Behaviorally Induced Insufficient Sleep

You might also like

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 11

Behaviorally Induced

I n s u ff i c i e n t S l e e p
Christer Hublin, MD, PhDa,b,*, Mikael Sallinen, PhDa,c

KEYWORDS
 Sleep  Insufficient sleep  Neurobehavioral function  Work  Behavior

KEY POINTS
 Behaviorally induced insufficient sleep (BIIS) is a common and often long-lasting condition but it is
ill-defined.
 BIIS can profoundly affect individual’s neurobehavioral and physiological functions and safety.
 There are no established methods for management except the recommendation to sleep more.

The inability to obtain sufficient sleep is a common been 7 to 8 hours among adults, and there is
condition, and its causes are various, ranging from a U-shaped relationship between age and average
sleep disorders and other medical conditions, sleep time, with the minimum being in middle-
irregular and/or extended working hours, to social aged individuals. In both extremes of the continuum,
activities and domestic responsibilities. When the there are people with sleep disorders, such as
condition lasts for a long period, it leads to chronic insomnia among short sleepers and hypersomnia
sleep deprivation, increasing sleepiness and im- among long sleepers, but also healthy individuals,
pairing cognitive function to a degree comparable because a few percentage of population are
to acute total sleep deprivation. For various so-called natural short sleepers or natural long
reasons, people with behaviorally induced insuffi- sleepers.2 In addition, there seem to be differences
cient sleep (BIIS), who have no difficulties with between countries. For example, a study by Steptoe
their sleep as such, voluntarily sleep less than their and colleagues3 that was based on data from 24
natural sleep requirement. When BIIS is long countries showed that sleep duration was shortest
lasting (3 months), it may meet the diagnostic (6.0-6.5 hours) among Japanese and Taiwanese
criteria of BIIS syndrome (BIISS), defined by the students and longest (about 8 hours) among
International Classification of Sleep Disorders Bulgarian, Greek, Romanian, and Spanish students.
(ICSD-2).1 This review is focused on BIIS in adults, It has been claimed that the sleep length has
with a special emphasis on work-related aspects. generally shortened during the last decades, but
there are so far few population-based studies to
WHEN IS SLEEP SUFFICIENT AND WHEN IS IT support this view. In the most recent UK survey,
INSUFFICIENT the mean sleep duration was 7.04 hours,4 and it
has changed little over the last 50 years.5 In a re-
Sleep duration is highly individual in all age groups, analysis of self-reported sleep duration (>440,000
and it is clearly dependent on age. In population- persons) in population-based surveys in Fin-
based studies, the mean sleep length has usually land from 1972 to 2005, only a minor decrease

Funding: None.
Conflicts of interest: The authors have no conflicts of interest.
a
sleep.theclinics.com

Centre of Expertise for Human Factors at Work, Finnish Institute of Occupational Health, Topeliuksenkatu 41
a A, FIN-00250 Helsinki, Finland; b Department of Clinical Neurosciences, Helsinki University, PB 22, FIN-00014
Helsinki, Finland; c Agora Center, University of Jyväskylä, PB 35, FIN-40014 Jyväskylä, Finland
* Corresponding author. Centre of Expertise for Human Factors at Work, Finnish Institute of Occupational
Health, Topeliuksenkatu 41 a A, FIN-00250 Helsinki, Finland.
E-mail address: christer.hublin@ttl.fi

Sleep Med Clin 7 (2012) 313–323


doi:10.1016/j.jsmc.2012.03.008
1556-407X/12/$ – see front matter Ó 2012 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.
314 Hublin & Sallinen

(18.3 minutes or about 4% to 7.3 hours) was One additional problem is that there are no
observed.6 In this study, sleep was shortest in exact and generally accepted definitions of and
middle-aged employees. However, there may be methods to measure either sleep need or sleep
substantial differences in the development of sleep sufficiency. Broman and collegues12 suggested
duration over decades between countries. In the using the amount of habitual sleep to the amount
United States, the self-reported modal sleep dura- of estimated need for sleep (sleep sufficiency
tion in the 1960s was about 8 hours,7 but, in more index [SSI]) ratio. To delineate subjects with
recent Gallup surveys, the estimates have been 7 a substantial chronic sleep loss, a condition
hours or even less (National Sleep Foundation termed persistent insufficient sleep (PIS) was
2005). In a large population-based survey in the operationally defined as SSI less than 80% and
United States, 28.3% of adults slept 6 hours or having an experience of getting too little sleep at
less per day.8 This amount of sleep per 24 hours least 3 times per week during the last 3 months.
may considerably decrease alertness, as a reduction This cutoff at 80% is arbitrary but corresponds
of total sleep time for one night by 1.0 to 1.5 hours in rather well to about 6.5 hours of sleep in the
normal young adults shortens their mean sleep average individual whose self-estimated daily
latencies on the Multiple Sleep Latency Test sleep need is about 8 hours.12 Despite the obvious
(MSLT) by up to one-third.9 need to operationalize the condition of insufficient
In addition to the mean length of sleep, it is sleep, the authors are unaware of any later publi-
essential to take into consideration its day-to-day cation using this definition or its modifications.
variation, for example, over a workweek, when as- Generally speaking, there are 2 major views on
sessing BIIS and its consequences. American the phenomenon of insufficient sleep and its
Time Use Surveys revealed that the average sleep consequences and significance. The first argues
time was longer on Sundays (9.59 hours) and that there are no data to show any such decrease
Saturdays (8.97 hours) than on the weekdays, in in sleep length during the last decades that would
which case it gradually decreased from Monday result in large-scale and severe increases in sleep
(8.41 hours) to Friday (8.03 hours).10 Similarly, in deprivation or excessive daytime sleepiness.5 In
an actigraphic study, the daily standard deviation addition, it has been pointed out that human adults
was more than an hour for both sleep duration probably are capable of biologically adapting to
and time in bed, meaning that the day-to-day vari- various sleep durations if the range of variation
ation within individuals was greater than the varia- remains between 6 and 9 hours per day.5,14 The
tion between their mean sleep durations.11 Thus, other major view underlines the gradual decrease
although a mean sleep length for a longer period in sleep length and the increase of sleep depriva-
can be usual (around the norm of 7–8 hours of tion in adult population and also cognitive deficits
sleep per day), a substantial part of it can be spent caused by these trends. In experimental studies,
in a condition of insufficient sleep that compro- cognitive deficits have been shown to exacerbate
mises alertness and cognitive function (see over days of sleep restriction, even though sleep-
section “Consequences of Sleep Deprivation”). deprived individuals may not be aware of the
In population-based studies, the assessment of cumulative deficits.15,16
sleep length is practically always based on self-
reports because use of objective methods such BIISS
as polysomnography has so far been too laborious
and expensive for this purpose. It is, however, ICSD-21 includes a description and diagnostic
probable that individuals differ in terms of how criteria of BIISS (Box 1). BIISS occurs when an indi-
accurately they are able to assess their sleep vidual persistently fails to obtain the amount of
length. Based on clinical experience, those with sleep required to maintain normal levels of alertness
fragmented sleep (eg, individuals with insomnia) and wakefulness. The ability to initiate and maintain
are more inclined to more often report unexpect- sleep is unimpaired or above average, with little or
edly short sleep length than those with consoli- no psychopathology and no medical explanation
dated sleep. In true insufficient sleep, as in BIIS, for the patient’s sleepiness. There is a substantial
sleep should be consolidated, and therefore self- disparity between the need for sleep and the
reports probably are more reliable. Even though amount actually obtained. A markedly extended
the occurrence of insufficient sleep seems to be sleep time on weekend nights or during holidays
strongly dependent on sleep length, it is also impor- compared with weekday nights is suggestive of
tant to take into account individual sleep need this disorder. In addition to sleepiness, patients
when assessing BIIS.12,13 Thus, it is important to may develop irritability, concentration and attention
take into account both self-reported sleep length deficits, reduced motivation, dysphoria, fatigue,
and sleep need, when assessing sleep sufficiency. incoordination, and restlessness.1
Insufficient Sleep 315

