Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 9

Journal of Counseling Psychology Copyright 1996 by the American Psychological Association, Inc.

1996, Vol. 43, No. 3, 338-346 0022-0167/96/$3.00

A Test of a Social Cognitive Model for Middle School Students:


Math and Science
Nadya A. Fouad and Philip L. Smith
University of Wisconsin—Milwaukee

This study tested Propositions 1, 3, and 4 of the R. W. Lent, S. D. Brown, and G. Hackett
(1994) social cognitive model with ethnically diverse middle school students (N = 380): that
an individual's vocational interests are reflective of his or her concurrent self-efficacy beliefs
and outcome expectations, that self-efficacy beliefs affect choice goals and actions, and that
outcome expectations affect choice goals and action. R. W. Lent et al. (1994) also proposed
that demographic and individual difference variables (such as gender or race-ethnicity)
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

mediate learning experiences that play a role in forming self-efficacy beliefs and outcome
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

expectations. The study investigated a model in which age and gender were represented as
antecedent conditions to learning experiences. Self-efficacy was modeled to have both a
direct influence on interests and an indirect influence on interests through outcome expect-
ancies. Finally, intentions were modeled to be influenced by self-efficacy, outcome expect-
ancies, and interests. Results support the R. W. Lent et al. Propositions 1, 3, and 4 for this
middle school population.

Recent reviews of literature in career theory and career Lent et al. argued that career interests and career intentions
decision making have noted that self-efficacy (Bandura, develop in part as a result of self-efficacy and outcome
1977, 1986) has been a major area of investigation (Borgen, expectations. They also proposed that demographic and
1991; Gelso & Fassinger, 1990; Fitzgerald & Betz, 1994; individual difference variables (such as gender or race-
Fouad, 1994; Hackett & Lent, 1992). Self-efficacy has been ethnicity) interact with background and contextual variables
found to play a role in a variety of career-related behaviors, to differentially mediate learning experiences that play a
such as indecision (Taylor & Betz, 1983), decision making role in forming self-efficacy beliefs and outcome expecta-
(Luzzo, 1993; Mathieu, Sowa, & Niles, 1993; Niles & tions. For example, socialization may affect the learning
Sowa, 1992), math tasks and performance (Cooper & Rob- experiences of boys and girls differently, which may lead to
inson, 1991; Hackett & Betz, 1989; Lent, Lopez, & dissimilar self-efficacy beliefs and outcome expectancies.
Bieschke, 1991,1993; Lopez & Lent, 1992), career salience Lent et al. (1994) formulated a number of propositions
(Matzeder & Krieshok, 1995), specific occupational tasks that may be empirically tested. The purpose of this study
(Rooney & Osipow, 1992; Williams & Betz, 1994), and was to test portions of this model with ethnically diverse
academic achievement and persistence (Brown, Lent, & middle school students. It is particularly critical to examine
Larkin, 1989; Hackett, Betz, Casas, & Rocha-Singh, 1992; these factors for middle school students, whose interests are
Lent, Brown, & Larkin, 1984, 1986). beginning to form and who are making choices that will
Social-cognitive theory has been applied to the career have strong influences on later decisions. These include
and vocational realm in relatively piecemeal fashion, with choice of courses in high school, choice of extracurricular
investigators examining the relationship of self-efficacy to activities, and persistence in high school. By understanding
other variables of interest in a variety of populations. Re- how self-efficacy operates in the development of career
cently, however, Lent, Brown, and Hackett (1994) have interests and vocationally related intentions, counseling
taken an important step, building on social-cognitive theory psychologists will be in a better position to advocate for
(Bandura, 1986) to explain and predict career outcomes. specific interventions.
Lent et al. (1994) proposed that self-efficacy expectations
Nadya A. Fouad and Philip L. Smith, School of Education, are related to expectations individuals have about the out-
Department of Educational Psychology, University of Wiscon- comes of behavior. In their model, outcome expectancies
sin—Milwaukee. and self-efficacy beliefs both predict interests. Interests
We are grateful for the assistance of Robert W. Lent, University (together with self-efficacy beliefs and outcome expectan-
of Maryland, College Park, and Valerie Wiebeck, Kosciuszko cies) predict intentions, which in turn lead to choice behav-
Middle School, Milwaukee, Wisconsin, in developing items for the iors. Lent et al. presented intercorrelations among variables
scales used in this study. We also thank V. Scott Solberg for his in their model drawn from a meta-analysis of studies exam-
helpful comments on a draft of this article.
ining social cognitive theory applied to career-related vari-
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to
Nadya A. Fouad, School of Education, Department of Educational ables. They discovered that, in general, studies supported
Psychology, Enderis Hall, P.O. Box 413, Milwaukee, Wisconsin their propositions. For example, they found an average
53201. Electronic mail may be sent via Internet to nfouad® weighted correlation of .49 between self-efficacy (measured
soe.uwm.edu. in a variety of ways) and outcome expectations, .53 between
338
MIDDLE SCHOOL MATH-SCIENCE SELF-EFFICACY 339

self-efficacy and interests, and .52 between outcome expect- 3, which hypothesizes that self-efficacy beliefs are directly
ancies and interests. Thus, self-efficacy beliefs and outcome associated with intentions; and Proposition 4, which hy-
expectancies each accounted for approximately 27% of the pothesizes that outcome expectancies are directly associated
variance in interests. with intentions. Because this study investigated middle
Hackett and Betz (1981) were the first to apply Bandura's school students, who are developmentally not ready for
(1986) social-cognitive theory to career choice, particularly choice goals or performance outcomes, intentions were sub-
focusing on self-efficacy to explain women's traditional stituted for choice goals. Thus, the study examined what
career choices. Bandura hypothesized that individuals' students intended to do, rather than the more ideal assess-
judgments about their ability to successfully perform some ment of their behavior, as hypothesized by Lent et al.
task increases the likelihood that they will attempt and Although intention to act is a necessary precursor, it is
persist in that task. He also postulated that self-efficacy is clearly not synonymous with action.
distinct from outcome expectancies, or the expectations Lent et al. (1994) also proposed (Hypothesis 1G) that
individuals have of the result of behavior. Bandura noted gender and race differences "arise largely through differen-
that "efficacy and outcome expectancies judgments are dif- tial access to opportunities, supports, and socialization pro-
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

