You are on page 1of 9

International Journal of Behavioral Development © 2000 The International Society for the

2000, 24 (4), 398–406 Study of Behavioural Development


http://www.tandf.co.uk/journals/pp/01650254.htm l

Perspectives on gender development


Eleanor E. Maccoby
Stanford University, California, USA

Two traditional perspectives on gender development—the socialisation and cognitive perspectives—


are reviewed. It is noted that although they deal quite well with individual differences within each sex
with regard to degree of sex-typing, they do not offer satisfactory explanations for some of the most
robust gender dimorphisms: namely, gender segregation and the divergent patterns of interaction
within all-male as compared with all-female dyads or groups. These patterns are briey summarised,
and their similarity to those found in nonhuman primates and other mammals is noted. It is argued
that an ethological perspective, and its modern successor the psychobiological perspective, are
needed, along with the more traditional perspectives, to provide a comprehensive account of gender
development as it occurs in dyads and groups as well as within individual children.

In the last several decades there have been important shifts in toward the version of ‘‘masculine’’ behaviour deemed proper
psychologists’ thinking about gender development. There were in the particular society where the children were growing up.
empirical questions Žrst of all: In what ways, to what degree, Implicit in this viewpoint is the idea that individual
and how consistently, did boys and girls differ in the differences within each sex, and mean differences between the
developmental pathways typically taken? And there have been sexes on any given trait, are essentially reections of the same
notable changes in the points of view psychologists have processes. Thus, it would be assumed that if one aspect of
brought to bear in their efforts to understand and explain becoming masculine for a boy is to learn not to cry, then boys
whatever gender differentiation was thought to occur. Two on the average would be subject to more socialisation
viewpoints about gender development were dominant for many pressures, that is, would be told, more often than girls, ‘‘That
years: the socialisation perspective and the gender-cognitive didn’t really hurt’’, or ‘‘It’s only a scratch’’, or ‘‘Don’t be a
perspective. These perspectives are Žrst described below, and crybaby’’, or ‘‘Oh, toughen up!’’—and would develop stronger
then their limitations are pointed out, stressing how narrow inhibitions against crying then would girls. At the same time,
these views were concerning the nature of the gender- some boys would receive stronger, more consistent pressures of
differentiated phenomena that need to be understood. The this kind than others, and so some would develop stronger
paper turns then to considering how much an ethological crying inhibitions, and become more ‘‘masculine’’ than others.
perspective, when added to the traditional pair, can contribute Differential socialisation pressures on boys and girls could
towards achieving a more comprehensive view of gender take a variety of additional forms, beyond differential positive
development. Finally, some more recent thinking from and negative reinforcement: for example, via providing
psychobiology is brought to bear in the interests of moving different toys for children of the two sexes, or attributing
toward an integration of the several perspectives. different characteristics to them. And by giving boys and girls
distinctive names and dressing them differently, their gender
was announced to allcomers with the message: ‘‘This child is a
The direct socialisation perspective boy/girl, to be treated accordingly.’’ Distinctive socialisation
At mid century, psychologists asked: By what processes do pressures on boys and girls were thought to begin at birth, to be
children become ‘‘sex-typed?’’ By sex-typing, they usually fairly strong, and to be consistent in some respects throughout
meant that children take on the attributes that are typical and/ a given culture.
or valued (expected, normative) for their own sex. In seeking A secondary perspective which inuenced some of the
answers, they worked from the stimulus-response (S-R) developmental research at mid century was derived from
principles of the reinforcement learning theories that domi- psychodynamic theory. This theory was more developmental
nated the Želd of psychology at that time. From this point of than S-R learning theory, which did not specify particular
view, sex-typed behaviours were a set of habits. Boys and girls periods during growth that would be optimal for children’s
would develop different sex-typed habits if socialisation acquisition of sex-typed behaviours. Freud’s theory of psycho-
agents—parents, teachers, older children—reinforced girls for sexual development posited a transition at about the age of four
‘‘feminine’’ behaviours and provided negative consequences or Žve, when the resolution of Oedipal conicts would involve
when they displayed behaviours thought to be more appro- children’s identifying with their same-sex parent and thus
priate for boys. Similarly, boys were thought to be ‘‘shaped’’ taking on the appropriate sex-typed characteristics of that

Correspondence should be addressed to Dr Eleanor E. Maccoby,


Department of Psychology, Stanford University, 2130
Downloaded from Stanford, CA
jbd.sagepub.com at PENNSYLVANIA STATE UNIV on May 9, 2016
94305–2130, USA.
INTERNATIONAL JOURNAL OF BEHAVIORAL DEVELOPMENT, 2000, 24 (4), 398–406 399