Box 1 experienced too little sleep, but only about one-


BIISS third of them had concomitant symptoms suggest-
ing insomnia.18 In a telephone survey done in
A. The patient has a complaint of excessive Australia, insufficient sleep was reported by
sleepiness or, in prepubertal children, 28%, and about three-quarters of these respon-
a complaint of behavioral abnormalities dents related it to external factors and the remain-
suggesting sleepiness. The abnormal sleep ing quarter to internal factors.19
pattern is present almost daily for at least 3
In another Swedish questionnaire study based on
months.
a population sample aged 20 to 64 years, PIS
B. The patient’s habitual sleep episode, estab- (defined earlier) was found in 12% of the subjects.12
lished using history, a sleep log, or actigra- One-half of the subjects with PIS also reported
phy, is usually shorter than expected from
concomitant sleeping difficulties, and, in the remain-
age-adjusted normative data. (Note: in the
case of individuals with long sleep time, ing, the most conspicuous causes were work-
habitual sleep periods may be normal, based related factors and simply too little time for sleep.
on age-adjusted normative data. However, Thus, behavioral causes of insufficient sleep seemed
these sleep periods may be insufficient for to be as common as symptoms indicating possible
this population.) sleep disorder. The following consequences of
C. When habitual sleep schedule is not main- insufficient sleep were reported by the respondents:
tained (weekends or vacation time), patients cognitive/behavioral fatigue, somatic symptoms,
sleep considerably longer than usual. sleepiness, swelling, headache, and dysphoric
D. If diagnostic polysomnography is performed mood. Insufficient sleep decreased with age.12
(not required for diagnosis), sleep latency is A Finnish population-based study, in which insuf-
less than 10 minutes and sleep efficiency ficient sleep was determined as a difference of 1
greater than 90%. During the MSLT, a short hour or more between the self-reports of the need
mean sleep latency of less than 8 minutes of sleep and the actual length of sleep, found that
(with or without multiple sleep-onset rapid the prevalence among 33- to 60-year-old subjects
eye movement period) may be observed. was 20.4% (16.2% in men and 23.9% in women).13
E. The hypersomnia is not better explained by In the same base population, the corresponding
another sleep disorder, medical or neuro- figure was 3.6% among men and 6.7% among
logic disorder, mental disorder, or medica- women after excluding regular nappers and insom-
tion use or substance use disorder. niacs.20 The occurrence of insufficient sleep was
Data from ICSD-2. International classification of sleep strongly dependent on sleep length.13 Insufficient
disorders, 2nd edition. Diagnostic and coding manual. sleep was reported by 37% of men and 54% of
American Academy of Sleep Medicine; 2005. women who slept for 6 hours or less but only by
3.5% of men and 4.3% of women who slept for 9
hours or more. Similarly, 49% of men and women
reporting need of sleep for 9 hours or more had
The differential diagnosis of BIISS includes other insufficient sleep in contrast with 3.3% of men
causes of excessive daytime sleepiness or short- and 5.2% of women with need of sleep of 7 hours
ening of nocturnal sleep.1 The diagnosis may be or less. There was a significant age effect, a self-
especially difficult to make in subjects who are report of insufficient sleep being more common in
natural long sleepers (see later). There are descrip- younger age groups in both genders. The strongest
tions of cases in which BIISS has incorrectly been significantly positively associated factors were
diagnosed as narcolepsy without cataplexy with daytime sleepiness (odds ratio, about 4), insomnia
typical findings on sleep registrations in the (odds ratio, 2.5–3.0), not able to sleep without
sleep-deprived condition.17 disturbance (odds ratio, 2.0–2.5), and evening
type (odds ratio, about 2). Among men, weekly
EPIDEMIOLOGY OF INSUFFICIENT SLEEP working hours of 75 or more were also strongly
associated with insufficient sleep (odds ratio, about
Sleeping difficulties are common, about one-third 3), and not working was a protective factor against
of the general population is affected by transient it in both genders (odds ratio, about 0.7). Insufficient
insomnia and about one-tenth by severe and/or sleep was measured twice with a 9-year interval in
chronic insomnia,2 and these difficulties can be the authors’ sample and showed considerable
assumed to be a major cause of insufficient sleep. stability: 44% of those with insufficient sleep in
However, other causes also seem to be prevalent. 1981 also had it 9 years later. Thus, insufficient
In a Swedish population sample aged 30 to sleep seems to be a long-standing condition in
65 years, 28% of women and 21% of men a large part of the population.13
316 Hublin & Sallinen