ferentiated because individuals can believe that a particular cesses" (p. 108) and suggested that "group differences will
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

course of action will produce certain outcomes, but they do be reduced when [these] are controlled" (p. 108). Although
not act on that outcome belief because they question this proposition is beyond the scope of our study, gender
whether they can actually execute the necessary activities" and age differences were included in the model as anteced-
(Bandura, 1986, p. 392). Hackett and Betz hypothesized that ent variables to investigate the relationship between those
low self-efficacy may explain the restricted range of wom- demographic variables and self-efficacy, outcome expect-
en's career options. Betz and Hackett (1981) empirically ancies, interests, and intentions. Gender was included be-
tested this hypothesis with college students, finding signif- cause of the extensive literature that documents gender
icant gender differences in their confidence that they could differences in self-efficacy (reviewed in Hackett & Lent,
complete the educational requirements and job duties for 1992; Hackett, 1995). Age is not specifically included in the
nontraditional occupations. Male students had higher self- Lent et al. model, although a developmental perspective is
efficacy for traditionally male occupations, whereas female implicit in their discussion. Self-efficacy beliefs have been
students had higher self-efficacy for traditionally female found to change and grow as children grow older (Flammer,
occupations. 1995; Zimmerman, 1995), and we are postulating that age is
Hackett (1995) traced the development of her and her a distinct person variable. Bandura (1995), for example,
colleagues' work from early studies with Betz to the expli- noted that children's "beliefs of cognitive efficacy can have
cation of the social-cognitive framework she developed reverberating effects on developmental trajectories" (p. 19),
with Lent and Brown (Betz & Hackett, 1986; Hackett & citing evidence that early low self-efficacy is related to later
Betz, 1989; Hackett, Betz, Casas, & Rocha-Singh, 1992; antisocial behavior and aggression. It is important to note
Lent, Brown, & Hackett, 1994; Lent, Lopez, & Bieschke, that Lent et al. postulated that person variables affect the
1993). She noted that most early studies focused on the learning experiences that influence self-efficacy beliefs and
content of choices, such as traditionality of choice for men outcome expectations rather than having a direct influence.
or women, but that attention has turned to focus on the For example, girls are not inherently less self-efficacious in
process of decision making and the variables that influence math but are less self-efficacious in math because different
that process. She advocated that future research focus on socialization and teaching practices have led to different
efficacy-based interventions with at-risk youth, particularly learning opportunities available to them. This perspective is
in the math and science areas, and noted that although implicit in our study, as well.
interests are too often seen as innate, self-efficacy studies The basis for the model tested in this study is shown in
have demonstrated that interests may be developed by using Figure 1. In this figure the basic elements of the Lent et al.
self-efficacy interventions. (1994) model are shown, with the relationships posited by
Although it is important to examine process self-efficacy Lent et al. depicted as dotted arrows. This model is very
in career decision making, it is also important to examine complex and it would be a difficult task, in the context of a
causal links leading to content-specific self-efficacy, partic- single study, to collect information relative to each of the
ularly in the math and science domain. By gaining a better variables indicated in Figure 1. Our study examined a some-
understanding of influences on math and science self- what truncated version of this general model, testing key
efficacy, counseling psychologists will be able to effec- elements of the structure depicted in Figure 1.
tively intervene to promote math and science activities for Variables indicated in the shaded boxes of Figure 1 were
children. This study was designed to test three propositions not measured in our study and therefore were not included
of Lent et al.'s (1994) model with a middle school popula- in the model that was tested. These omissions lead to a few
tion on math and science content, examining the relation- key differences between the model proposed by Lent et al.
ships among self-efficacy, outcome expectancies, interests, (1994) and the one tested in this study. First, "person
and intentions. inputs" as described by Lent et al. are represented by only
Specifically, the study tested Lent et al.'s (1994) Propo- gender and age in this study. Obviously, there are a number
sition 1, which hypothesizes that interests are reflective of of person inputs that might be included in a study of this
self-efficacy beliefs and outcome expectancies; Proposition sort. We limited ourselves to these two variables in this
340 FOUAD AND SMITH

Asian (primarily southeast Asian), 3% were American Indian, and


Contextual 2% indicated other race or ethnicity.
Influences Students participating in this study (N = 380) were seventh
Self Efficacy (54%) and eighth (46%) graders, 58% of whom were girls, and
were all seventh- and eighth-grade English-speaking students in
the school present on the day of assessment. Twenty-six percent of
the students were 12-year-olds, 45% were 13-year-olds, 22% were
-Learning
Experiences
Interests >•
Intentions 14-year-olds, and 6% were 15-year-olds. Students were in a Career
Linking program (Fouad, 1995) designed to promote math and
science performance and career information that was made avail-
able to the seventh and eighth graders. Although a substantial
Outcome • Variable in current Model number of the Spanish-speaking seventh and eighth graders were
Expecations • Variable not included also involved in Career Linking, the assessment tools were not
• Path in current model translated and thus were only given to English-speaking students.
• Lent et al posited path Ethnic representation of students in the study was similar to
representation in the whole school: 55 (15%) were Caucasian, 225
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Hackett (1994) model. (59%) were Hispanic American, 40 (11%) were African American,
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

