Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 15

European Journal of Work and Organizational Psychology

ISSN: 1359-432X (Print) 1464-0643 (Online) Journal homepage: https://www.tandfonline.com/loi/pewo20

Why employee psychopathy leads to


counterproductive workplace behaviours : an
analysis of the underlying mechanisms

Miriam Schilbach, Anja Baethge & Thomas Rigotti

To cite this article: Miriam Schilbach, Anja Baethge & Thomas Rigotti (2020): Why
employee psychopathy leads to counterproductive workplace behaviours : an analysis of the
underlying mechanisms, European Journal of Work and Organizational Psychology, DOI:
10.1080/1359432X.2020.1739650

To link to this article: https://doi.org/10.1080/1359432X.2020.1739650

View supplementary material

Published online: 20 Mar 2020.

Submit your article to this journal

Article views: 23

View related articles

View Crossmark data

Full Terms & Conditions of access and use can be found at


https://www.tandfonline.com/action/journalInformation?journalCode=pewo20
EUROPEAN JOURNAL OF WORK AND ORGANIZATIONAL PSYCHOLOGY
https://doi.org/10.1080/1359432X.2020.1739650

Why employee psychopathy leads to counterproductive workplace behaviours : an


analysis of the underlying mechanisms
a
Miriam Schilbach , Anja Baethgeb and Thomas Rigotti a,c

a
Leibniz Institute for Resilience Research, Mainz, Germany; bDepartment of Psychology, Medical School Hamburg, Hamburg, Germany; cDepartment
of Psychology, Johannes Gutenberg-University, Mainz, Germany

ABSTRACT ARTICLE HISTORY


The purpose of this study is to examine possible explanatory mechanisms linking employee secondary Received 16 September 2019
psychopathy to counterproductive workplace behaviour (CWB). Based on the emotion-centred model of Accepted 4 March 2020
voluntary work behaviour we argue that secondary psychopathy is characterized by unfavourable KEYWORDS
cognitive appraisal tendencies, which in turn positively relate to negative affectivity. We further assume corporate psychopathy;
that this cognitive-affective process enhances CWB. We also include primary psychopathy into our appraisal; negative affect;
research model to test if the presumed mechanism applies to both psychopathy dimensions. We Counterproductive Work
collected daily-survey data from 470 employees (1670 days) and analysed these data using multilevel Behaviour (CWB); serial
structured equation modelling. We found strong support for the hypothesized serial mediation model, mediation
indicating that secondary psychopathy triggers dysfunctional cognitive-affective tendencies and conse-
quently increases the likelihood of CWB. The proposed model did not hold up for primary psychopathy.
Our study outlines the presence of distorted cognition-affective patterns in employee secondary psycho-
pathy only. These patterns seem to play a key role in explaining the link between employee secondary
psychopathy and deviant workplace behaviour. Based on this procedural knowledge relevant implica-
tions for theory and practice are provided.

Introduction commonly operationalized psychopathy as a unidimensional


construct (e.g., Baloch et al., 2017; Palmer et al., 2017). A
Depending on the utilized diagnostic criteria, prevalence
recently conducted study however highlighted the pivotal
rates for corporate psychopathy range from roughly 1% up
role of disinhibition, a hallmark feature of secondary psycho-
to 9.5% (Coid et al., 2012; Hare, 2003). On a subclinical level,
pathy in positively predicting CWB (Neo et al., 2018). Fittingly,
the numbers are assumed to be much higher (cf., Forster &
Blickle and Schütte (2017) as well as Schütte et al. (2018) show
Lund, 2018). Thus, individuals exhibiting psychopathic ten-
that secondary psychopathy positively relates to CWB while
dencies represent a substantial part of our workforce, striv-
primary psychopathy only relates to CWB if education or poli-
ing for power and control, potentially willing to engage in
tical skills were low. Consequently, the present study attempts
unethical behaviour to attain their goals (Jonason & Ferrell,
to derive an explanatory mechanism linking secondary psycho-
2016; Semenyna & Honey, 2015). Empirical research high-
pathy, the seemingly more predictive psychopathy dimension
lights the detrimental effects psychopathic tendencies may
for deviant workplace behaviour, to CWB. We will further ana-
have via extreme work behaviours, such as manipulation,
lyse if the presumed process explaining the link between sec-
intimidation or coercion causing conflicts, impaired well-
ondary psychopathy and CWB also applies to primary
being among co-workers, and decreased organizational pro-
psychopathy. By doing so we intent to test whether the
ductivity (Blickle & Schütte, 2017; Boddy et al., 2015; Walker
hypothesized relationships are indeed specific to secondary
& Jackson, 2017). The financial damages in the United States
psychopathy.
associated with such behaviours are estimated at several
Based on the emotion-centred model of voluntary work
hundred billion dollars every year (McGee & Fillon, 1995;
behaviour (Spector & Fox, 2002) we argue that cognitive
Murphy, 1993) not taking into account the vast negative
appraisal of the workday and the resulting emotions represent
impact on psychological well-being of individuals who have
an explanatory process as to why secondary psychopathy posi-
fallen victim to psychopathic schemes. Thus, the present
tively relates to CWB. Presumably, due to high levels of disin-
study aims to better comprehend underlying mechanisms
hibition and reflection impulsivity we hypothesize that
causing employees with psychopathic features to engage in
secondary psychopathy contributes to viewing the workday
counterproductive work behaviours (CWB).
as less challenging and simultaneously more hindering as well
Despite the widely acknowledged two-dimensionality of
as threatening. We further argue that this pattern of cognitive
psychopathy (i.e., primary and secondary psychopathy;
appraisal relates to negative emotions, which in turn enhance
Cleckley, 1976; Falkenbach et al., 2008; Levenson et al., 1995),
CWB (Dalal et al., 2009; Spector & Fox, 2002).
earlier research on the link between psychopathy and CWB

CONTACT Miriam Schilbach mischilb@uni-mainz.de; rigotti@uni-mainz.de


Supplemental data for this article can be accessed here.
© 2020 Informa UK Limited, trading as Taylor & Francis
2 M. SCHILBACH ET AL.

The present study contributes to the growing body of 2010). Furthermore they often appear to be charming persua-
research suggesting secondary psychopathy to be of high rele- sive individuals with high intelligence and abilities on first
vance in predicting CWB (Blickle & Schütte, 2017; Neo et al., impression (Lilienfeld et al., 2015; Patrick et al., 2009).
2018; Schütte et al., 2018). Taking appraisal and emotional Secondary psychopathy on the other hand, develops from
processes into account, our study provides an explanatory environmental causes and thus represents an acquired affec-
mechanism as to why secondary psychopathy traits may trans- tive disturbance (Del Gaizo & Falkenbach, 2008; Skeem et al.,
late into deviant workplace behaviour. Understanding mechan- 2007). It is associated with poor behavioural control often
isms behind the behavioural manifestation of psychopathic linked to high levels of impulsivity, a lacking ability to set
traits represents a first step towards preventing the negative long-term, realistic goals, and a proneness to boredom
impact secondary psychopathy potentially disperses within the (Hare, 2003; Levenson et al., 1995). Unlike the low levels of
work environment. Additionally, by including primary psycho- anxiety and inability to feel remorse in primary psychopathy
pathy into our analyses we outline that the two psychopathy (Cleckley, 1976; Harpur et al., 1989), secondary psychopathy is
dimensions may be characterized by diverging cognitive-affec- associated with high levels of neuroticism and a propensity
tive processes within the work context which may partly for feelings of guilt (Benning et al., 2003; Gudjonsson &
explain the differential results reported in earlier studies (see Roberts, 1983).
Blickle & Schütte, 2017; Schütte et al., 2018). Further, this In their triarchic model of psychopathy, Patrick et al. (2009)
research contributes to the emotion-centred model of volun- state that both primary and secondary psychopathy are com-
tary work behaviour (Spector & Fox, 2002) as we (a) apply it to a posed of a combination of three different phenotypic features:
specific personality trait and test if the presumed mechanisms disinhibition, boldness, and meanness (see also differential-
hold up and (b) integrate the cognitive appraisal forms of configuration-model; Lilienfeld et al., 2015). The authors view
challenge, hindrance and threat which to our knowledge has these constructs to be pivotal to understand psychopathy in its
not been done previously. various manifestations, namely primary and secondary, criminal
On the upcoming pages, we introduce our key constructs, and noncriminal. The feature disinhibition refers to a propen-
beginning with psychopathy and CWB. Following this, we sity towards impulse control problems, such as deficient beha-
implement our presumed mediator variables and outline the vioural restraint. Boldness describes the capacity to remain
hypothesized process linking secondary psychopathy with calm and focused in high-pressure situations, the ability to
deviant workplace behaviour. recover quickly from stressful events (resilience) as well as
high levels of social efficacy. Meanness refers to a constellation
of various attributes including deficient empathy, the tendency
Corporate primary and secondary psychopathy
to exploit others, or empowerment through cruelty (Patrick et
Psychopathy is a personality construct constituted of a combi- al., 2009). Whereas primary psychopaths score high on bold-
nation of various affective, interpersonal, and behavioural def- ness and meanness, secondary psychopaths tend to show high
icits, including impulsivity, manipulativeness, pathological levels of disinhibition and meanness. Boldness does not seem
lying, lack of empathy, and high levels of emotional detach- to feature in secondary psychopathy while disinhibition does
ment (Hare, 1996; Patrick et al., 1993; Walker & Jackson, 2017). not seem to hallmark primary psychopathy (Drislane, Patrick,
These features are present on a continuum ranging from very Sourander et al., 2014). Among other personality traits, such as
low to very high and thus may reflect various levels of psycho- narcissism or machiavelism, previous studies report a positive
pathic tendencies (Babiak & Hare, 2006; Marcus et al., 2012). link between psychopathy and CWB (Blickle & Schütte, 2017;
Corporate psychopathy refers to individuals within an organi- O’Boyle et al., 2012; Schütte et al., 2018). Following, we intro-
zational setting who exhibit psychopathic traits and may cause duce the concept of CWB and outline why psychopathy and
harm in ways other than overt criminal conduct (Babiak & Hare, especially secondary psychopathy may predict deviant work
2006; Boddy, 2014; Neo et al., 2018). Through social learning behaviour.
(Bandura, 1979) the exhibited toxic behaviours may be carried
out by others and potentially multiply within an organization,
Linking corporate psychopathy to CWB
causing even greater damage to the organizational climate as
well as the well-being and satisfaction of the employees CWB is defined as voluntary behaviour that significantly vio-
(Boddy, 2014; Mathieu et al., 2014). lates organizational norms leading to a decrease in the well-
The dual process model (Fowles & Dindo, 2009; Levenson being of the organization, its members, or both (Robinson &
et al., 1995) distinguishes between two forms of psychopathy: Bennett, 1995). CWB consists of two factors: There are CWB
primary and secondary psychopathy. These dimensions exist harming individuals within the organization (CWBI) and CWB
in criminal as well as non-criminal populations (Drislane, harmful directly to the organization (CWBO; Fox et al., 2001;
Patrick, Sourander et al., 2014; Miller & Lynam, 2012) and differ Robinson & Bennett, 1995). An example for CWBI is the exclu-
in the presence of various characteristics. There is widespread sion of a colleague from group activities. An individual enga-
agreement that primary psychopathy is an inherited affective ging in CWBO on the other hand might talk disrespectfully
deficit leading to callous and manipulative behaviours (Del about the company or reduce his work pace. The negative
Gaizo & Falkenbach, 2008; Karpman, 1941; Skeem et al., 2007). psychological impact of working in a CWB-enhanced climate
Individuals with increased levels of primary psychopathy tend or being the target of CWBI can be substantial and often
to experience shallow affect, little or no remorse, guilt, or becomes apparent in the form of depressive (Kivimäki et al.,
empathy, and a grandiose sense of self-worth (Miller et al., 2003) or psychosomatic symptoms (Mikkelsen & Einarsen,
EUROPEAN JOURNAL OF WORK AND ORGANIZATIONAL PSYCHOLOGY 3

