Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 7

See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.

net/publication/280916699

The Development of Children's Memory

Article in Child Development Perspectives · July 2015


DOI: 10.1111/cdep.12129

CITATIONS READS
62 2,800

2 authors:

Wolfgang Schneider Peter A. Ornstein


University of Wuerzburg University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill
600 PUBLICATIONS 19,742 CITATIONS 155 PUBLICATIONS 6,303 CITATIONS

SEE PROFILE SEE PROFILE

All content following this page was uploaded by Peter A. Ornstein on 17 October 2022.

The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file.


CHILD DEVELOPMENT PERSPECTIVES

The Development of Children’s Memory


Wolfgang Schneider1 and Peter A. Ornstein2
1
urzburg, and 2University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill
University of W€

ABSTRACT—The development of memory has been studied constructive activities long before these topics became main-
for more than a century and is one of the most active stream in the 1970s and 1980s. In addition, research on sug-
areas of research in cognitive development. In this article, gestibility (4, 5) and autosuggestibility (4) was inspired by
we first describe historical developments in research on applied questions concerning children’s abilities to provide evi-
children’s memory, focusing on systematic studies that dence in legal settings and anticipated many core research
began in the late 1960s. We then describe three impor- themes on eyewitness testimony (6). Moreover, parallel to their
tant new lines of inquiry: short- and long-term memory research on testimony, in the early 20th century, Stern and Stern
development in infancy, the development of autobio- (7) explored young children’s ability to report personally impor-
graphical memory, and longitudinal research on memory tant events, and at about the same time, Freud (8) identified the
strategies and metamemory. We end by contrasting phenomenon of childhood amnesia, that is, adults’ inability to
research on memory development with that on the develop- recall events that occurred before ages 3 or 4. Finally, in
ment of memory, and identifying an emerging interest in Hunter’s study (9) of his young daughter’s performance on a
biological and social factors that affect developmental delayed reaction task, her active search for a hidden object var-
change. ied as a function of the length of the delay, anticipating modern
studies of working memory (e.g., 10).
KEYWORDS—memory development; history
Although these early studies were important, research on the
development of children’s memory did not catch on in the early
years of the 20th century, despite interest in the applied ques-
A HISTORICAL INTRODUCTION tion of possible links between memory span and intelligence (1,
11). However, interest in studying children’s memory returned
Research on children’s memory can be traced to the early days by the middle 1960s, when Flavell launched a series of studies
of psychology in the 19th century, and many themes explored on memory that focused on strategies for encoding information,
then are decidedly modern in character (1). For example, labora- such as rehearsal (e.g., 12). Flavell positioned this work in the
tory work on age differences in digit span (2) identified memory context of neobehaviorist studies of verbal symbols as mediators
capacity as a foundational construct, and studies of memory for in complex learning situations. Thus, even though he was inter-
prose passages and lists of unrelated words by Binet and Henri ested in age-related differences in children’s cognition, Flavell
(e.g., 3) demonstrated the importance of knowledge and et al. felt compelled to discuss deliberate strategy use in terms
of the more passive mediational framework.
Wolfgang Schneider, University of W€ urzburg; Peter A. Ornstein, From this perspective, it had become clear that even though
University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill. young children might have the appropriate words as mediators
We wish to thank the two anonymous reviewers for their valuable (quite literally between stimuli and responses), their use did not
feedback. always facilitate performance. In fact, Flavell’s research began
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to with a focus on distinguishing between mediational deficiencies
Wolfgang Schneider, Department of Psychology, University of (i.e., failures of generated mediators to work in the sense of facil-
W€urzburg, Wittelsbacherplatz 1, D-97074 W€ urzburg, Germany; itating remembering) and production deficiencies (i.e., failures
e-mail: Schneider@psychologie.uni-wuerzburg.de; or Peter A.
Ornstein, Department of Psychology, University of North Carolina
to produce the mediators). These pioneering studies dramatically
at Chapel Hill, Chapel Hill, NC 27599-3270; e-mail: pao@unc.edu. influenced the next generation of researchers, who used a cogni-
© 2015 The Authors
tive framework to focus on age-related changes in children’s
Child Development Perspectives © 2015 The Society for Research in Child Development information-processing skills (e.g., 13, 14). Aided by the
DOI: 10.1111/cdep.12129