parent. This transition was thought to be more difŽcult for infant is chosen whose sex cannot be easily identiŽed when the
boys, who had to break a prior identiŽcation with their child is dressed. Unfamiliar adults are then offered the
mothers, and in becoming masculine would need to distance opportunity to interact with the infant, or view the infant on
themselves from all things feminine. Echoes of this theory are videotape, the child having been introduced to some of the
found in the work of Chodorow (1978). In the 1950s and subjects as a girl, to others as a boy. The adults’ reactions to the
1960s, Robert Sears and colleagues (Sears, Maccoby, &Levin, child, and interpretations of the child’s behaviour, are then
1957; Sears, Rau, & Alpert, 1965) attempted to integrate S-R recorded. Such studies control for the direction-of-effects
and psychodynamic theories in their studies of the relation- problem, because differential eliciting properties of male and
ships between parental child-rearing practices and the degree female infants are ruled out by design. Although early studies
of children’s sex-typing. The results failed to conŽrm Freudian suggested that adults’ reactions and interpretations were
theory. As Sears and colleagues wrote, in summarising their indeed inuenced by the child’s gender label, a review of 23
Žndings: ‘‘The box score for primary identiŽcation theory as an gender-labelling studies (Stern & Karraker, 1989) found that
explanation of gender role is poor’’ (Sears et al., 1965, p. 194). overall effects were quite weak and quite inconsistent from
How well have the assumptions about children of the two study to study.
sexes being socialised differently stood up empirically? It is No doubt there are subtle differences in parental treatment
certainly true that parents give their children sex-typed names, that are not captured in the rather coarse-grained net that
dress them differently, and decorate their rooms differently. researchers have cast, and certainly what seem to be rather
Also, all languages of the world provide different ways of minor differences can accumulate over many repetitions into
speaking about male and female persons. In English and other signiŽcant inuences on children. And surely, socialisation
Indo-European languages, the sexes are distinguished by differences would be more apparent in traditional societies
pronouns—he, she, his, hers—which surely facilitates chil- where there are more rigid status differences between men and
dren’s learning to code themselves and others as to gender. In women. Still, we would have to say that to date, the
addition, there are distinctive ways of speaking to children that socialisation theory that grew out of the S-R learning
emphasise stereotypical qualities, such as saying to a four-year- perspective of the mid century and stressed the role of parents
old ‘‘That’s my sweet little girl’’ or ‘‘There’s my big strong as ‘‘shapers’’ of sex-typed behaviour rests on a weak empirical
boy’’. Still, it has proved surprisingly difŽcult to document foundation.
differential treatment of boys and girls by their parents, Of course, a socialisation perspective does not need to focus
especially when children are young. Several reviews have so exclusively on parents as the developmental psychology of
summarised child-rearing practices used by parents with sons the 1950s, 1960s, and 1970s tended to do. There were a
and daughters (mainly in modern Western societies). When it number of reasons for this focus. First of all, much of the
comes to the traditional dimensions of child rearing (e.g., research at that time involved infants and children of preschool
permissiveness, restrictiveness, monitoring, responsiveness, age, in families in which few mothers worked outside the home.
warmth) few differences have been found in the way parents Thus it was natural to assume that parents were indeed the
deal with sons as compared with daughters (Huston, 1983; most inuential socialisation agents. In addition, there was a
Lytton &Romney, 1991; Maccoby &Jacklin, 1974). Although pervasive assumption that the early years were a time of great
it is often assumed that parents react differently to assertive plasticity, when children were especially subject to ‘‘shaping’’
behaviour by sons and daughters, and draw young daughters with respect to characteristics thought to be pervasive and
into greater emotional closeness with themselves than they do long-lasting, such as the gendered aspects of the self. In recent
with young sons, evidence to date does not support these years much more attention has been given to the inuence of
assumptions. There are some ways in which parents do other socialisation agents, such as out-of-home caregivers,
consistently differentiate: they do more roughhousing with peers, teachers, and coaches, whose positive or negative
sons, offer dolls more often to girls (and toy trucks to boys), reactions to children’s behaviour can provide additional
and talk about feelings more with girls (see Maccoby, 1998, for shaping for children’s sex-role development in ways that can
a summary of studies). And fathers in particular show negative supplement—or sometimes even contradict—the inuence of
reactions to any behaviour by their sons that seems effeminate. parents.
Of course, when socialisation differences are found, the In fact, to test a theory of direct socialisation adequately, it
ubiquitous issue of direction of effects arises. Do parents offer would be necessary not only to demonstrate that socialisation
dolls to girls, trucks to boys, because they want their children agents deal differentially with the two sexes, but that these
to be appropriately ‘‘masculine’’ or ‘‘feminine’’, or because different socialisation experiences are related to any differences
they have discovered that these are the toys the children prefer? in developmental trajectories that appear in male and female
If they roughhouse more with boys, is this because they have a children. Much depends, then, on what aspects of gender
stereotypical view of what kind of play boys ought to like, or enactment are chosen as ‘‘outcomes’’ in efforts to test
because boys actually do like it more than girls and ‘‘train’’ socialisation theory. This issue will be considered more fully
their parents over time to play in ways that boys Žnd most below.
enjoyable? There is good evidence that boys’ and girls’
different initiatives can indeed evoke different reactions from
their parents (see the summary in Maccoby, 1998). But, these The indirect socialisation perspective
reactions, in their turn, can then inuence the children, so the Beginning in the 1960s, social learning theory added learning
existence of child-to-parent effects do not by any means by imitation (often called modelling) to reinforcement as a
preclude parent-to-child effects (Ge et al., 1996; O’Connor, powerful process involved in gender socialisation. In social
Deater-Deckard, Fulker, Rutter, & George, 1998). learning theory, children were still seen as being shaped by
Experimental studies have been doneDownloaded
in which unfamiliar direct positive and negative reinforcement. But it was also
from jbd.sagepub.com at PENNSYLVANIA STATE UNIV on May 9, 2016
children are given an arbitrary gender label. In such studies, an shown that children could learn vicariously from seeing how
400 MACCOBY / PERSPECTIVES ON GENDER DEVELOPMENT

other children’s gender-appropriate or gender-inappropriate Knowledge about the characteristics of the two sexes, and
behaviour were reacted to by others (Bandura, 1965). Apart about the norms for their behaviour, is clearly necessary, but is
from learning about the consequences of such behaviour, it sufŽcient by itself to motivate children to adopt socially
children could also learn by observation what behaviours were prescribed roles and standards? Several hypotheses have been
characteristic of each sex. And of course, they could learn these advanced concerning the ways in which children’s knowledge
things not only from what they observed within their families, of their own gender identity and that of others could function
schools, and neighbourhoods, but from Žlms or TV, or as to set motivational processes in motion:
depicted in stories (Mischel, 1966). The introduction of
observational learning gave great added power to learning 1. When observing the contingencies that are experienced
theory. But it also brought some complications. In daily life, by other children, the observer is able to select the
boys and girls are exposed to models of both sexes. Both boys experiences of children of their own sex as most relevant
and girls presumably would learn the same facts concerning to inferences about what might happen to themselves.
what behaviour is appropriate for boys, what for girls. The This allows observational learning to be focused
theory called for selective imitation, such that boys would adopt speciŽcally on the acquisition of behaviours and stan-
behaviour depicted by male models, or adapt their own dards that apply differentially to the child’s own sex.
behaviour according to the reinforcement patterns they saw 2. Out of a need for cognitive consistency, children want to
being provided for male, rather than female, children. At the adapt themselves (i.e., conform) to what they believe is
least, this required that children would know their own gender appropriate for their own sex. Kohlberg (1966) proposed
and that of the people whom they observed. Then too, it that this motivation would not appear until approxi-
required that children should be able to summarise and mately the age of 5–7, when he thought children achieve
generalise from multiple exemplars, and deal with exceptions. a Žrm level of gender constancy. Because Kohlberg
In addition, it called for some motivation to adopt the urged the importance of gender constancy, evidence has
behaviour patterns of people who are ‘‘the same as me’’ with accumulated that the functional elements of gender
respect to gender. Clearly, the theory called for the extensive constancy (namely, identity and stability; see Maccoby,
incorporation of cognitive elements in gender development. 1990; Ruble & Martin, 1998) are achieved at consider-
ably younger ages than Kohlberg believed.
3. When they have achieved a stable gender identity,
children classify themselves as members of a same-sex
The cognitive perspective group. They identify with this group, see members of the
In the 1970s and 1980s, there was a strong surge of interest in other sex as belonging to an outgroup, and want to be
gender cognitions. Although a cognitive perspective on like members of their own-sex group.
developmental processes had been strongly anticipated in
Europe for many decades, it was the ‘‘cognitive revolution’’ These possibilities are not mutually exclusive, and may all
that took place in American psychology beginning in the 1960s combine to generate the motivation for children to: (a)
that set the stage for an active revision of American views about selectively imitate same-sex models (if they are known to be
child development. Children were increasingly seen not as good exemplars of their gender category—see Perry & Bussey,
passive recipients of socialisation pressures, but as active 1979); (b) seek, select, and remember preferentially informa-
selectors and users of information pertinent to their develop- tion that is relevant to, and consistent with, children’s own-sex
mental levels and personal goals. And children began to be schemas, and (c) reject, ignore, forget, or distort schema-
seen as developing the capacity to adopt standards, and inconsistent material (see Ruble & Martin, 1998, for a
regulate their own behaviour in conformity to these standards, summary). With the acquisition of a stable gender identity,
thus contributing to their own socialisation. children can also begin to monitor their own behaviour with
Vast amounts of information are available to children reference to a self-accepted standard of what is appropriate for
concerning the way gender is enacted in the world around their own sex (Bussey & Bandura, 1992).
them. In the 1970s, 1980s, and continuing into the 1990s, The socialisation and cognitive perspectives discussed so far
research focused heavily on how children acquire knowledge combine into a kind of social constructivist approach. Insofar
and develop stereotypes and scripts concerning what is usual, as children of the two sexes are found to differ in their
or considered ‘‘appropriate’’, for people of the two sexes. (See behaviour, interests and/or value, it is thought that this
Ruble & Martin, 1998, for a review.) It became evident that differentiation comes about for three reasons: because adults
gender is a highly salient category for children, perhaps because shape children in this way; because peers shape each other
it is neatly binary, because it is so heavily culturally according to the way they themselves have been socialised; and
emphasised, and because socially ascribed sex and biological because children—once they have established a Žrm gender
sex are so completely redundant. The distinction, in other identity—socialise themselves to conform to what they know to
words, is easy to make, and there is good evidence that children be stereotypical for children of their own sex, within the limits
do indeed make it very early in life. Gender categories, once of what their own competencies permit (Bandura & Bussey,
applied, have been shown to be a convenient hook on which 1999).
children can easily hang stereotypes about gender attributes Sometimes, the use of the word ‘‘stereotypes’’ is taken to
(Gelman, Coleman, & Maccoby, 1986) and assimilate new mean that attributes assimilated to social categories are
incoming information to these stereotypes. Gender schema arbitrary, so that our concepts about the two sexes may be
theories introduced in the early 1980s held that children form quite artiŽcial or distorted. How accurate are our gender
cognitive structures that organise their gender knowledge into a stereotypes? In their review, Deaux and LaFrance (1998) note
set of expectations that guide and Downloadedorganise their social that it is hardly possible to check the reality base of some of our
from jbd.sagepub.com at PENNSYLVANIA STATE UNIV on May 9, 2016
perceptions (Bem, 1981; Martin & Halverson, 1981). stereotypes, because they refer to characteristics that are very
INTERNATIONAL JOURNAL OF BEHAVIORAL DEVELOPMENT, 2000, 24 (4), 398–406 401

