Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 13

Current Developmental Disorders Reports

https://doi.org/10.1007/s40474-020-00219-5

COMORBIDITIES (DM DEWEY, SECTION EDITOR)

Associations between Neurodevelopmental Disorders


and Attachment Patterns in Preschool-Aged Children:
Systematic Review
A. Potter-Dickey 1,2 & N. Letourneau 2,3,4 & A. P. J. de Koning 5

Accepted: 25 September 2020


# Springer Nature Switzerland AG 2020

Abstract
Purpose of Review Attachment is a biological evolutionary system contributing to infant survival. In neurotypical children,
secure attachment is critical to healthy brain and organ-system development. Attachment patterns in children with
neurodevelopmental disorders (NDDs) have not been extensively explored. The purpose of this review is to appraise and
summarize the literature examining attachment in children with NDDs.
Recent Findings Children with autism spectrum disorder (ASD), intellectual disability (ID) or developmental language disorder
(DLD) displayed behavior indicative of secure attachments. Approximately 42–50% of children with NDDs were deemed
securely attached compared with 62% in neurotypical children. Factors influencing attachment patterns were autistic symptoms,
developmental level, maternal sensitivity, and maternal insightfulness/resolve.
Summary Many children with NDDs can form secure attachments; however, this is based on limited data, primarily focusing on
children with ASD. Specific challenges were identified in assessing attachment patterns in children with NDDs. This demon-
strates the need for research investigating attachment in children with various NDDs and co-occurring disorders.

Keywords Neurodevelopmental disorder . Autism . Attachment . Systematic review . Parenting . Preschool-aged children

Introduction haven from stress [2]. The Diagnostic and Statistical Manual
of Mental Disorders Fifth Edition (DSM-5) [3] designates
Attachment is a relationship between infants and their care- neurodevelopmental disorders (NDDs) as dyslexia, develop-
givers that represents a brain-based biological evolutionary mental coordination disorder, developmental language disor-
system promoting infant survival [1, 2]. Securely attached der (DLD), autism spectrum disorder (ASD), and attention-
infants use their caregivers as a base for exploration and a deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) [3]. NDDs frequently
co-occur, and characteristic deficits may include delays in
This article is part of the Topical Collection on Comorbidities reaching developmental milestones of learning, executive
function, and social interaction [3]. The limited knowledge
* A. Potter-Dickey surrounding attachment in NDDs is from studies primarily
amelia.potter@ucalgary.ca focusing on ASD, with very little literature addressing other
NDDs and co-occurring NDDs. Previously, it was thought
1
ACHRI Owerko Centre, Child Development Centre, 3rd Floor, 2888 that children with ASD were unable to form secure attach-
Shaganappi Trail, Calgary, AB T3B 6A8, Canada ments given challenges with social interaction and communi-
2
Faculty of Nursing, University of Calgary, Calgary, Canada cation [3–5]. More recently, studies have revealed that chil-
3 dren with NDDs do develop secure attachments but that sig-
Department of Pediatrics, Cumming School of Medicine, University
of Calgary, Calgary, Canada nificantly fewer children with ASD tend to form secure attach-
4 ments than neurotypical children [6–8]. However, more infor-
Departments of Psychiatry and Community Health Sciences,
Cumming School of Medicine, University of Calgary, mation is needed to better understand the influencing factors
Calgary, Canada (e.g., caregiving quality, co-occurring NDDs) and attachment
5
Department of Biochemistry & Molecular Biology and Medical in children with ASD and other NDDs.
Genetics, University of Calgary, Calgary, Canada
Curr Dev Disord Rep

Attachment Theory with their caregiver, forgoing exploration [2, 20]. Types A, B,
and C are considered organized attachment patterns, as the chil-
John Bowlby initially developed attachment theory in the dren organize their behavior around reliably predictable caregiv-
1940s to explain children’s ties to their caregivers and intro- ing [2]. Disorganized attachment (type D) is observed in children
duced one of the most influential neo-Darwinian theories of who are unable to develop and organize a display of attachment
evolutionary psychology [9, 10]. The central theme of attach- behaviors [20]. Disorganized children may exhibit a combination
ment theory is that a sense of security is developed when a of fear or apprehension towards the caregiver, repetitive and
caregiver is available and responsive [11]. Infants anticipate undirected stereotypical behavior (e.g., flapping and waving of
their caregivers’ responses to their distress and shape their the arms, hand flapping, head nodding, and rocking back and
attachment behaviors accordingly [12]. Attachment behavior forth), and freezing or stilling of all movement for substantial
has social-biological underpinnings promoting the proximity periods of time [16, 20]. Broader classifications can be
of a vulnerable infant to the attachment figure, improving their subclassified into A1, A2, B1–B4, and C1, C2 through the ex-
chance of survival [9, 10]. amination of specific attachment behaviors (e.g., B3 is very se-
cure with a well-balanced display of emotion) [2, 22].
Assessing Attachment Patterns
Attachment in NDDs
Early childhood attachment can be evaluated using the
Strange Situation Procedure (SSP) or Attachment Q-Sort NDDs are conditions characterized by early-onset develop-
(AQS) [2, 13, 14]. Both measures categorize attachment be- mental deficits resulting in impaired functioning in specific
havior into corresponding attachment patterns. The Q-Sort domains [3]. Social and affective deficits in recognizing emo-
was developed for neurotypical children aged 1–5 and in- tions and challenges in orienting towards people in a social
volves sorting 90 cards describing attachment behaviors environment are a well-documented presentation in some
(e.g., proximity seeking, crying) in a home or laboratory to NDDs [3, 23, 24]. In the 1980s, it was thought that some
determine a classification [14]. The cards are sorted by either NDDs such as ASD interfered with the development of secure
the parent or a trained observer into one of nine piles based on attachment resulting in intrinsic difficulties with social inter-
the observed data, and the data can be analyzed through the action and communication [4, 5, 25, 26]. In addition, overlap-
use of individual items or summary scales [14]. ping symptoms of NDDs (e.g., inflexibility, atypical play,
The SSP is the most widely used attachment assessment in poor social interaction, poor communication, deficits of emo-
observational and experimental studies and involves a com- tional regulation) and behavior characteristic of insecure and
plex parent-infant-stranger interaction in an unfamiliar labora- disorganized attachments may lead to misclassifications [6].
tory setting [2, 15–17]. The SSP examines the balance be- Two previous systematic reviews examined associations
tween attachment and exploratory behavior in a strange between NDD and attachment; however, both only focused
situation, which subjects the child to increasing, moderate on ASD [7, 8]. Rutgers et al. [7••] conducted a meta-analysis
stress [11]. It consists of a series of separations and reunions of ten studies (total n = 287) in 2004 and reported 53% (n =
with the child’s caregiver [2, 11]. The purpose of this struc- 72) formed secure attachments (SSP; secure vs. not secure)
tured interaction is to activate the child’s attachment system to [7••]. They concluded that children with ASD displayed less
display attachment behaviors [2, 11]. Attachment behavior is attachment security than neurotypical children; however, this
coded on four scales (i.e., proximity seeking, contact mainte- effect disappeared when the sample was at a higher level of
nance, proximity avoidance, and contact resistance) [2]. The mental development and demonstrated less severe symptoms
coding scale is designed for neurotypical children between 12 of ASD. More recently, Teague et al. [8••] narratively
and 20 months of age but is commonly used up to 24 months reviewed 40 studies and updated this estimate; of the samples
[2, 11, 18]. From these scales, an Overall Security Score may (n = 7) that examined attachment, 47% of the children with
be derived as a continuous rating scale [19]. Another method ASD were categorized as secure (n = 186). They reported that
of obtaining a continuous rating is through the use of the insecure and disorganized attachment was predicted by great-
Richters Attachment Security Scale [2, 20, 21], also derived er severity of autistic symptoms and developmental delay,
from the four scales [2, 20, 21]. difficulty in reciprocal social interaction, less maternal sensi-
Typically with the SSP, children are categorized as type A, B, tivity and insightfulness, and parents’ insecure attachment to-
C, or D [2, 20]. Type B (secure) children strike a balance between wards their children.
proximity to the caregiver and exploring the strange environment In sum, given the need for an updated review that includes
[2, 20]. Type A (insecure-avoidant) pay little attention to separa- other NDDs, and more varied measurement of attachment
tions and reunions with the caregiver and explore their environ- classifications, this systematic narrative review aims to build
ment, although less robustly than type B children [2, 20]. Type C on previous work by answering the following question: What
(insecure-resistant/ambivalent) children are wholly preoccupied are the associations between NDDs and attachment patterns in
Curr Dev Disord Rep