Volume 9, Number 3, 2015, Pages 190–195


The Development of Children’s Memory 191

widespread interest in cognition, research on the development of NEW DEVELOPMENTS IN THE FIELD
memory grew rapidly, and by 1971, enough research had been
done for Flavell to organize a symposium entitled “What is The first 20 years of research on children’s memory and its
memory development the development of?” at the biennial development focused mostly on documenting age-related pro-
meeting of the Society for Research in Child Development. This gression in children’s verbally based memory skills. The empha-
foundational question stimulated new research that changed the sis was on deliberate memory, that is, on what children of
focus of theoretical analyses of children’s memory, shifting the different ages could remember and what they did when con-
emphasis to the interplay between the development of cognitive fronted with deliberate memory demands. Research participants
resources, in general, and the functions of memory, in were primarily school-age children and the studies were almost
particular. completely cross-sectional. All of this changed in the next
The new perspective on cognitive development proved genera- 20 years.
tive and led to a melding of Piagetian and information-process- During this time, several new lines of research emerged, more
ing points of view in the influential research of neo-Piagetian or less in parallel. We outline three because of their strong links
researchers such as Case (e.g., 15) and Pascual-Leone (16). with the historical approaches described earlier. First, research
This, in turn, yielded insights into the nature of memory capac- with infants revealed surprising mnemonic skills that had been
ity and its development. These theoretical perspectives on mem- overlooked. Second, studies of children’s autobiographical mem-
ory capacity, as well as new models developed by Baddeley and ory revealed early-developing skills in using language to report
Hitch (17) and Cowan (18), attracted attention and stimulated previous experiences. Third, longitudinal studies of remember-
empirical research on the importance of working memory for ing enabled the study of changes in individuals’ skills over time,
other domains and for understanding the constraints on chil- and provided an effective way of integrating several themes.
dren’s memory performance and development. Related research
demonstrated further that the capacity of an individual’s working Memory Development in Infancy
memory limits the higher-order cognitive operations one can Although Hunter’s classic study (9) on his young daughter’s
perform (19), including those called for in academic domains working memory did not receive much attention in the early
such as arithmetic and in carrying out deliberate strategies for days, research on infant memory gained momentum with the
remembering. invention of new technologies in the early 1980s. Early work
Given the resulting interest in children’s memory, a detailed with a visual paired comparison task provided evidence that
picture of the development of deliberate memory skills emerged infants can retain briefly experienced visual information for up
over the next 15 years. Strategies for encoding information— to 2 weeks, working on the assumption that memory is indexed
such as organization, rehearsal, and elaboration—were observed by longer looking at a pre-exposed (and hence now-familiar)
to play considerable roles in children’s memory performance, stimulus than at a novel one (e.g., 28). However, because the
with age-related changes in the complexity of these strategies resulting measures of infants’ performance reflect a combination
contributing to changes in performance with age (e.g., 20). How- of both short- and long-term memory processes—a distinction
ever, researchers also recognized that children’s mnemonic introduced by William James (29) that became one of the
skills can be influenced dramatically by context variables (e.g., themes of the information-processing perspective—researchers
task demands, effort requirements, motivation; 21), by what they had to develop methods for generating uncontaminated measures
know about the materials being remembered (22, 23), and by of these forms of memory. (Because the constructs of short-term
their metamemory, or their understanding of the operation of and working memory cannot be easily distinguished in young
memory, and the task and situational conditions that affect suc- children [10, 30], we will not differentiate them).
cessful remembering (24, 25). Recently, researchers (31) have obtained uncontaminated
Researchers also understood that automatic factors may estimates of infants’ short-term memory by adapting a change-
sometimes influence the use of strategies. For example, highly detection paradigm used with adults. With this technique,
organized or salient sets of stimulus materials may elicit infants are presented simultaneously with two visual displays
advanced rehearsal and organizational strategies—via interitem that blink on and off repeatedly, one of which changes continu-
word associations—at a time in development when they might ously (e.g., three squares presented in different locations) while
not otherwise be observed (26). Furthermore, with practice and the other does not change (e.g., three squares presented in the
experience in executing particular strategies—along with the same locations). Assuming that infants would look longer at the
development of underlying information-handling capabilities changing display if they could form a memory representation of
such as processing speed (27)—procedures that were once the information displayed and keep it active in the brief delay
difficult to perform may later be executed with relative between presentations, 4- and 6½-month-olds remembered only
ease because they become automatized and thus demand less 1 or 2 items, whereas 10- and 13-month-olds remembered 3 or
effort (21). 4 items. Thus, infants’ initial short term/working memory is lim-