difŽcult to measure objectively. People’s beliefs about certain Limitations of these perspectives
attributes of the two sexes that can be objectively assessed,
In the 1980s and 1990s, it began to be evident that these
however, turn out to be quite accurate (e.g., that women are
perspectives were not serving well, on the whole. First of all, it
more involved than men in the care of young children). This
was increasingly clear that sex differences in children’s
work has been done with adults, and we can only assume that
psychological attributes as usually measured were not sub-
the accuracy of children’s gender stereotypes would improve
stantial, and Žndings were inconsistent from one study to
with age, as they accumulate information about a larger and
another. Focusing on individual variation along dimensions
larger sample of exemplars.
such as ‘‘masculinity’’, ‘‘femininity’’, ‘‘androgyny’’, or ‘‘degree
At the end of the 20th century, then, a predominant
of sex-typing’’ was not turning out to be a strategy that
perspective on gender development is a dual one focusing on
accounted for much variance in behavioural outcomes.
individual differences. Its central themes are that children will
Although this might mean that gender simply is not an
differ in the degree to which they become sex-typed as a result
important factor in children’s daily lives, it could also mean
of: (a) the strength of the socialisation pressures they have
that gender matters only in certain contexts, so that aggregat-
experienced; and (b) the nature and coherence of their gender
ing across contexts attenuates gender-related phenomena that
schemas—their knowledge about the characteristics stereo-
are in fact quite strong. If so, it would appear that research
typically associated with each sex, and about what the social
should turn to studies of moderating contexts. And, in
expectations are for persons of their own sex. Of course,
addition, look for gender-related outcome variables that are
socialisation and cognitive factors in gender development are
more robust than ‘‘sex-typing’’ as we have measured it with
not truly distinct. For example, socialisation pressures are one
toy- or activity-preference tests or clusters of personality traits.
source of information enabling children to develop their
The socialisation and cognitive perspectives have proved
knowledge concerning the gendered norms that they are
disappointing in another respect: Empirical tests have failed to
expected to adopt. The direct socialisation experiences
give consistent support for the predicted connections between
children have, in other words, constitute a major source of
processes and outcomes. As noted above, the similarities in the
information upon which cognitive structures are built. Indeed,
ways socialisation agents treat boys and girls far outweigh the
the whole cultural milieu in which a child grows up presents to
differences. Still, some differences are found. Ruble and
children an array of cultural beliefs and practices concerning
Martin’s comprehensive review of studies examining connec-
gender, and when children draw on these to construct their
tions between differential socialisation and sex-typed outcomes
gender schemas, it can reasonably be said that they are being
shows very meagre relationships. They say: ‘‘Although adults
socialised by the surrounding culture into becoming co-
and peers treat boys and girls differently in many ways,
practitioners of these cultural forms.
especially concerning activities and interests, the role of these
An emphasis on cognitive and socialisation factors by no
processes in children’s gender-related preferences and behaviors
means precludes a recognition of possible biological inuences
remains to be demonstrated’’ (Ruble &Martin, 1998, p. 982).
that may generate different predispositions in boys and girls.
In a similar vein, they note that variations among children in
Nowadays, there is widespread recognition of the importance
the level of their knowledge of gender stereotypes are generally
of biological factors. However, as biological sex and socially
unconnected to individual differences in sex-typed behaviour
ascribed gender are so completely redundant, it has proved
(e.g., Powlishta, 1995). This is by no means a fatal blow to
difŽcult to tease them apart. Something is known concerning
cognitive theories of gender development, because in many
sex differences in brain structures and functions. For example,
respects gender cognitions are important in their own right,
in males, more functions are lateralised, so that they are
regardless of how and whether they ‘‘drive’’ individual
associated with activation primarily in one hemisphere of the
differences in sex-typed behaviour. Furthermore, children’s
brain, whereas in females, the two hemispheres are more likely
understanding of gender identity—their own and that of other
to be both activated for a speciŽc function. However, the
people—does appear to be somewhat connected to other
possible behavioural impacts of these structural differences are
aspects of gender development early in life. However, under-
far from being understood. A good deal is known concerning
standing of gender identity is virtually complete in most
the physiological events during gestation that differentiate the
children by the age of about 3, and hence (as it varies so little)
genital structures of male and female fetuses. And we know,
cannot correlate with individual differences in sex-typing that
too, something about the way in which prenatal hormones
emerge after approximately the Žrst three years of life.
organise the developing fetal brain so as to create different
Similarly, in some contexts and at some ages, there is very
propensities and sensitivities in the two sexes—tendencies
little individual variation in the choice of same-sex playmates,
which will manifest themselves behaviourally at various times
so that this aspect of sex-typing cannot be predicted from
during postnatal development, perhaps requiring either a
individual cognitive or personality characteristics.
biological or environmental trigger for their activation.
Clearly, a rethinking of the long-dominant perspectives
As noted above, most accounts of gender development do
described above is in order, and has begun. A major
note possible biological underpinnings for some of the gender
reorientation is a shift away from a focus on individual
differentiation that occurs. And there is considerable interest in
differences in outcomes, and an increasing focus on some
taking biological explanations one step backward to the genetic
robust outcomes for groups—gender effects which might
factors that may control biological differentiation. But we are a
appear primarily, or only, in certain speciŽc social contexts.
long way from having traced pathways from genes to the
behavioural attributes that typically differentiate the sexes. In
psychology, the great bulk of work has been concerned with the
social and cognitive factors that are thought to underlie this
Robust gender phenomena in the context of groups
differentiation, over and above what anyDownloaded
biological predisposi- What might these outcomes and contexts be? Indications
from jbd.sagepub.com at PENNSYLVANIA STATE UNIV on May 9, 2016
tions may call for. began to emerge in the late 1970s that the answer could be
402 MACCOBY / PERSPECTIVES ON GENDER DEVELOPMENT