preschool-aged children (0–5 years of age)? Furthermore, this Table 1 PyscINFO search terms February 4, 2020
review sets out to examine factors influencing attachment pat- 1. exp. Attachment Behavior/or exp. Attachment Theory/
tern formation and the challenges faced in classifying attach- 2. attachment.tw,id,tm.
ment in the NDD population [24, 25]. 3. attachment q-sort.tw,id,tm.
4. “preschool assessment of attachment”.tw,id,tm.
5. (macarthur or mac arthur).tw,id,tm.
Methods 6. strange situation.tw,id,tm.
7. 1 or 2 or 3 or 4 or 5 or 6
In this systematic narrative review, we sought to synthesize
8. autism spectrum disorders/
the empirical evidence according to pre-specified eligibility
9. (autism or autistic).tw,id.
criteria and employed explicit, systematic methods to mini-
10. asperger*.tw,id.
mize bias and uncover reliable findings [27]. We employ the
11. asd.tw,id.
recommendations put forth by Preferred Reporting Items for
12. exp. Attention Deficit Disorder with Hyperactivity/
Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses (PRISMA) [28].
13. adhd.tw,id.
14. attention deficit disorder*.tw,id.
Search Strategy
15. exp. reading disabilities/
16. read* disability.tw,id.
Searches occurred during February 2020 in MEDLINE,
17. exp. dyslexia/
EMBASE, PsycINFO, ERIC, SocINDEX, Cumulative Index
18. (dyslexia or dyslexic).tw,id.
to Nursing & Allied Health (CINAHL), and Education
Research Complete, for published studies in the English lan- 19. exp. dyspraxia/
guage with no date limits. Relevant subject heading and text 20. development* coordination disorder*.tw,id.
words were used according to each database (see Table 1). 21. exp. developmental language disorder/
Synonymous terms were combined using the Boolean “OR” 22. development* language disorder*.tw,id.
and then combined with other concepts using a Boolean 23. 8 or 9 or 10 or 11 or 12 or 13 or 14 or 15 or 16 or 17 or 18 or 19 or 20
or 21 or 22
“AND.” Truncation symbols were used appropriately to en-
24. (infant* or baby or babies or toddler* or pre-school* or pre-school* or
sure all variations of search terms were captured. child* or kindergarten*).tw,id.
25. 7 and 23 and 24
Study Inclusion and Exclusion Criteria 26. limit 25 to english

The articles retrieved were uploaded into Covidence, a web-


based tool for article screening organization [29]. Two re-
viewers screened titles and abstracts for studies that met the comparison groups, mean chronological age of participants,
inclusion criteria, including articles from peer-reviewed and results (see Table 2).
journals utilizing an observational cohort study design that
examined the relationship between NDDs and attachment
Plan for Analysis
using the SSP or AQS, coded for infants or preschoolers (0–
5 years of age). Snowball sampling was also undertaken using
A best evidence narrative systematic review was chosen to
literature reviews, previously conducted systematic reviews,
synthesize the findings to ensure that the range of research
and bibliographies. The exclusion criteria were qualitative
exploring attachment in children with NDDs was presented
studies, editorials, and non-peer-reviewed articles.
appropriately. A meta-analysis was not appropriate given the
range of research topics, research designs, sample composi-
Study Selection and Data Extraction
tion, sample sizes, and measurement scores.
A total of 1529 articles were identified through the search.
After removing duplicates (n = 222), titles and abstracts were
screened (n = 1307). Full texts were obtained on 52 articles Results
that met the inclusion criteria. A total of 12 articles met all
inclusion criteria and addressed the research question (see Description of the Literature
Fig. 1). Each article was appraised and scored by the two
reviewers according to the Newcastle-Ottawa Scale (NOS) The 12 articles reviewed included a total of 387 children and
for assessing the quality of cohort studies [41]. Data were were published between 1987 and 2018. Four studies were
extracted and summarized relating to diagnoses, sample size, from the USA [24, 30, 31, 40], four from Israel [28, 35, 37,
Curr Dev Disord Rep

Fig. 1 PRISMA procedural flow


diagram

Identification
Records identified through Additional records identified through
database searching other sources
(n = 1528) (n = 1)

Records after duplicates removed


(n = 222)

Screening
Records screened Records excluded
(n = 1307) (n = 1255)

Full-text articles assessed Full-text articles excluded,


Eligibility

for eligibility with reasons


(n = 52) (n = 40)

Resons for exclusion:

Wrong Study Design (n = 12)


Not a published study (n = 6)
Does not include
neurodevelopmental/behaviour
Included

Studies included in al disorders (n = 5)


narrative synthesis Does not Include attachment
(n = 12) measures: Attachment Q-sort,
Strange Situation Procedure
(SSP), Preschool Assessment
of Attachment (PAA),
MacArthur (n = 4)
Children are not of preschool
age (n = 5)
Does not answer research
question (n = 4)
Does not measure attachment
in children (n = 4)

38], three from the Netherlands [32, 34, 42], and one from ranged from seven to nine stars out of nine, suggesting high
Italy [39•]. All studies primarily examined ASD and attach- quality overall with a 92% inter-rater agreement (see Table 2).
ment patterns. Of the articles that reported on sex distribution
(n = 351), 273 children were males and 78 were females, and Scoring of the SSP
only one article did not report sex distribution of their sample
(n = 36) [24]. Eleven studies concurrently discussed pervasive The Shapiro et al. [24•] study used Ainsworth’s (type A, B, C)
developmental disorder (PDD) [24, 30, 32, 34–40, 42], which classification for the SSP [2, 24]. At the time of publication,
is any class of disorder characterized by severe and wide- Main and Solomon had not yet suggested disorganized (type
spread impairment in social interaction and communication D) as a fourth classification [20]. Eight studies utilized the
[43]. In the DSM-5, PDD has been subsumed under the four-fold attachment classification system for the SSP (type
ASD diagnosis [44]. Five studies examined DLD and intel- A, B, C, D) [2, 20, 31, 32, 35–40]. Rogers et al. [30•] used the
lectual disability (ID), previously referred to as mental retar- Overall Security Score [2, 19]. Similarly, two studies [34, 42]
dation (MR) [24, 30, 32, 34, 42]. The SSP was conducted with assigned each child a Richters Attachment Security Score in
mothers in nine studies, while three looked at unspecified addition to classifying them as type A, B, or C [2, 20, 21].
caregivers [32, 40, 42]. The SSP was modified in four studies
to accommodate the needs of some children and decrease Attachment Classifications in Children with NDDs
distress from the change of routine [24, 30, 31, 39]. Some
studies adapted the SSP coding scale for age and overlapping Eight studies did not utilize a neurotypical comparison group.
symptoms [30–32, 39, 40]. NOS scores for these articles Four separate publications from the Oppenheim and Koren-
Table 2 Summary of articles included for the review