Child Development Perspectives, Volume 9, Number 3, 2015, Pages 190–195


192 Wolfgang Schneider and Peter A. Ornstein

ited but changes significantly in the first year of life. The perfor- dren had more overall recall, reported more information in
mance of the older infants is impressive, given that adults’ response to open-ended questions, and thus depended less on
capacity for the objects used in these experiments is also limited yes/no questions to elicit memory (e.g., 36). Moreover, older
to three or four items. children forgot less over time (e.g., 37) and were less susceptible
Moving beyond the assessment of short-term/working memory, to suggestive questions (38).
studies using conditioning and imitation-based paradigms have Young children’s autobiographical memory is also reflected in
informed our understanding of infants’ long-term memory. A their conversations with parents and other adults about events
conjugate reinforcement task (e.g., 32) used an operant condi- they have experienced. In this regard, children begin to talk
tioning method to study learning and retention in 2- to 6- about past activities almost as soon as they produce their first
month-olds. In this work, a ribbon connected an infant’s foot to words, and their skills for recalling past experiences in
an attractive mobile that was placed overhead so with each parent–child conversations develop rapidly between ages 2 and
kick, children saw the mobile move. Remembering was inferred 4. Nevertheless, when children first begin to reminisce, the adult
if the rate of kicking when the ribbon was disconnected from partner provides most of the content and structure, with the
the mobile was greater than that in a baseline period. In the child assuming more responsibility for the conversation as he or
elicited imitation-based tasks (33), an infant witnessed the mod- she grows older. Indeed, with age and increased experience
eling of simple sets of actions (e.g., making a gong), and mem- talking about the past, children’s reports become more detailed
ory was inferred if he or she could later act out the modeled and complex, and depend less on information provided by adult
sequences. conversational partners (e.g., 39).
Although the researchers who developed these tasks differ in One salient feature of the now-extensive literature on remi-
their interpretation of the underlying memory systems that are niscing is that of variability across parents in how they structure
tapped by the conjugate reinforcement and imitation paradigms, these conversations. Put simply, researchers have identified two
taken together, research with these techniques demonstrates broad reminiscing styles: high elaborative engagement and low
long-term memory in young infants, illustrating that they can elaborative engagement (39). In contrast with parents who use a
retrieve information after extended periods. In many studies low elaborative style, those who use a high elaborative style ask
using variants of the conjugate reinforcement procedure, reten- many questions, follow in on their children’s efforts to contribute
tion increases linearly between 2 and 18 months. The imitation- to the conversation, and continue to add new information even
based procedures also reveal long-term memory in very young when children do not. These reminiscing styles generalize across
children and indicate strides in recall across the first year of different types of discussions about past events (e.g., excursions
infancy and beyond. Whereas the temporal extent of memory and holidays, outings to the zoo or a museum) and are consistent
seems limited in the first 6 months of life, 6-month-olds can over several years with the same children. Children exposed to
remember simple events after 24 hr, and 9- to 12-month-olds highly elaborative conversation remember more than children
recall multistep sequences after 4–6 weeks. who experience low elaborative conversation. These differences
Although additional research is needed to understand how in maternal reminiscing are associated with later differences in
these indicators converge to characterize children’s skill at any children’s abilities to recall personally experienced events.
one point in development, a high-functioning memory system is To some extent, research on children’s autobiographical mem-
in place before language is available for encoding and reporting ory was prompted by concerns about their abilities to provide
information. Of course, children’s abilities change once they can evidence in legal situations (38), and findings such as those
use language, as is seen in studies of autobiographical memory. reported here have enhanced our understanding of children’s
testimony. Overall, several early findings (e.g., 5) have been
Development of Autobiographical Memory confirmed (e.g., age differences in recall and the negative effects
As already indicated by the Sterns’ early case studies (7), chil- of misleading questions and repeated questioning), but early
dren’s autobiographical memory is reflected in their accounts of assumptions that young children are generally unreliable wit-
specific, distinctive events that they have experienced. In this nesses have not been supported. Admittedly, young children’s
section, we first discuss children’s memory for salient target eyewitness testimony can be less accurate than that of older
events and then consider event memories expressed in conversa- children and adults, but when young children are questioned
tions between parents and children, as well as the implications with appropriate interview protocols about events they under-
of this work for understanding eyewitness testimony. stand, they can provide accurate reports. Guidelines for inter-
In some studies of autobiographical memory, preschoolers and viewing children in the legal system have emerged from this
children in the early elementary grades were exposed to spe- research (e.g., 40).
cially crafted stimulus events, such as visiting a pretend zoo
(34), whereas in others, the focus was on naturally occurring The Impact of Recent Longitudinal Memory Research
medical experiences (e.g., 35). Many studies of children’s auto- Most of the foundational work on memory development was
biographical memory indicate that with increases in age, chil- based on cross-sectional studies that documented the skills of