2002), job dissatisfaction, and turnover intentions (Boddy & psychopathy which may lower the threshold even further.
Taplin, 2016; Lim et al., 2008). Hence while both dimensions of psychopathy seem to have a
The emotion-centred model of voluntary work behaviour tendency to engage in CWB, primary psychopathy features
provides a procedural framework aiming to clarify the antece- protective factors which allow individuals to engage in a
dents and determining factors of organizational citizenship broader range of non-deviant behaviours. Contrary, secondary
behaviour and CWB (Spector & Fox, 2002). Besides other com- psychopathy characteristics seem to strengthen the tendency
ponents which will be discussed later on, Spector and Fox to engage in CWB.
(2002) acknowledge the importance of personality characteris- Previous research already reported a positive link between
tics as precursors to behavioural tendencies and propose a secondary psychopathy and CWBI (Schütte et al., 2018) as well
direct effect of personality on CWB. Concerning general corpo- as secondary psychopathy and CWBO (Blickle & Schütte, 2017).
rate psychopathy, O’Boyle et al. (2012) conducted a meta-ana- The present study attempts to replicate these results.
lysis and report a weak but positive link between psychopathy Respectively, we derive the following hypothesis:
and CWB. However, and as in other employee related studies
(Palmer et al., 2017; Scherer et al., 2013) authors did not H1: Secondary psychopathy is positively linked to (a) CWBI and
account for the multi-dimensionality of the psychopathic per- (b) CWBO.
sonality which may distort results (Blickle & Schütte, 2017). In
fact, recently conducted studies indicate that individuals with
secondary psychopathic tendencies may be particularly prone
Mechanisms linking secondary psychopathy
to engage in CWB. For example, Neo et al. (2018) assessed the
with CWB
link between the triarchic psychopathy features and CWB.
Results show that only disinhibition, the hallmark feature of To further expand the knowledge as to why secondary psycho-
secondary psychopathy positively predicts CWB. Other findings pathy seems to be pivotal in predicting CWB, we drew upon the
are similar in that secondary psychopathy positively relates to afore mentioned emotion-centred model of voluntary work
CWB while primary psychopathy only relates to CWB if educa- behaviour (Spector & Fox, 2002). The model proposes that
tion or political skills are low (Blickle & Schütte, 2017; Schütte et personality influences cognitive appraisal which may result in
al., 2018). To begin to comprehend why secondary psychopa- negative emotions, eliciting avoidance- and retaliation-type
thy seems to be more pivotal in predicting CWB we started by actions, such as CWB. Based on the model we argue that two
analysing what the two dimensions have in common and how procedural components explain behavioural tendencies in sec-
they differ. ondary psychopathy: (a) secondary psychopathy is character-
In general individuals with psychopathic characteristics ized by unfavourable cognitive appraisal patterns; and (b) the
experience affective deficits and a lack of empathy as well as appraisal patterns lead to negative emotional states, which in
insensitivity (Cleckley, 1976; Patrick et al., 1993). Hence, under- turn promote the use of CWB. Hence, according to our hypoth-
standing how others feel if treated disrespectfully and further eses, behavioural tendencies in secondary psychopathy to
to care about other individuals’ well-being is something corpo- engage in CWB originate in a catenation of appraisal patterns
rate psychopaths struggle with. As a result, the overall inhibi- and negative affect. Figure 1 illustrates the proposed process
tion threshold to engage in CWB should be lower in individuals model.
with psychopathic tendencies. However, primary psychopathy
is characterized by various features which may detain indivi-
1st Procedural component: appraisal patterns in
duals from engaging in CWB. First, primary psychopathy is
secondary psychopathy
associated with the ability to set agendas, plan and follow
long term goals (Lilienfeld et al., 2012b; Snowden & Gray, According to the transactional stress model, individuals per-
2011). As a result, individuals are more likely to engage in ceive situations as challenging, hindering or threatening
CWB only if this action brings them closer to attaining their (Cavanaugh et al., 2000; Lazarus & Folkman, 1984; Searle &
goals. Additionally, individuals with primary psychopathic ten- Auton, 2015; Webster et al., 2011). Whereas challenges produce
dencies are persuasive, skilled at impression management and positive feelings and a sense of achievement, hindrances inter-
can influence others without acting in a destructive or impul- fere with an individual’s ability to achieve valued goals and are
sive manner (Titze et al., 2017). Thus, there may be means to commonly viewed as irritating (Cavanaugh et al., 2000).
achieve goals other than engaging in CWB for individuals with Perceived threats refer to job demands or circumstances that
primary psychopathic tendencies. In secondary psychopathy tend to be associated with personal harm or loss (Lazarus &
on the other hand the ability to pursue goals and plan beha- Folkman, 1984). Threats impact felt levels of anxiety and may
viour is limited (Snowden & Gray, 2011). Further, there is defi- negatively affect self-worth (Lazarus & Folkman, 1984; Tuckey
cient anticipation of negative consequences of inappropriate et al., 2015). Hence, we operationalize cognitive appraisal via
behaviour as well as rebelliousness (Lilienfeld & Widows, 2005). the perception of a workday as challenging, hindering, or
Individuals with secondary psychopathic tendencies therefore threatening. Following, we outline the proposed relationships
tend to evaluate which behaviour brings immediate benefit between secondary psychopathy and each of the cognitive
and in their evaluation, do not account for potentially negative appraisal forms.
future outcomes (Bjørnebekk & Gjesme, 2009; Snowden & Gray, A feeling of challenge occurs if employees get closer to
2011). Last, the already low inhibition threshold to engage in attaining a valued goal (Cavanaugh et al., 2000). In accordance,
CWB is met by high levels of general disinhibition in secondary transactional stress theory states that an environmental
4 M. SCHILBACH ET AL.

Challenge
Appraisal
CWBI

Secondary Hindrance Negative


Psychopathy Appraisal Affect
CWBO

Threat
Appraisal

Figure 1. Presumed Process Linking Secondary Psychopathy with CWB.


Direct paths from secondary psychopathy to negative affect, CWBI and CWBO as well as direct paths from challenge appraisal, hindrance appraisal and threat appraisal to CWBI and CWBO are
omitted for clarity of presentation. Further primary psychopathy will be included in the statistical analyses to test if the proposed mechanisms applies to both dimensions.

condition is a source of benefit only if it confronts a person’s Next to disinhibition, neuroticism is another key feature of
motivational and cognitive characteristics (Folkman & Lazarus, secondary psychopathy (Cleckley, 1976; Karpman, 1941; Lilienfeld
1987; Lazarus & Folkman, 1984). Thus, the environmental con- et al., 2012a). Previously conducted research highlights the effect
dition (e.g., goals set by the organization) must fit an indivi- of neuroticism on appraisal tendencies, showing that individuals
dual’s motives so that a perception of challenge and potential high in neuroticism commonly view available resources as inade-
benefits can occur. quate and consequently perceive situations as rather threatening
Secondary psychopathy is characterized by a lacking ability to to the self (Gallagher, 1990; Schneider, 2004). Reflection impulsivity
plan behaviour and to anticipate positive or negative conse- will further prevent individuals to carefully assess possible out-
quences of one’s actions (Bjørnebekk & Gjesme, 2009; Snowden comes and (re)evaluate the actual threat posed in a work-related
& Gray, 2011). Thus, comprehension of how work-related actions situation. As a result, individuals scoring higher on secondary
may be beneficial in the future for either the development of psychopathy, should be more likely to perceive threats within
personal competences or the attainment of task specific goals the work setting. Thus, we propose the following hypothesis:
should be limited. As a result, individuals with secondary psycho-
pathic tendencies should experience less motivation for goal H2: Secondary psychopathy is positively related to the appraisal
attainment and consequently reduced levels of challenge apprai- of a workday as (a) unchallenging, (b) hindering, and (c)
sal in the work context. This assumption is supported by the threatening.
results of Hall et al. (2004) where secondary psychopathy nega-
tively related to achievement motivation.
2nd procedural component: emotional response to
As outlined earlier, a hallmark characteristic of secondary
appraisal and its impact on CWB
psychopathy is the high level of disinhibition resulting in impul-
sive tendencies (Patrick et al., 2009). Researchers consider Cognitive interpretation of environmental events affects our
impulsivity to be a multifactorial construct (Aron et al., 2004; emotional state (Lazarus, 1982; Schachter & Singer, 1962).
Clark et al., 2006). One particular form of impulsivity is the Early research highlighted the natural fusion of cognition and
reflection impulsivity which was introduced by Kagan (1966). emotion and showed how emotions are the immediate
It refers to the tendency to gather and evaluate information response to situational perception (Folkman et al., 1979;
before reaching a decision (Clark et al., 2006). Based on the Lazarus, 1991; Payne, 1999). The model of emotion-centred
increased disinhibitory levels in secondary psychopathy we voluntary work behaviour also acknowledges this relation
argue that reflection impulsivity is high, i.e., individuals do not between appraisal and emotions (Spector & Fox, 2002).
reflect upon situations by gathering and evaluating informa- Situations that are perceived as challenging have the poten-
tion carefully, but rather reach a decision impulsively. Thus, tial to promote personal gain and consequently tend to trigger
disinhibition and impulsivity are not only reflected on a beha- positive emotions (Searle & Auton, 2015; Skinner & Brewer,
vioural level, but may also be a key determinant of cognitive 2002). Regarding an absence of perceived challenges, there is
processes in secondary psychopathy: only limited knowledge about the emotional responses.
First, impulsive individuals struggle with delayed gratifica- However, evidence suggests that personal thriving is inhibited
tion making immediate need satisfaction within or outside the in the absence of challenge perception (Prem et al., 2017).
work setting a central goal (Bjørnebekk & Gjesme, 2009; Block Further, the perception of reduced personal thriving positively
et al., 2002). Job requirements may hinder immediate need relates to negative emotions (Porath et al., 2012). Hence, since
satisfaction and thus are perceived as obstacles to achieving a the absence of challenge appraisal prevents individuals from
goal, i.e., hindrances (Cavanaugh et al., 2000). Reflection impul- anticipating future gains as well as experiencing thriving, we
sivity will further prevent individuals to take into account con- assume increased levels of negative emotions if challenge
text and other relevant information, which would allow for appraisal is low.
prompt or slightly delayed need satisfaction. Based on this Regarding hindrance appraisals, Daniels et al. (2004) use their
argumentation we assume secondary psychopathy to posi- model of work events and unpleasant affect to argue that if goal
tively relate to the appraisal of a workday as hindering. progress is perceived to be impeded and slower than desired,
EUROPEAN JOURNAL OF WORK AND ORGANIZATIONAL PSYCHOLOGY 5