13 (3%) were Asian American, 14 (3%) were American Indian,


and the remaining 33 students did not indicate their ethnicity or
study because of the known (gender) and hypothesized marked other.
(age) relationships between these two variables and other
elements of the model.
Second, learning experiences were not measured in this Procedure
study and were not included in the model. As can be seen,
Students were given the instrument during a class period. Intact
Lent et al. (1994) postulated that person inputs on both
classrooms were chosen on the basis of teacher participation in a
self-efficacy and outcome expectations are mediated by math or science lab. Teacher participation was based on English
learning experiences, thus their effect on these variables is proficiency of students, with teachers of monolingual Hmong,
indirect. Nonetheless, these effects can be modeled as direct Laotian, and Spanish and one Spanish bilingual teacher not par-
effects (solid lines in Figure 1) with the expectation of ticipating in the lab. The latter thought the English proficiency of
obtaining an estimate of the effects of the person inputs on her students was not sufficient for participation. Student partici-
both of these variables without denying the possibility that pation was voluntary, and all students participated. Parental con-
any effects might be present only through their relationship sent was given for all Career Linking activities at the beginning of
to learning experiences. The exclusion of this variable from the school year. Total test-taking was completed in 45 min.
our study does not allow for the evaluation of the additional
influence that important learning experiences have on self-
efficacy and outcome expectations. Future research needs to Instruments
focus on these complex relationships.
The self-efficacy, outcome expectancies, and intentions instru-
Finally, our study did not include variables related to ments used in this study were developed by the first author and are
context: neither individual background context variables nor described in detail elsewhere (Fouad, Smith, & Enochs, 1995).
those contextual influences that occur at some point in time Instruments were developed to assess math-science self-efficacy,
as intentions are being formulated. It is clear that a test of math-science outcome expectancies, and math-science intentions.
the complete model as proposed by Lent et al. would require In addition to the math-science-related instruments developed for
the inclusion of these important variables. this study, students were administered additional scales intended to
assess career decision-making self-efficacy, outcome expectan-
cies, and intentions. Items for the self-efficacy scale were con-
Method structed by modifying items from the Career Decision-Making
Self-Efficacy Scale (CDMSE; Taylor & Betz, 1983) for middle
Participants school students and by writing items for the outcome expectancy
and intentions scales with the cooperation of middle school teach-
Participants were located in an inner city (low socioeconomic ers. Internal consistency reliability coefficients were .79 for the
status) middle school in a large Midwestern urban city. Student modified CDMSE, .70 for outcome expectancies, and .74 for
performances in the middle school on national tests indicated that intentions (Fouad et al., 1995). These general scales are relevant to
90% of the 750 students in the entire school were at the 10th-20th our study in that they serve as discriminant validity criteria for the
percentile compared with national norms on the Iowa Tests of more content-oriented math-science scales. Because the constructs
Basic Skills in language, reading, and math. The average school measured are domain specific (Bandura, 1986), the discriminant
grade point average was 2.03, suspension rate was 45-50% over an validity coefficients can be expected to be attenuated relative to
academic year, and the average daily attendance rate was 80%. those that might have arisen from parallel construct definitions.
Socioeconomic status of students at the school in which this study Other than providing evidence of discriminant validity, the career
was conducted was homogeneous, with 85-95% of students of low decision-making scales were not used in this study.
socioeconomic status (SES): 85% of students qualified for federal Math-science self-efficacy. Items in the Math-Science Self-
funds for free lunch and 5% received reduced-fee lunch. Students Efficacy subscale followed the format used in the Math Tasks
in the school represented a variety of ethnic groups: 58% were subscale of the Math Self-Efficacy Scale (Betz & Hackett, 1983;
Hispanic, 17% were African American, 17% were White, 3% were Bieschke, 1993). The Math Tasks subscale assesses confidence
MIDDLE SCHOOL MATH-SCIENCE SELF-EFFICACY 341

that college students have in completing various tasks. The internal subscales had internal consistency reliabilities of .80 and .81,
consistency reliability of the Math Tasks subscale was .90 with a respectively.
college sample, .93 with a high school sample, with moderate Math-science interests. The Math-Science Interest scale con-
test-retest reliability (Hackett & O'Halloran, 1985, in Bieschke, sisted of twenty 3-point Likert-type scale items designed to assess
1993). Bieschke reported that math self-efficacy beliefs contrib- the individual's interest in mathematics- and science-related activ-
uted significantly to the prediction of interest in math-science ities. Items on the Math-Science Interest scale were developed by
activities. The Math Self-Efficacy subscale was modified in our the first author and middle school teachers, based on Holland's
study to assess tasks that middle school students might encounter (1985) theory of vocational types. Specifically, items were drawn
("Determine the amount of sales tax on clothes I want to buy;" from the investigative theme in areas relevant to middle school
"Design and describe a science experiment I want to do") rather students. Examples of items that followed the stem, "Indicate the
than those encountered by college students (compute your income degree to which you dislike or like the activity," were: "working in
tax; calculate recipe quantities for a dinner for 41 when the original a science lab," "using a calculator," "participating in a science
recipe is for 12 people). Items assessing science self-efficacy were fair," and "taking classes in math." The reliability of this scale was
added to the math items. Items were developed iteratively, origi- .90. Scores on this scale could range from 20 to 60 with higher
nally generated by the first author using the Math Tasks Scale and scores indicating lower interests.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