Child Development Perspectives, Volume 9, Number 3, 2015, Pages 190–195


The Development of Children’s Memory 193

children of different ages but provided little insight into devel- remained vibrant and relevant for both basic understanding and
opmental change in individual children. Longitudinal research application. We also showed how the 1971 symposium, “What
designs are necessary to track such change, and several studies is memory development the development of?” set the stage for
(e.g., the Munich Longitudinal Study of the Ontogenesis of Indi- 40 years of productive research (47). Of course, the answers to
vidual Competencies) were launched in the mid-1980s that Flavell’s question have changed over the years and include
lasted a decade or more, included many participants, and strategies, metacognition, capacity, and autobiographical reports.
tapped several features of memory and cognition. As a result, researchers have learned much about memory
One contribution of this longitudinal work is the understand- development, in the sense of characterizing the skills of children
ing that several factors contribute to the growth of working mem- of different ages. However, what has been missing is a focus on
ory, including a type of domain-general cognitive primitive the development of memory, that is, on understanding the endog-
mechanism (41). More specifically, factors such as brain growth, enous and exogenous factors that affect developmental changes
increases in knowledge, strategy use, and processing speed, as in the skills that have been well-documented. Nonetheless,
well as changes in the rate of memory trace decay, contribute to researchers have begun to address this critical issue, and we
developmental changes in working memory (42). can see tentative answers emerging from research in develop-
A second contribution relates to our greater understanding of mental cognitive neuroscience (e.g., 48) and studies of the
developmental changes in children’s strategic competence. In socialization of children’s memory (e.g., 49). Both of these areas
cross-sectional studies, strategy use appears to improve continu- demonstrate how one can study both the development of memory
ously with age, but longitudinal studies indicate that these tech- and memory development.
niques do not develop in such a straightforward way. Instead,
individual children’s use of memory strategies may actually REFERENCES
increase abruptly at different points, and the ages at which
mnemonic techniques are acquired are apparently relative and 1. Schneider, W. (2000). Research on memory development: Historical
variable across different strategies. For example, even pre- trends and current themes. International Journal of Behavioral
school- and kindergarten-age children use intentional strategies, Development, 24, 407–420. doi: 10.1080/016502500750037955
both in ecologically valid settings such as hide-and-seek tasks 2. Jacobs, J. (1887). Experiments on “prehension”. Mind, 12, 75–
79.
and in assessments in the laboratory (43). The longitudinal stud-
3. Binet, H., & Henri, V. (1894). La memoire des phrases (memoire
ies also demonstrate that acquiring strategies involves more than des idees) [Memory for sentences (memory for ideas)]. L’Annee Psy-
replacing an ineffective technique with a more effective proce- chologique, 1, 24–59.
dure. Indeed, simple and inefficient strategies reside alongside 4. Binet, H. (1900). La suggestibilite [Suggestibility]. Paris, France:
more sophisticated and efficient ones, and strategies do not Schleicher.
always help performance, although most children use strategies 5. Stern, W. (1910). Abstracts of lectures on the psychology of testi-
competently by the end of elementary school (see also 44, 45). mony and on the study of individuality. American Journal of Psy-
chology, 21, 270–282. doi: 10.2307/1413003
A third contribution of longitudinal work has been to clarify
6. Bruck, M., & Ceci, S. J. (1999). The suggestibility of children’s
the developmental links between metamemory and later use of memory. Annual Review of Psychology, 50, 419–439. doi: 10.1146/
strategies. Most researchers have assumed that children would annurev.psych.50.1.419
not use strategies for remembering until they have adequate 7. Stern, C., & Stern, W. (1909). Recollection, testimony and lying in
levels of metamnemonic understanding. However, testing this early childhood. Leipzig, Germany: J.A. Barth.
assumption requires information about strategies and metame- 8. Freud, S. (1905/1953). Childhood and concealing memories. In A.
mory from the same child over time to determine whether A. Brill (Trans. & Ed.), The basic writings of Sigmund Freud (pp.
62–68). New York, NY: The Modern Library.
knowledge of the use of strategies leads to their later use. In
9. Hunter, W. S. (1917). The delayed reaction in a child. Psychological
fact, longitudinal studies support the link between earlier Review, 24, 74–87. doi: 10.1037/h0074768
metamemory and later use of strategies (46). Longitudinal 10. Reznick, J. S. (2009). Working memory in infants and toddlers. In
work also shows that links among strategy usage, metamemory, M. L. Courage & N. Cowan (Eds.), The development of memory in
and recall strengthen with age, and that knowledge of a strat- infancy and childhood (pp. 343–365). Hove, UK: Psychology
egy is connected more strongly to use of a strategy than to Press.
recall performance, primarily because strategy use directed by 11. Baker-Ward, L., & Ornstein, P. A. (2014). The coaction of theory
metamemory is only one of several determinants of perfor- and methods in the study of the development of memory. In P. J.
Bauer & R. Fivush (Eds.), The Wiley handbook on the development
mance (30).
of children’s memory (Vol. I, pp. 41–64). Chichester, UK: Wiley-
Blackwell. doi: 10.1002/9781118597705.ch3
CONCLUDING REMARKS 12. Flavell, J. H., Beach, D. H., & Chinsky, J. M. (1966). Spontaneous
verbal rehearsal in a memory task as a function of age. Child Devel-
In this article, we provided a historical perspective for the study opment, 37, 283–299. doi: 10.2307/1126804
of children’s memory and demonstrated that the field has