Authors Country Total Sample description Comparison Method Attachment Attachment Attachment pattern results Additional findings/remarks Quality
of origin sample groups figure classification score
size (number
of stars)
Curr Dev Disord Rep

Shapiro, T., Sherman, USA n = 36 ASD (DSM 3); n = 15; Other Modified Mother Type A, B, C - 34 of the children were able to be - Quality of attachment not Reviewer
M., Calamari, G., and 30–63 months psychiatric SSP classified associated with diagnosis, 1: 8
Koch, D. [24•] Mean age and SD not diagnosis - 42% of children with a diagnosis of developmental quotients, mood Reviewer
provided (PDD, ID, DLD, ID, ASD displayed reunion change, or behavior change 2: 8
Sex not indicated DLD) behavior and were classified as
n = 21 securely attached
- Secure attachment breakdown: DLD
(12.5%), ID (33.3%), ASD (53.3%),
PDD (50%)
Rogers, S. J., Ozonoff, USA n = 32 ASD (DSM 3); n = 21; Other Modified Mother Overall Security - 50% of the children demonstrated - Developmental level rather than the Reviewer
S., and Maslin-Cole, mean age of psychiatric SSP Score (accounting secure attachment (i.e., proximity severity of ASD was the strongest 1: 8
C. [30•] 46.3 months diagnosis for overlapping seeking, contact seeking, and contact predictor of attachment security Reviewer
(SD = 11.5) (PDD, ID, symptoms) maintenance) (mental age, r = 0.49, p ≤ 0.01; 2: 8
Age range not provided DLD) - No children who were “unattached” chronological age, r = 0.48,
Males: n = 25 n = 11 (i.e., demonstrated neither positive or p ≤ 0.01; language r = 0.39,
Females: n = 7 negative behaviors towards mother, p ≤ 0.05)
compared with a stranger at reunion)
Capps, L., Sigman, M., USA n = 19 ASD (DSM 3); age No comparison Modified Mother Type A, B, C, D - Primary classification: n = 4 not able to - Attachment security is associated Reviewer
and Mundy, P. [31•] range group SSP (accounting for classify; n = 15 disorganized with greater sensitivity in mothers 1: 7
36–72 months; overlapping - Secondary classifications: 20% (n = 3) (t = 3.33, df = 13, p < 0.01) and Reviewer
mean age of 48.6 symptoms) could not be secondarily classified, more social initiatives in children 2: 8
(SD = 12.4; 40% (n = 6) securely attached (B), (t = 2.52, df = 8.36, p < 0.02)
classified children) 13% (n = 2)
Males: n = 16 insecure-ambivalent/resistant (C), 7%
Females: n = 3 (n = 1) insecure-avoidant (A), 20%
(n = 3) truly disorganized (D)
Willemsen-Swinkels, S. The n = 70 ASD (DSM 4; PDD), Normally SSP Not Specified Type A, B, C, D - No differences between clinical and - Having disorganized attachment Reviewer
H. N., Netherl- ID, and DLD n = 51; developing (accounting for non-clinical groups in proportions of does not simply reflect the 1: 9
Bakermans-Kranenb- ands 36–84 months children overlapping insecurely attached children presence of autistic behavior Reviewer
urg, M. J., Buitelaar, Mean age and SD not n = 19 symptoms) (multinomial test, p ≥ 0.05) 2: 9
J. K., van Ijzendoorn, provided - ASD and ID were found to be more
M. H., and van Males: n = 52 Females: often classified as disorganized
Engleland, H. [32•] n = 18 Fisher’s exact test, p = 0.018)
van Ijzendoorn, M. H., The n = 55 ASD (DSM 4; PDD), Normally SSP Not specified Richters Scale of - NDD had lower security score F(1, - A MANCOVA revealed a Reviewer
Rutgers, A. H., Netherl- ID, and DLD developing Attachment 53) = 4.36, p = 0.04, and more significant relationship between 1: 8
Bakermans-Kranenb- ands Mean age of children attachment disorganization, F(1, diagnosis, parental sensitivity F(1, Reviewer
urg, M. J., Swinkels, 28.4 months n = 15 53) = 8.93, p < 0.01, than 42) = 6.48, p = 0.02, children 2: 8
S. H. N., Van Daalen, (SD = 4.9) n = 40 neurotypical children without ASD parental sensitivity
E., Dietz, C., Van Age range not provided - ID scored lower on attachment, F(1, was associated with attachment
Engeland, H. [33] Males: n = 35 Females: 53) = 4.44, p = 0.04 and displayed security r(31) = 0.49 p < 0.01
n = 20 more attachment disorganization, F(1, - Higher parental sensitivity
53) = 11.56, p < 0.01 demonstrated more secure
- ASD showed more attachment children; two covariates that were
disorganization F(1, 53) = 4.46, associated with attachment
p = 0.04 disorganization are Richters
security F(1, 48) = 6.21, p = 0.02
and developmental level F(1,
48) = 6.11, p = 0.02
Naber, F. B. A., n = 80 ASD (DSM 4; PDD), Normally SSP Mother Richters Scale of - In a hierarchical regression, there Reviewer
Swinkels, S. H. N., ID, and DLD; developing Attachment was a significant result in adding 1: 8
Table 2 (continued)

Authors Country Total Sample description Comparison Method Attachment Attachment Attachment pattern results Additional findings/remarks Quality
of origin sample groups figure classification score
size (number
of stars)

Buitelaar, J. K., The n = 62; mean age children Type A, B, C, D - NDD children were less securely the number of autistic symptoms Reviewer
Bakermans-Kranenb- Netherl- 27.65 months n = 18 attached than neurotypical children, to attachment security, F(2, 2: 8
urg, M. J., van ands (SD = 6.04) χ2 (1, n = 80) = 8.18 p < 0.01 57) = 3.87, p = 0.03 explaining
Ijzendoorn, M. H., Age range not provided - No differences found in different 12% of variance
Dietz, C., van Males: n = 57 Females: diagnostic groups (ASD PDD, ID, - Developmental level significantly
Engeland, H. [34•] n = 23 DLD) predicted disorganized attachment
- Only the DLD group showed no in the children with an ASD
differences in attachment pattern diagnosis F(1, 57) = 5.56,
distribution; ASD versus control χ2 p = 0.02 explaining 9% of the
(1, n = 38) = 9.08 p < 0.01; ASD variance
(PDD) versus control χ2 (1,
n = 32) = 5.72 p = 0.02; ID versus
control χ2 (1, n = 30) = 7.75 p < 0.01
- Disorganized attachment was
overrepresented in clinical groups
versus control groups, χ2 (1,
n = 80) = 7.74 p < 0.01, ASD versus
control χ2 (1, n = 38) = 12.21
p < 0.01; ASD (PDD) versus control
χ2 (1, n = 32) = 4.26 p = 0.04; ID
versus control χ2 (1, n = 30) = 6.92
p < 0.01; DLD versus control χ2 (1,
n = 34) = 3.70 p = 0.05
Oppenheim, D., Israel n = 45 ASD (DSM 4; PDD) No comparison SSP Mother Type A, B, C, D - 42.2% (n = 19) were classified as - Examined if insightfulness or Reviewer
Koren-Karie, N., n = 45; age group securely attached (type B), 20% resolution was associated with 1: 8
Dolev, S., and range = 32–69 (n = 9) insecure-avoidant (type A), secure attachment. 83% of Reviewer
Yirmiya, N. [35•] months; mean age 15.6% (n = 7) were classified as mothers who were both resolved 2: 8
49.35 months insecure-ambivalent (type C), 22.2% and insightful were securely
(SD = 9.56) (n = 10) were classified at attached compared with 20% just
Males: n = 45 Females: disorganized (type D) insightful or resolved, or 30%
n=0 neither insightful nor resolved
Marcu, I., Oppenheim, Israel n = 45 ASD (DSM 4; PDD) No comparison SSP Mother Type A, B, C, D - 42.2% (n = 19) were classified as Reviewer
D., Koren-Karie, N., n = 45; age group securely attached (type B), 20% 1: 8
Dolev, S., and range = 32–69 (n = 9) insecure-avoidant (type A), Reviewer
Yirmiya, N. [36•] months; mean age 15.6% (n = 7) were classified 2: 8
49.35 months insecure-resistant/ambivalent (type C),
(SD = 9.56) 22.2% (n = 10) were classified at
Males: n = 45 Females: disorganized (type D)
n=0
Koren-Karie, N., Israel n = 45 ASD (DSM 4; PDD) No comparison SSP Mother Type A, B, C, D - 42.2% (n = 19) were classified as - The findings support the notion that Reviewer
Oppenheim, D., n = 45; age group securely attached (type B), 20% securely attached children had 1: 8
Dolev, S., and range = 32–69 (n = 9) insecure -avoidant (type A), mothers that displayed more Reviewer
Yirmiya, N. [37] months; mean age 15.6% (n = 7) were classified as sensitivity towards their children 2: 8
49.35 months insecure-resistant/ambivalent (type C), F(2, 39) = 3.74, p = 0.03, effect
(SD = 9.56) 22.2% (n = 10) were classified at size 0.16
Males: n = 45 Females: disorganized (type D)
n=0
Oppenheim, D., Israel n = 45 ASD (DSM 4; PDD) No comparison SSP Mother Type A, B, C, D - 42.2% (n = 19) were classified as - Predictor (insightfulness and Reviewer
Koren-Karie, N., n = 45; age group securely attached (type B), 20% resolution) and outcome 1: 8
Curr Dev Disord Rep