unpleasant affect occurs. This assumption is based on previous in the form of CWB (H4a,b; Dalal et al., 2009). Combining these
findings where the velocity towards goal attainment predicted main effects, we hypothesize the following mechanisms to link
positive and negative emotions (Lawrence et al., 2002). Other secondary psychopathy with CWBI and CWBO:
researchers also concluded that job conditions, which interfered
with an individual’s goals would induce negative emotions H5a: There is a positive indirect effect between (I) secondary
(Carver & Scheier, 1990; Spector, 1978). Per definition, hindrances psychopathy and CWBI and (II) secondary psychopathy and
represent obstacles to achieving valued goals (Cavanaugh et al., CWBO, via the cognitive appraisal of the workday as unchallen-
2000) and are therefore assumed to increase the likelihood for ging and negative emotions.
negative affectivity.
Threat appraisal at work refers to circumstances that are H5b: There is a positive indirect effect between (I) secondary
perceived to cause personal harm or loss (Lazarus & Folkman, psychopathy and CWBI and (II) secondary psychopathy and
1984; Tuckey et al., 2015). Facing personal harm and conse- CWBO, via the cognitive appraisal of the workday as hindering
quently a threat to self-worth and self-efficacy is a highly aver- and negative emotions.
sive situation. Multiple studies show that this aversive cognitive
state of threat perception positively relates to negative emo- H5 c: There is a positive indirect effect between (I) secondary
tions such as anger, anxiety or nervousness (Lazarus & Folkman, psychopathy and CWBI and (II) secondary psychopathy and
1984; Lepine et al., 2005; Tuckey et al., 2015). Hence, we argue CWBO, via the cognitive appraisal of the workday as threaten-
that the presumed cognitive appraisal tendencies in secondary ing and negative emotions.
psychopathy as described above enhance negative emotions.
We do not assume these mechanisms to apply to primary
H3: The appraisal of the workday as (a) unchallenging (b) hinder- psychopathy. As outlined earlier, boldness and related to that
ing and (c) threatening positively relate to negative affect. the immunity to stress are key features of primary psychopathy
(Lykken, 1995; Patrick et al., 2009). Further, primary psychopa-
thy is neither characterized by the inability to pursue goals nor
high levels of disinhibition and reflection impulsivity. As a
3rd procedural component: the impact of emotional
result, we do not derive any hypotheses linking primary psy-
responses on CWB
chopathy to the appraisal forms and hence we do not assume
Emotions further energize and motivate subsequent behaviour the presumed cognitive-affective mechanism to apply to pri-
(Cartwritght & Cooper, 1997; Spector, 1998). The emotion-centred mary psychopathy. Nevertheless, to empirically analyse if the
model of voluntary work behaviour views CWB as a consequence mechanism would be similar in primary psychopathy, we will
of negative emotions (Spector & Fox, 2002). In this context, several include primary psychopathy into our statistical model.
studies have highlighted the predictive power of negative emo-
tions regarding deviant workplace behaviour towards individuals
and the organization (Fox et al., 2001; Samnani et al., 2014). Method
Various explanations as to why this link exists have been sug-
Procedure and sample
gested. One possible view is that CWB represents a behavioural
manifestation of negative emotions (Dalal et al., 2009). In this To recruit participants, we contacted German organizations. In
context, emotions hold information about behavioural judge- return for participation, we offered to conduct part of a psycho-
ment. Negative information is easily accessible during states of logical risk assessment, which in Germany is legally mandatory.
negative emotionality and tends to result in negative behaviour- Employees within organizations were recruited via informative
related judgement. Thus, negative emotions are linked to negative meetings, email, intranet, or video-messages. To be eligible for
views of the work environment (co-workers, managers, company) participation, employees had to work at least 20 hours per week.
and less favourable evaluations of others – CWB is a behavioural To assure data protection and to match the questionnaires, we
manifestation of these judgements (Dalal et al., 2009). used pseudonymization. Participants were extensively informed
about data processing procedures. Before conducting our study,
H4: Negative affect positively relates to (a) CWBI and (b) CWBO. we received ethical approval from the ethics board of our
research institute.
Participants could register for the study via email. Once regis-
tered, participants received an online link to the initial survey.
Linking secondary psychopathy with CWB: a serial
Complying with the European Data Protection Guidelines partici-
mediation model
pants had to explicitly give their consent that they want to take
As outlined above we assume that a lacking ability to follow part in the study before access to the initial survey was granted.
goals and plan behaviour as well as disinhibitory tendencies in Besides demographic variables, the initial questionnaire included
secondary psychopathy promote unfavourable cognitive apprai- various trait-constructs (e.g., primary and secondary psychopathy).
sal patterns (H2a-c). Based on the emotion-centred model of Following, participants were asked to choose one out of six weeks
voluntary work behaviour (Spector & Fox, 2002), we further state where they would complete a daily-diary study consisting of two
that the dysfunctional appraisal tendencies in secondary psy- questionnaires per day, over five days in a workweek. Specifically,
chopathy are a precursor of negative emotionality (H3a-c). participants answered the first daily questionnaire at the end of a
Negative emotions potentially manifest on a behavioural level workday and the second daily questionnaire before going to sleep.
6 M. SCHILBACH ET AL.

To increase commitment, we offered participants to choose if they Unterrainer et al. (2016) who previously used the LSRP in a
wanted to complete the survey online using the survey tool German speaking sample. The authors kindly provided us
Unipark (Questback GmbH, 2017) or in an analogue paper pencil with their items. We translated these items back into English.
version. A total of 36 participants preferred to answer at least one There was high congruence between the original and the back
of the questionnaires in the analogue version. Regarding the translated English version. Minor differences were discussed
analogue questionnaires, participants were asked to provide the with a native English speaker and adjustments to the German
date as well as exact daytime of filling out the survey. version were made accordingly. We further looked at the inter-
A total of 572 employees participated in the initial survey. relationships between the psychopathy dimensions and the Big
Regarding the daily-diary study, 522 employees answered the 5 personality traits. Correlations were as expected and congru-
questionnaire at the end of their regular working hours for ent with previously reported studies of the LSRP indicating
2,206 days. The second daily questionnaire was answered by external validity (e.g., Lynam et al., 1999; Ross et al., 2004).
512 employees for 2,046 days before going to sleep. Days A sample item of the primary psychopathy subscale was
where participants filled out only one of the questionnaires “Success is based on survival of the fittest; I am not concerned
and/or answered the questionnaires at the wrong time (e.g., about the losers”, a secondary psychopathy item was, “When I
answering the second questionnaire the next morning) were get frustrated, I often ‘let off steam’ by blowing my top”.
excluded, resulting in a final data set of 470 employees who Answers were provided on a 5-point scale (1 = I do not agree;
answered both questionnaires at the right time on a total of 5 = I agree). Cronbach’s alpha for the primary psychopathy
1,670 days. On average, employees answered the daily ques- subscale was .77, for secondary psychopathy alpha was .66.
tionnaires on 3.6 days. Of the final sample, 66.1% were female,
mean age was 41.1 years (SD = 11.61), and most of the partici- Cognitive appraisal
pants were married or in a relationship (77.5%). Overall, parti- We assessed cognitive appraisal at the end of a workday,
cipants were highly educated, with 53.9% holding a university following the works of Moise (2014) and Feldman et al.
degree. Among all participants, 27.5% occupied leadership (2004). The appraisal of the workday as challenging (“To what
positions. Participants worked in a wide range of industries, extent did you view your workday as a positive challenge?”),
including financial spheres, public services, health care, law hindering (“To what extent did today’s work hinder the attain-
firms, engineering, or private service providers. On average, ment of personal goals?”), and threatening (“To what extent did
participants worked 40.13 hours per week (SD = 7.85). you feel that today’s work had a negative impact on you?”) was
evaluated. Additionally, we provided a definition for each of the
appraisal forms: ”Challenges refer to situations/demands that
Measures
may be stressful, but at the same time may help you to achieve
All variables were assessed in German. Table 1 shows descrip- valued goals or learn new things”; ”Hindrances refer to situa-
tive information and correlation for the constructs included in tions/demands that represent obstacles to success and may
our study. hinder you to work efficiently“; “Threats refer to situations/
demands where you had the feeling that they may negatively
Primary and secondary psychopathy affect you or that represented a negative experience to you”. By
Primary and secondary psychopathy were assessed in the initial providing definitions we aimed to assure that participants
survey, using the Levenson Self-Report Psychopathy Scale understood what each of the appraisal forms meant and
(LSRP; Levenson et al., 1995). The LSRP is a widely accepted further, to outline the distinction between hindrance and
and economical instrument for measuring psychopathy in non- threat. Participants provided their answers on a 5-point scale
institutionalized populations (Akhtar et al., 2013; Falkenbach et (1 = not at all; 5 = very much).
al., 2007). In accordance with the dual process model (Fowles &
Dindo, 2009; Levenson et al., 1995) the LSRP assesses primary Negative affect
(16 items) and secondary psychopathy (10 items). Since the Daily negative affect was assessed at the end of a workday
LSRP was originally developed in English we contacted using negative affect items of the Positive and Negative

Table 1. Correlation, Means, Standard Deviations, and Intraclass Coefficients.


1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 MB SDB MW SDW ICC(1)
1.Primary Psychopathy - - - - - - - - 1.87 0.42 - - -
2.Secondary Psychopathy .21 - - - - - - - 2.06 0.48 - - -
3.Challenge Appraisal .09 .38 .20 .19 .20 .10 .23 - 2.67 0.81 2.67 1.02 .47
4.Hindrance Appraisal .05 .27 .28 .38 .25 .14 .07 - 1.94 0.78 1.94 1.00 .47
5.Threat Appraisal .03 .28 .41 .64 .30 .15 .16 - 1.53 0.72 1.54 0.88 .53
6.Negative Affect .02 .27 .36 .43 .67 .15 .15 - 1.24 0.34 1.24 0.42 .49
7.CWBI .19 .25 .32 .40 .50 .55 .22 - 1.30 0.37 1.30 0.47 .46
8.CWBO .17 .30 .35 .26 .33 .32 .56 - 1.77 0.59 1.80 0.74 .62
9.Neuroticism −.06 .38 .30 .23 .30 .41 .12 .13 2.94 0.87 - - -
Means and standard deviations at the between-person level are displayed in columns 10 and 11; means and standard deviations at the within-person level are
displayed in columns 12 and 13. The item assessing challenge appraisal was inverted. Correlation printed in bold numbers did not reach a significant level (p >.05). All
other correlations reached significance at p <.05. Correlations below the diagonal are between-person correlations, correlations above the diagonal are within-person
correlations. MB, SDB: Means and Standard Deviations on the between-person-level, MW, SDW: Means and Standard Deviations on the within-person-level, ICC
(1) = variance between persons/(variance between persons + variance within persons).
EUROPEAN JOURNAL OF WORK AND ORGANIZATIONAL PSYCHOLOGY 7