Child Development Perspectives, Volume 9, Number 3, 2015, Pages 190–195


194 Wolfgang Schneider and Peter A. Ornstein

13. Hagen, J. W. (1971). Some thoughts on how children learn to 30. Schneider, W. (2015). Memory development from early childhood to
remember. Human Development, 14, 262–271. doi: 10.1159/ emerging adulthood. New York, NY: Springer. doi: 10.1007/978-3-
000271220 319-09611-7
14. Ornstein, P. A., Naus, M. J., & Liberty, C. (1975). Rehearsal and 31. Oakes, L. M., Ross-Sheehy, S., & Luck, S. J. (2007). The develop-
organizational processes in children’s memory. Child Development, ment of visual short-term memory in infancy. In L. M. Oakes & P. J.
46, 818–830. doi: 10.2307/1128385 Bauer (Eds.), Short- and long-term memory in infancy and child-
15. Case, R. (1978). Intellectual development from birth to adulthood: hood: Taking the first steps toward remembering (pp. 75–102). New
A neo-Piagetian interpretation. In R. S. Siegler (Ed.), Children’s York, NY: Oxford University Press.
thinking: What develops? (pp. 37–72). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. 32. Rovee-Collier, C., & Cuevas, K. (2009). The development of infant
16. Pascual-Leone, J. (1970). A mathematical model for the transition memory. In M. L. Courage & N. Cowan (Eds.), The development of
rule in Piaget’s developmental stages. Acta Psychologica, 63, 301– memory in infancy and childhood (pp. 11–41). Hove, UK: Psychol-
345. doi: 10.1016/0001-6918(70)90108-3 ogy Press.
17. Baddeley, A. D., & Hitch, G. J. (1974). Working memory. In G. 33. Bauer, P. J. (2007). Remembering the times of our lives: Memory in
Bower (Ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation: Advances in infancy and beyond. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
research and theory (Vol. 8, pp. 47–89). New York, NY: Academic 34. McGuigan, F., & Salmon, K. (2004). The time to talk: The influence
Press. doi: 10.1016/S0079-7421(08)60452-1 of the timing of adult–child talk on children’s event memory. Child
18. Cowan, N. (1988). Evolving conceptions of memory storage, Development, 75, 669–686. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2004.00700.x
selective attention, and their mutual constraints within the human 35. Ornstein, P. A., Baker-Ward, L., Gordon, B. N., & Merritt, K. A.
information processing system. Psychological Bulletin, 104, (1997). Children’s memory for medical experiences: Implications for
163–191. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.104.2.163 testimony. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 11, 87–104. doi: 10.1002/
19. Bjorklund, D. F., & Sellers, P. D., II. (2014). Memory development (SICI)1099-0720(199712)11:7<S87::AID-ACP556>3.0.CO;2-Z
in evolutionary perspective. In P. J. Bauer & R. Fivush (Eds.), The 36. Ornstein, P. A., Haden, C. A., & San Souci, P. (2008). The develop-
Wiley handbook on the development of children’s memory (Vol. I, pp. ment of skilled remembering in children. In H. L. Roediger, III & J.
126–156). Chichester, UK: Wiley-Blackwell. doi: 10.1002/ H. Byrne (Eds.), Cognitive psychology of memory. Learning and
9781118597705.ch7 memory: A comprehensive reference (Vol. 2, pp. 715–744). Oxford,
20. Schneider, W., & Pressley, M. (1997). Memory development between UK: Elsevier.
two and twenty (2nd ed.). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. 37. Bauer, P. J., & Larkina, M. (2014). Childhood amnesia in the mak-
21. Ornstein, P. A., Baker-Ward, L., & Naus, M. J. (1988). The develop- ing: Different distributions of autobiographical memories in children