In another Finnish population study, the preva- Insufficient sleep is common in the general popu-
lence of similarly defined insufficient sleep was lation, but there is considerable variation in the re-
even higher (36%), being about one-third more ported frequencies, ranging from 12% to 36%. This
common in women than in men (31% vs 41%).21 is explained at least partly by differences in definition
There was also a significant age effect: insufficient of insufficiency and other methodological aspects.
sleep was twice as prevalent in the youngest Insufficient sleep seems to decrease with age.
compared with the oldest individuals (age range, Some studies indicate that one-quarter to one-half
24–65 years). The association with work status of the subjects with insufficient sleep have no simul-
was also similar to the results of the earlier- taneous symptoms suggesting insomnia or other
mentioned Finnish study. Also in the Finnish sleep disorder. This indicates that behavioral factors
studies, behavioral causes (including work- are common causes of insufficient sleep.
related factors) of insufficient sleep were found to
be prevalent. CONSEQUENCES OF SLEEP DEPRIVATION
In a large-scale telephone survey of noninstitu-
tionalized US population aged 18 years or more, Many earlier reports suggested that human adults
frequent (14 days in the past 30 days) insufficient are highly adaptable to chronic sleep restriction
sleep was reported by 26%.22 It was significantly down to 4 to 5 hours per day, but most of these
more common in women, adults younger than 55 studies were performed outside laboratory
years, persons employed or unable to work or settings, with little or no control over potentially
students (vs retired), and those with fair/poor confusing factors such as napping, actual length
general health. Insufficient sleep was significantly of sleep periods, use of stimulants (caffeine and
more likely if frequent physical or mental distress, nicotine), and physical activity.16 The later well-
activity limitations, anxiety, pain, or depressive controlled studies conducted under standardized
symptoms were also present. Additional examples laboratory conditions with continuous behavioral,
in which insufficient sleep are more significantly physiologic, and medical monitoring have shown
common include smoking, physical inactivity, many significant disadvantageous changes due
obesity, and heavy alcohol use in men.22 to cumulative sleep restriction over several con-
There are few epidemiologic studies on BIISS. secutive days. The results from these studies are
ICSD-21 states that this condition affects both discussed in the next section.
sexes across the life span. It may be more frequent
in adolescence when sleep need is high, but social Sleep Architecture and Physiologic Sleepiness
pressure and tendency to delayed sleep phase
Although timing and duration of sleep and the
often lead to chronic restricted sleep. In a Japanese
number of days with restricted sleep affect sleep
study including more than 1200 patients referred to
architecture, the general feature is the conserva-
an outpatient clinic for complaint of excessive
tion of slow wave sleep and the reduction of the
daytime sleepiness, the rate of BIISS was 7.1%,
other non–rapid eye movement sleep stages and
with BIISS being the fourth most common cause
rapid eye movement sleep.25–27 There is also an
after obstructive sleep apnea, idiopathic hyper-
increase in slow wave activity during and after
somnia, and narcolepsy.23 In this patient series
a period of sleep restriction.16,25,27
with polysomnographically verified diagnosis, the
The MSLT is a well-documented objective
male to female ratio was 7:3, the median age of
measure of sleepiness, showing a significant
symptom onset was 28.6 years, and the average
negative correlation between total sleep time at
sleep length during weeknights was 5.5 and 7.9
night and sleep latency on the following day.28 In
hours on weekends. The mean Epworth Sleepiness
a large population-based study including more
Scale score before treatment was 13.6.23
than 600 subjects, a significant association was
In a Norwegian questionnaire study, the estimated
found between self-reported sleep duration and
prevalence of BIISS was 10.4% among a represen-
the risk of falling asleep on the MSLT; compared
tative sample of nearly 1300 high-school students
with those reporting more than 7.50 hours of sleep
aged 16 to 19 years.24 A diagnosis of BIISS was
a day, individuals reporting 6.75 to 7.50 hours and
given if the following 3 criteria were met: (1) exces-
less than 6.75 hours of sleep a day had a 27% and
sive daytime sleepiness was present, (2) total sleep
73% increase in risk for falling asleep on the test,
time on weekdays was less than 7 hours, and (3)
respectively.29
sleep duration was at least 2 hours longer on week-
ends/vacations than on weekdays. Use of alcohol
Neurobehavioral Effects
and living in an urban area were positively related
to BIISS, and it was also associated with poor Studies on the neurobehavioral effects of cumula-
grades and symptoms of anxiety and depression.24 tive sleep restriction have shown performance
Insufficient Sleep 317