found in the context of pairs or larger groups of children Preferences need no inferences’’ (1980) presented evidence
engaged in social interaction. Jacklin and Maccoby (1978) that indeed preferences (liking/disliking, approach/avoidance)
observed 92 pairs of previously unacquainted children, all close can be primary, immediate, without (or prior to) activation of
to 33 months of age, as they interacted with each other and related cognitions. Recent writings by Panksepp (1998), stress
with toys. Tallies were kept of the instances of social not only the immediacy of emotional reactions, but their deep
behaviours (both positive and negative, verbal and nonverbal) mammalian origins. ‘‘Primal’’ emotions, including affective
each child directed toward his/her partner. Some of the pairs reactions to certain characteristics of same- or other-sex
were composed of two boys, some of two girls, and some were conspeciŽcs, are claimed to be instinctive and species-wide.
mixed-sex pairs. Results were that children of both sexes Given a pattern of wariness toward children of the other sex,
directed about twice as much social behaviour toward partners and/or especial interest in, or compatibility with, own-sex other
who were of their own sex as they did to other-sex partners.1 It children, it is predictable that when children have a choice of
is notable that when data were computed without regard to the playmates, they will congregate in same-sex pairs or groups. In
sex of the child’s partner, boys and girls displayed virtually childhood, there is a clear pattern of cross-sex avoidance and/
identical levels of social behaviour. In other words, there was or same-sex preference that begins in about the third year of
no overall sex difference in a personality dimension that might life and becomes progressively stronger though middle child-
be called ‘‘sociability’’. This Žnding underlines the fact that hood. Children’s tendency to congregate socially with others of
analyses which look only at the behaviour of individual their own sex has long been noted in the developmental
children without regard to social context can totally obscure literature, and has been thoroughly documented, in a variety of
powerful gender phenomena. It is no surprise, then, that many cultures and subcultures (see Hartup, 1983; Maccoby &
simple comparisons between boys and girls have shown sex Jacklin, 1987; and Ruble & Martin, 1998, for reviews). In
differences to be weak or absent. Theorists who have modern Western societies, it is manifested most strongly in
emphasised the importance of context, and variations in the situations not structured by adults, though in more traditional
salience of gender from one context to another, might rightly societies the structures adults provide for children certainly
see the Žndings of the Jacklin and Maccoby study as a contribute to it. The phenomenon of gender segregation in
vindication of their position. But there is something very childhood is remarkably robust, with very little overlap, by the
speciŽc about the context that turned out to be important in age of 5 or 6, between the distributions of the two sexes with
this work: It was the sex of a child’s interactive partner that respect to the gender of the other children with whom they
mattered, not context construed as environmental setting or spend their free social time (Maccoby, 1998).
prior priming conditions. The study points to the importance The students of gender segregation have been concerned
of group composition, and/or of relationships, in how gender is with the factors that bring it about (see Leaper, 1994) and with
enacted. the implications for how gender development should be
Astudy of preschoolers, conducted at about the same time, studied. Clearly, one implication of gender segregation is that
helps us to understand the above Žndings. Wasserman and it is important to continue to study and understand the nature
Stern (1978) laid down a strip of carpet on a playroom oor, of the group processes that occur in all-male as compared to
and asked a child (sometimes a boy, sometimes a girl) to stand all-female dyads or groups, sustaining the progress that has
quietly at one end of it. Another child was then placed at the already been made in this work. There is now considerable
‘‘starting’’ end of the strip, and asked to walk along it to the evidence that the groups or dyads formed by girls, as compared
standing child. It turned out that children would walk up quite with boys, differ with respect to the agendas they enact, and in
close to the standing child, facing directly forward, if the their prevailing interaction ‘‘styles’’. The nature of these
standing child was of their own sex. Approaching an other-sex differences has been summarised elsewhere (Maccoby, 1998;
child, however, both boys and girls would turn away as they Ruble & Martin, 1998). Here it is sufŽcient to note a few
approached and stop sooner. Notably, this occurred whether dominant trends:
they were acquainted with the standing child or not, indicating
that we are seeing here a form of other-sex avoidance that is not 1. The themes that appear in boys’ fantasies, in the stories
driven by previous experience with another child, but rather by they invent, the scenarios they enact when playing with
the other child’s membership in a gender category. other boys, and the Žctional fare they prefer (books and
Perhaps we are seeing here an instance of the kind of self- TV) involve danger, conict, destruction, heroic actions,
regulation pointed to by cognitive social learning theorists, in and trials of physical strength. Girls’ fantasy and play
which a child recognises the sex of another child, knows that it themes tend to be oriented around domestic or romantic
is considered inappropriate to associate with children of the scripts, portraying characters who are involved in social
other sex, and hence inhibits the approach to the other child. It relationships and depicting the maintenance or restora-
appears, though, that children begin to show cross-sex tion of order and safety.
avoidance at such a young age that they may not yet be able 2. Interaction among boys, more often than among girls,
to code their own gender identity and that of other children involves rough-and-tumble play, competition, conict,
reliably and explicitly. In traditional theories, there has never ego displays, risk-taking, and striving for dominance.
been a claim that the cognitions involved are conscious or Girls, by contrast, are more responsive to the inputs of
deliberate, but still, gender-cognitive processes are assumed to their interactive partners, more likely to use suggestions
occur at some level. An alternative possibility is that there is rather than imperative demands, and more likely to
simply an uncognised raw emotion connected with gender construct collaborative scripts in which the actions of
categories. The early paper by Zajonc: ‘‘Feeling and thinking: play characters are reciprocal (see Leaper, 1991, on girls’
collaborative discourse style). This does not imply that
1
These results were replicated in a study of English children
Downloaded ranging in age
from jbd.sagepub.com
girls do not assert their own objectives, or that their
at PENNSYLVANIA STATE UNIV on May 9, 2016
from 19 to 39 months (Lloyd & Smith, 1986). interactions are conict-free, only that they seek their
INTERNATIONAL JOURNAL OF BEHAVIORAL DEVELOPMENT, 2000, 24 (4), 398–406 403