ment of mnemonic skill. In F. E. Weinert & M. Perlmutter (Eds.), and adults. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 143, 597–
Memory development: Universal changes and individual differences 611. doi: 10.1037/a0033307
(pp. 31–50). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. 38. Ceci, S. J., & Bruck, M. (1995). Jeopardy in the courtroom: A scien-
22. Chi, M. T. H. (1978). Knowledge structures and memory develop- tific analysis of children’s testimony. Washington, DC: American
ment. In R. Siegler (Ed.), Children’s thinking: What develops? (pp. Psychological Association.
73–96). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. 39. Fivush, R., Haden, C. A., & Reese, E. (2006). Elaborating on elabo-
23. Bjorklund, D. F. (1985). The role of conceptual knowledge in the rations: Role of maternal reminiscing style in cognitive and socio-
development of organization in children’s memory. In C. J. Brainerd emotional development. Child Development, 77, 1568–1588. doi:
& M. Pressley (Eds.), Basic processes in memory development (pp. 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2006.00960.x
103–142). New York, NY: Springer. 40. Pipe, M.-E., & Salmon, K. (2009). Memory development and the
24. Flavell, J. H., & Wellman, H. M. (1977). Metamemory. In R. V. Kail forensic context. In M. L. Courage & N. Cowan (Eds.), The develop-
& J. W. Hagen (Eds.), Perspectives on the development of memory ment of memory in infancy and childhood (pp. 241–282). Hove, UK:
and cognition (pp. 3–33). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Psychology Press.
25. Schneider, W. (1985). Developmental trends in the metamemory- 41. Kail, R. V., & Ferrer, E. (2007). Processing speed in childhood and
memory behavior relationship: An integrated review. In D.-L. For- adolescence: Longitudinal models for examining developmental
rest-Pressley, G. E. MacKinnon & T. G. Waller (Eds.), Cognition, change. Child Development, 78, 1760–1770. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-
metacognition, and human performance (Vol. 1, pp. 57–109). New 8624.2007.01088.x
York, NY: Academic Press. 42. Cowan, N. (2014). Short-term and working memory in childhood. In
26. Lange, G. (1973). The development of conceptual and rote P. J. Bauer & R. Fivush (Eds.), The Wiley handbook on the develop-
recall skills among school age children. Journal of Experimental ment of children’s memory (Vol. I, pp. 202–229). Chichester, UK:
Child Psychology, 15, 394–406. doi: 10.1016/0022-0965(73) Wiley-Blackwell. doi: 10.1002/9781118597705.ch10
90090-8 43. Schneider, W. (2014). Individual differences in memory develop-
27. Kail, R. V. (1993). Processing time decreases globally at an expo- ment and educational implications: Cross-sectional and longitudinal
nential rate during childhood and adolescence. Journal of Experi- evidence. In P. J. Bauer & R. Fivush (Eds.), The Wiley handbook on
mental Child Psychology, 56, 254–265. doi: 10.1006/jecp.1993. the development of children’s memory (Vol. II, pp. 947–971). Chich-
1034 ester, UK: Wiley-Blackwell. doi: 10.1002/9781118597705.ch40
28. Fagan, J. F., III. (1970). Memory in the infant. Journal of Experi- 44. Kron-Sperl, V., Schneider, W., & Hasselhorn, M. (2008). The devel-
mental Child Psychology, 9, 217–226. doi: 10.1016/0022-0965(70) opment and effectiveness of memory strategies in kindergarten and
90087-1 elementary school: Findings from the W€ urzburg and G€ottingen lon-
29. James, W. (1890). The principles of psychology. New York, NY: gitudinal studies. Cognitive Development, 23, 79–104. doi: 10.1016/
Dover. j.cogdev.2007.08.011