Affect Schedule (PANAS; Watson et al., 1988). To keep the daily same time, neuroticism is a key feature of secondary psycho-
questionnaire reasonably short, we used a validated short ver- pathy (Benning et al., 2003; Gudjonsson & Roberts, 1983). To
sion of the PANAS (Thompson, 2007), consisting of five adjec- ensure that effects of secondary psychopathy on appraisal,
tives measuring negative affect (scared, nervous, distressed, negative emotions as well as CWBI and CWBO are not merely
ashamed, hostile). We based our translation on the German due to neurotic tendencies, we used the validated German
version of PANAS (Krohne et al., 1996). Participants were short version of the Big Five Inventory (Rammstedt & John,
asked about their momentary feelings and provided answers 2007) and included neuroticism as a control variable into our
on a 5-point scale (1 = not at all; 5 = very much). research model. Neuroticism was assessed in the initial survey,
using two items. An example item was “I see myself as someone
CWB who gets nervous easily”. Answers were provided on a 5-point
Outcome variables CWBI and CWBO were measured before scale (1 = disagree strongly; 5 = agree strongly).
participants went to sleep, using a 12-item scale, (Dalal et al.,
2009) explicitly developed to measure within-person work Statistical analyses
behaviours. To obtain a German version of the scale we used
the translation back translation method (e.g., Brislin, 1970; Due to hierarchically structured data present in daily-diary
Chapman et al., 1979). Each construct was measured using six studies (i.e., days nested within individuals), we tested our 2-
items. Sample items are, “Today, I criticized my supervisor’s/a 1-1-1 models using a multilevel approach. Specifically, follow-
coworker‘s opinion or suggestion” (CWBI) and “Today, I spent ing the recommendation of Preacher et al. (2010), we used the
time on tasks unrelated to work” (CWBO). Answers were pro- multilevel structural equation modelling (MSEM) framework.
vided on a 5-point scale (1 = I do not agree; 5 = I agree). Our large sample size exceeds common recommendations for
To examine construct validity, we performed a multilevel multilevel-designs (Preacher et al., 2010), providing enough
confirmatory factor analysis (see Table 2). A five-factor model power even for the complex serial mediation model. To attain
with negative affect, CWBI, and CWBO modelled at the between- unbiased results, we modelled our paths at the within-person
as well as within-person level and primary and secondary psy- (level 1) and the between-person (level 2) levels, whenever
chopathy modelled at the between-person level only provided there was a 1–1 combination (i.e., two variables on level 1;
the best fit, χ2 = 1515.10, df = 564, comparative fit index Preacher et al., 2010). We centred our independent variable,
(CFI) = .81, Tucker-Lewis index (TLI) = .79, root mean square secondary psychopathy (level 2), at the grand-mean. The
error of approximation (RMSEA) = .03. Since CWB measures remaining level 1 variables remained uncentered since they
distinct behaviours which are unlikely to co-occur within a contain within-person as well as between-person variance
given workday we did not expect high factor loadings for this components (Preacher et al., 2010). We tested our hypotheses
construct (e.g., Wang et al., 2011). To account for this, we used using Mplus, Version 7.4 (Muthén & Muthén, 1998-2015).
item parcelling for CWBI and CWBO, building two parcels per
subscale (e.g., Plummer, 2000). Arguably our fit indexes do not Results
reach the commonly used cut-off criteria proposed by Hu and
Bentler (1999). However, Marsh et al. (2004) argue that the good- Variance composition
ness of fit criteria are too restrictive. Further, other authors high- We examined intraclass correlations (ICCs (1)) to analyse var-
light that fit indexes do not reflect the plausibility of a model (e. iance composition at the within- and between-person levels.
g., Byrne, 2001). Other aspects such as validity, adequacy and ICCs (1) ranged between .47 and .62 (see Table 1). This indicates
interpretability should also be considered (Hu & Bentler, 1998). that between 38% and 53% of the total variance of our level 1
Thus, since our five-factor model fits the data significantly better variables (i.e., challenge appraisal, hindrance appraisal, threat
than alternative models, has a good RMSEA value and conforms appraisal, negative affect, CWBI, and CWBO) was within-person
with the presumed factor structure based on theoretical consid- variance. Thus, the ICCs (1) outline the adequacy of multilevel
erations, we would classify our model as acceptable misspecified, approaches for hypothesis testing.
a term used by Marsh et al. (2004).

Hypotheses testing
Control variable
In accordance with hypotheses 1a and 1b, secondary psycho-
Neuroticism is a powerful predictor of cognitive appraisal as pathy positively predicted CWBI and CWBO. This was also the
well as negative emotionality (e.g., Gunthert et al., 1999). At the case for primary psychopathy (see Table 4). Further, Table 3

Table 2. Confirmatory Factor Analysis.


χ2 df CFI TLI AIC RMSEA SCF S-B χ2 df
Model 1: Five factors 1,515.10 564 0.81 0.79 5,0580.44 0.03 1.08
Model 2: Three factors 3,525.65 958 0.64 0.61 8,2866.09 0.04 1.14 1,942.23 394
Model 3: One factor 5,436.24 982 0.37 0.33 8,5050.34 0.05 1.15 3,732.72 418
Model 1: negative affect, CWBI, and CWBO modelled at between- and within-person levels, primary and secondary psychopathy modelled at between-person level
only. Model 2: same as Model 1 except CWBI/CWBO and primary/secondary psychopathy modelled on one factor each. Model 3: all constructs modelled on one factor
at between-person level. CFI = comparative fit index; TLI = Tucker-Lewis index; AIC = Akaike information criterion; RMSEA = root mean square error of approximation;
SCF = scaling correction factor; S-B χ2 = Satorra-Bentler χ2, referring to comparison with the five-factor model.
8 M. SCHILBACH ET AL.

displays unstandardized path coefficients for the prediction of we found a positive link between primary psychopathy and
cognitive appraisal, one of our mediator variables. As hypothe- CWBI as well as CWBO. Difference tests showed that estimators
sized (H2a-c) and after controlling for neuroticism, secondary of primary and secondary psychopathy regarding CWBI (differ-
psychopathy was associated with a tendency to perceive ence score = −.017, p = .780) and CWBO (difference score = −.155,
reduced levels of challenge as well as increased levels of hin- p = .202) did not deviate significantly from one another.
drance and threat within the work context. Primary psychopa- To examine serial indirect effects of secondary psychopathy
thy was unrelated to any of the appraisal forms. to CWBI and CWBO via appraisal and negative affect (hypoth-
Additionally, lacking challenge appraisal as well as the pre- eses 5a-c) we computed Monte Carlo confidence intervals (CIs).
sence of hindrance and threat appraisal positively predicted For all serial mediation models, we included neuroticism as a
negative affectivity (hypotheses 3a-c; see Table 4). Negative control variable on both mediators as well as both forms of
affectivity in turn positively predicted CWBI and CWBO (hypoth- CWB and tested if the presumed mechanism would hold up for
esis 4a and 4b; see Table 4). All main effects became significant primary psychopathy. Serial mediations from secondary psy-
at within- as well as between-person level (except for hypoth- chopathy via appraisal and negative affect to CWBI were sig-
eses 1a and 1b since they were only tested at between-person nificant for the appraisal of work as unchallenging (indirect
level). Hence, our data supports hypotheses 1 to 4. Additionally, effect = 0.027, 95% CI = [0.013, 0.047]), as hindering (indirect

Table 3. Secondary Psychopathy Predicting Appraisal Tendencies.


Challenge Appraisal (r) Hindrance Appraisal Threat Appraisal
Estimate SE z p Estimate SE z p Estimate SE z p
Intercept 2.68 0.04 76.26 <.001 1.95 0.04 54.40 <.001 1.54 0.03 47.91 <.001
Primary Psychopathy 0.07 0.09 0.77 0.441 0.02 0.08 0.22 0.823 0.01 0.07 0.08 0.940
Secondary Psychopathy 0.43 0.08 5.15 <.001 0.30 0.08 3.72 <.001 0.25 0.08 3.25 0.001
Neuroticism 0.15 0.05 3.30 0.001 0.12 0.05 2.46 0.014 0.17 0.04 4.23 <.001
Residual Variance 0.40 0.04 11.01 <.001 0.42 0.02 10.73 <.001 0.36 0.04 8.69 <.001
Estimates of the tested model (2–1) are unstandardized and at between-person level. The item assessing challenge appraisal was inverted.

Table 4. Predicting Negative Affectivity and CWB.


Negative Affect CWBI CWBO
Estimate SE z p Estimate SE z p Estimate SE z p
PP/SP → NA/CWBI & CWBO
Intercept 1.26 0.02 81.75 <.001 1.31 0.02 77.63 <.001 1.82 0.03 62.65 <.001
Primary Psychopathy 0.01 0.03 0.29 0.776 0.11 0.04 2.80 0.005 0.16 0.07 2.17 0.030
Secondary Psychopathy 0.09 0.04 2.43 0.015 0.13 0.04 3.76 <.001 0.33 0.07 4.273 <.001
Neuroticism 0.13 0.02 5.90 <.001 0.02 0.02 0.92 0.358 0.03 0.04 0.81 0.419
Residual Variances 0.08 0.02 4.95 <.001 0.09 0.01 7.45 <.001 0.43 0.05 9.13 <.001
CH → NA/CWBI & CWBO
Intercept 0.96 0.09 10.58 <.001 0.93 0.09 10.70 <.001 1.04 0.15 7.09 <.001
Challenge Appraisal (between) 0.11 0.04 3.16 0.002 0.14 0.03 4.19 <.001 0.29 0.06 5.34 <.001
Neuroticism (between) 0.12 0.02 6.66 <.001 0.01 0.02 0.40 0.687 0.02 0.04 0.58 0.562
Residual Variances (between) 0.07 0.01 5.43 <.001 0.09 0.01 7.32 <.001 0.31 0.03 9.47 <.001
Challenge Appraisal (within) 0.08 0.02 5.36 <.001 0.05 0.02 2.91 0.004 0.14 0.02 6.62 <.001
Residual Variances (within) 0.09 0.01 10.06 <.001 0.12 0.01 12.47 <.001 0.21 0.01 16.37 <.001
HI → NA/CWBI & CWBO
Intercept 0.94 0.06 15.18 <.001 0.96 0.06 15.66 <.001 1.43 0.11 13.50 <.001
Hindrance Appraisal (between) 0.16 0.04 4.58 <.001 0.18 0.03 5.27 <.001 0.20 0.05 3.74 <.001
Neuroticism (between) 0.11 0.02 6.58 <.001 0.01 0.02 0.41 0.680 0.05 0.03 1.63 0.104
Residual Variances (between) 0.07 0.01 5.31 <.001 0.08 0.01 7.45 <.001 0.33 0.03 10.17 <.001
Hindrance Appraisal (within) 0.10 0.01 7.49 <.001 0.06 0.02 3.85 <.001 0.04 0.02 2.17 0.030
Residual Variances (within) 0.09 0.01 9.76 <.001 0.12 0.01 12.49 <.001 0.22 0.01 15.94 <.001
TH → NA/CWBI & CWBO
Intercept 0.82 0.06 14.33 <.001 0.93 0.06 16.09 <.001 1.38 0.09 15.76 <.001
Threat Appraisal (between) 0.28 0.04 7.02 <.001 0.25 0.04 6.17 <.001 0.29 0.06 5.07 <.001
Neuroticism (between) 0.08 0.02 4.75 <.001 −0.02 0.02 −0.82 0.413 0.03 0.03 0.82 0.41
Residual Variances (between) 0.05 0.01 5.21 <.001 0.08 0.01 8.11 <.001 0.31 0.03 10.22 <.001
Threat Appraisal (within) 0.15 0.02 7.15 <.001 0.08 0.02 4.00 <.001 0.12 0.02 5.00 <.001
Residual Variances (within) 0.08 0.01 9.80 <.001 0.12 0.01 12.48 <.001 0.21 0.01 15.80 <.001
NA → CWBI & CWBO
Intercept - - - - 0.50 0.12 4.24 <.001 1.02 0.18 5.60 <.001
Negative Affect (between) - - - - 0.64 0.10 6.57 <.001 0.64 0.15 4.29 <.001
Neuroticism (between) - - - - −0.05 0.02 −2.40 0.017 0.00 0.04 0.00 0.997
Residual Variances (between) - - - - 0.07 0.01 7.37 <.001 0.32 0.03 10.27 <.001
Negative Affect (within) - - - - 0.17 0.04 4.33 <.001 0.23 0.05 4.77 <.001
Residual Variances (within) - - - - 0.12 0.01 12.50 <.001 0.21 0.01 16.18 <.001
Estimates are unstandardized resulting from separate models. Main effects on CWBI and CWBO were tested in one model; main effects on negative affect were tested
separately. The item assessing challenge appraisal was inverted. PP = primary psychopathy, SP = secondary psychopathy, NA = negative affect, CH = challenge
appraisal, HI = hindrance appraisal, TH = threat appraisal.
EUROPEAN JOURNAL OF WORK AND ORGANIZATIONAL PSYCHOLOGY 9