range = 32–69 (n = 9) insecure -avoidant (type A),


Table 2 (continued)

Authors Country Total Sample description Comparison Method Attachment Attachment Attachment pattern results Additional findings/remarks Quality
of origin sample groups figure classification score
size (number
of stars)
Curr Dev Disord Rep

Dolev, S., and months; mean age 15.6% (n = 7) were classified as (attachment) was significant, Reviewer
Yirmiya, N. [38•] 49.35 months insecure-resistant/ambivalent (type C), β = 0.4, t = 2.91, p = 0.006 2: 8
(SD = 9.56) 22.2% (n = 10) were classified at - Predictor and mediator (maternal
Males: n = 45 Females: disorganized (type D) sensitivity) was significant,
n=0 β = 0.54, t = 4.37, p < 0.001
- Mediator and outcome were
significant controlling for the
predictor, β = 0.58, t = 4.00,
p < 0.001
- Predictor and outcome became
insignificant when controlling for
the mediator, suggesting the
indirect effect of
insightfulness/resolution on
attachment was mediated through
sensitivity
Filippello, P., Marino, F., Italy n = 20 ASD (DSM 4; PDD) Normally Modified Mother Ainsworth interactive - Attachment did not differ between the - No differences in secure/insecure Reviewer
Chila, P., and n = 10; age developing SSP subscale (A1, A2, ASD and non-ASD group overall. In classification; the only difference 1: 8
Sorrenti, L. [39•] range = 35–54 children B1–B4, and C1, subgroups, only in B3 (very secure) emerged in how secure Reviewer
2
months; mean age n = 10 C2) and Overall was there a difference (χ attachment was expressed 2: 8
43.2 months Security Score 1(n = 15) = 8.07 p = 0.02) in that it did
(SD = 15,13) (accounting for not exist in ASD but was present in
Males: n = 10 Females: overlapping 67% of ND children
n=0 symptoms)
Rozga, A., Hesse, E., USA n = 30 ASD (DSM 5; PDD); No comparison SSP Mothers and 1 Type A, B, C, D - Children were classified into primary - Based on these classifications, 45% Reviewer
Main, M., n = 30; mean age group father (accounting for secure (n = 7), relationally were classified as secure (n = 13) 1: 8
Duschinsky, R., 47 months (SD = 9) overlapping disorganized (n = 5), D-autism and 55% as insecure (n = 16) Reviewer
Beckwith, L., and Age range not provided symptoms) (n = 17), and one child could not be 2: 8
Sigman, M. [40•] Males: n = 23 Females: classified because of a mixture of
n=7 avoidant, resistant/ambivalent, and
disorganized behaviors
-Of the children who were relationally
disorganized, they received secondary
classifications of secure (type B;
n = 3), insecure-resistant/ambivalent
(type C; n = 1), and insecure-avoidant
(type A; n = 1). Of the children who
received a D-autism classification, 16
received secondary classifications
(one child was dropped due to coders
being unable to differentiate between
D-autism and relational
disorganization) of secure (type B;
n = 6), insecure-resistant/ambivalent
(type C; n = 1), insecure-avoidant
(type A; n = 5), and relationally
disorganized (n = 4; within the
primary classification of D-autism)
Curr Dev Disord Rep

