredrickson, B. L. (2006). Nice to know you: Positive emo-
Randall, C., & Corp, A. (2014). Measuring national well‐being: European tions, self–other overlap, and complex understanding in the formation
comparisons. Office for National Statistics. Retrieved from http:// of a new relationship. The Journal of Positive Psychology, 1(2), 93–106.
www.ons.gov.uk/ons/dcp171766_363811.pdf
Westman, M., Vinokur, A. D., Hamilton, V. L., & Roziner, I. (2004). Cross-
Robila, M., & Krishnakumar, A. (2005). Effects of economic pressure over of marital dissatisfaction during military downsizing among
on marital conflict in Romania. Journal of Family Psychology, 19(2), Russian army officers and their spouses. Journal of Applied Psychology,
246–251. 89(5), 769.
Rusu, P. P. (2016). Dyadic coping in Romanian couples. In M. K. Falconier, A. Williams, L. A., & DeSteno, D. (2008). Pride and perseverance: The motiva-
K. Randall, & G. Bodenmann (Eds.), Couples coping with stress: A cultural tional role of pride. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 94(6),
perspective. New York, NY: Routledge. 1007–1017.
Rusu, P. P., Hilpert, P., Turliuc, M. N., & Bodenmann, G. (2016). Dyadic cop- Yee, C. I., Gonzaga, G. C., & Gable, S. L. (2014). Positive emotions in close
ing in an Eastern European context: Validity and measurement relationships. Handbook of Positive Emotions, 215–228.
invariance of the Romanian version of Dyadic Coping Inventory. Zautra, A. J., Affleck, G. G., Tennen, H., Reich, J. W., & Davis, M. C. (2005).
Measurement and Evaluation in Counseling and Development, 49(4). Dynamic approaches to emotions and stress in everyday life: Bolger
Schermelleh‐Engel, K., Moosbrugger, H., & Müller, H. (2003). Evaluating the and Zuckerman reloaded with positive as well as negative affects. Jour-
fit of structural equation models: Tests of significance and descriptive nal of Personality, 73(6), 1511–1538.
goodness‐of‐fit measures. Methods of Psychological Research Online,
8(2), 23–74.
Schiffrin, H. H., & Nelson, S. K. (2010). Stressed and happy? Investigating How to cite this article: Rusu PP, Hilpert P, Falconier M,
the relationship between happiness and perceived stress. Journal of
Bodenmann G. Economic strain and support in couple: The
Happiness Studies, 11(1), 33–39.
mediating role of positive emotions. Stress and Health.
Shah, A. K., Mullainathan, S., & Shafir, E. (2012). Some consequences of
having too little. Science, 338(6107), 682–685. 2017;1–11. https://doi.org/10.1002/smi.2794

You might also like