Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 27

Psychological Review

© 2022 American Psychological Association 2023, Vol. 130, No. 5, 1262–1288


ISSN: 0033-295X https://doi.org/10.1037/rev0000406

Metacognition and Self-Control: An Integrative Framework


Marie Hennecke1 and Sebastian Bürgler2
1
Department of Psychology, University of Siegen
2
Department of Psychology, University of Zurich

Self-control describes the processes by which individuals control their habits, desires, and impulses in the
service of long-term goals. Research has identified important components of self-control and proposed
theoretical frameworks integrating these components (e.g., Inzlicht et al., 2021; Kotabe & Hofmann, 2015).
In our perspective, these frameworks, however, do not yet fully incorporate important metacognitive aspects
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

of self-control. We therefore introduce a framework explicating the role of metacognition for self-control.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

This framework extends existing frameworks, primarily from the domains of self-regulated learning and
problem-solving (e.g., Schraw & Moshman, 1995; Zimmerman, 2000), and integrates past and contempo-
rary research and theorizing on self-control that involves aspects of metacognition. It considers two groups
of metacognitive components, namely, (a) individual metacognitive characteristics, that is a person’s
declarative, procedural, and conditional metacognitive knowledge about self-control, as well as their self-
awareness (or metacognitive awareness), and (b) metacognitive regulatory processes that unfold before a
self-control conflict (forethought and prevention), when a self-control conflict is identified, during a self-
control conflict (regulation and monitoring), and after a self-control conflict (reflection and evaluation). The
proposed framework integrates existing research and will be useful for highlighting new directions for
research on the role of metacognition for self-control success and failure.

Keywords: metacognition, self-control, self-regulation, individual differences, processes

It has been Julia’s habit for years to treat herself to a delicious donut her strategy works well). These processes are metacognitive pro-
on her walk home from work. Years of daily donuts, however, have cesses, given that they are cognitive processes referring to other
consequences and one day Julia’s doctor advises that she should start cognitive processes or phenomena, here: self-control conflicts. Until
watching her weight. She decides that she will cut the daily pastry but now, no comprehensive theoretical framework exists that organizes
anticipating having to walk by the donut bakery with its delicious and integrates these and other metacognitive influences on self-control
smell every day makes it clear to her that she might struggle to break in a coherent manner. We would like to provide such an integrative
her habit. She knows that her self-control has strengths but that framework and take a shot at more comprehensively connecting the
resisting donuts is not one of them. To make it easier for her, she plans two intertwined but so far rarely explicitly connected research fields
to take a slightly different route home from now on, one that does not of self-control and metacognition. Before we explain further the
pass the bakery to help herself avoid the daily temptation. After a contribution of our framework, let us introduce its two central
couple of days, she realizes that avoiding the temptation this way constructs, self-control and metacognition in more detail.
works well and decides to keep using this strategy in the future.
As this example demonstrates, self-control is not only about Self-Control
resisting temptation in the heat of the moment. People can, like Julia,
use self-knowledge about their own self-control (Julia knows that Self-control has been defined as the “ability to override impulses
resisting donuts is not her strength), anticipate the future (she antici- to act as well as the ability to make oneself initiate or persist
pates that it will be difficult for her to resist), make plans (she plans to in boring, difficult, or disliked activity” (Carver, 2019, p. 477). It
take a slightly different route home from now on), choose self-control is furthermore best conceptualized with two defining features
strategies (she actually does not walk past the bakery anymore), (Duckworth et al., 2016): First, it is required when two mutually
and evaluate their success with these strategies (she realizes that exclusive options cause an intrapsychic conflict, the self-control
conflict: One option serves an impulse, desire, habit, or immediate
gratification (e.g., eating a donut, smoking, or reading text mes-
sages), whereas the other option serves a more enduring and
This article was published Online First December 15, 2022. important goal (e.g., losing weight, staying healthy, or not failing
Marie Hennecke https://orcid.org/0000-0002-0263-4598 an exam). Whereas some scholars have proposed the notion of
Sebastian Bürgler https://orcid.org/0000-0002-7304-507X “effortless self-control” when beneficial habits serve goals that are
This research was supported by a research Grant (100019_179207/1) from usually associated with the need for self-control (e.g., eating health-
the Swiss National Science Foundation awarded to Marie Hennecke. The
ily; Adriaanse et al., 2014; Gillebaart & de Ridder, 2015), most
authors have no conflict of interest to disclose. The ideas presented in this
article have never been disseminated elsewhere.
perspectives on self-control emphasize a second feature in the
Marie Hennecke and Sebastian Bürgler contributed equally to this work. definition of self-control: That it requires intention and self-initiated
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Marie efforts to resolve the conflict in the service of a goal (Bargh et al.,
Hennecke, Department of Psychology, University of Siegen, Obergraben 2010; Baumeister et al., 2008; Duckworth et al., 2016; Trope &
23, 57072 Siegen, Germany. Email: marie.hennecke@uni-siegen.de Fishbach, 2000; Wood et al., 2014). Under this premise, a situation

1262
METACOGNITION AND SELF-CONTROL 1263

in which a person advances a goal “by accident” or without Vohs, 2007). The model has recently faced widespread criticism
experiencing conflict (Myrseth & Fishbach, 2009), should not be given failures to replicate the so-called ego-depletion effect that had
considered as a situation involving self-control (Duckworth been seen as primary support for the model (Hagger et al., 2016;
et al., 2016). Vohs et al., 2021). In addition, its central theoretical assumptions
Note, that the term “cognitive control” is often used interchange- and methodological operationalizations have been questioned
ably with self-control but more common in the cognitive, neurosci- (Friese et al., 2019; Lurquin & Miyake, 2017). Second, recent
ence, and clinical literature (Inzlicht, Schmeichel, & Macrae, 2014). research has shown that the role of inhibitory control, a subprocess
The presumed overlap becomes clear when looking at definitions of cognitive control that can be understood as the suppression of
of cognitive control as the “coordination of mental processes and motor responses, impulses, and previously activated cognitive
action in accordance with current goals and future plans” (Menon & content or processes (Friedman & Miyake, 2004; Harnishfeger,
D’Esposito, 2022, p. 90), or “the ability to regulate, coordinate, and 1995), and that has for a long time been presumed to be the process
sequence thoughts and actions in accordance with internally main- behind successful self-control, may have been exaggerated in the
tained behavioral goals” (Braver, 2012, p. 106). Empirically, the past, given that computer-based measures of inhibitory control do
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

association is more ambiguous though. On the one hand, research not predict self-control in daily life (Saunders et al., 2018). The lack
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

supports the overlap: Individuals high in cognitive control also show of convergence may, as for the broader construct cognitive control,
better self-control (Lawrence et al., 2009), have the same benefits in be caused by different assessment methods (Wennerhold & Friese,
terms of positive life outcomes as individuals high in self-control 2020) or be more substantial if, as others have argued, inhibition
(Hall et al., 2008; Mischel et al., 1989), and imaging studies suggest might be better understood as an outcome, rather than a process
overlapping brain regions in the prefrontal cortex (Hare et al., 2009; of self-control (Werner et al., 2022). In any case, along with the
Peters & Büchel, 2011; Shamosh et al., 2008). On the other hand, diminishing significance of these formerly prominent assumptions
recent research also calls this overlap into question: It shows that about self-control and its underlying processes, the consideration of
laboratory-based assessments of cognitive control barely correlate alternative processes contributing to success and failure in self-
with indicators of self-control (Allom et al., 2016; Nęcka et al., 2018; control have made their way into scholarship. As we will argue,
Saunders et al., 2018), increases in cognitive control do not transfer to many of these processes, sometimes without due acknowledgement
increases in self-control (Scherbaum et al., 2018), and self-control and so far without sufficient theoretical integration, are metacog-
does not show the same age-related decline as cognitive control nitive in nature.
(Hennecke & Freund, 2014; Williams et al., 1999). This lack of an
overlap may be caused by the fact that the computer-based tasks used
Metacognition
to measure cognitive control (e.g., the Flanker task, Eriksen &
Eriksen, 1974, or the Stop-Signal task, Verbruggen & Logan, The term metacognition was introduced to psychology by devel-
2008) are unreliable for the assessment of stable individual differ- opmental and educational psychologists in the 1970s (Flavell, 1977;
ences (Hedge et al., 2018). In addition, self-control conflicts are, in see also Schwarz, 2015). In their subdisciplines, metacognition is
potentially crucial ways, quite different from such cognitive response primarily understood as cognition about cognition related to learning
conflicts as they typically occur in the computer-based reaction tasks and refers to the processes by which learners make plans and
that are used to measure cognitive control (Kotabe & Hofmann, organize their studying (e.g., by determining study slots and what
2015): They are, for example, affectively charged (Inzlicht & and how to study during them), monitor their progress (e.g., by
Legault, 2014), as also shown by the fact that they involve subcortical testing their own knowledge), and evaluate their own study strategies
reward- and emotion-related brain activity (Heatherton & Wagner, in order to adapt them if necessary (e.g., Pintrich et al., 2000). Still to
2011), and allow for a wider range of responses to resolve them date, most research on metacognition investigates its beneficial role
(Hennecke & Bürgler, 2020). We would therefore caution against for self-regulated learning and memory (Dimmitt & McCormick,
simply equating the two constructs but discuss where research on 2012; Dunlosky & Metcalfe, 2008; Hacker et al., 2009; Wang et al.,
cognitive control can inform our framework about metacognition in 1990). Nevertheless, the concept has also captured the attention of
self-control. researchers outside the field of education and its role has, for
There is ample evidence that self-control is highly important. In example, been acknowledged for memory processes in general
fact, self-control predicts high achievement at work and school, (e.g., Dunlosky & Bjork, 2013) and for processes of social cognition
health-promoting behaviors, helps people to stay away from drugs (Wyer et al., 1984), for example, when people form judgments of
and crime, and promotes the quality of social relationships (de truth, attitude strength, or the informational value of feelings (Briñol
Ridder et al., 2012; Hofmann et al., 2014; Moffitt et al., 2011; & DeMarree, 2012; Petty et al., 2007; Schwarz, 2015). A more
Tangney et al., 2004). Given the importance of good self-control for inclusive definition therefore understands metacognition as people’s
people’s psychological adjustment, it is also important to understand beliefs and knowledge about their own cognition and the cognition of
the factors and processes that contribute to it, for example, for others, including mental processes, states, and capacities, as well as
intervention purposes. Currently, there is a surge of research trying the processes involved when people think about their own cognition
to identify these factors and processes that allow people to success- (Jost et al., 1998; Pintrich et al., 2000; Schraw & Moshman, 1995).
fully control themselves. This surge likely results from the fact that Our use of the term metacognition in the context of self-control is
two formerly dominant assumptions about self-control have recently rather broad and does not only include cognition about cognitive
been challenged, raising interest in alternative accounts: First, the phenomena. Rather it should also, if relevant to a self-control conflict,
strength model of self-control purported the assumption that self- include cognition about motivation, that is, metamotivation (Miele
control relies on a limited resource that is diminished through the et al., 2020; Scholer et al., 2018), which refers to the “processes by
exertion of self-control (Baumeister et al., 1998; Baumeister & which individuals monitor and control their motivational states
1264 HENNECKE AND BÜRGLER

in order to achieve their goals” (Scholer et al., 2018, p. 437), as well general theory of self-regulation and well-suited to address matters of
as individuals’ knowledge about their motivational states and how monitoring, discrepancy detection, and discrepancy reduction that
they impact their goal pursuit (Miele & Scholer, 2016). Where are all relevant for self-control, but it neither addresses individual
suitable, we will refer to and integrate findings about metamotiva- differences in the extent to which people engage in monitoring nor
tion into our framework. We furthermore use metacognition in the the anticipatory metacognitive processes aimed at preventing the
context of self-control also when referring to cognition about occurrence of discrepancies in the first place. Kotabe and Hofmann’s
affective phenomena relevant for self-control. Desires, for example, (2015) integrative self-control theory very well explicates the com-
are affectively charged responses to objects or activities associated ponents that are necessary for the presence, identification, and
with pleasure or relief from discomfort (Kavanagh et al., 2005). resolution of self-control conflicts but does not say much about
How people think or feel about their desires, is also in the realm of the metacognitive processes that occur before or after self-control
metacognition as we use it here. conflicts like the anticipation or retrospective evaluation of self-
control conflicts or about the deployment of self-regulatory strate-
gies. Duckworth et al.’s (2016) process model of self-control focuses
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Why Do We Need a Framework for heavily on the deployment of self-regulatory strategies, but neither
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Metacognition in Self-Control? addresses individual differences nor conflict detection or monitoring


We are convinced that, for a comprehensive understanding of processes during the experience of self-control conflicts. Choice
self-control, there is the need for a framework that explicates the role models (Berkman et al., 2017; Buckholtz, 2015; Neal et al.,
of metacognition for it. As others have noted, “self-control … is 2017) strive to predict the value-based considerations people may
inherently metacognitive” (Duckworth et al., 2014, p. 204; see also make when weighing the pros and cons of delaying gratification in a
Achtziger et al., 2012). It shares with all metacognitive phenomena self-control conflict but do not consider self-awareness, self-
the characteristic that there are two levels of cognition: an object- knowledge, or the ongoing processes before, during, and after a
level and a meta-level. If a person experiences a desire (e.g., for a self-control conflict that deal with its anticipation, resolution, or
donut) this experience represents the object-level. If that person then evaluation. Trait models (e.g., Whiteside & Lynam, 2001) are useful
cognitively engages to resist the desire (e.g., realizes that it conflicts for describing individual differences in self-control and impulsivity
with their goal of losing weight and thinks about whether and how to but say little about the underlying within-person processes of self-
resist it), this happens on the meta-level. However, even though self- control (Inzlicht et al., 2021; but see DeYoung, 2015). Last, models
control itself is a metacognitive phenomenon, there is no compre- of self-regulated learning (e.g., Boekaerts, 1996; Efklides, 2011;
hensive model or framework, yet, that explicates structural and Pintrich, 2000; Winne & Hadwin, 1998; Zimmerman & Moylan,
functional components of metacognition before, during, and after 2009; for a review, see Panadero, 2017) are too limited and special-
the experience of a self-control conflict and in the service of self- ized in their scope to be applicable to different types of self-control
control itself. What exists so far are disconnected empirical findings conflicts.
speaking to metacognition in self-control as well as numerous This is not to say that these models and theories are not valuable,
theories of self-control (see Inzlicht et al., 2021, for an overview) rather we borrow from them to the extent that they are helpful for
that already consider certain metacognitive aspects of and for self- understanding the role of metacognition in self-control (see Table 1).
control but without being comprehensive in doing so. Cybernetic In conclusion, we present a framework that, albeit heuristic, is both
control theory (Carver & Scheier, 1998), for example, is a more an individual-difference and a process-oriented model with a

Table 1
Overview of Existing Theories and Their Components Integrated in the Present Framework

Theories Theory components integrated in the present framework

Cybernetic control theory (Carver & Scheier, 1998, 2012) Role of performance monitoring and responding to feedback
Conflict monitoring theory (Botvinick et al., 2001, 2004) Role of monitoring and (self-control) conflict detection to instigate regulatory
processes
Integrative self-control theory (Kotabe & Hofmann, 2015) View that desire-goal conflict functions like other response conflicts and
instigates cognitive and behavioral adjustments
Process model of self-control (Duckworth et al., 2014, 2016) View of self-control as inherently metacognitive; strategy deployment before
(prevention) and during (intervention) a self-control conflict; assumption
that strategy use relies on “general capacities for metacognition and
prospection” (p. 204)
Metamotivation (Scholer et al., 2018) Role of self-knowledge and processes by which individual monitor and
control themselves to achieve goals
Trait models (e.g., Whiteside & Lynam, 2001) Role of stable individual differences in people’s self-control
Models of self-regulated learning and problem-solving Role of different types of metacognitive knowledge and metacognitive
(e.g., Pintrich, 2000; Schmitz et al., 2007; Zimmerman, 2000) regulatory processes for self-regulation, in particular processes of
forethought and prevention, regulation and monitoring, and reflection and
evaluation
Preventive-interventive model of self-control Distinction between processes aimed at preventing self-control conflicts and
(Hofmann & Kotabe, 2012) processes aimed at resolving present self-control conflicts; importance of
self-monitoring for conflict identification
METACOGNITION AND SELF-CONTROL 1265

consideration of the metacognition that happens before, during, and between two groups of components: (a) individual metacognitive
after a self-control conflict. The framework furthermore integrates characteristics of the person including their metacognitive knowl-
theories from different psychological subdisciplines including cog- edge about self-control and their self-awareness, and (b) metacog-
nitive, social, personality, and educational psychology, as well as nitive regulatory processes that may be at work before, during, and
empirical findings speaking to metacognition in self-control, that after a self-control conflict (see Figure 1).
have so far not been discussed in the context of larger theories (e.g., Distinguishing these two groups of components is not meant to
on implicit beliefs about willpower, Job et al., 2010). The resulting exclude the possibility that components of the framework mutually
framework accommodates recent assumptions about self-control influence each other. It is likely that individual metacognitive
being smart (Fujita et al., 2020), proactive (Hofmann & Kotabe, characteristics feed into the metacognitive processes and determine
2012; Sklar et al., 2017), strategic (Duckworth et al., 2016; their effectiveness and that metacognitive processes feed back into
Hennecke et al., 2019; Williamson & Wilkowski, 2020), flexible metacognitive characteristics. For example, people’s knowledge
(Bürgler et al., 2021; Wenzel et al., 2021), and sometimes, to a about self-regulatory strategies should have an effect on the strate-
certain degree, effortless (Gillebaart & de Ridder, 2015) rather than gies they use to regulate their behavior during a self-control conflict.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

being primarily reactive, depleting, and tied to the mechanism of Moreover, the process of monitoring one’s self-control performance
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

inhibition (Baumeister et al., 2007). It is furthermore applicable to likely informs the person about which strategies worked or did not
understanding self-control outcomes in various life domains, includ- work well in response to a self-control conflict. They thereby help to
ing education, work, health, and social relationships, can highlight update a person’s metacognitive knowledge about self-control. We
new directions for research on psychological components and will dive deeper into these mutual pathways later. Let us first
processes underlying self-control, and inform the design of inter- introduce the components of our framework one by one.
ventions aimed at supporting people in their self-control efforts. We
will later come back to these more applied benefits of the model in
the discussion.
Individual Differences in Metacognition
About Self-Control
The Framework for Metacognition in Self-Control When we refer to individual differences, we refer to both rela-
tively stable characteristics and traits of individuals as well as to
Overview
momentary expressions of these characteristics, so-called states.
Many accounts of metacognition during self-regulated learning Stable individual differences refer to what people typically feel,
consider metacognitive characteristics of the person, first and fore- think, or do in ways that differentiate them from other people
most their metacognitive knowledge, that is, what one knows (includ- (Funder, 2001). Despite their temporal and cross-situation stability,
ing theories and beliefs) about cognition, and distinguish it from these characteristics nevertheless also show momentary fluctuations
metacognitive regulation, that is, how one uses this knowledge to within individuals. Think, for example, of a person who is typically
regulate cognition and behavior (Baker, 1991; Brown & Palincsar, highly self-aware during goal pursuit, that is, has their own thoughts,
1982; Efklides, 2011; Flavell, 1979; Pintrich et al., 2000; Schraw & emotions, and behavior in focus (e.g., Duval & Wicklund, 1972).
Moshman, 1995). In line with this, our framework also distinguishes Even such a person may sometimes be relatively self-unaware, for

Figure 1
Metacognition in Self-Control Framework

Individual Metacognitive Knowledge, Theories, and Beliefs About Self-Control


Difference
Level Metacognitive/Self-Awareness

1. Forethought and 3. Regulation and 4. Reflection and


Prevention Monitoring Evaluation
Anticipation of self- Regulation Reflection and evaluation
control conflicts Allocation of of short-term success and
Self-Control Conflict
2. Identification of

Goal setting for self- resources long-term consequences


Process control conflicts Implementation of Attributions and inferences
Level Planning interventive strategies for future self-control
Allocation of Monitoring performance conflicts
resources Response to feedback
Selection of self- Allocation of
regulatory strategies resources
Implementation of Adaptation of
preventive strategies strategies
Goal revision