impairments on various mental tasks. These include, loss and cumulative sleep restriction), whether
for example, measures of sustained attention (eg, the neurobehavioral effects were better explained
vigilance and reaction time tasks), information pro- by the cumulative loss of sleep time or by cumula-
cessing speed (eg, serial addition/subtraction tasks), tive wake extension. They concluded that the latter
working memory (eg, digit symbol substitution task), was the primary cause of progressively impaired
and dynamic allocation of attention between multiple vigilance performance. This observation of the
tasks (multitasks consisting of multiple simulta- importance of excess wakefulness is useful when
neously active subtasks).26,27,30–33 Until now, the trying to compare different conditions resulting in
study by Balkin and colleagues34 is the only one insufficient sleep in the real world.
that has focused on the question which of the neuro- Also, it is important to know which factors are
behavioral tasks are the most sensitive to cumulative the strongest modifiers of the neurobehavioral
sleep restriction. In this study, participants were daily consequences of cumulative sleep restriction.
presented with 11 mental tasks while being sub- The current body of research evidences that these
jected to 3, 5, 7, or 9 hours time in bed per night for factors include (1) amount of sleep per night,26,27
a week. The Psychomotor Vigilance Task proved to (2) excess wakefulness,27 (3) the number of sleep
be the most sensitive to sleep restriction, whereas restriction days,26,27,30,31,33 (4) time of day,35 and
a logical reasoning task showed least sensitivity. (5) the amount of sleep before the beginning of
The differences in sensitivity to the sleep restriction a sleep restriction regimen.31 The importance of
were quite large between the mental tasks, sug- the last factor was demonstrated by Rupp and
gesting that the type of the task is of considerable colleagues31 who subjected healthy volunteers to
importance when assessing the neurobehavioral either habitual sleep length (about 7 hours that
consequences of cumulative sleep restriction. may not have been totally sufficient for all partici-
Besides the cognitive demands of a task, task pants) or extended sleep (10 hours time in bed)
duration seems to be an important factor. This for a week before the sleep restriction regimen
was particularly demonstrated in the study by (3 hours time in bed for 7 days). In practice, the
Haavisto and colleagues33 that examined multi- sleep extension group obtained approximately 2
tasking performance in the condition of cumulative hours more sleep than the habitual sleep group.
sleep restriction (4 hours time in bed) over a period During the days of sleep restriction, the sleep
of 5 days. The investigators found that the deteri- extension group showed less severe impairments
oration of performance within 50-minute task in their vigilance performance and better ability
sessions (so-called time-on-task effect) became to maintain wakefulness than the habitual sleep
intensified during the course of the experiment in group, suggesting a protective effect from the
the group of sleep-restricted individuals but not prior additional sleep. This observation may
in the group of controls permitted to sleep 8 hours encourage people to sleep longer than habitually
each night. This finding emphasizes the impor- before a sleep restriction period if it is possible to
tance of task duration when assessing risks anticipate the occurrence of the period.
caused by insufficient sleep. Recovery from cumulative sleep restriction has
Another question of interest is how severely been under intense examination since the study
neurobehavioral functions are compromised by by Belenky and colleagues26 in 2003, which was
cumulative sleep restriction compared with the the first study that properly investigated this issue.
effects of total sleep loss. Until now, only the study In light of the results of total sleep deprivation
by Van Dongen and colleagues27 has explicitly studies, the investigators somewhat surprisingly
investigated this issue. The investigators found found that the decrements in vigilance perfor-
that performance on all 3 tasks that measured vigi- mance observed during the course of sleep
lance, working memory, and information process- restriction (3 or 5 hours time in bed for 7 days)
ing speed deteriorated under 2 weeks of did not completely recover after 3 days of normal
cumulative sleep restriction (4 or 6 hours time in sleep (8 hours time in bed). The investigators sug-
bed per night) similarly to what was observed gested that the brain adapts to cumulative sleep
following 1 to 2 days of total sleep loss. Another restriction, which makes sleep-restricted individ-
interesting finding was that self-reported sleepi- uals inclined to make performance errors, not
ness did not show such a pattern. It reached its only during a period of sleep restriction but also
maximum after already 2 days with the 4- or 6- during several ensuing days.
hour sleep opportunity per night. This level of A later study by Banks and colleagues32 was the
self-reported sleepiness was equivalent to the first to systematically examine the dose-response
level that was observed following one night effect of recovery sleep following a sleep restriction
without any sleep. The investigators analyzed, regimen. In this experiment, 142 healthy adults were
using the data from both conditions (total sleep first subjected to 5 days of sleep restriction (4 hours
318 Hublin & Sallinen

time in bed per night), and, following this, they in this field deal with traffic, and up to 20% of all
were allocated to 1 of 6 recovery sleep groups (0, traffic accidents in industrial societies are sleep
2, 4, 6, 8, or 10 hours time in bed for 1 night). The and vigilance related.38 Insufficient sleep among
study revealed that recovery of vigilance perfor- drivers is common. For example, half of 2196
mance and subjective sleepiness followed by the randomly stopped French drivers had decreased
sleep restriction was strongly dependent on the their total sleep time in the 24 hours before the
amount of sleep on the recovery night; the greater interview compared with their regular self-
the amount of recovery sleep, the better the level reported sleep time.39 About 12.5% had a sleep
of recovery. In addition, the 2-hour increase in the debt greater than 180 minutes, and 2.7% had
recovery sleep opportunity was more valuable a sleep debt greater than 300 minutes. Being
among the short recovery sleep groups (ie, 0, 2, young, commuting to work, driving long distances,
and 4 hours) than among the longer groups (ie, 6, starting the trip at night, being an “evening”
8, and 10 hours). However, not even the longest person, being a long sleeper during the week,
recovery sleep yielded a full recovery. On the other and sleeping in on the weekend were risk factors
hand, performance on 2 other tasks (a modified significantly associated with sleep debt.39 In
version of the Maintenance of Wakefulness Test a sample of American truck drivers actigraphically
and on the Digit Symbol Substitution Task) recov- measured, mean sleep of 1 week was less than 6
ered in a linear manner and showed a full recovery hours in 34%, and short sleep was significantly
after either the 8-hour or the 10-hour recovery associated with decreased performance.40
sleep opportunity. These findings suggest that the The data on sleep-related accidents in other
recovery process of neurobehavioral functions fields (eg, industry and health care) are scarce
following cumulative sleep restriction takes more compared with traffic. One reason may be that
time than the corresponding process following total the overall accident risk in these sectors is lower
sleep loss, although the level of performance deteri- than in the transport sector, where, for example,
oration caused by these 2 types of sleep loss would a single perceptual error may be fatal.37 In a popu-
be equal.32 lation-based study by Akerstedt and colleagues,41
The earlier-mentioned studies show that when it was found that disturbed sleep and shift work
individuals with 7 to 9 hours of daily sleep restrict were associated with a 50% increased risk of fatal
their daily sleep dose to 4 to 6 hours per night for occupational accidents.
at least 5 days, many of the individuals’ neurobeha-
vioral functions become impaired to the extent that
it is comparable to 1 to 2 days of total sleep loss. In
Physiologic Effects
addition, the studies show that the recovery
process takes several days. Although these main There is increasing evidence of associations
findings are of importance when evaluating neuro- between sleep length and different health out-
behavioral consequences of BIIS (and also BIISS), comes in population. However, the mediating
they do not provide a practical basis for the clinical mechanisms are still incompletely understood,
evaluation of these conditions. In addition, there although studies have found detrimental effects
are at least 2 main theoretical questions that are of sleep loss on endocrine, metabolic, immune,
still open: are the neurobehavioral consequences and inflammatory functions.42,43 For example, the
of cumulative sleep restriction found among mostly link between insufficient sleep and insulin resis-
young healthy men in the studies described earlier tance can be explained by several plausible
similar to that in other pertinent groups of people mechanisms: (1) increased release of counter-
(eg, in the elderly and in women) and what happens regulatory hormones, including epinephrine and
when individuals are subjected to periods of cortisol; (2) increased sympathetic nervous activity
cumulative sleep restriction and following recovery with elevated levels of norepinephrine; (3) inflam-
opportunities repeatedly. A study by Everson and mation, with elevations of interleukin 1b, tumor
Szabo36 showed that rats exposed to recurrent necrosis factor a, interleukin 6, and C-reactive
periods of cumulative sleep restriction developed protein; (4) increased risk of weight gain and
marked behavioral changes in food and water obesity, a major risk factor for insulin resistance.44
intake, leading to weight loss and structural Failure to obtain adequate amounts of sleep
changes in the small intestine and adipocytes. promotes low-level systemic inflammation, and,
although the physiologic mechanisms underly-
ing the links between sleep deprivation and
Risk of Accidents
these immune and inflammatory responses re-
One important outcome of insufficient sleep is main largely unknown, neuroendocrine-depen-
increased risk of accidents.37 Most of the studies dent, autonomic vascular stress–dependent, and
Insufficient Sleep 319