modified by a self-efficacy expert and a team of middle school


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

teachers.
Data Analysis
The final version of the subscale comprised 12 items. In each
item, students responded to the stem "Indicate your ability to do Structural equation modeling procedures were used to explore
each of the following statements by circling the appropriate num- the theoretical relations that exist among the constructs and to test
bers: 1 = very high, 2 = high ability, 3 = uncertain, 4 = low the plausibility of the hypothesized causal system among certain
ability, and 5 = very low ability. Scores could range from 12 to 60 demographic variables and math-science self-efficacy, outcome
with higher values indicating lower self-efficacy. Internal consis- expectancies, interests, and intentions. Our study involved all
tency reliability for the Math-Science Self-Efficacy (MSSE) sub- observed variables and involved techniques traditionally associ-
scale for this student population was .84. Aside from the content ated with path analysis along with model-fitting criteria associated
validation described earlier, the MSSE scale was administered with structural equation modeling in general. This methodology
along with the Career Decision-Making Self-Efficacy Middle uses a confirmatory approach to investigating the hypothesized
School subscale (Fouad et al., 1995). The discriminant validity causal system and therefore requires an a priori postulation of
coefficient of the MSSE scale with the CDMSE middle school model structure that emerges from theory or empirical research, or
subscale was .54, corrected for attenuation. both.
Math-science outcome expectancies and math-science inten- Analyses were based on the LISREL 8 (Joreskog & Sorbom,
tions. The Math-Science Outcome Expectancies (MSOE) sub- 1993a) program and involved the observed indicators associated
scale contained seven 5-point Likert items, such as "If I learn math with demographics of age and gender, and math-science self-
well, then I will be able to do lots of different types of careers;" "If efficacy, outcome expectancies, interests, and intentions. The raw
I get good grades in math, then my parents will be pleased;" "If I data matrix was prepared before LISREL, using Prelis 2 (Joreskog
do well in science, then I will be better prepared to go to college." & Sorbom, 1993b). This was done to ensure that the data met the
Scores on this scale could range from 7 to 35 with higher values normality assumption associated with structural modeling tech-
indicating lower outcome expectations. The MSOE scale scores niques (West, Finch, & Curran, 1995) and to compute the appro-
were correlated with the Career Decision-Making Outcome Ex- priate correlations and covariances for use in the modeling proce-
pectancies subscale (Fouad et al., 1995), resulting in a discriminant dure.
validity coefficient of .71, corrected for attenuation. If one ignores the path coefficients for the time being, the initial
The Math-Science Intentions (MSI) subscale contained six analysis tested the structural relations hypothesized in Figure 2. An
5-point Likert items, such as "I intend to take science classes in additional analysis based on a variation of this model was done to
high school;" "I intend to enter a career that will use math," improve model fit. Once it was evident that a better fit could be
assessing intent to choose math and science activities or goals. achieved, we respecified the initial model to include new paths and
This scale also measures individuals' intent to persist in pursuing eliminate old paths, resulting in a better fitting model. Model
those activities. Students responded to the stem "Indicate the modification was based on the significance of the path coefficients
degree to which you agree or disagree with each statement below."
Scores on this scale ranged from 6 to 30 with higher scores
indicating lower intentions with respect to math and science. The
MSI scale was correlated with the Career Decision-Making Inten-
tions and Goals subscale (Fouad et al., 1995), which was also
administered to the study group, resulting in a discriminant validity
coefficient of .66, corrected for attenuation.
Items assessing math-science outcome expectancies and inten-
tions were developed by using a format similar to Lent et al. (1991,
1993). Lent et al. (1991) found an internal consistency of .90 for
their 10-item scale with a college sample, and a 2-week test-retest
reliability of .91. Following the Lent et al. format, outcome ex-
pectancies were developed by the researchers, in cooperation with
self-efficacy experts and teachers, again in an iterative process. In
other words, items were developed that would be age appropriate, Figure 2. Initial structure and associated path coefficients (with
then items were examined by self-efficacy experts and middle standard errors). Significant (p < .05) coefficients are shown in
school teachers. The final outcome expectancies and intentions bold typeface.
342 FOUAD AND SMITH

derived from the original model (nonsignificant paths were elim- essary assumptions for structural equation modeling and
inated from the subsequent model) and examination of the fitted produced the correlation matrix among the variables shown
residuals from the original model (paths were added to a modified in Table 1. A point-biserial correlation was used to represent
model where large residuals existed). In the latter case, paths were
the relationship between gender (dichotomous variable) and
added to the modified model only in instances that were theoret-
ically justified. The final model was cross-validated by using a age (polychotomous variable). All relationships between
bootstrap-type procedure in which the final model was tested using age or gender and the other variables were computed by
five independently drawn random samples (n = 200 each) from using polyserial correlations; the rest of the relationships
the full data set. The model was further validated separately for were expressed by using the Pearson product-moment cor-
each of the three most prevalent ethnic groups represented in the relation. A summary of the results of the model-fitting
study sample. procedures are in Table 2. (The initial model tested with
Assessment of model fit in each instance was based on multiple these data is shown in Figure 2 along with the associated
considerations, including statistical and practical ones. In any path coefficients and their significance.) Following our ad-
model-fitting exercise, it is important to use multiple criteria aptation of Lent et al. (1994) for the variables included in
because of the influence of sample size on various fit indexes.
this study, our model emphasized paths leading from the
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Convergence of several criteria of fit increases confidence in the


demographic characteristics (in this case, age and gender) to
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

