Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 7

Received: 26 March 2021 | Accepted: 28 June 2021

DOI: 10.1111/jasp.12811

ORIGINAL ARTICLE

Future orientation helps regulate emotion in the future

Lei Zheng1,2,3,4 | Yidi Chen1 | Yiqun Gan1

1
School of Psychological and Cognitive
Sciences and Beijing Key Laboratory Abstract
of Behavior and Mental Health, Peking Although the adaptive function of future orientation has been demonstrated, whether
University, Beijing, China
2 an individual's orientation toward the future regulates emotions to cope with future
School of Economics and Management,
Fuzhou University, Fujian, China stressors remains unknown. The present study aimed to investigate the emotion
3
Center for China Social Trust Research, regulation function of future orientation in a goal-­related stressful situation. Eighty
Fuzhou University, Fujian, China
4
participants were randomly assigned to an experiment group or a control group. The
Institute of Psychological and Cognitive
Sciences, Fuzhou University, Fujian, China experiment group was primed by a future-­oriented manipulation, whereas the con-
trol group was primed by a neutral condition; then, both groups performed the Trier
Correspondence
Yiqun Gan, School of Psychological and Social Stress Test (TSST). The results showed that the experiment group exhibited
Cognitive Sciences, Peking University, reduced negative mood, increased positive mood, and a lower peak cortisol level
Beijing 100871, China.
Email: ygan@pku.edu.cn after the TSST task. In conclusion, future orientation is considered an effective ap-
proach to regulate future emotions in response to stress. Our results render insights
Funding information
This research was funded by the National to developing strategies in coping with occupational stress.
Social Science Foundation of China (Grant
number: 20BSH139; 20CSH073).

1 | I NTRO D U C TI O N future orientation is particularly important in the emotion regulation


process and for mental health.
Why are some people able to manage stress and realize their dreams, Prior research has proposed that being future-­oriented could be
while others live in a state of suffering and pain? The famous movie, an approach to help people manage emotions and reduce the poten-
The Shawshank Redemption, provides us with part of the answer. In the tial adverse impact of stress (Folkman & Moskowitz, 2004). These
film, Andy was wrongly accused of murder and sentenced to life im- studies have shown future orientation enhanced the capacity to reg-
prisonment, yet he dreamed of escaping to a coastal town and living ulate emotions in the face of future possible obstacles, thus result-
with freedom. This future-­oriented thinking provided him the strength ing in fewer negative psychological outcomes (Johnson et al., 2014).
to fight with fate, overcome difficulties, and reach his final goal. As opposed to reactive cognition, this proactive self-­
regulation
Future orientation is a goal-­related concept and is characterized leads people to move toward future goals and self-­
promotion
by thinking about and acting toward future outcomes (Nurmi, 1991; (Aspinwall, 2006). In particular, being future-­oriented causes indi-
Seginer, 2008). Prior research has demonstrated the adaptive function viduals to think about the future in relation to stressful contexts and
of future orientation (Gan et al., 2017). Thinking about the future may helps them to shift their attention to a hopeful future for better lifes-
lead people to weigh the benefits of the distant future more heavily, as pan development (Gan et al., 2017; Tucker et al., 2013). Therefore,
opposed to the present stressful situation, which helps them regulate future-­oriented people weigh the benefits of long-­term goals more
emotions and exhibit adaptive behaviors (MacLeod & Conway, 2005). heavily, which may lead them to focus on the potential for inherent
Numerous studies have investigated the adaptive function of gain or growth rather than the aspect of harms and losses, suggest-
future orientation (Sze et al., 2017). For instance, people who were ing the adaptive function of future-­oriented thinking.
highly future-­oriented exhibited fewer negative outcomes when fac- Taken together, people who are strongly oriented toward the fu-
ing stress (Zheng et al., 2019). After receiving future-­oriented train- ture tend to be affected by future goals (Gan et al., 2013; MacLeod
ing (e.g., goal setting and planning), individuals reported a decrease & Conway, 2005). However, there is no empirical evidence showing
in depressive symptoms and higher levels of well-­being compared to that being oriented toward the future is able to effect one's emo-
a control group (MacLeod et al., 2008; Vilhauer et al., 2012). Thus, tional reactions against stress in an acute stressful situation. Given