slow wave sleep hormone–dependent changes ICSD-21 includes under the heading “Isolated
are likely involved.45 Symptoms, Apparently Normal Variants and Unre-
Most of the population studies assessing health solved Issues” the entity “Long Sleeper” (also
risk associated with sleep length do not include called healthy hypersomnia or extreme high end
detailed information of factors affecting the length, of normal sleep duration continuum). The diag-
that is, whether self-reported short sleep is a result nostic criteria include daily total sleep time of 10
of insufficient sleep (either behavioral or other hours or more (documented by a sleep log over
cause), of insomnia, or of being a natural short a minimum of 7 days), excessive daytime sleepi-
sleeper. Although the occurrence of insufficient ness following less than 10 hours of sleep, and
sleep cannot directly be assessed from sleep having had that particular sleep pattern since
length solely, it seems to be clearly more common childhood. The prevalence of long sleep is poorly
in short sleepers.13 There is a significant U-shaped known because the numerous studies on sleep
association between sleep length and subsequent length and different health outcomes have not
mortality, showing that those sleeping 7 hours had separated it from other causes of long sleep (about
the lowest risk of mortality.46 In addition, it has 2% of the population sleeps at least 10 hours per
been shown that there is a significant association night) such as untreated sleep apnea.1 However,
between short sleep and obesity especially in it can be assumed that natural long sleepers are
younger people47 and somatic diseases or disor- especially at risk for BIIS (and also BIISS) as exem-
ders such as type 2 diabetes48 and cardiovascular plified by the case described earlier.
disease.49 It is well known that individuals differ consider-
ably in their response to insufficient sleep caused
by shift work or extended working hours. Age,
FACTORS AFFECTING RISK OF AND sex, diurnal type, physical fitness, and domestic
ADAPTATION TO INSUFFICIENT SLEEP and personality factors explain only a minor part
of the variation.53 A study by Van Dongen and
Insufficient sleep is significantly associated with colleagues54 suggested that these individual
diurnal type (also called chronotype), which in differences constitute a trait. In their study, 21
turn is strongly genetically determined; about healthy young adults were monitored for wakeful-
one-half of the interindividual variability in adults ness during 3 separate laboratory visits, each of
is explained by genetic effects.50 Insufficient which included 36 hours of total sleep deprivation
sleep, defined as a difference of at least 1 hour and neurobehavioral testing at 2-hour intervals.
between the self-reports of the need of sleep and During the week prior to the sessions, the partici-
the actual length of sleep, was reported by 12% pants’ daily sleep opportunity was either restricted
among morning types (29% in this population- to 6 hours (prior sleep restriction condition) or
based adult sample), and it had an increasing extended to 12 hours (prior sleep extension condi-
trend toward the evening types (10% of the tion). The main findings of the study showed that
sample), 28% of whom reported it.50 Some of (1) there are substantial individual differences in
those who evaluate themselves as extreme sleepiness and performance impairment due to
evening-type individuals actually suffer from de- sleep deprivation, (2) these individual differences
layed sleep phase syndrome, which is character- are highly replicable over repeated exposures to
ized by habitual sleep-wake times that are sleep deprivation, and (3) the individual differences
delayed usually several hours relative to conven- in response to sleep deprivation were not pre-
tional times.1 This condition is more common dicted by prior sleep history, circadian rhythm
among adolescents and young adults. parameters, age, or sex or psychosocial factors.
Other studies have shown that evening type From these findings, it was concluded that vulner-
is clearly associated with poor sleep hygiene. ability to performance impairment due to sleep
Compared with morning types, evening types deprivation constitutes a trait.54,55 Later studies
more often show a considerable sleep debt on have suggested that genes involved in the adeno-
work days, consume more caffeine and alcohol, sinergic and circadian regulation (PER3 polymor-
and are more often habitual smokers.51,52 The phism) of sleep are possible candidate predictors
concept of “social jet lag” has been proposed of individuals’ resistance or vulnerability to perfor-
because so many people in the present society shift mance impairments under sleep deprivation,56,57
their sleep and activity times several hours between even though negative findings have also been
the workweek and the weekend in a way compa- reported.58
rable with genuine jetlag.52 Thus, many behavioral The abruptness of sleep restriction plays a role
aspects may contribute to insufficient sleep, espe- in the adaptation process. Drake and coworkers59
cially in evening-type people. assessed alertness, memory, and performance
320 Hublin & Sallinen