viability of the hypothesized model. In this study, model fit was


assessed by using the chi-squared likelihood ratio statistic, the both self-efficacy and outcome expectancies. Self-efficacy
adjusted goodness-of-fit index (AGFI), the root-mean-square re- was modeled as a determinant of outcome expectancies,
sidual (RMR), and the j^ldf ratio. The chi-square statistic is a interests, and intentions. Outcome expectancy was modeled
measure of the overall fit of the specified model to the data as a determinant of interest and intentions, and interests
(Joreskog & Sorbom, 1993a). A small chi-square statistic corre- were modeled as determinants of intentions. The first line in
sponds to a good fit and a large value to a poor fit. However, the Table 2 identifies the parameters and fit statistics associated
chi-square statistic is not always a good indication of a model fit,
especially with larger samples, because it tends to take on larger
with this model and suggests that, although adequate, a
values and therefore may result in a misleading interpretation of revised model may more adequately fit the data.
the data. The goodness-of-fit index (GFI) and the AGFI were Model modification. On the basis of the initial fit statis-
introduced to eliminate this dependence on sample size (Joreskog tics, the correlations between the original variables, and the
& Sorbom, 1989) and indicate the relative amount of variance and path coefficients identified by the first model, the hypoth-
covariance in the data explained by the hypothesized model esized model was revised and refitted. In this revised model,
(Byrne, 1989). The AGFI theoretically takes on values between 0 the paths leading from age to outcome expectancies and
and 1, with higher numbers indicating a better fit. In general, those from gender to both self-efficacy and outcome expec-
AGFIs greater than .9 are considered good model fits (Byrne,
1989; Loehlin, 1987). RMR is an indication of the average dis-
tations were eliminated from the model because of nonsig-
crepancy between elements in the observed and hypothesized nificant path coefficients. Paths from both age and gender to
covariance matrices and should be less than .05 if a good model fit interest were added to the model to assess the influence of
exists (Byrne, 1989). A frequently used procedure for determining these demographic variables as antecedents. An analysis of
the adequacy of model fit is the ffldf ratio (Joreskog & Sorbom, the fitted residuals provided by the LISREL output sug-
1979). This ratio, if large, suggests that the model can be im- gested the addition of these paths to achieve a better model
proved. Small values (less than 2 or 3) of this ratio indicate good fit. The fit statistics associated with this revised model are
model fit. Marsh, Balla. and McDonald (1988) provided a reason- shown in the second line of Table 2. These fit statistics
ably comprehensive discussion of the various criteria that have
been suggested for determining model fit in structural equation
indicate that the data fit the model very well. The third line
modeling. of Table 2 shows the differences between the original model
and the revised model and indicates that the revised model
provides a significantly better fit than the original model.
Results Figure 3 illustrates this revised model, including the path
coefficients and associated standard errors.
Analysis of the raw data by using Prelis 2 (Joreskog & Of particular interest in Figure 3 are the direct paths from
Sorbom, 1993b) indicated that the data met all of the nec- self-efficacy to intentions. Direct path coefficient estimates

Table 1
Correlations Between Variables Used in the Study
Variable 1 2 3 4 5 6
1. Age
2. Gender .1020
3. Self-efficacy -.0690 .0940 —
4. Outcome expectancies -.0620 -.1260 .5330
5. Interests -.1230 .1380 .4020 .3190
6. Intentions -.1370 -.0700 .4490 .5470 .4530 —
M 13.572 _ 42.850 24.473 44.663 22.898
SD 0.662 — 7.784 4.089 8.146 4.313
MIDDLE SCHOOL MATH-SCIENCE SELF-EFFICACY 343

Table 2
Fit Statistics Associated With Study Models
Group/model x2 df P AGFI RMR Ratio
Full population
Initial 19.01 4 .001 .91 .041 4.75
Modified 5.91 3 .120 .96 .022 1.97
Difference 13.10 1 <.01
Ethnic group fit
White 1.78 3 .800 .97 .027 .59
Hispanic 3.60 3 .310 .96 .021 1.20
African American 2.12 3 .550 .88 .030 .71
Note. AGFI = adjusted goodness-of-fit index; RMR = root-mean-square residual.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

appear on the arrows directly joining the measures of inter- data and the final model was tested for fit with the covari-
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

est, whereas estimates of the indirect effects are obtained by ance structure derived from these samples. In all five sam-
multiplying the coefficient estimates associated with the ples the data provided an acceptable fit to the model
combination of arrows that form the indirect path. In the (AGFI > .90 and RMR < .05), lending further credibility to
case of the self-efficacy to intention paths, the direct path the robustness of the specified model for this sample.
coefficient estimate is .13, whereas the indirect paths
through outcome expectations and interests are estimated to
be .21 and .08, respectively. The magnitudes of these indi- Discussion
rect paths relative to the direct path are noteworthy.
Most of the studies that have examined social-cognitive
Because the sample study comprised (primarily) three
theory in career-related behavior have focused on high
distinct ethnic groups, it was important to assess the extent
school and college students. These studies have, in general,
to which the data for each of these three groups fit the final
examined decision making as it occurs for students making
model. The last three lines of Table 2 indicate the fit
postsecondary education and career choices. Recently, how-
statistics associated with this final model when applied to
ever, attention has shifted to examining self-efficacy in
each of the three primary ethnic groups represented. Group
younger students in areas such as academic achievement
1 refers to students who self-identified as White (N = 55),
and motivation (Zimmerman, 1995) and math performance
Group 2 refers to Hispanic students (N = 225), and Group
(Meece, Wigfield, & Eccles, 1990) as well as interventions
3 refers to African American students (N = 40). In each
to promote self-efficacy that might lead to better academic
case, the data fit the model well. It is important to note that
and career choices and persistence in math and science
these fit statistics do not necessarily imply that the model is
activities. The model proposed by Lent et al. (1994) results
invariant across these three ethnic groups, rather, that the
in a fairly elaborate model of how basic career interests
data from each of these groups could plausibly have arisen
develop and was tested to examine how it might be ex-
from the proposed model. A test of this former hypothesis
tended to middle school students. Math and science perfor-
was not appropriate because of the relatively small sample
mance is one area on which early interventions have fo-
sizes associated with two of these three groups.
cused to promote achievement as well as course selection in
Finally, as with any model-fitting procedure, the question high school (e.g., Fouad, 1995; Kahle & Danzl-Tauer, 1991;
of the sample dependence of model fit is likely to arise. Matyas, 1992; Waks, 1991).
Aside from the tests associated with each of the most
prevalent ethnic groups represented, a modified bootstrap The results of this analysis are revealing with respect to
approach was used to test the model. In this approach, five several aspects of the model proposed by Lent et al. (1994)
independent samples of 200 were randomly drawn from the as it applies to the middle school population of focus in this
study. First, the analysis suggests that there are small, but
significant, effects of person inputs (gender and age) on
outcome expectancies and interests in mathematics and sci-
ence but no such effects on self-efficacy. With respect to
age, our model depicted in Figure 3 posits a significant
negative relationship with interests in mathematics and sci-
Gender ence. Because all of the students in the study were seventh
Intentions
and eighth graders with an age distribution that might be
Age expected for this level, this relationship may signal a general
longitudinal decline in interest in mathematics and science
Outcome for this population. There are, of course, a number of
Expecations hypotheses that might account for this trend. These include
the increasing challenge and scope of the mathematics and
Figure 3. Final structure and path coefficients (with standard science content in the middle school curriculum, or a gen-
errors). All coefficients are significant (p < .05). eral broadening of the scope of academic content presented
344 FOUAD AND SMITH