Self-Control
Time Conflict
1266 HENNECKE AND BÜRGLER

example, when an external task fully absorbs their mental resources subsume theories and beliefs under it. Notably, the term “metacog-
(Wicklund, 1975). Intraindividual variation in states does not nitive knowledge” has appeared before in the context of self-control,
contradict that, over time and situations, people nevertheless more specifically, in action control theory (e.g., Kuhl, 1984, 1987).
show stability in their traits and characteristics. In fact, the density Here, metacognitive knowledge includes, for example, knowledge
distributions of states are quite typical for individuals and can be about how emotional states could influence a person’s self-control
used to describe their characteristics over longer temporal episodes or about specific objects (e.g., a “fudge cake,” Kuhl, 1984, p. 126)
(Fleeson, 2001). that might have a strong motivational pull on someone and pose a
The characteristics we describe later may vary with regard to how problem for a person trying to lose weight. Such early mentions of
much intraindividual variation they show. Self-knowledge, for the term in self-control are, however, few and far between and they
example, is likely more stable than individual differences in self- were not yet properly integrated into a comprehensive framework
awareness, which, as described before, could show more intraindi- of metacognition in self-control.
vidual variation given its dependence on the availability of mental Declarative Knowledge About Self-Control. Declarative
resources. Nevertheless, knowledge is malleable and can be updated knowledge generally refers to “knowledge about things.” Here, it
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

if conflicting information is attended to (Metcalfe & Kornell, 2007; refers to knowledge and theories about self-control in general, one’s
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Piaget, 1970). In addition, the influence of metacognitive knowl- own self-control, and about the factors that influence it, including
edge on self-control likely depends on whether it is activated, and knowledge about tasks, situations, and contexts that are relevant for
attended to (Förster & Liberman, 2007). self-control, and knowledge of different self-regulatory strategies
(e.g., Pintrich et al., 2000; Schraw & Moshman, 1995). One example
Metacognitive Knowledge, Theories, and for declarative knowledge about self-control in general are the
Beliefs About Self-Control abovementioned theories people have about whether self-control
is a limited resource or not (Job et al., 2010). These beliefs, referred
Generally, metacognitive knowledge refers to knowledge about to as implicit beliefs or lay theories about willpower, have been
one’s cognition or about cognition in general (Schraw & Dennison, assessed with items such as “After a strenuous mental activity your
1994). Accordingly, metacognitive knowledge about self-control is energy is depleted and you must rest to get it refueled again.” The
the knowledge that people have about their self-control and about extent to which people endorse an unlimited as opposed to a limited
self-control in general. Metacognitive knowledge can furthermore theory of willpower has been shown to affect their self-control in
be distinguished into various types. We here borrow the distinction various tasks, including laboratory tasks (Job et al., 2010) and self-
of three types of knowledge about metacognition from research on regulation during exam preparation (Job et al., 2015). Similarly, it
self-regulated learning, which distinguishes declarative knowledge, was shown that people differ in the extent to which they believe that
procedural knowledge, and conditional knowledge (Pintrich et al., the limits of self-control are fixed versus malleable and these beliefs
2000; Schraw & Moshman, 1995). We explain these terms in have consequences as well: People who believed that self-control
detail later. is a malleable but inherently limited resource tend to set fewer
Furthermore, our use of the term knowledge here is also meant to resolutions and are also less likely to succeed at them if they are
include the metacognitive theories people hold about self-control. also low in self-efficacy (Mukhopadhyay & Johar, 2005).
We understand the term “theory” as a relatively systematic structure Whereas implicit beliefs assess people’s beliefs about whether
of knowledge that can be used to explain and predict a broad range self-control is a limited and depletable resource, self-reports of
of (empirical) phenomena (Schraw & Moshman, 1995), here: in the personality assess what people believe about their trait levels of
domain of self-control. Such theories might differ with regard to self-control. Such self-report scales capitalize on people’s self-
how explicit and formalized they are in people’s heads. People’s knowledge (or self-concept) about the extent to which they can,
subjective theories about willpower as being a more or less limited in general and across situations, control their impulses and achieve
resource have, for example, been described as implicit (Job et al., their long-term goals with items such as “I am good at resisting
2012). What is meant here is that people might hold a theory without temptation” or “People would say that I have iron self-discipline.”
being readily aware of the theory itself or of evidence supporting or (Brief Self-Control Scale by Tangney et al., 2004). The predictive
refuting it (Schraw & Moshman, 1995). Nevertheless, they system- validity of such reports of self-knowledge for predicting important
atically express the theory or act in accordance with it. People who outcomes as well as their self-control success in daily life (de Ridder
more strongly endorse the subjective theory that self-control is a et al., 2012; Hennecke et al., 2019; Tangney et al., 2004) provides
limited resource, for example, take longer breaks after tasks that evidence that people have relatively accurate knowledge about their
required their self-control (Job et al., 2015). own trait levels of self-control. This is supported both by studies
Note, that whereas later we refer to different types of metacog- showing that self-perceptions of other personality traits are also
nitive “knowledge” we do not mean to suggest, however, that this correlated with objective criteria and with others’ perceptions of
knowledge is necessarily correct. Both knowledge and theories them (Vazire & Carlson, 2010). In addition, self-reports of personality
about metacognition may be misguided and reflect beliefs rather (including conscientiousness, a trait that is broader than self-control
than actual knowledge. For example, based on current failures to and also encompasses propensities to be orderly, hard-working, and
replicate the ego depletion effect (Vohs et al., 2021), it looks as if rule-abiding, Roberts et al., 2009, 2014) generally predict important
individuals who endorse the theory that self-control is a limited life outcomes like mortality, divorce, or occupational success (Ozer &
resource (Job et al., 2010, 2015), endorse an, in fact, inaccurate Benet-Martínez, 2006; Roberts et al., 2007). These correlations,
belief. While the term “belief” would more accurately express that however, are far from perfect and suggest that people also have
what people seem to know may be inaccurate, the term “knowledge” blind spots with regard to their personalities. There are many possible
is commonly used in the literature on self-regulated learning and we reasons for individuals’ insufficient declarative self-knowledge
METACOGNITION AND SELF-CONTROL 1267

including self-serving motivations and a general lack of insight into during any given conflict are also more successful in resolving it
unconscious processes (see Wilson & Dunn, 2004). So far, however, (Bürgler et al., 2021; Milyavskaya et al., 2020). This speaks to the
no research has investigated what consequences it has if people have importance of strategy repertoire as one component of declarative
largely inaccurate declarative knowledge about their own self- self-control knowledge.
control. It is likely that individuals who, for example, systematically Finally, a recent strand of research has looked at people’s
overestimate their own self-control, are less prepared for dealing metamotivational knowledge, that is, people’s knowledge and
with self-control conflicts (Jia et al., 2022). People who underesti- beliefs about the benefits or harms of certain motivational states
mate their own self-control, however, could be more cautious and, (see Scholer et al., 2018, for a review). This research has shown that
for example, rely more on preventive self-regulatory strategies to people tend to have false conceptions about motivation: They are
avoid or attenuate anticipated self-control conflicts. unaware that extrinsic incentives undermine their intrinsic motiva-
Besides such general declarative knowledge about self-control, tion (Murayama et al., 2016) and fail to anticipate the importance of
people might also have more or less appropriate knowledge about intrinsic incentives and of being autonomously motivated for future
their idiosyncratic strengths and weaknesses in the domain of self- task accomplishment (Werner & Milyavskaya, 2018; Woolley &
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

control (Pintrich et al., 2000). Such knowledge might be the result of Fishbach, 2015). These findings are relevant for self-control, too,
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

intraindividual comparison processes. A person may, for example, given that people might have to invest more self-control to finish
think or know about themselves that they are quite good at getting up tasks for which intrinsic and autonomous motivation is lacking.
for exercise but bad at resisting chocolate. Such knowledge might Conditional Knowledge About Self-Control. Conditional
then inform subsequent behavior, if the person decides that it is self-control knowledge represents a special, more contextualized
better not to buy any chocolate in order not to get tempted at home or type of declarative knowledge. “Conditional” here refers to the
that in order to lose weight, exercising more will be more feasible conditions that moderate how self-control works. It includes
than eating less chocolate. knowledge about all types of situational or personal influences
In addition, declarative knowledge about self-control also in- on self-control, such as knowledge about how one’s self-control
cludes people’s knowledge of different self-regulatory strategies works under specific conditions (e.g., being alone vs. with others)
(Pintrich et al., 2000). Self-regulatory strategies can be defined as or beliefs about when to use different self-regulatory strategies
the “means through which individuals, in order to help themselves (Pintrich et al., 2000; Schraw & Moshman, 1995).
achieve their goals, actively alter their cognitive, motivational, People may know more or less about the external and internal
affective, or behavioral reactions to a self-regulatory challenge” conditions that affect their own self-control. The presence of other
(Hennecke et al., 2019, p. 104). Mischel and Mischel (1983) were people may, for example, have effects on a person’s self-control
the first who were interested, in particular, in the development of (Hofmann et al., 2012). A person might, for example, know or
knowledge about self-control strategies. In their studies, children believe that the presence of other people usually helps them to avoid
were asked to indicate whether they wanted immediately available overeating (e.g., because they would feel ashamed in front of others
smaller rewards to be in sight or not while they were waiting in the if they did) but triggers a desire for alcohol consumption (e.g.,
hope to receive a larger reward. The studies showed that 4-year old because it makes them more relaxed around others). In addition,
children often preferred the self-defeating strategy of having the internal states like certain moods or mental fatigue (Aspinwall,
immediate rewards exposed. Children nearing 5 years of age then 1998; Clarkson et al., 2016; Fishbach & Labroo, 2007; Leith &
more frequently preferred the empirically superior strategy of Baumeister, 1996; Saunders & Inzlicht, 2016) may affect a person’s
having the rewards out of sight while waiting. A more recent finding self-control, and people may be more or less aware of these effects.
furthermore shows that adults appear to be aware that mentally Knowing about external and internal conditions under which self-
reframing a situation in an abstract or high-level construal as control is more difficult should furthermore be helpful preconditions
opposed to a concrete or low-level construal promotes self-control for taking precautionary measures to avoid, attenuate, or resolve
(MacGregor et al., 2017; Nguyen et al., 2019). For example, self-control conflicts. A person who knows that fatigue at the end of
participants who were prompted to act in a self-controlled manner the day usually keeps them from sticking to their exercise plans,
during a cookie taste test rated high-level statements describing the might plan to exercise before lunch instead.
task like “I need to evaluate the cookies conscientiously” or “I will Whereas knowledge about the existence of certain self-regulatory
think carefully about the cookies” as more helpful for their self- strategies belongs to the category of declarative knowledge, condi-
control goal than low-level statements like “I will be crunching on tional knowledge about when strategies work better or less well
cookies” and “I will be tasting some cookies.” In addition, dieters seems paramount too. It has, for example, been argued that the
and students who had knowledge about the benefits of high-level effectiveness of self-regulatory strategies depends on given self-
construals appeared to pursue their self-control goals, dieting and control demands (Hennecke & Bürgler, 2020; Hennecke et al., 2019).
achieving good grades, more successfully (MacGregor et al., 2017). In other words: A strategy that is effective in one type of self-control
In sum, it looks that people who know that reframing a self-control conflict might not be equally effective in another. Related evidence
situation in higher level terms may use this knowledge strategically stems from the field of emotion regulation. Here, researchers have
to attain better self-control. acknowledged that it is not true, that some strategies are inherently,
Recent research furthermore shows that people benefit from a across situations, adaptive, whereas others are inherently, across
larger repertoire of self-regulatory strategies (Bürgler et al., 2021, situations, maladaptive. Problem-focused emotion strategies, are, for
2022). The number of different strategies used over the course of a example, helpful when stressors are controllable but harmful when
10-day experience sampling study predicted momentary success at stressors are uncontrollable (Aldridge & Roesch, 2007; Clarke,
self-control conflicts. In addition, people who used more strategies 2006). The same might be true for self-regulatory strategies: Any
1268 HENNECKE AND BÜRGLER

given strategy may be adaptive in one context but not in another. In Metacognitive/Self-Awareness
line with this assumption, a larger degree of variability in choosing
from a repertoire of strategies is indeed associated with higher We posit that people do not only differ regarding the metacog-
self-regulatory success (Wenzel et al., 2021). That the fit between a nitive knowledge they have about self-control but also with regard to
self-regulatory strategy and the self-control challenge it is used for how much attention they pay to their own self-control. In the
literature, there is a large number of constructs referring to individ-
matters, is furthermore supported by the observation that modera-
ual differences in people’s capacity or tendency to pay attention to
tion (eating a smaller, predetermined amount of something
their own thoughts, feelings, and actions (see Leary & Terry, 2012).
unhealthy) is a strategy that works better for people high than for
These constructs include self-awareness (Duval & Wicklund, 1972),
people low in self-control, who might fare better with complete
self-reflection (Grant et al., 2002), private self-consciousness
abstinence (Haws et al., 2011). A couple of different factors
(Fenigstein et al., 1975), metacognitive capacity (e.g., Weil et al.,
including such individual characteristics, but also task demands
2013), metacognitive awareness (e.g., Schraw & Dennison, 1994),
and goal characteristics might moderate the effectiveness of strate-
and self-monitoring (Briggs et al., 1980; Wilmot, 2015). Whereas
gies like this (Hennecke & Bürgler, 2020). For example, the strategy
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

different theories emphasize different aspects of the construct and


task enrichment, which entails adding a positive stimulus (e.g.,
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

even further differentiate various components (e.g., private vs.


music) to an otherwise effortful and aversive task (e.g., working out)
public; capacity vs. inclination; state vs. trait), the common denom-
is not generally helpful for self-control but has positive effects on
inator of these concepts is that they all refer to the extent that an
self-control during boring and physically effortful tasks (Hennecke individual pays attention to aspects of the self, including their own
et al., 2019). thoughts, feelings, and behaviors. The kind of self-awareness we
That people benefit from conditional knowledge about self- refer to has been assessed with items such as “I frequently take time
control is furthermore supported by recent work showing that people to reflect on my thoughts,” “I am usually aware of my thoughts,” or
benefit from knowing the conditions under which a high-level “I usually have a clear idea about why I’ve behaved in a certain
construal, a cognitive representation of objects or events that high- way.” (Self-Reflection and Insight Scale, Grant et al., 2002).
lights their abstract and essential elements as opposed to a low-level Some research has attested to the important role of individual
construal which highlights concrete and surface-level details, is differences in self-awareness for self-control. Beaman et al. (1979)
beneficial for goal attainment and self-control (MacGregor et al., showed that children were less likely to eat more than the allowed
2017; Nguyen et al., 2019) amount of candy when they were seated in front of a mirror. Study
Procedural Knowledge About Self-Control. This last type of participants who were primed to be more self-aware (vs. who
metacognitive knowledge about self-control refers to knowledge received a control prime) furthermore did not drop in performance
about “how to do things” when it comes to self-control, that is, how during a second self-control task, thereby not showing the alleged
to deal with specific self-control conflicts. On first look, it might ego depletion effect (Alberts et al., 2011). In addition, self-focused
seem difficult to distinguish procedural from declarative knowledge emotions like (anticipated) shame and guilt are antecedents of
that refers to people’s repertoire of self-regulatory strategies. How- subsequent self-control (Baumeister, 1997; Chun et al., 2007;
ever, it is different from repertoire in the sense that it is not just Hofmann & Fisher, 2012; Kivetz & Zheng, 2006; Zemack-Rugar
knowledge about the existence of a given strategy but more specifi- et al., 2007). Finally, people who are either chronically or momen-
cally knowledge that refers to its proper implementation (Pintrich tarily self-aware, are also more likely to detect obstacles, likely
et al., 2000; Schraw & Moshman, 1995). For example, a person including self-control conflicts, to their goals (Kreibich et al., 2020).
might, in principle, know a self-regulatory strategy but fail to apply In addition, self-awareness and the process of self-monitoring
it to a current self-control conflict. Procedural self-control knowl- (which we will discuss in more detail later) have been linked to a
edge is therefore a precondition for the successful development of person’s increased awareness of their own behavior in comparison
self-regulatory “tactics” which have been described as concrete to a desired standard (Carver & Scheier, 1981). In line with this
implementations of strategies in accordance with situational de- perspective, the process of self-monitoring has often been discussed
mands (e.g., Ford et al., 2019; McRae et al., 2012; Werner & Ford, as an important predictor of self-control more generally, given its
2021). Think of the strategy of self-reinforcement or self-reward as importance for identifying that a current desire is in conflict with a
an example: Even if a person has this strategy in their repertoire, they long-term goal (e.g., Baumeister & Heatherton, 1996; Hofmann &
still need to decide how to implement it in any given instance. Kotabe, 2012).
A person who has, for example, rewarded themselves with delicious Note that self-awareness should also be an important prerequisite
food for most of their life will have to find a new type of reward for to acquiring metacognitive knowledge about self-control (see
sticking to their diet. Fleming & Dolan, 2012). Through self-observation and reflection,
Note that the two terms “strategies” and “tactics” refer to people should, for example, acquire declarative knowledge about
constructs on a continuum from relatively general regulatory their own self-regulatory strengths and weaknesses, procedural
approaches (strategies) to more concrete regulatory actions (tactics, knowledge about the correct or incorrect implementation of self-
Ford et al., 2019). Such a continuum should therefore also be regulatory strategies, and conditional knowledge about the context
assumed for declarative versus procedural knowledge about strat- for which these strategies are suitable and lead to success.
egies. In addition, because an appropriate procedural application of
any given strategy or tactic requires conditional knowledge about
Metacognitive Processes in Self-Control
the situation and how it will affect the suitability of a strategy or
tactic, the three types of metacognitive knowledge are obviously Whereas people’s metacognitive knowledge about self-control and
closely intertwined. their self-awareness describe characteristic individual differences,
METACOGNITION AND SELF-CONTROL 1269

we would like to now turn to the specific metacognitive mechanisms personal future (e.g., Atance & O’Neill, 2001, 2005; Schacter
or processes that might unfold before, during, and after a self-control et al., 2017). The extent to which episodic future thinking results
conflict. We adopt the distinction of these different phases from in the correct anticipation of upcoming self-control conflicts is
prominent models of self-regulation and self-regulated learning: likely, at least to some degree, contingent on the metacognitive
The model of action phases (Heckhausen & Gollwitzer, 1986, 1987) knowledge people have acquired about themselves. Knowledge of
distinguishes different phases (a) before (predecisional phase includ- one’s weak spots when it comes to self-control should be helpful for
ing deliberation about possible goals, preactional phase including assessing whether a self-control conflict could emerge in an upcom-
making plans for goal pursuit), (b) during (actional phase where goal ing situation. Being invited to a party, for example, could entail a
pursuit happens), and (c) after goal pursuit (postactional phase where self-control conflict to the dieter who is easily tempted by the food
an evaluation of goal pursuit and attainment takes place). Similar offered at such gatherings. Knowing or anticipating that one will be
phases (sometimes with more fine-grained distinctions) can also be tempted in this situation is likely, at least to some extent, the result of
found in many models of self-regulated learning (e.g., Pintrich, 2000; recollecting past experiences in similar situations. It is therefore not
Schmitz et al., 2007; Winne, 2011; Zimmerman, 2000; Zimmerman surprising that episodic future thinking depends critically on the
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

& Moylan, 2009; see Panadero, 2017, or Puustinen & Pulkkinen, episodic memory system, which underlies people’s ability to
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

2001, for reviews). These models use different terms and emphasize recollect past personal experiences. According to the constructive
different aspects of self-regulated learning but all consider (a) a episodic simulation hypothesis, for example, episodic memory
forethought or preparatory phase during which learning goals are supports the anticipation of future events by allowing people to
set and learning episodes are planned, (b) a performance and moni- flexibly retrieve and recombine elements of past experiences into
toring phase during which learning behavior is regulated, monitored, novel representations of future events (Schacter & Addis, 2007).
and, if necessary, adapted, and (c) a self-reflection or appraisal phase The anticipation of self-control conflicts is certainly important for
during which the previous learning episode is reflected and attribu- their avoidance. We will later return to the preventive strategies
tions are made. individuals may use. However, even if a self-control conflict cannot
We borrow the distinction into phases before, during, and after a be avoided, anticipation might help to set goals and make plans for
self-control conflict. Besides these phases, we include the identifi- dealing with it.
cation of a self-control conflict as an important point of transition Goal Setting for Self-Control Conflicts. Self-control conflicts
necessary to instigate metacognitive regulation during the respective are, per definition, conflicts in which current desires, habits, or
conflict. impulses conflict with more valued or long-term goals. Goal setting
for a self-control conflict might, however, go beyond these valued,
long-term goals (whose characteristics might also matter for self-
Metacognitive Processes Before a Self-Control Conflict:
control, Inzlicht, Legault, & Teper, 2014) and rather refer to more
Forethought and Prevention immediate and contextualized goals for upcoming self-control
Before a self-control conflict emerges, people can take cautionary conflicts. For example, a person may, in anticipation of the desire
measures to prevent it from occurring or to prepare themselves for its to drink alcohol at a birthday party and the resulting conflict with
occurrence. The following two groups of processes we call fore- their long-term goal of health maintenance, commit to the goal of
thought and prevention could be relevant in this regard. Under “having only one alcoholic drink at the party.” Like other goals,
forethought, we subsume (a) anticipating of self-control conflicts, these goals may vary too with regard to various characteristics.
(b) goal setting for self-control conflicts, and (c) planning. Preven- Besides, for example, their level of ambition, their degree of
tion refers to the implementation of preventive strategies. specificity might be an important factor in supporting the person’s
Anticipation of Self-Control Conflicts. The saying “Preven- degree of success in the self-control conflict, as has been found true
tion is better than cure” reveals that in many situations, it is advisable for goals in general (Locke & Latham, 2006). For example, the goal
to take care of problems before they occur. According to the of “not drinking much alcohol” is less specific than the goal of
Preventive-Interventive Model of Self-Control (Hofmann & “having only one alcoholic drink.” And, given that what is “much
Kotabe, 2012) precautions taken before a self-control conflict alcohol” is not defined in the first goal, the person may find it
difficult to decide when to stop drinking in a tempting situation.
may affect to what extent problematic desires will emerge at a later point Planning. Once individuals have set goals for anticipated self-
in time, how conflicted people will feel about tempting stimuli, how control conflicts, they can make plans on how to achieve these goals.
motivated and capable they will be to resist, and to what extent their
Selection of Self-Regulatory Strategies. The person who has
range of behavioral options may allow them to give in to tempta-
tion. (p. 709)
decided that they want to drink only one drink could plan to use their
car to get back home after the party, thereby enforcing that they can
The use of precautions, for example, preventive strategies like have only one drink (through a form of precommitment, Kurth-
the removal of distractions or temptations (Duckworth et al., 2016; Nelson & Redish, 2012). Or they may plan to bring their own favorite
Kuhl, 1984), however, requires people to anticipate upcoming nonalcoholic drinks to make abstaining from more alcohol easier.
conflicts. Unless the strategy is strongly habitualized, a student Generally, planning refers to committing to concrete actions to
would, for example, have to anticipate that they could be distracted advance one’s goals by specifying intended actions in terms of
by incoming messages before being able to take the precaution of when, where, and how to act (Leventhal et al., 1965). A related
turning off the sound of their cell phone. concept are implementation intentions (Gollwitzer, 1999; Gollwitzer
Anticipating future self-control conflicts requires episodic future & Brandstätter, 1997), which consist of an “if”-part defining the
thinking, a form of projection (Gilbert & Wilson, 2007) that includes situational triggers for planned actions defined in the “then”-part, for
imagining or simulating experiences that might occur in one’s example, “If someone offers me a second drink, then I will ask for
1270 HENNECKE AND BÜRGLER