following 3 schedules of approximately 8 hours of Given the origin of BIIS, it is possible to equate it
sleep loss (slow, intermediate, and rapid accumu- with other unhealthy behaviors, such as physical
lation) in comparison with an 8-hour time in bed inactivity, smoking, or heavy alcohol consumption.
sleep schedule. Twelve young healthy adults There are a few influential theories that have been
completed each of 4 conditions: no sleep loss applied to unhealthy behaviors until now. An
(8-hours for 4 nights) and slow (6 hours for 4 example of these is the Transtheoretical Model of
nights), intermediate (4 hours for 2 nights), and Behavior Change (TTM).62,63 The basic idea under-
rapid (0 hours in bed for 1 night) sleep loss. The lying this model is that individuals differ significantly
rapid sleep loss produced significantly more in terms of their readiness to change their unhealthy
severe impairments on tests of alertness, memory, behavior. Individuals can be categorized into one of
and performance than the slow accumulation of the following stages: (1) precontemplation stage in
a comparable amount of sleep loss. The impairing which there is no intention to change the behavior,
effects of sleep loss vary as a function of rate, sug- probably because of unawareness of the need to
gesting the presence of a compensatory adaptive change; (2) contemplation stage in which there is
mechanism operating in conjunction with the awareness of the problem but no readiness to do
accumulation of a sleep debt.59 anything concrete, probably because of not being
sure if the pros outweigh the cons; (3) preparation
MANAGEMENT stage in which there are plans to take action in the
near future but also worries of failure; (4) action
Despite the relatively high prevalence of BIIS and stage in which individuals do something concrete
its possible health and neurobehavioral conse- but worry about the need to work hard to maintain
quences, there are no established ways of treating the new behavior; or (5) maintenance stage in which
this condition, except the recommendation of ex- individuals try to prevent relapse into the old
tending sleeping time to better satisfy the individ- unhealthy behavior, particularly in stressful situa-
ual’s sleep need. To the best of the authors’ tions. It is apparent that treatment of these different
knowledge, there are no studies assessing the groups requires different approaches. For example
effect of treatment of BIIS or BIISS. The need for in BIIS or BIISS, those at the precontemplation
effective and innovative treatments is emphasized stage would need an awakening to the unhealthi-
by results indicating that insufficient sleep may be ness of their sleep-wake behavior, whereas those
quite long lasting (stable in 44% over a period of 9 at the preparation stage would need support to
years, see earlier),13 and therefore it may also be take action to increase their sleep length and/or
quite resistant to general recommendations to to have more regular sleeping habits.
extend sleep time. The long-lasting nature of BIIS Treatments applying TTM have been shown to
is also suggested by an early study by Friedmann be effective in reducing stress,64 improving medi-
and colleagues.60 They described 4 young adult cation adherence,65,66 and facilitating smoking
collegiate couples who gradually restricted their cessation67 and weight loss.68 There is some
sleep down to about 5 hours per night over a period evidence of the usefulness of the TTM in identifying
of 6 to 8 months (measured using sleep log). At the patients with obstructive sleep apnea syndrome
end of an additional 12-month follow-up, total who have low or high adherence to CPAP therapy
sleep time was still 1.0 to 2.5 hours below base- and low or high intention to exercise.69–71 On the
line,60 indicating a long-lasting change in sleep- other hand, there has also been criticism regarding
wake behavior. the effectiveness of the TTM in facilitating changes
Because BIIS is behaviorally induced by defini- in health behavior.72–74 Whether TTM-based
tion, it is obvious that pharmacologic treatment approaches could be effective in treating BIISS
such as sleep-promoting medication is not among remains to be investigated.
the first-line choices. The behavioral component is Among adults, long working hours and shift
strongly present in many other sleep disturbances work are often associated with insufficient
also, such as in insomnia and sleep apnea. Rumi- sleep.13,75,76 In such cases, attention should be
native thoughts at bedtime making sleep initiation paid not only to sleep but also to the duration
difficult and compliance with use of the continuous and arrangement of working hours, to ensure
positive airway pressure (CPAP) device are exam- about 8 hours time in bed per day.
ples of the behavioral component involved in sleep
disturbances. Cognitive behavioral treatments are SUMMARY
most effective in management of different forms of
insomnia,61 and it is probable that similar tech- BIIS, both as a phenomenon and as a syndrome, is
niques could also be useful in the management a common condition. It is well documented that
of BIIS and BIISS. the detrimental effects of chronic sleep deprivation
Insufficient Sleep 321