J Appl Soc Psychol. 2021;00:1–7. wileyonlinelibrary.com/journal/jasp© 2021 Wiley Periodicals LLC | 1


2 | ZHENG et al.

the literature review above, a benefit of having long-­term goals is potential health risks caused by the acute stress associated with
that they are able to promote adaptive behaviors (Sze et al., 2017), performing the TSST (Kirschbaum et al., 1993; Liu et al., 2017), we
and thereby result in positive outcomes. excluded participants with cardiovascular disease or psychiatric dis-
This study employed a widely used paradigm—­the Trier Social orders including anxiety and depression. Students who majored in
Stress Test (TSST) (Kirschbaum et al., 1993), which has an impact psychology were also excluded because many of them had already
on both the affective system and hypothalamic–­
pituitary–­
adrenal heard of the TSST. In order to control individual factors that may have
(HPA) axis activity (Liu et al., 2017). In this study, salivary cortisol an effect on the measurement of cortisol responses to stress, par-
was chosen as the biomarker to characterize HPA axis responses to ticipants confirmed that they did not have a chronic or acute illness,
stress levels. Salivary cortisol offers a more objective physiological use drugs or alcohol, or take medication regularly. Additionally, female
significance compared to the self-­report subjective stress method participants confirmed that they were not in their menstrual cycle.
(Hellhamamer et al., 2009). Previous studies have found the peaks There were 90 participants who were randomly assigned to ei-
of salivary cortisol lag behind the self-­reported stress after the TSST ther a control or experiment group. After the removal of 10 par-
because the release of salivary cortisol triggered by HPA axis gradu- ticipants who reported minimal stress after the Trier Social Stress
ally increases following a stressor, reaching peak levels at about 20–­ Task, the current study analyzed the remaining data containing 80
30 min after the onset of stressors (Helminen et al., 2019). Moreover, participants: 41 in the control group and 39 in the experiment group,
previous studies on subjective stress and cortisol levels have not 32 males and 48 females, age ranging from 17 to 29 years (Mean =
reached a consistent conclusion (Jentsch & Wolf, 2020). Dickerson 22.01, SD = 2.75). Among them, 50 were undergraduate students,
and Kemeny (2004) did not find a correspondence between cortisol and the rest were graduate students. Their average number of in-
responses and subjective distress, while a weak correlation was ob- ternships was 1.45 (SD = 1.71). In addition, ten participants only
tained between perceived stress and HPA axis activities (i.e., corti- completed the psychological assessments, and therefore, heart rate
sol levels) in a large sample of adolescents (Oldehinkel et al., 2011). data and salivary samples were collected from 70 participants.
A meta-­analysis reported that only 25% of studies found a correla-
tion between subjective stress and cortisol under TSST (Campbell &
Ehlert, 2011). Therefore, in this study, we reported both self-­reported 2.3 | Materials
stress and salivary cortisol levels following TSST. In addition, this
study only employed the speech content part of the TSST, which has 2.3.1 | Positive affect and Negative affect scale
been shown to have an effect on the affective system and cortisol
level as well (Liu et al., 2017). We hypothesized that future-­oriented PANAS is a Chinese version of the Positive and Negative Affect Schedule
participants would report less negative affect, more positive affect, (Guo & Gan, 2010), which was adapted from Watson, Clark, and Tellegen
and lower levels of peak cortisol concentration in response to the (1988). As some of the dimensions are unrelated to the present research
stress situation when compared to control group participants. (i.e., guilty), positive affect and negative affect were measured by the 7-­
item, respectively. Responses were provided using a scale ranging from
1 (not at all) to 7 (extremely). Each dimension has been demonstrated to
2 | M E TH O DS have good psychometric properties in previous studies. The Cronbach's
alpha ranged from .88 to .97 for the present research.
2.1 | Sample size calculation