individual goals in the context of also striving to maintain evolutionary one. The principle of natural selection was
group harmony (see Sheldon, 1992). invoked to explain species-wide adaptive behaviours, such that
3. Girls’ and boys’ friendships are qualitatively different, each animal species was equipped to survive in its particular
girls’ friendships being more intimate in the sense that environmental niche. As the perspective began to be applied to
friends share information about the details of their lives human behaviour, it was of great interest to trace similarities
and concerns, whereas boys typically know less about between humans and nonhuman primates, their closest
their friends’ lives and base their friendships on shared relatives on the evolutionary tree. In the 1950s and 1960s,
activities. The break-up of girls’ friendships is more John Bowlby (1969) drew on the work of Harlow (1961) and
emotionally intense than for boys’ friendships. Hinde (1966) to show striking resemblances between the
patterns of attachment behaviour in human infants or toddlers
By age 6, too, boys typically play in larger groups. and what was seen in young monkeys and apes. In the 1970s,
Benenson, Apostolaris, and Parnass (1997) showed, in a study work by Blurton-Jones (1972) and Strayer (1977; Strayer &
of same-sex six-child groups, that between the ages of 4 and 6, Strayer, 1978) focused on patterns of children’s play, social
boys greatly increased the time they spent in coordinated group dominance, aggression, and peer afŽliation, identifying further
activities, so that by the age of 6, they were spending 74% of parallels with the young of nonhuman primates.
their time in such activities. No such increase occurred for In observing sex differences in behaviours of this kind,
girls, whose coordinated group activities dropped below 20% ethologists do not look for generalised ‘‘trait’’ differences, but
of their time at age 6. And, girls have been found to show more rather for the situations under which a difference does or does
enjoyment than boys when engaged in dyadic interaction not appear, and the form the behaviour takes. It would not be
(Benenson, 1993), whereas this differential is not found for meaningful to an ethologist, for example, to ask whether male
interaction in larger groups. Girls, too, are found to sustain ungulates are more aggressive than female ungulates. Rather,
longer bouts of interaction in dyads than do boys. In other they would note that both males and females attack predators,
words, girls actively seek, prefer, and elaborate dyadic and do so in the same way: with their hooves. Only males,
interactions, whereas boys do not appear to Žnd such however, Žght over territory and mates, and they use their
interactions to be especially gratifying, and instead gravitate horns, rather than their hooves, to attack or threaten other
toward coordinated activities carried out in larger groups. males. In a similar way, in studying human children,
There is evidence, too, that boys’ groups are not only larger, ethologists look for speciŽc behavioural topography, and
but also stronger in some sense, that is, more cohesive, with speciŽc contexts in which sex differentiation is seen. Striking
stronger ingroup identiŽcation and stronger boundaries, in the parallels between human children and the young of other
sense of more strongly excluding both girls and adults (see primates have indeed been found, and this fact has been
Maccoby, 1998, for an elaboration of these processes). interpreted as pointing to evolved, genetically guided under-
The fact that boys congregate in larger groups has important pinnings for certain elements of human behaviour. The
implications. When in dyads, children of both sexes are evolutionary history of human development continues to be a
relatively noncompetitive, and more emotionally supportive matter of great interest up to the present time (see Geary &
of their interactive partners, than they are when participating in Bjorklund, 2000, for a review). Throughout, thinkers involved
larger groups (see Benenson, Nicholson, Waire, Roy, & in this work have been concerned with what adaptive purpose
Simpson, in press), something that was noted many years an evolved behaviour pattern might serve.
ago in interactions among adults (Bales & Borgotta, 1955). In what way might an evolutionary perspective be pertinent
Can it be, then, that the greater competitiveness and lesser to gender development? Most evolutionary adaptations, after
positive intimacy in male-male interaction can be accounted all, are seen as species-wide, occurring in both sexes. However,
for by boys’ being more often in larger play groups?Perhaps so, in bisexual species, the distinctive roles of the two sexes in
but the fact that they congregate in larger groups may itself be a reproduction is thought to have produced different adaptive
reection of their preference for certain forms of competitive behaviours (i.e., different strategies for mate selection, and
but coordinated activity that can only be performed in larger differential involvement in the rearing of young). With respect
groups. Indeed, boys form coalitions to achieve group goals— to these domains, then, the two sexes are seen as distinct
and gain group power thereby—to an extent that girls seldom subspecies.
do. Might evolution have anything to do with gender differ-
The above account suggests that the two sexes are pursuing entiation that occurs in childhood, before the age when the
different agendas in their same-sex groups. But children in activities of mate selection and care of offspring emerge? The
groups are not always engaged in enacting these differentiated fact that young monkeys and apes separate into same-sex
agendas, and when they are not, male-male and female-female playgroups, and display some of the same sex-differentiated
interactions can be much alike. It should be noted, too, that playstyles as those seen in human children, strongly suggests
researchers have not yet spelled out the developmental time- that there is indeed an evolutionary basis for these behaviours.
line for these aspects of gender differentiation. Thus, although So does the fact that there appears to be substantial uniformity
there is reason to have a good deal of conŽdence in their in these patterns across human cultures. Geary and Bjorklund
occurrence, it is not yet clear how they wax and wane, and (2000) say: ‘‘From an evolutionary perspective, these sex
whether there are privileged sequences such that some differences are predicted to be a reection of and a preparation
processes need to occur before others can come into play. for sex differences in adult reproductive activities’’ (p. 60).
Thus, boys’ competitiveness and dominance strivings are seen
as preparation for adult male competition over mates, whereas
The ethological perspective girls’ greater social responsiveness and cooperativeness with
Ethology originally referred to the studyDownloaded
of animal behaviour, other girls can be seen as preparation for participation in the
from jbd.sagepub.com at PENNSYLVANIA STATE UNIV on May 9, 2016
and a guiding perspective in ethological research was an kin-based social groups of females in which most rearing of
404 MACCOBY / PERSPECTIVES ON GENDER DEVELOPMENT