on well-being, performance, and safety are com- recorded sleep measures: the CARDIA study. Sleep
parable to those caused by acute total sleep depri- 2007;30:793–6.
vation lasting 1 to 2 days. As insufficient sleep is 12. Broman JE, Lundh LG, Hetta J. Insufficient sleep in the
common among short sleepers, it is possible that general population. Neurophysiol Clin 1996;26:30–9.
chronic sleep deprivation plays a significant role 13. Hublin C, Kaprio J, Partinen M, et al. Insufficient
in the associations shown between short sleep sleep: a population based study in adults. Sleep
and many common diseases. There are observa- 2001;24:392–400.
tions indicating that insufficient sleep may be 14. Horne J. Is there a sleep debt? Sleep 2004;27:1047–9.
long lasting and can therefore also be resistant 15. Dinges DF. Sleep debt and scientific evidence.
to treatments based on recommendations only. Sleep 2004;27:1050–2.
Thus, it is important to investigate cost-effective 16. Banks S, Dinges DF. Chronic sleep deprivation. In:
preventive and treatment methods for it. Assess- Kryger M, Roth T, Dement WC, editors. Principles
ment of sleep sufficiency should be one focus in and practice of sleep medicine. 5th edition. St Louis
health care system, especially when dealing with (MO): Elsevier Saunders; 2011. p. 67–75.
adolescents, young adults, and employees (partic- 17. Janjua T, Samp T, Cramer-Bornemann M, et al. Clin-
ularly having shift work, extended working hours, ical caveat: prior sleep deprivation can affect the
and safety critical jobs). MSLT for days. Sleep Med 2003;4:69–72.
18. Liljenberg B, Almqvist M, Hetta J, et al. The preva-
lence of insomnia: the importance of operationally
REFERENCES defined criteria. Ann Clin Res 1988;20:393–8.
19. Lack L, Miller W, Turner D. A survey on sleeping diffi-
1. ICSD-2. International classification of sleep disor- culties in an Australian population. Community
ders. Diagnostic and coding manual. 2nd edition. Health Stud 1988;12:200–7.
Westchester (IL): American Academy of Sleep Medi- 20. Hublin C, Kaprio J, Partinen M, et al. Daytime sleep-
cine; 2005. iness in an adult Finnish population. J Intern Med
2. Partinen M, Hublin C. Epidemiology of sleep disor- 1996;239:417–23.
ders. In: Kryger M, Roth T, Dement WC, editors. Prin- 21. Sallinen M, Härmä M, Kalimo R, et al. The preva-
ciples and practice of sleep medicine. 5th edition. St lence of sleep debt and its association with fatigue,
Louis (MO): Elsevier Saunders; 2011. p. 694–715. performance and accidents in the modern society.
3. Steptoe A, Peacey V, Wardle J. Sleep duration and In: Rantanen J, Lehtinen S, Saarela KL, editors.
health in young adults. Arch Intern Med 2006;166: Proceedings of the European Conference on Safety
1689–92. in the Modern Society, 15–17 September 1999. Hel-
4. Groeger JA, Zijlstra FR, Dijk DJ. Sleep quantity, sinki (Finland): Finnish Institute of Occupational
sleep difficulties and their perceived consequences Health; 2000. p. 140–3.
in a representative sample of some 2000 British 22. Strine TW, Chapman DP. Associations of frequent
adults. J Sleep Res 2004;13:359–71. sleep insufficiency with health-related quality of life
5. Horne J. The end of sleep: ‘sleep debt’ versus bio- and health behaviors. Sleep Med 2005;6:23–7.
logical adaptation of human sleep to waking needs. 23. Komada Y, Inoue Y, Hayashida K, et al. Clinical
Biol Psychol 2011;7:1–14. significance and correlates of behaviorally induced
6. Kronholm E, Partonen T, Laatikainen T, et al. Trends insufficient sleep syndrome. Sleep Med 2008;9:
in self-reported sleep duration and insomnia-related 851–6.
symptoms in Finland from 1972 to 2005: a compara- 24. Pallesen S, Saxvig IW, Molde H, et al. Brief report:
tive review and re-analysis of Finnish population behaviorally induced insufficient sleep syndrome in
samples. J Sleep Res 2008;17:54–62. older adolescents: prevalence and correlates.
7. Kripke DF, Simons RN, Garfinkel L, et al. Short and J Adolesc 2011;34:391–5.
long sleep and sleeping pills. Is increased mortality 25. Brunner DP, Dijk DJ, Borbély AA. Repeated partial
associated? Arch Gen Psychiatry 1979;36:103–16. sleep deprivation progressively changes in EEG
8. Krueger PM, Friedman EM. Sleep duration in the during sleep and wakefulness. Sleep 1993;16:100–13.
United States: a cross-sectional population-based 26. Belenky G, Wesensten NJ, Thorne DR, et al. Patterns
study. Am J Epidemiol 2009;169:1052–63. of performance degradation and restoration during
9. Bonnet MH, Arand DL. We are chronically sleep sleep restriction and subsequent recovery: a sleep
deprived. Sleep 1995;18:908–11. dose-response study. J Sleep Res 2003;12:1–12.
10. Basner M, Fomberstein KM, Razavi FM, et al. Amer- 27. Van Dongen HP, Maislin G, Mullington JM, et al. The
ican time use survey: sleep time and its relationship cumulative cost of additional wakefulness: dose-
to waking activities. Sleep 2007;30:1085–95. response effects on neurobehavioral functions and
11. Knutson KL, Rathouz PJ, Yan LL, et al. Intra-indi- sleep physiology from chronic sleep restriction and
vidual daily and yearly variability in actigraphically total sleep deprivation. Sleep 2003;26:117–26.
322 Hublin & Sallinen