Previous studies that examined the relationships between future 2.3.2 | Chinese resilience scale
temporal perspective and moods (i.e., positive mood and nega-
tive mood) reported effect sizes ranging from r = .29 to r = .44 Chinese resilience scale was measured with a Chinese resilience
(MacLeod, 1996). Based on these effect sizes, we first transformed scale (Hu & Gan, 2008). The scale includes two subscales, personal
them to Cohen's f, and selected the minimum Cohen's f as the prior strength, and support strength. Based on the current research pur-
effect size to calculate the minimum-­required sample size. For the pose, only the personal strength subscale was selected to assess
between factors design of a 2 × 2 mixed model with an alpha = .05, participants' resilience. The measure has 15 items, such as “I always
power = .80, and correlations among repeated measures = .5, we felt sad when I fail.” Responses are measured using a scale ranging
obtained a sample size of n = 68. Therefore, we used a sample size of from 1 (strongly disagree) to 5 (strongly agree). The Cronbach's alpha
68 as the minimum sample size for the present study. was .84 in the current research.

2.2 | Participants 2.3.3 | Manipulation checks

Participants were recruited by providing information about the study Manipulation checks included perceived stress and future
on a campus bulletin board at a University in Beijing, China. Due to perspective.
ZHENG et al. | 3

1. Perceived stress was assessed by a single item with the state- The experiment group received the following version of instruc-
ment: “how stressed do you feel right now, please indicate tions: “Please imagine your future career goals, map your career path,
the degree on a scale from 0 to 7, 0 being not stressed at and describe a concrete plan and how you deal with difficulties you
all, 10 being extremely stressed.” might face in the future. Avoid including any emotional expressions or
2. Future-­oriented thinking was measured using two items: “Please feelings. As you write, do not worry about punctuation or grammar, just
indicate the temporal locus of the things you just wrote about, really let go and write as much as you can about your experience.”
using a scale from 1 to 7, 1 = now, 7 = far future.”, “Please indicate
the vividness of the future that you just thought about, where 0 =
not at all vivid and 7 = extremely vivid.” Person correlation was .68. 2.4.2 | The trier social stress test

The present study revised the instructions of the TSST by dispensing


2.4 | Procedures with the mental arithmetic task to make it more like a job interview
(Liu et al., 2017). Participants in both groups received the following
Participant recruitment and study procedures were approved instructions: Please imagine that you have applied for a job and have
by the Institutional Review Board of the corresponding author's been invited for an interview. In contrast to a real interview, however,
University. Informed consent was obtained from all participants you are supposed to give a talk in which you are to convince the panel in
prior to their enrollment in the study. Participants could withdraw five minutes why you think that you would be the best candidate for this
from the study at any time, and they were assured their individual position. Please note that your performance will be recorded by a cam-
results would be kept confidential. First, each participant was re- era and a microphone for subsequent voice and behavioral analysis. The
quested to wear a wrist strap sensor to record his/her heart rate, members are trained in behavioral analysis and will take notes during
rinse their mouth using warm water, and remain calm. Then, par- your talk. You should try to leave the best possible impression and as-
ticipants completed the pre-­test including the PANAS, resilience sume the role of the applicant for the duration of the talk as best as you
scale, perceived stress, and providing their saliva sample. Second, can. The panel will reserve the right to ask follow-­up questions in case of
participants were requested to think about and write down either uncertainties to receive all necessary information from you. Following
ordinary events (control group) or future goals (future orientation your talk, you will be given a second task by the panel, which will only be
group) for the next 10 min. Third, participants completed an in- explained to you by the panel. You may take some notes now, which you
terim test including the PANAS, future-­oriented thinking, and a must not use during your talk. Do you have any questions?
saliva sample was collected. Fourth, participants completed the Each participant was given five minutes for preparation, and
stress test, after which they provided a saliva sample. Fifth, partici- then he or she was brought to another room where he or she gave
pants completed the post-­test and their saliva samples were col- a 10-­min talk in front of two trained researchers and a camera
lected at 10 and 20 min right after the stress test (Figure 1). After (Kirschbaum et al., 1993).
completing the study, each participant was carefully debriefed and
was compensated with ¥70.
2.5 | Cortisol measurement