effect = 0.026, 95% CI = [0.012, 0.048]) and as threatening our results support the idea of a general tendency in primary and
(indirect effect = 0.032, 95% CI = [0.010, 0.054]). Regarding secondary psychopathy to engage in deviant workplace beha-
CWBO we also found significant serial mediation via unchal- viours. This may be traced back to the key role of meanness in
lenged (indirect effect = 0.020, 95% CI = [0.013, 0.047]), hin- both psychopathy dimensions (Drislane, Patrick, Arsal et al., 2014).
drance (indirect effect = 0.022, 95% CI = [0.008, 0.044]), and In fact, when using the LSRP, Drislane, Patrick, Arsal et al. (2014) do
threat appraisal (indirect effect = 0.025; 95% CI = [0.002, 0.059]). not report a positive relationship of primary psychopathy with
For primary psychopathy, serial mediation to CWBI or CWBO boldness but only with meanness. This detracts a potentially
did not become significant for unchallenged, hindrance and protective factor from the primary psychopathy dimension. The
threat appraisal. This is due to the absent main effects of operationalization of the construct via the LSRP may thus pose a
primary psychopathy on any of the appraisal forms. The pre- possible explanation for the relationships found between primary
sumed serial mediation models explained between 39% and and secondary psychopathy and CWB. In any case, engaging in
40% of the variance in CWBI and 18% and 21% of the variance CWB may be due to different motives in primary and secondary
in CWBO. psychopathy. While individuals with primary psychopathic ten-
To test for the robustness of our results, we conducted dencies may engage in CWB strategically, secondary psychopathy
further analyses separately for the primary and secondary psy- may positively relate to spontaneous and impulsive acts of CWBs
chopathy dimensions, as well as without the control variable. (Wu & LeBreton, 2011).
The pattern of results remained the same with and without the To our knowledge, our study is the first to provide an
inclusion of the other psychopathy dimension, as well as con- explanatory mechanism as to why the relations between sec-
trol variable. In any case, secondary psychopathy related to the ondary psychopathy and CWBs appear to be highly prevalent.
unfavourable appraisal pattern. Results of the serial mediations Based on the definition of psychopathic personality (i.e., dis-
also persisted. Primary psychopathy still did not systematically torted perception of the environment as well as negative affect
relate to the appraisal forms. Detailed results are available as an in secondary psychopathy; Cleckley, 1976; Hare, 1996; Patrick et
online supplementary. al., 1993; Walker & Jackson, 2017), we developed a procedural
understanding of the link between the personality trait and
behaviour. Our results indicate that secondary psychopathy
Discussion
enhances dysfunctional appraisal tendencies. In line with our
The present study examined possible explanatory mechanisms hypotheses, individuals scoring higher on secondary psycho-
linking secondary psychopathy with CWBI and CWBO. pathy appraised their workday as significantly less challenging
Specifically, we hypothesized that secondary psychopathy is and simultaneously more hindering and threatening. This cog-
associated with unfavourable cognitive appraisal tendencies nitive appraisal pattern was in turn associated with increased
(i.e., reduced challenge and increased hindrance and threat negative affect throughout the workday. Results at the within-
appraisal of the workday). Based on the emotion-centred person level (Table 4) show on days where individuals appraise
model of voluntary work behaviour (Spector & Fox, 2002) we their workday as less challenging, more hindering and threa-
argued that these appraisal patterns positively relate to nega- tening, they report higher levels of negative affectivity.
tive affectivity, which in turn may be a precursor for CWBI and Furthermore, days where employees experienced increased
CWBO. We further tested if the presumed mechanism would levels of negative affect were associated with elevated usage
also apply to primary psychopathy. Results strongly support the of CWBI and CWBO. Serial mediation analysis based on the
hypothesized process for secondary but not for primary MSEM framework integrates the hypothesized main effects,
psychopathy. highlighting an unfavourable cognitive-affective process,
whereby individuals scoring higher on secondary psychopathy
are more likely to engage in CWBI and CWBO. This did not apply
Theoretical contributions
to primary psychopathy. Primary psychopathy was unrelated to
Empirical evidence outlines the potentially harmful effect of cor- any of the cognitive appraisal forms and hence, was not char-
porate psychopathy onto the work environment in the form of acterized by unfavourable appraisal tendencies. Based on key
CWB (Blickle & Schütte, 2017; Boddy et al., 2015, 2015; Walker & characteristics of primary psychopathy i.e., boldness, resilience
Jackson, 2017). In this context, only a few studies distinguished and reduced stress vulnerability (Hall et al., 2004; Patrick et al.,
between primary and secondary psychopathy. If a distinction was 2009), one could assume that individuals exhibiting primary
made, results indicate that primary psychopathy positively pre- psychopathic traits would tend to experience reduced levels
dicts CWBs only when political skills and educational levels are low of threats within the work environment. Since this was not the
(Blickle & Schütte, 2017; Schütte et al., 2018). Secondary psycho- case, it leaves us with the question of how primary psychopathy
pathy however positively relates to both forms of CWB regardless may affect work related perceptions and behaviours. Possibly,
of other interpersonal characteristics (Blickle & Schütte, 2017; due to factors related to primary psychopathy such as a com-
Schütte et al., 2018). Within our sample, we replicated this main monly high level of intelligence (Cleckley, 1976; Salekin et al.,
effect, showing that individuals possessing secondary psychopa- 2004), balanced levels of inhibition and thus lower reflection
thy characteristics are more likely to engage in CWBI and CWBO. impulsivity (Neo et al., 2018) individuals exhibiting primary
Additionally, and without controlling for other variables, we found psychopathy might be able to better analyse their environment
a positive link between primary psychopathy and both forms of and extract available resources. If so, there is no general apprai-
CWB. The strengths of the relationships between psychopathy sal tendency but rather an ability to evaluate each situation
and CWB did not differ for the psychopathy dimensions. Hence, depending on situational characteristics. In any case, our results
10 M. SCHILBACH ET AL.

highlight that the different dimensions of corporate psychopa- changing environments combined with new data regarding
thy are characterized by differing cognitive-affective processes, one’s reaction may initiate reappraisal processes. If reappraisal
providing an explanation as to why secondary psychopathy occurs, the original appraisal changes (E. Garland et al., 2009).
may be a pivotal precursor of CWB. Various studies have shown positive reappraisal can be increased
Besides contributing to procedural knowledge about harm- via training of specific techniques, such as mindfulness (E. L.
ful behaviours in secondary psychopathy, our study adds to the Garland et al., 2015). If individuals exhibiting secondary psycho-
emotion-centred model of voluntary work behaviour (Tett & pathy tendencies succeed in reappraising their workday as more
Guterman, 2000). Regarding secondary psychopathy, we found challenging, levels of positive affect may increase which in turn
that the presumed process persisted, strengthening the ratio- may decrease CWBs. Thus, we suggest the implementation of
nale of the theoretical model for this specific personality trait. cognitive reappraisal trainings when it comes to minimizing the
Further, we specified the general term of appraisal used by negative impact of secondary psychopathy within the work
Spector and Fox (2002). Based on Lazarus and Folkman context. In this context we would like to note that cognitive
(1984), we defined appraisal as the appraisal of a workday as reappraisal may be particularly challenging for individuals with
unchallenging, hindering or threatening. Results showed that secondary psychopathic tendencies due to high levels of disin-
all appraisal forms positively related to negative affect on the hibition and reflection impulsivity. Hence effectiveness of such
within- as well as between-person level. These results highlight training programmes would have to be carefully evaluated.
the importance of individuals’ work perception regarding daily In addition, organizations could undertake measures that
and general personal well-being as well as work related facilitate challenge appraisal of the workday. For individuals to
behaviour. perceive their workday as challenging, the availability of ade-
quate resources to cope with daily job demands must be secured
(Bakker & Demerouti, 2017). Hence, organizations could analyse
Practical implications
resource availability and adequacy. If job resources do not match
In terms of practical implications, our study allows the deriva- job demands, organizations can implement resource-enhancing
tion of suggestions for personnel selection, staff training, and schemes and/or reduce job demands.
workplace organization. To begin with, our results highlight the
potentially negative effects of secondary psychopathy. High
Limitations and direction for future research
levels of secondary psychopathy in employees therefore seem
undesirable. However, with respect to personnel selection, con- Despite the study’s contributions, it is not without limitations.
sideration of the position in question is not to be neglected. First, we assessed all variables with self-report measures. This
Depending on the job, secondary psychopathy may have may raise concerns about common method bias. To minimize
greater or smaller impacts on the work environment. For exam- potential distortion of the results, we tested all our within-
ple, the work of a nurse requires a great amount of patience, person effects simultaneously at within- and between-person
compassion, commitment, humility, and conscience (Mayeroff, levels. As Sonnentag et al. (2019) point out, this approach
1971; Roach, 1992). Secondary psychopathy is characterized by eliminates the possibility of interindividual differences (e.g.,
rather contraindicative features (i.e., impulsivity, lacking ability social desirability) influencing intraindividual findings. Further
to commit, and reduced levels of empathy (Hare, 2003; and to reduce common method bias our dependent variable
Levenson et al., 1995). Thus, individuals scoring high on sec- was measured before participants went to bed while the other
ondary psychopathy do not seem suitable for a job in the variables were measured right at the end of a workday or as is
nursing sector. In our opinion, the same applies to leadership the case for primary and secondary psychopathy as part of the
positions. Leaders are in the position to influence their fol- initial survey. However, these differing assessment points do
lowers’ behaviour (Ilies et al., 2007; Schyns & Schilling, 2013, not allow for a causal interpretation. The order of variables in
2013). Via social learning (Bandura, 1979) a leaders’ CWB may our serial mediation model is theory driven, but the design of
be carried out by followers’ and potentially multiplies within an the study does not provide a conclusive proof for a certain
organization. Further, studies suggest that experienced incivi- chain of mechanisms. Therefore, future research should aim
lity may lead to perpetrated incivility which in turn negatively to further clarify chains of causality for example, by assessing
affects work-related outcomes and employee well-being appraisals as well as negative affect at noon and CWB at the
(Bowling & Beehr, 2006; Gallus et al., 2014; Schyns & Schilling, end of a workday. Additionally, to minimize common method
2013). We therefore suggest using nonclinical measures, such bias future research should include other-person ratings in their
as the Levenson Psychopathy Scale (Levenson et al., 1995) or survey. For example, one could ask co-workers to answer ques-
the Psychopathy Personality Inventory (Lilienfeld & Andrews, tions regarding the level of conducted CWB by their colleagues.
1996) to determine levels of secondary psychopathy in jobs Second, our study design does not allow us to draw causal
where this trait is expected to have particularly strong negative statements. We measured both mediators, cognitive appraisal
effects on others. and negative affect, at the same point in time (i.e., at the end of
The reported unfavourable pattern of cognitive appraisal in a workday). Hence, with respect to the proposed serial media-
secondary psychopathy leads to another practical implication. tion, there can be no definite statement regarding its causality.
Although personality traits are relatively stable (McCrae & Costa, On the other hand, we believe a temporal separation of cogni-
1994), cognitive appraisal is dynamic and mutable (E. Garland et tive appraisal and the resulting affect is not appropriate since
al., 2009; Lazarus & Folkman, 1984). Novel information from cognitions and emotions closely relate to one another on a
EUROPEAN JOURNAL OF WORK AND ORGANIZATIONAL PSYCHOLOGY 11