Karie team were identified that reported on one sample of 45 and ID were more frequently disorganized (Fisher’s exact test,
males (mean age in months = 49.35, SD = 9.56) from northern p = 0.018).
Israel, diagnosed with ASD or PDD [35–38]. Each article In contrast, van Ijzendoorn et al. [42] examined children
reported that 42.2% were securely attached (type B), 20% (n = 55) with a mean age of 28.4 months (SD = 4.9). They
insecure-avoidant (type A), 15.6% as insecure-resistant/am- looked at ASD, ID, ASD with ID, and DLD (n = 40) in rela-
bivalent (type C), and 22.2% as disorganized (type D) tion to neurotypical children (n = 15). Overall, children with
[35–38]. NDD tended to score lower on the Richters Attachment
Shapiro et al. [24] conducted an American study, which Security Scale (F(1, 53) = 4.36, p = 0.04), and displayed more
found that 42% of children with a diagnosis of DLD, ID, or attachment disorganization (F(1, 53) = 8.93, p < 0.01) than
ASD (n = 36, age range = 30–63 months) demonstrated se- neurotypical children. In looking at the breakdown of attach-
cure attachment behaviors [24]. The breakdown of secure ment for ASD, ID, ASD with ID, and DLD, only children with
attachment for each diagnosis indicated 12.5% in DLD, ASD showed more disorganization (F(1, 53) = 4.46, p =
33.3% in ID, and 53.3% in ASD. Rogers et al. [30•] studied 0.04), whereas children with ID scored lower on the security
attachment behaviors in children with ASD (n = 32), ID, and scale (F(1, 53) = 4.44, p = 0.04) and displayed more disorga-
DLD (mean age of 46.3 months, SD = 11.5) and found that nization (F(1, 53) = 11.56, p < 0.01).
50% of the sample demonstrated some evidence of secure Naber et al. [34•] assessed children (n = 80) with a mean
attachment; however, they did not provide a breakdown for age of 27.65 months (SD = 6.04) diagnosed with ASD, PDD,
each diagnosis [30•]. ID, and/or DLD, and a non-clinical control group (n = 18;
Capps et al. [31•] examined attachment security in children Richters Attachment Security Scale and type A, B, C, or D)
with ASD (n = 19) aged 36–72 months. Four children were [2, 20, 21]. Children with NDDs were less often securely
not classified due to their own or their mothers’ behaviors (i.e., attached than neurotypical children (χ2 (1, n = 80) = 8.18
frequent embraces, continuous running). All 15 classified chil- p < 0.01); however, there were no differences identified in
dren were primarily classified as disorganized (type D). the distribution of attachment patterns among diagnostic
However, in assigning a secondary classification that coded groups (ASD PDD, ID, DLD). When comparing each diag-
the child while recognizing stereotypical behaviors as an at- nostic group to the non-clinical control group, only the DLD
tribute of ASD rather than attachment disorganization, six group showed no differences in attachment distribution be-
children (40%) were re-coded as securely attached, and nine tween secure and insecure (ASD versus control χ2 (1, n =
(60%) insecurely attached. Most recently, Rozga et al. [40•] 38) = 9.08 p < 0.01; ASD (PDD) versus control χ2 (1, n =
found that with a secondary classification accounting for over- 32) = 5.72 p = 0.02; ID versus control χ2 (1, n = 30) = 7.75
lapping symptoms, 45% of the children were classified as p < 0.01).
secure (type B).
Predictors of Attachment
Comparison to Neurotypical Children
Several studies (n = 8) examined factors that predicted attach-
Four studies utilized a non-clinical, neurotypical group for ment in children with NDDs. Three studies examined if spe-
comparison. Filippello et al. [39•] found no differences be- cific NDDs impacted attachment. Shapiro et al. did not find
tween secure versus insecure classifications (type A, B, C, any association between the quality of attachment and the
D) when they explored the quality of the relationship of at- severity of NDDs [24]. Rogers et al. [30•] reported that devel-
tachments in children aged 32–54 months (mean = 43.2 ± 15, opmental level (measured through Merrill-Palmer Test of
13) with an ASD diagnosis (n = 10) in comparison with Mental Abilities [45], Leiter International Performance
neurotypical children (n = 10). The only difference found be- Scale [46], the Bayley Scales of Infant Development Mental
tween children with ASD and neurotypical children was how Scale [47], and the language subscale of Early Intervention
secure attachment was expressed in the subgroup classifica- Profile and Preschool Profile on the Early Intervention [48])
tion (χ2 1(n = 15) = 8.07 p = 0.02). None of the children with was the strongest predictor of attachment. However, in a hier-
ASD fell into the very secure subclassification (B3), whereas archical regression performed by Naber et al. [34•], more au-
67% of neurotypical children fit into this category. tistic symptoms predicted a lower score on the Richters
Willemsen-Swinkels et al. [32•] studied children (n = 70) Attachment Security Scale, explaining 12% of the variance
diagnosed with DLD (mean age = 63 ± 9 months), PDD [21]. In addition, developmental level, assessed through lan-
(mean age = 69 ± 12 months), ID (mean age = 67 ± 16), as guage, motor, and perceptual abilities on the Mullen Scales of
well as neurotypical children (mean age = 54 ± 15 months). Early Learning [49], significantly predicted disorganized at-
No differences in insecure attachment distribution were iden- tachment explaining 9% of the variance [34•].
tified between the neurotypical and NDD groups (multinomial The association between parental qualities and attachment
test, p ≥ 0.05). However, children diagnosed with both PDD was examined in several studies (n = 5). Three studies found
Curr Dev Disord Rep

that securely attached children with NDDs had caregivers that Comparison of Children with NDDs to Neurotypical
displayed higher degrees of sensitivity [31, 37, 42]. Children
Oppenheim et al. [35•] found that mothers who were more
insightful and resolved had more securely attached children. To examine the broad-scale distribution of attachment in
Maternal insightfulness is the capacity to see things from the neurotypical children using the four-fold A, B, C, and D clas-
child’s point of view; insightful mothers interpret and accu- sification, van Ijzendoorn et al. [16] conducted a meta-
rately respond to their child’s signals, which, in neurotypical analysis with data from nearly 80 studies (n = 2104). Of the
children, fosters the development of a secure attachment neurotypical, middle-class samples, approximately 15% were
[50–53]. With NDDs, insightfulness should be considered disorganized (type D) [16]. Our review found that 22.2%
with resolution as it accounts for the child’s point of view [35–38] to 100% of children [31•] with ASD were disorga-
while accepting and understanding the challenges with the nized. Furthermore, in examining secure attachment (type B),
diagnosis [35, 54]. In 2012, Oppenheim et al. [38•] deter- the distribution in neurotypical children is approximately
mined that maternal sensitivity mediated the association be- 62%. In comparison, approximately 42% in DLD, ID, or
tween combined maternal insightfulness and resolution and ASD are secure upon primary classification [24, 35–38], and
attachment; that is, mothers with higher sensitivity in the con- 60% receive a secondary classification of secure attachment
text of maternal insightfulness/resolve were more likely to [31•]. Children with ASD were most likely to be securely
have securely attached children [38•]. attached, followed by children with ID and DLD [24•]. Only
five studies [31, 35–38] examined insecure types A and C.
While van Ijzendoorn et al. [16] found that 15% of
Discussion neurotypical children were insecure-avoidant (type A), 6 to
20% of children with NDDs received the same classification
Attachment in NDDs appears to serve the same essential [31, 35–38]. Finally, 9% of neurotypical children were inse-
functions as in neurotypical children, including providing cure-resistant/ambivalent (type C) [16], whereas 13–15.6% of
them with a sense of safety and security [8]. Eight studies children with ASD [31, 35–38]. This suggests that children
identified secure attachment in approximately 42–50% of with ASD are more frequently classified as disorganized and
children with NDDs [24, 30, 31, 35–38, 40]. In studies less secure than neurotypical children. These results build on
with a neurotypical comparison group (n = 4), there were and support the previous reviews by Rutgers et al. [7] and
conflicting findings [32, 34, 39, 42]. Two studies reported Teague et al. that found that 47–53% of ASD children were
no differences in proportions of children classified as se- securely attached versus insecurely attached not taking into
curely attached when compared with neurotypical chil- account the classification of disorganized. The consensus of
dren [32, 39], while others suggested that children with replicated studies on ASD children suggests that approximate-
NDDs were less often securely attached than neurotypical ly half form secure attachments; additional research is needed
children [34, 42]. Of the studies examining other NDDs in on ID and DLD to determine if findings to date can be
addition to ASD [24, 32, 34, 42], only three conducted replicated.
statistical analyses to determine if differences existed in However, with the continuous scoring of the SSP using the
attachment pattern distribution among ASD, ID, and DLD Richters Scale of Attachment, there were conflicting findings,
[32, 34, 42]; two of the studies found that children with with two of four studies suggesting that children with NDDs
ID tend to be less secure and more disorganized [32, 42], were scored less securely than neurotypical children [34, 42].
similar to the findings of studies that have included chil- At the same time, two studies reported no differences in at-
dren with ASD [34, 42]. This review adds further support tachment security scores between neurotypical children and
for the suggestion that children with NDDs display behav- children with NDDs [32, 39]. Additional work validating
ior indicative of secure attachment, which was found in these measures for children with NDDs may address these
the previous narrative [8] and meta-analytic reviews [7]. conflicting results.
In addition, this review identified two additional studies
[39, 40] not included in the most recent 2017 systematic Predictors of Attachment in Children with NDDs and
review [8]. Both these studies also presented findings that Neurotypical Children
support the capacity of children with ASD to develop
secure attachment patterns [39, 40]. Finally, this review Factors predicting attachment in preschool children with
provides further support that there remains a minimal NDDs included the number of autistic symptoms, develop-
amount of research with regard to NDDs and attachment, mental level, maternal sensitivity, and maternal insightfulness
and identifies concerns with the varying degrees in which and resolve [30, 31, 34]. Naber et al. [34] reasoned that asso-
the SSP is conducted and scored when assessing children ciations between either developmental level, autistic symp-
with NDDs. toms, and attachment could stem from the child not
Curr Dev Disord Rep