2.4.1 | Manipulation materials For the purpose of monitoring the HPA axis stress response, Sarstedt
tubes (Genebase Gene-­Tech, Inc., China) were utilized to collect
The control group received the following instructions: “Please recall saliva sample. Participants were informed to abstain from eating,
the events that happened to you from Monday to Friday, choose an or- drinking, smoking, or rigorous exercise for at least one hour before
dinary day and describe how you spent the day from the time you got arrival at the laboratory. They were requested to arrive from 13:00
up, until you went to bed. Avoid including any emotional expressions or p.m. to 17:00 p.m. and to remain calm before the experiment. Saliva
feelings. As you write, do not worry about punctuation or grammar, just sample collections were performed four times for each participant.
really let go and write as much as you can about your experience.” After each experiment, saliva samples were immediately stored at

Future-
Pre-test Orientation Mid-test TSST Post-test

-35 -15 -5 0 +15 +25 +35 Time (minutes)

t1 t2 t3 t4
Salivary Cortisol Sample

FIGURE 1 The protocol of present experiment. TSST, tier social stress test
4 | ZHENG et al.

−20°C in a refrigerator prior to extraction. Before biochemical analy- 2.71, p = .046, 𝜂 2p = 0.02). Simple main effect analysis indicated a
sis, saliva samples were first centrifuged at 2,500 rpm for 15 min at significant difference between the two groups on cortisol secretion
20°C to obtain supernatant. During the centrifuge process, samples at time 3 (F[1,62] = 6.04, p = .017, 𝜂 2p = 0.09), suggesting that the
from 6 participants (2 from the Future orientation group, 4 from experiment group demonstrated significantly lower HPA axis activ-
the control group) were excluded due to insufficient supernatant. ity during the TSST task. In this study, we detected the peak cortisol
The chemiluminescence assay method was then employed to detect level about 25 min (time 3) after the onset of TSST. This delayed ef-
optical density, which was transformed into cortisol concentrations fect is because cortisol levels gradually increase caused by HPA axis
through fitting in a 4-­parameter logistic curve (R 2 = .986). activities in the blood and then transfer from blood to saliva (Childs
et al., 2006; Hellhammer et al., 2009; Helminen et al., 2019).