the young occurs, in nonhuman primates as well as— instinctive response. In both rodents and primates, it has been
presumably—in the hunter-gatherer bands where some under- shown that animals coming from different genetic strains will
pinnings of present-day human characteristics evolved. manifest their different predispositions only under certain early
In what possible way could sex segregation in childhood rearing conditions (i.e., being deprived of contact with
serve an adaptive purpose, from the standpoint of the maternal animals or peers—see Hood & Cairns, 1989; Suomi,
subsequent successful reproductive activities as adults? One 1997). And for some songbirds, a young male will not acquire
hypothesis is: It functions to prevent incest, with its attendant the species-speciŽc male courting song unless he is reared in
risks for expression, in offspring, of genetic defects carried on the company of older females, who ‘‘train’’ him by responding
recessive genes (see Maccoby, 1998, and Wolf, 1995, for selectively to the elements of song that females of the species
elaboration and evidence). According to this account, humans Žnd most compelling (West & King, in press).
are predisposed to lose sexual interest in anyone with whom Conditions of rearing matter, too, in how different the two
they have been closely associated in childhood, such as siblings sexes become. Wallen (1996) has summarised a series of
and other close relatives. Thus, paradoxical though it may studies with rhesus monkeys, showing that males display
seem, cross-sex avoidance in childhood can be seen as a means elevated levels of threat/aggression toward other animals if they
of fostering or safeguarding future heterosexual attraction. were earlier raised with only limited access to peers, whereas
It is more difŽcult to see how some of the other sex- such limited access increases the amount of submissive, not
differentiated patterns we have noted might contribute to aggressive, behaviour in females. For males, the amount of
individual reproductive success and viability of an individual’s rough play is also affected by the social conditions of rearing,
offspring. Rather, it would seem timely to expand the although for females, the level of such play remains low
ecological perspective to include group success, group viability. regardless of conditions of rearing. When females are
Among chimpanzees, and in the human societies closest to the prenatally exposed to androgens (late in gestation, or over an
way our ancestors probably lived (Collier & Risaldo, 1981), extended period), they subsequently show elevated levels of
males form coalitions to engage in cooperative group hunting rough play, although there is no such effect for males. Clearly,
and group warfare,2 enterprises not directly related to both biology and conditions of rearing are important, but
individual reproductive success, but relevant to survival of differently for the two sexes. Wallen notes that it is the sex-
the troupe. Some of the processes seen in boys’ groups, then, dimorphic behaviours most strongly affected by prenatal
may have the function of regulating hostility and competition hormones in a given sex that show the least effects of rearing
among group members in the interests of allowing cooperative conditions for that sex. And he concludes: ‘‘These studies
group enterprises to emerge. demonstrate that the expression of consistent juvenile beha-
vioral sex differences results from hormonally induced predis-
positions to engage in speciŽc patterns of juvenile behavior
The psychobiological perspective whose expression is shaped by the speciŽc social environment
The ethological perspective largely describes instinctive ele- experienced by the developing monkey’’ (Wallen, 1996,
ments in a species’ behavioural repertoire (i.e., behavioural p. 364).
dispositions assumed to be governed directly by evolved Humans are very different from birds, mice, and monkeys,
genetic programmes). From this perspective, individual life but nevertheless there are useful parallels here. We see that
experience and environmental contexts have little inuence, whatever differential predispositions boys and girls may have, it
other than to provide the innate environmental triggers is likely that the way they are enacted will depend greatly on the
required to ‘‘release’’ an instinctive behaviour. In fact, social conditions provided by the adults and peers with whom
however, there are many aspects of the environment, other they interact. Societies differ with respect to how much time
than innate releasers, that function jointly with genetic factors children of each sex spend with adults, with peers of their own
to inuence behavioural development. Many years of research sex, and with peers or siblings of the other sex. They also differ
have revealed that the way genetic instructions are carried out with respect to how much autonomy peer groups have at what
depends on environmental inputs at every stage of develop- developmental periods. These cultural variations may be
ment from conception to maturity. expected to produce variations in the degree and kind of sex
The modern psychobiological view is that genes (G) and differentiation that appears as children grow up in different
environment (E) have a bidirectional, reciprocal relationship, societies.
and cannot properly be understood as separate components
whose effects can be independently estimated and then
compared or summed (Gottlieb, Wahlsten, and Lickliter,
Integrating perspectives
1998). G E interactions are widespread, and recent work has The above account is meant to show that there is a good case
begun to Žll in the gap between genes and behavioural for including the ethological and psychobiological perspectives
outcomes, by showing how both environment (e.g., conditions in any attempt to understand gender differentiation in child-
of rearing) and an allele of a given gene can affect a speciŽc hood. These perspectives are not meant to replace the
intervening biochemical process, which then inuences beha- socialisation and cognitive perspectives, only to enrich them
vioural outcomes (Anisman, Zaharia, Meany, & Merali, 1998; by expanding our view of how biological and experiential and
Suomi, in press; and see Maccoby, 2000, for a summary). The cognitive factors work together when it comes to the enactment
role of environment, as it interacts with genetic predispositions, of gender.
is much more complex than simply providing a releaser for an The two biological perspectives are especially useful in
helping us to understand any characteristics where between-
2 sex differences are robust and consistent across cultures and
For information on male hunting and warfare in chimpanzees, see: Boesch
and Boesch, 1989; McGrew, Marchant, and Nishida, 1996;
Downloaded and Stanford,
from jbd.sagepub.com
even across species. However, there is considerable variation
at PENNSYLVANIA STATE UNIV on May 9, 2016
Wallis, Matama, and Goddall, 1994. within each sex and among cultural groups in the nature of
INTERNATIONAL JOURNAL OF BEHAVIORAL DEVELOPMENT, 2000, 24 (4), 398–406 405