to students during these years. Factors such as these could that interventions that focus on outcome expectancies are
contribute to a diffusion of interests in general, accounting needed.
for a decline in interest in mathematics and science. Inter- Self-efficacy also has a fairly large direct influence on
ventions may need to be directed to middle school students interests (.29), which in turn has a fairly large effect on
to counteract a decrease in interests in science and math intentions (.28). The effect of this indirect path is nearly as
courses. Research on the genetic influence of interests large as the direct path from self-efficacy to intentions. It is
(Betsworth et al., 1994) indicates that 55% of the variance clear that self-efficacy plays a significant role in the devel-
in interests were due to nonshared environmental effects, or opment of intentions for these middle school students, but it
those that are unique to individuals rather than to environ- appears to do so primarily through its relationship to inter-
ments (Gottfredson, 1995), and that heritability estimates ests and outcome expectations rather than in a direct way.
increase with age. These results suggest that the middle The results indicate that the relationships among the per-
school years are a prime time to influence the development sonal indicators, self-efficacy, outcome expectations, inter-
of interests of minority youth and girls in math and science ests, and intentions are complex, as suggested by the Lent et
activities. al. (1994) model.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

With respect to gender, we observed there were signifi- Finally, not only do these data support the model posited
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

cant paths to interests and outcome expectancies, the latter by Lent et al. (1994) for middle school students, the model
relationship being negative. These paths indicate that male appears to be robust for the three major ethnic groups
students have lower interest in mathematics and sciences represented in the study. It is critical to test models with
than do female students, but that outcome expectancies are diverse populations to determine whether career develop-
higher for male students in these areas. Also noteworthy are ment models developed with research using predominantly
the lack of significant relationships between the age and college students apply to other groups. We believe this
gender variables and self-efficacy. Although the path from study indicates that the Lent et al. social-cognitive model
gender to self-efficacy was negative, which supports earlier may explain career behaviors of African Americans,
research (e.g., Betz & Hackett, 1981; Hackett, 1985; Post Whites, and Hispanics. More research is needed that di-
rectly tests the influence of race and ethnicity as an ante-
Kammer & Smith, 1986), the path coefficient did not con-
cedent variable affecting self-efficacy, outcome expectan-
tribute to a significantly better fitting model. This may have
cies, interests, and intentions-goals.
been due to the SES of the students. Although not directly
tested in this study, the path from gender to self-efficacy is This study was conducted with middle school students
through the learning experiences that are postulated as who were homogeneous with respect to SES. Although
sources of self-efficacy, and the relatively low SES of this holding SES relatively constant is a strength of the study, it
group of students may have provided them with fewer is also a limitation; more research is needed that examines
learning experiences. In a similar vein, little is known about these factors across other SES groups, as well as other
the interaction between ethnicity, gender, and learning ex- geographic locations, and other racial and ethnic groups.
periences, and the results may have been specific to this Research needs to focus on low-SES individuals to under-
ethnically diverse sample of students. stand how various vocational variables may be tied to
economic variables. However, examining one homogeneous
Although in this study we used only two person input group restricts the range of relationships, and that is a
variables (gender and age), it may be important to note that limitation of the study. Another limitation of this study is
the direct paths from these variables to interests are absent that learning experiences were not included. This is an
in the Lent et al. (1994) model. It seems likely that direct important component of the Lent et al. (1994) model and is
paths from input variables to interest emerge through both theoretically critical to include. As noted earlier, the effect
formal and informal learning experiences in this critical of gender and age implicitly influences interests and out-
developmental stage as well as variation in these experi- come expectancies through learning experiences. Addi-
ences by gender. In short, school and out-of-school experi- tional person input variables could have been tested as well,
ences directly affect the interests of students in mathematics particularly race and ethnicity. Although teacher selection
and science, and these effects are not comparable for both in the program was based on language proficiency of stu-
genders. Future research should include a broader array of dents, our study would have been strengthened by having
these personal variables to more completely address their multiple school sites to draw from as well as validated
effects of self-efficacy, outcome expectations, and inten- translations of the instruments to assess all students in the
tions. building. Finally, although bootstrapping procedures al-
The remaining paths in the model conform to the propo- lowed some evaluation of sample independence, the best
sitions and model set forth by Lent et al. (1994) but are of way to assure this is to test the model on a completely
interest because of the relative magnitudes of the path separate sample. This, too, is a limitation of the study and
coefficients. Especially noteworthy are the strong positive should be an area for future research.
paths between self-efficacy and outcome expectations (.55) The need for further research has already been identified
and between outcome expectations and intentions (.39). in a number of areas, but one additional area should be
These coefficients imply that the influence of self-efficacy examined. This study did not investigate self-efficacy, out-
on intentions is due less to a direct path (.13) and more to come expectancies, interests, and intentions as determinants
the indirect effect through outcome expectations. It is clear of behavioral indexes, such as actual choices made by
MIDDLE SCHOOL MATH-SCIENCE SELF-EFFICACY 345