3 | R E S U LT S
4 | D I S CU S S I O N
3.1 | Preliminary analysis
What is the relationship between future-­oriented thinking and the
The results of the Chi-­square for independence indicated that de- stress response in the future? In the present study, people who were
mographic variables did not significantly differ between the two oriented toward the future reported less negative affect and more
groups (Gender: 𝜒 2[1] = 0.41, p = .523; Age: t[78] = 0.36, p = .718; positive affect after a stress-­producing experience. Moreover, this
Education: 𝜒 2[1] = 0.77, p = .773; Number of internship: t[78] = 1.51, study demonstrated that having a future orientation not only ad-
p = .134). There were also no significant differences in pre-­test justed the affective response but also modulated HPA axis activity
results between the two groups (Stress: t[77.68] = 0.42, p = .676; (i.e., cortisol concentration) in response to a stressful event.
Positive Affect: t[78] = 0.83, p = .412; Negative Affect: t[61.16] = 1.37, Learning how to manage or reduce the detrimental effects of a
p = .176; Resilience: t[78] = 1.13, p = .264). stressor in advance is particularly important for mental and physical
health (Folkman & Moskowitz, 2004). Our results provided empir-
ical evidence that future-­oriented thinking enhances the adaptive
3.2 | Manipulation check capacity to regulate emotions when faced with a stress-­producing
situation. Specifically, thinking in advance about the future was as-
The experiment group scored higher in future (F[1,78] = 92.89, sociated with more positive affect and less negative affect in the
p < .001, 𝜂 2p = 0.54), indicating the future temporal perspective prim- aftermath of a stressful job interview. This is consistent with a previ-
ing was successful. Stress was successfully induced for both groups ous study that emphasized the adaptive function of future orienta-
by the TSST (F[1,78] = 149.06, p < .001, 𝜂 2p = 0.66). However, neither tion in relation to stress management (Gan et al., 2017).
the between group effect (F[1,78] = 0.91, p = .343, 𝜂 2p = 0.01) nor the This study is the first study to demonstrate the adaptive function
interaction (F[1,78] = 0.09, p = .772, 𝜂 2p = 0.001) reached significance of future orientation in an acute stressful context. Prior research has
for perceived stress, indicating that both groups perceived the same emphasized the benefits of future-­oriented thinking, suggesting it
level of stress after the TSST. can be an approach for successful adjustment and health behaviors
(MacLeod et al., 2008; Schacter et al., 2017; Vilhauer et al., 2012;
Zheng et al., 2019). In contrast to prior studies, we examined the
3.3 | Self-­report measures adaptive function of future orientation not only by using multiple
indicators, such as positive affect, negative affect, and salivary cor-
To examine mood changes across pre-­, mid-­, and post-­measurement, tisol, but also using a job interview, which is a potential stressor that
a 2 × 3 mixed ANOVA with time as the within-­groups factor (pre-­, almost every person is likely to face at some time in their career or
mid-­, and post-­test) was conducted and revealed a significant inter- when seeking employment.
active effect on negative affect (F[2,156] = 6.17, p = .005, 𝜂 2p = 0.07; In addition, we demonstrated that the group primed to be future-­
Figure 2a) and positive affect (F[2,156] = 7.73, p = .001, 𝜂 2p = 0.09; oriented had less cortisol concentration after the stress test, sug-
Figure 2b). Individuals in the experiment group exhibited a stronger gesting that future-­oriented thinking can moderate their HPA axis
capacity for emotion regulation to reduce negative affect and to en- activity. In this study, we detected a difference in cortisol levels at
hance positive affect after the social stress test. 25 min after the onset of TSST. This is in line with previous studies
that determine the release of salivary cortisol does not instanta-
neously reach their peak after the onset of the stressor (Hellhammer
3.4 | Salivary cortisol concentrations et al., 2009; Helminen et al., 2019). This delayed effect is because
HPA axis triggers a cascade of physiological activities that causes the
Baseline-­
corrected salivary cortisol concentrations are shown in adrenal glands to produce cortisol, gradually increasing and reach-
Figure 3. The 2 × 4 ANOVA indicated a main effect of group (F[1,62] ing the peak roughly 20–­30 min following the exposure of stressors
= 4.69, p = .034, 𝜂 2p = 0.05), as well as an interaction (F[3,186] = (Childs et al., 2006). This study detected a significant difference in
ZHENG et al. | 5

FIGURE 2 The moods changes between two groups across tests. TSST, tier social stress test

FIGURE 3 Baseline corrected salivary cortisol changes between two groups across tests. TSST, tier social stress test