gendered behaviour displayed and the contexts in which it importance of shared norms in the inuence peer groups have
appears. Although some boys, for example, establish a network on their members, and Kinderman (1993) has shown that a
of good male friends, and participate actively in male group group can have its own identity over and above the identity of
activities, others are loners or peripheral ‘‘hangers-on’’ to these its members, that is, a group norm persists throughout a school
groups, and still others are the victims of teasing and year despite considerable turnover in the membership of the
humiliation by other boys. Some children join peer groups group. It is time for gender to take a more central place than it
that are basically prosocial, others associated mainly with has occupied so far in such work, with more consistent
same-sex peers who engage in risky, antisocial behaviour. attention to the gender composition of dyads and groups. Only
Among girls, too, there is variation in how fully they participate with this knowledge in hand will we be able to understand the
in ‘‘girl culture’’, and in how much interest they have in less role of peers as gender-socialisation agents, and the way in
‘‘feminine’’ activities such as team sports. There is evidence which children build shared gender cognitions that can serve
that these individual differences reect developed differences either to amplify or dampen the gender differentiation of roles
in competencies or vulnerabilities acquired at earlier periods of and status.
development—differences which in their turn undoubtedly Manuscript received June 2000
reect both within-sex genetic variability and individual Revised manuscript received June 2000
socialisation histories. Thus, the socialisation and cognitive
perspectives should be especially pertinent to the under-
standing of such within-sex variation.
In the last century, there have been substantial changes in References
gender roles and the relationships between the sexes, changes
that have occurred much too rapidly to be explained in genetic Anisman, H., Zaharia, M.D., Meany, M.J., & Merali, Z. (1998). Do early life
events permanently alter behavioral and hormonal responses to stressors?
terms. These changes underscore how large the social factors International Journal of Developmental Neuroscience, 16, 149–164.
are in such matters as the relative dominance and power of the Bales, R.F., & Borgatta, E.F. (1955). Size of group as a factor in the interaction
two sexes, and make it clear that these matters are indeed open proŽle. In A.P. Hare, E.F. Borgatta & R.F. Bales (Eds.), Small groups: Studies
to change and not built in to human nature as a result of our in social interaction (pp. 496–512). Toronto: Random House.
Bandura, A. (1965). Inuence of models’ reinforcement contingencies on the
species evolution. The above review has suggested that male acquisition of imitative responses. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
power in society stems in no small degree from male groups 1, 589–595.
and male alliances, despite the internal conicts and competi- Bem, S.L. (1981). Gender schema theory: A cognitive account of sex typing.
Psychological Review, 88, 354–364.
tion that characterise male interaction. The paradox is this: We Benenson, J.F. (1993). Greater preference among females than males for dyadic
have seen that girls and women have especially strong interaction in early childhood. Child Development, 64, 544–555.
interactive skills that support collaboration and cooperation Benenson, J.F., Apostolaris, N.H., & Parnass, J. (1997). Age and sex differences
in their close relationships. Yet, women’s social groupings do in dyadic and group interaction. Developmental Psychology, 33, 538–543.
Benenson, J.F., Nicholson, C., Waire, A., Roy, R., &Simpson, A. (in press). The
not appear to yield power in out-of-home contexts to the extent inuence of size of social context on children’s competitive behaviour. Child
that men’s do. Perhaps it will be possible to gain insight into Development.
this issue with more focused developmental research on the Blurton-Jones, N. (Ed.) (1972). Ethological studies of child behaviour. London:
way power is exercised within same-sex groups and between Cambridge University Press.
Boesch, C., & Boesch H. (1989). Hunting behavior of wild chimpanzees in the
the sexes. Tai National Park. American Journal of Physical Anthropology, 78, 547–573.
The fuller incorporation of the ethological and psychobio- Bowlby, J. (1969). Attachment. New York: Basic Books.
logical perspectives on gender into our existing frameworks Brown, B.B. (1990). Peer groups and peer cultures. In S.S. Feldman & G.R.
Elliott (Eds.), At the threshold (pp. 171–196). Cambridge, MA: Harvard
should enrich the research agenda of students of social University Press.
development. More detailed observation of children in groups Bussey, K., & Bandura, A. (1992). Self-regulatory mechanisms governing gender
is called for, with more attention to the agendas that children of development. Child Development, 63, 1236–1250.
the two sexes seem motivated to enact, and more careful Bussey, K., & Bandura, A. (1999). Social-cognitive theory of gender develop-
ment and differentiation. Psychological Review, 106, 676–713.
delineation of how gender-related patterns change with age. Of Chodorow, N. (1978). The reproduction of mothering: Psychoanalysis and the
especial interest are the changes that occur in gender sociology of gender. Berkeley, CA: University of California Press.
enactment during the transition into adolescence. Useful cues Collier, J., & Risaldo, S. (1981). Politics and gender in simple societies. In S.B.
Ortner & H. Whitehead (Eds.), Sexual meanings. New York: Cambridge
for how such work can be carried out comes from pioneers in University Press.
neighbouring disciplines (e.g., the sociologists Maltz &Borker, Deaux, K. & LaFrance, M. (1998). Gender. In D.T. Gilbert, S.T. Fiske, &
1982 and Barrie Thorne, 1986; and sociolinguist Penelope G. Lindsey (Eds.), Handbook of social psychology (Vol. 1). New York: McGraw
Eckert, 1996, 2000). Hill.
Eckert, P. (1996). Vowels and nailpolish: The emergence of linguistic style in the
Developmental psychology is in the process of building a preadolescent heterosexual marketplace. In J. Ahlers, L. Bilmes, M. Chen,
coherent social psychology of childhood. Considerable atten- M. Oliver, N. Warner, & S. Werhsteim (Eds.), Gender and belief systems.
tion has been focused on individual children as members of Berkeley, CA: Berkeley Women and Language Group.
groups—their acceptance or rejection by peers, their group Eckert, P. (2000). Linguistic variation as social practice. Cambridge, UK:
Blackwell.
entry skills—and the causes and consequences of individual Ge, X., Conger, R., Cadoret, R., Niederhiser, J., Yates, W., et al. (1996). The
variation in these things. In addition, there has been some work developmental interface between nature and nurture: A mutual inuence
on the nature of groups, apart from the individuals who make model of child anti-social behavior and parent behavior. Developmental
Psychology, 32, 574–589.
them up. In their comprehensive review, Rubin, Bukowski, and Geary, D.C., & Bjorklund, D.F. (2000). Evolutionary developmental psychol-
Parker (1998) distinguish group processes from the interac- ogy. Child Development, 71, 57–65.
tions that occur within dyadic relationships, noting that groups Gelman, S.A., Coleman, P., & Maccoby, E.E. (1986). Inferring properties from
can have properties that individuals and dyads cannot have categories, versus inferring categories from properties: The case of gender.
Child Development, 57, 396–404.
(e.g., hierarchies, density of relationships, norms). Studies of Gottleib,
Downloaded from jbd.sagepub.com at PENNSYLVANIA G., UNIV
STATE Wahlsten,
on May 9,D.,
2016& Lickliter, R. (1998). The signiŽcance of biology
‘‘crowds’’ and ‘‘cliques’’ (e.g., Brown, 1990) underscore the for human development: A developmental psychobiological systems view. In
406 MACCOBY / PERSPECTIVES ON GENDER DEVELOPMENT