students. The focus of this study was on determinants of Fitzgerald, L. F., & Betz, N. E. (1994). Career development in
intentions for middle school students, operating under the cultural context: The role of gender, race, class, and sexual
assumption that intentions of choices and persistence in orientation. In M. L. Savickas & R. W. Lent (Eds.), Conver-
early adolescence is an important variable to examine. How- gence in career development theories (pp. 103-118). Palo Alto,
ever, future studies should examine the relationship of in- CA: Consulting Psychologists Press.
tentions to actual choice, such as extracurricular activities or Flammer, A. (1995). Developmental analysis of control beliefs. In
A. Bandura (Ed.), Self-efficacy in changing societies (pp. 6 9 -
course selection, and to overall persistence in math and
113). New York: Cambridge University Press.
science (particularly postsecondary training and career
Fouad, N. A. (1994). Career assessment with Latinos/Hispanics.
choice). Journal of Career Assessment, 2, 226-239.
This study is an initial step in testing a model that may Fouad, N. A. (1995). Career linking: An intervention to promote
lead directly to self-efficacy and outcome expectancy inter- math and science career awareness. Journal of Counseling &
ventions for middle school students. It is very important that Development, 73, 527-534.
counseling psychologists design, help implement, and eval- Fouad, N. A., Smith, P., & Enochs, L. (1995). Reliability and
uate these interventions. Counseling psychologists should validity of the Middle School Self-Efficacy Scale. Manuscript
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

play an active role in the national School to Work Initiative submitted for publication.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

and in implementing the Education 2000 goals. To do so, Gelso, C. J., & Fassinger, R. E. (1990). Counseling psychology:
they must understand whether models and theory apply to Theory and research on interventions. In M. R. Rosenzweig &
racially, socioeconomically, and developmentally diverse L. W. Porter (Eds.), Annual review ofpsychology (pp. 355-386).
populations. Palo Alto, CA: Annual Reviews.
Gottfredson, L. S. (1995, August). The nature of interests: A
discussion. Paper presented at the 103rd Annual Convention of
the American Psychological Association, New York.
References Hackett, G. (1985). The role of mathematics self-efficacy in the
choice of math-related majors of college women and men: A
Bandura, A. (1977). Self-efficacy: Toward a unifying theory of
path model. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 32, 47-56.
behavioral change. Psychological Review, 84, 191-215.
Hackett, G. (1995). Self-efficacy in career choice and develop-
Bandura, A. (1986). Social foundations of thought and action: A
social cognitive theory. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall. ment. In A. Bandura (Ed.), Self-efficacy in changing society (pp.
232-258). New York: Cambridge University Press.
Bandura, A. (1995). Exercise of personal and collective efficacy in
changing societies. In A. Bandura (Ed.), Self efficacy in chang- Hackett, G., & Betz, N. E. (1981). A self-efficacy approach to the
ing societies (pp. 1-45). New York: Cambridge University career development of women. Journal of Vocational Behavior,
Press. 18, 326-339.
Betsworth, D. G., Bouchard, T. J., Cooper, C. R., Grotevant, Hackett, G., & Betz, N. E. (1989). An exploration of the mathe-
H. D., Hansen, J. C , Scarr, S., & Weinberg, R. A. (1994). Ge- matics self-efficacy/mathematics performance correspondence.
netic and environmental influences on vocational interests as- Journal of Research in Mathematics Education, 20, 261-273.
sessed using adoptive and biological families and twins reared Hackett, G., Betz, N. E., Casas, J. M., & Rocha-Singh, LA.
apart and together. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 44, 263- (1992). Gender, ethnicity, and social cognitive factors predicting
278. the academic achievement of students in engineering. Journal of
Betz, N. E., & Hackett, G. (1981). The relationship of career- Counseling Psychology, 39, 527-538.
related self-efficacy expectations to perceived career options in Hackett, G., & Lent, R. W. (1992). Theoretical advances and
college women and men. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 28, current inquiry in career psychology. In S. D. Brown & R. W.
399-410. Lent (Eds.), Handbook of counseling psychology (2nd ed., pp.
Betz, N., & Hackett, G. (1983). The relationship of mathematics 419-452). New York: Wiley Interscience.
self-efficacy expectations to the selection of science-based col- Hackett, G., & O'Halloran, M. S. (1985). [Test-retest reliability of
lege majors. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 23, 329-345. career self-efficacy measures]. Unpublished raw data.
Betz, N. E., & Hackett, G. (1986). Applications of self-efficacy Holland, J. L. (1985). Making vocational choices: A theory of
theory to understanding choice behavior. Journal of Social and vocational personalities and work environments (2nd ed.).
Clinical Psychology, 4, 279-289. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.
Bieschke, K. J. (1993). A causal model of math/science career Joreskog, K. G., & Sorbom, D. (1979). Advances in factor analysis
aspirations. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Michigan State and structural equation models. Cambridge, MA: Abt Books.
University. Joreskog, K. G., & Sorbom, D. (1989). USREL 7: A guide to the
Borgen, F. H. (1991). Megatrends and milestones in vocational program and applications. Chicago: SPSS.
behavior: A 20-year counseling psychology retrospective. Jour- Joreskog, K. G., & Sorbom, D. (1993a). USREL 8: Structural
nal of Vocational Behavior, 39, 263-290. equation modeling with the SIMPLIS command language.
Brown, S. D., Lent, R. W., & Larkin, K. C. (1989). Self-efficacy as Mooresville, IN: Scientific Software.
a moderator of scholastic aptitude-academic performance rela- Joreskog, K. G., & Sorbom, D. (1993b). Prelis Z: A program for
tionships. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 35, 64-75. multivariate data screening and data summarization: A prepro-
Byrne, B. M. (1989). A primer ofUSREL: Basic applications and cessor for LISREL. Chicago: Scientific Software International.
programming for confirmatory factor analytic models. New Kahle, J., & Danzl-Tauer, L. (1991). The underutilized majority:
York: Springer-Verlag. The participation of women in science. In S. Majumdar, L. M.
Cooper, S. E., & Robinson, D. A. (1991). The relation of mathe- Rosenfeld, P. A. Rubba, E. W. Miller, & R. F. Schmalz (Eds.),
matics self-efficacy beliefs to mathematics anxiety and perfor- Science education in the United States: Issues, crises and pri-
mance. Measurement and Evaluation in Counseling and Devel- orities (pp. 483-502). Easton, PA: Pennsylvania Academy of
opment, 24, 4-11. Science.
346 FOUAD AND SMITH