the peak cortisol level at 25th minute after the stress task, suggest- change cortisol levels. A future-­oriented individual may weigh the
ing future-­oriented thinking can enhance people's capacity to cope potential benefits of the interview, which may distract them from
with stress. the current stressor (being interviewed) and therefore have a quicker
Our results that future orientation can regulate HPA axis activity recovery than a non-­future-­oriented individual does. Thus, although
in an acute stress are in line with previous studies. Dickerson and stress was perceived, emotion regulation facilitated a quicker recov-
Kemeny (2004) proposed that cortisol responses are not only in- ery after stress exposure, including emotional recovery and cortisol
dicators of the stress response, but also suggest the association of recovery (Jentsch & Wolf, 2020).
coping processes with perceived stress. In line with that, a recent After removing the mental arithmetic part, the revised TSST was
fMRI study proposed a top-­down regulation of the psychological more like a real job interview. Consistent with previous research, the
approach in physiological responses (Mochizuki-­Kawai et al., 2020). speech part of the TSST induced both psychological and physiological
As future-­oriented anticipation leads people to focus on their future responses to stress (Bagley et al., 2011). This shed light on developing
goals, which perhaps induces distraction from the stressor. Such dis- strategies in coping with occupational stress for career training.
traction can help regulate emotion and downregulate both psycho- Our study introduces a new direction for understanding emo-
logical and physiological responses to stressors. Thus, psychological tion regulation in response to the future stress. However, there are
factors could serve as an approach to actively regulate HPA axis some limitations to the study that have implications for future re-
activity and impact the cortisol concentration (Abelson et al., 2014; search. First, it is unknown whether future-­oriented thinking could
Olivera-­Figueroa et al., 2015). buffer the detrimental effects of stress exposure or alleviate feel-
Although differences in cortisol levels were found, this study ings of stress associated with a particular event. If the prior state-
did not find differences in stress levels between the two groups. In ment is true, future orientation may regulate emotions in response
line with our results, Jentsch and Wolf (2020) found that while emo- to stress. If the latter statement is true, future-­oriented people may
tion management did not change perceived stress after TSST, it did inherently resist stress or perceive less threat and therefore have
6 | ZHENG et al.

