Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 2

934702

editorial2020
BBXXXX10.1177/1074295620934702Beyond Behavior

Editorial
Beyond Behavior

Positive Reinforcement in Schools:


2020, Vol. 29(2) 67­–68
© Hammill Institute on Disabilities 2020
Article reuse guidelines:
Logic and Application sagepub.com/journals-permissions
DOI: 10.1177/1074295620934702
https://doi.org/10.1177/1074295620934702
journals.sagepub.com/home/bbx

Terrance M. Scott, PhD1 and Timothy J. Landrum, PhD1

Keywords
behavior(s), change, discipline

Special thanks go to the Editors of Beyond Behavior for dignity. That is, it is not just what may make the student
allowing us to guest edit this special issue focused on the successful, but what can affect this outcome in a manner
effective use of positive reinforcement for students with that also maximizes the student’s self-worth, indepen-
emotional and behavioral disorders (EBD). Having both dence, and dignity. This clearly rules out any interventions
taught in self-contained classrooms for students with involving physical or emotional pain, and, for the sake of
EBD, we are acutely aware of the power and necessity of argument, let us say it also eliminates tangible reinforce-
positive feedback with students whose histories are rife ment, that is, no toys, food, goodies, and so on. We submit
with failure and negativity. Still, despite overwhelming that the evidence is clear that positive student outcomes in
and in our view, inarguable evidence of positive reinforce- terms of both dignity and behavioral success are maxi-
ment’s positive potential, myths regarding its ineffective mized by the use of positive reinforcement. But once we
or even counter-productive nature continue to resonate in rule out tangible reinforcement it may be that school per-
educational circles. While there are one or two purveyors sonnel are not familiar with other more instructionally rel-
of this faulty notion that have been particularly responsi- evant options. We wrote the lead article in this issue to
ble for our most recent frustrations (and the impetus for tackle this basic logic and to provide logical responses to
this issue), we have also found that slivers of misinforma- these most common myths.
tion permeate the majority of schools. These faulty notions In the second article, George Sugai and Brandi Simonsen
are what we counter with science and logic in this series of discuss a full array of applications of reinforcement for stu-
articles. dents with challenging behavior. They provide a context for
We see reinforcement not merely as an educational tool, considering function of behavior and describe the applica-
but as a ubiquitously profound principle of behavior. What tion of instruction and reinforcement for such function-
is technically known as reinforcement is simply a descrip- based replacement behaviors. Following this, the third
tion of an acknowledgment for positive behavior, for the article by Todd Whitney and Kera Ackerman provides a
purpose of making such positive behavior more likely to review of research-based methods for the delivery of posi-
occur again in the future. When a student behaves correctly tive feedback during instruction. They describe how posi-
we may acknowledge such with praise, such as “good job.” tive reinforcement can be constructive in helping students
to build knowledge from instructional engagement with
But even this most simple version of positive reinforce-
teachers.
ment has been the target of myths suggesting such state-
The fourth and fifth articles provide a focus on age-ori-
ments actually have the potential of harming students’
ented populations. First, Jessica Hardy and Ragan McLeod
self-perception and motivation (Kohn, 2001). To put this in
provide an overview of special considerations for the effec-
perspective, when someone holds a door for us, the social
tive use of reinforcement procedures with young children.
convention is to thank that person. We believe that nobody
They describe the unique considerations necessary with
worries that this acknowledgment will make the person
this special population and provide a decision-making
less likely to hold doors in the future. So the purpose of this
flowchart to guide practices. Next, Todd Haydon, Meredith
special issue is to clarify a simple universal logic for con-
Murphy, Shobana Musti-Rao, William Hunter, and Jason
sidering when and how to use reinforcement under a vari-
ety of circumstances.
We kick this off with a simple question: what are the 1
University of Louisville, KY, USA
behaviors (outcomes) we wish for our students if they are
Corresponding Author:
to maximize their probability for future success? Lest we Terrance M. Scott, University of Louisville, 106 Education Bldg,
fall into the faulty trap of purporting that the ends justify Louisville, KY 40292, USA.
the means, let us add to the question a notion of student Email: t.scott@louisville.edu
68 Beyond Behavior 29(2)

Boone describe unique considerations in applying positive Declaration of Conflicting Interests


feedback to older students. They focus on some more sys- The author(s) declared no potential conflicts of interest with respect
temic strategies for the delivery of reinforcement, includ- to the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article.
ing the Good Behavior Game, positive peer notes, and
Tootling. Funding
As a final piece, two of our field’s most distinguished The author(s) received no financial support for the research,
elder-scholars, Mike Nelson and Jim Kauffman, provide a authorship, and/or publication of this article.
summarizing commentary. They discuss a history of issues
related to positive reinforcement and pontificate on both the Reference
nature of myths in education and how we must continue our Kohn, A. (2001). Five reasons to stop saying “good job.” Young
commitment to science. Children, 56(5), 24–30.

You might also like