time dimension (Lazarus, 1982; Lazarus & Folkman, 1984). hindrance and threat appraisal) and hence translate the result-
Third, we believe self-selection bias may be present in our ing positive affect into positive behaviours.
sample and affect levels of primary and secondary psychopa-
thy. Daily-diary studies require high levels of compliance as well
Conclusion
as commitment (Ohly et al., 2010). Since previous studies report
a lack of planning behaviour and commitment especially in In summary, this study brought to light an explanatory
secondary psychopathy (Levenson et al., 1995), one can assume mechanism as to why secondary psychopathy positively relates
that individuals scoring high on secondary psychopathy would to CWB. Secondary psychopathy was characterized by dysfunc-
refrain from taking part in such a study. Our relatively low tional cognitive-affective patterns within the work context,
sample means of primary and secondary psychopathy support which in turn behaviourally translated into CWB. The gained
this assumption. Since we were interested in analysing psycho- procedural understanding delivers a starting point for organi-
pathy on a continuum and our analyses are based on correla- zations that aim to minimize the negative effects of employee
tive interrelations, we do not consider this to be a major secondary psychopathy. Additionally, results highlight that cor-
weakness. However, future studies could use more sensitive porate primary psychopathy was unrelated to unfavourable
instruments where answers can be provided on a scale ranging appraisal tendencies. Our findings gave rise to a number of
between 1 and 7. This would allow for greater variation with questions including, the role of trait-activation within the emo-
respect to socially undesirable behaviours (e.g., CWB) in a tion-centred model of voluntary work behaviour, which we will
socially adapted sample. Future research should further test if attempt to answer in future research. We further hope that our
the proposed research model applies to individuals scoring in study initiates future research intended to identify organiza-
the top 5% to 10% of employee secondary psychopathy. tional and personal features, which could help individuals with
Additionally, future research should examine boundary con- tendencies in secondary psychopathy to behave in socially
ditions facilitating positive cognitive (re)appraisal of the work- compatible ways.
day. For instance, various job resources, such as control
(Karasek, 1979), feedback (Bakker & Demerouti, 2007), or
Disclosure Statement
health-oriented leadership style (Franke et al., 2014) may affect
the way individuals with secondary psychopathy tendencies No potential conflict of interest was reported by the authors.
perceive their workday. This could also be of particular interest
regarding corporate primary psychopathy. We suggest a careful
ORCID
evaluation of demands and resources as well as analysing the
perception of these job conditions when attempting to better Miriam Schilbach http://orcid.org/0000-0001-5035-0664
comprehend behavioural aspects of corporate primary psycho- Thomas Rigotti http://orcid.org/0000-0001-9189-0018
pathy. Kaiser et al. (2015) already followed a similar approach
when analysing conditions under which the dark side of per- References
sonality is likely to have counterproductive effects. On a theo-
retical level, the concept of trait activation (Tett & Guterman, Akhtar, R., Ahmetoglu, G., & Chamorro-Premuzic, T. (2013). Greed is good?
2000) could be integrated within the emotion-centred model of Assessing the relationship between entrepreneurship and subclinical
psychopathy. Personality and Individual Differences, 54(3), 420–425.
voluntary work behaviour (Spector & Fox, 2002). Trait activation https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2012.10.013
focuses on the interplay between individuals’ traits and situa- Aron, A. R., Robbins, T. W., & Poldrack, R. A. (2004). Inhibition and the right
tional characteristics stating that trait-relevant situations pro- inferior frontal cortex. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8(4), 170–177. https://
voke the behavioural expression of that trait. Possibly, for doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2004.02.010
individuals with primary psychopathic tendencies, trait relevant Babiak, P., & Hare, R. D. (2006). Snakes in suits: When psychopaths go to work.
HarperCollins.
situations trigger a specific form of cognitive appraisal which in Bakker, A. B., & Demerouti, E. (2007). The job demands-resources model:
turn may affect emotions and behaviour (Spector & Fox, 2002). State of the art. Journal of Managerial Psychology, 22(3), 309–328. https://
Further, we suggest future research to analyse our proposed doi.org/10.1108/02683940710733115
model considering the other two dark triad traits narcissism Bakker, A. B., & Demerouti, E. (2017). Job demands-resources theory: Taking
and machiavelism (Paulhus & Williams, 2002). Both positively stock and looking forward. Journal of Occupational Health Psychology, 22
(3), 273–285. https://doi.org/10.1037/ocp0000056
relate to CWB (O’Boyle et al., 2012). Regarding narcissism it Baloch, M. A., Meng, F., Xu, Z., Cepeda-Carrion, I., Danish,, & Bari, M. W.
would be important to consider it’s multidimensionality by (2017). Dark triad, perceptions of organizational politics and counter-
distinguishing between grandiose and hypersensitive narcis- productive work behaviors: The moderating effect of political skills.
sism (Russ et al., 2008). Similarly to primary and secondary Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 1972. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.
psychopathy, this would allow a comparison of cognitive-affec- 01972
Bandura, A. (1979). Sozial-kognitive Lerntheorie [Social-cognitive learning
tive processes at work. One could also transfer our model theory] (1st. Ed.). Konzepte der Humanwissenschaften. Klett-Cotta.
towards positive psychology and positive work behaviours by Bandura, A. (1994). Self-efficacy. In V. S. Ramachaudran (Ed.), Encyclopedia of
analysing the role of personality-influenced cognitive appraisal human behavior (pp. 71–81). Academic Press.
patterns with respect to organizational citizenship behaviour or Benning, S. D., Patrick, C. J., Hicks, B. M., Blonigen, D. M., & Krueger, R. F.
work engagement (Smith et al., 1983). Possibly, features asso- (2003). Factor structure of the psychopathic personality inventory:
Validity and implications for clinical assessment. Psychological
ciated with higher levels of resources, such as self-efficacy Assessment, 15(3), 340–350. https://doi.org/10.1037/1040-3590.15.3.340
(Bandura, 1994) or resilience (Britt et al., 2016), are character- Bjørnebekk, G., & Gjesme, T. (2009). Future time orientation and tempera-
ized by favourable appraisal patterns (i.e., more challenge, less ment: Exploration of their relationship to primary and secondary
12 M. SCHILBACH ET AL.

psychopathy. Psychological Reports, 105(1), 275–292. https://doi.org/10. Drislane, L. E., Patrick, C. J., Sourander, A., Sillanmäki, L., Aggen, S. H.,
2466/PR0.105.1.275-292 Elonheimo, H., Parkkola, K., Multimäki, P., & Kendler, K. S. (2014).
Blickle, G., & Schütte, N. (2017). Trait psychopathy, task performance, and Distinct variants of extreme psychopathic individuals in society at
counterproductive work behavior directed toward the organization. large: Evidence from a population-based sample. Personality Disorders,
Personality and Individual Differences, 109, 225–231. https://doi.org/10. 5(2), 154–163. https://doi.org/10.1037/per0000060
1016/j.paid.2017.01.006 Falkenbach, D., Poythress, N., & Creevy, C. (2008). The exploration of sub-
Block, J. A., Santa Ana, E., Rodriguez, M. L., & Colsman, M. L. (2002). Delay of clinical psychopathic subtypes and the relationship with types of
gratification: Impulsive choices and problem behaviors in early and late aggression. Personality and Individual Differences, 44(4), 821–832.
adolescence. Journal of Personality, 70(4), 533–552. https://doi.org/10. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2007.10.012
1111/1467-6494.05013 Falkenbach, D., Poythress, N., Falki, M., & Manchak, S. (2007). Reliability and
Boddy, C., Miles, D., Sanyal, C., & Hartog, M. (2015). Extreme managers, validity of two self-report measures of psychopathy. Assessment, 14(4),
extreme workplaces: Capitalism, organizations and corporate psycho- 341–350. https://doi.org/10.1177/1073191107305612
paths. Organization, 22(4), 530–551. https://doi.org/10.1177/ Feldman, P. J., Cohen, S., Hamrick, N., & Lepore, S. J. (2004). Psychological
1350508415572508 stress, appraisal, emotion and Cardiovascular response in a public speak-
Boddy, C. R. (2014). Corporate psychopaths, conflict, employee affective ing task. Psychology & Health, 19(3), 353–368. https://doi.org/10.1080/
well-being and counterproductive work behaviour. Journal of Business 0887044042000193497
Ethics, 121(1), 107–121. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10551-013-1688-0 Folkman, S., & Lazarus, R. S. (1987). Transactional theory and research on
Boddy, C. R., & Taplin, R. (2016). The influence of corporate psychopaths on emotions and coping. European Journal of Personality, 1(3), 141–169.
job satisfaction and its determinants. International Journal of Manpower, https://doi.org/10.1002/per.2410010304
37(6), 965–988. https://doi.org/10.1108/IJM-12-2015-0199 Folkman, S., Schaefer, C., & Lazarus, R. S. (1979). Cognitive processes as
Bowling, N. A., & Beehr, T. A. (2006). Workplace harassment from the victim’s mediators of stress and coping. In V. Hamilton & D. M. Warburton (Eds.),
perspective: A theoretical model and meta-analysis. Journal of Applied Human stress and cognition (pp. 265–298). Wiley.
Psychology, 91(5), 998–1012. https://doi.org/10.1037/0021-9010.91.5.998 Forster, N., & Lund, D. W. (2018). Identifying and dealing with functional
Brislin, R. W. (1970). Back-translation for cross-cultural research. Journal of psychopathic behavior in higher education. Global Business and
Cross-Cultural Psychology, 1(3), 185–216. https://doi.org/10.1177/ Organizational Excellence, 38(1), 22–31. https://doi.org/10.1002/joe.v38.1
135910457000100301 Fowles, D. C., & Dindo, L. (2009). Temperament and Psychopathy: A dual-
Britt, T. W., Shen, W., Sinclair, R. R., Grossman, M. R., & Klieger, D. M. (2016). pathway model. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 18(3), 179–
How much do we really know about employee resilience? Industrial and 183. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8721.2009.01632.x
Organizational Psychology, 9(2), 378–404. https://doi.org/10.1017/iop. Fox, S., Spector, P. E., & Miles, D. (2001). Counterproductive Work Behavior
2015.107 (CWB) in response to job stressors and organizational justice: Some med-
Byrne, B. M. (2001). Structural equation modeling with AMOS: Basic concepts, iator and moderator tests for autonomy and emotions. Journal of Vocational
applications, and programming. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Behavior, 59(3), 291–309. https://doi.org/10.1006/jvbe.2001.1803
Cartwritght, S., & Cooper, C. L. (1997). Managing workplace stress. SAGE Franke, F., Felfe, J., & Pundt, A. (2014). The impact of health-oriented leader-
Publications. ship on follower health development and test of a new instrument
Carver, C. S., & Scheier, M. F. (1990). Origins and functions of positive and measuring health-promoting leadership. Zeitschrift für Personalforschung,
negative affect: A control-process view. Personnel Review, 97(1), 19–35. 28(1–2), 139–161. https://doi.org/10.1177/239700221402800108
Cavanaugh, M. A., Boswell, W. R., Roehling, M. V., & Boudreau, J. W. (2000). Gallagher, D. J. (1990). Extraversion, neuroticism and appraisal of stressful
An empirical examination of self-reported work stress among U.S. man- academic events. Personality and Individual Differences, 11(10), 1053–
agers. Journal of Applied Psychology, 85(1), 65–74. https://doi.org/10. 1057. https://doi.org/10.1016/0191-8869(90)90133-C
1037/0021-9010.85.1.65 Gallus, J. A., Bunk, J. A., Matthews, R. A., Barnes-Farrell, J. L., & Magley, V. J.
Chapman, David, W., & Carter (1979). Translation procedures for the (2014). An eye for an eye? Exploring the relationship between workplace
cross cultural use of measurement instruments. Educational incivility experiences and perpetration. Journal of Occupational Health
Evaluation and Policy Analysis, 1(3), 71–78. https://doi.org/10.3102/ Psychology, 19(2), 143–154. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0035931
01623737001003071 Garland, E., Gaylord, S., & Park, J. (2009). The role of mindfulness in positive
Clark, L., Robbins, T. W., Ersche, K. D., & Sahakian, B. J. (2006). Reflection reappraisal. Explore (New York, N.Y.), 5(1), 37–44. https://doi.org/10.1016/
impulsivity in current and former substance users. Biological Psychiatry, j.explore.2008.10.001
60(5), 515–522. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2005.11.007 Garland, E. L., Hanley, A., Farb, N. A., & Froeliger, B. E. (2015). State mind-
Cleckley, H. (1976). The mask of sanity (5th ed. ed.). Mosby. fulness during meditation predicts enhanced cognitive reappraisal.
Coid, J., Freestone, M., & Ullrich, S. (2012). Subtypes of psychopathy in the Mindfulness, 6(2), 234–242. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12671-013-0250-6
British household population: Findings from the national household Gudjonsson, G. H., & Roberts, J. C. (1983). Guilt and self-concept in ‘second-
survey of psychiatric morbidity. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric ary psychopaths’. Personality and Individual Differences, 4(1), 65–70.
Epidemiology, 47(6), 879–891. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00127-011-0395-3 https://doi.org/10.1016/0191-8869(83)90053-3
Dalal, R., Lam, H., Weiss, H. M., Welch, E. R., & Hulin, C. L. (2009). A within- Gunthert, K. C., Cohen, L. H., & Armeli, S. (1999). The role of neuroticism in
person approach to work behavior and performance: Concurrent and daily stress and coping. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 77
lagged citizenship-counterproductivity associations, and dynamic rela- (5), 1087–1100. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.77.5.1087
tionships with affect and overall job performance. Academy of Hall, J. R., Benning, S. D., & Patrick, C. J. (2004). Criterion-related validity of
Management Journal, 52(5), 1051–1066. https://doi.org/10.5465/amj. the three-factor model of psychopathy: Personality, behavior, and adap-
2009.44636148 tive functioning. Assessment, 11(1), 4–16. https://doi.org/10.1177/
Daniels, K., Harris, C., & Briner, R. B. (2004). Linking work conditions to 1073191103261466
unpleasant affect: Cognition, categorization and goals. Journal of Hare, R. D. (1996). Psychopathy: A clinical construct whose time has come.
Occupational and Organizational Psychology, 77(3), 343–363. https:// Criminal Justice and Behavior, 23(1), 25–54. https://doi.org/10.1177/
doi.org/10.1348/0963179041752628 0093854896023001004
Del Gaizo, A. L., & Falkenbach, D. M. (2008). Primary and secondary psycho- Hare, R. D. (2003). Manual for the hare psychopathy checklist-revised (2nd
pathic-traits and their relationship to perception and experience of ed.). Multi-Health Systems.
emotion. Personality and Individual Differences, 45(3), 206–212. https:// Harpur, T. J., Hare, R. D., & Hakstian, A. R. (1989). Two-factor conceptualiza-
doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2008.03.019 tion of psychopathy: Construct validity and assessment implications.
Drislane, L. E., Patrick, C. J., & Arsal, G. (2014). Clarifying the content cover- Psychological Assessment: A Journal of Consulting and Clinical
age of differing psychopathy inventories through reference to the Psychology, 1(1), 6–17. https://doi.org/10.1037/1040-3590.1.1.6
Triarchic Psychopathy Measure. Psychological Assessment, 26(2), 350– Hu, L.-T., & Bentler, P. M. (1998). Fit indices in covariance structure model-
362. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0035152 ing: Sensitivity to underparameterized model misspecification.
EUROPEAN JOURNAL OF WORK AND ORGANIZATIONAL PSYCHOLOGY 13