water.”. Indeed, implementation intentions outperform mere goal strategy can be helpful for making plans for future conflicts. In fact,
intentions (here, e.g., “I will have only one drink.”) in helping people it has been shown that people exhibit more successful self-control
to advance their goals (Gollwitzer & Oettingen, 1998; Orbell & when they can easily recall previous successful instances of self-
Sheeran, 2000). Implementation intentions have also been shown to control (Nikolova et al., 2016).
be effective for dealing with self-control conflicts. In these studies, Implementation of Preventive Strategies. Preventive strate-
participants were instructed to form implementation intentions that gies can be implemented before self-control conflicts in order to avoid
reminded them of their own long-term goals when faced with a them or increase one’s chances of resolving them. The process model
temptation (e.g., “If I see or smell chocolate then I will follow my of self-control distinguishes between two types of strategies that
goal to diet”; Kroese et al., 2011; Van Koningsbruggen et al., 2011). prevent or attenuate upcoming self-control conflicts. First, situation
While these types of implementation intentions do not contain a selection is referred to as the “most forward-looking of all self-control
specific intervention strategy, they could be even more helpful if they maneuvers” (p. 40) and entails that people avoid environments that
did (e.g., “If I see or smell chocolate then I will avert my attention could present temptations and seek out environments that promote
away from it.”). We will come back to discussing different inter- goal pursuit (Duckworth et al., 2016). To avoid drinking too much,
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

ventive strategies later when describing how people can implement a person might, for example, never show up at parties. To help
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

them during a self-control conflict. themselves stay focused during studying, a student may move to the
Allocation of Resources. Planning may also include decisions quiet library. Avoiding temptations is a strategy that, as recently
about the allocation of resources to deal with upcoming self-control shown, is in fact used by individuals high in self-control who are, for
conflicts. In models of self-regulated learning, time, effort, and pace example, more likely to choose working in a distraction-free envi-
of learning have been proposed as resources that learners allocate to ronment than individuals low in self-control (Ent et al., 2015).
learning processes (Pintrich, 2000). In the domain of self-control, Second, a person can use situation modification (Duckworth
the most important resource might be effort. Effort can be under- et al., 2016; see also “environment control,” Kuhl, 1984, p. 126),
stood as the intensity of mental and/or physical activity that an for example, if they do not get to select the situations they find
organism invests into pursuing an outcome (Eisenberger, 1992; themselves in. It implies making changes to a situation to avoid or
Inzlicht et al., 2018). It can be distinguished from demand or attenuate upcoming self-control conflicts. Supporting the effective-
difficulty, although higher demands require more effort to be ness of situation modification, recent studies showed that students
met. When, however, a task becomes too demanding, individuals who were introduced to the idea of moving temptations, like their
may disengage their effort completely (Brehm & Self, 1989). cell phones, out of sight, reported higher levels of goal accomplish-
Generally, humans and other animals are thought to avoid effort ment and study quality than students who had received the instruc-
if possible but to invest it into valued outcomes (Hull, 1943; Kool tion to “use their willpower” in response to self-control conflicts or
et al., 2010). How much effort a person plans to invest into the no instruction at all (Duckworth et al., 2016). Additional situation
resolution of a self-control conflict is therefore likely a consequence modification strategies might include installing reminders of con-
of how important the long-term goal with whom the temptation or flict, seeking social support from others, or the ingestion of sub-
impulse is in conflict, is to the person (Richter et al., 2016). It may stances like coffee or energy drinks to promote better self-control
also be a consequence of the type of motivation underlying the goal. (Hennecke et al., 2019; Hofmann & Kotabe, 2012).
Individuals who feel autonomously motivated for a given goal might In addition, other strategies may also be implemented before a
be more willing to invest effort into dealing with self-control conflicts self-control conflict that do not change the observable situation but
on the way as compared to individuals whose motivation for the goal that nevertheless bolster a person’s motivation or volition to resolve
is controlled (Milyavskaya et al., 2018). This could be one path an upcoming conflict. As described before, people may set goals and
through which the positive effects of autonomous motivation on goal make plans for how to deal with an anticipated conflict. In addition,
progress might be explained (Werner & Milyavskaya, 2018). they may mentally prepare themselves for an upcoming conflict in
Individuals may, however, also make plans that budget the various ways: They may, for example, try to bolster their motivation
amount of effort they want to invest into resolving an anticipated to resolving the conflict by reminding themselves of the negative
self-control conflict, if they believe that their self-control is a limited consequences of failing to do so or the positive consequences of
resource (Job et al., 2010) and that they later need to invest more succeeding (Hennecke et al., 2019; Kuhl, 1984). In addition, they
self-control. In the context of ego depletion theory, it has been may decide to impose certain contingencies on themselves and
argued that after a first self-control task, participants who anticipated use self-reinforcement (Bandura, 1976; Goldfried & Merbaum,
exerting self-control in the future performed worse in an intervening 1973; Grunschel et al., 2016), in order to bolster their motivation
self-control task than participants who either had not performed the for upcoming self-control conflicts. Imposing contingencies is also
first self-control task or who did not expect to exert self-control in at the core of many so-called “precommitment” strategies, which are
the future (Muraven et al., 2006; Tyler & Burns, 2009). While ego more generally defined as strategies that take away “a choice from
depletion theory has faced widespread criticism for being theoreti- one’s future self in order to enforce one’s present preferences”
cally vague and empirically questionable (Lurquin & Miyake, 2017; (Kurth-Nelson & Redish, 2012, p. 2). Precommitment may involve
Vohs et al., 2021), it could be that some people who endorse the forms of situation selection and modification to prevent oneself from
metacognitive theory that self-control is limited, treat it like that, unwanted behaviors (e.g., avoiding the bakery or flushing one’s
and budget it in their plans to save it for later tasks. cigarettes down the toilet), making public commitment to harness
Note that memory processes are probably not only important for social influence, but even self-imposed punishments should a given
the anticipation of self-control conflicts but also for making good goal not be fulfilled (e.g., on https://stickk.com, see also Ariely &
plans on how to deal with them. Episodic memories of prior Wertenbroch, 2002). Recent studies investigating precommitment
solutions and procedural knowledge on how to implement a specific furthermore show that on the neural level, its use can be distinguished
METACOGNITION AND SELF-CONTROL 1271

from the use of the interventive inhibition which had previously triggers the adjustment and mobilization of cognitive control pro-
been considered to be the cognitive process underlying self-control: cesses in the prefrontal cortex. In fact, individuals who do not show
Whereas inhibition can be traced back to activity in the dorsolateral the typical ERN in the ACC during self-control conflicts also show
and inferior frontal cortices as well as the anterior cingulate cortex worse self-control in a food choice task (van der Laan et al., 2014).
(ACC; Dias et al., 1997; Garavan et al., 1999; Konishi et al., 1998; In addition, a reduced error-related activity in a broader conflict-
Shimamura, 1995; Taylor et al., 1996), precommitment strategies monitoring network (involving the anterior midcingulate cortex, the
have been traced back specifically to activation in the frontopolar presupplementary motor area, and anterior insula) during a conflict
prefrontal cortex, a large region in the anterior portion of the frontal task in the laboratory even predicts a higher likelihood of failure in
lobe (Crockett et al., 2013; Soutschek et al., 2017), that is also daily self-control conflicts, above and beyond what can be predicted
involved in related high-order cognition like prospection and coun- by self-reported trait self-control (Krönke et al., 2018, 2020;
terfactual thinking (Boorman et al., 2009; Boschin et al., 2015; Overmeyer et al., 2021). But please note, that as in other research
Okuda et al., 2003, 2007). Importantly, this dissociation supports correlating computer-based indicators of cognitive control and more
the distinction that interventive self-control as inhibition works ecologically valid measures of self-control, their association is not
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

through a different route than self-control as achieved through unambiguous: Here, a recent study showed that the ERN and other
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

preventive strategies of precommitment. event-related potential failed to predict personal goal progress
Certainly, not all self-control conflicts can be anticipated and even (Saunders et al., 2022).
within the pool of conflicts that can, the extent to which people can be Whereas neurocognitive theories imply that the identification of
prepared for them and can rely on goals and plans that were set during self-control conflicts is primarily a matter of conflict monitoring
the previous phase may vary. Maybe a friend comes by the house more generally, individuals may also fail to recognize self-control
unexpectedly with homemade cookies, or one gets spontaneously conflicts despite an otherwise intact conflict monitoring system.
invited to go out drinking with colleagues after work. In that case, a This might be the case if they consider a potential action in an
self-control conflict might emerge without prior preparation. We will isolated way: In fact, the effects of eating one donut, smoking one
now turn to the metacognitive processes that can take place during cigarette, or having one alcoholic drink on a person’s health are
self-control conflicts. These processes are, however, to a great deal negligible. The identification of conflict relies on viewing an action
dependent on the identification of a situation as a self-control conflict. opportunity (e.g., eating a donut) not in isolation but with width, that
Accordingly, we consider the identification of self-control conflicts is, in relation to future opportunities, and with consistency, as a
as an important point of passage before turning to the next phase. decision that will be replayed in the future (Ainslie, 2021; Fishbach
& Converse, 2010; Fujita et al., 2006; Myrseth & Fishbach, 2009).
Identification of a Self-Control Conflict In addition, people might fail to recognize self-control conflicts
because they simply lack the knowledge that an action opportunity is
In their influential model of action phases, Heckhausen and at odds with a goal. For example, if a person does not know that a
Gollwitzer (1986, 1987) referred to the proverbial Rubicon as the salad with a fatty dressing actually has a lot of calories, they may not
important transition at which people take the step from deliberating perceive eating a rich salad as conflicting with their personal goal to
goal adoption to actual goal commitment. This Rubicon therefore lose weight (see, e.g., Klohe-Lehman et al., 2006, for findings on
marks the transition from the first, predecisional, to the second, the importance of nutrition knowledge in weight loss). If a person,
preactional phase of goal pursuit. With regard to self-control, there is however, correctly identifies a current situation as a self-control
an equally important transition, the point at which the person conflict, they can either try to successfully resolve it or not. This
identifies a self-control conflict at hand. In our perspective, the decision can be based on multiple factors, such as expectancy-value
identification of a self-control conflict is per definition a metacog- considerations (Atkinson, 1964; Eccles & Wigfield, 2002), self-
nitive process. It requires that the person recognizes that in a given efficacy (Bandura, 1994), importance of the goal or the person’s
situation a behavioral tendency (sometimes called a “desire,” e.g., commitment to it (Fishbach & Ferguson, 2007; Kruglanski et al.,
Kotabe & Hofmann, 2015) is at odds with a goal. 2002), as well as a multitude of other possible situational or personal
That the detection of conflict is crucial for the activation of factors (e.g., social factors such as social comparison, see Diel et al.,
effortful self-control processes has recently been acknowledged 2021, or personal factors such as impulsivity, see Price et al., 2013;
in various models of self-control (e.g., Inzlicht, Legault, & Waxman, 2009). Having discussed the different metacognitive
Teper, 2014; Milkman et al., 2008; Myrseth & Fishbach, processes that can unfold before self-control conflicts occur, we
2009). The detection of a “desire-goal conflict” is furthermore next want to focus on the metacognitive processes during self-
one of seven key components in Kotabe and Hofmann’s (2015) control conflicts.
integrative self-control model. They argue that in the sequence of
events, the detection of a conflict between a higher-order goal and a Metacognitive Processes During a Self-Control Conflict:
desire is what triggers control motivation, and given sufficient Regulation and Monitoring
control motivation and control capacity, in turn leads to control
effort, the “effective use of control capacity” (Kotabe & Hofmann, When a self-control conflict is identified, people can try to
2015. p. 620). On a neural basis, the detection of self-control (metacognitively) regulate their own behavior, thoughts, and feel-
and other response conflicts is probably processed in a conflict- ings to solve the conflict in favor of their goals. This primary
monitoring system (e.g., Botvinick et al., 2001, 2004), located in the regulation in response to a self-control conflict, however, likely does
brain’s ACC where, in case of conflict, an “alarm signal” can be not suffice but must be accompanied by a close monitoring of one’s
captured by an evoked brain potential called the error-related performance and a secondary form of regulation, namely, a suitable
negativity (ERN; Gehring et al., 1993). The alarm signal then response to feedback about this performance that includes an
1272 HENNECKE AND BÜRGLER

adaptation of one’s efforts and/or goal revision. We will describe less tempting manner (Metcalfe & Mischel, 1999), for example, by
these processes in more detail below. thinking of the crisps as “fatty, health-threatening potato slices” or
Regulation. Regulation refers to people’s active attempts of by adopting a high-level construal and thinking of eating crisps as
dealing with a current self-control conflict. It includes the allocation “satisfying my desire for junk food” rather than as “crunching on
of resources and the implementation of interventive self-regulatory salty potato slices” (Chiou et al., 2013; Fujita & Carnevale, 2012;
strategies. Fujita et al., 2006). Finally, a person can try to modulate or suppress
Allocation of Resources. In the framework, we include the an unwanted response. This strategy can be used if a self-control
allocation of resources at three different points. We have already conflict has fully emerged and is hence thought as a “last resort” in
described above that people can plan their allocation of resources the impulse-generation cycle (Duckworth et al., 2016). Initially, it
during a self-control conflict. Here, we refer to this actual allocation, was proposed that strategies used earlier in the impulse-generation
may it be as planned or different from any initial plans (if there were cycle are generally more effective than strategies used later
any) and later, we will return to the allocation of resources as an (Duckworth et al., 2016), but recent studies show that strategies
adaptational process in response to feedback (that more or less used later are not necessarily less effective, at least not generally
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

resources are needed). speaking (Lopez et al., 2021; Milyavskaya et al., 2020).
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

As already described, effort, the intensity of mental and/or Several studies have shown that people do indeed spontaneously
physical activity, is likely the most importance resource that people use a variety of different strategies in their everyday lives and that
can allocate to the solution of self-control conflicts (Inzlicht et al., many strategies can be effective (Bürgler et al., 2021; Hennecke &
2018). How much effort is expended probably depends on the Bürgler, 2020; Hennecke et al., 2019; Lopez et al., 2021;
difficulty of the self-control conflict (Brehm & Self, 1989), the Milyavskaya et al., 2020; Wenzel et al., 2022). People also fre-
importance of the current goal (Richter et al., 2016), self-efficacy quently use multiple strategies within a single self-control conflict.
(Bandura, 1994), and the reason that a goal is pursued for (autono- This so-called polyregulation, a concept which has been mainly
mous goals lead to higher effort, Milyavskaya et al., 2018). Effort studied in emotion regulation (Ford et al., 2019), appears to be
expenditure may, in addition, depend on how people try to resolve a important in self-control as well, as several studies have shown that
self-control conflict through interventive strategies: It has been using more strategies to deal with a single conflict is associated with
shown that when people with high levels of self-control are con- higher levels of success in resolving that conflict (Bürgler et al., 2021,
fronted with a temptation cue, they automatically activate cognitive 2022; Lopez et al., 2021; Milyavskaya et al., 2020).
representations of their goals and that this process does not demand If a person consciously tries to resolve a self-control conflict, the
cognitive resources (Fishbach et al., 2003). It can be debated and choice of their self-regulatory strategy could either be based on their
might be a question of nomenclature rather than of content whether planning before the conflict took place, or it could be made in the
cognitive regulation still counts as “metacognitive” if it is uncon- moment. As previously described, planning requires a person to be
sciously triggered and not consciously experienced. In many situa- able to correctly anticipate an upcoming self-control conflict and to
tions, however, people report on their regulatory attempts, thereby actively plan the use of one or more self-regulatory strategies with
clearly showing signs of self-control processes that are metacogni- which to resolve that conflict. Correctly anticipating an upcoming
tive and potentially effortful in nature. These regulatory attempts, self-control conflict might, however, not always be possible. In
utilizing the allotted resources, include the use of interventive those cases, a person has to decide on which self-regulatory strategy
strategies. to use in the moment. This decision would be made similar to the
Implementation of Interventive Strategies. The process-model process of planning, but, because it has to be made quickly and “on-
of self-control has distinguished various types of self-regulatory the-fly,” it might be more prone to biases due to stress, limited time,
strategies according to the stage of the impulse-generation cycle at and mental resources (Edland & Svenson, 1993; Keinan, 1987;
which the strategy applies (Duckworth et al., 2016). Strategies of Payne et al., 1988; Weenig & Maarleveld, 2002), error-prone
situation selection and situation modification can, as described heuristics might be used (Tversky & Kahneman, 1974) or behavior
before, already be used to avoid self-control conflicts but they in previous situations might just be repeated, without putting much
may also be used to leave or modify a situation in which a self- additional thought into it. This last-mentioned case would be
control conflict is already in progress. A person who is tempted by different from the habitual and automatic use of a strategy, because
continuing to eat crisps from a bowl in front of them, might, for here, the strategy choice would still be made consciously.
example, leave the room or remove the bowl of crisps. Alternatively, Monitoring Performance. Monitoring one’s performance
without making any changes to the situation, a person can, through should constitute another important part of metacognitive regulation
attentional deployment strategies, distract themselves from situa- during self-control conflicts (Inzlicht, Legault, & Teper, 2014). In
tional aspects that cause or amplify undesired impulses and direct self-regulated learning, monitoring has been described as “a per-
their attention to aspects of the situation that attenuate the conflict son’s tracking of specific aspects of his or her own performance, the
and promote its resolution. Looking away from the crisps and conditions that surround it, and the effects that it produces” (Zim-
focusing on the TV instead might help in the aforementioned merman & Campillo, p. 242, in reference to Zimmerman & Paulsen,
situation. Conversely, actively focusing the attention on goal-related 1995). In the goal literature, monitoring similarly refers to the
information, perhaps looking at the bowl of carrots instead of the observation of one’s goal progress, either on the process level
crisps, could support the goal of eating less unhealthy snacks, too (e.g., how much one has eaten) or the outcome level (e.g., how
(e.g., Ach, 1910; Kuhl, 1984, 1987). To deploy strategies for much weight one has lost) and the comparison of these with a
cognitive change, people can also try to alter the way they appraise desired standard (e.g., to lose 2 lbs; Harkin et al., 2016). Generally,
and mentally construe the situation to promote self-control. This monitoring is also at the heart of cybernetic control models which
could imply reappraising an object in a more negative or a “cooler,” assume that control can be attained by the orchestration of three
METACOGNITION AND SELF-CONTROL 1273

processes (a) goal setting, (b) monitoring for discrepancies between conflicts with a goal. Monitoring one’s experience should then
the goal and current behavior, and (c) the implementation of actions remain important as the self-control conflict evolves and is regulated.
that bring behavior closer to the goal (Carver & Scheier, 1982, 1990, Given that desire strength predicts conflict resolution (Hofmann
1998). Note that in our framework, we have introduced goal setting et al., 2012), monitoring desire strength is therefore crucial to detect
as a process in the forethought phase and the implementation of the need for potential strategic adaptations (see “Adaptations of
actions as regulation. So, we will focus here on what monitoring Strategies”).
during self-control conflicts may look like here. Whereas, there is a Finally, decisions about the use of specific strategies also require
vast literature showing that monitoring one’s behavior and goal an additional aspect of monitoring, namely, the monitoring of the
progress during goal pursuit more generally is a predictor of goal context in which a self-control conflict occurs. In self-regulated
attainment (Aittasalo et al., 2006; Amzil, 2014; Burke et al., 2011; learning, analyzing a learning situation (i.e., the nature of the
Butryn et al., 2007; Chang, 2010; Gleeson-Kreig, 2006; Mevarech, learning materials) is seen as important for effective learning and
1999; Mevarech & Kramarski, 2003; Patel et al., 2019; Razı & correct strategy choice (Brown et al., 1981). Similarly, context
Çubukçu, 2014; Turner-McGrievy et al., 2013), we are not aware of sensitivity (Bonanno & Burton, 2013) is a prerequisite for achieving
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

any research investigating monitoring one’s performance during strategy-situation-fit, a concept that has been discussed and investi-
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