W. Damon & R.M. Lerner (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 1. adolescence: Antisocial behavioral problems and coercive parenting. Develop-
Theoretical models of human development. (5th ed.), New York: Wiley. mental Psychology, 34, 970–981.
Harlow, H.F. (1961). The development of affectional patterns in infant Panksepp, J. (1998). Affective neuroscience: The foundations of human and animal
monkeys. In B.M. Foss (Ed.), The determinants of infant behaviour (Vol. 1). emotions. New York: Oxford University Press.
London: Methuen. Perry, D.G., & Bussey, K. (1979). The social-learning theory of sex differences:
Hartup, W. (1983). Peer relations. In P.H. Mussen (Series Ed.) and E.M. Imitation is alive and well. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 37,
Hetherington (Vol. Ed.), Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 4. Socialization, 1699–1712.
personality, and social development (4th ed.). New York: Wiley. Powlishta, K.K. (1995). Intergroup processes in childhood: Social categorization
Hinde, R.A. (1966). Animal behaviour: A synthesis of ethology and comparative and sex-role development. Developmental Psychology, 31, 781–788.
psychology. New York: McGraw-Hill. Rubin, K.H., Bukowski, W., & Parker, J.G. (1998). Peer interactions, relation-
Hood, K.E., & Cairns, R.B. (1989). A developmental genetic analysis of ships, and groups. In W. Damon & N. Eisenberg (Eds.), Handbook of child
aggressive behavior in mice: 4. Genotype-environment interaction. Aggressive psychology (5th ed.), (Vol. 3, pp. 619–700). New York: Wiley.
Behavior, 15, 361–380. Ruble, D.N., & Martin, C.L. (1998). Gender Development. In W. Damon &
Huston, A.C. (1983). Sex typing. In P.H. Mussen (Series Ed.) and E.M. N. Eisenberg (Eds.), Handbook of chlid psychology (5th ed.), (Vol. 3, pp. 933–
Hetherington (Vol. Ed.), Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 4. Socialization, 1016). New York: Wiley.
personality, and social development (4th ed., pp. 387–467). New York: Wiley. Sears, R.R., Maccoby, E.E., & Levin, H. (1957). Patterns of child rearing.
Jacklin, C.N., & Maccoby, E.E. (1978). Social behavior at 33 months in same- Evanston, IL: Row Peterson.
sex and mixed-sex dyads. Child Development, 49, 557–569. Sears, R.R., Rau, L., & Alpert, R. (1965). IdentiŽcation and child rearing.
Kinderman, T.A. (1993). Natural peer groups as contexts for individual Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.
development: The case of children’s motivation in school. Developmental Sheldon, A. (1992). Conict talk: Sociolinguistic challenges to self-assertion and
Psychology, 29, 970–977. how young girls meet them. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 38, 95–117.
Kohlberg, L. (1966). A cognitive-developmental analysis of children’s sex-role
Stanford, C.B., Wallis, J., Matama, H., & Goddall, J. (1994). Patterns of
concepts and attitudes. In E. Maccoby (Ed.), The development of sex differences.
predation by chimpanzees on red colobus monkeys in Gombe National Park,
Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.
1982–1991. American Journal of Physical Anthropology, 94, 213–228.
Leaper, C. (1991). Inuence and involvement in children’s discourse: Age,
Stern, M., & Karraker, K.H. (1989). Sex stereotyping of infants: A review of
gender and partner effects. Child Development, 62, 797–811.
gender labeling studies. Sex Roles, 20, 501–522.
Leaper, C. (1994). Childhood gender segregation: Causes and consequences. New
Directions for Child Development. San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass. Strayer, F.F. (1977). Peer attachment and afŽliative subgroups. In F.F. Strayer
(Ed.), Ethological perspectives on preschool social organization. Research memo,
Lloyd, B., & Smith, C. (1986). The effects of age and gender on social behaviour
No. 5. Department of Psychologie, University of Quebec.
in very young children. British Journal of Social Psychology, 25, 33–41.
Lytton, H., & Romney, D.M. (1991). Parents’ differential socialization of boys Strayer, F., & Strayer, F.F. (1978). Social aggressive and power relations among
and girls: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 109, 267–296. preschool children. Aggressive Behavior, 4, 173–182.
Maccoby, E.E. (1990). The role of gender identity and gender constancy in sex- Suomi, S.J. (1997). Long-term effects of different early rearing experiences on
differentiated development. In D. Schrader (Ed.), The legacy of Lawrence social, emotional and physiological development in non-human primates. In
Kohlberg: No. 47. New directions for child development. San Francisco, CA: M.S. Keshevan & R.M. Murra (Eds.), Neurodevelopmental models of adult
Jossey-Bass. psychopathology. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
Maccoby, E.E. (1998). The two sexes: Growing up apart, coming together. Suomi, S.J. (in press). A biobehavioral perspective on develpmental psycho-
Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. pathology: Excessive aggression and serotonergic dysfunction in monkeys. In
Maccoby, E.E. (2000). Parenting and its effects on children: On reading and A.J. Samaroff, M. Lewis, & S. Miller (Eds.), Handbok of developmental
misreading behavior genetics. Annual Review of Psychology, 51, 1–27. psychopathology. New York: Plenum.
Maccoby, E.E., & Jacklin, C.N. (1974). The psychology of sex differences. Stanford, Thorne, B. (1986). Boys and girls together . . . but mostly apart: Gender
CA: Stanford University Press. arrangements in elementary schools. In W.H. Hartup & Z. Rubin (Eds.),
Maccoby, E.E., & Jacklin, C.N. (1987). Gender segregation in childhood. In Relationships and development. Hillsdale. NJ: Erlbaum.
H. Reese (Ed.), Advances in child behavior and development. New York: Wallen, K. (1996). Nature needs nurture: The interaction of hormonal and
Academic Press. social inuences on the development of behavioral sex differences in rhesus
Maltz, D.N. & Borker, R.A. (1982). A cultural approach to male-female monkeys. Hormones and Behavior, 30, 364–378.
miscommunication. In J.A. Gumperz (Ed.), Language and social identity (pp. Wasserman, G.A., & Stern, D.N. (1978). An early manifestation of differential
195–216). New York: Cambridge University Press. behavior toward children of the same and opposite sex. Journal of Genetic
Martin, C.L., & Halverson, C.F. (1981). A schematic processing model of sex Psychology, 133, 129–137.
typing and stereotyping in children. Child Development, 54, 563–574. West, M.J., & King, A.P. (in press). Science lies its way to the truth—really. In
McGrew, W.C., Marchant, L.F., & Nichida, T. (1996). Great ape societies. New E. Blass (Ed.), Developmental psychobiology, developmental neurobiology, and
York: Cambridge University Press. behavioral ecology: Mechanisms and early principles: Vol. 12. Handbook of
Mischel, W. (1966). Asocial-learning view of sex differences in behavior. In E.E. behavioral neurobiology. New York: Plenum Press.
Maccoby (Ed.), The development of sex differences. Stanford, CA: Stanford Wolf, A. (1995). Sexual attraction and childhood association: A Chinese brief for
University Press. Edward Westermark. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.
O’Connor, T.G., Deater-Deckard, K., Fulker, D., Rutter, M., & Plomin, R. Zajonc, R.B. (1980). Feeling and thinking: Preferences need no inferences.
(1998). Genotype-environment correlations in late childhood and early American Psychologist, 35, 151–175.

Downloaded from jbd.sagepub.com at PENNSYLVANIA STATE UNIV on May 9, 2016

You might also like