Psychological Methods, 3(4), 424–453. https://doi.org/10.1037/1082- Lim, S., Cortina, L. M., & Magley, V. J. (2008). Personal and workgroup
989X.3.4.424 incivility: Impact on work and health outcomes. Journal of Applied
Hu, L.-T., & Bentler, P. M. (1999). Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance Psychology, 93(1), 95–107. https://doi.org/10.1037/0021-9010.93.1.95
structure analysis: Conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Lykken, D. T. (1995). The antisocial personalities. Erlbaum.
Structural Equation Modeling: A Multidisciplinary Journal, 6(1), 1–55. Lynam, D. R., Whiteside, S., & Jones, S. (1999). Self-reported psychopathy: A
https://doi.org/10.1080/10705519909540118 validation study. Journal of Personality Assessment, 73(1), 110–132.
Ilies, R., Nahrgang, J. D., & Morgeson, F. P. (2007). Leader-member exchange https://doi.org/10.1207/S15327752JPA730108
and citizenship behaviors: A meta-analysis. Journal of Applied Marcus, D. K., Fulton, J. J., & Edens, J. F. (2012). “The two-factor model of
Psychology, 92(1), 269–277. https://doi.org/10.1037/0021-9010.92.1.269 psychopathic personality: Evidence from the psychopathic personality
Jonason, P. K., & Ferrell, J. D. (2016). Looking under the hood: The psycho- inventory”: correction to Marcus, Fulton, and Edens (2011). Personality
genic motivational foundations of the Dark Triad. Personality and Disorders: Theory, Research, and Treatment, 3(2), 195. https://doi.org/10.
Individual Differences, 94, 324–331. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2016. 1037/a0027132
01.039 Marsh, H. W., Hau, K.-T., & Wen, Z. (2004). In search of golden rules:
Kagan, J. (1966). Reflection–impulsivity: The generality and dynamics of Comment on hypothesis-testing approaches to setting cutoff values
conceptual tempo. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 71(1), 17–24. for fit indexes and dangers in overgeneralizing Hu and Bentler’s (1999)
https://doi.org/10.1037/h0022886 findings. Structural Equation Modeling: A Multidisciplinary Journal, 11(3),
Kaiser, R. B., LeBreton, J. M., & Hogan, J. (2015). The dark side of personality 320–341. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15328007sem1103_2
and extreme leader behavior. Applied Psychology: An International Mathieu, C., Neumann, C. S., Hare, R. D., & Babiak, P. (2014). A dark side of
Review, 64(1), 55–92. https://doi.org/10.1111/apps.2015.64.issue-1 leadership: Corporate psychopathy and its influence on employee well-
Karasek, R. A. (1979). Job demands, job decision latitude, and mental strain: being and job satisfaction. Personality and Individual Differences, 59, 83–
Implications for job redesign. Administrative Science Quarterly, 24(2), 285. 88. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2013.11.010
https://doi.org/10.2307/2392498 Mayeroff, M. (1971). On caring. Harper & Row.
Karpman, B. (1941). On the need of separating psychopathy into two McCrae, R. R., & Costa, P. T. (1994). The stability of personality: Observations
distinct clinical types: The symptomatic and the idiopathic. Journal of and evaluations. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 3(6), 173–
Criminology and Psychopathology, 3, 112–137. 175. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-8721.ep10770693
Kivimäki, M., Virtanen, M., Vartia, M., Elovainio, M., Vahtera, J., & Keltikangas- McGee, M. K., & Fillon, M. (1995). Honesty is still the best policy. Information
Järvinen, L. (2003). Workplace bullying and the risk of cardiovascular Week, 519, 156.
disease and depression. Occupational and Environmental Medicine, 60 Mikkelsen, E. G., & Einarsen, S. (2002). Relationships between exposure to
(10), 779–783. https://doi.org/10.1136/oem.60.10.779 bullying at work and psychological and psychosomatic health com-
Krohne, H. W., Egloff, B., Kohlmann, C.-W., & Tausch, A. (1996). plaints: The role of state negative affectivity and generalized self-effi-
Untersuchungen mit einer deutschen Version der “Positive and cacy. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 43(5), 397–405. https://doi.org/
Negative Affect Schedule” (PANAS). Diagnostica, 42(2), 139–156. doi: 10.1111/sjop.2002.43.issue-5
10.1037/t49650-000 Miller, J. D., Dir, A., Gentile, B., Wilson, L., Pryor, L. R., & Campbell, W. K.
Lawrence, J. W., Carver, C. S., & Scheier, M. F. (2002). Velocity toward goal (2010). Searching for a vulnerable dark triad: Comparing factor 2 psycho-
attainment inimmediate experience as a determinant of affect. Journal pathy, vulnerable narcissism, and borderline personality disorder.
of Applied Psychology, 32(4), 788–802. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1559- Journal of Personality, 78(5), 1529–1564. https://doi.org/10.1111/jopy.
1816.2002.tb00242.x 2010.78.issue-5
Lazarus, R. S. (1982). Thoughts on the relations between emotion and Miller, J. D., & Lynam, D. R. (2012). An examination of the psychopathic
cognition. American Psychologist, 37(9), 1019–1024. https://doi.org/10. personality inventory’s nomological network: A meta-analytic review.
1037/0003-066X.37.9.1019 Personality Disorders, 3(3), 305–326. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0024567
Lazarus, R. S. (1991). Emotion and Adaptation. Oxford Univ. Press. Moise, G. (2014). Good stress or bad stress? Relationships between stress
Lazarus, R. S., & Folkman, S. (1984). Stress, appraisal, and coping. Springer. appraisals and strains in health care practitioners [ProQuest
Lepine, J. A., Podsakoff, N. P., & Lepine, M. A. (2005). A meta-analytic test of Dissertations & Theses Global]. (UMI No. 1563382680).
the challenge stressor-hindrance stressor framework: An explanation for Murphy, K. R. (1993). The cypress series in work and science. Honesty in the
inconsistent relationships among stressors and performance. Academy workplace. Thomson Brooks/Cole Publishing Co.
of Management Journal, 48(5), 764–775. https://doi.org/10.5465/amj. Muthén, L. K., & Muthén, B. O. (1998-2015). Mplus user’s guide (7th ed.).
2005.18803921 Muthén & Muthén.
Levenson, M. R., Kiehl, K. A., & Fitzpatrick, C. M. (1995). Asessing psycho- Neo, B., Sellbom, M., Smith, S. F., & Lilienfeld, S. O. (2018). Of boldness and
pathy attributes in a noninstitutionalized population. Journal of badness: Insights into workplace malfeasance from a triarchic psycho-
Personality and Social Psychology, 68(1), 151–158. https://doi.org/10. pathy model perspective. Journal of Business Ethics, 149(1), 187–205.
1037/0022-3514.68.1.151 https://doi.org/10.1007/s10551-016-3108-8
Lilienfeld, S. O., & Andrews, B. P. (1996). Development and preliminary O’Boyle, E. H., Forsyth, D. R., Banks, G. C., & McDaniel, M. A. (2012). A meta-
validation of a self-report measure of psychopathic personality traits in analysis of the Dark Triad and work behavior: A social exchange perspective.
noncriminal populations. Journal of Personality Assessment, 66(3), 488– Journal of Applied Psychology, 97(3), 557–579. https://doi.org/10.1037/
524. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327752jpa6603_3 a0025679
Lilienfeld, S. O., Patrick, C. J., Benning, S. D., Berg, J., Sellbom, M., & Edens, J. Ohly, S., Sonnentag, S., Niessen, C., & Zapf, D. (2010). Diary studies in
F. (2012a). The role of fearless dominance in psychopathy: Confusions, organizational research. Journal of Personnel Psychology, 9(2), 79–93.
controversies, and clarifications. Personality Disorders: Theory, Research, https://doi.org/10.1027/1866-5888/a000009
and Treatment, 3(3), 327–340. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0026987 Palmer, J. C., Komarraju, M., Carter, M. Z., & Karau, S. J. (2017). Angel on one
Lilienfeld, S. O., Waldman, I. D., Landfield, K., Watts, A. L., Rubenzer, S., & shoulder: Can perceived organizational support moderate the relation-
Faschingbauer, T. R. (2012b). Fearless dominance and the U.S. presi- ship between the Dark Triad traits and counterproductive work beha-
dency: Implications of psychopathic personality traits for successful vior? Personality and Individual Differences, 110, 31–37. https://doi.org/
and unsuccessful political leadership. Journal of Personality and Social 10.1016/j.paid.2017.01.018
Psychology, 103(3), 489–505. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0029392 Patrick, C. J., Bradley, M. M., & Lang, P. J. (1993). Emotion in the criminal
Lilienfeld, S. O., Watts, A. L., & Smith, S. F. (2015). Successful psychopathy. psychopath: Startle reflex modulation. Journal of Abnormal Psychology,
Current Direction in Psychological Science, 24(4), 298–303. https://doi.org/ 102(1), 82–92. https://doi.org/10.1037/0021-843X.102.1.82
10.1177/0963721415580297 Patrick, C. J., Fowles, D. C., & Krueger, R. F. (2009). Triarchic conceptualiza-
Lilienfeld, S. O., & Widows, M. R. (2005). PPI-R: Psychopathic personality tion of psychopathy: Developmental origins of disinhibition, boldness,
inventory revised: Proffessional manual. Psychological Assessment and meanness. Development and Psychopathology, 21(3), 913–938.
Resources, Incorporated. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579409000492
14 M. SCHILBACH ET AL.