Child Development Perspectives, Volume 9, Number 3, 2015, Pages 190–195


The Development of Children’s Memory 195

45. Siegler, R. S. (2006). Microgenetic analyses of learning. In D. 48. Ghetti, S., & Lee, J. K. (2014). The development of recollection and
Kuhn & R. S. Siegler (Vol. Eds.), W. Damon & R. M. Lerner familiarity during childhood: Insight from studies of behaviour and
(Editors-in-Chief), Handbook of child psychology: Cognition, per- brain. In P. J. Bauer & R. Fivush (Eds.), The Wiley handbook on the
ception, and language (Vol. 2, pp. 464–510). New York, NY: development of children’s memory (pp. 309–335). Chichester, UK:
Wiley. doi: 10.1002/9780470147658.chpsy0211 Wiley-Blackwell. doi: 10.1002/9781118597705.ch14
46. Grammer, J. K., Purtell, K. M., Coffman, J. L., & Ornstein, P. A. 49. Ornstein, P. A., Haden, C. A., & Coffman, J. L. (2011). Learning to
(2011). Relations between children’s metamemory and strategic per- remember: Mothers and teachers talking with children. In N. L.
formance: Time-varying covariates in early elementary school. Jour- Stein & S. Raudenbush (Eds.), Developmental science goes to school
nal of Experimental Child Psychology, 108, 139–155. doi: 10.1016/ (pp. 69–83). New York, NY: Taylor & Francis.
j.jecp.2010.08.001
47. Ornstein, P. A., & Haden, C. A. (2001). Memory development or the
development of memory? Current Directions in Psychological Science,
10, 202–209. doi: 10.1111/1467-8721.00149

Child Development Perspectives, Volume 9, Number 3, 2015, Pages 190–195

View publication stats

You might also like