self-control conflicts in particular. gated in emotion-regulation research and recently applied to self-
At this point, goals set in anticipation of specific self-control control where, depending on the situation, different demands might
conflicts (see “goal setting”) can be used as standards or reference be present (see e.g., Aldao et al., 2015; Bonanno & Burton, 2013;
values. Such specific goals (e.g., only eating 3 pieces of candy in Gan et al., 2007; Haines et al., 2016; Hennecke & Bürgler, 2020;
front of the TV) are more contextualized and potentially more Wenzel et al., 2020). Take, for example, the strategy task enrichment
helpful and informative for detecting discrepancies than the more which people can use to make tasks more enjoyable by adding some
abstract and decontextualized goals that originally motivated the kind of positive stimulus (Butler, 1998; Hennecke et al., 2019;
person to invest self-control effort (e.g., to lose weight). Monitoring Woolley & Fishbach, 2015). As described before, making an
should then involve the observation of potential discrepancies aversive task more pleasant by listening to one’s favorite music
between a current state, which defines what one is doing, and these may be helpful during a workout but not when studying for a
goals, that serve as standards or reference values, which define what difficult exam (Hennecke et al., 2019).
one should be doing in a given situation (Carver & Scheier, 1982). As a self-control conflict evolves and people’s behavior and
A person could, for example, notice that they are currently eating experience evolve through it, monitoring processes might give rise
candy in front of the TV and become aware that this is not what they to metacognitive feelings and judgments with regard to the self-
should do given that they have already missed their goal, the control conflict. This might include feelings of ease or difficulty of the
reference value, which was to eat only three pieces of candy. self-control conflict or confidence judgments about their own likeli-
One important aspect of monitoring one’s performance, as it is hood of eventually resolving the self-control conflict (see Efklides,
described for metacognition in self-regulated learning, which is 2001, 2006a, 2006b, for similar ideas in the domain of self-regulated
largely neglected in cybernetic control models, is that a person learning). Such judgments have also been discussed in the realm of
can not only monitor the discrepancy between the current state control theory, which proposes that people make expectancy-
and the goal, or the rate of discrepancy-reduction (e.g., Carver & assessment judgments (Carver & Scheier, 1982; Scheier & Carver,
Scheier, 1990), but make more nuanced observations about whether 1988) about whether or not discrepancy reduction between a current
the strategy they are using is effective toward reaching their goal and a desired state is likely to be successful. These predictive
(e.g., Cleary & Zimmerman, 2004; Ghatala, 1986; Zimmerman & judgments are also similar to the concept of self-efficacy (Bandura,
Paulsen, 1995). Monitoring may, if discrepancies are detected, then 1994) and related to aspects of decision-making, like expectancy-
lead to behavioral adaptations that we will discuss in more detail later value considerations (Atkinson, 1964; Eccles & Wigfield, 2002).
(see “Response to Feedback”). Generally, detecting discrepancies To conclude, monitoring processes are a crucial source of infor-
could lead the person to increase their effort for resolving the self- mation for the successful resolution of self-control conflicts and, in
control conflict (Carver & Scheier, 1982, 1998), to realize that a self- particular, detecting the need to adapt regulatory efforts, a process
regulatory strategy is inadequate and promote change to more that we will discuss in the next section.
suitable ones (see also Pressley & Ghatala, 1990), or cause self- Response to Feedback. Above, we have described that one
experimentation, whereby a person varies aspects of their behavior to important function of monitoring lies in the detection of discrepan-
solve a problem (see also Bandura, 1991). People who do not (yet) cies between a desired state and the current state and the adaptation of
know suitable self-regulatory strategies or how to best implement a one’s behavior to better match the desired state. Here, we want to
strategy can, through monitoring, learn what works or does not work further elaborate on various possible types of behavioral adaptations
in a given self-control conflict and thereby update their metacognitive (see also Bonanno & Burton, 2013; Garofalo & Lester, 1985;
knowledge about self-regulatory strategies. Pintrich, 2000; Pintrich et al., 2000; Schraw, 1998; Schunk, 2005;
Monitoring oneself during a self-control conflict may not only Zimmerman & Campillo, 2003; Zimmerman & Paulsen, 1995).
include monitoring one’s (overt) behavior (e.g., eating candy) but These behavioral adaptations are also a form of “regulation” but
also—and this may in fact be equally important—monitoring one’s they are responses to the feedback received through monitoring. We
experience. One highly important aspect of that experience is the focus on behavioral adaptations in three domains: adaptations in the
strength of the impulse or desire that conflicts with a goal and hence, allocation of resources, the adaptation of strategies, and goal revision.
causal for the self-control conflict. Accordingly, monitoring is also Allocation of Resources. While dealing with a self-control
crucial for identifying a self-control conflict in the first place, by conflict, a person can make changes to how much effort they put
allowing the person to realize that there is a current desire that into the successful resolution of that conflict, based on the information
1274 HENNECKE AND BÜRGLER

gathered from monitoring. This is different from the previously keep studying a bit longer than planned, or, as another example,
described allocation of resources during planning, where a person to increase the normal workout-routine by an additional 15 min,
estimates and plans how much effort they want to put into the therefore increasing their initial reference value (Carver & Scheier,
resolution of an upcoming conflict. During the conflict, however, a 1982, 1990). Second, the opposite case is also possible: A person
person may realize that certain circumstances make the resolution of might realize that they are not going to be able to finish their planned
the conflict much harder and that their current progress falls behind workout, maybe because their muscles start to hurt much sooner
their expectations, which might then lead them to invest more effort, than they normally do. Here, a person might decide that it’s okay to
also known as pushing (Fulford et al., 2010). The opposite is also cut the workout short by a few minutes, therefore, decreasing their
possible, where a person realizes that their progress exceeds ex- reference value (Carver & Scheier, 1982, 1990). Third, similar to
pectations, which could lead to a reduction in effort, also known as what is described as disengagement or withdrawal in control theory
coasting (Fulford et al., 2010). Furthermore, a person might experi- (Carver & Scheier, 1990), a person might feel that they are no longer
ence motivational shifts, where other goals (e.g., to enjoy the candy) capable of successfully resolving a self-control conflict (maybe
become more attractive (Inzlicht & Schmeichel, 2012; Inzlicht, because of difficulties or setbacks) and that any further invested
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Schmeichel, & Macrae, 2014) which could lead them to divert effort would be wasted (see motivational intensity theory; Brehm &
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

their effort away from the original goal. Self, 1989) or that they no longer want to resolve the self-control
Adaptation of Strategies. Monitoring may also suggest making conflict, maybe because their priorities have shifted (Inzlicht &
changes to currently used strategies (Bonanno & Burton, 2013; Schmeichel, 2012; Inzlicht, Schmeichel, & Macrae, 2014). Here,
Pintrich, 2000; Schunk, 2005). As previously mentioned, a person this person might completely abandon the goal of working out,
might realize, through monitoring, that their current strategy is not maybe just for that day, possibly for longer.
successful for resolving the self-control conflict at hand, either Goal revision may, depending on the type of self-control conflict
because it does not lead to the expected or desired outcome, or at hand, come in different shapes: In conflicts where a person wants
because the strategy requires a great deal of effort and is therefore to persist in doing something aversive (e.g., continuing to study
inefficient and not sustainable. There are various reasons why boring materials, continuing an effortful workout), one obvious
people might have to realize this: They may simply have lacked aspect that can be changed is how long a person persists in that task.
the necessary metacognitive knowledge about suitable strategies for In conflicts where a person wants to resist a temptation, simply
a given conflict or about their appropriate implementation, or they stretching out the duration of, for example, refraining from eating
may have failed to take potentially unforeseen aspects of the self- the candy for a little longer, could only delay failing the conflict if
control conflict or the situation into account when choosing which the desire for candy remains. Here, a person could also make
strategy to use. Upon realizing that the current strategy is not changes to the degree with which they want to resist (e.g., telling
effective, a person can then (a) cease using a strategy, (b) adjust oneself that eating some candy is allowed). For conflicts where a
a currently used strategy, or (c) select a new strategy (Bonanno & person wants to initiate an aversive task (e.g., getting off the couch
Burton, 2013). When experiencing the desire to stop running while for a workout), a person could make initiation easier by lowering
exercising on the treadmill, a person could, for example, (a) turn off their level of ambition or by changing aspects of the dreaded
the music that was supposed to make running on the treadmill more activity. Allowing oneself to only exercise for 30 min instead of
bearable, because it appears to not help much; (b) slightly adapt this the originally planned 45 min or to only do the kinds of exercises one
strategy of task enrichment and listen to an audiobook instead; or enjoys, might be helpful goal revisions if a self-control conflict
(c) completely change the strategy and start thinking about the cannot be resolved with the original goals in mind. Having described
positive consequences of continuing to working out. Alternatively, a the processes of metacognition before and during self-control
newly selected strategy might also be used concurrently with the conflicts, let us now turn to the processes that can unfold after a
previous strategy. A person could, for example, think about the self-control conflict occurred.
positive consequences of working out while listening to music or an
audiobook. This would indicate an attempt at polyregulation, the Metacognitive Processes After a Self-Control Conflict:
simultaneous use of multiple strategies in a single self-control
Reflection and Evaluation
conflict (Bürgler et al., 2021; Ford et al., 2019; Lopez et al.,
2021; Werner & Ford, 2021). Presumably, humans are motivated to analyze and understand their
Goal Revision. According to control theory, reference values own behavior (e.g., Heider, 1958; Jones & Davis, 1965). Accord-
do not have to remain where they are but can be adapted (Carver & ingly, people may look back and reflect on their behavior and
Scheier, 1982, 1990). Likewise, people may revise the goals that experiences during self-control conflicts. In the context of self-
serve as reference values and that they had set in anticipation of a regulated learning, a variety of possible targets of reflection have
self-control conflict. They may increase the reference value, been named including reflections on and an evaluation of one’s
decrease the reference value, or completely abandon the goal. First, success during the past learning episode, potentially with an addi-
while dealing with a self-control conflict, a person might realize that tional consideration of long-term consequences, as well as attribu-
they are making much better progress than expected, for example, tions about success and failure to various causes and including
they are able to stay focused on their studies for longer than usual. reflections on the suitability of the strategies that were used
This could be due to a strategy working better than expected (maybe (Pintrich, 2000, 2004; Winne & Hadwin, 2013; Zimmerman,
they are studying at the library and not at home), daily fluctuations in 2002). Similar reflections might help people to understand how
motivation (see, e.g., Bellhäuser et al., 2021; Navarro et al., 2013), they fared during a self-control conflict. At best, these reflections
or other circumstances. A person might then think to themselves, identify which adaptations should be made if a similar self-control
that they are capable of “going the extra mile” and, for example, conflict should occur again in the future (Winne & Hadwin, 2013).
METACOGNITION AND SELF-CONTROL 1275

Reflection and Evaluation of Short-Term Success and person (e.g., to having been seduced or supported by others), as
Long-Term Consequences. As described in our section on moni- controllable (e.g., one’s own effort) versus uncontrollable (e.g., the
toring, people may, to varying degrees, already observe and evaluate difficulty of a task), as temporally stable (e.g., a lack of trait self-
their performance during a self-control conflict. In addition, they may control) versus variable (e.g., a lack of effort; Weiner et al., 1971),
afterward, when the impulse is no longer felt, look back and evaluate and as global, that is, as caused by factors present in a wide range of
to what extent they were successful in resolving the self-control situations (e.g., one’s trait self-control) versus specific to the current
conflict. The extent to which a person considers themselves as situation (e.g., a specific strength or weakness; Abramson et al.,
successful or not likely depends on salient comparison standards 1978; Mikulincer, 1986). Some of these attributions may be more
(Carver & Scheier, 1990). One possible standard of comparison is problematic than others: Attributing failure to an internal, stable,
provided by the goals a person might or might not have set for the uncontrollable, and global cause would probably be the most prob-
self-control situation, for example, was the person successful in only lematic attribution, given that it could affect a person’s self-concept
having one drink at the party or in persisting on the treadmill for (or their metacognitive knowledge about their own self-control) more
20 min? As described before, how clearly one’s level of success permanently and lead to the inference that their self-control efforts
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

(or failure) can be quantified should also depend on the degree of will generally be futile (Abramson et al., 1978; Hong et al., 1999;
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

specificity of set goals (Locke & Latham, 2002). Even in hindsight, Sweeney et al., 1986). This could damage a person’s motivation and
attainment of the goal of “having only one drink” is easier to evaluate lower their levels of ambition or lead them to disengage from certain
than attainment of the goal of “not drinking much.” long-term goals (e.g., weight loss) altogether.
Alternatively, standards of comparison might be provided by the Other attributions of failure seem more adaptive: Seeing the
performance of other people (Festinger, 1954; Taylor et al., 1996; causes of failure as more specific and controllable, in a lack of
Zimmerman, 2002), especially if no personal goals for the situation effort or the use of an ineffective strategy, could have the person
had been set. Thinking that, for example, other party guests got draw the inference that more effort or a different strategy is
drunk as well or that other exercisers in one’s gym class did not try to necessary to succeed and motivate the person to be more engaged
be more persistent than oneself either, might make oneself feel better or try out a different strategy in future similar situations (see Dweck
in hindsight, whereas feeling like everyone acted in a very self- & Leggett, 1988; Zimmerman, 2002). Seeing failure as caused by a
controlled manner might cause dissatisfaction with one’s own specific and external cause (e.g., a person who always pressures
behavior. Such social comparisons have, in fact, been shown to other people into drinking too much alcohol at certain parties or the
play a powerful role in motivating people to push or coast in the smell of pastry at a favorite bakery) might lead the person to update
pursuit of their own goals (Diel et al., 2021). their conditional metacognitive knowledge about self-control and to
The evaluation of short-term success during a self-control conflict the inference that similar situations should be avoided in the future.
may also take into account the consequences of the outcome of the Specific attributions to an internal cause, like inferences about one’s
self-control conflict for the long-term goal. If the conflict was own strength or weaknesses, appear also less problematic, given that
resolved very successfully, the person may conclude that they they should not damage expectancies for all types of self-control
have gotten closer to their long-term goal. If their success has conflicts across the board but might also lead to the inference that
even exceeded expectations, the person may feel that they can certain situations should be avoided in the future. Finally, even
now reduce their effort and coast regarding the respective goal or internal and global attributions to a personal lack of self-control
pay more attention to the advancement of other important goals might not be problematic in the long-term, if people believe that
(“balancing”; Carver, 2003; Fishbach & Dhar, 2005). If the outcome their personality is malleable rather than fixed and become moti-
of the self-control conflict has been bad, however, the person may vated to work on improving their self-control (Chiu et al., 1997;
come to the conclusion that more effort has to be invested into the Dweck, 2008).
long-term goal and that compensatory measures (e.g., more exer- Inferences may also differ depending on whether people think
cise) have to be taken. Such evaluations might also cause people to about what their handling of the self-control conflict says about their
make changes regarding a long-term goal, for example, lead to own progress regarding the long-term goal or about their goal
changes in level of ambition or even to goal disengagement commitment (Fishbach & Dhar, 2005). Having done well can either
(Brandstätter & Schüler, 2013; Carver, 2003). We will elaborate indicate that one has made good progress and lead to a reduction of
on other possible inferences in more detail later. Let us first turn to effort or it can indicate that one is highly committed to the goal and
another crucial basis for such inferences, that is, causal attributions. lead to an increase of effort. Vice versa, not having done well can
Attributions and Inferences for Future Self-Control either indicate that one has made little progress and lead to an
Conflicts. After the self-control conflict, people may also try to increase of effort or it can indicate that one is not very committed to
identify the causes for them being more or less successful during the the goal and lead to a decrease of effort. Which inferences are made
conflict. In principle, various types of attributions are possible and might, for example, be affected by pre-existent commitment cer-
they may also come with different emotional reactions and infer- tainty (Fishbach et al., 2009). Similar inferences—that little progress
ences with effects on future behavior. results from little commitment and that large effort results from a lot
In the context of various types of outcomes, including learning of commitment to the goal—may also result from needs for effort
outcomes, research has distinguished between attributions to ability, justification or dissonance reduction and have long-lasting effects on
effort/engagement, task difficulty, or luck/lack of luck (e.g., future motivation (Aronson & Mills, 1959; Festinger, 1957)
Schuster et al., 1989). These attributions can be characterized Finally, emotional reactions to the self-control conflicts should
according to whether they identify the cause of a given outcome also vary based on different types of attributions (Weiner, 2010;
as internal, that is, as located within the person (e.g., one’s capacity Weiner et al., 1971). Both shame and pride, in the event of failure or
for self-control) versus as external, that is, as located outside of the success, respectively, occur when individuals attribute the outcome
1276 HENNECKE AND BÜRGLER

of the self-control conflict to an internal cause. If, however, in- task was associated with higher levels of evaluation, as task
dividuals feel that something external, for example, another person knowledge provided a standard against which participants could
has hindered or helped their self-regulatory efforts, they may evaluate their performance (Hogan, 1999).
respond with different emotions, for example, anger or gratitude, Whereas individual differences in self-awareness should affect the
respectively (Ortony et al., 1988). intensity by which people engage in metacognition for self-control,
their metacognitive knowledge should affect how well-informed they
engage in these processes. Both should matter for self-control
Discussion success. But the association between individual differences and
Mutual Relations Between Individual Differences in metacognitive regulatory processes is likely not a one-way-street
and metacognitive processes should feed back into metacognitive
Metacognition and Metacognitive Processes
knowledge and metacognitive awareness. First, individuals who have
As already proposed at various points in the description of the benefitted from monitoring could become inclined to monitor them-
framework, there should be many mutual pathways between the more selves more frequently and across more situations, thereby increasing
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

stable, trait-like aspects of metacognition, namely, metacognitive their trait-level of self-awareness. In addition, there should be various
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

knowledge and self-awareness, and the metacognitive processes, pathways from metacognitive regulation to metacognitive knowl-
during, and after a self-control conflict. We want to briefly highlight edge. Most importantly, self-monitoring should be an important
a couple of these pathways here: precondition for gaining and updating self-related knowledge
By definition of the trait-state-correspondence (Fleeson, 2001; (Higgins, 1996; Schoeneman, 1981; Schoeneman et al., 1984),
Fleeson & Jayawickreme, 2015), individuals who score high on the including metacognitive knowledge about self-control. The same
trait of self-awareness should also be more likely to self-monitor applies to evaluation and especially the inferences that follow from
during self-control conflicts and to reflect on and evaluate their attributions for self-control success and failure: They could lead to
actions afterward. Generally, our framework describes metacogni- the revision of knowledge about self-control “tasks” as being more or
tive processes as if they would commonly occur during self-control less difficult than expected, situational factors as being more or less
but it is, in fact, more likely that the extent to which people engage in important or harm- or helpful for self-control than expected, strate-
these processes is highly variable between and within persons and gies as being more or less successful than expected, or their own
that their levels of self-awareness could account for inter individual strength and weaknesses as being different than expected (see De
differences. Jager et al., 2005, for similar arguments in the literature on self-
In addition, individual differences in metacognitive knowledge regulated learning).
should also affect metacognitive processes in various ways (Sperling
et al., 2004). Baker (1989) went as far as to state that, in self-regulated Other Individual Differences and Metacognition in
learning, metacognitive knowledge is a prerequisite for metacogni-
Self-Control
tive regulation. We take a weaker position but would nevertheless
assume that metacognitive knowledge about self-control also affects It is likely that metacognition in self-control is also related to other
metacognitive regulation for self-control on various pathways: For individual differences which we have not integrated into our
example, metacognitive knowledge should help people to anticipate framework, given that they are not themselves metacognitive con-
future self-control conflicts. Knowing under which circumstances structs. We would nevertheless predict that the extent to which
one will feel tempted is likely a very important precondition for people deploy metacognition during self-control, given its presumed
the prevention of self-control conflicts through strategies of situation importance for self-control, should be correlated with a person’s
selection or modification (Duckworth et al., 2016; Kotabe & global self-control ability (Tangney et al., 2004, see also Grund &
Hofmann, 2015). In addition, what a person thinks to know about Carstens, 2019). Moreover, as a recent study has shown, metacog-
their own self-control, upcoming self-control conflicts, and their nition is correlated with the trait of conscientiousness (Bürgler et al.,
conditions, could, for example, have effects on the planned allocation 2022), one of the “Big Five” traits which entails self-control as a
of resources or plans to use specific strategies for self-control facet along with other aspects like people’s tendencies to be planful,
conflicts that cannot be avoided (Livingston, 2003; Nelson & to think ahead, be prepared, and aspire to excellence (Roberts et al.,
Narens, 1990). Additionally, a person should be more successful 2014). Conscientiousness is a highly important trait which predicts
if they set goals based on the knowledge of their own strengths and people’s academic success, life satisfaction, occupational perfor-
weaknesses with regard to that self-control conflict. For example, it mance, and wealth (Duckworth et al., 2012; Ivcevic & Brackett,
has been shown that people benefit from setting goals that are 2014; Wilmot & Ones, 2019). The clear overlap and empirical
demanding, but not too hard to accomplish (Locke & Latham, correlation between conscientiousness and metacognition in self-
1990, 2013). Setting goals in that “sweet spot” requires a certain control suggests that metacognition could be one of the processes
knowledge of one’s abilities, but also one’s shortcomings. through which conscientiousness “gets outside the skin” (among
Metacognitive knowledge could have an influence maybe not on others, including the use of effortful strategies, cognitive function-
the intensity but on the accuracy of both monitoring and evaluation, ing, persistence, and self-regulatory restraint, Corker et al., 2012;
as people with higher levels of metacognitive knowledge have also Hill et al., 2011; Wilmot & Ones, 2019). Therefore, correlations of
been shown to more accurately monitor their performance on a conscientiousness and metacognition could indicate that metacog-
reading task (Schraw, 1994). Knowledge of one’s own strengths and nition is another way through which people with higher levels of
weaknesses could provide useful reference points for monitoring conscientiousness achieve more positive life outcomes.
that are based on actual experience. Likewise, it was found in self- There is furthermore evidence suggesting that metacognition is,
regulated learning, that metacognitive knowledge about a certain at least to some degree, domain-general given that assessments of
METACOGNITION AND SELF-CONTROL 1277

metacognition in different domains including language, memory, have looked at isolated pieces of self-knowledge or theories about
and perception show substantial overlap (Mazancieux et al., 2020) self-control, for example, at the effects of willpower theories (Job
and that metacognition in different domains partially depends on the et al., 2010), people’s knowledge about the benefits of a high-level
same neural structures (Geurten et al., 2018; see also, e.g., de construal (MacGregor et al., 2017), or their metacognitive knowl-
Gardelle & Mamassian, 2014; Mazancieux et al., 2020; McCurdy edge about self-regulatory strategies (Bürgler et al., 2021). In
et al., 2013; Schraw et al., 1995; Shimamura, 2000). This implies addition, some research has attested to the importance of metacog-
that metacognition in self-control could also be related to metacog- nitive regulation for self-control, for example, research showing that
nition in other tasks and domains. As, however, self-control conflicts making plans on how to deal with upcoming self-control conflicts is
are probably more affectively charged than the cognitive problems helpful (Kroese et al., 2011; Van Koningsbruggen et al., 2011), that
in these other domains, there might nevertheless be aspects of implementing self-control strategies to avoid or deal with self-
metacognition that are unique to the domain of self-control. control conflicts is helpful (Duckworth et al., 2016; Ent et al.,
2015), and that monitoring oneself during a self-control conflict
(Bürgler et al., 2021) is helpful for self-control. Another study
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Limitations of the Framework


showed that metacognition, measured at the trait-level, predicted
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