fewer negative mood states. Future studies should seek to answer Gan, Y., Miao, M., Zheng, L., & Liu, H. (2017). Temporal Doppler effect
and future orientation: Adaptive function and moderating condi-
this question using more elaborate experimental designs.
tions. Journal of Personality, 85(3), 313–­325. https://doi.org/10.1111/
Second, we induced stress by a widely used social evaluation jopy.12242
task (i.e., the TSST). Stressors could be either physiological or psy- Gan, Y., Xie, X., Wang, T., Rodriguez, M. A., & Tang, C. S. (2013). Thriving
chological (Kogler et al., 2015). This study focused on the responses in the shadow of the 2008 Sichuan earthquake: Two studies on resil-
ience in adolescents. Journal of Health Psychology, 18(9), 1232–­1241.
to a specific psychological stressor. However, in a physiologically
https://doi.org/10.1177/13591​05312​459897
stressful context, such as a life-­threatening situation, physical in- Guo, M. Z., & Gan, Y. Q. (2010). Reliability and validity of the Chinese
jury, or physical discomfort, whether future orientation can regulate version of positive and negative affect schedule-­expanded in 660
emotions in response to such stressors remains unknown. college students. Journal of Chinese Mental Health, 24(7), 524–­528.
Third, future orientation includes several stages, such as goal set- (in Chinese).
Hellhammer, D. H., Wüst, S., & Kudielka, B. M. (2009). Salivary cortisol as
ting, planning, and evaluation (Nurmi, 1991). Although these stages
a biomarker in stress research. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 34(2), 163–­
play different roles in future orientation, it is still unknown about the 171. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psyne​uen.2008.10.026
effects of these stages on emotion regulation. Future studies should Helminen, E. C., Morton, M. L., Wang, Q., & Felver, J. C. (2019). A meta-­
test these effects, underlying the relationship between future orien- analysis of cortisol reactivity to the trier social stress test in virtual
environments. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 110, 104437. https://doi.
tation and emotion regulation.
org/10.1016/j.psyne​uen.2019.104437
Hu, Y.-­Q., & Gan, Y. (2008). Development and psychometric validity of
AC K N OW L E D G E M E N T S the resilience scale for Chinese adolescents. Acta Psychologica Sinica,
The authors would like to thank Danyang Li, Yuxiu Nie, Li Zhou, 40(8), 902–­912. https://doi.org/10.3724/sp.j.1041.2008.00902
Jentsch, V. L., & Wolf, O. T. (2020). The impact of emotion regulation on
Rui Yang, Zihua Ye, Dongdong Jiang, Qianqian Ju, Chenyang Wang,
cardiovascular, neuroendocrine and psychological stress responses.
Zhaohui Xiang, Yaoyuan Ding, and Kun Liu for data collection. Biological Psychology, 154, 107893. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biops​
ycho.2020.107893
C O N FL I C T O F I N T E R E S T S Johnson, S. R., Blum, R. W., & Cheng, T. L. (2014). Future orientation:
A construct with implications for adolescent health and wellbeing.
The authors declared no potential conflicts of interest with respect
International Journal of Adolescent Medicine and Health, 26(4), 459–­
to the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article. 468. https://doi.org/10.1515/ijamh​-­2013-­0333
Kirschbaum, C., Pirke, K.-­M., & Hellhammer, D. H. (1993). The ‘trier so-
ORCID cial stress test’–­A tool for investigating psychobiological stress re-
sponses in a laboratory setting. Neuropsychobiology, 28(1–­2), 76–­81.
Yiqun Gan https://orcid.org/0000-0001-9886-6862
https://doi.org/10.1159/00011​9004
Kogler, L., Müller, V. I., Chang, A., Eickhoff, S. B., Fox, P. T., Gur, R. C.,
REFERENCES & Derntl, B. (2015). Psychosocial versus physiological stress—­
Abelson, J. L., Erickson, T. M., Mayer, S. E., Crocker, J., Briggs, H., Lopez-­ Meta-­analyses on deactivations and activations of the neural cor-
Duran, N. L., & Liberzon, I. (2014). Brief cognitive intervention relates of stress reactions. NeuroImage, 119, 235–­251. https://doi.
can modulate neuroendocrine stress responses to the trier social org/10.1016/j.neuro​image.2015.06.059
stress test: Buffering effects of a compassionate goal orientation. Liu, J. J., Ein, N., Peck, K., Huang, V., Pruessner, J. C., & Vickers, K. (2017).
Psychoneuroendocrinology, 44, 60–­ 70. https://doi.org/10.1016/j. Sex differences in salivary cortisol reactivity to the trier social stress
psyne​uen.2014.02.016 test (TSST): A meta-­analysis. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 82, 26–­37.
Aspinwall, L. G. (2006). The psychology of future-­ oriented thinking: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psyne​uen.2017.04.007
From achievement to proactive coping, adaptation, and aging. MacLeod, A. K. (1996). Affect, emotional disorder, and future-­
Motivation and Emotion, 29(4), 203–­ 235. https://doi.org/10.1007/ directed thinking. Cognition & Emotion, 10(1), 69–­ 86. https://doi.
s1103​1-­0 06-­9013-­1 org/10.1080/02699​93963​8 0394
Bagley, S. L., Weaver, T. L., & Buchanan, T. W. (2011). Sex differences MacLeod, A. K., Coates, E., & Hetherton, J. (2008). Increasing well-­being
in physiological and affective responses to stress in remitted de- through teaching goal-­setting and planning skills: Results of a brief
pression. Physiology & Behavior, 104(2), 180–­ 186. https://doi. intervention. Journal of Happiness Studies, 9(2), 185–­196. https://doi.
org/10.1016/j.physb​eh.2011.03.004 org/10.1007/s1090​2-­0 07-­9057-­2
Campbell, J., & Ehlert, U. (2011). Acute psychosocial stress: Does MacLeod, A. K., & Conway, C. (2005). Well-­being and the anticipation
the emotional stress response correspond with physiological re- of future positive experiences: The role of income, social networks,
sponses? Psychoneuroendocrinology, 37(8), 1111–­ 1134. https://doi. and planning ability. Cognition & Emotion, 19(3), 357–­374. https://doi.
org/10.1016/j.psyne​uen.2011.12.010 org/10.1080/02699​93044​1000247
Childs, E., Vicini, L. M., & De Wit, H. (2006). Responses to the Mochizuki-­Kawai, H., Matsuda, I., & Mochizuki, S. (2020). Viewing a
trier social stress test (TSST) in single versus grouped par- flower image provides automatic recovery effects after psycholog-
ticipants. Psychophysiology, 43(4), 366–­ 371. https://doi. ical stress. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 70, 101445. https://
org/10.1111/j.1469-­8986.2006.00414.x doi.org/10.1016/j.jenvp.2020.101445
Dickerson, S. S., & Kemeny, M. E. (2004). Acute stressors and cortisol Nurmi, J.-­E. (1991). How do adolescents see their future? A review of
responses: A theoretical integration and synthesis of laboratory re- the development of future orientation and planning. Developmental
search. Psychological Bulletin, 130(3), 355–­391. https://doi.org/10.10 Review, 11(1), 1–­59. https://doi.org/10.1016/0273-­2297(91)90002​-­6
37/0033-­2909.130.3.355 Oldehinkel, A. J., Ormel, J., Bosch, N. M., Bouma, E. M. C., Van Roon,
Folkman, S., & Moskowitz, J. T. (2004). Coping: Pitfalls and promise. A. M., Rosmalen, J. G. M., & Riese, H. (2011). Stressed out?
Annual Review of Psychology, 55, 745–­774. https://doi.org/10.1146/ Associations between perceived and physiological stress responses
annur​ev.psych.55.090902.141456 in adolescents: The TRAILS study: Perceived and physiological
ZHENG et al. | 7