understanding what happens during the separations and re- Clinical and Research Implications
unions of the SSP, and therefore, they may experience confu-
sion rather than stress, leading to a misclassification [34•]. Secure caregiver-child attachment appears to be equally im-
This review further supports the notion that children with portant for preschool-aged children with NDDs as it is for
greater severity of ASD symptoms and developmental delays neurotypical children. The lack of standardization with regard
are more likely to demonstrate less secure attachments. This to measurement tools and the presence of overlapping symp-
finding suggests that possessing co-occurring NDDs (e.g., ID) toms with disorganization in some NDDs make it difficult to
may place a child at a greater risk for an insecure or disorga- know whether children with NDDs are accurately coded [62].
nized attachment [7, 8, 30, 33, 55]. As this review was limited These concerns identify a need to conduct research to high-
to literature on preschool-aged children, and so few studies light behaviors that may reflect NDD-related neurological dif-
were found, future work should examine attachment and co- ferences rather than attachment behavior and develop and val-
occurring NDDs in both preschool and school-aged children idate standardized tools or modifications to existing tools to
[7, 8]. assess attachment patterns in children with NDDs reliably.
The findings regarding maternal insightfulness, resolve, When examining the impact of NDDs and co-occurring
and sensitivity in predicting secure attachment in children NDDs examining school-aged children in addition to
with NDDs are consistent with the findings in neurotypical preschool-aged children, future research is needed to develop
children and with previous reviews [7, 8]. Unfortunately, the tailored, standardized measures of attachment in children with
reviewed studies only focused on ASD, not the other NDDs. NDDs.
Insightful mothers interpret and accurately respond to their This review reveals the limited research examining factors
child’s signals, which, in neurotypical children, fosters the that may influence attachment in children with NDDs. Despite
development of secure attachment [50–53]. Fonagy et al. being regarded as an important predictor of attachment, in
[52] found that mothers with greater insight showed less neurotypical children, maternal sensitivity accounts for only
avoidant behavior and more contact maintenance (r = − 0.37 one-third of the association between maternal-infant attach-
and − 0.30, respectively). As previously mentioned in NDDs, ment, leaving a large gap of unexplained variance [63–65].
insightfulness and resolution should be considered together In a neurotypical population, factors such as genetics or ma-
[35, 54]. Like maternal insightfulness and resolve, maternal ternal mental health may influence attachment [66–68]. For
sensitivity can be variable. In the neurotypical population, example, the dopamine receptor D4 gene (DRD4) genotype
children whose caregivers are less sensitive tend to develop has been associated with increased risk for disorganization
secure attachment less frequently [56–58]. [69, 70]. Also, higher levels of maternal depressive symptoms
are consistently associated with disorganization in young chil-
Strengths and Limitations dren [71]. This review has identified that predictors of attach-
ment, such as maternal sensitivity, appear to be just as impor-
While a strength of this review includes the measures tant in children with NDDs. Therefore, it would be important
used by the studies included, a limitation is that attach- to examine the role of other predictors of attachment, such as
ment measurement modifications were not standardized or genetics or maternal mental health in children with NDDs.
validated for NDDs, potentially limiting generalizability Nonetheless, in neurotypical children, interventions designed
[16, 31]. In nine studies, attachment was studied with to promote maternal sensitivity and insightfulness are found to
mothers, while in three, it was unclear who the caregivers be effective in promoting secure attachment [57, 72, 73]. The
were or if they were primary caregivers [32, 40, 42]. As promise of interventions built on a range of factors that may
attachment patterns may differ between caregivers (e.g., influence attachment patterns for children with NDDs remains
mothers versus fathers, primary caregiver versus not) [59, to be explored.
60], findings may be limited by this lack of information.
None of the studies considered differences in attachment
behaviors between male and female children in both Conclusion
neurotypical and NDD populations. This consideration is
significant, as neurotypical females tend to seek proximity This systematic review demonstrates that many young chil-
more than males in threatening situations, leading to an dren with NDDs can develop a secure attachment with their
overestimation of security in females [61]. Finally, the caregiver. Understanding the developmental stages and be-
inclusion criteria for the measurement tools and age range havioral nuances of children with NDDs will improve the
used in this review may limit the NDDs being examined ability to apply assessments and predict attachment in this
as some diagnoses (e.g., dyslexia, ADHD) typically are population. Secure attachment is linked to lifespan health
not diagnosed until a child is of school-age (i.e., 6– and reduced morbidity [74]; thus, continued emphasis on fac-
12 years) [3]. tors predicting attachment in children with NDDs is
Curr Dev Disord Rep