The present framework has relevant limitations that require con- self-regulatory success (Bürgler et al., 2022). We believe that our
sideration. Most importantly, it should be understood as a heuristic framework should be helpful for organizing these and other findings
framework that probably oversimplifies matters, especially in its that, sometimes without explicitly acknowledging it, already looked
assumption of linearly progressing and clearly separable phases and at various metacognitive influences on self-control. Integrating them
processes. Even though we posit that distinguishing the phases as components of our framework might furthermore help to generate
before, during and after a self-control conflict and the point of predictions about their role in a larger compound of metacognitive
transition at which a self-control conflict is identified may be useful components relevant for self-control. Possible research questions
for organizational purposes and for integration of previous work, we pertaining to mutual pathways between these components and the
do not mean to imply that all phases of our framework have to occur components and self-control are manifold. Let us just highlight two
for any given self-control conflict and that individuals progress illustrative examples.
through the different phases in a linear way. People may, of course, One line of research zooming into the nuts and bolts of the
encounter self-control conflicts without having used conflict-related framework could look at one aspect of metacognitive knowledge,
metacognition before the conflict. Aside from simple “negligence,” namely, people’s self-knowledge about their own capacity for self-
this is likely the case whenever a self-control conflict emerges control and their specific strengths and weaknesses. For example, one
surprisingly. A person may also engage in preconflict metacognition could investigate the role of self-awareness and monitoring for the
without actually experiencing the conflict later. This might be the acquisition of such self-knowledge, the effects of causal attributions
case if an anticipated conflict never materializes either for external and adaptive inferences on the updating of such self-knowledge, or
reasons (e.g., the birthday party where one did not want to get too the effects of such self-knowledge on the extent to which individuals
drunk got cancelled) or because the person, due to preconflict correctly anticipate self-control conflicts, prevent them, or plan the
metacognition, effectively managed to avoid the conflict (e.g., use of strategies. For example, are people who are not well-calibrated
through situation selection, e.g., by cancelling their visit to the and overestimate their actual self-control “in the heat of the moment”
birthday party). In addition, postconflict metacognition occurs less likely to take precautionary measures and use preventive strate-
only when a conflict was experienced but even then does not gies to avoid or attenuate self-control conflicts? Are they less
have to occur. People may, of course, solve or fail at self-control successful in turn? And are people who underestimate their self-
conflicts without devoting much attention to evaluating and reflecting control or correctly identify it as relatively low more inclined to avoid
on their experience or performance later. Even within the phases of critical situations or to plan for unavoidable self-control conflicts?
our framework, no single process is necessary for a person to Are they more successful in turn? Generally, devoting more attention
experience and go through a self-control conflict. We posit, however, to the role of individual metacognitive knowledge about self-control
that engaging in these processes of metacognition before, during, and could advance our knowledge about the factors under which self-
after a self-control conflict should increase a person’s chance of control succeeds and fails (Scholer et al., 2018).
success. On a more abstract level, the relation between metacognition and
In addition, the proposed framework and our elaboration of it trait self-control would also be interesting to study: On the one hand,
contains many theoretical ideas for which empirical evidence is still metacognition might be an aspect of self-control and controlling for
lacking or only indirect, often taken from the literature on self- it statistically could reduce the effects of trait self-control on self-
regulated learning. We therefore propose, in the next section, some control performance. On the other hand, the relationship between
ideas on how our framework can inform future research on meta- metacognition and self-control capacity could be compensatory. It
cognition and self-control. could be that people who are “naturally” bad at self-control might
benefit even more from metacognitive knowledge and regulation.
How the Framework Can Inform Future Research Such a compensatory relationship has been observed for metacog-
nition in the context of self-regulated learning and intellectual
and Intervention Design
aptitude (Swanson, 1990).
As we have hopefully illustrated, metacognition should be an While these questions are basic in nature, they also inspire
important contributor to self-control, yet, we have relatively little thinking about possible applications. No matter whether metacog-
actual knowledge about the underlying processes. Some studies nition can compensate for a lack of self-control or increase people’s
1278 HENNECKE AND BÜRGLER

self-control as a component of it, thinking about interventions based To be effective, teaching metacognitive knowledge about self-
on metacognition could be fruitful for helping people get better at control should be accompanied by also promoting metacognitive
attaining their goals. How could such interventions focusing on regulation. As proposed in our framework, only if people anticipate
metacognition look like? and plan for potential self-control conflicts, identify them, monitor
In self-regulated learning, interventions aimed at increasing and adapt their behavior, and learn from success and failure through
metacognitive knowledge or metacognitive regulation have shown reflection, the application of metacognitive knowledge can be
promising results (see Dignath & Büttner, 2008, for a meta-analy- successful (see Livingston, 2003, for similar arguments about
sis). In our own, nonexhaustive literature research, we found a wide teaching study learning strategies to students). For interventions
variety of different interventions showing that training in metacog- aimed at planning, there is a rich field of research on implementation
nitive knowledge, planning, monitoring, and evaluation (most intentions that possible interventions could be based on (see e.g.,
interventions focused on multiple aspects of metacognition), had Gollwitzer, 1999; Gollwitzer & Brandstätter, 1997; Gollwitzer &
positive effects on various school-related performance outcomes Schaal, 1998). For metacognition in self-control, implementation
such as critical thinking (Gholami et al., 2016), reading (Amzil, intentions could, for example, specify the use of certain strategies.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

2014; Chan, 1996), writing (Briesmaster & Etchegaray, 2017), and More generally, implementation intentions could also be used to
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

performance in math or physics (Hohn & Frey, 2002; Mevarech & prompt people to monitor and evaluate their own resolution efforts
Kramarski, 2003; Montague et al., 2014; Tzohar-Rozen & of self-control conflicts to learn for future situations.
Kramarski, 2014; Verschaffel et al., 1999). One aspect that those Regarding interventions targeted at evaluation and reflection,
interventions have in common is that they are very time-consuming, exam wrappers are a possible approach (see e.g., Edlund, 2020;
often spanning multiple-training sessions embedded into the school Gezer-Templeton et al., 2017; Schendel, 2020; Soicher & Gurung,
curriculum, over weeks, sometimes months. Trainings of that nature 2017; Stephenson et al., 2017). In self-regulated learning, students
could be very hard to implement outside of a school context. are given exam wrappers, which are small handouts or “flyers,” after
Therefore, we want to highlight possible approaches to interventions an exam. Those exam wrappers contain a few questions that should
that are much less time-consuming and still provided encouraging prompt metacognitive evaluation and reflection of their learning
results in other fields of research. efforts regarding that exam. Such questions include “Approximately
Interventions could aim at communicating general knowledge how much time did you spend preparing for this exam?” and “…
name at least three things that you plan to do differently in preparing
about self-control, for example, by correcting the widely held but
for the next exam.” (Edlund, 2020, p. 160). Transferred to self-
presumably wrong belief that self-control is a limited and depletable
control, individuals could be asked to make causal attributions about
resource (Job et al., 2010). It also appears useful to aim at expanding
self-control conflicts they experienced in the recent past and draw
people’s “toolbox” of self-regulatory strategies (Fujita et al., 2020).
adaptive inferences for similar future conflicts. Such “self-control
In self-regulated learning, cognitive strategy instruction is already a
wrappers” could, in addition, contain prompts to promote adaptive
common instructional approach. It is aimed at enabling students to
inferences, for example, by asking people to identify controllable
become more strategic, flexible, and successful in their learning
causes for self-control failure (e.g., lack of effort or an ineffective
endeavors (Livingston, 2003; Scheid, 1993). The idea is that there
strategy) and to set goals and make plans for better dealing with
are concrete cognitive strategies that can be taught to students and in
similar self-control conflicts in the future. Of course, the distribution
fact, the approach has proven successful (Borkowski et al., 1987;
of “self-control wrappers” like these could be aided with technology
Garner, 1990). For example, a larger strategy repertoire does not as well, for example, through mobile apps and daily diaries delivered
only lead to better performance in self-regulated learning (Pressley online.
et al., 1987; Pressley & Harris, 2006; Schraw & Gutierrez, 2015)
but also to more adaptive emotion regulation (Lam & McBride-
Chang, 2007; Pearlin & Schooler, 1978), and, most relevant to our Conclusion
argument, to higher levels of success when dealing with daily self- Self-control can be difficult but appears to be the key to attaining
control conflicts (Bürgler et al., 2021). To supplement this declara- various important life outcomes, from achievement in school and at
tive strategy knowledge with conditional knowledge, interventions work to fulfilling social relationships and good health (de Ridder
could be individualized and, after careful observation, provide et al., 2012; Hofmann et al., 2014; Moffitt et al., 2011; Tangney
participants with feedback about the strategies that seem to work et al., 2004). However, the factors and processes that promote good
best for them and about the circumstances under which they work self-control are still not well-understood. Here, we have proposed a
for them. Generally, self-tracking technology could be a helpful tool framework that emphasizes the role of metacognition for self-control
for people to prompt self-awareness and produce knowledge about and explains how individual characteristics in metacognitive knowl-
the conditions of individual’s self-control success and failure edge about self-control and self-awareness as well as metacognitive
(Bergroth, 2019). More generally, daily diaries and smartphone processes should promote good self-control. We hope that it serves
apps that have been used to foster people’s self-monitoring of their (a) to illustrate that metacognition, including both individual differ-
long-term goal progress could be adapted to point people’s attention ences in metacognition as well as metacognitive processes before,
to the circumstances and their handling of self-control conflicts during, and after a self-control conflict should affect self-control
(Aittasalo et al., 2006; Burke et al., 2011; Butryn et al., 2007; through multiple pathways, (b) to not only integrate previous research
Gleeson-Kreig, 2006; Goodman et al., 2013; Patel et al., 2019; but also (c) to organize these effects and pathways in a comprehen-
Turner-McGrievy et al., 2013; Wang et al., 2018). With regular use, sive but parsimonious way, as well as serve (d) to stimulate and
people could, over time, become true experts with regard to their inform the generation of new predictions for basic and intervention
own self-control. research about the role of metacognition for self-control.
METACOGNITION AND SELF-CONTROL 1279

References Bandura, A. (1991). Self-regulation of motivation through anticipatory and


self-reactive mechanisms. In R. A. Dienstbier (Ed.), Perspectives on
Abramson, L. Y., Seligman, M. E., & Teasdale, J. D. (1978). Learned motivation: Nebraska symposium on motivation (Vol. 38, pp. 69–164).
helplessness in humans: Critique and reformulation. Journal of Abnormal University of Nebraska Press.
Psychology, 87(1), 49–74. https://doi.org/10.1037/0021-843X.87.1.49 Bandura, A. (1994). Self-efficacy. In V. S. Ramachaudran (Ed.), Encyclo-
Ach, N. (1910). Über den Willensakt und das temperament: Eine experi- pedia of human behavior (Vol. 4, pp. 71–81). Academic Press.
mentelle untersuchung [About act of will and temperament: An experi- Bargh, J. A., Gollwitzer, P. M., & Oettingen, G. (2010). Motivation. In S.
mental examination]. Quelle & Meyer. Fiske, D. T. Gilbert, & G. Lindzey (Eds.), Handbook of social psychology
Achtziger, A., Martiny, S. E., Oettingen, G., & Gollwitzer, P. M. (2012). (5th ed., Vol. 1, pp. 268–316). Wiley. https://doi.org/10.1002/97804705
Meta-cognitive processes in the self-regulation of goal pursuit. In P. Briñol 61119.socpsy001008
& K. DeMarree (Eds.), Social meta-cognition. Frontiers of social psychol- Baumeister, R. F. (1997). Esteem threat, self-regulatory breakdown, and
ogy (pp. 121–141). Psychology Press. emotional distress as factors in self-defeating behavior. Review of General
Adriaanse, M. A., Kroese, F. M., Gillebaart, M., & De Ridder, D. T. (2014). Psychology, 1(2), 145–174. https://doi.org/10.1037/1089-2680.1.2.145
Effortless inhibition: Habit mediates the relation between self-control and Baumeister, R. F., Bratslavsky, E., Muraven, M., & Tice, D. M. (1998). Ego
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

unhealthy snack consumption. Frontiers in Psychology, 5(444), Article depletion: Is the active self a limited resource? Journal of Personality and
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

444. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00444 Social Psychology, 74(5), 1252–1265. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514


Ainslie, G. (2021). Willpower with and without effort. Behavioral and Brain
.74.5.1252
Sciences, 44, Article e30. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X20000357
Baumeister, R. F., & Heatherton, T. F. (1996). Self-regulation failure: An
Aittasalo, M., Miilunpalo, S., Kukkonen-Harjula, K., & Pasanen, M. (2006).
overview. Psychological Inquiry, 7(1), 1–15. https://doi.org/10.1207/
A randomized intervention of physical activity promotion and patient self-
s15327965pli0701_1
monitoring in primary health care. Preventive Medicine, 42(1), 40–46.
Baumeister, R. F., Sparks, E. A., Stillman, T. F., & Vohs, K. D. (2008). Free
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ypmed.2005.10.003
will in consumer behavior: Self-control, ego depletion, and choice.
Alberts, H. J., Martijn, C., & De Vries, N. K. (2011). Fighting self-control
Journal of Consumer Psychology, 18(1), 4–13. https://doi.org/10.1016/j
failure: Overcoming ego depletion by increasing self-awareness. Journal
.jcps.2007.10.002
of Experimental Social Psychology, 47(1), 58–62. https://doi.org/10.1016/
Baumeister, R. F., & Vohs, K. D. (2007). Self-regulation, ego depletion, and
j.jesp.2010.08.004
motivation. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 1(1), 115–128.
Aldao, A., Sheppes, G., & Gross, J. J. (2015). Emotion regulation flexibility.
https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1751-9004.2007.00001.x
Cognitive Therapy and Research, 39(3), 263–278. https://doi.org/10
Baumeister, R. F., Vohs, K. D., & Tice, D. M. (2007). The strength model
.1007/s10608-014-9662-4
of self-control. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 16(6),
Aldridge, A. A., & Roesch, S. C. (2007). Coping and adjustment in children
351–355. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8721.2007.00534.x
with cancer: A meta-analytic study. Journal of Behavioral Medicine,
Beaman, A. L., Klentz, B., Diener, E., & Svanum, S. (1979). Self-awareness
30(2), 115–129. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10865-006-9087-y
and transgression in children: Two field studies. Journal of Personality
Allom, V., Panetta, G., Mullan, B., & Hagger, M. S. (2016). Self-report and
and Social Psychology, 37(10), 1835–1846. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-
behavioural approaches to the measurement of self-control: Are we
3514.37.10.1835
assessing the same construct? Personality and Individual Differences,
Bellhäuser, H., Mattes, B., & Liborius, P. (2021). Daily fluctuations in
90, 137–142. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2015.10.051
motivation. Zeitschrift für Entwicklungspsychologie und Pädagogische
Amzil, A. (2014). The effect of a metacognitive intervention on college
Psychologie, 51(4), 228–242. https://doi.org/10.1026/0049-8637/a000226
students’ reading performance and metacognitive skills. Journal of Edu-
Bergroth, H. (2019). “You can’t really control life”: Dis/assembling self-
cational and Developmental Psychology, 4(1), 27–45. https://doi.org/10
knowledge with self-tracking technologies. Distinktion: Journal of Social
.5539/jedp.v4n1p27
Ariely, D., & Wertenbroch, K. (2002). Procrastination, deadlines, and Theory, 20(2), 190–206. https://doi.org/10.1080/1600910X.2018.1551809
performance: Self-control by precommitment. Psychological Science, Berkman, E. T., Livingston, J. L., & Kahn, L. E. (2017). Finding the “self” in
13(3), 219–224. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9280.00441 self-regulation: The identity-value model. Psychological Inquiry, 28(2–3),
Aronson, E., & Mills, J. (1959). The effect of severity of initiation on liking 77–98. https://doi.org/10.1080/1047840X.2017.1323463
for a group. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 59(2), 177–181. Boekaerts, M. (1996). Self-Regulated learning at the junction of cognition
https://doi.org/10.1037/h0047195 and motivation. European Psychologist, 1(2), 100–112. https://doi.org/10
Aspinwall, L. G. (1998). Rethinking the role of positive affect in self- .1027/1016-9040.1.2.100
regulation. Motivation and Emotion, 22(1), 1–32. https://doi.org/10.1023/ Bonanno, G. A., & Burton, C. L. (2013). Regulatory flexibility: An
A:1023080224401 individual differences perspective on coping and emotion regulation.
Atance, C. M., & O’Neill, D. K. (2001). Episodic future thinking. Trends in Perspectives on Psychological Science, 8(6), 591–612. https://doi.org/
Cognitive Sciences, 5(12), 533–539. https://doi.org/10.1016/S1364- 10.1177/1745691613504116
6613(00)01804-0 Boorman, E. D., Behrens, T. E. J., Woolrich, M. W., & Rushworth, M. F. S.
Atance, C. M., & O’Neill, D. K. (2005). The emergence of episodic future (2009). How green is the grass on the other side? Frontopolar cortex
thinking in humans. Learning and Motivation, 36(2), 126–144. https:// and the evidence in favor of alternative courses of action. Neuron, 62(5),
doi.org/10.1016/j.lmot.2005.02.003 733–743. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2009.05.014
Atkinson, J. W. (1964). An introduction to motivation. Van Nostrand. Borkowski, J. G., Carr, M., & Pressley, M. (1987). “Spontaneous” strategy
Baker, L. (1989). Metacognition, comprehension monitoring, and the adult use: Perspectives from metacognitive theory. Intelligence, 11(1), 61–75.
reader. Educational Psychology Review, 1(1), 3–38. https://doi.org/10 https://doi.org/10.1016/0160-2896(87)90027-4
.1007/BF01326548 Boschin, E. A., Piekema, C., & Buckley, M. J. (2015). Essential functions of
Baker, L. (1991). Metacognition, reading and science education. In C. Santa primate frontopolar cortex in cognition. Proceedings of the National
& D. Alverman (Eds.), Science learning, processes and applications Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 112(9), E1020–
(pp. 351–371). International Reading Association. E1027. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1419649112
Bandura, A. (1976). Self-reinforcement: Theoretical and methodological Botvinick, M. M., Braver, T. S., Barch, D. M., Carter, C. S., & Cohen, J. D.
considerations. Behaviorism, 4(2), 135–155. https://www.jstor.org/stable/ (2001). Conflict monitoring and cognitive control. Psychological Review,
27758862 108(3), 624–652. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.108.3.624
1280 HENNECKE AND BÜRGLER