28. Arand D, Bonnet M, Hurwitz T, et al. The clinical use 44. Van Cauter E. Sleep disturbances and insulin resis-
of the MSLT and MWT. Sleep 2005;28:123–44. tance. Diabet Med 2011;28:1455–62.
29. Punjabi NM, Bandeen-Roche K, Young T. Predictors 45. Faraut B, Boudjeltia KZ, Vanhamme L, et al.
of objective sleep tendency in the general popula- Immune, inflammatory and cardiovascular conse-
tion. Sleep 2003;26:678–83. quences of sleep restriction and recovery. Sleep
30. Axelsson J, Kecklund G, Akerstedt T, et al. Sleepiness Med Rev 2012;16(2):137–49.
and performance in response to repeated sleep 46. Gallicchio L, Kalesan B. Sleep duration and
restriction and subsequent recovery during semi- mortality: a systematic review and meta-analysis.
laboratory conditions. Chronobiol Int 2008;25:297–308. J Sleep Res 2009;18:148–58.
31. Rupp TL, Wesensten NJ, Bliese BD, et al. Banking 47. Nielsen LS, Danielsen KV, Sørensen TI. Short sleep
sleep: realization of benefits during subsequent sleep duration as a possible cause of obesity: critical anal-
restriction and recovery. Sleep 2009;32:311–21. ysis of the epidemiological evidence. Obes Rev
32. Banks S, Van Dongen HP, Maislin G, et al. Neurobe- 2011;12:78–92.
havioral dynamics following chronic sleep restriction: 48. Cappuccio FP, D’Elia L, Strazzullo P, et al. Quantity
dose-response effects of one night for recovery. and quality of sleep and incidence of type 2 dia-
Sleep 2010;33:1013–26. betes: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Dia-
33. Haavisto ML, Porkka-Heiskanen T, Hublin C, et al. betes Care 2010;33:414–20.
Sleep restriction for the duration of a work week 49. Kronholm E, Laatikainen T, Peltonen M, et al. Self-re-
impairs multitasking performance. J Sleep Res ported sleep duration, all-cause mortality, cardio-
2010;19:444–54. vascular mortality and morbidity in Finland. Sleep
34. Balkin TJ, Bliese PD, Belenky G, et al. Comparative Med 2011;12:215–21.
utility of instruments for monitoring sleepiness- 50. Koskenvuo M, Hublin C, Partinen M, et al. Heritability
related performance decrements in the operational of diurnal types: a nationwide study of 9212 adult
environment. J Sleep Res 2004;13:219–27. twin pairs. J Sleep Res 2007;16:156–62.
35. Mollicone DJ, Van Dongen HP, Rogers NL, et al. 51. Taillard J, Philip P, Bioulac B. Morningness/eveningness
Time of day effects on neurobehavioral performance and the need for sleep. J Sleep Res 1999;8:291–5.
during chronic sleep restriction. Aviat Space Environ 52. Wittmann M, Dinich J, Merrow M, et al. Social jetlag:
Med 2010;81:735–44. misalignment of biological and social time. Chrono-
36. Everson CA, Szabo A. Recurrent restriction of sleep biol Int 2006;23:497–509.
and inadequate recuperation induce both adaptive 53. Härmä M. Sleepiness and shiftwork: individual differ-
changes and pathological outcomes. Am J Physiol ences. J Sleep Res 1995;4(Suppl 2):57–61.
Regul Integr Comp Physiol 2009;297:R1430–40. 54. Van Dongen HP, Baynard MD, Maislin G, et al.
37. Akerstedt T, Philip P, Capelli A, et al. Sleep loss and Systematic interindividual differences in neurobe-
accidents—work hours, life style, and sleep havioral impairment from sleep loss: evidence of
pathology. Prog Brain Res 2011;190:169–88. trait-like differential vulnerability. Sleep 2004;27:
38. Philip P, Sagaspe P, Taillard J. Drowsy driving. In: 423–33.
Kryger M, Roth T, Dement WC, editors. Principles 55. Van Dongen HP, Belenky G. Individual differences in
and practice of sleep medicine. 5th edition. St Louis vulnerability to sleep loss in the work environment.
(MO): Elsevier Saunders; 2011. p. 769–74. Ind Health 2009;47:518–26.
39. Philip P, Taillard J, Guilleminault C, et al. Long 56. Viola AU, Archer SN, James LM, et al. PER3 poly-
distance driving and self-induced sleep deprivation morphism predicts sleep structure and waking
among automobile drivers. Sleep 1999;22:475–80. performance. Curr Biol 2007;17:613–8.
40. Pack AI, Maislin G, Staley B, et al. Impaired perfor- 57. Landolt HP. Sleep homeostasis: a role for adenosine
mance in commercial drivers: role of sleep apnea in humans? Biochem Pharmacol 2008;75:2070–9.
and short sleep duration. Am J Respir Crit Care 58. Goel N, Banks S, Mignot E, et al. PER3 polymor-
Med 2006;174:446–54. phism predicts cumulative sleep homeostatic but
41. Akerstedt T, Fredlund P, Gillberg M, et al. A pro- not neurobehavioral changes to chronic partial
spective study of fatal occupational accidents— sleep deprivation. PLoS One 2009;4:e5874.
relationship to sleeping difficulties and occupational 59. Drake CL, Roehrs TA, Burduvali E, et al. Effects of
factors. J Sleep Res 2002;11:69–71. rapid versus slow accumulation of eight hours of
42. Grandner MA, Drummond SP. Who are the long sleep loss. Psychophysiology 2001;38:979–87.
sleepers? Towards an understanding of the mortality 60. Freidmann J, Globus G, Huntley A, et al. Perfor-
relationship. Sleep Med Rev 2007;11:341–60. mance and mood during and after gradual sleep
43. Grandner MA, Hale L, Moore M, et al. Mortality asso- reduction. Psychophysiology 1977;14:245–50.
ciated with short sleep duration: the evidence, the 61. Morin CM, Bootzin RR, Buysse DJ, et al. Psychological
possible mechanisms, and the future. Sleep Med and behavioral treatment of insomnia: update of the
Rev 2010;14:191–203. recent evidence (1998-2004). Sleep 2006;29:1398–414.
Insufficient Sleep 323

62. Prochaska JO, DiClemente CC. Stages and 70. Aloia MS, Arnedt JT, Stepnowsky C, et al. Predicting
processes of self-change of smoking: toward an treatment adherence in obstructive sleep apnea
integrative model of change. J Consult Clin Psychol using principles of behavior change. J Clin Sleep
1983;51:390–5. Med 2005;1:346–53.
63. Prochaska JO. Decision making in the transtheoret- 71. Smith SS, Doyle G, Pascoe T, et al. Intention to exer-
ical model of behavior change. Med Decis Making cise in patients with obstructive sleep apnea. J Clin
2008;28:845–9. Sleep Med 2007;3:689–94.
64. Evers KE, Prochaska JO, Johnson JL, et al. 72. Riemsma RP, Pattenden J, Bridle C, et al. Systematic
A randomized clinical trial of a population- and review of the effectiveness of stage based interven-
transtheoretical model-based stress-management tions to promote smoking cessation. BMJ 2003;326:
intervention. Health Psychol 2006;25:521–9. 1175–7.
65. Johnson SS, Driskell MM, Johnson JL, et al. Trans- 73. Bridle C, Riemsma RP, Pattenden J, et al. Systematic
theoretical model intervention for adherence to review of the effectiveness of health behavior inter-
lipid-lowering drugs. Dis Manag 2006;9:102–14. ventions based on the transtheoretical model. Psy-
66. Johnson SS, Driskell MM, Johnson JL, et al. Efficacy chol Health 2005;20:283–301.
of a transtheoretical model-based expert system for 74. Aveyard P, Lawrence T, Cheng KK, et al.
antihypertensive adherence. Dis Manag 2006;9: A randomized controlled trial of smoking cessation
291–301. for pregnant women to test the effect of a transtheor-
67. Velicer WF, Redding CA, Sun X, et al. Demographic etical model-based intervention on movement in
variables, smoking variables, and outcome across stage and interaction with baseline stage. Br J
five studies. Health Psychol 2007;26:278–87. Health Psychol 2006;11(Pt 2):263–78.
68. Johnson SS, Paiva AL, Cummins CO, et al. Trans- 75. Virtanen M, Ferrie JE, Vahtera J, et al. Long working
theoretical model-based multiple behavior interven- hours and sleep disturbances: the Whitehall II
tion for weight management: effectiveness on prospective cohort study. Sleep 2009;32:737–45.
a population basis. Prev Med 2008;46:238–46. 76. Sallinen M, Kecklund G. Shift work, sleep, and
69. Stepnowsky CJ, Marler MR, Palau J, et al. Social- sleepiness—differences between shift schedules
cognitive correlates of CPAP adherence in experi- and systems. Scand J Work Environ Health 2010;
enced users. Sleep Med 2006;7:350–6. 36:121–33.

You might also like