warranted. Investigating attachment patterns in children with of one-year-olds. Child Dev. 1990;61(6):1965–73. https://doi.org/
10.1111/j.1467-8624.1990.tb03578.x.
NDDs could lead to understanding and improving parent-
14. Waters E, Deane KE. Defining and assessing individual differences
child relationships and wellbeing for children with NDDs. in attachment relationships: Q-methodology and the organization of
behavior in infancy and early childhood. Monogr Soc Res Child
Dev. 1985;50(1/2):41. https://doi.org/10.2307/3333826.
15. Sroufe LA. The role of infant-caregiver attachments in develop-
ment. In: Belsky J, Nezworski T, editors. Clinical implications of
References attachment. 3rd ed. Hillsdale: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates; 1988.
p. 18–38.
Papers of particular interest, published recently, have been 16.•• van Ijzendoorn MH, Schuengel C, Bakermans-Kranenburg MJ.
Disorganized attachment in early childhood: meta-analysis of pre-
highlighted as: cursors, concomitants, and sequelae. Dev Psychopathol.
• Of importance 1999;11(2):225–50. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579499002035
•• Of major importance The metanalytic systematic review examining disorganized
and organized attachment in neurotypical children.
1. Chisholm J. The evolutionary ecology of attachment organization. 17. Oppenheim D, Koren-Karie N, Dolev S, Yirmiya N. Secure attach-
Hum Nat. 1996;7(1):1–37. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF02733488. ment in children with autistic spectrum disorder: the role of mater-
2. Ainsworth MDS, Blehar MC, Waters E, Wall S. Patterns of attach- nal insightfulness. Zero to three. 2008;4:25–30.
ment: a psychological study of the strange situation. Hillsdale: 18. van Ijzendoorn MH, Kroonenberg PM. Cross-cultural patterns of
Erlbaum; 1978. attachment: a meta-analysis of the strange situation. Child Dev.
3. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and statistical man- 1988;59:147–56.
ual of mental disorders : DSM-5. 5th edition.. ed. American 19. Lamb M, Thompson R, Gardner W, Charnov E, Estes D. Security
Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental of infantile attachment as assessed in the “strange situation”: its
disorders, 5th edition. Arlington : Author; 2013. study and biological interpretation. Behav Brain Sci. 1984;7:127–
4. Cohen DJ, Paul MR, Volkmar FR. Issues in the classification of 47. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X00026522.
pervasive developmental disorders and associated conditions. In: 20. Main M, Solomon J. Procedures for identifying infants as
Cohen DJ, Donnellan AM, Paul R, editors. Handbook of autism disorganized/disoriented during the Ainsworth strange situation.
and pervasive developmental disorders. 3rd ed. New York: Wiley; In: Greenberg M, Cicchetti D, Cummings EM, editors.
1987. p. 221–43. Attachment in the pre-school years. Chicago: University of
5. Rutter M. Diagnosis and definition of childhood autism. J Autism Chicago Press; 1990. p. 121–60.
Child Schizophrenia. 1978;8(2):139–61. https://doi.org/10.1007/ 21. Richters JE, Waters E, Vaughn BE. Empirical classification of
BF01537863. infant-mother relationships from interactive behavior and crying
6. McKenzie R, Dallos R. Autism and attachment difficulties: overlap during reunion. Child Dev. 1988;59(2):512–22. https://doi.org/10.
of symptoms, implications and innovative solutions. Clin Child 2307/1130329.
Psychol Psychiatry. 2017;22(4):632–48. https://doi.org/10.1177/ 22. Waters E. Comments on strange situation classification2002.
1359104517707323. 23. Dulcan MK, Ballard RR, Jha P, Sadhu JM. Concise guide to child
7.•• Rutgers AH, Bakermans-Kranenburg MJ, van Ijzendoorn MH, and adolescent psychiatry. Washington: American Psychiatric
Berckelaer-Onnes IA. Autism and attachment: a meta-analytic re- Publishing; 2017.
view. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2004;45(6):1123–34. https://doi. 24.• Shapiro T, Sherman M, Calamari G, Koch D. Attachment in autism
org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.2004.t01-1-00305.x The only and other developmental disorders. J Am Acad Child Adolesc
metanalytic systematic review found examining attachment in Psychiatry. 1987;26(4):480–4. https://doi.org/10.1097/00004583-
children with ASD. 198707000-00003 A study that investigated how children with
8.•• Teague SJ, Gray KM, Tonge BJ, Newman LK. Attachment in NDDs form attachment patterns.
children with autism spectrum disorder: A systematic review. Res 25. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and statistical man-
Autism Spectr Disord. 2017;35:35–50. https://doi.org/10.1016/j. ual of mental disorders : DSM-3. 3rd edition. ed. American
rasd.2016.12.002 The most recent narrative systematic review Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental
found examining attachment in children with ASD. disorders, 3rd edition. Washington: Author; 1987.
9. Cassidy J. The nature of the child’s ties. In: Cassidy J, Shaver PR, 26. Volkmar FR, Cohen DJ, Paul R. An evaluation of DSM-III criteria
editors. Handbook of attachment: theory, research, and clinical ap- for infantile autism. J Am Acad Child Psychiatry. 1986;25(2):190–
plications. 3rd ed. New York: The Guilford Press; 2016. p. 3–24. 7. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0002-7138(09)60226-0.
10. Simpson JA, Belsky J. Attachment theory within a modern evolu- 27. Liberati A, Altman DG, Tetzlaff J, Mulrow C, Gøtzsche PC,
tionary framework. In: Cassidy J, Shaver PR, editors. Handbook of Ioannidis JPA, et al. The PRISMA statement for reporting system-
attachment: theory, research, and clinical applications. 3rd ed. New atic reviews and meta-analyses of studies that evaluate healthcare
York: The Guilford Press; 2016. p. 91–116. interventions: explanation and elaboration. BMJ. 2009;339:b2700.
11. Solomon J, George C. The measurement of attachment security and https://doi.org/10.1136/bmj.b2700.
related constructs in infancy and early childhood. In: Cassidy J, 28. Moher D, Liberati A, Tetzlaff J, Altman DG. Preferred reporting
Shaver PR, editors. Handbook of attachment: theory, research, items for systematic reviews and meta-analyses: the PRISMA state-
and clinical applications. 3rd ed. New Yor: The Guilford Press; ment. Ann Intern Med. 2009;151(4):264–9.
2016. p. 366–96. 29. Veritas Health Innovation. Covidence systematic review software.
12. Benoit D. Infant-parent attachment: definition, types, antecedents, Melbourne: Veritas Health Innovation; 2020.
measurement and outcome. Paediatr Child Health. 2004;9(8):541– 30.• Rogers SJ, Ozonoff S, Maslin-Cole C. Developmental aspects of
5. https://doi.org/10.1093/pch/9.8.541. attachment behavior in young children with pervasive developmen-
13. Vaughn BE, Waters E. Attachment behavior at home and in the tal disorders. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 1993;32(6):
laboratory: Q-sort observations and strange situation classifications 1274–82. https://doi.org/10.1097/00004583-199311000-00023
Curr Dev Disord Rep

Examined differences in attachment behavior in ASD and 42.• van Ijzendoorn MH, Rutgers AH, Bakermans-Kranenburg MJ,
PDD. Swinkels SHN, Van Daalen E, Dietz C, et al. Parental sensitivity
31.• Capps L, Sigman M, Mundy P. Attachment security in children and attachment in children with autism spectrum disorder: compar-
with autism. Dev Psychopathol. 1994;6(2):249–61. https://doi. ison with children with mental retardation, with language delays,
org/10.1017/S0954579400004569 A study conducted and with typical development. Child Dev. 2007;78(2):597–608.
examining attachment security in preschool-aged children with https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2007.01016.x This study
ASD. examined sensitivity and attachment in toddlers with ASD.
32.• SHN W-S, Bakermans-Kranenburg MJ, Buitelaar JK, van 43. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and statistical man-
Ijzendoorn MH, van Engleland H. Insecure and disorganized at- ual of mental disorders : DSM-4. 3d edition. ed. American
tachment in children with a pervasive developmental disorder: re- Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental
lationship with social interaction and heart rate. J Child Psychol disorders, 4th edition. Washington, DC : Author; 1994.
Psychiatry Allied Discip. 2000;41(6):759. https://doi.org/10.1111/ 44. American Psychological Association. APA dictionary of psychol-
1469-7610.00663 This study was carried out with both NDD ogy: Pervasive developmental disorder 2020. https://dictionary.apa.
and neurotypical children examined attachment and autistic org/pervasive-developmental-disorder. Accessed 20 April 2020
behaviour. 45. Stutsman R, editor. The Merrill-Palmer scale of mental tests. 3rd ed.
33. van Ijzendoorn MH, Rutgers AH, Bakermans-Kranenburg MJ, New York: Harcourt Brace Jovanovich, Inc; 1948.
Swinkels SHN, Van Daalen E, Dietz C, et al. Parental sensitivity 46. Leiter RG. Leiter international performance scale. Chicago:
and attachment in children with autism spectrum disorder: compar- Stoelting; 1979.
ison with children with mental retardation, with language delays, 47. Bayley N. Bayley scales of infant development. New York:
and with typical development. Child Dev. 2007;78(2):597–608. Psychological Corporation; 1969.
https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2007.01016.x. 48. Schafer DS, Moersch MS, editors. Developmental programming
34.• FBA N, SHN S, Buitelaar JK, Bakermans-Kranenburg MJ, van for infants and young children. Ann Arbor: University of
Ijzendoorn MH, Dietz C, et al. Attachment in toddlers with autism Michigan Press; 1981.
and other developmental disorders. J Autism Dev Disord. 49. Mullen EM. Mullen scales of early learning. Ciricle Pines:
2007;37(6):1123–38. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-006-0255-2 American Guidance Services, Inc.; 1995.
Attachment was assessed in toddlers with NDDs and a 50. Ainsworth MDS, Wittig BA. Attachment and the exploratory be-
neutotypical comparison group. haviour of 1-year-olds in a strange situation. Derterminants Infant
35.• Oppenheim D, Koren-Karie N, Dolev S, Yirmiya N. Maternal Behav. 1969;4:113–36.
insightfulness and resolution of the diagnosis are associated with 51. Fonagy P, Steele H, Steele M. Maternal representations of attach-
secure attachment in preschoolers with autism spectrum disorders. ment during pregnancy predict the organization of infant-mother
Child Dev. 2009;80(2):519–27. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467- attachment at one year of age. Child Dev. 1991;62(5):891–905.
8624.2009.01276.x A study that examines the associations https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.1991.tb01578.x.
between maternal insightfulness and/or resolution and attach- 52. Fonagy P, Steele M, Steele H, Moran GS, Higgitt AC. The capacity
ment in ASD. for understanding mental states: the reflective self in parent and
36.• Marcu I, Oppenheim D, Koren-Karie N, Dolev S, Yirmiya N. child and its significance for security of attachment. Infant Ment
Attachment and symbolic play in preschoolers with autism spec- Health J. 1991;12(3):201–18. https://doi.org/10.1002/1097-
trum disorders. J Autism Dev Disord. 2009;39(9):1321–8. https:// 0355(199123)12:3<201::AID-IMHJ2280120307>3.0.CO;2-7.
doi.org/10.1007/s10803-009-0747-y This study examines the 53. Slade A. Parental reflective functioning: an introduction. Attach
associations between attachment and symbolic play in Hum Dev. 2005;7(3):269–81. https://doi.org/10.1080/
preschool-age boys with ASD. 14616730500245906.
37. Koren-Karie N, Oppenheim D, Dolev S, Yirmiya N. Mothers of 54. Marvin RS, Pianta RC. Mothers’ reactions to their child’s diagno-
securely attached children with autism spectrum disorder are more sis: relations with security attachment. J Clin Child Psychol.
sensitive than mothers of insecurely attached children. J Child 1996;25(4):436–45.
Psychol Psychiatry. 2009;50(5):643–50. https://doi.org/10.1111/j. 55. Naber FBA, Swinkels SHN, Buitelaar JK, Bakermans-Kranenburg
1469-7610.2008.02043.x. MJ, van Ijzendoorn MH, Dietz C, et al. Attachment in toddlers with
38.• Oppenheim D, Koren-Karie N, Dolev S, Yirmiya N. Maternal sen- autism and other developmental disorders. J Autism Dev Disord.
sitivity mediates the link between maternal insightfulness/ 2007;37(6):1123–38. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-006-0255-2.
resolution and child-mother attachment: the case of children with 56. Feeney B, Woodhouse SS. Caregiving. In: Cassidy J, Shaver PR,
autism spectrum disorder. Attach Hum Dev. 2012;14(6):567–84. editors. Handbook of attachment: theory, research, and clinical ap-
https://doi.org/10.1080/14616734.2012.727256 This study plications. 3rd ed. New York: The Guilford Press; 2016. p. 827–51.
examined maternal sensitivity and insightfulness/resolution 57. Bakermans-Kranenburg MJ, van Ijzendoorn MH, Juffer F. Less is
and attachment in preschool-age boys with ASD. more: meta-analyses of sensitivity and attachment interventions in
39.• Filippello P, Marino F, Chila P, Sorrenti L. Attachment and social early childhood. Psychol Bull. 2003;129(2):195–215.
behavior in children’s autistic disorders. Life Span and Disability. 58.• Koren-Karie N, Oppenheim D, Dolev S, Sher E, Etzion-Carasso A.
2015;18(1):101–18 This study explores the quality of the rela- Mothers’ insightfulness regarding their infants’ internal experience:
tionship of attachments in children with ASD and the differ- relations with maternal sensitivity and infant attachment. Dev
ences in attachment classification. Psychol. 2002;38(4):534–42. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.
40.• Rozga A, Hesse E, Main M, Duschinsky R, Beckwith L, Sigman 38.4.534 This study examines the links between maternal
M. A short-term longitudinal study of correlates and sequelae of sensitivity and children’s secure attachment in preschool-age
attachment security in autism. Attach Hum Dev. 2018;20(2):160– boys with ASD.
80. https://doi.org/10.1080/14616734.2017.1383489 A study that 59. Bretherton I. Fathers in attachment theory and research: a review.
examined attachment, developmental assessment, and Early Child Dev Care. 2010;180(1–2):9–23. https://doi.org/10.
observational assessments of empathy in children with ASD. 1080/03004430903414661.
41. Wells GA, Shea B, O’Connell D, Peterson J, Welch V, Losos M 60. Paquette D. Theorizing the father-child relationship: mechanisms
et al. The Newcastle-Ottawa Scale (NOS) for assessing the quality and developmental outcomes. Hum Dev. 2004;47:193–219. https://
of nonrandomised studies in meta-analysesn.d. doi.org/10.1159/000078723.
Curr Dev Disord Rep