Botvinick, M. M., Cohen, J. D., & Carter, C. S. (2004). Conflict monitoring Carver, C. S., & Scheier, M. F. (1998). On the self-regulation of behavior.
and anterior cingulate cortex: An update. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, Cambridge University Press. https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9781139
8(12), 539–546. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2004.10.003 174794
Brandstätter, V., & Schüler, J. (2013). Action crisis and cost–benefit Carver, C. S., & Scheier, M. F. (2012). Cybernetic control processes and the
thinking: A cognitive analysis of a goal-disengagement phase. Journal self-regulation of behavior. In R. M. Ryan (Ed.), The Oxford handbook of
of Experimental Social Psychology, 49(3), 543–553. https://doi.org/10 human motivation (pp. 28–42). Oxford University Press.
.1016/j.jesp.2012.10.004 Chan, L. K. (1996). Combined strategy and attributional training for seventh
Braver, T. S. (2012). The variable nature of cognitive control: A dual grade average and poor readers. Journal of Research in Reading, 19(2),
mechanisms framework. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 16(2), 106–113. 111–127. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9817.1996.tb00092.x
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2011.12.010 Chang, M. M. (2010). Effects of self-monitoring on web-based language
Brehm, J. W., & Self, E. A. (1989). The intensity of motivation. Annual learner’s performance and motivation. CALICO Journal, 27(2), 298–310.
Review of Psychology, 40(1), 109–131. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev https://doi.org/10.11139/cj.27.2.298-310
.ps.40.020189.000545 Chiou, W. B., Wu, W. H., & Chang, M. H. (2013). Think abstractly, smoke
Briesmaster, M., & Etchegaray, P. (2017). Coherence and cohesion in EFL less: A brief construal-level intervention can promote self-control, leading
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

students’ writing production: The impact of a metacognition-based inter- to reduced cigarette consumption among current smokers. Addiction,
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

vention. Íkala, Revista de Lenguaje y Cultura, 22(2), 183–202. https:// 108(5), 985–992. https://doi.org/10.1111/add.12100
doi.org/10.17533/udea.ikala.v22n02a02 Chiu, C. Y., Hong, Y. Y., & Dweck, C. S. (1997). Lay dispositionism and
Briggs, S. R., Cheek, J. M., & Buss, A. H. (1980). An analysis of the self- implicit theories of personality. Journal of Personality and Social Psy-
monitoring scale. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 38(4), chology, 73(1), 19–30. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.73.1.19
679–686. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.38.4.679 Chun, H., Vanessa, M. P., & Deborah, J. M. (2007). Making prudent vs.
Briñol, P., & DeMarree, K. (2012). Social metacognition. Psychology Press. impulsive choices: The role of anticipated shame and guilt on consumer
https://doi.org/10.4324/9780203865989 self-control. In G. Fitzsimons & V. Morwitz (Eds.), Advances in consumer
Brown, A. L., Campione, J. C., & Day, J. D. (1981). Learning to learn: research (Vol. 34, pp. 715–719). Association for Consumer Research.
On training students to learn from texts. Educational Researcher, 10(2), Clarke, S. (2006). The relationship between safety climate and safety
14–21. https://doi.org/10.3102/0013189X010002014 performance: A meta-analytic review. Journal of Occupational Health
Brown, A. L., & Palincsar, A. S. (1982). Inducing strategic learning from texts Psychology, 11(4), 315–327. https://doi.org/10.1037/1076-8998.11.4.315
by means of informed, self-control training (Technical report No. 262). Clarkson, J. J., Otto, A. S., Hassey, R., & Hirt, E. R. (2016). Perceived mental
Buckholtz, J. W. (2015). Social norms, self-control, and the value of fatigue and self-control. In E. R. Hirt, J. J. Clarkson, & L. Jia (Eds.), Self-
antisocial behavior. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences, 3, 122–129. regulation and ego control (pp. 185–202). Academic Press. https://
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cobeha.2015.03.004 doi.org/10.1016/B978-0-12-801850-7.00010-X
Bürgler, S., Hoyle, R. H., & Hennecke, M. (2021). Flexibility in using self- Cleary, T. J., & Zimmerman, B. J. (2004). Self-regulation empowerment
regulatory strategies to manage self-control conflicts: The role of program: A school-based program to enhance self-regulated and self-
metacognitive knowledge, strategy repertoire, and feedback monitoring. motivated cycles of student learning. Psychology in the Schools, 41(5),
European Journal of Personality, 35(6), 861–880. https://doi.org/10 537–550. https://doi.org/10.1002/pits.10177
.1177/0890207021992907 Corker, K. S., Oswald, F. L., & Donnellan, M. B. (2012). Conscientiousness
Bürgler, S., Kleinke, K., & Hennecke, M. (2022). The metacognition in in the classroom: A process explanation. Journal of Personality, 80(4),
self-control scale (MISCS). Personality and Individual Differences, 199, 995–1028. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-6494.2011.00750.x
Article 111841. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2022.111841 Crockett, M. J., Braams, B. R., Clark, L., Tobler, P. N., Robbins, T. W., &
Burke, L. E., Wang, J., & Sevick, M. A. (2011). Self-monitoring in weight Kalenscher, T. (2013). Restricting temptations: Neural mechanisms
loss: A systematic review of the literature. Journal of the American Dietetic of precommitment. Neuron, 79(2), 391–401. https://doi.org/10.1016/j
Association, 111(1), 92–102. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jada.2010.10.008 .neuron.2013.05.028
Butler, D. (1998). The strategic content learning approach to promoting self- de Gardelle, V., & Mamassian, P. (2014). Does confidence use a common
regulated learning: A report of three studies. Journal of Educational currency across two visual tasks? Psychological Science, 25(6), 1286–
Psychology, 90(4), 682–697. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.90.4.682 1288. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797614528956
Butryn, M. L., Phelan, S., Hill, J. O., & Wing, R. R. (2007). Consistent self- De Jager, B., Jansen, M., & Reezigt, G. (2005). The development of
monitoring of weight: A key component of successful weight loss mainte- metacognition in primary school learning environments. School Effective-
nance. Obesity, 15(12), 3091–3096. https://doi.org/10.1038/oby.2007.368 ness and School Improvement, 16(2), 179–196. https://doi.org/10.1080/
Carver, C. (2003). Pleasure as a sign you can attend to something else: 09243450500114181
Placing positive feelings within a general model of affect. Cognition and de Ridder, D. T., Lensvelt-Mulders, G., Finkenauer, C., Stok, F. M., &
Emotion, 17(2), 241–261. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699930302294 Baumeister, R. F. (2012). Taking stock of self-control: A meta-analysis of
Carver, C. S. (2019). Personality. In E. J. Finkel & R. F. Baumeister (Eds.), how trait self-control relates to a wide range of behaviors. Personality and
Advanced social psychology: The state of the science (2nd ed., pp. 471– Social Psychology Review, 16(1), 76–99. https://doi.org/10.1177/10888
498). Oxford University Press. 68311418749
Carver, C. S., & Scheier, M. F. (1981). The self-attention-induced feedback DeYoung, C. G. (2015). Cybernetic big five theory. Journal of Research in
loop and social facilitation. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, Personality, 56, 33–58. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jrp.2014.07.004
17(6), 545–568. https://doi.org/10.1016/0022-1031(81)90039-1 Dias, R., Robbins, T. W., & Roberts, A. C. (1997). Dissociable forms of
Carver, C. S., & Scheier, M. F. (1982). Control theory: A useful conceptual inhibitory control within prefrontal cortex with an analog of the Wisconsin
framework for personality-social, clinical, and health psychology. Psy- Card Sort Test: Restriction to novel situations and independence from “on-
chological Bulletin, 92(1), 111–135. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909 line” processing. The Journal of Neuroscience, 17(23), 9285–9297.
.92.1.111 https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.17-23-09285.1997
Carver, C. S., & Scheier, M. F. (1990). Origins and functions of positive Diel, K., Grelle, S., & Hofmann, W. (2021). A motivational framework of
and negative affect: A control-process view. Psychological Review, 97(1), social comparison. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 120(6),
19–35. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.97.1.19 1415–1430. https://doi.org/10.1037/pspa0000204
METACOGNITION AND SELF-CONTROL 1281

Dignath, C., & Büttner, G. (2008). Components of fostering self-regulated Festinger, L. (1954). A theory of social comparison processes. Human
learning among students. A meta-analysis on intervention studies at Relations, 7(2), 117–140. https://doi.org/10.1177/001872675400700202
primary and secondary school level. Metacognition and Learning, 3(3), Festinger, L. (1957). A theory of cognitive dissonance (Vol. 2). Stanford
231–264. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11409-008-9029-x University Press. https://doi.org/10.1515/9781503620766
Dimmitt, C., & McCormick, C. B. (2012). Metacognition in education. In Fishbach, A., & Converse, B. A. (2010). Identifying and battling temptation.
K. R. Harris, S. Graham, T. Urdan, C. B. McCormick, G. M. Sinatra, & J. In K. D. Vohs & R. F. Baumeister (Eds.), Handbook of self-regulation:
Sweller (Eds.), APA educational psychology handbook: Vol. 1. Theories, Research, theory, and applications (pp. 244–260). Guilford Press.
constructs, and critical issues (pp. 157–187). American Psychological Fishbach, A., & Dhar, R. (2005). Goals as excuses or guides: The liberating
Association. https://doi.org/10.1037/13273-007 effect of perceived goal progress on choice. The Journal of Consumer
Duckworth, A. L., Gendler, T. S., & Gross, J. J. (2014). Self-control in Research, 32(3), 370–377. https://doi.org/10.1086/497548
school-age children. Educational Psychologist, 49(3), 199–217. https:// Fishbach, A., & Ferguson, M. J. (2007). The goal construct in social
doi.org/10.1080/00461520.2014.926225 psychology. In A. W. Kruglanski & E. T. Higgins (Eds.), Social psychol-
Duckworth, A. L., Gendler, T. S., & Gross, J. J. (2016). Situational strategies ogy: Handbook of basic principles (pp. 490–515). Guilford Press.
for self-control. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 11(1), 35–55. Fishbach, A., Friedman, R. S., & Kruglanski, A. W. (2003). Leading us not
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691615623247 unto temptation: Momentary allurements elicit overriding goal activation.


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Duckworth, A. L., Weir, D., Tsukayama, E., & Kwok, D. (2012). Who does Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84(2), 296–309. https://
well in life? Conscientious adults excel in both objective and subjective doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.84.2.296
success. Frontiers in Psychology, 3, Article 356. https://doi.org/10.3389/ Fishbach, A., & Labroo, A. A. (2007). Be better or be merry: How mood
fpsyg.2012.00356 affects self-control. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 93(2),
Dunlosky, J., & Bjork, R. A. (2013). Handbook of metamemory and memory. 158–173. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.93.2.158
Psychology Press. https://doi.org/10.4324/9780203805503 Fishbach, A., Zhang, Y., & Koo, M. (2009). The dynamics of self-regulation.
Dunlosky, J., & Metcalfe, J. (2008). Metacognition. SAGE Publications. European Review of Social Psychology, 20(1), 315–344. https://doi.org/10
Duval, S., & Wicklund, R. A. (1972). A theory of objective self-awareness. .1080/10463280903275375
Academic Press. Flavell, J. H. (1977). The development of knowledge about visual percep-
Dweck, C. S. (2008). Can personality be changed? The role of beliefs in tion. Nebraska Symposium on Motivation, 25, 43–76.
personality and change. Current Directions in Psychological Science, Flavell, J. H. (1979). Metacognition and cognitive monitoring: A new area
17(6), 391–394. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8721.2008.00612.x of cognitive-developmental inquiry. American Psychologist, 34(10), 906–
Dweck, C. S., & Leggett, E. L. (1988). A social-cognitive approach to 911. https://doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.34.10.906
motivation and personality. Psychological Review, 95(2), 256–273. Fleeson, W. (2001). Toward a structure- and process-integrated view of
https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.95.2.256 personality: Traits as density distribution of states. Journal of Personality
Eccles, J. S., & Wigfield, A. (2002). Motivational beliefs, values, and goals. and Social Psychology, 80(6), 1011–1027. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-
Annual Review of Psychology, 53(1), 109–132. https://doi.org/10.1146/ 3514.80.6.1011
annurev.psych.53.100901.135153 Fleeson, W., & Jayawickreme, E. (2015). Whole trait theory. Journal of
Edland, A., & Svenson, O. (1993). Judgment and decision making under Research in Personality, 56, 82–92. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jrp.2014.10.009
time pressure. In A. Edland & O. Svenson (Eds.), Time pressure and stress Fleming, S. M., & Dolan, R. J. (2012). The neural basis of metacognitive
in human judgment and decision making (pp. 27–40). Springer. https:// ability. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London, Series
doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4757-6846-6_2 B: Biological Sciences, 367(1594), 1338–1349. https://doi.org/10.1098/
Edlund, J. E. (2020). Exam wrappers in psychology. Teaching of Psychol- rstb.2011.0417
ogy, 47(2), 156–161. https://doi.org/10.1177/0098628320901385 Ford, B. Q., Gross, J. J., & Gruber, J. (2019). Broadening our field of view:
Efklides, A. (2001). Metacognitive experiences in problem solving. In A. The role of emotion polyregulation. Emotion Review, 11(3), 197–208.
Efklides, J. Kuhl, & R. M. Sorrentino (Eds.), Trends and prospects in https://doi.org/10.1177/1754073919850314
motivation research (pp. 297–323). Springer. https://doi.org/10.1007/0- Förster, J., & Liberman, N. (2007). Knowledge activation. In A. W. Kru-
306-47676-2 glanski & E. T. Higgins (Eds.), Social psychology: Handbook of basic
Efklides, A. (2006a). Metacognition and affect: What can metacognitive principles (pp. 201–231). Guilford Press.
experiences tell us about the learning process? Educational Research Friedman, N. P., & Miyake, A. (2004). The relations among inhibition and
Review, 1(1), 3–14. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.edurev.2005.11.001 interference control functions: A latent-variable analysis. Journal of
Efklides, A. (2006b). Metacognitive experiences: The missing link in the Experimental Psychology: General, 133(1), 101–135. https://doi.org/10
self-regulated learning process. Educational Psychology Review, 18(3), .1037/0096-3445.133.1.101
287–291. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10648-006-9021-4 Friese, M., Loschelder, D. D., Gieseler, K., Frankenbach, J., & Inzlicht, M.
Efklides, A. (2011). Interactions of metacognition with motivation and affect (2019). Is ego depletion real? An analysis of arguments. Personality and
in self-regulated learning: The MASRL model. Educational Psychologist, Social Psychology Review, 23(2), 107–131. https://doi.org/10.1177/10888
46(1), 6–25. https://doi.org/10.1080/00461520.2011.538645 68318762183
Eisenberger, R. (1992). Learned industriousness. Psychological Review, Fujita, K., & Carnevale, J. J. (2012). Transcending temptation through
99(2), 248–267. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.99.2.248 abstraction: The role of construal level in self-control. Current Directions
Ent, M. R., Baumeister, R. F., & Tice, D. M. (2015). Trait self-control and the in Psychological Science, 21(4), 248–252. https://doi.org/10.1177/09637
avoidance of temptation. Personality and Individual Differences, 74, 12– 21412449169
15. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2014.09.031 Fujita, K., Orvell, A., & Kross, E. (2020). Smarter, not harder: A toolbox
Eriksen, B. A., & Eriksen, C. W. (1974). Effects of noise letters upon the approach to enhancing self-control. Policy Insights from the Behavioral
identification of a target letter in a nonsearch task. Perception & Psycho- and Brain Sciences, 7(2), 149–156. https://doi.org/10.1177/2372732
physics, 16(1), 143–149. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03203267 220941242
Fenigstein, A., Scheier, M. F., & Buss, A. H. (1975). Public and private self- Fujita, K., Trope, Y., Liberman, N., & Levin-Sagi, M. (2006). Construal
consciousness: Assessment and theory. Journal of Consulting and Clinical levels and self-control. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
Psychology, 43(4), 522–527. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0076760 90(3), 351–367. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.90.3.351
1282 HENNECKE AND BÜRGLER

Fulford, D., Johnson, S. L., Llabre, M. M., & Carver, C. S. (2010). Pushing Gollwitzer, P. M., & Schaal, B. (1998). Metacognition in action: The
and coasting in dynamic goal pursuit: Coasting is attenuated in bipolar importance of implementation intentions. Personality and Social Psychol-
disorder. Psychological Science, 21(7), 1021–1027. https://doi.org/10 ogy Review, 2(2), 124–136. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327957pspr0202_5
.1177/0956797610373372 Goodman, J. D., McKay, J. R., & De Philippis, D. (2013). Progress
Funder, D. C. (2001). Personality. Annual Review of Psychology, 52(1), monitoring in mental health and addiction treatment: A means of improv-
197–221. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.52.1.197 ing care. Professional Psychology, Research and Practice, 44(4), 231–
Gan, Y., Shang, J., & Zhang, Y. (2007). Coping flexibility and locus of 246. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0032605
control as predictors of burnout among Chinese college students. Social Grant, A. M., Franklin, J., & Langford, P. (2002). The self-reflection and
Behavior and Personality, 35(8), 1087–1098. https://doi.org/10.2224/sbp insight scale: A new measure of private self-consciousness. Social Behav-
.2007.35.8.1087 ior and Personality, 30(8), 821–835. https://doi.org/10.2224/sbp.2002.30
Garavan, H., Ross, T. J., & Stein, E. A. (1999). Right hemispheric dominance .8.821
of inhibitory control: An event-related functional MRI study. Proceedings Grund, A., & Carstens, C. A. (2019). Self-control motivationally recon-
of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, sidered: “Acting” self-controlled is different to “being good” at self-
96(14), 8301–8306. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.96.14.8301 control. Motivation and Emotion, 43(1), 63–81. https://doi.org/10.1007/
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Garner, R. (1990). When children and adults do not use learning strategies: s11031-018-9721-3
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Toward a theory of settings. Review of Educational Research, 60(4), Grunschel, C., Schwinger, M., Steinmayr, R., & Fries, S. (2016). Effects of
517–529. https://doi.org/10.3102/00346543060004517 using motivational regulation strategies on students’ academic procrasti-
Garofalo, J., & Lester, F. K. (1985). Metacognition, cognitive monitoring, nation, academic performance, and well-being. Learning and Individual
and mathematical performance. Journal for Research in Mathematics Differences, 49, 162–170. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lindif.2016.06.008
Education, 16(3), 163–176. https://doi.org/10.2307/748391 Hacker, D. J., Dunlosky, J., & Graesser, A. C. (Eds.). (2009). Handbook of
Gehring, W. J., Goss, B., Coles, M. G. H., Meyer, D. E., & Donchin, E. metacognition in education. Routledge. https://doi.org/10.4324/9780203
(1993). A neural system for error detection and compensation. Psycho- 876428
logical Science, 4(6), 385–390. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.1993 Hagger, M. S., Chatzisarantis, N. L. D., Alberts, H., Anggono, C. O.,
.tb00586.x Batailler, C., Birt, A. R., Brand, R., Brandt, M. J., Brewer, G., Bruyneel,
Geurten, M., Meulemans, T., & Lemaire, P. (2018). From domain-specific to S., Calvillo, D. P., Campbell, W. K., Cannon, P. R., Carlucci, M., Carruth,
domain-general? The developmental path of metacognition for strategy N. P., Cheung, T., Crowell, A., De Ridder, D. T. D., Dewitte, S., …
selection. Cognitive Development, 48, 62–81. https://doi.org/10.1016/j Zwienenberg, M. (2016). A multilab preregistered replication of the ego-
.cogdev.2018.08.002 depletion effect. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 11(4), 546–573.
Gezer-Templeton, P. G., Mayhew, E. J., Korte, D. S., & Schmidt, S. J. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691616652873
(2017). Use of exam wrappers to enhance students’ metacognitive skills in Haines, S. J., Gleeson, J., Kuppens, P., Hollenstein, T., Ciarrochi, J.,
a large introductory food science and human nutrition course. Journal of Labuschagne, I., Grace, C., & Koval, P. (2016). The wisdom to know
Food Science Education, 16(1), 28–36. https://doi.org/10.1111/1541- the difference: Emotion regulation strategy-situation fit in daily life is
4329.12103 associated with well-being. Psychological Science, 27(12), 1651–1659.
Ghatala, E. S. (1986). Strategy-monitoring training enables young learners https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797616669086
to select effective strategies. Educational Psychologist, 21(1–2), 43–54. Hall, P. A., Fong, G. T., Epp, L. J., & Elias, L. J. (2008). Executive function
https://doi.org/10.1080/00461520.1986.9653023 moderates the intention-behavior link for physical activity and dietary
Gholami, M., Moghadam, P. K., Mohammadipoor, F., Tarahi, M. J., Sak, behavior. Psychology & Health, 23(3), 309–326. https://doi.org/10.1080/
M., Toulabi, T., & Pour, A. H. H. (2016). Comparing the effects of 14768320701212099
problem-based learning and the traditional lecture method on critical Hare, T. A., Camerer, C. F., & Rangel, A. (2009). Self-control in decision-
thinking skills and metacognitive awareness in nursing students in a making involves modulation of the vmPFC valuation system. Science,
critical care nursing course. Nurse Education Today, 45, 16–21. https:// 324(5927), 646–648. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1168450
doi.org/10.1016/j.nedt.2016.06.007 Harkin, B., Webb, T. L., Chang, B. P. I., Prestwich, A., Conner, M., Kellar,
Gilbert, D. T., & Wilson, T. D. (2007). Prospection: Experiencing the I., Benn, Y., & Sheeran, P. (2016). Does monitoring goal progress promote
future. Science, 317(5843), 1351–1354. https://doi.org/10.1126/science goal attainment? A meta-analysis of the experimental evidence. Psycho-
.1144161 logical Bulletin, 142(2), 198–229. https://doi.org/10.1037/bul0000025
Gillebaart, M., & de Ridder, D. T. (2015). Effortless self-control: A novel Harnishfeger, K. (1995). Development of cognitive inhibition. In F. Dempster
perspective on response conflict strategies in trait self-control. Social and & C. Brainerd (Eds.), Interference and inhibition in cognition (pp. 175–
Personality Psychology Compass, 9(2), 88–99. https://doi.org/10.1111/ 204). Academic. https://doi.org/10.1016/B978-012208930-5/50007-6
spc3.12160 Haws, K., Lamberton, C., Dzhogleva, H., & Fitzsimons, G. (2011). A life in
Gleeson-Kreig, J. M. (2006). Self-monitoring of physical activity: balance or a slippery slope? Exploring the use and effectiveness of
Effects on self-efficacy and behavior in people with type 2 diabetes. moderation versus avoidance self-control strategies. In R. Ahluwalia,
The Diabetes Educator, 32(1), 69–77. https://doi.org/10.1177/0145721 T. L. Chartrand, & R. K. Ratner (Eds.), Advances in consumer research
705284285 (pp. 27–28). Association for Consumer Research.
Goldfried, M. R., & Merbaum, M. (1973). Behavior change through self- Heatherton, T. F., & Wagner, D. D. (2011). Cognitive neuroscience of self-
control. Holt, Rinehart & Winston. regulation failure. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 15(3), 132–139. https://
Gollwitzer, P. M. (1999). Implementation intentions: Strong effects of simple doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2010.12.005
plans. American Psychologist, 54(7), 493–503. https://doi.org/10.1037/ Heckhausen, H., & Gollwitzer, P. (1986). Information processing before
0003-066X.54.7.493 and after the formation of an intent. In F. Klix & H. Hagendorf (Eds.), In
Gollwitzer, P. M., & Brandstätter, V. (1997). Implementation intentions and memoriam Hermann Ebbinghaus: Symposium on the structure and
effective goal pursuit. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, function of human memory (pp. 1071–1082). Elsevier/North-Holland
73(1), 186–199. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.73.1.186 Biomedical Press.
Gollwitzer, P. M., & Oettingen, G. (1998). The emergence and implemen- Heckhausen, H., & Gollwitzer, P. M. (1987). Thought contents and cognitive
tation of health goals. Psychology & Health, 13(4), 687–715. https:// functioning in motivational versus volitional states of mind. Motivation
doi.org/10.1080/08870449808407424 and Emotion, 11(2), 101–120. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00992338
METACOGNITION AND SELF-CONTROL 1283