stress responses. Psychophysiology, 48(4), 441–­ 452. https://doi. Vilhauer, J. S., Young, S., Kealoha, C., Borrmann, J., IsHak, W. W., Rapaport,
org/10.1111/j.1469-­8986.2010.01118.x M. H., Hartoonian, N., & Mirocha, J. (2012). Treating major depres-
Olivera-­Figueroa, L. A., Juster, R.-­P., Morin-­Major, J. K., Marin, M.-­F., & sion by creating positive expectations for the future: A pilot study
Lupien, S. J. (2015). A time to be stressed? Time perspectives and for the effectiveness of future-­directed therapy (FDT) on symptom
cortisol dynamics among healthy adults. Biological Psychology, 111, severity and quality of life. CNS Neuroscience & Therapeutics, 18(2),
90–­99. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biops​ycho.2015.09.002 102–­109. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1755-­5949.2011.00235.x
Schacter, D. L., Benoit, R. G., & Szpunar, K. K. (2017). Episodic future Watson, D., Clark, L. A., & Tellegen, A. (1988). Development and valida-
thinking: Mechanisms and functions. Current Opinion in Behavioral tion of brief measures of positive and negative affect: the PANAS
Sciences, 17, 41–­50. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cobeha.2017.06.002 scales. Journal of personality and social psychology, 54(6), 1063.
Seginer, R. (2008). Future orientation in times of threat and challenge: https://doi.org/10.1037//0022-­3514.54.6.1063
How resilient adolescents construct their future. International Zheng, L., Lippke, S., Chen, Y., Li, D., & Gan, Y. (2019). Future orientation
Journal of Behavioral Development, 32(4), 272–­ 282. https://doi. buffers depression in daily and specific stress. PsyCh Journal, 8(3),
org/10.1177/01650​25408​090970 342–­352. https://doi.org/10.1002/pchj.283
Sze, Y. Y., Stein, J. S., Bickel, W. K., Paluch, R. A., & Epstein, L. H. (2017).
Bleak present, bright future: Online episodic future thinking, scar-
city, delay discounting, and food demand. Clinical Psychological
How to cite this article: Zheng, L., Chen, Y., & Gan, Y. (2021).
Science, 5(4), 683–­697. https://doi.org/10.1177/21677​02617​696511
Tucker, R. P., Wingate, L. R., O’Keefe, V. M., Mills, A. C., Rasmussen, K.,
Future orientation helps regulate emotion in the future.
Davidson, C. L., & Grant, D. M. (2013). Rumination and suicidal ide- Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 00, 1–­7. https://doi.
ation: The moderating roles of hope and optimism. Personality and org/10.1111/jasp.12811
Individual Differences, 55(5), 606–­ 611. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
paid.2013.05.013

You might also like