Paulhus, D. L., & Williams, K. M. (2002). The Dark Triad of personality: Narcissism, Semenyna, S. W., & Honey, P. L. (2015). Dominance styles mediate sex
machiavellianism, and psychopathy. Journal of Research in Personality, 36(6), differences in Dark Triad traits. Personality and Individual Differences,
556–563. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0092-6566(02)00505-6 83, 37–43. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2015.03.046
Payne, R. (1999). Stress at work: A conceptual framework. In J. Firth-Cozens Skeem, J., Johansson, P., Andershed, H., Kerr, M., & Louden, J. E. (2007). Two
& R. Payne (Eds.), Stress in health professionals: Psychological and organi- subtypes of psychopathic violent offenders that parallel primary and
sational causes and interventions (pp. 3–16). Wiley. secondary variants. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 116(2), 395–409.
Plummer, B. A. (2000). To parcel or not to parcel: The effects of item parceling https://doi.org/10.1037/0021-843X.116.2.395
in confirmatory factor analysis (Open Access Dissertations, paper 917). Skinner, N., & Brewer, N. (2002). The dynamics of threat and challenge
University of Rhode Island. https://digitalcommons.uri.edu/oa_diss/917 appraisals prior to stressful achievement events. Journal of Personality
Porath, C., Spreitzer, G., Gibson, C., & Garnett, F. G. (2012). Thriving at work: and Social Psychology, 83(3), 678–692. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-
Toward its measurement, construct validation, and theoretical refine- 3514.83.3.678
ment. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 33(2), 250–275. https://doi.org/ Smith, C. A., Organ, D. W., & Near, J. P. (1983). Organizational citizenship
10.1002/job.756 behavior: Its nature and antecedents. Journal of Applied Psychology, 68
Preacher, K. J., Zyphur, M. J., & Zhang, Z. (2010). A general multilevel SEM (4), 653–663. https://doi.org/10.1037/0021-9010.68.4.653
framework for assessing multilevel mediation. Psychological Methods, 15 Snowden, R. J., & Gray, N. S. (2011). Impulsivity and psychopathy:
(3), 209–233. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0020141 Associations between the barrett impulsivity scale and the psychopathy
Prem, R., Ohly, S., Kubicek, B., & Korunka, C. (2017). Thriving on challenge checklist revised. Psychiatry Research, 187(3), 414–417. https://doi.org/
stressors? Exploring time pressure and learning demands as antecedents 10.1016/j.psychres.2011.02.003
of thriving at work. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 38(1), 108–123. Sonnentag, S., Eck, K., Fritz, C., & Kühnel, J. (2019). Morning reattachment to
https://doi.org/10.1002/job.v38.1 work and work engagement during the day: A look at day-level media-
Questback GmbH. (2017). EFS survey, version summer 2017. tors. Journal of Management, 014920631982982. Epub ahead of print.
Rammstedt, B., & John, O. P. (2007). Measuring personality in one minute or https://doi.org/10.1177/0149206319829823
less: A 10-item short version of the big five inventory in English and Spector, P. E. (1978). Organizational frustration: A model and review of the
German. Journal of Research in Personality, 41(1), 203–212. https://doi. literature. Personnel Psychology, 31(4), 815–829. https://doi.org/10.1111/
org/10.1016/j.jrp.2006.02.001 peps.1978.31.issue-4
Roach, M. S. (1992). The human act of caring: A blueprint for the health Spector, P. E. (1998). A control theory of the job stress process. In C. L. Cooper
professions. Canadian Hospitals Association. (Ed.), Theories of organizational stress (pp. 153–169). Oxford Univ. Press.
Robinson, S. L., & Bennett, R. J. (1995). A typology of deviant workplace Spector, P. E., & Fox, S. (2002). An emotion-centered model of voluntary
behaviors: A multidimensional scaling study. Academy of Management work behavior. Human Resource Management Review, 12(2), 269–292.
Journal, 38(2), 555–572. doi:10.2307/256693 https://doi.org/10.1016/S1053-4822(02)00049-9
Ross, S. R., Lutz, C. J., & Bailley, S. E. (2004). Psychopathy and the five factor Tett, R. P., & Guterman, H. A. (2000). Situation trait relevance, trait expres-
model in a noninstitutionalized sample: A domain and facet fevel ana- sion, and cross-situational consistency: Testing a principle of trait activa-
lysis. Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment, 26(4), 213– tion. Journal of Research in Personality, 34(4), 397–423. https://doi.org/10.
223. https://doi.org/10.1023/B:JOBA.0000045337.48535.a5 1006/jrpe.2000.2292
Russ, E., Shedler, J., Bradley, R., & Westen, D. (2008). Refining the construct of Thompson, E. R. (2007). Development and validation of an internationally
narcissistic personality disorder: Diagnostic criteria and subtypes. The reliable short-form of the Positive and Negative Affect Schedule
American Journal of Psychiatry, 165(11), 1473–1481. https://doi.org/10. (PANAS). Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 38(2), 227–242. https://
1176/appi.ajp.2008.07030376 doi.org/10.1177/0022022106297301
Salekin, R. T., Neumann, C. S., Leistico, A.-M. R., & Zalot, A. A. (2004). Titze, J., Blickle, G., & Wihler, A. (2017). Fearless dominance and performance
Psychopathy in youth and intelligence: An investigation of Cleckley’s in field sales: A predictive study. International Journal of Selection and
hypothesis. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, 33(4), Assessment, 25(3), 299–310. https://doi.org/10.1111/ijsa.2017.25.issue-3
731–742. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15374424jccp3304_8 Tuckey, M. R., Searle, B. J., Boyd, C. M., Winefield, A. H., & Winefield, H. R.
Samnani, A.-K., Salamon, S. D., & Singh, P. (2014). Negative affect and (2015). Hindrances are not threats: Advancing the multidimensionality of
counterproductive workplace behavior: The moderating role of moral work stress. Journal of Occupational Health Psychology, 20(2), 131–147.
disengagement and gender. Journal of Business Ethics, 119(2), 235–244. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0038280
https://doi.org/10.1007/s10551-013-1635-0 Unterrainer, H.-F., Ruttinger, J., Lewis, A. J., Anglim, J., Fink, A., &
Schachter, S., & Singer, J. E. (1962). Cognitive, social, and physiological Kapfhammer, H.-P. (2016). Vulnerable Dark Triad personality facets are
determinants of emotional state. Psychological Review, 69(5), 379–399. associated with religious fundamentalist tendencies. Psychopathology,
https://doi.org/10.1037/h0046234 49(1), 47–52. https://doi.org/10.1159/000443901
Scherer, K. T., Baysinger, M., Zolynsky, D., & LeBreton, J. M. (2013). Predicting Walker, B. R., & Jackson, C. J. (2017). Moral emotions and corporate psycho-
counterproductive work behaviors with sub-clinical psychopathy: pathy: A review. Journal of Business Ethics, 141(4), 797–810. https://doi.
Beyond the five factor model of personality. Personality and Individual org/10.1007/s10551-016-3038-5
Differences, 55(3), 300–305. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2013.03.007 Wang, M., Liao, H., Zhan, Y., & Shi, J. (2011). Daily customer mistreatment
Schneider, T. R. (2004). The role of neuroticism on psychological and and employee sabotage against customers: Examining emotion and
physiological stress responses. Journal of Experimental Social resource perspectives. Academy of Management Journal, 54(2), 312–
Psychology, 40(6), 795–804. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2004.04.005 334. https://doi.org/10.5465/amj.2011.60263093
Schütte, N., Blickle, G., Frieder, R. E., Wihler, A., Schnitzler, F., Heupel, J., & Watson, D., Clark, L. A., & Tellegen, A. (1988). Development and validation of
Zettler, I. (2018). The role of interpersonal influence in counterbalancing brief measures of positive and negative affect: The PANAS scales. Journal
psychopathic personality trait facets at work. Journal of Management, 44 of Personality and Social Psychology, 54(6), 1063–1070. https://doi.org/10.
(4), 1338–1368. https://doi.org/10.1177/0149206315607967 1037/0022-3514.54.6.1063
Schyns, B., & Schilling, J. (2013). How bad are the effects of bad leaders? A Webster, J. R., Beehr, T. A., & Love, K. (2011). Extending the challenge-hindrance
meta-analysis of destructive leadership and its outcomes. The Leadership model of occupational stress: The role of appraisal. Journal of Vocational
Quarterly, 24(1), 138–158. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.leaqua.2012.09.001 Behavior, 79(2), 505–516. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jvb.2011.02.001
Searle, B. J., & Auton, J. C. (2015). The merits of measuring challenge and Wu, J., & LeBreton, J. M. (2011). Reconsidering the dispositional basis of counter-
hindrance appraisals. Anxiety, Stress, and Coping, 28(2), 121–143. https:// productive work behavior: The role of aberrant personality. Personnel
doi.org/10.1080/10615806.2014.931378 Psychology, 64(3), 593–626. https://doi.org/10.1111/peps.2011.64.issue-3

You might also like