Hedge, C., Powell, G., & Sumner, P. (2018). The reliability paradox: Why Inzlicht, M., Schmeichel, B. J., & Macrae, C. N. (2014). Why self-control
robust cognitive tasks do not produce reliable individual differences. seems (but may not be) limited. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 18(3),
Behavior Research Methods, 50(3), 1166–1186. https://doi.org/10 127–133. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2013.12.009
.3758/s13428-017-0935-1 Inzlicht, M., Shenhav, A., & Olivola, C. Y. (2018). The effort paradox: Effort
Heider, F. (1958). The naive analysis of action. In F. Heider (Ed.), The is both costly and valued. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 22(4), 337–349.
psychology of interpersonal relations (pp. 101–124). Wiley. https:// https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2018.01.007
doi.org/10.1037/10628-004 Inzlicht, M., Werner, K. M., Briskin, J. L., & Roberts, B. W. (2021).
Hennecke, M., & Bürgler, S. (2020). Many roads lead to Rome: Self- Integrating models of self-regulation. Annual Review of Psychology,
regulatory strategies and their effects on self-control. Social and Personal- 72(1), 319–345. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-061020-105721
ity Psychology Compass, 14(6), 1–16. https://doi.org/10.1111/spc3.12530 Ivcevic, Z., & Brackett, M. (2014). Predicting school success: Comparing
Hennecke, M., Czikmantori, T., & Brandstätter, V. (2019). Doing despite conscientiousness, grit, and emotion regulation ability. Journal of
disliking: Self-regulatory strategies in everyday aversive activities. Research in Personality, 52, 29–36. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jrp.2014
European Journal of Personality, 33(1), 104–128. https://doi.org/10 .06.005
.1002/per.2182 Jia, L., Yuen, W. L., Ong, Q., & Theseira, W. E. (2022). Pitfalls of self-
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Hennecke, M., & Freund, A. M. (2014). Identifying success on the process reported measures of self-control: Surprising insights from extreme debt-
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

level reduces negative effects of prior weight loss on subsequent weight ors. Journal of Personality. Advance online publication. https://doi.org/10
loss during a low-calorie diet. Applied Psychology. Health and Well- .1111/jopy.12733
Being, 6(1), 48–66. https://doi.org/10.1111/aphw.12021 Job, V., Bernecker, K., & Dweck, C. S. (2012). Are implicit motives the
Higgins, E. T. (1996). The “self digest”: Self-knowledge serving self- need to feel certain affect? Motive-affect congruence predicts relation-
regulatory functions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, ship satisfaction. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 38(12),
71(6), 1062–1083. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.71.6.1062 1552–1565. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167212454920
Hill, P. L., Turiano, N. A., Hurd, M. D., Mroczek, D. K., & Roberts, B. W. Job, V., Dweck, C. S., & Walton, G. M. (2010). Ego depletion—Is it all in
(2011). Conscientiousness and longevity: An examination of possible your head? implicit theories about willpower affect self-regulation. Psy-
mediators. Health Psychology, 30(5), 536–541. https://doi.org/10.1037/ chological Science, 21(11), 1686–1693. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797
a0023859 610384745
Hofmann, W., Baumeister, R. F., Förster, G., & Vohs, K. D. (2012). Job, V., Walton, G. M., Bernecker, K., & Dweck, C. S. (2015). Implicit
Everyday temptations: An experience sampling study of desire, conflict, theories about willpower predict self-regulation and grades in everyday
and self-control. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 102(6), life. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 108(4), 637–647.
1318–1335. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0026545 https://doi.org/10.1037/pspp0000014
Hofmann, W., & Fisher, R. R. (2012). How guilt and pride shape subsequent Jones, E. E., & Davis, K. E. (1965). From acts to dispositions the attribution
self-control. Social Psychological & Personality Science, 3(6), 682–690. process in person perception. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in experi-
https://doi.org/10.1177/1948550611435136 mental social psychology (vol. 2, pp. 219–266). Academic Press. https://
Hofmann, W., & Kotabe, H. (2012). A general model of preventive and doi.org/10.1016/S0065-2601(08)60107-0
interventive self-control. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, Jost, J. T., Kruglanski, A. W., & Nelson, T. O. (1998). Social metacognition:
6(10), 707–722. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1751-9004.2012.00461.x An expansionist review. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 2(2),
Hofmann, W., Luhmann, M., Fisher, R. R., Vohs, K. D., & Baumeister, R. F. 137–154. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327957pspr0202_6
(2014). Yes, but are they happy? Effects of trait self-control on affective Kavanagh, D. J., Andrade, J., & May, J. (2005). Imaginary relish and
well-being and life satisfaction. Journal of Personality, 82(4), 265–277. exquisite torture: The elaborated intrusion theory of desire. Psychological
https://doi.org/10.1111/jopy.12050 Review, 112(2), 446–467. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.112.2.446
Hogan, K. (1999). Thinking aloud together: A test of an intervention to foster Keinan, G. (1987). Decision making under stress: Scanning of alternatives
students’ collaborative scientific reasoning. Journal of Research in Sci- under controllable and uncontrollable threats. Journal of Personality and
ence Teaching, 36(10), 1085–1109. https://doi.org/10.1002/(SICI)1098- Social Psychology, 52(3), 639–644. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.52
2736(199912)36:10<1085::AID-TEA3>3.0.CO;2-D .3.639
Hohn, R. L., & Frey, B. (2002). Heuristic training and performance in Kivetz, R., & Zheng, Y. (2006). Determinants of justification and self-
elementary mathematical problem solving. Journal of Educational control. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 135(4), 572–587.
Research, 95(6), 374–380. https://doi.org/10.1080/00220670209596612 https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-3445.135.4.572
Hong, Y. Y., Chiu, C. Y., Dweck, C. S., Lin, D. M. S., & Wan, W. (1999). Klohe-Lehman, D. M., Freeland-Graves, J., Anderson, E. R., McDowell,
Implicit theories, attributions, and coping: A meaning system approach. T., Clarke, K. K., Hanss-Nuss, H., Cai, G., Puri, D., & Milani, T. J.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 77(3), 588–599. https:// (2006). Nutrition knowledge is associated with greater weight loss in
doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.77.3.588 obese and overweight low-income mothers. Journal of the American
Hull, C. L. (1943). Principles of behavior: An introduction to behavior Dietetic Association, 106(1), 65–75. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jada.2005
theory. Appleton-Century. .09.047
Inzlicht, M., & Legault, L. (2014). No pain, no gain: How distress underlies Konishi, S., Nakajima, K., Uchida, I., Sekihara, K., & Miyashita, Y. (1998).
effective self-control (and unites diverse social psychological phenom- No-go dominant brain activity in human inferior prefrontal cortex revealed
ena). In J. P. Forgas & E. Harmon-Jones (Eds.), Motivation and its by functional magnetic resonance imaging. European Journal of Neuro-
regulation: The control within (pp. 115–132). Psychology Press. science, 10(3), 1209–1213. https://doi.org/10.1046/j.1460-9568.1998
Inzlicht, M., Legault, L., & Teper, R. (2014). Exploring the mechanisms of .00167.x
self-control improvement. Current Directions in Psychological Science, Kool, W., McGuire, J. T., Rosen, Z. B., & Botvinick, M. M. (2010). Decision
23(4), 302–307. https://doi.org/10.1177/0963721414534256 making and the avoidance of cognitive demand. Journal of Experimental
Inzlicht, M., & Schmeichel, B. J. (2012). What is ego depletion? Toward a Psychology: General, 139(4), 665–682. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0020198
mechanistic revision of the resource model of self-control. Perspectives on Kotabe, H. P., & Hofmann, W. (2015). On integrating the components of
Psychological Science, 7(5), 450–463. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691 self-control. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 10(5), 618–638.
612454134 https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691615593382
1284 HENNECKE AND BÜRGLER

Kreibich, A., Hennecke, M., & Brandstätter, V. (2020). The effect of self- patterns: An experience sampling study in college-aged females.
awareness on the identification of goal-related obstacles. European Jour- PsyArXiv. https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/73cuh
nal of Personality, 34(2), 215–233. https://doi.org/10.1002/per.2234 Lurquin, J. H., & Miyake, A. (2017). Challenges to ego-depletion research
Kroese, F. M., Adriaanse, M. A., Evers, C., & De Ridder, D. T. (2011). go beyond the replication crisis: A need for tackling the conceptual crisis.
“Instant success”: Turning temptations into cues for goal-directed behav- Frontiers in Psychology, 8(568), Article 568. https://doi.org/10.3389/
ior. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 37(10), 1389–1397. fpsyg.2017.00568
https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167211410889 MacGregor, K. E., Carnevale, J. J., Dusthimer, N. E., & Fujita, K. (2017).
Krönke, K. M., Wolff, M., Mohr, H., Kräplin, A., Smolka, M. N., Bühringer, Knowledge of the self-control benefits of high-level versus low-level
G., & Goschke, T. (2018). Monitor yourself! Deficient error-related brain construal. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 112(4), 607–
activity predicts real-life self-control failures. Cognitive, Affective & Behav- 620. https://doi.org/10.1037/pspp0000130
ioral Neuroscience, 18(4), 622–637. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13415-018- Mazancieux, A., Fleming, S. M., Souchay, C., & Moulin, C. J. A. (2020).
0593-5 Is there a G factor for metacognition? Correlations in retrospective
Krönke, K. M., Wolff, M., Shi, Y., Kräplin, A., Smolka, M. N., Bühringer, G., metacognitive sensitivity across tasks. Journal of Experimental Psy-
& Goschke, T. (2020). Functional connectivity in a triple-network saliency chology: General, 149(9), 1788–1799. https://doi.org/10.1037/xge
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

model is associated with real-life self-control. Neuropsychologia, 149, 0000746


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Article 107667. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2020.107667 McCurdy, L. Y., Maniscalco, B., Metcalfe, J., Liu, K. Y., de Lange, F. P.,
Kruglanski, A. W., Shah, J. Y., Fishbach, A., & Friedman, R. (2002). A & Lau, H. (2013). Anatomical coupling between distinct metacognitive
theory of goal systems. In M. P. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in experimental systems for memory and visual perception. The Journal of Neurosci-
social psychology (pp. 331–378). Academic Press. https://doi.org/10 ence, 33(5), 1897–1906. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1890-
.1016/S0065-2601(02)80008-9 12.2013
Kuhl, J. (1984). Volitional aspects of achievement motivation and learned McRae, K., Ciesielski, B., & Gross, J. J. (2012). Unpacking cognitive
helplessness: Toward a comprehensive theory of action control. Progress reappraisal: Goals, tactics, and outcomes. Emotion, 12(2), 250–255.
in Experimental Personality Research, 13, 99–171. https://doi.org/10 https://doi.org/10.1037/a0026351
.1016/B978-0-12-541413-5.50007-3 Menon, V., & D’Esposito, M. (2022). The role of PFC networks in cognitive
Kuhl, J. (1987). Action control: The maintenance of motivational states. control and executive function. Neuropsychopharmacology, 47(1),
In F. Halisch & J. Kuhl (Eds.), Motivation, intention, and volition 90–103. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41386-021-01152-w
(pp. 279–291). Springer. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-642-70967-8_19 Metcalfe, J., & Kornell, N. (2007). Principles of cognitive science
Kurth-Nelson, Z., & Redish, A. D. (2012). Don’t let me do that! – Models of in education: The effects of generation, errors, and feedback. Psycho-
precommitment. Frontiers in Neuroscience, 6, Article 138. https://doi.org/ nomic Bulletin & Review, 14(2), 225–229. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03
10.3389/fnins.2012.00138 194056
Lam, C. B., & McBride-Chang, C. A. (2007). Resilience in young adulthood: Metcalfe, J., & Mischel, W. (1999). A hot/cool-system analysis of delay
The moderating influences of gender-related personality traits and coping of gratification: Dynamics of willpower. Psychological Review, 106(1),
flexibility. Sex Roles, 56(3–4), 159–172. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11199- 3–19. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.106.1.3
006-9159-z Mevarech, Z. R. (1999). Effects of metacognitive training embedded in
Lawrence, A. J., Luty, J., Bogdan, N. A., Sahakian, B. J., & Clark, L. (2009). cooperative settings on mathematical problem solving. Journal of Educa-
Impulsivity and response inhibition in alcohol dependence and problem tional Research, 92(4), 195–205. https://doi.org/10.1080/0022067990
gambling. Psychopharmacology, 207(1), 163–172. https://doi.org/10 9597597
.1007/s00213-009-1645-x Mevarech, Z. R., & Kramarski, B. (2003). The effects of metacognitive
Leary, M., & Terry, M. (2012). Hypo-egoic mindsets. In M. Leary & J. P. training versus worked-out examples on students’ mathematical reason-
Tangney (Eds.), Handbook of self and identity (pp. 268–288). Guilford ing. The British Journal of Educational Psychology, 73(4), 449–471.
Press. https://doi.org/10.1348/000709903322591181
Leith, K. P., & Baumeister, R. F. (1996). Why do bad moods increase self- Miele, D. B., & Scholer, A. A. (2016). Self-regulation of motivation. In K. R.
defeating behavior? Emotion, risk taking, and self-regulation. Journal of Wentzel & D. B. Miele (Eds.), Handbook of motivation at school (2nd ed.,
Personality and Social Psychology, 71(6), 1250–1267. https://doi.org/10 pp. 363–384). Routledge.
.1037/0022-3514.71.6.1250 Miele, D. B., Scholer, A. A., & Fujita, K. (2020). Metamotivation: Emerging
Leventhal, H., Singer, R., & Jones, S. (1965). Effects of fear and specificity research on the regulation of motivational states. Advances in Motivation
of recommendation upon attitudes and behavior. Journal of Personality Science, 7, 1–42. https://doi.org/10.1016/bs.adms.2019.10.001
and Social Psychology, 2(1), 20–29. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0022089 Mikulincer, M. (1986). Attributional processes in the learned helplessness
Livingston, J. A. (2003). Metacognition: An overview. Retrieved August 11 paradigm: Behavioral effects of global attributions. Journal of Personality
2022, from https://files.eric.ed.gov/fulltext/ED474273.pdf and Social Psychology, 51(6), 1248–1256. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-
Locke, E. A., & Latham, G. P. (1990). A theory of goal setting and task 3514.51.6.1248
performance. Prentice Hall. Milkman, K. L., Rogers, T., & Bazerman, M. H. (2008). Harnessing our
Locke, E. A., & Latham, G. P. (2002). Building a practically useful theory of inner angels and demons: What we have learned about want/should
goal setting and task motivation. A 35-year odyssey. American Psycholo- conflicts and how that knowledge can help us reduce short-sighted
gist, 57(9), 705–717. https://doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.57.9.705 decision making. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 3(4), 324–
Locke, E. A., & Latham, G. P. (2006). New directions in goal-setting theory. 338. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1745-6924.2008.00083.x
Current Directions in Psychological Science, 15(5), 265–268. https:// Milyavskaya, M., Saffran, M., Hope, N., & Koestner, R. (2018). Fear of
doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8721.2006.00449.x missing out: Prevalence, dynamics, and consequences of experiencing
Locke, E. A., & Latham, G. P. (Eds.). (2013). New developments in goal FOMO. Motivation and Emotion, 42(5), 725–737. https://doi.org/10
setting and task performance. Routledge. https://doi.org/10.4324/97802 .1007/s11031-018-9683-5
03082744 Milyavskaya, M., Saunders, B., & Inzlicht, M. (2020). Self-control in daily
Lopez, R., Cosme, D., Werner, K. M., Saunders, B., & Hofmann, W. (2021). life: Prevalence and effectiveness of diverse self-control strategies. Jour-
Associations between use of self-regulatory strategies and daily eating nal of Personality, 89(4), 634–651. https://doi.org/10.1111/jopy.12604
METACOGNITION AND SELF-CONTROL 1285

Mischel, H. N., & Mischel, W. (1983). The development of children’s memory. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 64(3), 233–246.
knowledge of self-control strategies. Child Development, 54(3), 603–619. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2006.09.009
https://doi.org/10.2307/1130047 Orbell, S., & Sheeran, P. (2000). Motivational and volitional processes in
Mischel, W., Shoda, Y., & Rodriguez, M. I. (1989). Delay of gratification in action initiation: A field study of the role of implementation intentions.
children. Science, 244(4907), 933–938. https://doi.org/10.1126/science Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 30(4), 780–797. https://doi.org/10
.2658056 .1111/j.1559-1816.2000.tb02823.x
Moffitt, T. E., Arseneault, L., Belsky, D., Dickson, N., Hancox, R. J., Ortony, A., Clore, G. L., & Collins, A. (1988). The cognitive structure of
Harrington, H., Houts, R., Poulton, R., Roberts, B. W., Ross, S., Sears, emotions. Cambridge University Press. https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO978
M. R., Thomson, W. M., & Caspi, A. (2011). A gradient of childhood self- 0511571299
control predicts health, wealth, and public safety. Proceedings of the Overmeyer, R., Berghäuser, J., Dieterich, R., Wolff, M., Goschke, T., &
National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 108(7), Endrass, T. (2021). The error-related negativity predicts self-control fail-
2693–2698. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1010076108 ures in daily life. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 14, Article 614979.
Montague, M., Krawec, J., Enders, C., & Dietz, S. (2014). The effects of https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2020.614979
cognitive strategy instruction on math problem solving of middle-school Ozer, D. J., & Benet-Martínez, V. (2006). Personality and the prediction of
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

students of varying ability. Journal of Educational Psychology, 106(2), consequential outcomes. Annual Review of Psychology, 57(1), 401–421.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

469–481. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0035176 https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.57.102904.190127


Mukhopadhyay, A., & Johar, G. V. (2005). Where there is a will, is there a Panadero, E. (2017). A review of self-regulated learning: Six models and
way? Effects of lay theories of self-control on setting and keeping resolu- four directions for research. Frontiers in Psychology, 8(422), Article 422.
tions. The Journal of Consumer Research, 31(4), 779–786. https://doi.org/ https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00422
10.1086/426611 Patel, M. L., Hopkins, C. M., Brooks, T. L., & Bennett, G. G. (2019).
Muraven, M., Shmueli, D., & Burkley, E. (2006). Conserving self-control Comparing self-monitoring strategies for weight loss in a smartphone app:
strength. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 91(3), 524–537. Randomized controlled trial. JMIR mHealth and uHealth, 7(2), Article
https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.91.3.524 e12209. https://doi.org/10.2196/12209
Murayama, K., Kitagami, S., Tanaka, A., & Raw, J. A. L. (2016). People’s Payne, J. W., Bettman, J. R., & Johnson, E. J. (1988). Adaptive strategy
naiveté about how extrinsic rewards influence intrinsic motivation. selection in decision making. Journal of Experimental Psychology:
Motivation Science, 2(3), 138–142. https://doi.org/10.1037/mot000 Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 14(3), 534–552. https://doi.org/10
0040 .1037/0278-7393.14.3.534
Myrseth, K. O. R., & Fishbach, A. (2009). Self-control: A function of Pearlin, L. I., & Schooler, C. (1978). The structure of coping. Journal
knowing when and how to exercise restraint. Current Directions in of Health and Social Behavior, 19(1), 2–21. https://doi.org/10.2307/
Psychological Science, 18(4), 247–252. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467- 2136319
8721.2009.01645.x Peters, J., & Büchel, C. (2011). The neural mechanisms of inter-temporal
Navarro, J., Curioso, F., Gomes, D., Arrieta, C., & Cortés, M. (2013). decision-making: Understanding variability. Trends in Cognitive
Fluctuations in work motivation: Tasks do not matter! Nonlinear Dynam- Sciences, 15(5), 227–239. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2011.03.002
ics Psychology and Life Sciences, 17(1), 3–22. Petty, R. E., Briñol, P., Tormala, Z. L., & Wegener, D. T. (2007). The role of
Neal, A., Ballard, T., & Vancouver, J. B. (2017). Dynamic self-regulation metacognition in social judgment. In A. W. Kruglanski & E. T. Higgins
and multiple-goal pursuit. Annual Review of Organizational Psychology (Eds.), Social psychology: Handbook of basic principles (pp. 254–284).
and Organizational Behavior, 4(1), 401–423. https://doi.org/10.1146/ The Guilford Press.
annurev-orgpsych-032516-113156 Piaget, J. (1970). Piaget’s theory. In P. H. Mussen (Ed.), Carmichael’s
Nęcka, E., Gruszka, A., Orzechowski, J., Nowak, M., & Wójcik, N. (2018). manual of child psychology (3rd ed., Vol. 1, pp. 703–732). Wiley.
The (in) significance of executive functions for the trait of self-control: A Pintrich, P. R. (2000). The role of goal orientation in self-regulated learning.
psychometric study. Frontiers in Psychology, 9(1139), Article 1139. In M. Boekaerts, P. R. Pintrich, & M. Zeidner (Eds.), Handbook of self-
https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2018.01139 regulation (pp. 451–502). Academic Press. https://doi.org/10.1016/B978-
Nelson, T. O., & Narens, L. (1990). Metamemory: A theoretical framework 012109890-2/50043-3
and new findings. In T. O. Nelson & L. Narens (Eds.), Psychology of Pintrich, P. R. (2004). A conceptual framework for assessing motivation and
learning and motivation (Vol. 26, pp. 125–173). Academic Press. https:// self-regulated learning in college students. Educational Psychology
doi.org/10.1016/S0079-7421(08)60053-5 Review, 16(4), 385–407. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10648-004-0006-x
Nguyen, T., Carnevale, J. J., Scholer, A. A., Miele, D. B., & Fujita, K. Pintrich, P. R., Wolters, C., & Baxter, G. (2000). Assessing metacognition
(2019). Metamotivational knowledge of the role of high-level and low- and self-regulated learning. In G. Schraw & J. Impara (Eds.), Issues in the
level construal in goal-relevant task performance. Journal of Personality measurement of metacognition (pp. 43–97). Buros Institute of Mental
and Social Psychology, 117(5), 876–899. https://doi.org/10.1037/pspa Measurement.
0000166 Pressley, M., Borkowski, J. G., & Schneider, W. (1987). Cognitive strate-
Nikolova, H., Lamberton, C., & Haws, K. L. (2016). Haunts or helps from gies: Good strategy users coordinate metacognition and knowledge.
the past: Understanding the effect of recall on current self-control. Journal Annals of Child Development, 4, 89–129.
of Consumer Psychology, 26(2), 245–256. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jcps Pressley, M., & Ghatala, E. S. (1990). Self-regulated learning: Monitoring
.2015.06.011 learning from text. Educational Psychologist, 25(1), 19–33. https://
Okuda, J., Fujii, T., Ohtake, H., Tsukiura, T., Tanji, K., Suzuki, K., doi.org/10.1207/s15326985ep2501_3
Kawashima, R., Fukuda, H., Itoh, M., & Yamadori, A. (2003). Thinking Pressley, M., & Harris, K. R. (2006). Cognitive strategy instruction: From
of the future and past: The roles of the frontal pole and the medial temporal basic research to classroom instruction. In P. A. Alexander & P. H. Winne
lobes. NeuroImage, 19(4), 1369–1380. https://doi.org/10.1016/S1053- (Eds.), Handbook of educational psychology (2nd ed., pp. 265–286).
8119(03)00179-4 Academic Press.
Okuda, J., Fujii, T., Ohtake, H., Tsukiura, T., Yamadori, A., Frith, C. D., & Price, M., Lee, M., & Higgs, S. (2013). Impulsivity, eating behaviour and
Burgess, P. W. (2007). Differential involvement of regions of rostral performance on a delay discounting task. Appetite, 71, Article 483. https://
prefrontal cortex (Brodmann area 10) in time- and event-based prospective doi.org/10.1016/j.appet.2013.06.053
1286 HENNECKE AND BÜRGLER