61. David DH, Lyons-Ruth K. Differential attachment responses of meta-analysis.(Research Article). PLoS One. 2018;13(10):
male and female infants to frightening maternal behavior: tend or e0204374. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0204374.
befriend versus fight or flight? Infant Ment Health J. 2005;26(1):1– 69. Lakatos K, Toth I, Nemoda Z, Ney K, Sasvari-Szekely M, Gervai J.
18. https://doi.org/10.1002/imhj.20033. Dopamine D4 receptor (DRD4) gene polymorphism is associated
62. Pipp-Siegel S, Siegel CH, Dean J. Neurological aspects of the with attachment disorganization in infants. Mol Psychiatry.
disorganized/disoriented attachment classification system: differen- 2000;5(6):633–7. https://doi.org/10.1038/sj.mp.4000773.
tiating quality of the attachment relationship from neurological im- 70. Bakermans-Kranenburg MJ, van Ijzendoorn MH. Attachment, par-
pairment. Monogr Soc Res Child Dev. 1999;64(3):25–44. enting, and genetics. In: Cassidy J, Shaver PR, editors. Handbook
63. Belsky J. Differential susceptibility to rearing influence: an evolu- of attachment: theory, research, and clinical applications. 3rd ed.
tionary hypothesis and some evidence. In: Bjorklund BED, editor. New York: The Guilford Press; 2016. p. 155–79.
Origins of the social mind: evolutionary psychology and child de- 71. Badovinac S, Martin JW, Guerin-Marion C, O'Neill M, Pillai
velopment. New York: Guildford Press; 2005. p. 139–63. Riddell R, Bureau J, et al. Associations between mother-
64. van Ijzendoorn MH. Adult attachment representations, parental re- preschooler attachment and maternal depression symptoms: a sys-
sponsiveness, and infant attachment: a meta-analysis on the predic- tematic review and meta-analysis. PLoS One. 2018;13(10):
tive validity of the adult attachment interview. Psychol Bull. e0204374. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0204374.
1995;117(3):387–403. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.117.3. 72. Letourneau N, Tryphonopoulos P, Giesbrecht GF, Dennis C,
387. Bhogal S, Watson B. Narrative and meta-analytic review of inter-
65. Verhage ML, Schuengel C, Madigan S, Fearon RMP, Oosterman ventions aiming to improve maternal-child attachment security.
M, Cassibba R, et al. Narrowing the transmission gap: a synthesis of Infant Ment Health J 2015;36(4):366–387. https://doi.org/10.
three decades of research on intergenerational transmission of at- 1002/imhj.21525.
tachment. Psychol Bull. 2016;142(4):337–66. https://doi.org/10.
73. Letourneau N, Anis L, Steele H, Steele M, Hart M. Attachment &
1037/bul0000038.
Child Health (ATTACH) pilot trials: effect of a parental reflective
66. Bakermans-Kranenburg MJ, van Ijzendoorn MH. Research review:
function intervention for families affected by toxic stress. Infant
genetic vulnerability or differential susceptibility in child develop-
Ment Health J in press.
ment: the case of attachment. J Child Psychol Psychiatry.
2007;48(12):1160–73. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.2007. 74. Puig J, Englund M, Simpson J, Collins W. Predicting adult physical
01801.x. illness from infant attachment: a prospective longitudinal study.
67. Gervai J. Environmental and genetic influences on early attach- Health Psychol. 2013;32(4):409–17. https://doi.org/10.1037/
ment. Child Adolesc Psychiatry Ment Health. 2009;3(1):25. a0028889.
https://doi.org/10.1186/1753-2000-3-25.
68. Badovinac S, Martin J, Guerin-Marion C, O’Neill M, Pillai Riddell Publisher’s Note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdic-
R, Bureau J, et al. Associations between mother-preschooler attach- tional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
ment and maternal depression symptoms: a systematic review and

You might also like