Lent, R. W., Brown, S. D., & Hackett, G. (1994). Toward a uni- and the prediction of work and home role salience. Journal of
fying social cognitive theory of career and academic interest, Career Assessment, 3, 331-340.
choice, and performance. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 45, Meece, J. L., Wigfield, A., & Eccles, J. S. (1990). Predictors of
79-122. math anxiety and its influence on young adolescents' course
Lent, R. W., Brown, S. D., & Larkin, K. C. (1984). Relation of enrollment intentions and performance in mathematics. Journal
self-efficacy expectations to academic achievement and persis- of Educational Psychology, 82, 60-70.
tence. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 31, 356-362. Niles, S. G., & Sowa, C. J. (1992). Mapping the nomological
Lent, R. W., Brown, S. D., & Larkin, K. C. (1986). Self-efficacy in network of career self-efficacy. Career Development Quarterly,
the prediction of academic performance and perceived career 41, 12-21.
options. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 33, 265-269. Post Kammer, P., & Smith, P. L. (1986). Sex differences in math
Lent, R. W., Lopez, F. G., & Bieschke, K. J. (1991). Mathematics and science career self-efficacy among disadvantaged students.
self-efficacy: Sources and relations to choice. Journal of Coun- Journal of Vocational Behavior, 29, 89-101.
seling Psychology, 38, 424-430. Rooney, R. A., & Osipow, S. H. (1992). Task-specific occupa-
Lent, R. W., Lopez, F. G., & Bieschke, K. J. (1993). Predicting tional self-efficacy scale: The development and validation of a
mathematics-related choice and success behaviors: Test of an prototype. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 40, 14-32.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

expanded social cognitive mode. Journal of Vocational Behav- Taylor, K. M., & Betz, N. E. (1983). Applications of self-efficacy
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

ior, 42, 223-236. theory to the understanding and treatment of career indecision.
Loehlin, J. C. (1987). Latent variable models: An introduction to Journal of Vocational Behavior, 22, 63- 81.
factor, path and structural analysis. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Waks, L. (1991). Technological literacy for the new majority. In S.
Lopez, F. G., & Lent, R. W. (1992). Sources of mathematics Majumdar, et al. (Eds.), Science education in the United States:
self-efficacy in high school students. Career Development Quar- Issues, crises and priorities (pp. 483-502). Easton, PA: Penn-
terly, 41, 3-12. sylvania Academy of Science.
Luzzo, D. A. (1993). Value of career decision-making self- West, S. G., Finch, J. F., & Curran, P. J. (1995). Structural equa-
efficacy in predicting career decision-making attitudes and tion models with nonnormal variables: Problems and remedies.
skills. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 40, 194-199. In R. H. Hoyle (Ed.), Structural equation modeling: Concepts,
Marsh, H. W., Balla, J. R., & McDonald, R. P. (1988). Goodness- issues and applications (pp. 56-73). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
of-fit indexes in confirmatory factor analysis: The effect of Williams, T. W., & Betz, N. E. (1994). The relationships among
sample size. Psychological Bulletin, 103, 391-410. occupational and task-specific measures of career self-efficacy.
Mathieu, P. S., Sowa, C. J., & Niles, S. G. (1993). Differences in Journal of Career Assessment, 2, 341-351.
career self-efficacy among women. Journal of Career Develop- Zimmerman, B. J. (1995). Self-efficacy and educational develop-
ment, 19, 187-196. ment. In A. Bandura (Ed.), Self-efficacy in changing societies
Matyas, M. (1992). Promoting undergraduate studies in science (pp. 202-231). New York: Cambridge University Press.
and engineering. In M. Matyas & L. Dix (Eds.), Science and
engineering programs: On target for women? (pp. 43-66). Received October 23, 1995
Washington, DC: National Academy Press. Revision received February 26, 1996
Matzeder, M. E., & Krieshok, T. S. (1995). Career self-efficacy Accepted February 26, 1996 •

Mentoring Program Available for International Scholars

APA's Committee on International Relations in Psychology is encouraging publication of inter-


national scholars' manuscripts in U.S. journals. To accomplish this initiative, the Committee is
looking for authors whose native language is not English to work with U.S. mentors. U.S. men-
tors will help authors bring manuscripts into conformity with English-language and U.S. publi-
cation standards.

The Committee also continues to update its mentor list and is looking for U.S. mentors, espe-
cially those with translating and APA journal experience. Interested individuals should contact
Marian Wood in the APA International Affairs Office, 750 First Street, NE, Washington, DC
20002-4242. Electronic mail may be sent via Internet to mzw.apa@email.apa.org; Telephone:
(202) 336-6025; Fax: (202) 336-5919.

You might also like