Puustinen, M., & Pulkkinen, L. (2001). Models of self-regulated learning: A Schoeneman, T. J., Tabor, L. E., & Nash, D. L. (1984). Children’s reports of
review. Scandinavian Journal of Educational Research, 45(3), 269–286. the sources of self-knowledge. Journal of Personality, 52(2), 124–137.
https://doi.org/10.1080/00313830120074206 https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-6494.1984.tb00348.x
Razı, S., & Çubukçu, F. (2014). Metacognition and reading: Investigating Scholer, A. A., Miele, D. B., Murayama, K., & Fujita, K. (2018). New
intervention and comprehension of EFL freshmen in Turkey. Procedia: directions in self-regulation: The role of metamotivational beliefs. Current
Social and Behavioral Sciences, 158, 288–295. https://doi.org/10.1016/j Directions in Psychological Science, 27(6), 437–442. https://doi.org/10
.sbspro.2014.12.090 .1177/0963721418790549
Richter, M., Gendolla, G. H., & Wright, R. A. (2016). Three decades of Schraw, G. (1994). The effect of metacognitive knowledge on local
research on motivational intensity theory: What we have learned about and global monitoring. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 19(2),
effort and what we still don’t know. Advances in Motivation Science, 3, 143–154. https://doi.org/10.1006/ceps.1994.1013
149–186. https://doi.org/10.1016/bs.adms.2016.02.001 Schraw, G. (1998). Promoting general metacognitive awareness. Instructional
Roberts, B. W., Jackson, J. J., Fayard, J. V., Edmonds, G., & Meints, J. Science, 26(1), 113–125. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1003044231033
(2009). Conscientiousness. In M. Leary & R. Hoyle (Eds.), Handbook of Schraw, G., & Dennison, R. S. (1994). Assessing metacognitive awareness.
individual differences in social behavior (pp. 369–381). Guilford Press. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 19(4), 460–475. https://doi.org/
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Roberts, B. W., Kuncel, N. R., Shiner, R., Caspi, A., & Goldberg, L. R. 10.1006/ceps.1994.1033
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

(2007). The power of personality: The comparative validity of personality Schraw, G., Dunkle, M. E., Bendixen, L. D., & Roedel, T. D. (1995). Does a
traits, socioeconomic status, and cognitive ability for predicting important general monitoring skill exist? Journal of Educational Psychology, 87(3),
life outcomes. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 2(4), 313–345. 433–444. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.87.3.433
https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1745-6916.2007.00047.x Schraw, G., & Gutierrez, A. P. (2015). Metacognitive strategy instruction
Roberts, B. W., Lejuez, C., Krueger, R. F., Richards, J. M., & Hill, P. L. that highlights the role of monitoring and control processes. In A. Pena-
(2014). What is conscientiousness and how can it be assessed? Devel- Ayala (Ed.), Metacognition: Fundaments, applications, and trends
opmental Psychology, 50(5), 1315–1330. https://doi.org/10.1037/a003 (pp. 3–16). Springer. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-11062-2_1
1109 Schraw, G., & Moshman, D. (1995). Metacognitive theories. Educational
Saunders, B., & Inzlicht, M. (2016). Vigour and fatigue: How variation in Psychology Review, 7(4), 351–371. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF02212307
affect underlies effective self-control. In T. S. Braver (Ed.), Motivation Schunk, D. H. (2005). Self-regulated learning: The educational legacy of
and cognitive control (pp. 211–234). Routledge/Taylor & Francis Group. Paul R. Pintrich. Educational Psychologist, 40(2), 85–94. https://doi.org/
Saunders, B., Milyavskaya, M., Etz, A., Randles, D., & Inzlicht, M. (2018). 10.1207/s15326985ep4002_3
Reported self-control is not meaningfully associated with inhibition- Schuster, B., Forsterlung, F., & Weiner, B. (1989). Perceiving the causes of
related executive function: A bayesian analysis. Collabra. Psychology, success and failure: A cross-cultural examination of attributional concepts.
4(39), 1–16. https://doi.org/10.1525/collabra.134 Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 20(2), 191–213. https://doi.org/10
Saunders, B., Milyavskaya, M., & Inzlicht, M. (2022). Longitudinal evi- .1177/0022022189202005
dence that Event Related Potential measures of self-regulation do not Schwarz, N. (2015). Metacognition. In M. Mikulincer, P. R. Shaver, E.
predict everyday goal pursuit. Nature Communications, 13(1), Article Borgida, & J. A. Bargh (Eds.), APA handbook of personality and social
3201. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41467-022-30786-7 psychology, Vol. 1. Attitudes and social cognition (pp. 203–229).
Schacter, D. L., & Addis, D. R. (2007). The cognitive neuroscience of American Psychological Association. https://doi.org/10.1037/14341-006
constructive memory: Remembering the past and imagining the future. Shamosh, N. A., Deyoung, C. G., Green, A. E., Reis, D. L., Johnson, M. R.,
Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London, Series B: Conway, A. R. A., Engle, R. W., Braver, T. S., & Gray, J. R. (2008).
Biological Sciences, 362(1481), 773–786. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb Individual differences in delay discounting: Relation to intelligence,
.2007.2087 working memory, and anterior prefrontal cortex. Psychological Science,
Schacter, D. L., Benoit, R. G., & Szpunar, K. K. (2017). Episodic future 19(9), 904–911. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02175.x
thinking: Mechanisms and functions. Current Opinion in Behavioral Shimamura, A. P. (1995). Memory and the prefrontal cortex. In J. Grafman,
Sciences, 17, 41–50. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cobeha.2017.06.002 K. J. Holyoak, & F. Boller (Eds.), Structure and functions of the human
Scheid, K. (1993). Helping students become strategic learners. Guidelines prefrontal cortex (pp. 151–159). New York Academy of Sciences.
for teaching. Cognitive strategy training series. Brookline Books. Shimamura, A. P. (2000). Toward a cognitive neuroscience of metacogni-
Scheier, M. F., & Carver, C. S. (1988). A model of behavioral self- tion. Consciousness and Cognition, 9(2), 313–323. https://doi.org/10
regulation: Translating intention into action. In M. F. Scheier & C. S. .1006/ccog.2000.0450
Carver (Eds.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 21, Sklar, A., Rim, S., & Fujita, K. (2017). Proactive and reactive self-control. In
pp. 303–346). Academic Press. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0065- D. de Ridder, M. Adriaanse, & K. Fujita (Eds.), The Routledge interna-
2601(08)60230-0 tional handbook of self-control in health and well-being (pp. 24–34).
Schendel, S. (2020). What you don’t know (can hurt you): Using exam Routledge. https://doi.org/10.4324/9781315648576-3
wrappers to foster self-assessment skills in law students. Pace Law Soicher, R. N., & Gurung, R. A. (2017). Do exam wrappers increase
Review, 40, 154–233. https://ssrn.com/abstract=3441382 metacognition and performance? A single course intervention. Psychology
Scherbaum, S., Frisch, S., Holfert, A. M., O’Hora, D., & Dshemuchadse, M. Learning & Teaching, 16(1), 64–73. https://doi.org/10.1177/147572571
(2018). No evidence for common processes of cognitive control and self- 6661872
control. Acta Psychologica, 182, 194–199. https://doi.org/10.1016/j Soutschek, A., Ugazio, G., Crockett, M. J., Ruff, C. C., Kalenscher, T., &
.actpsy.2017.11.018 Tobler, P. N. (2017). Binding oneself to the mast: Stimulating frontopolar
Schmitz, B., Landmann, M., & Perels, F. (2007). Das Selbstregulationspro- cortex enhances precommitment. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuro-
zessmodell und theoretische Implikationen [The self-regulation process science, 12(4), 635–642. https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsw176
model and theoretical implications]. In M. Landmann & B. Schmitz (Eds.), Sperling, R. A., Howard, B. C., Staley, R., & DuBois, N. (2004). Metacog-
Selbstregulation erfolgreich fördern. Praxisnahe Trainingsprogramme nition and self-regulated learning constructs. Educational Research and
für effektives Lernen (pp. 312–326). Kohlhammer. Evaluation, 10(2), 117–139. https://doi.org/10.1076/edre.10.2.117.27905
Schoeneman, T. J. (1981). Reports of the sources of self-knowledge. Journal Stephenson, B., Craig, M., Zingaro, D., Horton, D., Heap, D., & Huynh, E.
of Personality, 49(3), 284–294. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-6494.1981 (2017, March). Exam wrappers: Not a silver bullet. In Proceedings of the
.tb00937.x 2017 ACM SIGCSE technical symposium on computer science education
METACOGNITION AND SELF-CONTROL 1287

(pp. 573–578). Association for Computing Machinery. https://doi.org/10 Wang, M. C., Haertel, G. D., & Walberg, H. J. (1990). What influences
.1145/3017680.3017701 learning? A content analysis of review literature. Journal of Educational
Swanson, H. L. (1990). Influence of metacognitive knowledge and aptitude Research, 84(1), 30–43. https://doi.org/10.1080/00220671.1990.10885988
on problem solving. Journal of Educational Psychology, 82(2), 306–314. Waxman, S. E. (2009). A systematic review of impulsivity in eating
https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.82.2.306 disorders. European Eating Disorders Review, 17(6), 408–425. https://
Sweeney, P. D., Anderson, K., & Bailey, S. (1986). Attributional style in doi.org/10.1002/erv.952
depression: A meta-analytic review. Journal of Personality and Social Weenig, M. W., & Maarleveld, M. (2002). The impact of time constraint on
Psychology, 50(5), 974–991. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.50.5.974 information search strategies in complex choice tasks. Journal of Economic
Tangney, J. P., Baumeister, R. F., & Boone, A. L. (2004). High self-control Psychology, 23(6), 689–702. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0167-4870(02)
predicts good adjustment, less pathology, better grades, and interpersonal 00134-4
success. Journal of Personality, 72(2), 271–324. https://doi.org/10.1111/j Weil, L. G., Fleming, S. M., Dumontheil, I., Kilford, E. J., Weil, R. S., Rees,
.0022-3506.2004.00263.x G., Dolan, R. J., & Blakemore, S. J. (2013). The development of
Taylor, S. E., Wayment, H. A., & Carrillo, M. (1996). Social comparison, metacognitive ability in adolescence. Consciousness and Cognition,
self-regulation, and motivation. In R. M. Sorrentino & E. T. Higgins 22(1), 264–271. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2013.01.004
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

(Eds.), Handbook of motivation and cognition (pp. 3–27). Guilford Press. Weiner, B. (2010). The development of an attribution-based theory of
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Trope, Y., & Fishbach, A. (2000). Counteractive self-control in overcoming motivation: A history of ideas. Educational Psychologist, 45(1), 28–36.
temptation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 79(4), 493– https://doi.org/10.1080/00461520903433596
506. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.79.4.493 Weiner, B., Frieze, I., Kukla, A., Reed, L., Rest, S., & Rosenbaum, R. M.
Turner-McGrievy, G. M., Beets, M. W., Moore, J. B., Kaczynski, A. T., (1971). Perceiving the causes of success and failure. In E. E. Jones, D. E.
Barr-Anderson, D. J., & Tate, D. F. (2013). Comparison of traditional Kanouse, H. H. Kelley, R. E. Nisbett, S. Valins, & B. Weiner (Eds.),
versus mobile app self-monitoring of physical activity and dietary intake Attribution: Perceiving the causes of behavior (pp. 95–120). General
among overweight adults participating in an mHealth weight loss program. Learning Press.
Journal of the American Medical Informatics Association: JAMIA, 20(3), Wennerhold, L., & Friese, M. (2020). Why self-report measures of self-
513–518. https://doi.org/10.1136/amiajnl-2012-001510 control and inhibition tasks do not substantially correlate. Collabra.
Tversky, A., & Kahneman, D. (1974). Judgment under uncertainty: Heuristics Psychology, 6(1), Article 9. https://doi.org/10.1525/collabra.276
and biases. In J. von Neumann & O. Morgenstern (Eds.), Theory of games Wenzel, M., Bürgler, S., Brandstätter, V., Kreibich, A., & Hennecke, M.
and economic behavior (2nd ed., pp. 1123–1131). Princeton University Press. (2022). Self-regulatory strategy use, efficacy, and strategy-situation fit in
Tyler, J. M., & Burns, K. C. (2009). Triggering conservation of the self’s self-control conflicts if initiation, persistence, and inhibition [Manuscript
regulatory resources. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 31(3), 255– submitted for publication].
266. https://doi.org/10.1080/01973530903058490 Wenzel, M., Bürgler, S., Rowland, Z., & Hennecke, M. (2021). Self-control
Tzohar-Rozen, M., & Kramarski, B. (2014). Metacognition, motivation and dynamics in daily life: The importance of variability between self-
emotions: Contribution of self-regulated learning to solving mathematical regulatory strategies and strategy differentiation. European Journal of
problems. Global Education Review, 1(4), 76–95. Personality, 35(5), 1–24. https://doi.org/10.1177/08902070211043023
van der Laan, L. N., de Ridder, D. T., Charbonnier, L., Viergever, M. A., & Wenzel, M., Rowland, Z., Weber, H., & Kubiak, T. (2020). A round peg in a
Smeets, P. A. (2014). Sweet lies: Neural, visual, and behavioral measures square hole: Strategy-situation fit of intra- and interpersonal emotion
reveal a lack of self-control conflict during food choice in weight- regulation strategies and controllability. Cognition and Emotion, 34(5),
concerned women. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience, 8, Article 1003–1009. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699931.2019.1697209
184. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnbeh.2014.00184 Werner, K. M., & Ford, B. (2021). Self-control: An integrative framework.
Van Koningsbruggen, G. M., Stroebe, W., Papies, E. K., & Aarts, H. (2011). PsyArXiv. https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/y6kxa
Implementation intentions as goal primes: Boosting self-control in tempt- Werner, K. M., Inzlicht, M., & Ford, B. Q. (2022). Whither Inhibition?
ing environments. European Journal of Social Psychology, 41(5), 551– Current Directions in Psychological Science, 31(4), 333–339. https://
557. https://doi.org/10.1002/ejsp.799 doi.org/10.1177/09637214221095848
Vazire, S., & Carlson, E. N. (2010). Self-knowledge of personality: Do Werner, K. M., & Milyavskaya, M. (2018). We may not know what we want,
people know themselves? Social and Personality Psychology Compass, but do we know what we need? Examining the ability to forecast need
4(8), 605–620. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1751-9004.2010.00280.x satisfaction in goal pursuit. Social Psychological & Personality Science,
Verbruggen, F., & Logan, G. D. (2008). Response inhibition in the stop- 9(6), 656–663. https://doi.org/10.1177/1948550617720274
signal paradigm. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 12(11), 418–424. https:// Whiteside, S. P., & Lynam, D. R. (2001). The five factor model and
doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2008.07.005 impulsivity: Using a structural model of personality to understand impul-
Verschaffel, L., De Corte, E., Lasure, S., Van Vaerenbergh, G., Bogaerts, sivity. Personality and Individual Differences, 30(4), 669–689. https://
H., & Ratinckx, E. (1999). Learning to solve mathematical application doi.org/10.1016/S0191-8869(00)00064-7
problems: A design experiment with fifth graders. Mathematical Think- Wicklund, R. A. (1975). Objective self-awareness. Advances in Experimen-
ing and Learning, 1(3), 195–229. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327833 tal Social Psychology, 8, 233–275. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0065-2601
mtl0103_2 (08)60252-X
Vohs, K. D., Schmeichel, B. J., Lohmann, S., Gronau, Q. F., Finley, A. J., Williams, B. R., Ponesse, J. S., Schachar, R. J., Logan, G. D., & Tannock, R.
Ainsworth, S. E., Alquist, J. L., Baker, M. D., Brizi, A., Bunyi, A., (1999). Development of inhibitory control across the life span. Develop-
Butschek, G. J., Campbell, C., Capaldi, J., Cau, C., Chambers, H., mental Psychology, 35(1), 205–213. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649
Chatzisarantis, N. L. D., Christensen, W. J., Clay, S. L., Curtis, J., … .35.1.205
Albarracín, D. (2021). A multi-site preregistered paradigmatic test of the Williamson, L. Z., & Wilkowski, B. M. (2020). Nipping temptation in the
ego depletion effect. Psychological Science, 32(10), 1566–1581. https:// bud: Examining strategic self-control in daily life. Personality and Social
doi.org/10.1177/0956797621989733 Psychology Bulletin, 46(6), 961–975. https://doi.org/10.1177/014616721
Wang, K., Varma, D. S., & Prosperi, M. (2018). A systematic review of the 9883606
effectiveness of mobile apps for monitoring and management of mental Wilmot, M. P. (2015). A contemporary taxometric analysis of the latent
health symptoms or disorders. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 107, structure of self-monitoring. Psychological Assessment, 27(2), 353–364.
73–78. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpsychires.2018.10.006 https://doi.org/10.1037/pas0000030
1288 HENNECKE AND BÜRGLER

Wilmot, M. P., & Ones, D. S. (2019). A century of research on conscien- Zemack-Rugar, Y., Bettman, J. R., & Fitzsimons, G. J. (2007). The effects of
tiousness at work. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the nonconsciously priming emotion concepts on behavior. Journal of Per-
United States of America, 116(46), 23004–23010. https://doi.org/10.1073/ sonality and Social Psychology, 93(6), 927–939. https://doi.org/10.1037/
pnas.1908430116 0022-3514.93.6.927
Wilson, T. D., & Dunn, E. W. (2004). Self-knowledge: Its limits, value, and Zimmerman, B. J. (2000). Attaining self-regulation: A social cognitive
potential for improvement. Annual Review of Psychology, 55(1), 493–518. perspective. In M. Boekaerts, P. R. Pintrich, & M. Zeidner (Eds.),
https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.55.090902.141954 Handbook of self-regulation (pp. 13–39). Academic Press. https://
Winne, P. H. (2011). A cognitive and metacognitive analysis of self- doi.org/10.1016/B978-012109890-2/50031-7
regulated learning. In B. J. Zimmerman & D. H. Schunk (Eds.), Handbook Zimmerman, B. J. (2002). Becoming a self-regulated learner: An over-
of self-regulation of learning and performance (pp. 15–32). Routledge. view. Theory Into Practice, 41(2), 64–70. https://doi.org/10.1207/
Winne, P. H., & Hadwin, A. F. (1998). Studying as self-regulated engagement s15430421tip4102_2
in learning. In D. Hacker, J. Dunlosky, & A. Graesser (Eds.), Metacogni- Zimmerman, B. J., & Campillo, M. (2003). Motivating self-regulated
tion in educational theory and practice (pp. 277–304). Erlbaum. problem solvers. In J. E. Davidson & R. Sternberg (Eds.), The psychology
Winne, P. H., & Hadwin, A. F. (2013). nStudy: Tracing and supporting self- of problem solving (pp. 233–262). Cambridge University Press. https://
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

regulated learning in the internet. In R. Azevedo & V. Aleven (Eds.), doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511615771.009


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

International handbook of metacognition and learning technologies Zimmerman, B. J., & Moylan, A. R. (2009). Self-regulation: Where metacog-
(pp. 293–308). Springer. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4419-5546-3_20 nition and motivation intersect. In D. Hacker, J. Dunlosky, & C. Graesser
Wood, W., Labrecque, J. S., Lin, P. Y., & Rünger, D. (2014). Habits in dual (Eds.), Handbook of metacognition in education (pp. 299–315). Routledge.
process models. In J. Sherman, B. Gawronski, & Y. Trope (Eds.), Dual Zimmerman, B. J., & Paulsen, A. S. (1995). Self-monitoring during colle-
process theories of the social mind (pp. 371–385). Guilford Press. giate studying: An invaluable tool for academic self-regulation. New
Woolley, K., & Fishbach, A. (2015). The experience matters more than Directions for Teaching and Learning, 1995(63), 13–27. https://
you think: People value intrinsic incentives more inside than outside an doi.org/10.1002/tl.37219956305
activity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 109(6), 968–982.
https://doi.org/10.1037/pspa0000035
Wyer, R. S., Jr., Srull, T. K., & Gordon, S. (1984). The effects of predicting a
person’s behavior on subsequent trait judgments. Journal of Experimental Received September 28, 2021
Social Psychology, 20(1), 29–46. https://doi.org/10.1016/0022-1031(84) Revision received October 11, 2022
90010-6 Accepted October 11, 2022 ▪

You might also like